University Press Scholarship Online Oxford Scholarship ...

79
Rhythm Page 1 of 79 PRINTED FROM OXFORD SCHOLARSHIP ONLINE (www.oxfordscholarship.com). (c) Copyright Oxford University Press, 2015. All Rights Reserved. Under the terms of the licence agreement, an individual user may print out a PDF of a single chapter of a monograph in OSO for personal use (for details see http://www.oxfordscholarship.com/page/privacy-policy ). Subscriber: UC - Santa Cruz; date: 02 May 2015 University Press Scholarship Online Oxford Scholarship Online Music, Language, and the Brain Aniruddh D. Patel Print publication date: 2007 Print ISBN-13: 9780195123753 Published to Oxford Scholarship Online: March 2012 DOI: 10.1093/acprof:oso/9780195123753.001.0001 Rhythm Aniruddh D. Patel DOI:10.1093/acprof:oso/9780195123753.003.0003 Abstract and Keywords Although there have already been several studies that investigated how rhythm is integrated with the domains of both music and speech, there have rarely been comparative studies regarding musical rhythm and linguistic rhythm. The lack of such comparative research may be attributed to how researchers of one domain barely have time to look into the complexities of the other since each possesses its own large share of complexities, rather than lack of interest. As such research may enable us to gain a wider view on how rhythm plays a central part in human cognition, one of the goals of this chapter involves providing both empirical and conceptual tools for researchers to examine the links between musical and linguistic rhythm. This chapter clarifies the definition of “rhythm”, introduces the notion of rhythm in speech, and provides an account regarding cross-domain comparisons of rhythm.

Transcript of University Press Scholarship Online Oxford Scholarship ...

Page 1: University Press Scholarship Online Oxford Scholarship ...

Rhythm

Page 1 of 79

PRINTED FROM OXFORD SCHOLARSHIP ONLINE (www.oxfordscholarship.com). (c) Copyright Oxford University Press, 2015.All Rights Reserved. Under the terms of the l icence agreement, an individual user may print out a PDF of a single chapter of amonograph in OSO for personal use (for details see http://www.oxfordscholarship.com/page/privacy-policy). Subscriber: UC - SantaCruz; date: 02 May 2015

UniversityPressScholarshipOnline

OxfordScholarshipOnline

Music,Language,andtheBrainAniruddhD.Patel

Printpublicationdate:2007PrintISBN-13:9780195123753PublishedtoOxfordScholarshipOnline:March2012DOI:10.1093/acprof:oso/9780195123753.001.0001

Rhythm

AniruddhD.Patel

DOI:10.1093/acprof:oso/9780195123753.003.0003

AbstractandKeywords

Althoughtherehavealreadybeenseveralstudiesthatinvestigatedhowrhythmisintegratedwiththedomainsofbothmusicandspeech,therehaverarelybeencomparativestudiesregardingmusicalrhythmandlinguisticrhythm.Thelackofsuchcomparativeresearchmaybeattributedtohowresearchersofonedomainbarelyhavetimetolookintothecomplexitiesoftheothersinceeachpossessesitsownlargeshareofcomplexities,ratherthanlackofinterest.Assuchresearchmayenableustogainawiderviewonhowrhythmplaysacentralpartinhumancognition,oneofthegoalsofthischapterinvolvesprovidingbothempiricalandconceptualtoolsforresearcherstoexaminethelinksbetweenmusicalandlinguisticrhythm.Thischapterclarifiesthedefinitionof“rhythm”,introducesthenotionofrhythminspeech,andprovidesanaccountregardingcross-domaincomparisonsofrhythm.

Page 2: University Press Scholarship Online Oxford Scholarship ...

Rhythm

Page 2 of 79

PRINTED FROM OXFORD SCHOLARSHIP ONLINE (www.oxfordscholarship.com). (c) Copyright Oxford University Press, 2015.All Rights Reserved. Under the terms of the l icence agreement, an individual user may print out a PDF of a single chapter of amonograph in OSO for personal use (for details see http://www.oxfordscholarship.com/page/privacy-policy). Subscriber: UC - SantaCruz; date: 02 May 2015

Keywords:rhythm,music,speech,musicalrhythm,linguisticrhythm,comparativeresearch

Chapter3Rhythm3.1Introduction963.2RhythminMusic97

3.2.1TheBeat:AStableMentalPeriodicity993.2.2Meter:MultiplePeriodicities1033.2.3Grouping:ThePerceptualSegmentationofEvents1063.2.4DurationalPatterninginMusic112DurationCategoriesinMusic112ExpressiveTiminginMusic1143.2.5ThePsychologicalDimensionsofMusicalRhythm117

3.3RhythminSpeech1183.3.1RhythmicTypology119PeriodicityandTypology119PhonologyandTypology122DurationandTypology126PerceptionandTypology1353.3.2PrinciplesGoverningtheRhythmicShapeofWordsandUtterances138DifferencesBetweenLinguisticandMusicalMetricalGrids141QuestioningthePrincipleofRhythmicAlternationinSpeech1413.3.3ThePerceptionofSpeechRhythm143ThePerceptionofIsochronyinSpeech143TheRoleofRhythmicPredictabilityinSpeechPerception145TheRoleofRhythminSegmentingConnectedSpeech147TheRoleofRhythminthePerceptionofNonnativeAccents1483.3.4FinalCommentsonSpeechRhythm:MovingBeyondIsochrony149

3.4Interlude:RhythminPoetryandSong1543.4.1RhythminPoetry1543.4.2RhythminSong156

3.5NonperiodicAspectsofRhythmasaKeyLink1593.5.1RelationsBetweenMusicalStructureandLinguisticRhythm1593.5.2RelationsBetweenNonlinguisticRhythmPerceptionandSpeechRhythm1683.5.3NeuralRelationshipsBetweenRhythminSpeechandMusic173

3.6Conclusion176

Appendixes

Page 3: University Press Scholarship Online Oxford Scholarship ...

Rhythm

Page 3 of 79

PRINTED FROM OXFORD SCHOLARSHIP ONLINE (www.oxfordscholarship.com). (c) Copyright Oxford University Press, 2015.All Rights Reserved. Under the terms of the l icence agreement, an individual user may print out a PDF of a single chapter of amonograph in OSO for personal use (for details see http://www.oxfordscholarship.com/page/privacy-policy). Subscriber: UC - SantaCruz; date: 02 May 2015

A.1ThenPVIEquation177A.2MusicalnPVIValuesofDifferentNations178

(p.96) 3.1IntroductionThecomparativestudyofspokenandmusicalrhythmissurprisinglyunderdeveloped.Althoughhundredsofstudieshaveexploredrhythmwithineachdomain,empiricalcomparisonsoflinguisticandmusicalrhythmarerare.Thisdoesnotreflectalackofinterest,becauseresearchershavelongnotedconnectionsbetweentheoriesofrhythminthetwodomains(e.g.,Selkirk,1984;Handel,1989).Thepaucityofcomparativeresearchprobablyreflectsthefactthatspecialistsinonedomainseldomhavethetimetodelveintotheintricaciesoftheother.Thisisregrettable,becausecross-domainworkcanprovideabroaderperspectiveonrhythminhumancognition.Onegoalofthischapteristoequipresearcherswithconceptualandempiricaltoolstoexploretheborderlandbetweenlinguisticandmusicalrhythm.Asweshallsee,thisisafertileareafornewdiscoveries.

Beforeembarking,itisworthaddressingtwooverarchingissues.Thefirstisthedefinitionofrhythm.Theterm“rhythm”occursinmanycontextsbesidesspeechandmusic,suchascircadianrhythms,oscillationsinthebrain,andtherhythmiccallsofcertainanimals.Inmostofthesecontexts,“rhythm”denotesperiodicity,inotherwords,apatternrepeatingregularlyintime.Althoughperiodicityisanimportantaspectofrhythm,itiscrucialtodistinguishbetweenthetwoconcepts.Thecruxofthematterissimplythis:Althoughallperiodicpatternsarerhythmic,notallrhythmicpatternsareperiodic.Thatis,periodicityisbutonetypeofrhythmicorganization.Thispointisespeciallyimportantforunderstandingspeechrhythm,whichhashadalong(andasweshallsee,largelyunfruitful)associationwiththenotionofperiodicity.Thusanydefinitionofrhythmshouldleaveopentheissueofperiodicity.Unfortunately,thereisnouniversallyaccepteddefinitionofrhythm.ThusIwilldefinerhythmasthesystematicpatterningofsoundintermsoftiming,accent,andgrouping.Bothspeechandmusicarecharacterizedbysystematictemporal,accentual,andphrasalpatterning.Howdothesepatternscompare?Whatistheirrelationshipinthemind?

(p.97) Thesecondissueistheverynotionofrhythminspeech,whichmaybeunfamiliartosomereaders.Onewaytoinformallyintroducethisconceptistoconsidertheprocessoflearningaforeignlanguage.Speakingalanguagewithnativefluencyrequiresmorethanmasteringitsphonemes,vocabulary,andgrammar.Onemustalsomasterthepatternsoftimingandaccentuationthatcharacterizetheflowofsyllablesinsentences.Thatis,eachlanguagehasarhythmthatispartofitssonicstructure,andanimplicitknowledgeofthisrhythmispartofaspeaker’scompetenceintheirlanguage.Afailuretoacquirenativerhythmisanimportantfactorincreatingaforeignaccentinspeech(Taylor,1981;Faber,1986;Chela-Flores,1994).

Thefollowingtwosections(3.2and3.3)giveoverviewsofrhythminmusicandspeech,respectively,focusingonissuespertinenttocross-domaincomparisons.(Suchcomparisonsaremadewithineachsectionwhereappropriate.)Theseoverviewsmotivateaparticularwayoflookingatrhythmicrelationsbetweenspeechandmusic.Thisnew

Page 4: University Press Scholarship Online Oxford Scholarship ...

Rhythm

Page 4 of 79

PRINTED FROM OXFORD SCHOLARSHIP ONLINE (www.oxfordscholarship.com). (c) Copyright Oxford University Press, 2015.All Rights Reserved. Under the terms of the l icence agreement, an individual user may print out a PDF of a single chapter of amonograph in OSO for personal use (for details see http://www.oxfordscholarship.com/page/privacy-policy). Subscriber: UC - SantaCruz; date: 02 May 2015

perspectiveisintroducedinthefinalsectionofthechapter,togetherwithempiricalevidencespanningacoustic,perceptual,andneuralstudies.

3.2RhythminMusicThefollowingdiscussionofrhythminmusicfocusesonmusicthathasaregularlytimedbeat,aperceptuallyisochronouspulsetowhichonecansynchronizewithperiodicmovementssuchastapsorfootfalls.Furthermore,thefocusisonmusicoftheWesternEuropeantradition,inwhichbeatsareorganizedinhierarchiesofbeatstrength,withalternationbetweenstrongerandweakerbeats.Thisformofrhythmicorganizationhasbeenthemostwidelystudiedfromatheoreticalandempiricalstandpoint,andisalsothetypeofrhythmmostoftencomparedwithspeech,eitherimplicitlyorexplicitly(Pike,1945;Liberman,1975;Selkirk,1984).

Itisimportanttorealize,however,thatthisisjustonewayinwhichhumansorganizemusicalrhythm.ItwouldbeconvenientiftherhythmicstructureofWesternmusicindicatedgeneralprinciplesofrhythmicpatterning.Realityismorecomplex,however,andonlyacomparisonofdifferentculturaltraditionscanhelpsiftwhatisuniversalfromwhatisparticular.Toillustratethispoint,onecannotemusicaltraditionsinwhichrhythmisorganizedinratherdifferentwaysthaninmostWesternEuropeanmusic.

OnesuchtraditioninvolvestheCh’in,asevenstringfretlesszitherthathasbeenplayedinChinaforover2,000years(vanGulik,1940).Themusicalnotationforthisinstrumentcontainsnotimemarkingsforindividualnotes,indicatingonlythestringandtypeofgestureusedtoproducethenote(thoughsometimesphraseboundariesaremarked).Theresultingmusichasnosenseofabeat.Instead,ithasaflowingqualityinwhichthetimingofnotesemergesfromthegesturaldynamicsofthehandsratherthanfromanexplicitlyregulated(p.98) temporalscheme.TheCh’inisjustoneofmanyexamplesofunpulsedmusicfromaroundtheglobe,allofwhichshowthatthemindiscapableoforganizingtemporalpatternswithoutreferencetoabeat.

AnothertraditionwhoserhythmsarequitedifferentfromWesternEuropeanmusicisBalkanfolkmusicfromEasternEurope(Singer,1974;London,1995).Thismusichassalientbeats,butthebeatsarenotspacedatregulartemporalintervals.Instead,intervalsbetweenbeatsareeitherlongorshort,withthelongintervalbeing3/2thelengthoftheshorterone.Rhythmiccyclesarebuiltfromrepeatingpatternsoflongandshortintervals,suchasS-S-S-L,S-S-L-S-S(notethatthelongelementisnotconstrainedtooccurattheendofthecycle).Onemightthinkthatsuchanasymmetricstructurewouldmakethemusicdifficulttofolloworsynchronizewith.Infact,listenerswhogrewupwiththismusicareadeptatfollowingthesecomplexmeters(Hannon&Trehub,2005),andmuchofthismusicisactuallydancemusic,inwhichfootfallsaresynchronizedtotheasymmetricbeats.

AsafinalexampleofarhythmictraditionwithadifferentorientationfromWesternEuropeanmusic,GhaniandrumminginWestAfricashowsanumberofinterestingfeatures.First,thebasicrhythmicreferenceisarepeating,non-isochronoustimepatternplayedonasetofhandbells(Locke1982;Pantaleoni,1985).Membersofadrum

Page 5: University Press Scholarship Online Oxford Scholarship ...

Rhythm

Page 5 of 79

PRINTED FROM OXFORD SCHOLARSHIP ONLINE (www.oxfordscholarship.com). (c) Copyright Oxford University Press, 2015.All Rights Reserved. Under the terms of the l icence agreement, an individual user may print out a PDF of a single chapter of amonograph in OSO for personal use (for details see http://www.oxfordscholarship.com/page/privacy-policy). Subscriber: UC - SantaCruz; date: 02 May 2015

ensemblekeeptheirrhythmicorientationbyhearingtheirpartsinrelationtothebell,ratherthanbyfocusingonanisochronousbeat.Furthermore,thefirstbeatofarhythmiccycleisnotheardasa“downbeat,”inotherwords,aspeciallystrongbeat(asinWesternmusic);ifanything,themostsalientbeatcomesattheendofthecycle(Temperley,2000).Finally,thismusicemphasizesdiversityintermsofthewayitcanbeheard.Asdifferentdrumsenter,eachwithitsowncharacteristicrepeatingtemporalpattern,apolyrhythmictextureiscreatedthatprovidesarichsourceofalternativeperceptualpossibilitiesdependingontherhythmiclayersandrelationshipsonechoosestoattendto(Locke1982;Pressing,2002).ThisisquitedifferentfromtherhythmicframeworkofmostWesternEuropeanmusic,inwhichtheemphasisisonrelativelysimpleandperceptuallyconsensualrhythmicstructures.OnepossiblereasonforthisdifferenceisthatWesternmusichasmajorpreoccupationsinothermusicaldimensions(suchasharmony),andarelativelysimplerhythmicframeworkfacilitatescomplexexplorationsintheseotherareas.AnotherreasonmaybethattempoinWesternEuropeanmusicisoftenflexible,withsalientdecelerationsandaccelerationsofthebeatusedforexpressivepurposes.Afairlysimplebeatstructuremayhelpalistenerstayorientedinthefaceofthesetemporalfluctuations(cf.Temperley,2004).

ThusitwouldbeanerrortoassumethattherhythmicstructureofWesternEuropeanmusicreflectsbasicconstraintsonhowthemindstructuresrhythmicpatternsintermsofproductionorperception.Aswitheverymusicaltradition,therhythmicpatternsofWesternEuropeanmusicreflectthehistoricalandmusicalconcernsofagivenculture.Ontheotherhand,acomparativeperspective(p.99) revealsthatcertainaspectsofrhythminWesternEuropeanmusic(suchasaregularbeatandgroupingofeventsintophrases)arealsofoundinnumerousothercultures,whichsuggeststhattheseaspectsreflectwidespreadcognitiveproclivitiesofthehumanmind.

ThediscussionbelowreliesattimesononeparticularmelodytoillustratevariousaspectsofrhythmicstructureinWesternmusic.Thisisthemelodyofachildren’ssong,indexedasmelodyK0016inadatabaseofBohemianfolkmelodies(Schaffrath,1995;Selfridge-Feld,1995).Figure3.1showsthemelodyinWesternmusicnotationandin“pianoroll”notationwitheachtone’spitchplottedasafunctionoftime(themelodycanbeheardinSoundExample3.1).

ThemelodywaschosenbecauseitishistoricallyrecentandfollowsfamiliarWesternconventions,yetisunlikelytobefamiliartomostreadersandisthusfreeofspecificmemoryassociations.Italsoillustratesbasicaspectsofrhythminasimpleform.Beyondthis,thereisnothingspecialaboutthismelody,andanynumberofothermelodieswouldhaveservedthesamepurpose.

3.2.1TheBeat:AStableMentalPeriodicity

Thephenomenonofamusicalbeatseemssimplebecauseitissofamiliar.Almosteveryonehastappedordancedalongtomusicwithabeat.Aregularbeatiswidespreadinmusicalcultures,anditisworthconsideringwhythismightbeso.Oneobviousfunctionofabeatistocoordinatesynchronizedmovement,suchasdance.(The

Page 6: University Press Scholarship Online Oxford Scholarship ...

Rhythm

Page 6 of 79

PRINTED FROM OXFORD SCHOLARSHIP ONLINE (www.oxfordscholarship.com). (c) Copyright Oxford University Press, 2015.All Rights Reserved. Under the terms of the l icence agreement, an individual user may print out a PDF of a single chapter of amonograph in OSO for personal use (for details see http://www.oxfordscholarship.com/page/privacy-policy). Subscriber: UC - SantaCruz; date: 02 May 2015

relationshipbetweendanceandmusiciswidespreadinhumansocieties;indeed,someculturesdonotevenhaveseparatetermsformusicanddance.)Asecondobviousfunctionofabeatistoprovideacommontemporalreferenceforensembleperformance.Indeed,across-culturalperspectiverevealsthatensemblemusicwithoutaperiodictemporalframeworkisarareexception.Perlman(1997)pointstoonesuchexceptioninJavanesemusicknownaspathetan,notingthat“Exceptforcertainisolatedphrases,pathetanhasnounifyingmetricframework.…Rhythmicunisonisnotdesired,andthemusiciansneednotmatchtheirattackswiththeprecisionmadepossiblebyadefinitemeter”(p.105).AdetaileddiscussionofpathetanbyBrinner(1995:245–267)suggeststhatitisanexceptionthatprovestherule:withoutametricframe,playerssubstitutecloseattentiontoaleadmelodicinstrument(typicallyarebaborbowedlute)inordertocoordinateandorienttheirperformance.Thuswhenperiodicityinensemblemusiciswithdrawn,itsfunctionalroleisfilledinotherways.

Figure3.1 Asimplemelody(K0016)in(A)musicnotationand(B)pianorollformat.In(B),they-axisshowsthesemitonedistanceofeachpitchfromC4(261.63Hz).

(p.100) Fromalistener’sperspective,perceptionofabeatisoftenlinkedtomovementintheformofsynchronizationtothebeat.Formanypeople,thissynchronizationisanaturalpartofmusicalexperiencerequiringnospecialeffort.Itmaycomeasasurprise,then,thathumansaretheonlyspeciestospontaneouslysynchronizetothebeatofmusic.Althoughsynchronyisknownfromotherpartsoftheanimalkingdom,suchasthechorusingoffrogsorthesynchronizedcallsofinsects(Gerhardt&Huber2002,Ch.8;Strogatz,2003),humansynchronizationwithabeatissingularinanumberofrespects(seeChapter7,section7.5.3,forfurtherdiscussionofthispoint).Ofcourse,beatperceptiondoesnotautomaticallycausemovement(onecanalwayssitstill),butthehumanuniquenessofbeatsynchronizationsuggeststhatbeatperceptionmeritspsychologicalinvestigation.Researchinmusiccognitionhasrevealedseveralinterestingfactsaboutbeatperception.

First,thereisapreferredtemporangeforbeatperception.Peoplehavedifficultyfollowingabeatthatisfasterthanevery200msandslowerthanevery1.2seconds.Withinthisrange,thereisapreferenceforbeatsthatoccurroughlyevery500–700ms(Parncutt,1994;vanNoorden&Moelants,1999).Itisinterestingtonotethatthisisthesamerangeinwhichpeoplearethemostaccurateatmakingdurationjudgments,inother

Page 7: University Press Scholarship Online Oxford Scholarship ...

Rhythm

Page 7 of 79

PRINTED FROM OXFORD SCHOLARSHIP ONLINE (www.oxfordscholarship.com). (c) Copyright Oxford University Press, 2015.All Rights Reserved. Under the terms of the l icence agreement, an individual user may print out a PDF of a single chapter of amonograph in OSO for personal use (for details see http://www.oxfordscholarship.com/page/privacy-policy). Subscriber: UC - SantaCruz; date: 02 May 2015

words,theyneitheroverestimatenorunderestimatethedurationoftemporalintervals(Eisler,1976;cf.Fraisse,1982).Furthermore,thisistherangeinwhichlistenersarethemostaccurateinjudgingslightdifferencesintempo(Drake&Botte,1993).Itisalsointerestingtonotethatinlanguageswithstressedandunstressedsyllables,theaveragedurationbetweenstressedsyllableshasbeenreportedtobeclosetoorwithinthisrange(Dauer,1983;Lea,1974,describedinLehiste,1977).

Second,althoughpeopleusuallygravitatetowardoneparticularbeattempo,theycantapatothertempithataresimpledivisorsormultiplesoftheirpreferredtappingrate(e.g.,atdoubleorhalftheirpreferredrate;Drake,Jones,&Baruch,2000).Forexample,considerSoundExample3.2,whichpresentsK0016along(p.101) withtwodifferentindicationsofthebeat.Bothareperfectlypossible,anditislikelythatmostpeoplecouldeasilytapateitherleveldependingonwhethertheyfocusonlowerorhigherlevelaspectsofrhythmicstructure.Drake,Jones,andBaruch(2000)haveshownthatpeoplevaryintheleveltheysynchronizewithinmusic,andthattheirpreferredlevelcorrelateswiththeirspontaneoustappingrate.Furthermore,althoughindividualsnaturallygravitatetooneparticularlevel,theycanmovetohigherorlowerlevelsiftheywish(e.g.,bydoublingorhalvingtheirtappingrate)andstillfeelsynchronizedwiththemusic.Thuswhenspeakingof“thebeat”ofapiece,itisimportanttokeepinmindthatwhatalistenerselectsasthebeatisjustonelevel(theirtactus)inahierarchyofbeats.

Third,beatperceptionisrobusttomoderatetempofluctuations.Inmanyformsofmusic,theoveralltimingofeventsslowsdownorspeedsupwithinphrasesorpassagesaspartofexpressiveperformance(Palmer,1997).Peoplearestillabletoperceiveabeatinsuchmusic(Large&Palmer,2002)andsynchronizetoit(Drake,Penel,&Bigand,2000),indicatingthatbeatperceptionisbasedonflexibletimekeepingmechanisms.

Fourth,thereisculturalvariabilityinbeatperception.DrakeandBenElHeni(2003)studiedhowFrenchversusTunisianlistenerstappedtothebeatofFrenchversusTunisianmusic.TheFrenchtappedataslowerratetoFrenchmusicthantoTunisianmusic,whereastheTunisiansshowedtheoppositepattern.DrakeandBenHeniarguethatthisreflectsthefactthatlistenerscanextractlarger-scalestructuralpropertiesinmusicwithwhichtheyarefamiliar.Thesefindingsindicatethatbeatperceptionisnotsimplyapassiveresponseoftheauditorysystemtophysicalperiodicityinsound:Italsoinvolvesculturalinfluencesthatmayrelatetoknowledgeofmusicalstructure(e.g.,sensitivitytohownotesaregroupedintomotives;cf.Toiviainen&Eerola,2003).

Fifth,andofsubstantialinterestfromacognitivesciencestandpoint,aperceivedbeatcantolerateagooddealofcounterevidenceintheformofaccentedeventsatnonbeatlocationsandabsentorweakeventsatbeatlocations,inotherwords,syncopation(Snyder&Krumhansl,2001).Forexample,considerSoundExamples3.3and3.4,twocomplextemporalpatternsstudiedbyPatel,Iversen,etal.(2005)withregardtobeatperceptionandsynchronization.Thepatternsbeginwithanisochronoussequenceof9tonesthatservestoindicatethebeat,whichhasaperiodis800ms.Afterthis“inductionsequence,”thepatternschangeintoamorecomplexrhythmbutwiththesamebeatperiod.Participantswereaskedtosynchronizetheirtapstotheisochronoustonesand

Page 8: University Press Scholarship Online Oxford Scholarship ...

Rhythm

Page 8 of 79

PRINTED FROM OXFORD SCHOLARSHIP ONLINE (www.oxfordscholarship.com). (c) Copyright Oxford University Press, 2015.All Rights Reserved. Under the terms of the l icence agreement, an individual user may print out a PDF of a single chapter of amonograph in OSO for personal use (for details see http://www.oxfordscholarship.com/page/privacy-policy). Subscriber: UC - SantaCruz; date: 02 May 2015

thencontinuetappingatthesametempoduringthecomplexsequence.Theirsuccessatthistaskwastakenasameasureofhowwelltheywereabletoextractabeatfromthesesequences.Inthe“stronglymetrical”(SM)sequences(SoundExample3.3),atoneoccurredateverybeatposition.Inthe“weaklymetrical”(WM)sequences,however,about1/3ofthebeatpositionsweresilent(SoundExample3.4).(NB:TheSMandWMsequenceshadexactlythesame(p.102) setofinteronsetintervals,justarrangeddifferentlyintime;cf.Povel&Essens,1985.)ThussuccessfulbeatperceptionandsynchronizationinWMsequencesrequiredfrequenttapsatpointswithnosound.

AllparticipantswereabletosynchronizewiththebeatoftheSMsequence:Theirtapswereverycloseintimetotheidealizedbeatlocations.(Infact,tapstypicallyprecededthebeatbyasmallamount,afindingtypicalofbeatsynchronizationstudies,indicatingthatbeatperceptionisanticipatoryratherthanreactive.)OfgreaterinterestwasperformanceontheWMsequences.AlthoughsynchronizationwasnotasaccurateaswiththeSMsequencesasmeasuredbytappingvariability,mostparticipants(eventhemusicallyuntrainedones)wereabletotaptothebeatofthesesequences,thoughfromaphysicalstandpointtherewaslittleperiodicityatthebeatperiod.Thatis,mostpeopletappedtothesilentbeatsasiftheywerephysicallythere,illustratingthatbeatperceptioncantolerateagooddealofcounterevidence.

Theabovefactsindicatethatbeatperceptionisacomplexphenomenonthatlikelyhassophisticatedcognitiveandneuralunderpinnings.Specifically,itinvolvesamentalmodeloftimeinwhichperiodictemporalexpectanciesplayakeyrole(Jones,1976).Thismaybeonereasonwhyitisuniquetohumans.

Beatperceptionisanactiveareaofresearchinmusiccognition,inwhichtherehaslongbeenaninterestinthecueslistenersusetoextractabeat.TemperleyandBartlette(2002)listsixfactorsthatmostresearchersagreeareimportantinbeatfinding(i.e.,ininferringthebeatfromapieceofmusic).Thesecanbeexpressedaspreferences:

1.Forbeatstocoincidewithnoteonsets2.Forbeatstocoincidewithlongernotes3.Forregularityofbeats4.Forbeatstoalignwiththebeginningofmusicalphrases5.Forbeatstoalignwithpointsofharmonicchange6.Forbeatstoalignwiththeonsetsofrepeatingmelodicpatterns

Becausebeatperceptionisfundamentaltomusicandisamenabletoempiricalstudy,ithasattractedcomputational,behavioral,andneuralapproaches(e.g.,Desain,1992;Desain&Honing,1999;Toddetal.,1999;Large,2000;Toiviainen&Snyder,2003;Hannonetal.,2004;Snyder&Large,2005;Zantoetal.,2006)andhasthepotentialtomatureintoasophisticatedbranchofmusiccognitioninwhichdifferentmodelscompetetoexplainacommonsetofbehavioralandneuraldata.Itsstudyisalsoattractivebecauseittouchesonlargerissuesincognitiveneuroscience.Forexample,synchronizationtoabeatprovidesanopportunitytostudyhowdifferentbrainsystemsarecoordinatedinperceptionandbehavior(inthiscase,theauditoryandmotorsystems).Abetter

Page 9: University Press Scholarship Online Oxford Scholarship ...

Rhythm

Page 9 of 79

PRINTED FROM OXFORD SCHOLARSHIP ONLINE (www.oxfordscholarship.com). (c) Copyright Oxford University Press, 2015.All Rights Reserved. Under the terms of the l icence agreement, an individual user may print out a PDF of a single chapter of amonograph in OSO for personal use (for details see http://www.oxfordscholarship.com/page/privacy-policy). Subscriber: UC - SantaCruz; date: 02 May 2015

understandingofthemechanismsinvolvedinbeatperceptionandsynchronizationcouldhaveapplicationsforphysicaltherapy,inwhichsynchronizationwithabeatisbeingusedtohelppatientswithneuromotordisorders(p.103) (suchasParkinson’sdisease)toinitiateandcoordinatemovement(Thautetal.,1999;cf.Sacks,1984,2007).

3.2.2Meter:MultiplePeriodicities

InWesternEuropeanmusic,beatsarenotallcreatedequal.Instead,somebeatsarestrongerthanothers,andthisservestocreateahigherlevelofperiodicityintermsofthegroupingand/oraccentuationofbeats.Forexample,thebeatsofawaltzaregroupedinthrees,withanaccentonthefirstbeatofeachgroup,whereasinamarchbeatsaregroupedintotwosorfours,withprimaryaccentonthefirstbeat(inafour-beatmarch,thereissecondaryaccentonthethirdbeat).

WaltzesandmarchesarebuttwotypesofmeterinabroaddiversityofmetersusedinWesternEuropeanmusic,buttheyservetoillustratesomegeneralfeaturesofmeterinthistradition.First,themetersofWesternmusicaredominatedbyorganizationintermsofmultiplesoftwoandthreeintermsofhowmanybeatsconstituteabasicunit(themeasure),andhowmanysubdivisionsofeachbeatthereare.Forexample,awaltzhasthreebeatspermeasure,eachofwhichcanbesubdividedintotwoshorterbeats,whereasamarchhastwo(orfour)beatspermeasure,eachofwhichcanalsobesubdividedintotwobeats.Manyotherpossibilitiesexist,forexampletwobeatspermeasure,eachofwhichissubdividedintothreebeats.1Thekeypointisthatmetertypicallyhasatleastonelevelofsubdivisionbelowthebeat(London,2002,2004:34),inadditiontoperiodicityabovethebeatcreatedbythetemporalpatterningofstrongbeats.Onewaytorepresentthisisviaametricalgrid,whichindicateslayersofperiodicityusingrowsofisochronousdots.Oneoftheserowsrepresentsthetactus,withtherowabovethisshowingtheperiodicpatternofaccentuationabovethetactus.Otherrowsaboveorbelowthetactusshowotherpsychologicallyaccessiblelevelsofperiodicity(Figure3.2showsametricalgridforK0016).

Thusoneshouldbeabletotaptoanyoftheselevelsandstillfeelsynchronizedwiththemusic.(Theuseofdotsinmetricalgridsindicatesthatmeterconcernstheperceptualorganizationofpointsintime,whichinphysicaltermswouldcorrespondtotheperceptualattacksoftones;Lerdahl&Jackendoff,1983.)Ingridnotation,therelativestrengthofeachbeatisindicatedbythenumberofdotsaboveit,inotherwords,thenumberoflayersofperiodicityitparticipatesin.Dotsatthehighestandlowestlevelmustfallwithinthe“temporalenvelope”formeter:Periodicitiesfasterthan200msandslowerthan∼4–6sareunlikelytobespontaneouslyperceivedaspartofametricframework.(Notethattheupperendofthisenvelopeissubstantiallylongerthanthe∼1.2secondlimitforfollowingabeatmentionedinsection3.2.1.Thatshorterlimitreferstobeat-to-beatintervals,whereas4–6sreferstothehighestmetricallevels,andislikelytoberelatedtooursenseofthepsychologicalpresent(cf.London,2002,2004:30).2

(p.104)

Page 10: University Press Scholarship Online Oxford Scholarship ...

Rhythm

Page 10 of 79

PRINTED FROM OXFORD SCHOLARSHIP ONLINE (www.oxfordscholarship.com). (c) Copyright Oxford University Press, 2015.All Rights Reserved. Under the terms of the l icence agreement, an individual user may print out a PDF of a single chapter of amonograph in OSO for personal use (for details see http://www.oxfordscholarship.com/page/privacy-policy). Subscriber: UC - SantaCruz; date: 02 May 2015

Figure3.2 MetricalstructureofK0016.Atypicaltactusisshownbythemetricallevellabeled1x.Phraseboundariesareindicatedbelowthepianorollnotation(p1=phrase1,etc.).

Beforemovingon,therelationshipbetweenaccentandmetershouldbediscussed.Thisisanimportantrelationship,becausestrongbeatsareperceptuallyaccentedpointsinthemusic.Thiskindofaccentdoesnotalwaysrelyon(p.105) physicalcuessuchasintensityorduration(note,forexample,thatalltonesinK0016areofequalintensity),andemergesfromthedetectionofperiodicityatmultipletime-scales.Thereareofcoursemanyphysical(or“phenomenal”)accentsinmusicduetoavarietyoffactors,includingduration,intensity,andchangesinmelodiccontour.Therearealso“structuralaccents”duetosalientstructuralpointsinthemusic,forexample,asuddenharmonicshiftorthestartofamusicalphrase(Lerdahl&Jackendoff,1983).Theinterplayofdifferentaccenttypesisoneofthesourcesofcomplexityinmusic(Jones,1993),particularlytheinterplayofmetricalaccentswithoff-beatphenomenalorstructuralaccents.Forexample,syncopationinmusicillustratesthesuccessfuluseofphenomenalaccents“againstthegrain”oftheprevailingmeter.Thisraisesakeypointaboutthemusicalmetricalgrid,namelythatitisamentalpatternofmultipleperiodicitiesinthemindofalistener,andnotsimplyamapoftheaccentualstructureofasequence.Thispointwillbecomerelevantinthediscussionofmetricalgridsinlanguage.

Theinfluenceofmusicalmeteronbehavior,perception,andbrainsignalshasbeendemonstratedinanumberofways.Sloboda(1983)hadpianistsperformthesamesequenceofnotessettodifferenttimesignatures(inmusic,thetimesignatureindicatesthegroupingandaccentuationpatternofbeats,i.e.,themeter).Thedurationalpatterningoftheperformancesdifferedsubstantiallydependingonthemeter,andinmanycasesagivenpianistdidnotevenrealizetheywereplayingthesamenotesequenceintwodifferentmeters.Ademonstrationofmeter’seffectonsynchronizationcomesfromPatel,Iversen,etal.(2005),whoshowedthattappingtoametricalpatterndiffersfromtappingtoasimplemetronomeatthesamebeatperiod.Specifically,tapstothefirstbeatofeachmetriccycle(i.e.,the“downbeats”inthestronglymetricalsequencesofSoundExample

Page 11: University Press Scholarship Online Oxford Scholarship ...

Rhythm

Page 11 of 79

PRINTED FROM OXFORD SCHOLARSHIP ONLINE (www.oxfordscholarship.com). (c) Copyright Oxford University Press, 2015.All Rights Reserved. Under the terms of the l icence agreement, an individual user may print out a PDF of a single chapter of amonograph in OSO for personal use (for details see http://www.oxfordscholarship.com/page/privacy-policy). Subscriber: UC - SantaCruz; date: 02 May 2015

3.3)wereclosertothephysicalbeatthantapsonotherbeats.Importantly,thesedownbeats(whichoccurredeveryfourbeats)wereidenticaltoothertonesintermsintensityandduration,sothattheinfluenceofdownbeatsontappingwasnotduetoanyphysicalaccentbuttotheirroleincreatingafour-beatperiodicstructureinthemindsoflisteners.

Intermsofmeter’sinfluenceonperception,PalmerandKrumhansl(1990)hadparticipantslistentoasequenceofisochronoustonesandimaginethateacheventformedthefirstbeatofgroupsoftwo,three,fourorsixbeats.Afterafewrepetitions,aprobetonewassoundedandparticipantshadtoindicatehowwellitfitwiththeimaginedmeter.Theratingsreflectedahierarchyofbeatstrength(cf.Jongsmaetal.,2004).Turningtoneuralstudies,Iversen,Repp,andPatel(2009)hadmusicallytrainedparticipantslistentoametricallyambiguousrepeatingtwo-notepatternandmentallyimposeadownbeatinaparticularplace.Specifically,inhalfofthesequencestheyimaginedthatthefirsttonewasthedownbeat,andintheotherhalftheyimaginedthatthesecondtonewasthedownbeat.Participantswereinstructednottomoveortoengageinmotorimagery.Measurementofbrainsignalsfromauditoryregionsusing(p.106)magnetoencephalography(MEG)revealedthatwhenanotewasinterpretedasthedownbeat,itevokedanincreasedamountofneuralactivityinaparticularfrequencyband(beta,20–30Hz)comparedtowhenitwasnotadownbeat(eventhoughthetoneswerephysicallyidenticalinthetwoconditions).3Acontrolexperimentshowedthatthepatternofincreasedactivitycloselyresembledthepatternobservedwhenthenoteinquestionwasinfactphysicallyaccented(Figure3.3).Theseresultssuggestthattheperceptionofmeterinvolvestheactiveshapingofincomingsignalsbyamentalperiodictemporal-accentualscheme.

3.2.3Grouping:ThePerceptualSegmentationofEvents

Groupingreferstotheperceptionofboundaries,withelementsbetweenboundariesclusteringtogethertoformatemporalunit.ThiscanbeillustratedwithK0016.Inlisteningtothismelody,thereisaclearsensethatitisdividedintophrases,schematicallymarkedinFigure3.4.

Theperceptualboundariesofthefirsttwophrasesaremarkedbysilences(musicalrests).Ofgreaterinterestaretheboundariesattheendofthethirdandthefourthphrases,whicharenotmarkedbyanyphysicaldiscontinuityinthetonesequence,butareneverthelesssalientperceptualbreakpoints.

AsemphasizedbyLerdahlandJackendoff(1983),groupingisdistinctfrommeter,andtheinteractionofthesetworhythmicdimensionsplaysanimportantroleinshapingtherhythmicfeelofmusic.Forexample,anacrusis,orupbeat,isarhythmicallysalientphenomenoninvolvingaslightmisalignmentbetweengroupingandmeter,inotherwords,aphrasestartingonaweakbeat(suchasphrase2ofK0016).

Psychologicalevidenceforperceptualgroupinginmusiccomesfromanumberofsources.Memoryexperimentsshowthatifalistenerisaskedtoindicatewhetherabrief

Page 12: University Press Scholarship Online Oxford Scholarship ...

Rhythm

Page 12 of 79

PRINTED FROM OXFORD SCHOLARSHIP ONLINE (www.oxfordscholarship.com). (c) Copyright Oxford University Press, 2015.All Rights Reserved. Under the terms of the l icence agreement, an individual user may print out a PDF of a single chapter of amonograph in OSO for personal use (for details see http://www.oxfordscholarship.com/page/privacy-policy). Subscriber: UC - SantaCruz; date: 02 May 2015

tonesequencewasembeddedinapreviouslyheardlongertonesequence,performanceisbetterwhentheexcerptendsatagroupboundaryintheoriginalsequencethanwhenitstraddlesagroupboundary(Dowling,1973;Peretz,1989).Thissuggeststhatgroupinginfluencesthementalchunkingofsoundsinmemory.Furtherevidenceforgroupingcomesfromstudiesthatshowhowgroupingwarpstheperceptionoftime.Forexample,clicksplacednearphraseboundariesinmusicalsequencesperceptuallymigratetothoseboundariesandareheardascoincidingwiththem(Sloboda&Gregory,1980;Stoffer,1985).MoreevidenceforperceptualwarpingbasedongroupingcomesfromastudybyRepp(1992a),inwhichparticipantsrepeatedlylistenedtoacomputer-generatedisochronousversionoftheopeningofaBeethovenminuet.Thetaskwastodetectalengtheningin1of47possiblepositionsinthemusic.Detectionperformancewasparticularlypooratphraseboundaries,probablyreflectinganexpectationforlengtheningatthesepoints(Reppfurthershowedthatthesewerethepointsatwhichhumanperformerstypicallysloweddowntomarkthephrasestructure).Finally,inagatingstudyofrecognitionforfamiliarmelodiesinwhichsuccessivelylongerfragmentsoftuneswerehearduntiltheywerecorrectlyidentified,Schulkindetal.(2003)foundthatidentificationperformancewashighestatphraseboundaries.4Thusthereisabundantevidencethatgroupingplaysaroleinmusicalperception.

(p.107)

Page 13: University Press Scholarship Online Oxford Scholarship ...

Rhythm

Page 13 of 79

PRINTED FROM OXFORD SCHOLARSHIP ONLINE (www.oxfordscholarship.com). (c) Copyright Oxford University Press, 2015.All Rights Reserved. Under the terms of the l icence agreement, an individual user may print out a PDF of a single chapter of amonograph in OSO for personal use (for details see http://www.oxfordscholarship.com/page/privacy-policy). Subscriber: UC - SantaCruz; date: 02 May 2015

Figure3.3 (A)Repeatingtwo-noterhythmicpattern,inwhichthelistenerimaginesthedownbeatoneitherthefirsttone(left)orsecondtone(right).(B)Evokedneuralresponses(measuredoverauditorybrainregions)tothetwo-tonepatternsubjectivelyinterpretedintwodifferentways,inotherwords,withthedownbeatontone1versustone2.(Theonsettimesoftones1and2areindicatedbythin,vertical,graylinesat0and0.2s).Thesolidanddashedblacklinesshowacross-subjectmeansforthetwoimaginedbeatconditions(solid=beatimaginedontone1,dashed=beatimaginedontone2).Dataarefromthebetafrequencyrange(20–30Hz).Thedifferenceisshownbythedottedline,withshadingindicating1standarderror.(C)Evokedneuralresponsesinthebetafrequencyrangetoatwo-tonepatternphysicallyaccentedintwodifferentways,withtheaccentontone1(solidline)versustone2(dashedline).

(p.108)

Figure3.4 K0016segmentedintomelodicphrases(p1=phrase1,etc.).

Page 14: University Press Scholarship Online Oxford Scholarship ...

Rhythm

Page 14 of 79

PRINTED FROM OXFORD SCHOLARSHIP ONLINE (www.oxfordscholarship.com). (c) Copyright Oxford University Press, 2015.All Rights Reserved. Under the terms of the l icence agreement, an individual user may print out a PDF of a single chapter of amonograph in OSO for personal use (for details see http://www.oxfordscholarship.com/page/privacy-policy). Subscriber: UC - SantaCruz; date: 02 May 2015

Whatcuesdolistenersuseininferringgroupingstructureinmusic?ReturningagaintoK0016,theendsofphrases3and4aremarkedbylocaldurationallengtheningandloweringofpitch.Itisnotablethatthesecueshavebeenfoundtobeimportantintheprosodicmarkingofclauseendingsinspeech(Cooper&Sorensen,1977).Eveninfantsshowsensitivitytotheseboundarycuesinbothspeechandmusic(Hirsh-Paseketal.,1987;Krumhansl&Jusczyk,1990;Jusczyk&Krumhansl,1993).Forexample,infantsprefertolistentomusicalsequencesinwhichpausesareinsertedafterlongerandlowersoundsratherthanatotherlocations,presumablybecauseintheformercasethepausescoincidewithperceptualboundaries.Ofcourse,thereismuchmoretogroupingthanjustthesetwocues.Deliège(1987)foundthatsalientchangesinintensity,duration,pitch,andtimbrecanallplayaroleindemarcatingtheedgesofgroups.Anotherfactorthatislikelytobeimportantismotivicrepetition,forexample,arepeatingpatternof(p.109)thesameoveralldurationandinternaldurationalpatterning.Whenthesedifferentcuesareinconflict,peoplecandisagreeaboutwheretheyheargroupingboundaries(Peretz,1989).Theinteractionofdifferentfactorsingroupingperceptionisatopicthatdrawscontinuinginterest,becausedataontheperceivedsegmentationofpiecesisrelativelyeasytocollect(Clarke&Krumhansl,1990;Deliègeetal.,1996;Frankland&Cohen,2004;Schaeferetal.,2004).

Groupingplaysaprominentroleinmoderncognitivetheoriesofmusic,inwhichitisconceivedofashierarchical,withlowerlevelgroupsnestedwithinhigherlevelones.Forexample,atheoreticalanalysisofgroupinginK0016wouldaddlayersaboveandbelowthephraselayer.Belowthephraselayer,eachphrasewouldbeparsedintosmallergroups(motives);abovethephraselayer,phraseswouldbelinkedintohigherlevelstructures.Forexample,onemightunitePhrases1and2intoagroup,followedbyagroupconsistingofPhrases3and4,andafinalgroupcoincidentwithPhrase5.OneofthemostdevelopedtheoreticaltreatmentsofhierarchicalgroupinginmusicisthatofLerdahlandJackendoff(1983),whoproposecertainbasicconstraintsongroupingstructuresuchastheconstraintthatapiecemustbefullyparsedintogroupsateachhierarchicallevel,andthatboundariesathigherlevelsmustcoincidewiththoseatlowerlevels.EvidenceformultiplelayersofgroupingstructureinmusiccomesfromresearchbyTodd(1985),whoshowedthattheamountoflengtheningatagivenphraseboundaryinmusicispredictedbythepositionofthatboundaryinahierarchicalphrasestructureofapiece.

Thehierarchicalviewofgroupingstructureinmusicshowsstrongparallelstotheoriesofprosodicstructureinmodernlinguistictheory,notablytheconceptofthe“prosodichierarchy”(Selkirk,1981,Nespor&Vogel,1983).Theprosodichierarchyreferstotheorganizationofsonicgroupingsatmultiplelevelsinspeech,rangingfromthesyllableuptotheutterance.Akeyconceptualpointmadebyallsuchtheoriesisthatthesegroupingsarenotsimplereflectionsofsyntacticorganization.Totakeawell-knownexample,considerthedifferencebetweenthesyntacticbracketingofasentencein3.1aversusitsprosodicphrasalbracketing3.1b(Chomsky&Halle,1968):

(3.1a)Thisis[thecat[thatcaught[therat[thatstole[thecheese]]]]]

Page 15: University Press Scholarship Online Oxford Scholarship ...

Rhythm

Page 15 of 79

PRINTED FROM OXFORD SCHOLARSHIP ONLINE (www.oxfordscholarship.com). (c) Copyright Oxford University Press, 2015.All Rights Reserved. Under the terms of the l icence agreement, an individual user may print out a PDF of a single chapter of amonograph in OSO for personal use (for details see http://www.oxfordscholarship.com/page/privacy-policy). Subscriber: UC - SantaCruz; date: 02 May 2015

(3.1b)[Thisisthecat][thatcaughttherat][thatstolethecheese]

Prosodicgroupingreflectsaseparatephonologicalleveloforganizationthatisnotdirectlydeterminedbysyntacticstructure.Instead,otherlinguisticfactorsplayanimportantrole,suchasthesemanticrelationsbetweenwordsandthedesiretoplacefocusoncertainelements(Marcus&Hindle,1990;Ferreira,1991).Furthermore,therearethoughttobepurelyrhythmicfactorssuchasatendencytoavoidgroupsthatareveryshortorverylong,andatendencytobalancethelengthsofgroups(Gee&Grosjean,1983;ZellnerKeller,2002).Theprosodicgroupingstructureofasentenceisbynomeansset(p.110) instone:Therearedifferencesamongindividualsintermsofhowtheygroupthewordsofthesamesentence,andthegroupingstructureofasentencecanvarywithspeechrate(Fougeron&Jun,1998).Nevertheless,groupingisnottotallyidiosyncratic,andpsycholinguistshavemadegoodprogressinpredictingwherespeakersplaceprosodicboundariesinasentencebasedonsyntacticanalysesofsentences(Watson&Gibson,2004).

AlthoughExample3.1aboveonlyshowsonelevelofprosodicphrasing,moderntheoriesoftheprosodichierarchypositmultiplelevelsnestedinsideoneanother.Theoriesvaryinthenumberoflevelstheypropose(Shattuck-Hufnagel&Turk,1996),5soforillustrativepurposesonlyonesuchtheoryisdiscussedhere.Hayes(1989)positsafive-levelhierarchycomprisedofwords,cliticgroups,phonologicalphrases,intonationalphrases,andutterances.Figure3.5showsaprosodichierarchyforasentenceaccordingtothistheory,withthesyntacticstructurealsoshownforcomparison.(Notethatacliticgroupcombinesalexicalwordthathasastressedsyllablewithanadjacentfunctionword—anunstressedsyllable—intoasingleprosodicunit.SeeHayes,1989,fordefnitionsofotherunits.)

Oneformofevidenceofferedfortheexistenceofagivenlevelintheprosodichierarchyisasystematicvariationintherealizationofaphonemicsegmentthatdependsonprosodicstructureatthatlevel.Forexample,Hayes(1989)discusses/v/deletioninEnglishspeechasanexampleofarulethatoperateswithinthecliticgroup.Thusitisacceptabletodeletethe/v/inAmericanEnglishwhensaying,“Willyou[saveme]aseat?”because“saveme”isacliticgroup.(Thatis,ifyoulistencarefullytoanAmericanEnglishspeakersaythisphraserapidly,“save”isoftenacousticallyrealizedas“say,”thoughitisintendedas—andheardas—“save”.)Incontrast,the/v/isnotdeletedwhensaying“[save][mom]”because[save]and[mom]aretwoseparatecliticgroups.Otherevidencethathasbeenadducedforprosodicconstituentsincludespreferencesforinterruptionpointsbetween,ratherthanwithin,constituents(Pilon,1981),andspeededwordspottingattheboundariesofconstituents(Kim,2003).

Evidencethattheprosodichierarchyhasmultiplelevelscomesfromphoneticmodificationsofspeechthatvaryinaparametricfashionwiththeheightoftheprosodicboundaryatthephoneme’slocation(thiscorrespondstothenumberofcoincidentprosodicboundariesatthatpoint,ashigherlevelboundariesarealwayscoincidentwithlowerlevelones).Forexample,ChoandKeating(2001)showedthatinKorean,thevoice-onsettimeofstopconsonantsislargerathigherprosodicboundaries,andDilley,

Page 16: University Press Scholarship Online Oxford Scholarship ...

Rhythm

Page 16 of 79

PRINTED FROM OXFORD SCHOLARSHIP ONLINE (www.oxfordscholarship.com). (c) Copyright Oxford University Press, 2015.All Rights Reserved. Under the terms of the l icence agreement, an individual user may print out a PDF of a single chapter of amonograph in OSO for personal use (for details see http://www.oxfordscholarship.com/page/privacy-policy). Subscriber: UC - SantaCruz; date: 02 May 2015

Shattuck-Hufnagel,andOstendorf(1996)haveshownthattheamountofglottalizationofword-onsetvowelsisgreaterathigherlevelboundaries.

(p.111)

Figure3.5 (A)Syntacticand(B)prosodichierarchyforasentenceofEnglish.Abbreviationsfor(A):S=sentence,PP=prepositionalphrase,NP=nounphrase,VP=verbphrase,Det=Determiner,A=adjective,N=Noun,V=Verb.Abbreviationsfor(B):U=utterance,I=Intonationphrase,P=phonologicalphrase,C=cliticgroup,W=word.AdaptedfromHayes,1989.

Anotherphenomenonthatsupportsthenotionofhierarchicalgroupinginspeechisvariationinperceivedjuncturebetweenwords(Jun,2003).Inconnectedspeech,wordsareacousticallyruntogetherandthesilentintervalsthatdooccur(e.g.,duetostopconsonants)arenotnecessarilylocatedatwordboundaries(cf.Chapter2,section2.3.3,subsection“ABriefIntroductiontotheSpectrogram”).Nevertheless,wordsareperceivedasseparatedfromoneanother.Unlikewithwrittenlanguage,however,theperceiveddegreeofspacingisnotidenticalbetweeneachpairofwords:Rather,somewordboundariesseemstrongerthanothers.Forexample,thesentencein3.1cbelowcontainsjuncturemarkingsfromasystemdevisedbyPriceetal.(1991).Inthissystem,aresearcherlistensrepeatedlytoagivensentenceandplacesnumeralsfrom0to6betweeneachpairofwordstoindicatethedegreeofperceivedseparationbetweenthem.A“breakindex”of0indicatestheweakestperceivedjuncture,inotherwords,betweenthewordsofacliticgroup.Attheoppositeextreme,abreakindexof6indicatestheendofasentence.

(3.1c)Only1one4remembered3the0lady1in1red6.

Wightmanetal.(1992)studiedtherelationshipbetweenthesebreakindicesandspeechdurationpatternsinalargespeechcorpus,andfoundacorrelationbetweenperceivedboundarystrengthandamountoflengtheningofthesyllableprecedingtheboundary(cf.Gussenhoven&Rietveld,1992).6Thisfindingis(p.112) strikinglyreminiscentoftheresearchonmusicbyTodd(1985)describedabove.Anotherparalleltomusicisthatdurationallengtheninginteractswithpitchandamplitudecuesindeterminingtheperceivedstrengthofprosodicboundaries(Streeter,1978;dePijper&Sanderman,1994).

Inconclusion,groupingisafundamentalrhythmicphenomenonthatappliestoboth

Page 17: University Press Scholarship Online Oxford Scholarship ...

Rhythm

Page 17 of 79

PRINTED FROM OXFORD SCHOLARSHIP ONLINE (www.oxfordscholarship.com). (c) Copyright Oxford University Press, 2015.All Rights Reserved. Under the terms of the l icence agreement, an individual user may print out a PDF of a single chapter of amonograph in OSO for personal use (for details see http://www.oxfordscholarship.com/page/privacy-policy). Subscriber: UC - SantaCruz; date: 02 May 2015

musicalandlinguisticsequences.Inbothdomains,themindparsescomplexacousticpatternsintomultiplelevelsofphrasalstructure,andmusicandlanguageshareanumberofacousticcuesformarkingphraseboundaries.Thesesimilaritiespointtosharedcognitiveprocessforgroupingacrossthetwodomains,andindicatethatgroupingmayproveafruitfulareaforcomparativeresearch.Asdiscussedinsection3.5,empiricalworkisprovingtheseintuitionscorrect.

3.2.4DurationalPatterninginMusic

Uptothispoint,thediscussionofmusicalrhythmhasbeenconcernedwithpointsandedgesintime:beatsandgroupingboundaries.Adifferentsetofissuesinrhythmresearchconcernshowtimegetsfilled,inotherwords,thedurationalpatterningofevents.

DurationCategoriesinMusicInmusic,thedurationalpatterningofeventsistypicallymeasuredbythetimeintervalsbetweeneventonsetswithinaparticulareventstream:Thisdefinesasequenceofinteronsetintervals(IOIs).Forexample,thesequenceofIOIsbetweenthetonesofamelodydefinesthedurationalpatterningofthatmelody.Typically,durationstendtobeclusteredaroundcertainvaluesreflectingtheorganizationoftimeinmusicintodiscretecategories.Fraisse(1982)pointedoutthattwocategoriesthatfigureprominentlyinWesternmusicalsequencesareshorttimesof200–300msandlongtimesof450–900ms(cf.Ross,1989).Hearguedthatthesetwodurationcategorieswerenotonlyquantitativelydifferentbutalsodifferentintermsoftheirperceptualproperties:Longintervalsareperceivedasindividualunitswithdistinctdurations,whereasshortintervalsareperceivedcollectivelyintermsoftheirgroupingpatternsratherthanintermsofindividualdurations.

Thereisempiricalevidencethatdurationsinmusicalrhythmsareperceivedintermsofcategories(Clarke,1987;Schulze,1989).Forexample,Clarke(1987)hadmusicstudentsperformacategoricalperceptionexperimentonrhythm.Participantsheardshortsequencesoftonesinwhichthelasttwotoneshadaratiothatvariedbetween1:1and1:2.Listenershadtoidentifythefinaldurationratioasoneortheotherofthese,andalsohadtocompleteataskthatrequiredthemtodiscriminatebetweendifferentratios.Theresultsshowedasteeptransitionintheidentificationfunction,andincreaseddiscriminationwhenstimuliwereneartheboundaryversuswithinagivenregion.(Clarkealso(p.113) foundthatthelocationoftheboundarydependedonthemetricalcontextinwhichthesequencewasperceived,providinganotherexampleoftheinfluenceofmeteronperception;cf.section3.2.2.)

Inspeech,thedurationofbasiclinguisticelements(suchasphonemesandsyllables)isinfluencedbyanumberoffactors.Forexample,therearearticulatoryconstraintsonhowfastdifferentsoundscanbeproduced,whichcreatesdifferentminimumdurationsfordifferentsounds(Klatt,1979).Therearealsosystematicphonologicalfactorsthatmakesomesoundslongerthanothers.Forexample,inEnglish,thesamevoweltendstobelongerifitoccursbeforeafinalstopconsonantthatisvoicedratherthanunvoiced

Page 18: University Press Scholarship Online Oxford Scholarship ...

Rhythm

Page 18 of 79

PRINTED FROM OXFORD SCHOLARSHIP ONLINE (www.oxfordscholarship.com). (c) Copyright Oxford University Press, 2015.All Rights Reserved. Under the terms of the l icence agreement, an individual user may print out a PDF of a single chapter of amonograph in OSO for personal use (for details see http://www.oxfordscholarship.com/page/privacy-policy). Subscriber: UC - SantaCruz; date: 02 May 2015

(e.g.,the/i/in“bead”vs.“beet”),andthisdifferenceinfluencestheperceptionofthefinalstopasvoicedorvoiceless(Klatt,1976).Asimplephonologicalfactorthatinfluencessyllabledurationisthenumberofphonemesinthesyllable:Syllableswithmorephonemestendtobelongerthanthosewithfewerphonemes(e.g.,“splash”vs.“sash”;Williams&Hiller,1994).Atopthesesourcesofvariationareothersourcesincludingvariationsinspeakingstyle(casualvs.clear),andvariationsinspeechraterelatedtodiscoursefactors,suchasspeedingupneartheendofasentenceto“holdthefloor”inaconversation(Schegloff,1982;Smiljanic&Bradlow,2005).Givenallthesefactors,itisnotsurprisingthatthedurationsofspeechelementsdonottendtoclusterarounddiscretevalues.Instead,measurementsofsyllableorphonemedurationtypicallyrevealacontinuousdistributionwithonemainpeak.Forexample,Figure3.6ashowsahistogramofsyllabledurationsforasampleofspokenEnglish.

Figure3.6a HistogramofsyllabledurationsinacorpusofspontaneousspeechinAmericanEnglish.Dataarefromapproximately16,000syllables.Meansyllableduration=191ms,sd=125ms.Syllableswithduration〉750msarenotshown(〈1%oftotal).Histogrambinsize=10ms.AnalysisbasedondatafromGreenberg,1996.

(p.114) Havingsaidthis,itisimportanttonotethatdurationalcategoriesdooccurinsomelanguages.Forexample,therearelanguageswithphonemiclengthcontrastsinwhichthesamewordcanmeanentirelydifferentthingswhenashortversuslongversionofthesamevowelorconsonantisused.Insomelanguages,suchasEstonian,therecanevenbethree-waylengthcontrasts.Forexample,“sata”canmeanthreeentirelydifferentthings(“hundred,”“send,”and“get”)dependingonthelengthofthefirst/a/.Itwouldbeinterestingtostudylengthcontrastsinagivenvowelphonemeandexaminetheamountoftemporalvariabilitywithineachdurationcategoryinconnectedspeech.Thiscouldbecomparedtotemporalvariabilityofagivendurationcategoryinmusic,toseewhethertheperceptualsystemhasasimilartoleranceforwithin-categoryvariabilityinthetwodomains.7

Page 19: University Press Scholarship Online Oxford Scholarship ...

Rhythm

Page 19 of 79

PRINTED FROM OXFORD SCHOLARSHIP ONLINE (www.oxfordscholarship.com). (c) Copyright Oxford University Press, 2015.All Rights Reserved. Under the terms of the l icence agreement, an individual user may print out a PDF of a single chapter of amonograph in OSO for personal use (for details see http://www.oxfordscholarship.com/page/privacy-policy). Subscriber: UC - SantaCruz; date: 02 May 2015

ExpressiveTiminginMusicIftheperceptualsystemcaredonlyaboutmusicaldurationsasasequenceofdiscretecategories,thencomputerrenditionsofmusicalpiecesbasedonexactrenderingsofmusicnotationwouldbeperfectlyacceptabletolisteners.Althoughsuchmechanicalperformancesdooccurinsomesettings(e.g.,rhythmtracksinsomemodernpopularmusic),inothercontexts,suchastheclassicalpianorepertoire,suchperformancesarerejectedasunmusical.Notsurprisinglythen,physicalmeasurementsofhumanperformancesrevealconsiderabledeviationsfromnotateddurations.Forexample,Figure3.6bshowsahistogramofIOIs,allofwhichrepresentrealizationsofnoteswiththesamenotatedduration(aneighthnoteorquaver)fromafamouspianist’srenditionofSchumann’sTräumerei(Repp,1992b).8Hadthepiecebeenperformedbyamachine,alloftheseIOIswouldbeasinglevalue.Instead,considerablevariationisseen.Thecrucialfactaboutthisvariationisthatitisnot“noise”:Itlargelyrepresentsstructuredvariationrelatedtotheperformer’sinterpretationofthepiece(Palmer,1997;Ashley,2002).Forexample,Repp(1992b)studiedseveralfamouspianists’renderingsofTräumereiandfoundthatallshowedslowingoftempoatstructuralboundaries,withtheamountofslowingproportionaltotheimportanceoftheboundary(cf.Todd,1985).Atafinertimescale,Reppfoundthatwithinindividualmelodicphrasestherewasatendencytoaccelerateatthebeginningandslowneartheend,withthepatternofIOIsfollowingasmoothparabolicfunction.Reppspeculatedthatthispatternmayreflectprinciplesofhumanlocomotion,inotherwords,amusicalallusiontophysicalmovement(cf.Kronman&Sundberg,1987).

(p.115)

Figure3.6b HistogramofdurationsofeighthnotesfromaperformanceofSchumann’sTräumereibyClaudioArrau.Thelargevaluesintherighttailofthehistogramareduetophrase-finalritards.Dataarefromapproximately170eighthnotes.Meannoteduration=652ms,sd=227ms.Noteswithduration〉1,600msarenotshown(〈1%oftotal).Histogrambinsize=50ms.

Page 20: University Press Scholarship Online Oxford Scholarship ...

Rhythm

Page 20 of 79

PRINTED FROM OXFORD SCHOLARSHIP ONLINE (www.oxfordscholarship.com). (c) Copyright Oxford University Press, 2015.All Rights Reserved. Under the terms of the l icence agreement, an individual user may print out a PDF of a single chapter of amonograph in OSO for personal use (for details see http://www.oxfordscholarship.com/page/privacy-policy). Subscriber: UC - SantaCruz; date: 02 May 2015

TheaboveparagraphfocusesontheroleofIOIsinexpressivetiming.IOIsarethebasisof“expressivetimingprofiles,”timeseriesthatshowtheactualpatternofeventtimingversustheidealizedpatternbasedonnotatedduration.Althoughstudiesoftheseprofileshavedominatedresearchonexpressivetiming,itisimportantnottooverlookanotheraspectofexpressivetiming,namely,articulation.AlthoughIOIreferstothetimeintervalbetweentheonsetsofsuccessivetones,articulationreferstothetimebetweentheoffsetofonetoneandtheonsetofthenext.Ifthereislittletimebetweentheseevents(orifthetonesoverlapsothattheoffsetofthepriortoneoccursaftertheonsetofthefollowingtone,whichispossibleinpianomusic),thisisconsidered“legato”articulation.Inthistypeofarticulation,onetoneisheardasflowingsmoothlyintothenext.Incontrast,staccatoarticulationinvolvesasalientgapbetweenoffsetandonset,givingthetonesarhythmicallypunctuatedfeel.InadditiontoIOIandarticulationpatterns,anotherimportantcuetomusicalexpressionisthepatterningoftoneintensity.

Duetothefactthattiming,articulationandintensityinmusiccanbemeasuredwithgreatprecisionusingmoderntechnology(e.g.,usingpianoswithdigitalinterfaces,suchastheYamahaDisklavier),expressionhasbeenafruitfulareaofresearchinstudiesofmusicproduction.Therehasalsobeensomeresearch(p.116) onexpressivefeaturesinperception.Forexample,listenerscanreliablyidentifyperformancesofthesamemusicasexpressive,deadpan(mechanical),orexaggerated(Kendall&Carterette,1990),andcanidentifytheperformer’sintendedemotiononthebasisofexpressivefeatures(Gabrielsson&Juslin,1996).

Palmer(1996)hasshownthatmusicallytrainedlistenerscanidentifyaperformer’sintendedmetricalandphrasestructureonthebasisofexpressivecues.OnecleverdemonstrationoftheperceptualimportanceofexpressivetimingwasprovidedbyClarke(1993),whousednaturallyperformedshortmelodies.Foreachmelody,Clarkeextracteditsexpressivetimingprofile,manipulatedit,andthenreimposeditonamechanicalperformanceofthemelody,thuscreatingaFrankensteinianmelodywithstructureandexpressionmismatched.Forexample,inoneconditiontheoriginalnote-by-noteexpressivetimingprofilewasshiftedseveralnotestotheright.Musiciansjudgedtheoriginalsversusthemismatchedmelodiesintermsofthequalityofperformance,andfavoredtheoriginals.Thuslistenersaresensitivetothewayexpressivetimingalignswiththestructureofmusicalpassages.

Expressivetiminginmusichasaninterestingrelationshiptoprosodicstructureinspeech.Justasamusicalpassageplayedbydifferentperformerswillhavedifferentexpressivetimingpatterns,thesamesentencespokenbydifferentspeakerswillhaveadifferenttemporalpatterningofsyllablesandphonemes.Inthepast,researchershavesuggestedthattheseindividualisticaspectsofperformanceare“normalizedaway”inmemoryformusicalandspokensequences,arguingthattheabstractmemoryrepresentationfavorsalessdetailed,morecategoricalstructure(Largeetal.,1995;Pisoni,1997).Morerecentresearch,however,suggeststhatlistenersretainsometemporalinformationinmemoryforspeechandmusic(Bradlowetal.,1999;Palmeretal.,2001).Forexample,Palmeretal.(2001)familiarizedlistenerswithparticularperformancesofshortmelodicsequences,

Page 21: University Press Scholarship Online Oxford Scholarship ...

Rhythm

Page 21 of 79

PRINTED FROM OXFORD SCHOLARSHIP ONLINE (www.oxfordscholarship.com). (c) Copyright Oxford University Press, 2015.All Rights Reserved. Under the terms of the l icence agreement, an individual user may print out a PDF of a single chapter of amonograph in OSO for personal use (for details see http://www.oxfordscholarship.com/page/privacy-policy). Subscriber: UC - SantaCruz; date: 02 May 2015

andthenlatertestedtheabilitytorecognizetheseperformancesagainstotherperformancesofthesamesequences.Thedifferentperformancesweregeneratedbyapianistwhoproducedthesameshortmelodicsequencesaspartoflongermelodiesthatdifferedintheirmetricalstructure(3/4vs.4/4time).Asaresultofthedifferingmetricalstructure,thesamemelodicsequencewasproducedwithdifferentpatternsofarticulationandintensity.Foreachsuchmelodicsequence,bothmusiciansandnonmusicianswereabletorecognizetheoriginalversiontheyhadheardwhenpresentedwithitversusanotherversion.Furthermore,even10-month-oldinfantsdiscriminatedbetweenfamiliarandunfamiliarperformances,orientinglongertowardtheformer.Palmeretal.relatethesefindingstoresearchinspeechperceptionshowingthatlistenersretainstimulus-specificacousticpropertiesofwordsalongwithabstractlinguisticproperties(Luce&Lyons,1998).

Anotherlineofresearchrelatingtiminginmusictospeechprosodyconcerns“tempopersistence.”Jungersetal.(2002)hadpianistsalternatebetweenlisteningtoshortmelodiesandsight-readingdifferentshortmelodies.The(p.117) participantsweretoldtoattendtoboththeheardandperformedmelodiesforalatermemorytest.Infact,therealquestionofinterestwastherelationshipbetweenthetempooftheheardandperformedmelodies.Theheardmelodiesoccurredinblocksofslowandfasttempi,andJungersetal.foundthatthetempoofperformedmelodieswasinfluencedbythetempoofheardmelodies:Thepianistsplayedmoreslowlyafterslowmelodiesandfasterafterfastmelodies.Asimilarexperimentusingspokensentencesratherthanmelodiesshowedasimilartempopersistenceeffectinspeech.Thesefindingsarereminiscentofresearchon“accommodation”insociolinguistics,whichhasshownthatwhenpeopleofdifferentsocialbackgroundsmeet,theirspeechbecomesmorealike(cf.Gilesetal.,1991).

Inaninterestingfollow-upstudy,DallaBellaetal.(2003)studiedtempopersistenceacrossmodalities.Listeners(bothmusiciansandnonmusicians)alternatedbetweenhearingmelodiesandreadingsentencesaloud.Themusiciansshowedatempopersistenceeffect:Theyspokefasterafterhearingfastermelodies.However,thenonmusiciansshowednosucheffect.Furthermore,whenthemusiciansdidthereverseexperiment(inwhichtheyalternatedbetweenhearingsentencesandsight-readingmelodies),therewasnoevidenceoftempopersistence.DallaBellaetal.suggestthatmusiciansmaybebetterthannonmusiciansatbeatextractioninmusic,andthatthismaydrivetheeffectoftempopersistenceseenintheirfirststudy.Followingthislogic,Iwouldsuggestthatthelackofaneffectintheirsecondstudyindicatesthatspeechperceptiondoesnotinvolveextractionofabeat.

3.2.5ThePsychologicalDimensionsofMusicalRhythm

Theinteractionsofbeat,meter,accent,grouping,andexpressivetimingmakemusicalrhythmapsychologicallyrichphenomenon(andthisisjustwithintheconfinesofWesternEuropeanmusic!).SomeideaofthisrichnessissuggestedbytheworkofGabrielsson,whohasconductedstudiesinwhichavarietyofrhythmsarecomparedandclassifiedbylistenersusingsimilarityjudgmentsandadjectiveratings(reviewedinGabrielsson,1993).Statisticaltechniquessuchasmultidimensionalscalingandfactoranalysisareused

Page 22: University Press Scholarship Online Oxford Scholarship ...

Rhythm

Page 22 of 79

PRINTED FROM OXFORD SCHOLARSHIP ONLINE (www.oxfordscholarship.com). (c) Copyright Oxford University Press, 2015.All Rights Reserved. Under the terms of the l icence agreement, an individual user may print out a PDF of a single chapter of amonograph in OSO for personal use (for details see http://www.oxfordscholarship.com/page/privacy-policy). Subscriber: UC - SantaCruz; date: 02 May 2015

touncovertheperceptualdimensionsinvolvedintheexperienceofmusicalrhythms.Thisresearchhasrevealedanastonishinglylargenumberofdimensions(15),whichgroupbroadlyintothoseconcernedwithstructure(e.g.,meter,simplicityvs.complexity),motion(e.g.,swinging,graceful),andemotion(e.g.,solemnityvs.playfulness).Althoughmuchofthecognitivescienceofmusicalrhythmfocusesonstructuralissues,itisimportanttokeepthelinkstomotionandemotioninmind,fortheseconnectionsarepartofwhatdistinguishesmusicalrhythmfromspeechrhythm,apointtowhichIwillreturnattheendofthediscussionofrhythminspeech.

(p.118) 3.3RhythminSpeechAlthoughthestudyofrhythminpoetryhasalonghistory,datingbacktoancientGreekandIndiantexts,thestudyofrhythminordinarylanguageisarelativelyrecentendeavorinlinguistics.Researchershavetakenatleastthreeapproachestothistopic.Thefirstapproachistypological,andseekstounderstandtherhythmicsimilaritiesanddifferencesamonghumanlanguages.Thedrivingforcebehindthisworkhasbeenideathatlinguisticrhythmsfallintodistinctcategories.Forexample,inonewidespreadtypologicalscheme(discussedinthenextsection),English,Arabic,andThaiareallmembersofasinglerhythmicclass(“stress-timedlanguages”),whereasFrench,Hindi,andYorubaaremembersofadifferentclass(“syllable-timedlanguages”).Asisevidentfromthisexample,membershipinarhythmicclassisnotdeterminedbythehistoricalrelationshipoflanguages;rhythmcanunitelanguagesthatareotherwisequitedistantbothhistoricallyandgeographically.

Thesecondapproachtospeechrhythmistheoretical,andseekstouncovertheprinciplesthatgoverntherhythmicshapeofwordsandutterancesinagivenlanguageorlanguages.Thisresearch,whichincludesanareacalled“metricalphonology,”seekstobringthestudyofthelinguisticrhythminlinewiththerestofmodernlinguisticsbyusingformalizedrulesandrepresentationstoderivetheobservedrhythmicpatterningofutterances.

Thethirdapproachisperceptual,andexaminestherolethatrhythmplaysintheperceptionofordinaryspeech.Oneprominentlineofresearchinthisareaconcernstheperceptualsegmentationofwordsfromconnectedspeech.Another,smallerlineofresearchexaminestheeffectsofrhythmicpredictabilityinspeechperception.

Thegoalofthispartofthechapteristointroduceeachoftheseareasandmakecomparisonstomusicalrhythmwhenappropriate.Beforecommencing,itisworthintroducingaconceptthatoccursineachsection:thenotionofprominenceinspeech.Inmanylanguages,itisnormaltoproducethesyllablesofanutterancewithdifferingdegreesofprominence.Thisistrueevenwhenasentenceissaidwithnospecialemphasisonanyparticularword.Forexample,whenspeakingthefollowingsentence,notehowthesyllablesmarkedbyanxaremoreprominentthantheirneighbors:

Page 23: University Press Scholarship Online Oxford Scholarship ...

Rhythm

Page 23 of 79

PRINTED FROM OXFORD SCHOLARSHIP ONLINE (www.oxfordscholarship.com). (c) Copyright Oxford University Press, 2015.All Rights Reserved. Under the terms of the l icence agreement, an individual user may print out a PDF of a single chapter of amonograph in OSO for personal use (for details see http://www.oxfordscholarship.com/page/privacy-policy). Subscriber: UC - SantaCruz; date: 02 May 2015

Themostimportantphysicalcorrelatesofprominenceareduration,pitchmovement,vowelquality,andloudness.9Prominenceinspeechraisesmany(p.119) interestingquestions.Howmanydifferentdegreesofprominencecanlistenersreliablydistinguish(Shattuck-Hufnagel&Turk,1996)?Dolanguagesdifferintheextenttowhichtheyrelyonparticularacousticcuestoprominenceinproductionandperception(Berinstein,1979;Lehiste&Fox,1992)?Empiricaldataontheseissuesisstillrelativelysparse,andwewillnotdelveintothemhere.Instead,mostsectionsbelowtreatprominenceasabinaryquantityreferredtoas“stress,”followingthetraditionofmuchworkonspeechrhythm.Anexceptionoccursinsection3.3.2,wheredegreesofprominencearediscussedinthecontextofmodernlinguistictheoriesofspeechrhythm.

Beforeembarkingonthefollowingsections,awordshouldbesaidabouttheconceptofstressinlinguistics.Stressisrecognizedasoneaspectofwordprosodyinhumanlanguages;toneandlexicalpitchaccentaretwootheraspects.Justasnotalllanguageshavelexicaltone(cf.Chapter2foradiscussionoftonelanguages)orlexicalpitchaccent,10notalllanguageshavelexicalstress,inotherwords,asystematicmarkingofcertainsyllableswithinawordasmoreprominentthanothers.Importantly,thesethreeaspectsofwordprosodyarenotmutuallyexclusive.Forexample,therearetonelanguageswithstress(e.g.,Mandarin)andwithoutit(e.g.,Cantonese),andpitchaccentlanguageswithorwithoutstress(e.g.,SwedishandJapanese,respectively;Jun,2005).Thusinthediscussionbelow,itshouldbekeptinmindthatstressisawidespreadbutnotuniversalfeatureofhumanlanguage.

3.3.1RhythmicTypology

Fourapproachestorhythmictypologyaredescribedbelow.Behindalloftheseapproachesisacommondesiretounderstandtherelationshipsoftheworld’slinguisticrhythms.

PeriodicityandTypologyThemostinfluentialtypologyoflanguagerhythmtodateisbasedonthenotionofperiodicityinspeech.ThistypologyhasitsrootsintheworkofKennethPike(1945),whoproposedatheoryofspeechrhythmbasedonadichotomybetweenlanguagesintermsofsyllableandstresspatterns.Hedubbedcertainlanguages(suchasSpanish)“syllable-timed,”basedontheideathatsyllables(p.120) markoffroughlyequaltemporalintervals.Thesestoodincontrastto“stress-timed”languagessuchasEnglish,whichwerecharacterizedbyroughlyequaltemporalintervalsbetweenstresses.Toillustratestress-timedrhythm,Pikeinvitedthereaderto“noticethemoreorlessequallapsesoftimebetweenthestressesinthesentence”:

Pikethenaskedthereadertocomparethetimingofstressesintheabovesentencewiththefollowingone,andnoticethesimilarity“despitethedifferentnumberofsyllables”(p.34):

Page 24: University Press Scholarship Online Oxford Scholarship ...

Rhythm

Page 24 of 79

PRINTED FROM OXFORD SCHOLARSHIP ONLINE (www.oxfordscholarship.com). (c) Copyright Oxford University Press, 2015.All Rights Reserved. Under the terms of the l icence agreement, an individual user may print out a PDF of a single chapter of amonograph in OSO for personal use (for details see http://www.oxfordscholarship.com/page/privacy-policy). Subscriber: UC - SantaCruz; date: 02 May 2015

Pikearguedthatinstress-timedlanguagestheintervalsbetweenstressedsyllables(referredtoas“feet”)wereapproximatelyequaldespitechangingnumbersofsyllablesperfoot.Toachieveevenlytimedfeet,speakerswouldstretchorcompresssyllablestofitintothetypicalfootduration.Pikebelievedthatlearningtherhythmofalanguagewasessentialtocorrectpronunciation.Henoted,forexample,thatSpanishspeakerslearningEnglish“mustabandontheirsharp-cutsyllable-by-syllablepronunciationandjamtogether—orlengthenwherenecessary—Englishvowelsandconsonantssoastoobtainrhythmunitsofthestress-timingtype”(p.35).

Abercrombie(1967:34–36,96–98)wentfurtherthanPikeandproposedaphysiologicalbasisforstressversussyllabletiming.Thisboldstepwasbasedonaspecifichypothesisforhowsyllablesareproduced.Abercrombiebelievedthateachsyllablewasassociatedwithacontractionofmusclesassociatedwithexhalation(theintercostalmusclesoftheribcage),andthatsomecontractionswereespeciallystrong:Theselattercontractionsproducedstressedsyllables.Hereferredtothesetwotypesofcontractionsas“chestpulses”and“stresspulses”(thusonlysomechestpulseswerestresspulses;cf.Stetson1951).Abercrombieproposedthatinanygivenlanguage,oneortheotherkindofpulseoccurredrhythmically.Hethenequatedrhythmwithperiodicity:“Rhythm,inspeechasinotherhumanactivities,arisesoutoftheperiodicrecurrenceofsomesortofmovement…”(p.96).Furthermore,heclaimedthat“asfarasisknown,everylanguageintheworldisspokenwithonekindofrhythmorwiththeother”(p.97),namingEnglish,Russian,andArabicasexamplesofstress-timedlanguages,andFrench,Telugu,andYorubaasexamplesofsyllable-timedlanguages.JustasPikehaddone,henotedthatalanguagecouldnotbebothstress-timedandsyllable-timed.Becausetherearevariablenumbersofsyllablesbetweenstresses,equalizingthedurationofinterstressintervalsmeantthat“therateofsyllablesuccessionhastobecontinuallyadjusted,inordertofitvaryingnumbersofsyllablesintothesametimeinterval.”

(p.121) ItishardtooverestimatetheimpactofPikeandAbercrombieonthestudyofrhythminspeech.Theterms“stress-timed”and“syllable-timed”havebecomepartofthestandardvocabularyoflinguistics.Athirdcategory,“moratiming,”isalsoinstandarduse,andisusedtodescribetherhythmofJapanesespeech.Themoraisaunitthatissmallerthanthesyllable,usuallyconsistingofaconsonantandvowel,butsometimescontainingonlyasingleconsonantorvowel.Ladefoged(1975:224)statedthat“eachmoratakesaboutthesamelengthoftimetosay,”thusarguingfortheroughisochronyofmorae.11SincethepublicationofAbercrombie’sbook,manylanguageshavebeenclassifiedintooneofthesetwocategories(Dauer,1983;Grabe&Low,2002),andmanyresearchstudieshaveexaminedtheissueofisochronyinspeech.Inthissense,thestressversussyllable-timedtheoryofspeechrhythmhasbeenveryfruitful.Itprovidedaclear,empiricallytestablehypothesistogetherwithaphysiologicaljustification.

Inanothersense,however,thetheoryhasbeenanutterfailure.Empirical

Page 25: University Press Scholarship Online Oxford Scholarship ...

Rhythm

Page 25 of 79

PRINTED FROM OXFORD SCHOLARSHIP ONLINE (www.oxfordscholarship.com). (c) Copyright Oxford University Press, 2015.All Rights Reserved. Under the terms of the l icence agreement, an individual user may print out a PDF of a single chapter of amonograph in OSO for personal use (for details see http://www.oxfordscholarship.com/page/privacy-policy). Subscriber: UC - SantaCruz; date: 02 May 2015

measurementsofspeechhavefailedtoprovideanysupportfortheisochronyofsyllablesorstresses(seereferencesinBertinetto,1989).12Totakejustafewexamplesfromthemanypapersthathavetestedtheisochronyhypothesis,Dauer(1983)showedthatEnglishstressfeetgrowindurationwithincreasingnumberofsyllables,ratherthanmaintainingtheevendurationnecessaryforisochrony(cf.Levelt,1989:393).Roach(1982)comparedEnglish,Russian,andArabictoFrench,Telugu,andYorubaanddemonstratedthattheformerstress-timedlanguagescouldnotbediscriminatedfromthelattersyllable-timedonesonthebasisofthetimingofinterstressintervals.Finally,Beckman(1982)andHoequist(1983)showedthatmoraearenotofequaldurationinJapanese.

Giventhatthenotionofperiodicityinordinaryspeechwasempiricallyfalsifiedover20yearsago,whydothelabelsofstress-timing,syllable-timing,andmoratimingpersist?Onereasonmaybethatitmatchessubjectiveintuitionsaboutrhythm.Forexample,Abercrombiehimself(1967:171)notedthattheideaofisochronousstressinEnglishdatesbacktothe18thcentury.AnotherreasonissuggestedbyBeckman(1992),whoarguesthatthistripartiteschemepersistsbecauseitcorrectlygroupstogetherlanguagesthatareperceivedasrhythmicallysimilar,evenifthephysicalbasisforthisgroupingisnotclearlyunderstood(andisnotisochronyofanykind).

(p.122) Thekeypointofthecurrentsection,then,isthatperiodicity,whichplayssuchanimportantroleinmuchmusicalrhythm,isnotpartoftherhythmofordinaryspeech.Thenextsectionexploresadifferentapproachtospeechrhythm,onethatsetsasidenotionsofisochrony.

PhonologyandTypologyThefactthatspeechisnotisochronousshouldnotleadustodiscardtheideaofspeechrhythm.Thatis,researchcanmoveforwardifonethinksofrhythmassystematictiming,accentuation,andgroupingpatternsinalanguagethatmayhavenothingtodowithisochrony.Oneproductiveapproachinthisframeworkisthephonologicalapproachtorhythmictypology.Thefundamentalideaofthisapproachisthattherhythmofalanguageistheproductofitslinguisticstructure,notanorganizationalprinciplesuchasstressorsyllableisochrony(Dauer,1983;cf.Dasher&Bolinger,1982).Inthisview,languagesarerhythmicallydifferentbecausetheydifferinphonologicalpropertiesthatinfluencehowtheyareorganizedaspatternsintime.OneclearexpositionofthisideaisthatofDauer(1983,1987),whopositedseveralfactorsthatinfluencespeechrhythm.

Thefirstfactoristhediversityofsyllablestructuresinalanguage.13Languagesvarysubstantiallyintheirinventoryofsyllabletypes.Forexample,Englishhassyllablesrangingfromasinglephoneme(e.g.,theword“a”)uptosevenphonemes(asin“strengths”),andallowsuptothreeconsonantsinonsetandcoda.Insharpcontrast,languagessuchasJapanese(andmanyPolynesianlanguages)allowfewsyllabletypesandaredominatedbysimpleCVsyllables.RomancelanguagessuchasSpanishandFrenchhavemoresyllabletypesthanJapaneseorHawaiianbutavoidthecomplexsyllablesfoundinlanguagessuchasEnglishandDutch,andinfactshowactiveprocessesthatbreakuporpreventthecreationofsyllableswithmanysegments(Dauer,1987).

Page 26: University Press Scholarship Online Oxford Scholarship ...

Rhythm

Page 26 of 79

PRINTED FROM OXFORD SCHOLARSHIP ONLINE (www.oxfordscholarship.com). (c) Copyright Oxford University Press, 2015.All Rights Reserved. Under the terms of the l icence agreement, an individual user may print out a PDF of a single chapter of amonograph in OSO for personal use (for details see http://www.oxfordscholarship.com/page/privacy-policy). Subscriber: UC - SantaCruz; date: 02 May 2015

Thediversityofsyllablesavailabletoalanguageinfluencesthediversityofsyllabletypesinspokensentences.Forexample,Dauer(1983)foundthatinasampleofcolloquialFrench,overhalfthesyllabletokenshadasimpleCVstructure,whereasinasimilarEnglishsample,CVsyllablesaccountedforonlyaboutone-thirdofthesyllabletokens.Thesedifferencesarerelevanttorhythmbecausesyllabledurationiscorrelatedwiththenumberofphonemespersyllable,suggestingthatsentencesofEnglishshouldhavemorevariablesyllabledurations(onaverage)thanFrenchsentences.

(p.123) Thesecondfactoraffectingspeechrhythmisvowelreduction.Insomelanguages,suchasEnglish,unstressedsyllablesoftenhavevowelsthatareacousticallycentralizedandshortinduration(linguistscommonlyrefertothissoundas“schwa,”aneutralvowelsoundinglike“uh”).Incontrast,inotherlanguages(suchasSpanish)thevowelsofunstressedsyllablesarerarelyifeverreduced,contributingtoalessvariablepatternofvoweldurationbetweenstressedandunstressedsyllables.

ThethirdrhythmicfactorproposedbyDaueristheinfluenceofstressonvowelduration.Insomelanguages,stresshasastrongeffectonthedurationofavowelinasyllable.Forexample,onerecentmeasurementofspokenEnglishfindsthatvowelsinstressedsyllablesareabout60%longerthanthesamevowelsinunstressedsyllables(Greenberg,2006).Incontrast,studiesofSpanishsuggestthatstressdoesnotconditionvoweldurationtothesamedegree(Delattre,1966).

Dauersuggestedthatlanguagestraditionallyclassifiedasstress-timedversussyllable-timeddifferintheabovephonologicalfeatures,withstress-timedlanguagesusingabroaderrangeofsyllabletypes,havingasystemofreducedvowels,andexhibitingastronginfluenceofstressonvowelduration.Thisnicelyillustratestheperspectiveofspeechrhythmasaproductofphonology,ratherthanacausalprinciple(e.g.,involvingperiodicity).14

Dauer’sproposalleadstotestablepredictions.Specifically,thethreefactorssheoutlines(diversityinsyllablestructure,vowelreduction,andtheinfluenceofstressonvowelduration)shouldallcontributetoagreaterdegreeofdurationalvariabilityamongthesyllablesofstress-timedversussyllable-timedutterances.Surprisingly,thereislittlepublisheddataondurationalvariabilityofsyllablesinsentencesofstressversussyllable-timedlanguages.Onereasonforthismaybethatthedemarcationofsyllableboundariesinspeechisnotalwaysstraightforward.Althoughpeoplegenerallyagreeonhowmanysyllablesawordorutterancehas,therecanbedisagreementaboutwheretheboundariesbetweensyllablesare,evenamonglinguists.Forexampledoesthefirst“l”intheword“syllable”belongtotheendofthefirstsyllableortothebeginningofthesecondsyllable,orisit“ambisyllabic,”belongingtobothsyllables?Althoughitistruethatsyllablemeasurementsaresubjecttodecisionsthatmayvaryfromoneresearchertothenext,thisshouldnotimpedeempiricalresearch:Itsimplymeansthatmeasurementsshouldbeaccompaniedbyanindicationofwhereeachsyllableboundarywasplaced.Ireturntothispointbelow.

Page 27: University Press Scholarship Online Oxford Scholarship ...

Rhythm

Page 27 of 79

PRINTED FROM OXFORD SCHOLARSHIP ONLINE (www.oxfordscholarship.com). (c) Copyright Oxford University Press, 2015.All Rights Reserved. Under the terms of the l icence agreement, an individual user may print out a PDF of a single chapter of amonograph in OSO for personal use (for details see http://www.oxfordscholarship.com/page/privacy-policy). Subscriber: UC - SantaCruz; date: 02 May 2015

(p.124) Beforeturningtoanotherphonologicalapproachtospeechrhythm,itisworthnotingthatthephonologicalpropertieslistedbyDauerdonotalwaysco-occur.ThusDauerarguedagainsttheideaofdiscreterhythmicclassesandforthenotionofarhythmiccontinuum.Insupportofthisidea,Nespor(1990)hasnotedthatPolishhascomplexsyllablestructurebutnovowelreduction(atnormalspeechrates),andCatalanhassimplesyllablestructurebutdoeshavevowelreduction.Thusthereiscurrentlyadebateinthefieldofspeechrhythmastowhetherlanguagesreallydofallintodiscreterhythmclassesorwhetherthereisacontinuumbasedonthepatternofco-occurrenceofrhythmicallyrelevantphonologicalfactors(cf.Arvaniti,1994;Grabe&Low,2002).Onlyfurtherresearchcanresolvethisissue,particularlyperceptualresearch(asdiscussedbelowinsection3.3.1,subsection“PerceptionandTypology”).

Inowturnbrieflytoadifferentphonologicaltheoryofspeechrhythm,proposedbyDwightBolinger(1981).AlthoughBolingerfocusedonEnglish,hisideasarequiterelevanttotypologicalissues.ThefoundationofBolinger’stheoryisthenotionthattherearetwodistinctsetsofvowelsinEnglish:fullvowelsandreducedvowels.By“reduced”vowelsBolingerdoesnotsimplymeanvowelsinunstressedsyllablesthatareshortandacousticallycentralized(i.e.,aphoneticdefinition).HearguesforaphonologicalclassofreducedvowelsinEnglish,whichbehavedifferentlyfromothervowels.Bolingerplacesthreevowelsinthisclass,an“ih”-likevowel,and“uh”-likevowel,anda“oh”-likevowel(moresimilarto“uh”thantothefullvowel“o”).Phoneticallyallofthesevowelsoccurinthecentralregionofvowelspace,neartheschwavowel/ə/ofEnglish(seeFigure2.19:Bolinger’s“ih”and“oh”vowelarenotshowninthatfigure,buttheformerwouldoccurjusttotheleftandupfrom/ə/,andthelatterwouldoccurjusttotherightandupfrom/ə/).Bolinger(1981:3–9)presentsargumentstosupportthenotionthatthesevowelsareaphonologicallydistinctsubclass,inotherwords,thattheybehaveincertainwaysthatfullvowelsdonot.Spacelimitationspreventadetaileddiscussionofthesearguments.HereIwillfocusontwoclaimsBolingermakesaboutfullandreducedvowelsthatarerelevantforspeechrhythm.

First,heclaimsthatsyllablescontainingfullandreducedvowelstendtoalternateinEnglishsentences.Second,heclaimsthatthereisa“lengtheningrule”suchthat“whenalongsyllableisfollowedbyashortone,theshortoneborrowstimefromitandmakesitrelativelyshort”(p.18).(Bya“long”syllable,hemeansasyllablewithafullvowel,andbya“short”syllable,hemeansasyllablewithareducedvowel;thereisnoclaimforaparticulardurationratiobetweenthetwotypesofsyllables.)Toillustratethisrule,Bolingeroffersthefollowingexample(notethatthefirstsentenceisfromanadforaspecialtypeofsoap):

(p.125)

Page 28: University Press Scholarship Online Oxford Scholarship ...

Rhythm

Page 28 of 79

PRINTED FROM OXFORD SCHOLARSHIP ONLINE (www.oxfordscholarship.com). (c) Copyright Oxford University Press, 2015.All Rights Reserved. Under the terms of the l icence agreement, an individual user may print out a PDF of a single chapter of amonograph in OSO for personal use (for details see http://www.oxfordscholarship.com/page/privacy-policy). Subscriber: UC - SantaCruz; date: 02 May 2015

Intheexampleabove,IhaveindicatedtheshortenedL’softhesecondsentencebyL-(afterFaber,1986).ThepointofthisexampleisthateachL-ofsentence3.6isshorterthantheL’sofsentence3.5,andthisoccurs(accordingtoBolinger)becauseeachS“borrowstime”fromtheprecedingL.Notethatsentence3.6hasstrictalternationbetweenLandS.Thisisaspecialcase:Bolingermakesnoclaimsforstrictalternation,onlyaclaimforatendency(thussequencessuchasLLSSSLSLL…areperfectlypossible).IsuspectBolingerchosethesentencesin3.5and3.6asexamplesbecausehefeltthateach(L-S)pairinsentence3.6isnotterriblydifferentindurationfromeachLinsentence3.5:ThisissuggestedbyhisgraphicalplacementoftheL’sinthetwosentencesaboveoneanother,inhisoriginaltext.However,thedurationalequivalenceofLand(L-S)isnotpartofBolinger’sclaim.Thisisanimportantpoint.Bolinger’stheorymayberelevanttothesubjectiveimpressionofiochrony(becauseoftheroughalternationofLandSandthelengtheningrule),butithasnoisochronyprinciple.

Faber(1986)arguesthatBolinger’stheoryissuperiortostress-timingtheorywhenitcomestoteachingtherhythmofEnglishtoforeignstudents(cf.Chela-Flores,1994).HealsopointsoutthatBolinger’stheorycanbeusedtoexplaincharacteristictimingpatternsthatstress-timingtheorycannotaccountfor,suchaswhy“cart”isshorterin:

thanin:

Orwhy“man”isshorterin:

thanin:

Bolinger’stheoryofspeechrhythmisdistinctfromthetheoryoutlinedbyDauerinthatitdealsnotjustwiththevariabilitysyllabledurationbutwiththepatterningofduration.Specifically,BolingerarguesthatthecharacteristicrhythmofEnglishisduetotheroughalternationofsyllableswithfullandreducedvowels,andtothewayfullvowelschangedurationwheninterveningreduced(p.126) syllablesareadded.Thisisalreadyenoughtosuggestabasisfortypologicaldistinctionsbetweenlanguages.Forexample,onemighttesttheideathatstress-timedlanguageshavemorecontrastinadjacentvoweldurationsthandosyllabletimedlanguages,andthatstress-timedlanguageshavelengtheningrules

Page 29: University Press Scholarship Online Oxford Scholarship ...

Rhythm

Page 29 of 79

PRINTED FROM OXFORD SCHOLARSHIP ONLINE (www.oxfordscholarship.com). (c) Copyright Oxford University Press, 2015.All Rights Reserved. Under the terms of the l icence agreement, an individual user may print out a PDF of a single chapter of amonograph in OSO for personal use (for details see http://www.oxfordscholarship.com/page/privacy-policy). Subscriber: UC - SantaCruz; date: 02 May 2015

ofthetypesuggestedbyBolingerforEnglish(Bolingerhimselfdoesnotsuggesttheseideas,buttheyareanobviouscorollaryofhiswork).IfBolingerhadstoppedhere,hewouldalreadyhavemadeavaluablecontributiontospeechrhythmresearch.Bolinger’stheoryhasonefurthercomponent,however,thatrepresentsafundamentaldivergencefromthetheoryoutlinedbyDauer.

OnceagainfocusingonEnglish,Bolingersuggestedthatjustastherearetwokindsofvowels(fullandreduced),therearealsotwokindsofrhythm.Thefirstistherhythmicpatterningalreadydescribed,inotherwords,theroughalternationoflongandshortsyllablesandthelengtheningrule.Abovethislevel,however,isasecondlevelofrhythmicpatterningconcernedwithtemporalrelationsbetweenaccentscuedbypitch.Notethatthisideaentailsthenotionthatsyllabicrhythmisfundamentallyaboutdurationanddoesnotrelyonpitchasacue.Inotherwords,“thereisabasicleveloftemporalpatterningthatisindependentoftonalpatterning”(Bolinger1981:24,citingBruceBruce,1981).Bolingerarguesthatthistemporalpatterningwouldbeobservableeveninspeechspokenonamonotone.Speechisnotspokenonamonotone,however,andBolingerarguesthatsyllablesaccentedbypitchformasecondlevelofrhythmicpatterninginwhichthefundamentalruleisatendencytoseparatepitchaccentssothattheydonotoccurtoocloselytogetherintime.Themechanismforavoiding“accentclash”istomoveadjacentaccentsawayfromeachother,aphenomenonsometimescalled“stress-shift”inEnglish.(Oneoft-citedexampleofstressshiftiswhen“thirtéen”becomes“thírteenmén”;Liberman&Prince,1977.)Theterm“stress-shift”issomewhatunfortunate,becausethereisevidencethatwhatisshiftingispitchaccent,notsyllabledurationoramplitude(Shattuck-Hufnageletal.,1994).

Theideathatspeechrhythminvolvestemporalpatterningattwodistinctlinguisticlevelsmeritsfarmoreempiricalresearchthanithasgarneredtodate.Iwillreturntothisideainsection3.3.4.

DurationandTypologyUntilveryrecently,themeasurementofdurationhashadalargelynegativeroleinthestudyofspeechrhythm,namelyinfalsifyingclaimsfortheperiodicityofstressesorsyllables.Theinsightsofthephonologicalapproach,however,havecreatedanewpositiverolefordurationalmeasurements.Akeyfeatureofthisworkhasbeentheabandonmentofanysearchforisochrony,andafocusondurationalcorrelatesofphonologicalphenomenainvolvedinspeechrhythm.Ramusandcolleagues(1999),inspiredbytheinsightsofDauer,examinedthedurationalpatteringofvowelsandconsonantsinspeech,basedonideasabouthowsyllablestructureshouldinfluencethispatterning.Forexample,languages(p.127) thatuseagreatervarietyofsyllabletypes(i.e.,stress-timedlanguages)arelikelytohaverelativelylesstimedevotedtovowelsinsentencesthanlanguagesdominatedbysimplesyllables,duetothefrequentconsonantclustersintheformerlanguages.Bysimilarreasoning,thedurationalvariabilityofconsonantalintervalsinsentences(definedassequencesofconsonantsbetweenvowels,irrespectiveofsyllableorwordboundaries)shouldbegreaterforlanguageswithmorediversesyllablestructures.Thislatterpointisschematicallyillustratedin3.11,inwhich

Page 30: University Press Scholarship Online Oxford Scholarship ...

Rhythm

Page 30 of 79

PRINTED FROM OXFORD SCHOLARSHIP ONLINE (www.oxfordscholarship.com). (c) Copyright Oxford University Press, 2015.All Rights Reserved. Under the terms of the l icence agreement, an individual user may print out a PDF of a single chapter of amonograph in OSO for personal use (for details see http://www.oxfordscholarship.com/page/privacy-policy). Subscriber: UC - SantaCruz; date: 02 May 2015

boundariesbetweensyllablesaremarkedwithadotandconsonantalintervalsareunderlined:

Notehowthegreaterdiversityofsyllabletypesin3.11aleadstogreatervariationinthenumberofconsonantsbetweenvowels(likelytotranslateintogreaterdurationalvariabilityofconsonantalintervals)aswellasalowervoweltoconsonantratio(likelytotranslateintoalowerfractionofutterancedurationspentonvowels).

Theseideaswereborneoutbyempiricalmeasurements.Figure3.7(fromRamusetal.,1999)showsagraphwithpercentofdurationoccupiedbyvowels(%V)versusconsonantalintervalvariability(ΔC)withinsentencesineightlanguages.(Thedataforeachlanguagecamefrom20sentencesreadbyfourspeakers,i.e.,fivesentencesperspeaker.)

Figure3.7 Percentageofsentencedurationoccupiedbyvowelsversusthestandarddeviationofconsonantalintervalswithinsentencesfor8languages.(CA=Catalan,DU=Dutch,EN=English,FR=French,IT=Italian,JA=Japanese,PO=Polish,SP=Spanish.)Errorbarsshow+/-1standarderror.FromRamus,Nespor,&Mehler,1999.

(p.128) Whatisinterestingaboutthisgraphisthatlanguagestraditionallyclassifiedasstress-timed(EnglishandDutch)havelow%VandhighΔCvalues,andoccupyadifferentregionofthegraphthanlanguagestraditionallyclassifiedassyllabletimed(French,Italian,andSpanish).Furthermore,Japanese,whichlinguistsplaceinadifferentrhythmiccategory(moratimed)isisolatedfromtheotherlanguages.(ThelocationofPolishandCatalaninthisgraphisdiscussedinthenextsection,onperception.)Thisdemonstratedanempiricalcorrelateoftraditionallinguisticrhythmicclasses,andhasinspiredotherresearcherstoexaminemorelanguagesinthisframework.OneinterestingstudyisthatofFrotaandVigário(2001),whoexaminedtherhythmofBrazilianPortugueseversusEuropeanPortuguese(henceforthBPandEP).Linguistshadoftenclaimedthatthesetwo

Page 31: University Press Scholarship Online Oxford Scholarship ...

Rhythm

Page 31 of 79

PRINTED FROM OXFORD SCHOLARSHIP ONLINE (www.oxfordscholarship.com). (c) Copyright Oxford University Press, 2015.All Rights Reserved. Under the terms of the l icence agreement, an individual user may print out a PDF of a single chapter of amonograph in OSO for personal use (for details see http://www.oxfordscholarship.com/page/privacy-policy). Subscriber: UC - SantaCruz; date: 02 May 2015

varietieswererhythmicallydifferent,withEPbeingstress-timed,andBPbeingsyllable-timedorhavingmixedrhythmiccharacteristics.ThismakesPortugueseafascinatingtopicforspeechrhythmresearch,becauseonecanstudysentenceswithexactlythesamewordsbutspokenwithdifferentrhythms.(BritishEnglishandSingaporeEnglishprovideanothersuchopportunity,becausetheformerisstress-timedandthelatterhasbeendescribedassyllable-timed;seeLowetal.,2000.)FrotaandVigáriocomparedsentencesspokenbyEuropeanandBrazilianspeakersofPortuguese,andfoundthatEPhadsignificantlyahigherΔCandlower%VthanBP,aspredictedbyRamusetal.’sfindings.15

OneimportantquestionaboutthislineofresearchconcernstheperceptualrelevanceofΔCand%V.Ramusetal.focusedonthesemeasuresbecauseoftheirinterestintheroleofrhythmininfantspeechperception.Thereisevidencethatnewbornsandyounginfantscandiscriminatelanguagesthatbelongtodifferentrhythmicclasses(Mehleretal.,1988,1996;seealsothenextsection).Mehlerandcolleagues(1996)havearguedthatthisabilityhelpsbootstraplanguageacquisition:Onceagivenrhythmicclassisdetected,class-specificacquisitionmechanismscanbetriggeredthatdirectattentiontotheunitsthatarerelevantforsegmentingwordsfromconnectedspeech(e.g.,stressesinthecaseofEnglish,syllablesinthecaseofFrench,asdiscussedinsection3.3.3,subsection“TheRoleofRhythminSegmentingConnectedSpeech”).Forthistheorytowork,infantsmusthavesomebasisfordiscrimi(p.129) natingrhythmicclass.ThusRamusetal.(1999)soughtanacousticcorrelateofrhythmicclassthatwouldrequireminimalknowledgeaboutlinguisticunits.ΔCand%Varetwosuchparameters,becauseoneonlyneedassumethattheinfantcandistinguishbetweenvowelsandconsonants(seeGalvesetal.,2002,foranacousticcorrelateofΔCthatdoesnotevenrequiresegmentationintovowelsandconsonants).

Onemayask,however,ifΔCand%Varedirectlyrelevanttotheperceptionofspeechrhythm,oriftheyaresimplycorrelatedwithanotherfeaturethatismorerelevanttorhythmperception.Thatis,onecouldarguethatthesemeasuresareglobalstatisticsreflectingvariabilityinsyllablestructure,andarenotthemselvesthebasisofrhythmperceptioninspeech(cf.Barryetal.,2003).Amorepromisingcandidateforperceptualrelevancemaybevariabilityinsyllableduration,whichislikelytobecorrelatedwithvariabilityinsyllablestructureandwithvowelreduction.Becausethesyllableiswidelyregardedasafundamentalunitinspeechrhythm,andbecausebothadultsandinfantsaresensitivetosyllablepatterning(e.g.,vanOoyenetal.,1997),itwouldbeworthexaminingthecorpusofsentencesusedbyRamusetal.forsyllabledurationvariabilitytoseeifthisparameterdifferentiatestraditionalrhythmicclasses.ThiswouldalsobeastraightforwardtestofDauer’sideas,asthephonologicalfactorssheoutlinesimplythatsyllabledurationvariabilityshouldbehigherinsentencesofstress-timedthanofsyllable-timedlanguages.

Surprisingly,therehasbeenlittleempiricalworkcomparingsentence-levelvariabilityinsyllabledurationamongdifferentlanguages.Asnotedintheprevioussection,thismayreflectthedifficultiesofassigningsyllableboundariesinconnectedspeech.Fromapurelypracticalstandpoint,itiseasiertodefinephonemeboundaries,usingcriteriaagreed

Page 32: University Press Scholarship Online Oxford Scholarship ...

Rhythm

Page 32 of 79

PRINTED FROM OXFORD SCHOLARSHIP ONLINE (www.oxfordscholarship.com). (c) Copyright Oxford University Press, 2015.All Rights Reserved. Under the terms of the l icence agreement, an individual user may print out a PDF of a single chapter of amonograph in OSO for personal use (for details see http://www.oxfordscholarship.com/page/privacy-policy). Subscriber: UC - SantaCruz; date: 02 May 2015

uponbymostphoneticians(e.g.,Peterson&Lehiste,1960).However,thisshouldnotstopresearchintosyllabicdurationpatterns,becausethesepatternsarelikelytobeperceptuallyrelevant.Toillustrateboththefeasibilityandthechallengesofasyllable-basedapproach,examples3.11canddbelowshowasentenceofEnglishandFrenchsegmentedatsyllableboundaries(thesegmentationsweredonebymyselfandFranckRamus,respectively).Periodsindicatesyllableboundariesthatwefeltwereclear,whereassquarebracketsindicatephonemesthatseemedambiguousintermsoftheirsyllabicaffiliation.Inthelattercase,onemustdecidewheretoplacethesyllableboundary.Forexample,ifthephonemesoundsambisyllabicthentheboundarycanbeplacedinthemiddleofthephoneme,orifitsoundslikeithasbeenresyllabifiedwiththefollowingvowel,theboundarycanbeplacedbeforethephoneme.

(3.11c)The.last.con.cert.gi.ven.at.the.o[p]era.was.a.tre.men.dous.suc.cess

(3.11d)Il.fau.dra.beau.coup.plus.d’ar.gent.pour.me.ne[r]à.bien.ce.pro.jet

Itislikelythatdifferentresearcherswillvaryinhowtheymakethesejudgmentcalls.Nevertheless,thisisnotaninsurmountableproblemforrhythmresearch.Infact,ifdifferentresearchersdefinesyllableboundariesinslightlydifferentwaysbutneverthelessconvergeontherhythmicdifferencestheyfindbetweenlanguages,thisisstrongevidencethattheobserveddifferencesarerobust.16Figure3.8aand3.8bshowmymarkingsofsyllableboundariesinthewaveformandspectrogramsofthesetwosentences(thesentencescanbeheardinSoundExamples3.5aandb;notethatinsentence3.5a,“opera”ispronounced“opra”).

(p.130)

Figure3.8a AsentenceofBritishEnglishsegmentedintosyllables.(Notethat“opera”ispronounced“opra”bythisspeaker.)

Forthesesentences,thevariabilityofsyllabledurationsasmeasuredbythecoefficientofvariation(thestandarddeviationdividedbythemean)is.53fortheEnglishsentence

Page 33: University Press Scholarship Online Oxford Scholarship ...

Rhythm

Page 33 of 79

PRINTED FROM OXFORD SCHOLARSHIP ONLINE (www.oxfordscholarship.com). (c) Copyright Oxford University Press, 2015.All Rights Reserved. Under the terms of the l icence agreement, an individual user may print out a PDF of a single chapter of amonograph in OSO for personal use (for details see http://www.oxfordscholarship.com/page/privacy-policy). Subscriber: UC - SantaCruz; date: 02 May 2015

and.42fortheFrenchsentence.MakingsimilarmeasurementsonalltheEnglishandFrenchsentencesintheRamusdatabaseyieldsthedatainFigure3.8c.Ascanbeseen,onaverageEnglishsentenceshavemorevariablesyllabledurationsthandoFrenchsentences(thedifferenceisstatisticallysignificant,p〈0.01,Mann-WhitneyUtest).Itwouldbeinterestingtohavesimilarvariabilitymeasurementsfornumerouslanguagesthathavebeenclassifiedasstress-versussyllable-timed:Wouldthesemeasurementsdividethelanguagesintotheirtraditionalrhythmicclasses?(SeeWagner&Dellwo,2004,forapromisingstart.)

(p.131)

Figure3.8b AsentenceofFrenchsegmentedintosyllables.

TurningnowtotheideasofDwightBolinger,recallBolinger’sclaimthatsyllablescontainingfullandreducedvowelstendtoalternateinEnglish.Thisleadstoanempiricalprediction,namelythatthedurationalcontrastbetweenadjacentvoweldurationsinEnglishsentencesshouldbegreaterthaninlanguagesofadifferentrhythmicclass,suchasFrenchorSpanish.Infact,thereisresearchsupportingthisprediction,thoughitwasnotinspiredbyBolinger’sworkbutbyaninterestintherolethatvowelreductionplaysintherhythmofstress-versussyllable-timedlanguages.Low,Grabe,andNolan(2000)setouttoexploretheideathatvowelreductioncontributestotheimpressionofstress-timingviaitsimpactonvoweldurationvariabilityinsentences.Theytestedthisideabyexaminingvoweldurationpatterninginastress-timedversusasyllable-timedvarietyofEnglish(Britishvs.SingaporeEnglish).Crucially,theydevelopedanindexofvariabilitythatwassensitivetothepatterningofduration.Their“normalizedpairwisevariabilityindex”(nPVI)measuresthedegreeofcontrastbetweensuccessivedurationsinanutterance.AnintuitionforthenPVIcanbegainedbyexaminingFigure3.9,whichschematicallydepictstwosequencesofeventsofvaryingduration(thelengthofeachbarcorrespondstothedurationoftheevent).

(p.132)

Page 34: University Press Scholarship Online Oxford Scholarship ...

Rhythm

Page 34 of 79

PRINTED FROM OXFORD SCHOLARSHIP ONLINE (www.oxfordscholarship.com). (c) Copyright Oxford University Press, 2015.All Rights Reserved. Under the terms of the l icence agreement, an individual user may print out a PDF of a single chapter of amonograph in OSO for personal use (for details see http://www.oxfordscholarship.com/page/privacy-policy). Subscriber: UC - SantaCruz; date: 02 May 2015

Figure3.8c Thecoefficientofvariation(CV)ofsyllabledurationin20Englishand20Frenchsentences.Errorbarsshow+/-1standarderror.

InsequenceA,neighboringevents(e.g.,events1and2,2and3)tendtohavealargecontrastinduration,andhencethesequencewouldhavealargenPVI.NowconsidersequenceB,whichhasthesamesetofdurationsassequenceA,arrangedinadifferenttemporalorder.Nowneighboringeventstendtohavelowcontrastinduration,givingthesequencealownPVIvalue.Hencethetwosequenceshaveasharpdifferenceindurationalcontrastiveness,eventhoughtheyhaveexactlythesameoverallamountofdurationalvariability,forexample,asmeasuredbythestandarddeviationofdurations.(Seethischapter’sappendix1forthenPVIequation.)

Figure3.9 Schematicofsequencesofeventswithvaryingduration,toillustratethenPVI(longerbars=longerdurations).Seetextfordetails.

(p.133) BecausethenPVIisfundamentallyameasureofcontrast,theuseoftheterm“variability”initsnameissomewhatunfortunate,asvariabilityandcontrastarenotnecessarilycorrelated,asshowninFigure3.9.Infact,itisquitepossibletohavetwosequencesAandBinwhichthevariabilityofdurationsinAisgreaterthanB,butthenPVIofdurationsisgreaterinBthanofA(anexampleisgiveninsection3.5.1).Thusabettertermforthismeasuremighthavebeenthe“normalizedpairwisecontrastivenessindex.”

Page 35: University Press Scholarship Online Oxford Scholarship ...

Rhythm

Page 35 of 79

PRINTED FROM OXFORD SCHOLARSHIP ONLINE (www.oxfordscholarship.com). (c) Copyright Oxford University Press, 2015.All Rights Reserved. Under the terms of the l icence agreement, an individual user may print out a PDF of a single chapter of amonograph in OSO for personal use (for details see http://www.oxfordscholarship.com/page/privacy-policy). Subscriber: UC - SantaCruz; date: 02 May 2015

IhavedelvedintothedetailsofthenPVIbecauseithasprovenquitefruitfulinthestudyofspeechrhythmandinthecomparativestudyoflinguisticandmusicalrhythm(discussedin3.5.1).GrabeandLow(2002)haveusedthenPVItoexaminethepatterningofvoweldurationsinsentencesofanumberoflanguages,andhaveshownthatseverallanguagestraditionallyclassifiedasstress-timed(suchasGerman,Dutch,BritishEnglish,andThai)havealargervocalicnPVIthananumberofotherlanguagestraditionallyclassifiedassyllabletimed(suchasFrench,Italian,andSpanish).ThissupportsBolinger’sideathatdurationalalternationofvowelsisimportanttostress-timedrhythm.17Inspiredbythiswork,Ramus(2002b)measuredthevowelnPVIforalleightlanguagesinhisdatabaseandfoundtheresultsshowninFigure3.10.

Figure3.10plotsthenPVIforvocalicintervalsagainsttherPVIforintervocalicintervals(i.e.,consonantalintervals).(TherPVI,or“rawpairwisevariabilityindex,”iscomputedinthesamewayasthenPVIbutwithoutthenormalizationterminthedenominator;cf.thischapter’sappendix1.GrabeandLow[2002]arguethatnormalizationisnotdesirableforconsonantalintervalsbecauseitwouldnormalizeforcross-languagedifferencesinsyllablestructure.)FocusingonthenPVIdimension,thestress-timedlanguages(EnglishandDutch)areseparatedfromthesyllable-timedlanguages(Spanish,Italian,andFrench),whichprovidesadditionalsupportforBolinger’sideas.18Furthermore,Polishisnowfarfromthestress-timedlanguages,whichisinterestingbecausethereisperceptualevidencethatPolishisrhythmicallydifferentfromtheselanguagesduetoitslackofvowelreduction(seesection3.3.1,subsection“PerceptionandTypology”).JapaneseissimilartoFrenchintermsofnPVI,however,suggestingthatnPVIaloneisnotenoughtosortlanguagesintotraditionalrhythmicclasses.AddingaseconddimensionofrPVIforconsonantalintervals,however,doessegregateoutJapanese,whichhasverylowdurationalcontrastbetweensuccessiveconsonantalintervals.Thissuggeststhatatleasttwophoneticdimensionsmaybeneededtocapturedifferencesbetweenrhythmicclasses(seealsoRamusetal.,1999).

(p.134)

Page 36: University Press Scholarship Online Oxford Scholarship ...

Rhythm

Page 36 of 79

PRINTED FROM OXFORD SCHOLARSHIP ONLINE (www.oxfordscholarship.com). (c) Copyright Oxford University Press, 2015.All Rights Reserved. Under the terms of the l icence agreement, an individual user may print out a PDF of a single chapter of amonograph in OSO for personal use (for details see http://www.oxfordscholarship.com/page/privacy-policy). Subscriber: UC - SantaCruz; date: 02 May 2015

Figure3.10 VocalicnPVIversusConsonantal(intervocalic)rPVIforsentencesineightlanguages.(CA=Catalan,DU=Dutch,EN=English,FR=French,IT=Italian,JA=Japanese,PO=Polish,SP=Spanish.)Errorbarsshow+/-1standarderror.FromRamus,2002a.

OneinterestinglinguisticapplicationofthenPVIhasbeentotheontogenyofspeechrhythm.IthasbeenclaimedthattherhythmofEnglish-speakingchildrenissyllable-timedincontrasttothestress-timedrhythmofadultspeech(Allen&Hawkins,1978).Grabeetal.(1999)conductedannPVIstudythat(p.135) supportedthisclaim.TheymeasuredthenPVIofvowelsinthespeechofEnglishversusFrenchspeaking4-year-oldsandtheirmothers.TheyfoundthatEnglishchildrenhadsignificantlylowernPVIvaluesthantheirmothers,whereasFrenchchildrenresembledtheirmothersinhavingalownPVIs.Thatis,bothEnglishandFrenchchildrenspokewithasyllable-timedrhythm(thoughthenPVIoftheEnglishchildrenwasalreadylargerthanthatoftheirFrenchcounterparts).ItwouldbeinterestingtotracknPVIasafunctionofageinEnglishandFrenchchildren,tostudythedevelopmentaltimecourseofspeechrhythminthetwolanguages.

AllnPVIstudiestodatehavefocusedonasinglerhythmiclayerinlanguage:thetemporalpatterningofvowelsorconsonants.InthespiritofBolinger’sideathatrhythmmayinvolvemultiplelevelsoftemporalorganization,itwouldbeworthusingthenPVItoexploretherelationshipofdurationalpatternsatvariousrhythmicallyrelevantlevelsinspeech(cf.Asu&Nolan,2006).Forexample,withinEnglishsentences,onecouldcomputethenPVIofinterstressintervals(ISIs)relativetothenPVIofsyllabledurationsbymeasuringbothofthesequantitiesineachsentenceandthentakingtheratiooftheformertothelatter.ItmaybethatthesubjectiveimpressionofisochronyinEnglisharisesinpartfromalowerdurationalcontrastbetweenISIsthanbetweensyllables,whichwouldmakethisratiosignificantlylessthan1.Ireturntothisideainsection3.3.4.

Thissectionhasreviewedafewdifferentacousticcorrelatesofspeechrhythm.Duetothesuccessofthiswork,itseemscertainthatmoresuchcorrelateswillbeproposedandexploredinthefuture(e.g.,Gut,2005).Ultimately,theusefulnessofsuchmeasureswilldependonwhethertheygrouptogetherlanguagesthatareperceivedasrhythmicallysimilaranddividelanguagesperceivedasrhythmicallydifferent.Perceptualstudiesare

Page 37: University Press Scholarship Online Oxford Scholarship ...

Rhythm

Page 37 of 79

PRINTED FROM OXFORD SCHOLARSHIP ONLINE (www.oxfordscholarship.com). (c) Copyright Oxford University Press, 2015.All Rights Reserved. Under the terms of the l icence agreement, an individual user may print out a PDF of a single chapter of amonograph in OSO for personal use (for details see http://www.oxfordscholarship.com/page/privacy-policy). Subscriber: UC - SantaCruz; date: 02 May 2015

thusfundamentaltoresearchonrhythmictypology,anditistosuchstudiesthatweturnnext.

PerceptionandTypologyAlltypologicaltheoriesoflanguagerhythmareultimatelyrootedinperception.Inthepast,linguistshavedefinedrhythmcategories(suchasstressvs.syllabletiming)basedontheirauditoryimpressionsoflanguages,andthenresearchershavesoughttoidentifyphonologicalandacousticcorrelatesoftheseclasses.Therecentsuccessinfindingdurationalcorrelatesoftraditionalrhythmclassesisatestamenttotheintuitionoflinguistsintheirauralrhythmicjudgments.However,itisalsoapparentthattheoldcategorizationsystemhasitsshortcomings.Forexample,somelanguagesstraddledifferentcategories(e.g.,PolishandCatalan,seeabove),andmanylanguagesdonotfitneatlyintoanyoftheexistingcategories(Grabe&Low,2002).Thustheoldsystemiscrackingattheseams,andanewscienceofrhythmclassificationiscalledfor.Suchasciencemusthaveasitsfoundationabodyofperceptualdatathatprovidesa(p.136)measureoftherhythmicsimilaritiesanddifferencesbetweenlanguages.Thesedatawillallowresearcherstoconstructaperceptualmapoflanguagerhythmsanddeterminetowhatextenttherhythmsofhumanlanguagesfallintodistinctclusters(vs.formingacontinuum).Itwillalsohelpsuggestnewavenuesforempiricalresearchintotheacousticfoundationsofspeechrhythm.

Fortunately,perceptualworkontherhythmicdifferencesbetweenlanguageshasalreadybegun.AninnovativestudybyRamusandMehler(1999)devisedamethodforstudyingtheperceptionofspeechrhythmpositedontheideathatifalistenercantelltwolanguagesapartwhentheonlycuesarerhythmic,thenthelanguagesbelongtodistinctrhythmicclasses.Speechresynthesistechniqueswereusedtoselectivelyremovevariousphoneticdifferencesbetweenlanguagesandfocusattentiononrhythm.SoundExamples3.6and3.7illustrateRamusandMehler’stechniqueonasentenceofEnglishandJapanese.Eachsentenceispresentedinfourversions,whichconverttheoriginalsentencetoanincreasinglyabstracttemporalpatternofvowelsandconsonants.Inthefirsttransformation,eachphonemeisreplacedbyaparticularmemberofitsclass:allfricativesreplacedby/s/,vowelsby/a/,liquids(l&r)by/l/,plosivesby/t/,nasalsby/n/,andglidesby/ai/(aconditiontheycalled“saltanaj,”pronounced“sal-tan-ai”).Theoriginalintonationofeachsentenceispreserved.Inthesecondtransformation,allconsonantsarereplacedby/s/andallvowelsby/a/(aconditiontheycall“sasasa”).Inthefinaltransformation,thevoicepitchisflattenedtoamonotone,leavingthetemporalpatternofvowelsandconsonantsastheonlydifferencebetweenthelanguages(aconditiontheauthorsrefertoas“flatsasasa”).RamusandMehlerfoundthatFrenchadultscoulddiscriminatebetweenEnglishandJapaneseinallthreeconditions,supportingthehypothesisthattherhythmsofEnglishandJapaneseareindeedperceptuallydistinct.

Focusingontheflatsasasatransformation,Ramusetal.(2003)alsotestedFrenchadults’abilitytodiscriminatetherhythmsofEnglish,Polish,Spanish,andCatalan.TheresultsindicatedthatPolishcouldbediscriminatedfromtheotherlanguages,whereasCatalancouldnotbediscriminatedfromSpanish,thoughitwasdistinctfromEnglishandPolish.

Page 38: University Press Scholarship Online Oxford Scholarship ...

Rhythm

Page 38 of 79

PRINTED FROM OXFORD SCHOLARSHIP ONLINE (www.oxfordscholarship.com). (c) Copyright Oxford University Press, 2015.All Rights Reserved. Under the terms of the l icence agreement, an individual user may print out a PDF of a single chapter of amonograph in OSO for personal use (for details see http://www.oxfordscholarship.com/page/privacy-policy). Subscriber: UC - SantaCruz; date: 02 May 2015

(Recallthatonphonologicalgrounds,PolishandCatalanseemedintermediatebetweenstress-timedandsyllable-timedlanguages;cf.section3.3.1,subsection“PhonologyandTypology.”)TheseperceptualdatasuggestthatPolishdoesbelonginaseparaterhythmiccategorythanEnglish,whereasCatalanbelongsinthesamecategoryasSpanish.Thisfindinghasimplicationsformapsoftheacousticcorrelatesofspeechrhythm,suchasFigure3.7.Inthatmap,Polishclusteredwithstress-timedlanguages,indicatingthatadifferentacousticdimensionisneededtoseparateperceivedrhythmicclasses.Indeed,Ramusetal.(1999,2003)havenotedthatPolishcanbeseparatedoutfromallotherlanguagesintheiroriginalstudyonadimensionthatmeasuresthevariabilityofvoweldurationinasentence,ΔV,becausePolishhasaverylowvoweldurationvariabilitycomparedtoallotherlanguagesin(p.137) theirsample.19Thusperceptualworkonspeechhasalreadysuggestedthatifonewishestopreservethenotionofrhythmclassesinlanguage,atleastfourclassesareneeded:stress-timed,syllable-timed,mora-timed(representedbyJapanese),andoneotheryet-to-be-namedcategoryrepresentedbyPolish.20

Anotherlineofperceptualresearchconcernedwithrhythmictypologyhasfocusedonnewbornsandinfants.Thischoiceofsubjectsmayseemsurprising,butthesestudiesaremotivatedbytheideathatveryyounghumansaresensitivetospeechrhythmanduseittoguidelearningoffine-grainedsoundpatternsoflanguage(Mehleretal.,1996).Nazzietal.(1998)studiednewbornrhythmperceptionusinglow-passfilteredspeech.Thisremovesmostofthephoneticinformationbutpreservessyllable,stress,andpitchpatterns.TheyshowedthatFrenchnewbornsareabletodiscriminateEnglishfromJapanese,butnotEnglishfromDutch,suggestingthatthelatteraremembersofthesamerhythmicclass.TheyalsoshowedthatthenewbornscoulddiscriminateEnglishandDutchfromSpanishandItalian,butnotEnglishandSpanishfromDutchandItalian,suggestingthattheformerpairingsmoreaccuratelycaptureperceptualrhythmicclasses(cf.Nazzietal.,2000,forconvergingfindingswith5-month-oldinfants).Thesefindingssupporttheauthors’hypothesisthatbabiescandiscriminatelanguagesonlywhentheybelongtodifferentrhythmicclasses,anotionthattheydubthe“rhythmhypothesis”forlanguageacquisition.Ifthisistrue,thentheearsofinfantsmaybeparticularlyimportantinstrumentsinmappinghumanspeechrhythmsinfutureresearch.21

ThestudiesofRamus,Nazzi,andcolleaguesraiseanumberofpointsforfutureresearchontheperceptionofspeechrhythm.First,itisimportanttodesignstimuliandtasksthatfocusattentiononthoseaspectsofspeechrhythmthatplayaroleinnormalspeechperception.Forexample,adangeroftheflatsasasaconditionitthatwhenalanguagewithahighlyvariablesyllablestructure(suchasEnglish)iscomparedtoalanguagedominatedbysimplesyllables(suchasJapanese),asalientperceptualdifferencebetweentheresultingflat(p.138) sasasastimuliisthemorefrequentoccurrenceoflong-duration/s/soundsintheformerstimulus(whichresultfromtransformingconsonantclustersintosingle,long/s/sounds).Thusdiscriminationcouldsimplybebasedonlisteningforfrequentlong/s/soundsratherthanonattendingtotemporalstructure.

Page 39: University Press Scholarship Online Oxford Scholarship ...

Rhythm

Page 39 of 79

PRINTED FROM OXFORD SCHOLARSHIP ONLINE (www.oxfordscholarship.com). (c) Copyright Oxford University Press, 2015.All Rights Reserved. Under the terms of the l icence agreement, an individual user may print out a PDF of a single chapter of amonograph in OSO for personal use (for details see http://www.oxfordscholarship.com/page/privacy-policy). Subscriber: UC - SantaCruz; date: 02 May 2015

Second,researchontheperceptualtaxonomyoflanguagerhythmsshouldnotonlybebasedondiscriminationtasks,butshouldalsoincorporatesimilarityjudgments.Musicalstudiesofrhythmicsimilarityprovideagoodmodelforsuchresearch(Gabrielsson,1973,1993).Inthisresearch,rhythmsarepresentedinapairwisefashionandlistenersratetheirperceivedsimilarityusinganumericalscale.Theresultingratingsarestudiedusingmultidimensionalscalingtouncoverperceptualdimensionsusedbylistenersinclassifyingrhythms.Thisparadigmcouldeasilybeadaptedtostudyspeechrhythm,usinglow-passfilteredspeechwithminimalpitchvariationasstimuli.Suchstudiesshouldbesensitivetotheideathattheimportantperceptualdimensionsforrhythmmayberelational,forexample,ahighcontrastivenessbetweensuccessivesyllabledurationswhilesimultaneouslyhavingalowerdurationalcontrastivenessbetweeninterstressintervals(cf.theendofsection3.3.1,subsection“DurationandTypology”).

Finally,afundamentalissueforallfuturestudiesofrhythmictypologyistheextenttowhichperceivedrhythmicsimilaritiesanddifferencesbetweenlanguagesdependonthenativelanguageofthelistener.Thetheoryofstress,syllable,andmoratimingwasproposedbynativeEnglishspeakers,anditisanopenquestionwhetherspeakersofotherlanguagesperceiverhythmiccuesinthesamewaythatEnglishspeakersdo.Forexample,ithasrecentlybeendemonstratedthatFrenchlistenershavesomedifficultydistinguishingnonsensewordsthatdifferonlyinthelocationofstress,whereasSpanishlistenershavenosuchdifficulty(Dupouxetal.,2001).ThislikelyreflectsthefactthatSpanishhascontrastivestress:Twowordscanhavethesamephonemesbutadifferentstresspattern,andthiscanchangethemeaningofthewordentirely(e.g.,sábanavs.sabána,whichmean“sheet”and“savannah”respectively;cf.Soto-Faracoetal.,2001).Frenchdoesnothavethisproperty,andDupouxetal.suggestthatthisdifferenceisresponsibleforthe“stressdeafness”theyfoundintheirFrenchlisteners.Resultssuchasthisraiseafundamentalquestion:Isthereasinglemapofperceivedrhythmicsimilaritiesanddifferencesamonglanguages,ordoesthegeographyofthemapdifferaccordingtothenativelanguageofthelistener?Onlyempiricalworkcanresolvethisissue,butitseemsarealpossibilitythatnativelanguageinfluencestheperceptionofrhythmicrelationsbetweenlanguages.

3.3.2PrinciplesGoverningtheRhythmicShapeofWordsandUtterances

Forthoseinterestedincomparingrhythminlanguageandmusic,itisimportanttobefamiliarwithabranchoftheoreticallinguisticsknownas“metrical(p.139) phonology.”Metricalphonologydealswithspeechrhythm,butitdoessoinamannerquitedifferentfromtheapproachesdescribedsofar.Firstandforemost,rhythmicprominenceistreatedashierarchical.Thatis,prominenceisincrementallyassignedateachleveloftheprosodichierarchyaccordingtosystematicprinciples.Forexample,inagiventheoryitmaybethecasethatallsyllablesbeginwithabasicamountofprominence,thenthelexicallystressedsyllableofeachword(orcliticgroup)isassignedanadditionaldegreeofprominence,thenaphrase-levelprominenceisaddedtoaparticularwordofaphrase(e.g.,the“nuclearstressrule”inEnglish),andsoon.Inthisview,prominenceisnotsimplyabinaryphoneticfeaturecalled“stress”thatsyllableseitherhaveordonot.Rather,prominenceisanacousticprojectionofthehierarchicalprosodicstructureofan

Page 40: University Press Scholarship Online Oxford Scholarship ...

Rhythm

Page 40 of 79

PRINTED FROM OXFORD SCHOLARSHIP ONLINE (www.oxfordscholarship.com). (c) Copyright Oxford University Press, 2015.All Rights Reserved. Under the terms of the l icence agreement, an individual user may print out a PDF of a single chapter of amonograph in OSO for personal use (for details see http://www.oxfordscholarship.com/page/privacy-policy). Subscriber: UC - SantaCruz; date: 02 May 2015

utterance,andassuch,hasseveraldegreesthatservetoindicateasyllable’spositioninanutterance’srhythmichierarchy(Halle&Vergnaud,1987;Halle&Idsardi,1996;Shattuck-Hufnagel&Turk,1996).22

OneoftheclearestexpositionsofmetricalphonologyisinSelkirk’s1984book,PhonologyandSyntax:TheRelationBetweenSoundandStructure.Onegoalofthisbookistoshowhowonecangofromastringofwordstoarepresentationofthesyllabicprominencepatternofthespokenutteranceinarule-governedfashion.Therelativeprominenceofsyllablesisrepresentedusinga“metricalgrid”thattreatseachsyllableasapointinabstracttime(Figure3.11),meaningthatprominencepatternsareconsideredwithoutregardtotheirexacttiming.

Twoaspectsofthelinguisticmetricalgrid,introducedbyLiberman(1975),embody“theclaimthattherhythmicorganizationofspeechisquiteanalogoustothatofmusic”(Selkirk,1984:9).First,asnotedabove,prominenceistreatedhierarchically,analogouslytohierarchicaltheoriesofmusicalmeter(Cooper&Meyer,1960;Lerdahl&Jackendoff,1983).Abovethebasiclevelofthesyllableareseveralotherlevels.Thesecondlevelmarksstressedsyllables,andisthelevelofthebasic“beat,”inanalogytothetactusinmusic(Selkirk,1984:10,40).Thethirdlevelmarkstheprimarylexicalstressofeachword,andthefourthlevelmarksthemainaccentofeachphrase.This“text-to-grid”assignmentofbeatsprovidestheinputonwhichrhythmicprinciplesoperate.Theseprinciples,whichrepresentthesecondlinktomusicalmeter,amounttoatendencytoalternatebetweenstrongerandweakerelementsateachlevelofthehierarchy.Theprinciplesareenforcedbyrulesthatcanadd,delete,andmovebeatstomakethepatternateachlevelmorecongruouswithanalternatingpattern.Forexample,aruleof“beataddition”mightaddabeatatthesecondleveltoavoidalongseriesofunstressedsyllables.Atthethirdlevel,aruleof“beatmovement”mightshifttheprimarystress/accentofwordtoavoidtheadjacencyofprimarylexicalstress/accent(aswhen“thirtéen”becomes“thírteenmén”;LibermanandPrince,1977,Shattuck-Hufnageletal.,1994;Grabe&Warren,1995).Theidealgoalisthatstrongbeatsatanygivenlevelareseparatedbynomorethantwoweakbeatsatthatlevel(the“principleofrhythmicalternation”).Thusmetricalphonologyderivestheprominencepatternofasentenceusingideasdirectlyinspiredbytheoriesofmusicalmeter.

(p.140)

Figure3.11 AmetricalgridforasentenceofEnglish.FromSelkirk,1984.

Thenotionthatspeechhasmultiplerhythmicallyrelevantlevelsisaninterestingabstractsimilaritybetweenrhythminlanguageandmusic,becauseafundamentalpropertyof

Page 41: University Press Scholarship Online Oxford Scholarship ...

Rhythm

Page 41 of 79

PRINTED FROM OXFORD SCHOLARSHIP ONLINE (www.oxfordscholarship.com). (c) Copyright Oxford University Press, 2015.All Rights Reserved. Under the terms of the l icence agreement, an individual user may print out a PDF of a single chapter of amonograph in OSO for personal use (for details see http://www.oxfordscholarship.com/page/privacy-policy). Subscriber: UC - SantaCruz; date: 02 May 2015

musicalmeteristheexistenceofperceptuallysalienttemporalpatteringonmultipletimescales(cf.section3.2.2).Furthermore,justasmusicalmeterinvolvesatleastonepsychologicallyaccessiblerhythmiclevelbelowthebeatandoneortwolevelsaboveit,metricalphonologyproposesrhythmiclevelsbelowandabovethe“beat”ofstressedsyllables.Thatis,boththeoriesconcernthepatterningoftimeintervalsatseveraltimescales.

Althoughthepicturepaintedbymetricalphonologyisanelegantone,itshouldbenotedthatitsclaimsarebynomeansuniversallyacceptedbyspeechscientists(Cooper&Eady,1986),andthatthepatternsofprominenceitproposesaretypicallyconstructedfromtheintuitionsoflinguistsratherthanfromacousticandperceptualdatacollectedinlaboratorysettings.However,thereishopethatthefieldcanbeputonanempiricalfooting.Forexample,thereisphoneticevidenceforfourdegreesofprominenceinspeech(atleastinstress-timedlanguages),correspondingtoreducedvowels,fullvowels,stressedsyllables,andaccentedsyllables(Terken&Hermes,2000).Forthecurrentpurposes,metricalphonologyisinterestingbecauseitdrawsattentiontoanumberofissuesinwhichcomparisonsoflinguisticandmusicalrhythmareinstructive.Oneoftheseissues(multiplelayeringinrhythmicstructure)leadstoideasforempiricalcomparativestudiesofrhythminspeechandmusic,andisdiscussedfurtherinsection3.3.4.Twootherissuesarediscussedbelow.

(p.141) DifferencesbetweenLinguisticandMusicalMetricalGridsAlthoughthehierarchiespositedbymetricalphonologywereinspiredbyWesternmusic,someveryimportantdifferencesbetweenthemetersofmusicandlanguagearereadilyapparent.Mostnotably,temporalperiodicityinmusicalmeterismuchstricterthananythingfoundinspeech,andthisdifferencehasdramaticcognitiveconsequences.Theregularperiodicitiesofmusicallowmetertoserveasamentalframeworkforsoundperception,suchthataneventcanbeperceivedasmetricallyprominentevenifisphysicallyquiteweak,asinsyncopatedrhythms.Bycomparison,theprominencesoflanguagearenotregularenoughtoallowforanythingasabstractassyncopation.Asaresult,linguisticmetricalgridsarenotabstractperiodicmentalpatterns(likemusicalmetricalgrids)butaresimplymapsofheardprominences,fulloftemporalirregularities.Forexample,Dauer(1983)reportsthattheaverageinterstressintervalinspeechwasaround450ms,withastandarddeviationofapproximately150ms.Dividingthestandarddeviationbythemeanyieldsacoefficientofvariationofabout33%.Thisvariabilityismarkedlydifferentfrommusic,inwhichperceivedbeatsoccurinamuchmoreevenlyspacedfashion.Forexample,whentappingtomusic,adultsshowacoefficientofvariationofabout5%.Thus“metricalgrids”inlanguageshouldperhapsbecalled“prominencegrids,”toavoidtheimplicationofanabstractmentalperiodicity.

QuestioningthePrincipleofRhythmicAlternationinSpeechSettingasidequestionsoftemporalperiodicity,onecanaskifspeechandmusicshareamoreabstractsimilarityintermsofatendencytoarrangeprominencesinalternatingpatternsofstrongandweakelements.Ifso,thismightsuggestabasiccognitiverelationshipbetweenrhythminlanguageandmusic.Evidenceinfavorofaprincipleof

Page 42: University Press Scholarship Online Oxford Scholarship ...

Rhythm

Page 42 of 79

PRINTED FROM OXFORD SCHOLARSHIP ONLINE (www.oxfordscholarship.com). (c) Copyright Oxford University Press, 2015.All Rights Reserved. Under the terms of the l icence agreement, an individual user may print out a PDF of a single chapter of amonograph in OSO for personal use (for details see http://www.oxfordscholarship.com/page/privacy-policy). Subscriber: UC - SantaCruz; date: 02 May 2015

alternationinlanguagecomesfromstudiesshowingthatEnglishspeakersadjustprominencepatternstomakethemmoreregular.Forexample,KellyandBock(1988)hadspeakerspronouncenonsensewordsembeddedinsentences,suchas:

(3.12a)Thefullteplezdecreased.

(3.12b)Throwtheteplezbadly.

Thefocusofinterestwaswhetherspeakersstressedthefirstorsecondsyllableofthenonsenseword.Overall,speakerstendedtostressthefirstsyllable,inaccordancewithageneraltrendinEnglishfordisyllabicnounstohaveinitialstress.However,thistendencywassignificantlyweakerwhenthenonsensewordwasprecededbyastressedsyllable(asinsentence3.12a),asifspeakerswantedtospreadouttheoccurrenceofstresses.Furtherevidenceforalternation(p.142) ofstresspatternsinspeechproductioncomesfromCutler(1980),whoexaminedsentencesinwhichspeakersinadvertentlyomittedasyllable,suchas:

versustheintendedsentence:

Muchmoreoftenthanchance,theerrorsservedtoshortenalongrunofunstressedsyllables,thustendingtopromotethealternationofstressedandunstressedsyllables.

Althoughthesefindingsseemtosupportapositiveprincipleofalternation,itisalsopossiblethattheyreflecttheactionofanegativeprinciplethatseekstobreakupclustersofprominentsyllablesorclustersofnonprominentsyllables(i.e.,“stressclashes”and“stresslapses,”Nespor&Vogel,1989).SomesupportforthelatterviewcomesfromtheobservationthattheregularizingtendenciesreportedbyKellyandBock(1988)andCutler(1980)areactuallyratherweak.Intheformerstudy,subjectsplacedinitialstressonthetargetnonsensewordinthemajorityofcases,whetherornottheimmediatelyprecedingsyllablewasstressed.Thepresenceofapriorstressedsyllablesimplyloweredtheproportionofinitialstressfrom80%to70%,suggestingonlyamildtendencytomaintainanalternatingstresspatterninspeech.Similarly,Cutler’sstudyisbasedoncollectingrelativelyraresyllableomissionerrors,meaningthatspeakersusuallymanagequitewellwithirregularprominencepatterns.

Thusatthecurrenttimeitisimpossibletoruleoutthehypothesisthatthetendencytoalternatestrongerandweakersyllablesinspeechistheresultofnonrhythmicforcesthatseektokeepprominencesatacomfortabledistancefromeachother.Infact,researchonGreeksuggeststhatthealternationofprominencemaynotevenbeauniversalpatternforhumanlanguages,becauseGreektolerateslongsequencesofunstressedsyllables

Page 43: University Press Scholarship Online Oxford Scholarship ...

Rhythm

Page 43 of 79

PRINTED FROM OXFORD SCHOLARSHIP ONLINE (www.oxfordscholarship.com). (c) Copyright Oxford University Press, 2015.All Rights Reserved. Under the terms of the l icence agreement, an individual user may print out a PDF of a single chapter of amonograph in OSO for personal use (for details see http://www.oxfordscholarship.com/page/privacy-policy). Subscriber: UC - SantaCruz; date: 02 May 2015

(Arvaniti,1994).Thusitmaybethattheonlyuniversalprincipleregardingprominencepatternsinlanguageisthatprominencesthataretooclosetogetheraresubjecttolinguisticmechanismsforclashavoidance(seeArvaniti,1994,forevidencefromGreek;andNespor,1990,forreferencestostudiesofclashavoidancemechanismsinnumerouslanguages).Thereasonsuchmechanismsexistmayultimatelyberootedinthemechanicsofarticulation.Stressedsyllablestendtobemadewithlargerjawmovementsthanunstressedsyllables(deJong,1995),anditmaybebiomechanicallyadvantageoustoavoidcrowdinglargerjawmovementstogetherwhenspeakingatthefastratestypicalofnormalconversation.

(p.143) 3.3.3ThePerceptionofSpeechRhythmTheroleofrhythminspeechperceptionhasbeenthefocusofatleastfourdifferentlinesofresearch.Twoofthesehaveobviousconceptualconnectionstomusicalrhythm:thestudyofperceivedisochronyinspeechandtheinvestigationoftheroleofrhythmicpredictabilityinperception.Thethirdlinehasinvestigatedtheroleofspeechrhythmintheperceptualsegmentationofwordsfromconnectedspeech.Althoughnotobviousatfirst,thisresearchisinfactquitepertinenttocomparativeperceptualstudiesofrhythminlanguageandmusic.Thefinal(andmostrecent)lineofworkconcernstherolethatrhythmplaysinperceptionofnonnativeaccents.Althoughnoconceptuallinkhasyetbeenmadebetweenthisworkandmusicresearch,itisbrieflydescribedbecauseitisapromisingnewareaforempiricalworkonspeechrhythm.

ThePerceptionofIsochronyinSpeechAsnotedinsection3.3.1(subsection“PeriodicityandTypology”),theideathatlinguisticrhythminvolvesregulartemporalintervals(e.g.,betweenstressesorsyllables)hasreceivednoempiricalsupportfrommeasurementsofspeech.However,allsuchmeasurementshavebeenbasedondatafromspeechproduction,inotherwords,onwaveformsorspectrogramsofspokenutterances.Inaninfluentialpaper,Lehiste(1977)madetheinterestingsuggestionthatperiodicitymaybestrongerinperceptionthaninproduction.Thatis,theearmayignoreorcompensateforsurfaceirregularitiesinjudgingperiodicityinspeech.Shebasedthisideaonempiricalworkinwhichsheexaminedtheabilityoflistenerstoidentifythelongestorshortestinterstressinterval(ISI)inshortsentencesoffourISIs,andtodothesametaskonnonspeechanalogsofthesentencesinwhichthestresseswerereplacedbyclicksandthespeechbynoise.Shefoundthatlistenersperformedbetterinthenonlinguisticcondition,andsuggestedthatiflistenershaddifficultyjudgingISIdurationdifferencesinspeech,thiswouldleadtoasensethatISIsweresimilarinduration,inotherwords,animpressionofisochrony.(Ofcourse,itmaynotbespeechpersethatmakessmalldurationdifferencesdifficulttodetect;itcouldbethatthepresenceofsemanticmeaninginlanguagepreoccupiesthelistener’sattentionsothatfinedurationjudgmentsaredifficult.Thusanimportantcontrolconditionforfuturestudiesofthissortistousealanguagewithwhichthelistenerisunfamiliar.)

Lehistewentontostudythejustnoticeabledifference(JND)indurationforsequencesoffournoise-filledintervals,reasoningthatthiswouldestablishaconservativeestimateof

Page 44: University Press Scholarship Online Oxford Scholarship ...

Rhythm

Page 44 of 79

PRINTED FROM OXFORD SCHOLARSHIP ONLINE (www.oxfordscholarship.com). (c) Copyright Oxford University Press, 2015.All Rights Reserved. Under the terms of the l icence agreement, an individual user may print out a PDF of a single chapter of amonograph in OSO for personal use (for details see http://www.oxfordscholarship.com/page/privacy-policy). Subscriber: UC - SantaCruz; date: 02 May 2015

theJNDsforISIsinspeech.Sheusedthreebasicreferencedurationsinhernoisesequences(300,400,and500ms).Ineachsequence,threeofthefourintervalshadthesameduration,andthefourthwasincreasedordecreasedinnine10-mssteps.Shefoundthatreliablejudgmentsidentifyingoneintervalaslongerorshorterthantheothersrequiredchangesofbetween(p.144) 30and100ms.ShearguedthatJNDsforISIsinspeecharenobetterthanthisandarelikelytobeworse,andthusthatphysicalmeasurementsofisochronyneedtotakethis“perceptualtolerance”intoaccount(cf.Kristofferson,1980).

Lehiste’sworkisinterestingbecauseitraisesthepossibilitythatlistenershearmoreisochronythanisreallythereinspeech.SomeevidenceofferedinfavorofthisargumentcomesfromDonovanandDarwin(1979),whohadindividualslistentoEnglishsentencesandthenimitatethetimingofeachsentence’sstresspatternbytapping.Thesubjectsalsoperformedthistaskwithsequencesofnoiseswhosetimingmimickedthestresspatternofsentences.Thecriticalfindingwasthatwhenimitatingspeech,subjectstappedwithlesstemporalvariabilitythantheactualtimingofstressedsyllables,whereaswhenimitatingnoisetheydidnotshowthispattern.

Althoughthesefindingsareintriguing,furtherworkhassuggestedthatthisparadigmmaybeflawed.Scottetal.(1985)replicatedthefindingsofDonovanandDarwinforEnglish,butalsofoundthatsubjectsshowedregularizationoftappingtoFrench(whichisnotconsideredtohaveperiodicstress),aswellastogarbledspeech.Thissuggeststhattheobservedregularizationmaybeasideconsequenceofthegreaterdifficultyofrememberingacousticallycomplexstimuliversusnoisepatterns.

ThusLehiste’sideasmeritfurtherinvestigation,becausetheyraisetheimportantissueofhowperceivedtimingpatternsrelatetothephysicalintervalsmeasuredinspeech.Nevertheless,thereisnothinginLehiste’sworkthatsupportstheideathatspeechisperceivedasisochronousunderordinarycircumstances.Asnotedinsection3.3.2(subsection“DifferencesBetweenLinguisticandMusicalMetricalGrids”),thevariabilityofISIdurationsinspeechisontheorderof33%.Fora500-msaverageISI,thisis150ms,whichisabovethethresholdforsubjectiveisochronysuggestedbyLehiste.

Lehiste’sideasdopointinoneinterestingdirectionintermsofcomparativestudiesofspeechandmusic,namelyadirectcomparisonofthethresholdfordetectingtemporalirregularitiesinperceptuallyisochronoussequences(e.g.,arepeatingsyllable“tatatata”)versusarepeatingmusicalsoundofequivalentacousticcomplexity.23Ifspeechcantoleratemoredurationalvariabilitythanmusicandstillsoundisochronous,thisraisesinterestingquestionsaboutdifferentmechanismsfortimeperceptioninthetwodomains.

Onecouldalsoexaminethethresholdfordetectingtempochangeinisochronoussequencesinspeechandmusic.Currentdatasuggestthatfornonmusicianlisteners,thethresholdfortempochangedetectioninsequencesofmusicalsoundsis5%–8%(Drake&Botte,1993;cf.Rivenezetal.,2003).Wouldthethresholdbehigherifspeechsoundswereused?

Page 45: University Press Scholarship Online Oxford Scholarship ...

Rhythm

Page 45 of 79

PRINTED FROM OXFORD SCHOLARSHIP ONLINE (www.oxfordscholarship.com). (c) Copyright Oxford University Press, 2015.All Rights Reserved. Under the terms of the l icence agreement, an individual user may print out a PDF of a single chapter of amonograph in OSO for personal use (for details see http://www.oxfordscholarship.com/page/privacy-policy). Subscriber: UC - SantaCruz; date: 02 May 2015

(p.145) TheRoleofRhythmicPredictabilityinSpeechPerceptionAnumberofresearchershavearguedthattheabilitytopredictthelocationofstressedsyllablesinEnglishisperceptuallybeneficial(e.g.,Martin,1972;Shieldsetal.,1974;Cutler&Foss,1977).Thereasoningbehindthisideaisbasedoncertainassumptions:Stressedsyllablescarryimportantsemanticinformation,andalistener’sattentionislimited,sothatitisusefultoexpendattentionalresourcesonthosepointsintimewherestressesoccur.Thustheabilitytoanticipatestresslocationcanhelpguideattentioninanefficientmanner.Thisideasuggestsapointofcontactbetweenrhythmperceptioninspeechandmusic,becausetherearetheorieslinkingrhythmandattentioninmusicpsychology(Jones,1976;Large&Jones,1999;Barnes&Jones,2000).Inordertodetermineifthisisreallyameaningfulparallel,however,twoquestionsmustbeanswered.First,isthereevidencethatrhythmicpredictabilityplaysanimportantroleinspeechperception?Second,arethemechanismsforrhythmicpredictionsimilarinspeechandmusic?

Thebestevidenceforaroleofrhythmicpredictabilityinspeechperceptioncomesfromstudiesusingphoneme-monitoringtasks.Intheseexperiments,listenersaretoldtolistentoonesentenceatatimeandpressabuttonwhentheyhearatargetphoneme(suchas/d/).CutlerandDarwin(1981)conductedastudyinwhichsentenceswererecordedthathadhigh,low,orneutralemphasisonagiventargetword.Forexample,sentences3.14aandbbelowwereusedtorecordhighversuslowemphasisontheword“dirt”(inthesentencesbelow,thewordbearingthemainemphasisofthesentenceisitalicized):

(3.14a)Shemanagedtoremovethedirtfromtherug,butnotthegrassstains.

(3.14b)Shemanagedtoremovethedirtfromtherug,butnotfromtheirclothes.

CutlerandDarwinthensplicedtheneutralversionofthetargetwordintohighandlowemphasissentences,sothatthetargetphoneme/d/(andtherestofthewordthatbeganwiththisphoneme)wereacousticallyidenticalinthetwocases.Afasterreactiontimetothetargetphonemeinhigh-emphasissentenceswouldindicatethatthepredictionofstresswasinfluencingspeechprocessing.Thisispreciselywhatwasfound:Listenerswerereliablyfasterindetectingthetargetphonemeinhigh-stresssentences.Ofparticularinterestisthatthisdifferencepersistedevenwhenfundamentalfrequencyvariationwasremovedfromthetwosentencetypes,suggestingthatpatternsofdurationandamplitudeweresufficienttopredicttheupcomingstress.

CutlerandDarwin’sstudyfocusedontargetwordsthateitherdidordidnotbearthemaincontrastivestressoftheentiresentence.Thatis,theywerenotstudyingtheperceptionofjustanystressedsyllable,butofaparticularlysalientstressedsyllableinasentence.PittandSamuel(1990)conductedastudyinwhichthecontextmanipulationwasnotsoextreme:Theyusedsentencesthat(p.146) predictedstressornonstressatatargetpointduetorhythmicandsyntacticfactors,forexample,thefirstsyllableoftheword“permit”in:

(3.15a)Theguardaskedthevisitorifshehadapermittoenterthebuilding.

Page 46: University Press Scholarship Online Oxford Scholarship ...

Rhythm

Page 46 of 79

PRINTED FROM OXFORD SCHOLARSHIP ONLINE (www.oxfordscholarship.com). (c) Copyright Oxford University Press, 2015.All Rights Reserved. Under the terms of the l icence agreement, an individual user may print out a PDF of a single chapter of amonograph in OSO for personal use (for details see http://www.oxfordscholarship.com/page/privacy-policy). Subscriber: UC - SantaCruz; date: 02 May 2015

(3.15b)Thewaiterdecidedhecouldnotpermitanyoneelseintherestaurant.

Insentence3.15a,thecontextleadsonetoexpectstressedsyllableatthetargetlocation,bothbecausethesyntaxofthesentencepredictsanoun(awordcategorythattendstostartwithastressedsyllableinEnglish)andfortherhythmicreasonthatthepriorstressinthesentenceisquitefaraway(thefirstsyllableof“visitor”).Insentence3.15b,thecontextleadsonenottopredictastressedsyllable,bothbecausethesyntaxpredictsaverb(awordcategorythattendstostartwithaweaksyllableinEnglish),andbecausethepriorstressisquitenearby(on“could”or“not”).

LikeCutlerandDarwin,PittandSamuelusedasplicingtechniquetoensurethatthephysicaltargetwordwasthesameinthetwocontexts,andaskedlistenerstorespondwhentheyheardatargetphoneme(e.g.,/p/intheaboveexample).UnlikeCutlerandDarwin,however,theyfoundnosignificantdifferenceinreactiontimetothetargetphonemeasfunctionoftheprecedingcontext.Thusitappearsthatalthoughrhythmmayhelplistenerspredictsentencelevelemphasis,itdoesnotplayastrongroleinpredictinglexicalstress,evenwhenreinforcedbysyntax.Thiscastssomedoubtontheideathatrhythmplaysanimportantroleinguidingattentiontothemajorityofstressedsyllablesinspokensentences.Clearly,moreworkisneededtodeterminetowhatextentstressispredictableundernormalcircumstances.

Evenifasignificantroleforrhythmicpredictioninspeechisdemonstrated,however,itisquitepossiblethatthemechanismsthatunderlierhythmicpredictioninspeechandmusicarequitedifferent.Inmusic,rhythmicpredictabilityreflectstheperiodicstructureoftemporalintervals.Inspeech,thebasisforrhythmicpredictability(e.g.,predictingwhenastresswilloccur)isunlikelytoinvolveperiodictimeintervals,becausethereisnoevidencethatsuchintervalsexistinnormalspeech.AfirststepinstudyingthebasisofrhythmicpredictioninspeechwouldbetostudythestimuliusedbyCutlerandDarwin(1981),especiallythosestimuliinwhichfundamentalfrequencyvariationwasremoved.Whattemporaland/oramplitudepatternshelpedguidelisteners’expectationsinthatstudy?

Thusatthecurrenttime,thehypothesisthatrhythmicpredictabilityinspeechconfersanadvantagebyguidingattentiontosemanticallyimportantpartsofutterancesisnotwellsupportedbyempiricalevidence.Inmusic,itisclearthatrhythmicpredictabilityhasanadaptivevalue:itallowstheformationofatemporalexpectancyschemethatplaysanimportantroleinmusicalperception(e.g.,beatperception),anditguidesthecoordinationofensembleperformanceandthesynchronizationofmovementsindance.Becausespeechdoesnothavearegularbeat,whatfunctionalrolewouldrhythmicpredictabilityplay?OneideasuggestedbyLehiste(1977)isthatitplaysaroleinsignalingphrase(p.147) boundariesinspeech.Specifically,shesuggestedthatonemethodspeakershavefordisambiguatingsyntacticallyambiguoussentencesisbysignalingastructuralboundaryvialengtheningofaninterstressinterval(ISI).Forexample,shestudiedspeakers’productionsofsentencessuchas“Theoldmenandwomenstayedathome,”whichissyntacticallyambiguous(eitherjustthemenwereoldorboththemenandwomenwereold).Shefoundthatwhenspeakerssaidthissentenceinsuchawaytomake

Page 47: University Press Scholarship Online Oxford Scholarship ...

Rhythm

Page 47 of 79

PRINTED FROM OXFORD SCHOLARSHIP ONLINE (www.oxfordscholarship.com). (c) Copyright Oxford University Press, 2015.All Rights Reserved. Under the terms of the l icence agreement, an individual user may print out a PDF of a single chapter of amonograph in OSO for personal use (for details see http://www.oxfordscholarship.com/page/privacy-policy). Subscriber: UC - SantaCruz; date: 02 May 2015

oneortheotherinterpretationclear,thesequence“menandwomen”wasverydifferentinduration,beingsubstantiallylongerwhenasyntacticboundarywasintendedbetween“men”and“women.”Furthermore,sheconductedafollow-upstudyinwhichthesamesentenceswereresynthesizedusingamonotone,andthedurationofthecriticalISIwasmanipulatedbyuniformlyexpandingthedurationofthephonemeswithinit,sothattherelativedurationsofsegmentsremainedthesame.ShefoundthatlistenerswereabletoperceivetheintendedmeaningsolelyonthebasisofthelengthofthecriticalISI,suggestingthatISIdurationcansignalaphraseboundary.

AsubsequentstudybyScott(1982)setouttotestLehiste’shypothesisagainstthemoreconventionalnotionthatphraseboundariesaresignaledbyphrase-finallengthening.ShefoundevidenceforaweakversionofLehiste’shypothesis,inthatthereappearedtobesomecasesinwhichlistenersusedISIpatterns,butothersinwhichtheyreliedontraditionalphrase-finallengthening.Nevertheless,theevidencewassuggestiveenoughforthislineofresearchtomeritfurtherstudy.Akeyconceptualpoint,however,isthatevidencethatISIdurationplaysaroleincreatingperceivedboundariesinspeechisnotequivalenttoevidenceforisochrony.TheexpectationforhowlonganISIshouldbeneednotbebasedonanexpectationforisochrony,butcouldbebasedonexpectationsforhowlongagivenISIshouldbegiventhenumber(andtype)ofsyllableswithinitandthecurrentspeechrate(cf.Campbell,1993).Accordingtothisview,aprosodicbreakismorelikelytobeheardwhenanISIissignificantlylongerthanexpected,andrhythmicpredictabilityissimplyimplicitknowledgeofthestatisticalrelationbetweenISIdurationandthenumberandtypeofsyllablesinanISI.Thiswouldallowafunctionalroleforrhythmicpredictabilitywithoutanyrecoursetonotionsofisochrony.

TheRoleofRhythminSegmentingConnectedSpeechToanativelistener,spokensentencesconsistofasuccessionofdiscretewords,yetthisperceptionisanillusion.AspointedoutinChapter2,wordboundariesinlanguagedonotmapinanysimplewayontoacousticbreaksinthespeechsignal,andasanyonewhohaslistenedtosentencesinaforeignlanguagecanattest,itisfarfromobviouswherethewordboundariesinconnectedspeechare.Thisproblemisparticularlyrelevantforinfants,whoareconstantlyfacedwithmultiwordutterances(vandeWeijer,1999)andwhodonothavethe(p.148) benefitofanexistingvocabularytohelpthemidentifywhereonewordendsandthenextbegins.

Asubstantialbodyofresearchinpsycholinguisticsindicatesthattherhythmicpropertiesofalanguageassistalistenerinsegmentingspeech.WorkonEnglish,forexample,haspointedtoasegmentationstrategybasedonstress:Listenersexpectstrongsyllablestobeword-initial.ThislikelyreflectsthepredominanceofwordswithinitialstressintheEnglishlexicon(Cutler&Carter,1987),andmanifestsitselfinanumberofdifferentwaysinperception.Forexample,CutlerandButterfield(1992)showedthatwhenlistenersmissegmentspeech,theytendtoplacewordboundariesbeforestressedsyllables,aswhen“bylooseanalogy”ismisheardas“byLuceandAllergy.”Furthermore,whenEnglishspeakersareaskedtospotrealmonosyllabicwordsembeddedinlargerpolysyllabicnonsensewords,theyfinditeasierwhentherealworddoesnotstraddle

Page 48: University Press Scholarship Online Oxford Scholarship ...

Rhythm

Page 48 of 79

PRINTED FROM OXFORD SCHOLARSHIP ONLINE (www.oxfordscholarship.com). (c) Copyright Oxford University Press, 2015.All Rights Reserved. Under the terms of the l icence agreement, an individual user may print out a PDF of a single chapter of amonograph in OSO for personal use (for details see http://www.oxfordscholarship.com/page/privacy-policy). Subscriber: UC - SantaCruz; date: 02 May 2015

twostressedsyllables.“Mint,”forexample,iseasiertospotin“mintef”thanin“mintayf,”presumablybecauseinthelatterwordthestrongsecondsyllable“tayf”triggerssegmentation,thussplitting“mint”intotwoparts(Cutler&Norris,1988).Cutler(1990)hasdubbedthestrategyofpositingawordonsetateachstrongsyllablethe“metricalsegmentationstrategy.”

Researchonsegmentationinotherlanguageshasrevealedthatstress-basedsegmentationisbynomeansuniversal.FrenchandSpanishspeakers,forexample,favorsyllabicallybasedsegmentation(Mehleretal.,1981;Pallieretal.,1993),whereasJapanesespeakersfavormoraicsegmentation(Otakeetal.,1993).Thussegmentationreliesonunitsthatarephonologicallyimportantinthenativelanguage.Onestrikingfindingofthiscross-linguisticresearchisthatthenativelanguage’ssegmentationstrategiesareappliedevenwhenlisteningtoaforeignlanguage,showingthatsegmentationtendenciesarenotsimplyareactiontoaparticularspeechrhythm,butaperceptualhabitofalistener(seeCutler,2000,forareview).OnepossibilitysuggestedbyCutleristhatthishabitisaresidueofearlylanguagelearning,whenrhythmicsegmentationplayedanimportantroleinbootstrappinglexicalacquisition.

Therelevanceofthisresearchtocomparativestudiesoflanguageandmusicisthatitshowsthatexperiencewithalanguage’srhythmleavesapermanentinfluenceonalistenerintermsofsegmentingspeechpatterns,whetherornotthesepatternscomefromthenativelanguage.Fromthisobservationitisbutonesteptoaskifexperiencewiththenativelanguageinfluenceshowonesegmentsnon-linguisticrhythmicpatterns.Thisquestionistakenupinsection3.5.2below.

TheRoleofRhythminthePerceptionofNonnativeAccentsWhenapersonlistenstotheirnativelanguage,s/heusuallyhasakeensenseofwhetherornotitisbeingspokenwithanativeaccent.Recentresearchonspeechrhythmhastakenadvantageofthisfactbyhavinglistenersjudgethedegreeofperceived“foreignaccentedness”inutterancesspokenbynonnativespeakers.(p.149) Empiricalrhythmicmeasurementsarethentakenofspeechofthedifferentnon-nativespeakers.Byexaminingthecorrelationbetweenperceiveddegreeofforeignaccentandthequantitativerhythmicmeasures,researchershopetoidentifytheperceptualcueslistenersuseingaugingspeechrhythmpatterns.

Usingthisapproach,WhiteandMattys(2007)examinedSpanishspeakersofEnglishandfoundthatthegreatertheirvoweldurationvariabilitywithinsentences,themorenative-soundingtheywereratedbyEnglishspeakers.Thisprobablyreflectsvowelreduction:Spanishspeakerswholearntoreducevowelsinunstressedsyllables(acharacteristicofEnglish,butnotofSpanish;cf.section3.3.1,subsection“PhonologyandTypology”)arelikelytosoundmorelikenativespeakers.Aconsequenceofvowelreductionwithinsentencesisthatvoweldurationvariabilityincreases,becausesomevowelsbecomeveryshort.

Asnotedinsection3.3.1(subsection“DurationandTypology”),anotherempiricalmeasureofrhythminfluencedbyvowelreductionisthenPVI,whichmeasuresthe

Page 49: University Press Scholarship Online Oxford Scholarship ...

Rhythm

Page 49 of 79

PRINTED FROM OXFORD SCHOLARSHIP ONLINE (www.oxfordscholarship.com). (c) Copyright Oxford University Press, 2015.All Rights Reserved. Under the terms of the l icence agreement, an individual user may print out a PDF of a single chapter of amonograph in OSO for personal use (for details see http://www.oxfordscholarship.com/page/privacy-policy). Subscriber: UC - SantaCruz; date: 02 May 2015

degreeofdurationalcontrastbetweenadjacentvowelsinasentenceratherthanoveralldurationalvariability.WhiteandMattysfoundthatthevowelnPVIofSpanishspeakersofEnglishwaspositivelycorrelatedwithhownativetheysounded.Crucially,however,voweldurationvariabilitywasabetterpredictorofaccentjudgmentthanwasnPVI.Thissuggeststhatvoweldurationvariabilitymaybemoreperceptuallyrelevantforspeechrhythmthandurationalcontrastiveness.

Thisisaverypromisingapproach,becausedifferentrhythmicmeasurescanbepittedagainsteachothertoseewhichbestpredictsperceptualdata.However,thefindingstodatemustbeconsideredtentativebecauseofanuncontrolledvariable.Thisisvariabilityinthedegreetowhichnonnativespeakersaccuratelyproducethephonemesofthesecondlanguage(i.e.,theindividualvowelsandconsonants).Whenjudgingaspeaker’sdegreeofforeignaccent,listenersalmostcertainlybasetheirjudgmentsonsomecombinationofsegmentalandsuprasegmentalcues.Thisisaproblembecausesomenonnativespeakersmayproducenative-soundingprosodybutnonnativesoundingsegmentalmaterial,orviceversa.Tocompoundtheproblem,differentlistenersmayvaryintheextenttowhichtheyweightsegmentalversussuprasegmentalcuesinjudginghow“foreign”agivennonnativespeakersounds.Thustotrulyfocuslisteners’attentiononrhythm,segmentalcuesmustbemadeuniform.Resynthesistechniques,suchasthoseusedbyRamusandcolleagues(cf.section3.3.1,subsection“PerceptionandTypology”)mightprovideonewaytomakesentencesspokenbydifferentnonnativespeakersuniformintermsofphonemicmaterialwhilepreservingprosodicdifferences.

3.3.4FinalCommentsonSpeechRhythm:MovingbeyondIsochrony

Althoughthehistoryofspeechrhythmresearchistightlyboundupwithnotionsofperiodicity(e.g.,theisochronyofstressesorsyllables),theevidence(p.150) reviewedabovesuggeststhatthecaseforperiodicityinspeechisextremelyweak.Thusprogressinthestudyofspeechrhythmrequiresconceptuallydecoupling“rhythm”and“periodicity,”apointmadeintheintroductionofthischapter.Itisquiteclearthatspeechhasrhythminthesenseofsystematictemporal,accentual,andgroupingpatternsofsound,andlanguagescanbesimilarordifferentintermsofthesepatterns.However,therhythmsoflanguagearenotbasedontheperiodicoccurrenceofanylinguisticunit.Instead,thepatterningislargelytheby-productofphonologicalphenomena,suchasthestructureofsyllables,vowelreduction,thelocationoflexicalprominence,stressclashavoidance,andtheprosodicphrasingofsentences.Thesephenomenaleadtodifferencesinthewayutterancesareorganizedintime.

Thenotionthatrhythminlanguageisprimarilyconsequenceratherthanconstructstandsinsharpcontrasttorhythminmusic,inwhichpatternsoftimingandaccentareafocusofconsciousdesign.Anothersalientdifferencebetweenrhythminspeechandmusic,relatedtothelackofaperiodicframeworkforspeechrhythm,isthefactthatspeechrhythmconveysnosenseofmotiontoalistener(cf.section3.2.5).Dothesedifferencesmeanthatrhythminlanguageandmusiccannotbemeaningfullycompared?Absolutelynot.Asdemonstratedinsection3.5below,empiricalcomparisonsarenotonlypossible,theycanbequitefruitful.Theyhavehadnothingtodowithperiodicity,however.

Page 50: University Press Scholarship Online Oxford Scholarship ...

Rhythm

Page 50 of 79

PRINTED FROM OXFORD SCHOLARSHIP ONLINE (www.oxfordscholarship.com). (c) Copyright Oxford University Press, 2015.All Rights Reserved. Under the terms of the l icence agreement, an individual user may print out a PDF of a single chapter of amonograph in OSO for personal use (for details see http://www.oxfordscholarship.com/page/privacy-policy). Subscriber: UC - SantaCruz; date: 02 May 2015

Forthoseinterestedincross-domainstudiesofrhythm,itishearteningtonotethatthereisrenewedinterestinempiricalstudiesofrhythminspeechproductionandspeechperception(Ramusetal.,1999;Ramus&Mehler,1999;Lowetal.,2000;Grabe&Low,2002;Lee&Todd,2004,White&Mattys,2007),andthatthereismuchroomforfurtherwork.Forexample,thereisaneedformoreempiricaldataonlisteners’judgmentsofhownative-soundingaforeignspeakers’utterancesare,fromthestandpointofrhythm.Suchstudieswillneedtoemploycreativewaysofisolatingtherhythmofspeechfromotherphoneticdimensionsoflanguage,forexample,usingresynthesizedspeechinwhichphoneticcontentandpitchcontourscanbecompletelycontrolled(cf.Ramus&Mehler,1999).Thereisalsoaneedforstudiesthatmeasuretemporalpatterningatmultiplelinguisticlevelsandthatquantifyrelationsbetweenlevels.Itmaybethatimportantperceptualdimensionsofspeechrhythmarerelational,suchashavingahighdegreeofcontrastbetweenadjacentsyllabledurationswhilesimultaneouslyhavingalowdegreeofcontrastbetweenthedurationofinterstressintervals(somedatapertinenttothisideaaregivenattheendofthissection).Thisisanareainwhichcollaborationsbetweenlinguistsandmusicresearcherswouldbeespeciallyuseful.

Intheremainderofthissection,Iwouldliketoconsiderwhyperiodicityhasbeen(andcontinuestobe)suchanenduringconceptinspeechrhythmresearch.BelowIofferseveralreasonsforthishistoricalphenomenon.

Thesimplestreason,ofcourse,isthemistakennotionthatrhythmisperiodicity,orthatrhythmisaregularalternationbetweenstrongandweakbeats,(p.151) ratherthanthebroadernotionofrhythmassystematictemporal,accentual,andphrasalpatterningofsound,whetherornotthispatterningisperiodic.Indeed,oneneednotlookbeyondmusictoseethatadefinitionofrhythmasperiodicityorasstrong-weakbeatalternationisoverlysimplistic:Manywidespreadmusicalformslackoneand/ortheotherofthesefeaturesyetarerhythmicallyorganized(cf.section3.2).

Thesecondreasonthatthenotionofperiodicityhasenduredmaybetheideathatithasausefulfunctioninspeechperception,suchasmakingsalientinformationpredictableintime.Therearepsychologicaltheoriesofauditoryperceptionthatproposethatattentioncanbeallocatedmoreefficientlywheneventsaretemporallypredictable,basedontheideathatauditoryattentionemploysinternaloscillatoryprocessesthatsynchronizewithexternalrhythmicpatterns(e.g.,Jones,1976;Large&Jones,1999).Suchtheoriesprovidearationaleforthoseinterestedintheideathatperiodicityinspeechisperceptuallyadaptive.Alternatively,thoseinterestedinperiodicitymightclaimthatitisusefulbecauseitcreatesaframeworkwithinwhichdeviationsaremeaningful.ThisisthebasisofLehiste’sideathatlengtheningofinterstressintervalsinEnglishcanbeusedtomarkphraseboundaries(cf.section3.3.3,subsection“TheRoleofRhythmicPredictabilityinSpeechPerception”).Theprincipaldrawbackoftheseperception-basedargumentsforperiodicityisthattheevidenceforthemisveryweak.Althoughfurtherresearchisneeded,thecurrentevidencesuggeststhatperiodicitydoesnothaveanimportantroletoplayinnormalspeechperception.Thisshouldnotbesurprising:The

Page 51: University Press Scholarship Online Oxford Scholarship ...

Rhythm

Page 51 of 79

PRINTED FROM OXFORD SCHOLARSHIP ONLINE (www.oxfordscholarship.com). (c) Copyright Oxford University Press, 2015.All Rights Reserved. Under the terms of the l icence agreement, an individual user may print out a PDF of a single chapter of amonograph in OSO for personal use (for details see http://www.oxfordscholarship.com/page/privacy-policy). Subscriber: UC - SantaCruz; date: 02 May 2015

comprehensionofspeechshouldberobusttovariationinthetimingofsalientevents,becausesuchvariationscanoccurforanumberofreasons.Forexample,aspeakermaysuddenlyspeeduporslowdownforrhetoricalreasonswithinaconversation.Undersuchconditions,relyingonperiodicityforcomprehensionseemsamaladaptivestrategy.

Thethirdreasonforperiodicity’salluremaybethebeliefthatbecausevarioustemporalpatternsinhumanphysiology(e.g.,heartbeat,walking,chewing)exhibitperiodicstructure,speechisalsolikelytobeperiodic,perhapsevengovernedbyrhythmicpatterngenerators.However,theuseofrhythmicneuralcircuitsforspeechisnotparticularlyplausible.Theconstantuseofnovelutterancesinlanguagemeansthatarticulatorsmustbecoordinatedindifferentwayseachtimeanewsentenceisproduced.Furthermore,themaneuversthatproduceparticularspeechsoundsdependonthelocalcontextinwhichtheyoccur.Thusthemotorpatternsofspeechcannotbepredictedinadvancewithahighdegreeofprecision.Withoutstereotypedmovementpatterns,evolutionhasnogroundsforplacingthecontrolofspeechinarhythmicneuralcircuit.Ananalogyhereistomultifingeredtouch-typing,abehaviorinvolvingthesequencingofoverlappingmovementsofmultiplearticulators(thefingers).Althoughtouch-typingishighlytemporallyorganized,theresultingsequencesarenotbasedonperiodicmovements.

(p.152) SofarIhavefocusedonnegativereasonsforthepersistenceoftheconceptofperiodicityinspeech.Iwillnowbrieflyspeculateonthepositivereasonsforthepersistenceofthisconcept,inotherwords,whyperiodicityinspeechhasbeensuchanintuitivelyappealingnotiontospeechresearchers,particularlythosewhosenativelanguageisEnglish.(ItisnotablethattheideaofperiodicityinspeechwaspromulgatedbylinguistswhowereEnglishspeakers,andthatargumentsforstressisochronyinEnglishhavebeenpresentsinceatleastthe18thcentury;cf.Abercrombie,1967:171;Kassler,2005).First,itseemsthatEnglishspeakersfindtheinterstressinterval(ISI)tobeasalienttemporalunitinspeech.Forexample,inapreliminarystudy,Cummins(2002)askedEnglishlistenerstorepeatnonsensephrasessuchas“manningthemiddle”intimewithanexternalpacingcue,sothatthetwostressedsyllableswereperceptuallyalignedwithaperiodicallyrepeatingtwo-tonepattern(forexample,“man”wouldalignwithahightone,and“mid”withalowtone;cf.Cummins&Port,1998).ThisisequivalenttoaligningthestartandendoftheISIwithtwotones.CumminsalsotestedspeakersofItalianandSpanishbecausetheselanguageshavelexicalstress,permittingphrasestobeconstructedinamanneranalogoustotheEnglishphrases(suchas“BUScaalMOto”inSpanish,stressindicatedbycapitalization).CumminsobservedthatalthoughEnglishspeakerslearnedthetaskquicklyandperformedaccurately,SpanishandItalianspeakerstookmuchlonger,wereuncomfortablewiththetask,andproducedagreatdealofvariabilityintheirresults.CumminssuggeststhatthisdifferenceisduetothefactthattheISIisnotasalientperceptualunitforspeakersofItalianandSpanish,despitethefactthatthereislexicalstressintheselanguages.

ThisintriguingfindingraisesthequestionofwhyISIissalienttoEnglishlisteners.Doesitplaysomefunctionallinguisticrole?Asdiscussedinsection3.3.3(subsection“TheRoleof

Page 52: University Press Scholarship Online Oxford Scholarship ...

Rhythm

Page 52 of 79

PRINTED FROM OXFORD SCHOLARSHIP ONLINE (www.oxfordscholarship.com). (c) Copyright Oxford University Press, 2015.All Rights Reserved. Under the terms of the l icence agreement, an individual user may print out a PDF of a single chapter of amonograph in OSO for personal use (for details see http://www.oxfordscholarship.com/page/privacy-policy). Subscriber: UC - SantaCruz; date: 02 May 2015

RhythmicPredictabilityinSpeechPerception”),ISIdurationmayplayaroleinsignalinglinguisticboundariestoEnglishlisteners,evenifISIsarenotisochronous.CurrentlythereisnotenoughevidencetosayconfidentlywhatroletheISIplays,butletusassumeforamomentthatEnglishspeakersandlistenersaresensitivetoitasanentity.GiventheempiricalobservationsaboutthelargevariabilityinISIdurationinEnglish(e.g.,coefficientsofvariationaround33%;Dauer,1983),whywouldlistenerseverfeelthatISIswereisochronous?OneanswermayconcerntherelativedegreeofvariabilityinISIdurationscomparedtosyllabledurations.Assuggestedabove,theimpressionofisochronymaybedueinparttoalowerdegreeofcontrastbetweensuccessiveISIdurationsrelativetosuccessivesyllabledurations.Forexample,considerFigure3.12,whichshowsthesamesentenceasFigure3.8aandSoundExample3.5a(“thelastconcertgivenattheoperawasatremendoussuccess”).

Syllableboundariesaremarkedwithverticallines(asinFigure3.8),butnowstressedsyllables(indicatedinbyboldfaceabove)havebeenmarkedwithanasterisk.Theasteriskwasplacedatthevowelonsetofthestressedsyllable,inotherwords,nearitsperceptualattack(its“P-center”;Mortonetal.,1976;Pateletal.,1999).Inthissentence,thenPVIofsyllabledurationsis59.3,andthenPVIofISIsis28.4,makingtherationPVIISI/nPVIsyllequalto0.48.Thusinthisparticularcase,theamountofdurationalcontrastbetweenadjacentISIsisonlyabouthalfofthatbetweenadjacentsyllables.

(p.153)

Figure3.12 TheEnglishsentenceofFigure3.8a,withstressesmarkedbyasterisks(*).

Ihavecomputedsimilarratiosforeachofthe20sentencesofBritishEnglishfromRamus’sdatabase.24For15outof20sentences,thisratiowaslessthan1.Theoverallmeanratioacrossthe20sentenceswas.83(std=.45),andwassignificantlylessthan1(p〈.0001byaone-tailedt-test).AnevenstrongereffectwasobservedwhenonecomputestheratioofISIdurationvariabilitytosyllabledurationvariability(usingthecoefficientofvariation,i.e.,CVISI/CVsyll)Here17outof20sentenceshadavaluelessthan1,andthemeanwas.69(std=.25),againsignificantlylessthan1.Thusiftheearissensitiveto

Page 53: University Press Scholarship Online Oxford Scholarship ...

Rhythm

Page 53 of 79

PRINTED FROM OXFORD SCHOLARSHIP ONLINE (www.oxfordscholarship.com). (c) Copyright Oxford University Press, 2015.All Rights Reserved. Under the terms of the l icence agreement, an individual user may print out a PDF of a single chapter of amonograph in OSO for personal use (for details see http://www.oxfordscholarship.com/page/privacy-policy). Subscriber: UC - SantaCruz; date: 02 May 2015

temporalpatterningatthelevelsofbothsyllablesandstresses,thelowdurationalvariabilityofISIsrelativetothevariabilityofsyllabledurationsmightcontributetoasensethatstressesaretemporallyregular.Ofcourse,forthisexplanationtohaveanymerititmustbeshownthattheratioofISItosyllablevariabilitydifferentiatesstress-timedfromsyllable-timed(p.154) languages.LanguagessuchasItalianandSpanishwouldbegoodcandidatesfortestingthishypothesis,becausetheyaresyllable-timedlanguagesinwhichstresscanbereliablymarked.

Althoughthenotionofisochronyinspeechcontinuestobeguileresearchers,Isuspectthatitwillhavelittleornoroleinthemostfruitfulresearchonspeechrhythminthecomingyears.Isochronywasimportanttothebirthofspeechrhythmstudies,butitisaconceptwhoseusefulnessisexhausted.Itistimetomoveontoaricherviewofspeechrhythm.

3.4Interlude:RhythminPoetryandSongAsintherestofthisbook,thefocusofthischapterisoncomparingordinaryspeechtoinstrumentalmusic.However,nocomparisonofrhythminlanguageandmusiciscompletewithoutadiscussionofpoetryandsong.Intheseartforms,wordsarecarefullychosenandconsciouslypatternedforrhythmiceffect.Ofcourse,poetryandsongarebuttwoofnumerousvocalgenreswithorganizedrhythms.IntheUnitedStatescertainstylesofpreachinginAfricanAmericanchurchesarenotablefortheirrhythmicpatterning,atasteofwhichcanbeheardinMartinLutherKingJr.’sfamous“IHaveaDream”speech.Inothercultures,itispossibletoidentifynumerousgenresofspeechinwhichrhythmicdesignplaysarole(seeAgawu,1995,forafascinatingcasestudyoftherangeofrhythmicallyregulatedformsofspeechinanAfricansociety).Thefocushereisonpoetryandsong,however,becausethesehavereceivedthegreatestamountofempiricalresearchintermsofrhythm.

3.4.1RhythminPoetry

Thestudyofpoeticrhythmhasbeenthefocusofagooddealofresearchbyliteraryscholars(forintroductions,seeGross,1979;Fussell,1979;Hollander,2001).Inthistradition,poetic“meter”referstotheabstractpatterningschemewhichgovernsthetemporalstructureofapoem,whereas“rhythm”referstotheactualpatterningofdurationsandaccents.Forexample,agreatdealofEnglishverseiswritteniniambicpentameter,averseformconsistingof5iambicfeet,inwhichaniambisa(weak+strong)syllablepattern.Naturallytherearemanyexceptionstothispatternwithiniambicpentameterpoetry:Aparticularlycommononeisthesubstitutionofatrochaicfoot,or(strong+weak)syllablepatternattheonsetofaline.Thustherhythmofaparticularlinemayviolatetheoverallmeterofthepoem.

Literaryprosodistsarguethatlistenersinternalizetheregularitiesofmeterandperceivedeparturesfromthisschemeasvariationfromastablebackground(Richards,1979:69;Adams,1997:12).Thatis,meterisseenashaving(p.155) anintimaterelationshipwithexpectancy.Thisideaisrelatedtothenotionofmusicalmeterasanabstractmentalscheme,butdiffersfrommusicalmeterinanimportantway.Musicalmeterrefersto

Page 54: University Press Scholarship Online Oxford Scholarship ...

Rhythm

Page 54 of 79

PRINTED FROM OXFORD SCHOLARSHIP ONLINE (www.oxfordscholarship.com). (c) Copyright Oxford University Press, 2015.All Rights Reserved. Under the terms of the l icence agreement, an individual user may print out a PDF of a single chapter of amonograph in OSO for personal use (for details see http://www.oxfordscholarship.com/page/privacy-policy). Subscriber: UC - SantaCruz; date: 02 May 2015

temporalperiodicity,whereaspoeticmeterinvolvesconfigurationalperiodicity,inotherwords,thefocusisontherepetitionofsomebasicprosodicunitratherthanontemporalperiodicityperse.Forexample,iniambicpentameteritistheweak+strongconfigurationoftheiambicfootthatisthedesignfocus,nottheisochronyofstressedsyllables.InvariousformsofFrenchandChineseverse,thenumberofsyllablesperlineisstrictlyregulated,butthereisnofocusonmakingsyllablesperiodic(equalinduration).

Itisinterestingtonotethatdifferentlanguagestendtofavordifferentkindsofpoeticmeters.Forexample,Englishversehasoftentendedtowardpurelystress-basedformsinwhichregulationofthenumberofstressesperlineisthefocus,independentofthenumberofsyllables(e.g.,themeterofBeowulf,withfourstressesperline).Incontrast,Englishversebasedonregulatingthenumberofsyllablesperlinewithoutregardtostressisrare(Fussell,1979:62–75).ThislikelyreflectsthepowerfulroleofstressinordinaryEnglishspeechrhythm.Indeed,Fussellhasarguedthat“ametercustomaryinagivenlanguageiscustomaryjustbecauseit‘measures’themostcharacteristicqualityofthelanguage”(Fussell,1974:498).ThusstressplaysadominantroleinEnglishpoetry,butlittleroleinFrench,inwhichthenumberofsyllablesperlineisamorecommonconcern.Japanese,inturn,oftenregulatesthenumberofmoraeperline,asinthe5–7–5morastructureofthehaiku.

LerdahlandHalle(1991)andLerdahl(2003)havesoughttounifythetheoreticaltreatmentofrhythminpoetryandmusic,usingsharedconceptssuchashierarchicalgroupingstructureandmetricalgrids.Ourfocushere,however,isonempiricalresearch.Overthepastfewdecades,poeticrhythmhasattractedtheinterestofanumberofspeechscientists,whohavemadequantitativemeasurementsofthetemporalpatternsofpoetry.Forexample,thephoneticianGunnarFantandcolleagueshavestudiedtheacousticsofiambicversustrochaiclinesofverseinSwedish(1991b).Theresearchersfoundthatiniambicfeet,theweaksyllableisabout50%aslongasthefollowingstrongsyllable,whereasintrochaicfeet,theweaksyllableisabout80%ofthedurationoftheprecedingstrongsyllable(cf.Nordetal.,1990).Thisdifferenceislikelyduetopreboundarylengthening,whichactstoincreasethedurationofthefinalsyllableineachfoot(i.e.,thestrongsyllableinaniambandtheweaksyllableinatrochee).Thusiambicandtrochaicfeetarenotsimplymirrorimagesofeachotherintermsoftheirtemporalprofiles:Iambicfeetaremuchmoretemporallyasymmetric.

Theseobservationsmayberelevanttothestudyoftheaestheticeffectofthetwokindsoffeetinpoeticlines.Forexample,Adams(1997:55–57)notesthattrochaicmetersareoftenassociatedwithaweandthesuspensionofreality,as(p.156) inBlake’spoem,“TheTyger,”inwhichtrochaicpatternsdominatethefirstthreelinesofthefirststanza:

(3.16)

Tyger!Tyger!burningbright

Intheforestsofthenight

Page 55: University Press Scholarship Online Oxford Scholarship ...

Rhythm

Page 55 of 79

PRINTED FROM OXFORD SCHOLARSHIP ONLINE (www.oxfordscholarship.com). (c) Copyright Oxford University Press, 2015.All Rights Reserved. Under the terms of the l icence agreement, an individual user may print out a PDF of a single chapter of amonograph in OSO for personal use (for details see http://www.oxfordscholarship.com/page/privacy-policy). Subscriber: UC - SantaCruz; date: 02 May 2015

Whatimmortalhandoreye

Couldframethyfearfulsymmetry?

Thisaestheticpropertyoftrochaicmetermaybepartlyduetoitsmoreuniformprofileofsyllabicdurations,whichgoesagainstthegrainofnormalEnglishspeechrhythmandthusgivestheresultingspeechanincantatoryfeel.

AnotherprominentphoneticianwhohaslongconductedresearchonpoeticrhythmisIlseLehiste(1991).Inonesetofstudies,Lehisteexaminedtherelationshipbetweenthetimingoffeetandofthelinesinwhichfeetareembedded.Shefoundthatthetemporalvariabilityoflinesislowerthanonewouldpredictbasedonthevariabilityoffeetduration,suggestingthatspeakersmaketemporalcompensationsbetweenfeetinordertokeeplineswithinacertainduration.Thatis,linesactasaunitoftemporalprogrammingintherecitationofpoetry(Lehiste,1990).RossandLehiste(1998,2001)havealsoexaminedtheinterplayoflinguisticandpoeticrhythminframingthetemporalpatternsofEstonianverseandfolksongs.

3.4.2RhythminSong

Forlanguageswithclearlydefinedstress,suchasEnglish,eachphraseorsentencecomeswithadistinctpatternofstrongerandweakersyllables.Whenwordsintheselanguagesaresettometricalmusic,arelationshipisestablishedbetweenthesyllabicaccentpatternsandmusicalmetricalaccentpatterns.Sensitivitytotheserelationshipsispartoftheskillofwritingmusicwithwords,andempiricalresearchsuggeststhatcomposersexploitthisrelationshipforartisticends.

PalmerandKelly(1992)studiedvocallinesinthemesfromGilbertandSullivan’s14operettas,focusingoncompoundnouns(likethesingleword“blackbird”)andadjective-nounpairs(likethetwowordphrase“blackbird”).InEnglish,compoundnounsreceivestressonthefirstsyllable,whereasadjective-nounpairsreceivestressonthesecondsyllable.Theystudiedhowsuchwordswerealignedwiththemetricalstructureofthemusic,andfoundthatthestressedsyllabletendedtoalignwithametricallystrongbeatinthemusic.GiventhecomplextextsofGilbertandSullivan’ssongs,thisstrategyofalignmentmaycontributeasenseofprecisionandbalancetothelyricsoftheseoperettas.

Temperley(1999)lookedatadifferentgenreofvocalmusic,namelyrocksongs.IncontrasttoPalmerandKelly,hefoundthatverbalstressfrequentlyanticipatedmetricalaccentinrocksongsbyafractionofabeat,asinthe(p.157) Beatles’“HereComestheSun”(Figure3.13).Thissystematicanticipationcontributesasenseofsyncopationandrhythmicenergytothesong,andprovidesanexampleofhowthesystematicmisalignmentofverbalandmusicalstressaddsdynamicenergytomusic.

Therelationshipbetweenrhythminspeechandsongisafertileareathatmeritsmuchmoreempiricalinvestigationthanithasreceivedtodate.Intheremainderofthissection,Ioutlinethreedirectionsthatthisresearchcouldtake.Thefirstpertainstocultural

Page 56: University Press Scholarship Online Oxford Scholarship ...

Rhythm

Page 56 of 79

PRINTED FROM OXFORD SCHOLARSHIP ONLINE (www.oxfordscholarship.com). (c) Copyright Oxford University Press, 2015.All Rights Reserved. Under the terms of the l icence agreement, an individual user may print out a PDF of a single chapter of amonograph in OSO for personal use (for details see http://www.oxfordscholarship.com/page/privacy-policy). Subscriber: UC - SantaCruz; date: 02 May 2015

differencesintheprevalenceofcertaintypesofmusicalrhythms.Forexample,Yamomoto(1996)notesthatchildren’ssongsbasedontriplerhythms(e.g.,6/8timesignature)arerareinJapanbutcommoninBritain,andsuggeststhatthismightbeduetodifferencesinEnglishversusJapanesespeechrhythm.IfYamomotoiscorrect,onewouldpredictthatJapanese-versusEnglish-speakingchildrenwoulddifferinhoweasilytheycanlearnsongsinthesemeters.AnotherlanguagewithwhichonecouldtestasimilarideaisGreek.Recallfromsection3.3.2(subsection“QuestioningthePrincipleofRhythmicAlternationinSpeech”)thatArvaniti(1994)showedthatGreek,alanguageoftheBalkanregion,toleratesamoreirregularalternationbetweenstressedandunstressedsyllablesthandoesEnglish.Alsorecallfromsection3.2thattheBalkanregionfeaturesmusicwithirregularlyspacedbeats.WouldGreek-speakingchildrenfinditeasiertolearntheirregularmetersofBalkansongsthanEnglish-speakingchildren(cf.Hannon&Trehub,2005)?Inthestudiesoutlinedabove,itwouldofcoursebeessentialthatthetwogroupsofchildrenbematchedforpriormusicalexposuretodifferentmusicalmeters.ItmaythusbebesttoworkwithimmigrantswhospeakthenativelanguageathomebutwhosechildrenareexposedtoWesternmusic.Insuchacase,iflearningexperimentsrevealthepredictedculturaldifferences,thiswouldsupporttheinterestinghypothesisthataculture’sspeechrhythmpredisposesittowardorawayfromcertainmusicalrhythms.

Aseconddirectionforresearchinthisareaistoexamineverballyimprovisedmusicthatisaccompaniedbyarhythmicmusicalcontext,suchascontemporaryrapmusic.Ifthevocalandmusicallinescanberecordedondifferentaudiotracks,andpointsofverbalandmusicalstresscanbeindependentlyidentified,thenonecouldstudytemporalrelationsbetweenverbalandmusicalaccentpointsasapieceunfoldsintime.Itwouldbeparticularlyinterestingtostudytheserelationsinnoviceversusexpertrapmusicians,toseeifpartofbeinganexpertinthisgenreisgreaterflexibilityand/orprecisioninthemannerinwhichthealignmentofthetwotypesofaccentsishandled.Inastudysuchasthis,identifyingprecisepointsintimeforverbalandmusicalaccentwillbeessential,andtheissueoftheperceptualattacktimeofsyllablesandofmusicaltones(“P-centers”)comestothefore(Mortonetal.,1976;Gordon,1987;Pateletal.,1999).

Figure3.13 AmusicalmetricalgridforaportionoftheBeatles’“HereComestheSun.”Thelyricsarealignedbelowthegrid,andlinguisticallystressedsyllablesareindicatedinboldface:Notehowmostsuchsyllablesslightlyprecedestrongmetricalpositionsinthemusic.FromTemperley,1999.

(p.158) AfinalpossiblelineofresearchissuggestedbyacorrespondencebetweenRichardStraussandRomainRollandin1905aboutmusicaltextsetting(Myers,1968).25Straussemphasizestheclear-cutrelationshipbetweensyllabicaccentinspeechandmetricalaccentinmusic:“InGerman‘she’onthestrongbeatofabarisabsolutely

Page 57: University Press Scholarship Online Oxford Scholarship ...

Rhythm

Page 57 of 79

PRINTED FROM OXFORD SCHOLARSHIP ONLINE (www.oxfordscholarship.com). (c) Copyright Oxford University Press, 2015.All Rights Reserved. Under the terms of the l icence agreement, an individual user may print out a PDF of a single chapter of amonograph in OSO for personal use (for details see http://www.oxfordscholarship.com/page/privacy-policy). Subscriber: UC - SantaCruz; date: 02 May 2015

impossible.Forexample,inabarof4/4,thefirstandthirdbeatalwayshaveanecessarystresswhichcanonlybemadeontheradical[stressed]syllableofeachword.”HealsoexpresseshisfrustrationovervariabilityinthealignmentofwordstressandmusicalstressinFrenchopera:“Yesterday,IagainreadsomeofDebussy’sPélleasetMélisande,andIamoncemoreveryuncertainabouttheprincipleofthedeclamationofFrenchwhensung.Thusonpage113,Ifound:‘Cheveúx,chéveux,décheveux.’Forheaven’ssake,Iaskyou,ofthesethreewaystherecanallthesameonlybeonewhichisright.”

RollandrepliesbyemphasizingthemutabilityandsubtletyofFrenchwordaccent:

Thenaturalvalueof“cheveux”ischevéux.Butamaninlovewill,whensayingthisword,putquiteaspecialstressonit:“teschéveux.”…Yousee,thegreatdifficultywithourlanguageisthatforaverylargenumberofwords,accentuationisvariable,—neverarbitrary,butinaccordancewithlogicalorpsychologicalreasons.Whenyousaytome:…”Ofthese3(cheveux)onlyonecanberight,whatyousayisdoubtlesstrueofGerman,butnotforFrench.

WhatthiscorrespondencesuggestsisthatGermantextsettingismorerigidinitsalignmentofverbalandmusicalaccent,whereasFrenchismorepermissiveintermsofaccentalignmentbetweentextandmusic.Indeed,DellandHalle(inpress;cf.Dell,1989)reportthatFrenchtext-settingisquitetolerantofmismatchesbetweenverbalandmusicalaccent,exceptattheendsoflines,wherealignmenttendstobeenforced.Theycontrastthishighdegreeof“mismatchtolerance”inFrenchsongstoamuchlowerdegreefoundinEnglishsongs.ThecorrespondencebetweenStraussandRolland,andtheworkofDellandHalle,suggestthatlanguageshavesalientdifferencesinthewaytheyalignmusicandtextintermsofrhythmicproperties,thoughquantitativeworkis(p.159)neededtoconfirmthis.Thesewritingsalsoleadtoideasforcross-culturalperceptualstudiestestingsensitivitytoaccentmismatchesinsongs.Specifically,listenerscouldbepresentedwithdifferentversionsofasongthathavetextandtunealignedindifferentways,withoneversionhavingmanymoreaccentmismatches.Listenerscouldthenbeaskedtojudgeinwhichversionthewordsandmusicgobesttogether.OnemightpredictthatGermanlistenersjudgingGermansongs(orEnglishlistenersjudgingEnglishsongs)wouldbemoreselectiveintermsofthepairingsthatsoundacceptablethanFrenchlistenersjudgingFrenchsongs.

3.5NonperiodicAspectsofRhythmasaKeyLinkOnemajorthemeofthischapteristhatlanguageshaverhythm(systematictemporal,accentual,andgroupingpatterns),butthatthisrhythmdoesnotinvolvetheperiodicrecurrenceofstresses,syllables,oranyotherlinguisticunit.Initiallyitmayseemthat“givinguponperiodicityinspeech”wouldmeanthatthereislittlebasisforcomparingrhythminmusicandlanguage.Infact,theoppositeistrue.Byabandoningafixationonperiodicityoneisfreedtothinkmorebroadlyaboutspeechrhythmanditsrelationshiptomusicalrhythm.Asweshallseebelow,afocusonnonperiodicaspectsoflinguisticrhythmisprovingfruitfulintermsofcomparinglanguageandmusicatstructuralandneurallevels.

Page 58: University Press Scholarship Online Oxford Scholarship ...

Rhythm

Page 58 of 79

PRINTED FROM OXFORD SCHOLARSHIP ONLINE (www.oxfordscholarship.com). (c) Copyright Oxford University Press, 2015.All Rights Reserved. Under the terms of the l icence agreement, an individual user may print out a PDF of a single chapter of amonograph in OSO for personal use (for details see http://www.oxfordscholarship.com/page/privacy-policy). Subscriber: UC - SantaCruz; date: 02 May 2015

3.5.1RelationsbetweenMusicalStructureandLinguisticRhythm

Thenotionthatanation’sinstrumentalmusicreflectstheprosodyofitslanguagehaslongintriguedmusicscholars,especiallythoseinterestedin“nationalcharacter”inmusic.GeraldAbrahamexploredthisideaatlength(1974,Ch.4),notingasoneexampleanobservationofRalphKirkpatrickonFrenchkeyboard-music:“BothCouperinandRameau,likeFauréandDebussy,arethoroughlyconditionedbythenuancesandinflectionsofspokenFrench.OnnoWesternmusichastheinfluenceoflanguagebeenstronger”(p.83).Inamoresuccinctexpressionofasimilarsentiment,Glinka(inTheaterArts,June1958)wrote:“Anationcreatesmusic,thecomposeronlyarrangesit”(citedinGiddings,1984:91).

Untilveryrecently,evidenceforthisideahasbeenlargelyanecdotal.Forexample,Garfias(1987)hasnotedthatinHungarianeachwordstartswithastressedsyllable,andthatHungarianmusicalmelodiestypicallystartonstrongbeats(i.e.,anacrusis,orupbeat,israre).Althoughthisisaninterestingobservation,itispossiblethatthisisduetothefactthatmanysuchmelodiescomefromfolksongs.Inthiscase,thelinguisticinfluenceonmusicalrhythmwouldbemediated(p.160) bytext.Amoreinterestingissue,impliedbyKirkpatrick,iswhetherlinguisticrhythminfluencestherhythmofinstrumentalmusic,inotherwords,musicthatisnotvocallyconceived.

OneapproachtothisquestionwassuggestedbyWenk(1987).Heproposedthatcultureswithrhythmicallydistinctlanguagesshouldbeexaminedtoseeifdifferencesinmusicalrhythmreflectdifferencesinspeechrhythm.WenkfocusedonEnglishandFrench,prototypicalexamplesofastress-timedversusasyllable-timedlanguage.WenkandWioland(1982)hadpreviouslyarguedthatasalientrhythmicdifferencebetweenthetwolanguageswasthatEnglishgroupedsyllablesintounitsbeginningwithastressedsyllable,whereasFrenchgroupedsyllablesintounitsendingwithastressedsyllable,asin:

WenkandWiolandfurtherarguedthatthestressattheendsofrhythmicgroupsinFrenchwasmarkedprimarilybydurationallengthening.Basedonthisidea,Wenk(1987)predictedthatphrase-finallengtheningwouldbemorecommoninFrenchversusEnglishinstrumentalmusic.HetestedthisideabyhavingaprofessionalmusicianmarkphraseboundariesinEnglishversusFrenchclassicalmusic.Thenumberofphrasesinwhichthefinalnotewasthelongestnoteinthephrasewasthentalliedforbothcultures.WenkfoundthatitwasindeedthecasethatmoresuchphrasesoccurredinFrenchthaninEnglishmusic.

Wenk’sstudywaspioneeringinitsempiricalorientation,butitalsohadlimitationsthatmakeitdifficulttoacceptthesefindingsasafirmanswertothequestionofinterest.Only

Page 59: University Press Scholarship Online Oxford Scholarship ...

Rhythm

Page 59 of 79

PRINTED FROM OXFORD SCHOLARSHIP ONLINE (www.oxfordscholarship.com). (c) Copyright Oxford University Press, 2015.All Rights Reserved. Under the terms of the l icence agreement, an individual user may print out a PDF of a single chapter of amonograph in OSO for personal use (for details see http://www.oxfordscholarship.com/page/privacy-policy). Subscriber: UC - SantaCruz; date: 02 May 2015

onecomposerfromeachculturewasexamined(FrancisPoulencandBenjaminBritten),andfromtheoeuvreofeachcomposer,onlyonemovementfromonepiecewasselected.Furthermore,nocomparableempiricaldataforlanguagerhythmwerecollected(e.g.,thedegreeofphrase-finallengtheninginEnglishvs.Frenchspeech).

Despiteitslimitations,Wenk’sstudyoutlinedausefulapproach,namelytoidentifyempiricalrhythmicdifferencesbetweentwolanguagesandthendetermineifthesedifferencesarereflectedinthemusicofthetwocultures.Pursuingthisideainarigorousfashionentailsthreerequirements.First,anempiricalmeasureofspeechrhythmisneededtoquantifyrhythmicdifferencesbetweenlanguages.Second,thissamemeasureshouldbeapplicabletomusicsothatlanguageandmusiccouldbecomparedinacommonframework.Third,boththelinguisticandmusicalsamplesneededtobebroadenoughtoinsurethatthefindingsarenotidiosyncratictoafewspeakersorcomposers.

(p.161) JosephDanieleandIconductedastudythatsetouttomeetthesecriteria(Patel&Daniele,2003a).LikeWenk,wefocusedonBritishEnglishandFrenchduetotheirdistinctspeechrhythmsandbecausetheyhavebeenthelocusofstrongintuitionsaboutlinksbetweenprosodyandinstrumentalmusic(e.g.,Hall,1953;Abraham,1974;Wenk1987).Ourworkwasinspiredbyrecentphoneticresearchonempiricalcorrelatesofstress-timedversussyllable-timedspeechrhythm(cf.section3.3.1,subsection“DurationandTypology”).Inparticular,theworkofLow,Grabe,andNolan(2000)attractedourattentionbecauseitfocusedonsomethingthatcouldbemeasuredinbothspeechandmusic,namelythedurationalcontrastbetweensuccessiveelementsinasequence.Theirmeasure,calledthenormalizedpairwisevariabilityindex,ornPVI,hadbeenappliedtovowelsinsentencesfromstress-timedandsyllable-timedlanguages,andhadbeenshowntobehigherinstress-timedlanguages,likelyduetothegreaterdegreeofvowelreductionintheselanguages(Grabe&Low,1990;Ramus,2002a;Lee&Todd,2004;seetheabove-mentionedsubsectionof3.3.1forbackgroundonthenPVI).

Twoaspectsofthismeasuremadeitappealingforusewithmusic.First,thenPVIaispurelyrelativemeasureofcontrast.Thatis,thedurationaldifferencebetweeneachpairofintervalsismeasuredrelativetotheaveragedurationofthepair.Thisnormalization,whichwasoriginallyintroducedtocontrolforfluctuationsinspeechrate,makesthenPVIadimensionlessquantitythatcanbeappliedtobothlanguageandmusic.(Forexample,nPVIcanbecomputedfromspeechdurationsmeasuredinsecondsandfrommusicaldurationsmeasuredinfractionsofabeat.)Second,thenPVIhasbeenappliedtovowels.Vowelsformthecoreofsyllables,whichcaninturnbecomparedtomusicaltones(i.e.,insettingwordstomusicitisquitecommonforeachnotetobeassignedtoonesyllable).26Ourstrategy,then,wastoapplythenPVItotonesequencesfromBritishandFrenchinstrumentalmusic,todetermineifdifferencesemergedthatreflectedtherhythmicdifferencesbetweenBritishEnglishandFrenchspeech.

Figure3.14showsthenPVItoBritishEnglishversuscontinentalFrenchspeech,basedonmeasurementsofvoweldurationsinsentencesutteredbynativespeakersofeachlanguage.(Thesentencesareshort,news-likeutterancesfromthecorpusofNazzietal.,1998.)27

Page 60: University Press Scholarship Online Oxford Scholarship ...

Rhythm

Page 60 of 79

PRINTED FROM OXFORD SCHOLARSHIP ONLINE (www.oxfordscholarship.com). (c) Copyright Oxford University Press, 2015.All Rights Reserved. Under the terms of the l icence agreement, an individual user may print out a PDF of a single chapter of amonograph in OSO for personal use (for details see http://www.oxfordscholarship.com/page/privacy-policy). Subscriber: UC - SantaCruz; date: 02 May 2015

(p.162)

Figure3.14 ThenPVIofBritishEnglishandFrenchsentences.Errorbarsshow+/-1standarderror.DatafromPatel,Iversen,&Rosenberg,2006.

ThenPVIissignificantlyhigherforEnglishthanforFrenchspeech.Figure3.15givesanintuitionforwhythisisthecasebyillustratingthepatternofvoweldurationforoneEnglishandFrenchsentenceinthiscorpus(cf.SoundExamples3.8a,b).

Forexample,inthetoppanel,thefirsttwovalues(about120msand40ms)arethedurationsofthefirsttwovowelsinthesentence(i.e.,thevowelsin“Finding”),andsoon.NotehowsuccessivevowelstendtodiffermoreindurationfortheEnglishsentencethanfortheFrenchsentence.IntheEnglishsentence,somevowelsareveryshort(oftenduetovowelreduction),whereasothervowelsarequitelong(oftenduetostress).Thisleadstoagreatertendencyfordurationalcontrastbetweenneighboringvowels,whichisreflectedinthenPVIscore.

Asmentionedabove,anappealingaspectofthenPVIisthatitcanbeappliedtomusicinordertomeasurethedurationalcontrastbetweensuccessivenotes.Westernmusicnotationindicatestherelativedurationofnotesinanunambiguousfashion,asshowninFigure3.16.

Inthefigure,thefirstnoteofeachthemeisarbitrarilyassignedadurationof1,andthedurationsoftheremainingnotesareexpressedasamultipleorfractionofthisvalue.(AnynumericalcodingschemethatpreservesrelativedurationofnoteswouldyieldthesamenPVI,becauseitisanormalizedmeasure.)Inthisexample,thenPVIoftheDebussythemeislowerthanthatoftheElgartheme,eventhoughtherawvariabilityofnotedurationintheDebussythemeisgreaterthanthatintheElgartheme(asmeasuredbythecoefficientofvariation,inotherwords,thestandarddeviationdividedbythemean).ThisemphasizesthefactthatthenPVIindexesthedegreeofcontrastbetweensuccessiveelementsinasequence,nottheoverallvariabilityofthoseelements.

(p.163)

Page 61: University Press Scholarship Online Oxford Scholarship ...

Rhythm

Page 61 of 79

PRINTED FROM OXFORD SCHOLARSHIP ONLINE (www.oxfordscholarship.com). (c) Copyright Oxford University Press, 2015.All Rights Reserved. Under the terms of the l icence agreement, an individual user may print out a PDF of a single chapter of amonograph in OSO for personal use (for details see http://www.oxfordscholarship.com/page/privacy-policy). Subscriber: UC - SantaCruz; date: 02 May 2015

Figure3.15 VoweldurationsinanEnglishandaFrenchsentence.Notethegreaterdegreeofshort-longcontrastintheEnglishsentencebetweenadjacentvoweldurations.ThenPVIfortheEnglishsentenceis54.9,andfortheFrenchsentenceis30.0.

Oursourceofmusicalmaterialwasastandardreferenceworkinmusicology,ADictionaryofMusicalThemes,SecondEdition(Barlow&Morgen-stern,1983),whichfocusesontheinstrumentalmusicofWesternEuropeancomposers.Inchoosingcomposerstoincludeinourstudywewereguidedbytwofactors.First,thecomposershadtobefromarelativelyrecentmusicalerabecausemeasurementsofspeechprosodyarebasedoncontemporaryspeech,andlanguagesareknowntochangeovertimeintermsofsoundstructure.Second,thecomposershadtobenativespeakersofBritishEnglishorFrenchwholivedandworkedinEnglandorFrance.Usingtheseguidelines,weexaminedallEnglishandFrenchcomposersfromBarlowandMorgensternwhowereborninthe1800sanddiedinthe1900s,andwhohadatleastfivemusicalthemesinthedictionarythatwereeligibleforinclusioninthestudy(seePatel&Daniele,2003a,forinclusioncriteria,andfortherationaleofusingmusicnotationratherthanrecordedmusicfornPVIanalysis).Wechosecomposerswhospannedtheturnofthecenturybecausethiseraisnotedbymusicologistsasatimeof“musicalnationalism”inEurope.

(p.164)

Page 62: University Press Scholarship Online Oxford Scholarship ...

Rhythm

Page 62 of 79

PRINTED FROM OXFORD SCHOLARSHIP ONLINE (www.oxfordscholarship.com). (c) Copyright Oxford University Press, 2015.All Rights Reserved. Under the terms of the l icence agreement, an individual user may print out a PDF of a single chapter of amonograph in OSO for personal use (for details see http://www.oxfordscholarship.com/page/privacy-policy). Subscriber: UC - SantaCruz; date: 02 May 2015

Figure3.16 Twomusicalthemeswiththerelativedurationsofeachnotemarked.nPVIofD122=42.2,ofE72=57.1.ThemesarefromBarlow&Morgenstern,1983.FromPatel&Daniele,2003a.

Basedonourcriteria,16composerswereincludedinthestudy,includingEnglishcomposerssuchasElgar,Delius,andVaughanWilliams,andFrenchcomposerssuchasDebussy,Poulenc,andSaint-Saëns.About300musicalthemeswererepresented,andonemusicalnPVIvaluewascomputedforeachtheme.TheresultsofouranalysisofmusicalnPVIareshowninFigure3.17,alongwiththespeechnPVIvalues.Remarkably,thetwocultureshavesignificantlydifferentmusicalnPVIvalues,withthedifferencebeinginthesamedirectionasthelinguisticnPVIdifference(seePatel&Daniele,2003a,andPateletal.,2006,forfurtherdetails).

Thusthereisempiricalevidencethatspeechrhythmisreflectedinmusicalrhythm,atleastinturn-of-thecenturyclassicalmusicfromEnglandandFrance.Howisthisconnectionbetweenlanguageandmusicmediated?Somemusicologistshaveproposedthatnationalcharacterarisesfromcomposersadaptingfolkmelodiesintotheircompositions.Becausesuchmelodiesaretypicallyfromsongs,itmaybethattherhythmofwordsinfluencestherhythmofthesemelodies,thusgivingthemelodiesalanguage-likerhythmicpattern.However,webelievethatthismaynotbethebestexplanationforourfinding,becauseourstudyincludednumerouscomposerswhoarenotthoughttobestronglyinfluencedbyfolkmusic,suchasElgarandDebussy(Grout&Palisca,2000).Instead,wefeeltheremaybeamoredirectroutefromlanguagetomusic.Itisknownfromstudiesoflanguageacquisitionthattheperceptualsystemissensitivetotherhythmicpatternsoflanguagefromaveryearlyage(Nazzietal.,1998;Ramus,2002b).Composers,likeothermembersoftheirculture,internalizethesepatternsaspartoflearningtospeaktheirnativelanguage.(Onemechanismforthisinternalizationisaprocesscalledstatisticallearning,whichisdiscussedinmoredetailinthenextchapter.)Wesuggestthatwhencomposerswritemusic,linguisticrhythmsare“intheirears,”andtheycanconsciouslyorunconsciouslydrawonthesepatternsinweavingthesonicfabricoftheirmusic.Thisdoesnotimplythattheconnectionbetweenlinguisticandmusicalrhythmisobligatory.Rather,thislinkislikelytobegreaterinhistoricalepochswherecomposersseekanationalcharacterfortheirmusic.

(p.165)

Page 63: University Press Scholarship Online Oxford Scholarship ...

Rhythm

Page 63 of 79

PRINTED FROM OXFORD SCHOLARSHIP ONLINE (www.oxfordscholarship.com). (c) Copyright Oxford University Press, 2015.All Rights Reserved. Under the terms of the l icence agreement, an individual user may print out a PDF of a single chapter of amonograph in OSO for personal use (for details see http://www.oxfordscholarship.com/page/privacy-policy). Subscriber: UC - SantaCruz; date: 02 May 2015

Figure3.17 ThenPVIofBritishEnglishandFrenchmusicalthemes.Errorbarsshow+/-1standarderror.DatafromPatel&Daniele,2003a,andPatel,Iversen,&Rosenberg,2006.

OurfindingsforEnglishandFrenchspeechandmusicimmediatelyraisedtwoquestions.WouldthemusicalnPVIdifferencebeobservedifabroadersampleofEnglishandFrenchthemesandcomposerswerestudied?Perhapsmoreimportantly,wouldourresultgeneralizetootherculturesinwhichstress-versussyllable-timedlanguagesarespoken?Fortunately,HuronandOllen(2003)providedanswerstothesequestions.UsinganelectronicversionofADictionaryofMusicalThemescreatedbyHuron,theycomputedthe(p.166) nPVIofamuchlargersampleofEnglishandFrenchmusicalthemes(about2000themes,composedbetweenthemid-1500sandmid-1900s).TheyconfirmedthatthenPVIofEnglishmusicwassignificantlyhigherthanthatofFrenchmusic,thoughthedifferencewassmallerthanthatfoundbyPatelandDaniele(likelyduetolessstringentsamplingcriteria).TheyalsocomputedthemusicalnPVIforarangeofothernations,analyzingalmost8,000themesfrom12nationalitiesovermorethan3centuries.Ofthenationalitiestheyexamined,fivecanbeassignedtostress-timedlanguagesandthreetosyllabletimedlanguages(Fantetal.,1991a;Grabe&Low,2002;Ramus,2002b).ThesearelistedinTable3.1alongwiththeirmusicalnPVIvalues.(ThedatainTable3.1representcorrectedvaluesoftheoriginaltableinHuron&Ollen,2003,kindlyprovidedbyDavidHuron.Seethischapter’sappendix2fordatafrommorecultures.)

Fouroutofthefivenationswithstress-timedlanguages(American,Austrian,English,andSwedish)doindeedhavehighermusicalnPVIvaluesthanthethreenationswithsyllable-timedlanguages,providingsupportfortheideathatstress-timedandsyllable-timedlanguagesareassociatedwithdistinctivemusicalrhythms.However,Germanmusicisanotableexception:IthasalowmusicalnPVIvaluedespitethefactthatGermanisastress-timedlanguagewithahighnPVIvalueforspeech(Grabe&Low,2002;Dellwo,2004).

However,theremaybeahistoricalreasonwhyGermanmusichasalownPVI,namelythewell-knowninfluenceofItalianmusiconGermanmusic(Kmetzetal.,2001).Because

Page 64: University Press Scholarship Online Oxford Scholarship ...

Rhythm

Page 64 of 79

PRINTED FROM OXFORD SCHOLARSHIP ONLINE (www.oxfordscholarship.com). (c) Copyright Oxford University Press, 2015.All Rights Reserved. Under the terms of the l icence agreement, an individual user may print out a PDF of a single chapter of amonograph in OSO for personal use (for details see http://www.oxfordscholarship.com/page/privacy-policy). Subscriber: UC - SantaCruz; date: 02 May 2015

ItalianmusichasalownPVI,stylisticimitationofthismusicmightoutweighanylinguisticinfluenceoftheGermanlanguageonthenPVIofGermanmusic.OnewaytotestthisideaistoexaminethenPVIinhistoricalperspective,forexample,asafunctionofeachcomposer’sbirthyear.Whenthemesfrom14Germancomposerswereexaminedinthisfashionastrikingtrendemerged,asshowninFigure3.18(Patel&Daniele,2003b;Daniele&Patel,2004).

Table3.1MusicalnPviValuesforEightDifferentNationalities MusicalnPVI

Mean S.E.NationalitiesWithStress-TimedLanguages American 46.7 1.0Austrian 45.1 0.6English 45.6 0.9German 43.2 0.6Swedish 50.0 2.4NationalitiesWithSyllable-TimedLanguages French 43.4 0.7Italian 41.4 1.0Spanish 42.5 1.9

(p.167) Overthecourseof250years,nPVIalmostdoubled,atrendthatishighlystatisticallysignificant.(Interestingly,thistrendisalsoevidentforthesixAustriancomposersweincludedinourstudy.)GivenwhatisknownaboutthehistoryoftheGermanlanguage,thisisunlikelytoreflectachangeintherhythmofGermanfromsyllable-timedtostress-timedduringthisperiod(C.Heeschen,personalcommunication).Instead,itmostlikelyreflectshistoricalchangesinmusicalstyle,perhapsincludingawaninginfluenceofItalianmusiconGermanmusicoverthisperiod.Infact,thefindingwouldbeconsistentwiththeideathatItalianmusichadastronginfluenceonGermanmusicduringtheBaroqueera(1600–1750),lessinfluenceduringtheClassicalera(1750–1825),andtheleastinfluenceduringtheRomanticera(1825–1900).Moregenerally,itsuggeststhatinstudyinglinguisticinfluencesonspeechrhythm,itisimportanttokeepinmindhistoricalinfluencesthatcanruncountertolinguisticinfluences.28

Page 65: University Press Scholarship Online Oxford Scholarship ...

Rhythm

Page 65 of 79

PRINTED FROM OXFORD SCHOLARSHIP ONLINE (www.oxfordscholarship.com). (c) Copyright Oxford University Press, 2015.All Rights Reserved. Under the terms of the l icence agreement, an individual user may print out a PDF of a single chapter of amonograph in OSO for personal use (for details see http://www.oxfordscholarship.com/page/privacy-policy). Subscriber: UC - SantaCruz; date: 02 May 2015

Figure3.18 nPVIasafunctionofcomposerbirthyearfor20composers.(Soliddots=Germancomposers;opendots=Austriancomposers.)Thebest-fittinglinearregressionlineisshown.FromPatel&Daniele,2003b.

(p.168) Takingastepback,musicalnPVIresearchdemonstratesthattherhythmicstructureofspeechandmusiccanbefruitfullycomparedwithoutanyresorttonotionsofperiodicity.Itisworthnotingthattheresearchdonesofarhardlyexhaustswhatcanbedoneusingthismeasure.Forexample,onecouldapplythenPVItorecordingsofperformedmusicratherthantomusicnotation.OnecouldalsoexaminethenPVIofperformancesofthesamepieceofinstrumentalmusicbymusicianswhospeakstress-timedversussyllable-timedlanguages,toseeifthenativelanguageinfluencestemporalpatternsinmusicperformance(cf.Ohgushi,2002).Finally,onecouldstudyimprovisedmusic,forexample,bystudyingjazzmusicianswhospeakdifferentdialectswithdifferentrhythmicqualities(e.g.,intheUnitedStates,perhapsanortheastdialectversusasoutherndialect).Inthiscase,thenPVIcouldbeusedtoinvestigatewhetherthetemporalpatternofspeechisreflectedintherhythmofimprovisedmusic.

3.5.2RelationsbetweenNonlinguisticRhythmPerceptionandSpeechRhythm

Theideathatnonlinguisticrhythmperceptioncanbeinfluencedbyone’snativelanguagehasbeenarticulatedbybothlinguistsandmusicresearchers.Over50yearsago,Jakobson,Fant,andHalle(1952:10–11)madethefollowingclaim:

Interferencebythelanguagepatternaffectsevenourresponsestononspeechsounds.Knocksproducedatevenintervals,witheverythirdlouder,areperceivedasgroupsofthreeseparatedbyapause.ThepauseisusuallyclaimedbyaCzechtofallbeforethelouderknock,byaFrenchmantofallafterthelouder;whileaPolehearsthepauseoneknockafterthelouder.Thedifferentperceptionscorrespondexactlytothepositionofwordstressinthelanguagesinvolved:inCzechthestressisontheinitialsyllable,inFrench,onthefinalandinPolish,onthepenult.

ThegroupingssuggestedbyJakobsonetal.canbeschematicallyrepresentedasfollows,

Page 66: University Press Scholarship Online Oxford Scholarship ...

Rhythm

Page 66 of 79

PRINTED FROM OXFORD SCHOLARSHIP ONLINE (www.oxfordscholarship.com). (c) Copyright Oxford University Press, 2015.All Rights Reserved. Under the terms of the l icence agreement, an individual user may print out a PDF of a single chapter of amonograph in OSO for personal use (for details see http://www.oxfordscholarship.com/page/privacy-policy). Subscriber: UC - SantaCruz; date: 02 May 2015

inwhicheachxrepresentaknockandtheuppercaseX’sarelouder:

(p.169) TheclaimofJakobsonetal.iscertainlyprovocative,buttherehasbeennoempiricalevidencetosupportit.Nevertheless,theideaofalinkbetweennativelanguageandnonlinguisticrhythmperceptionpersists.Forexample,StobartandCross(2000)havedocumentedaformofmusicfromtheViachapeopleoftheBolivianhighlandsinwhichthelocalmannerofmarkingthebeatisdifferentfromwhatmostEnglish-speakinglistenersperceive.SoundExample3.9illustratesthismusicwithanEastersongplayedonasmallguitar(charango).ThepositionatwhichtheViachaclaportaptheirfoottothebeatcanbeheardattheendoftheexcerpt.ThistendencytomarktheshortereventineachgroupoftwonotesasthebeatiscontrarytothetendencyofEnglishspeakerstohearthepatterniambically,thatis,withthebeatonthesecondeventofeachpair.StobartandCrossspeculatethatthetendencytomarkthebeattrochaically(onthefirstmemberofeachgroup)isrelatedtostresspatternsinwordsofthelocallanguage,Quechua.

Thetwoexamplesabovedifferinthattheformerconcernssegmentation(rhythmicgrouping),whereasthelatterconcernsbeatperception.Note,however,thatneitherreferstonotionsofperiodicityinspeech.Instead,theybothrefertopatternsoflexicalstressandhowthisinfluencesnonlinguisticauditoryperception.Thusonceagainweseethatinterestingclaimsaboutrhythmicrelationsbetweenmusicandlanguagecanbemadewithoutanyreferencetoperiodicityinspeech.

Howcanoneassesswhetherthenativelanguageinfluencestheperceptionofnonlinguisticrhythm?Asafirststep,itisnecessarytodemonstratethatthereareculturaldifferencesinnonlinguisticrhythmperception.Rhythmicsegmentationorgroupingisofparticularinterestinthisregard,asintimatedbyJakobsonetal.(1952).Thisisbecausepsycholinguisticresearchindicatesthatthattherhythmofone’snativelanguageleadstosegmentationstrategiesthatareappliedevenwhenlisteningtoaforeignlanguage.Theideathatthenativelanguagecaninfluencenonlinguisticrhythmicsegmentationisthusjustonestepawayfromthisidea(cf.section3.3.3,subsection“TheRoleofRhythminSegmentingConnectedSpeech”).

Yetatthecurrenttime,itiswidelybelievedthatelementarygroupingoperationsreflectgeneralauditorybiasesnotinfluencedbyculture.Thisbeliefstemsfromacentury-oldlineofresearchinwhichresearchershaveinvestigatedrhythmicgroupingusingsimpletonesequences(Bolton,1894;Wood-row,1909).Forexample,listenersarepresentedwithtonesthatalternateinloudness(…loud-soft-loud-soft…)orduration(…long-short-long-short…)andareaskedtoindicatetheirperceivedgrouping.Twoprinciplesestablishedacenturyago,andconfirmedinnumerousstudiessince,arewidelyaccepted:

1.Aloudersoundtendstomarkthebeginningofagroup.

Page 67: University Press Scholarship Online Oxford Scholarship ...

Rhythm

Page 67 of 79

PRINTED FROM OXFORD SCHOLARSHIP ONLINE (www.oxfordscholarship.com). (c) Copyright Oxford University Press, 2015.All Rights Reserved. Under the terms of the l icence agreement, an individual user may print out a PDF of a single chapter of amonograph in OSO for personal use (for details see http://www.oxfordscholarship.com/page/privacy-policy). Subscriber: UC - SantaCruz; date: 02 May 2015

2.Alengthenedsoundtendstomarktheendofagroup.

(p.170) Theseprincipleshavecometobeviewedasuniversallawsofperception,underlyingtherhythmsofbothspeechandmusic(Hayes,1995b;Hay&Diehl,2007).However,thecross-culturaldatahavecomefromalimitedrangeofcultures(American,Dutch,andFrench).Aretheprinciplestrulyuniversal?AstudybyKusumotoandMoreton(1997)suggestedotherwise,findingthatAmericanversusJapaneselistenersdifferedwithregardtoPrinciple2above.ThisstudymotivatedareplicationandextensionofthisworkbyIversen,Patel,andOhgushi(2008),describedbelow.

Iversenetal.hadnativespeakersofJapaneseandnativespeakersofAmericanEnglishlistentosequencesoftones.Thetonesalternatedinloudness(“amplitude”sequences,SoundExample3.10a)orinduration(“duration”sequences,SoundExample3.10b),asshownschematicallyinFigure3.19.

Listenerstoldtheexperimentershowtheyperceivedthegrouping.TheresultsrevealedthatJapaneseandEnglishspeakersagreedwithprinciple1):bothreportedthattheyheardrepeatingloud-softgroups.However,thelistenersshowedasharpdifferencewhenitcametoprinciple2.)AlthoughEnglishspeakersperceivedthe“universal”short-longgrouping,manyJapaneselistenersstronglyperceivedtheoppositepattern,inotherwords,repeatinglong-shortgroups.(cf.Figure3.19).Becausethisfindingwassurprisingandcontradicteda“law”ofperception,Iversenetal.replicateditwithlistenersfromdifferentpartsofJapan.Thefindingisrobustandcallsforanexplanation.WhywouldnativeEnglishandJapanesespeakersdifferinthisway?

Figure3.19 Leftside:Schematicofsoundsequencesusedintheperceptionexperiment.Thesesequencesconsistoftonesalternatinginloudness(“amplitudesequence,”top),orduration(“durationsequence,”bottom).Intheamplitudesequence,thinbarscorrespondtosoftersoundsandthickbarscorrespondtoloudersounds.Inthedurationsequence,shortbarscorrespondtobriefersoundsandlongbarscorrespondtolongersounds.Thedotsbeforeandafterthesequencesindicatethatonlyanexcerptofalongersequenceofalternatingtonesisshown.Rightside:PerceivedrhythmicgroupingbyAmericanandJapaneselisteners,indicatedbyovals.Solidblackovalsindicatepreferencesthatfollow“universal”principlesofperception,whilethedashedblackovalindicatesapreferencethatviolatesthepurporteduniversals.

(p.171) Assumingthatthatthesedifferentperceptualbiasesarenotinnate,theykeyquestioniswhataspectofauditoryexperiencemightberesponsibleforthisdifference.

Page 68: University Press Scholarship Online Oxford Scholarship ...

Rhythm

Page 68 of 79

PRINTED FROM OXFORD SCHOLARSHIP ONLINE (www.oxfordscholarship.com). (c) Copyright Oxford University Press, 2015.All Rights Reserved. Under the terms of the l icence agreement, an individual user may print out a PDF of a single chapter of amonograph in OSO for personal use (for details see http://www.oxfordscholarship.com/page/privacy-policy). Subscriber: UC - SantaCruz; date: 02 May 2015

Twoobviouscandidatesaremusicandspeech,becausethesesoundpatternssurroundhumansthroughouttheirlife.Bothpatternspresenttheearwithsequencesofsoundthatmustbebrokenintosmallercoherentchunks,suchasphrasesinmusic,orphrasesandwordsinspeech.Mightthetemporalrhythmofthesechunksdifferformusicorspeechinthetwocultures?Thatis,mightshort-longpatternsbemorecommoninAmericanmusicorspeech,andlong-shortbemorecommoninJapanesemusicorspeech?Ifso,thenlearningthesepatternsmightinfluenceauditorysegmentationgenerally,andexplainthedifferencesweobserve.

Focusingfirstonmusic,onerelevantissueconcernstherhythmofhowmusicalphrasesbegininthetwocultures.Forexample,ifmostphrasesinAmericanmusicstartwithashort-longpattern(e.g.,a“pick-upnote”),andmostphrasesinJapanesemusicstartwithalong-shortpattern,thenlistenersmightlearntousethesepatternsassegmentationcues.Totestthisidea,weexaminedphrasesinAmericanandJapanesechildren’ssongs(becausewebelievetheseperceptualbiasesareprobablylaiddownearlyinlife).Weexamined50songsperculture,andforeachphrasewecomputedthedurationratioofthefirsttothesecondnoteandthencountedhowoftenphrasesstartedwithashort-longpatternversusotherpossiblepatterns(e.g.,long-short,orequalduration).WefoundthatAmericansongsshownobiastostartphraseswithashort-longpattern.Interestingly,Japanesesongsshowabiastostartphraseswithalong-shortpattern,consistentwithourperceptualfindings.However,themusicaldataalonecannotexplaintheculturaldifferencesweobserve,becausethisdatacannotexplaintheshort-longgroupingbiasofAmericanlisteners.

Turningtolanguage,onebasicdifferencebetweenEnglishandJapaneseconcernswordorder(Baker,2001).Forexample,inEnglish,shortgrammatical(or“function”)wordssuchas“the,”“a,”“to,”andsoforth,comeatthebeginningofphrasesandcombinewithlongermeaningful(or“content”)words(suchasanounorverb).Functionwordsaretypically“reduced,”havingshortdurationandlowstress.Thiscreatesfrequentlinguisticchunksthatstartwithashortelementandendwithalongone,suchas“thedog,”“toeat,”“abigdesk,”andsoforth.ThisfactaboutEnglishhaslongbeenexploitedbypoetsincreatingtheEnglishlanguage’smostcommonverseform,iambicpentameter.

Japanese,incontrast,placesfunctionwordsattheendsofphrases.CommonfunctionwordsinJapaneseinclude“casemarkers,”shortsoundsthatcanindicatewhetheranounisasubject,directobject,indirectobject,andsoforth.Forexample,inthesentence“John-san-gaMari-san-nihon-woagemashita,”(“JohngaveabooktoMari”)thesuffixes“ga,”“ni,”and“wo”arecasemarkersindicatingthatJohnisthesubject,Mariistheindirectobjectand“hon”(book)isthedirectobject.Placingfunctionwordsattheendsofphrasescreatesfrequentchunksthatstartwithalongelementandendwithashortone,(p.172) whichisjusttheoppositeoftherhythmofshortphrasesinEnglish(cf.Morganetal.,1987).

Apartfromshortphrases,theothershortmeaningfulchunksinlanguagearewords.Becauseourperceptionexperimentfocusedontwo-elementgroups,weexaminedthetemporalshapeofcommondisyllabicwordsinEnglishandJapanese.Englishdisyllabic

Page 69: University Press Scholarship Online Oxford Scholarship ...

Rhythm

Page 69 of 79

PRINTED FROM OXFORD SCHOLARSHIP ONLINE (www.oxfordscholarship.com). (c) Copyright Oxford University Press, 2015.All Rights Reserved. Under the terms of the l icence agreement, an individual user may print out a PDF of a single chapter of amonograph in OSO for personal use (for details see http://www.oxfordscholarship.com/page/privacy-policy). Subscriber: UC - SantaCruz; date: 02 May 2015

wordstendtobestressedonthefirstsyllable(e.g.,MO-ney,MAY-be;Cutler&Carter,1987),whichmightleadonetothinkthattheywouldhavealong-shortrhythmicpatternofsyllableduration.Totestthis,weexaminedsyllabledurationpatternsforthe50mostcommondisyllabicwordsinthelanguage(fromacorpusofspontaneousspeech),andmeasuredtherelativedurationofthetwosyllables.Surprisingly,commonwordswithstressonthefirstsyllabledidnothaveastrongbiastowardalong-shortdurationpattern.Incontrast,commonwordswithstressonthesecondsyllable,suchas“a-BOUT,”“be-CAUSE,”and“be-FORE,”hadaverystrongshort-longdurationpattern.Thustheaveragedurationpatternforcommontwo-syllablewordsinEnglishwasshort-long(Figure3.20).

Figure3.20 Distributionofsyllabledurationratiosforcommontwo-syllablewordsinspontaneousspeechinAmericanEnglish.Separatehistogramsareshownforinitial-stressversusfinal-stresswordsin(a)and(b),andcombineddataareshownin(c),weightedbywordfrequency.Averagesindicatedbyarrowheads.Theoveralldistributionin(c)hasasignificantshort-longbias(averageratio=1:1.11).

(p.173) Thismeansthatashort-longrhythmpatternisreflectedatboththelevelofsmallphrasesandcommondisyllabicwordsinEnglish.Wealsoexaminedsyllabledurationpatternsinthe50mostcommondisyllabicwordsinJapanese.IncontrasttoEnglish,theaveragedurationpatternforsuchwordswaslong-short.Thusonceagain,linguisticrhythmmirroredtheresultsoftheperceptionexperiment.

Takingastepback,ourresultsshowthattheperceptionofrhythmicgrouping,longthoughttofollowuniversalprinciples,actuallyvariesbyculture.Ourexplanationforthisdifferenceisbasedontherhythmsofspeech.Specifically,wesuspectthatlearningthe

Page 70: University Press Scholarship Online Oxford Scholarship ...

Rhythm

Page 70 of 79

PRINTED FROM OXFORD SCHOLARSHIP ONLINE (www.oxfordscholarship.com). (c) Copyright Oxford University Press, 2015.All Rights Reserved. Under the terms of the l icence agreement, an individual user may print out a PDF of a single chapter of amonograph in OSO for personal use (for details see http://www.oxfordscholarship.com/page/privacy-policy). Subscriber: UC - SantaCruz; date: 02 May 2015

typicalrhythmicshapeofphrasesandwordsinthenativelanguagehasadeepeffectonrhythmperceptioningeneral.Ifourideaiscorrect,thenrhythmicgroupingpreferencesshouldbepredictablefromthetemporalstructureofsmalllinguisticchunks(phrasesandwords)inalanguage.

Thesefindingshighlighttheneedforcross-culturalworkwhenitcomestotestinggeneralprinciplesofauditoryperception.MuchoftheoriginalworkonrhythmicgroupingoftoneswasdonewithspeakersofWesternEuropeanlanguages(e.g.,English,Dutch,andFrench).Althoughtheselanguagesdoindeedhaveimportantdifferences,theyallfollowthepatternofputtingshortfunctionwordsattheonsetofsmalllinguisticphrases,whichmayaccountforthesimilarityofperceptualgroupinginthesecultures.Amoreglobalperspectiverevealsthatlanguageswithphrase-finalshortfunctionwordsarewidespread,butexistlargelyoutsideofEurope,forexample,inIndiaandEastAsia(Haspelmathetal.,2005).WepredictthatnativespeakersoftheselanguageswillgrouptonesofalternatingdurationlikeJapaneselistenersdo(long-short).

Animportantfuturedirectionforthisworkconcernsthedevelopmentofrhythmicgroupingpreferencesinchildhood.Doinfantshaveaninnatebiasforaparticulargroupingpattern(e.g.,short-long),whichisthenmodifiedbyexperience(cf.Trainor&Adams,2000)?Oraretheyrhythmic“blankslates”?Regardingtheperceptionofrhythmbyadults,ifspeakersofdifferentlanguagesperceivenonlinguisticrhythmdifferently,thiscouldhelpexplainreportsofdifferencesbetweenWesternersandJapaneseintheperformanceofsimplemusicalrhythms(Ohgushi,2002;Sadakataetal.,2004).Thatis,simplerhythmsmaybeperformeddifferentlyindifferentculturesbecausetheyareperceiveddifferentlyduringlearning.Thiswouldindicatethatexperiencewithspeechshapesnonlinguisticrhythmcognitionataverybasiclevel.

3.5.3NeuralRelationshipsbetweenRhythminSpeechandMusic

Inthischapter,Ihaveclaimedthatcertainaspectsofspeechrhythmandmusicalrhythmshowastrikingsimilarity,suchasthegroupingofeventsintophrases,(p.174) whereasotheraspectsarefundamentallydifferent,suchastheroleoftemporalperiodicity.Towhatextentdoneuraldatasupportthisclaim?Isthereevidencethatsomeaspectsofrhythminspeechandmusicarehandledbysimilarbrainsystems,whereasotheraspectsshowlittleneuraloverlap?

Focusingfirstongrouping,thereisevidenceforoverlapinbrainprocessingofphraseboundariesinbothdomains.Thisevidencecomesfromelectricalbrainresponses(event-relatedpotentials,ERPs)innormalindividuals.Steinhaueretal.(1999)demonstratedthattheperceptionofphraseboundariesinlanguageisassociatedwithaparticularERPcomponenttermedthe“closurepositiveshift”(CPS),acentro-parietalpositivityofafewhundredmillisecondsthatstartssoonaftertheendofanintonationalphrase.Furtherstudiesusingfilteredorhummedspeech(toremovelexicalcuesandleaveprosodiccues)showedthattheCPSissensitivetoprosodicratherthansyntacticcuestophraseboundaries(Steinhauer&Friederici,2001;Pannekampetal.,2005).Inspiredbythiswork,Knöscheetal.(2005)examinedtheERPsinmusicianstotheendsofmusical

Page 71: University Press Scholarship Online Oxford Scholarship ...

Rhythm

Page 71 of 79

PRINTED FROM OXFORD SCHOLARSHIP ONLINE (www.oxfordscholarship.com). (c) Copyright Oxford University Press, 2015.All Rights Reserved. Under the terms of the l icence agreement, an individual user may print out a PDF of a single chapter of amonograph in OSO for personal use (for details see http://www.oxfordscholarship.com/page/privacy-policy). Subscriber: UC - SantaCruz; date: 02 May 2015

phrases,andfoundacomponentsimilartotheCPSreportedbySteinhaueretal.UsingMEG,theyalsoidentifiedbrainareasthatwerelikelytobeinvolvedinthegenerationoftheCPSinmusic.Theseareasincludedtheanteriorandposteriorcingulatecortexandtheposteriorhippocampus.Basedontherolestheseareasplayinattentionandmemory,theresearchersarguethatthemusicalCPSdoesnotreflectthedetectionofaphraseboundaryperse,butmemoryandattentionprocessesassociatedwithshiftingfocusfromonephrasetothenext.

ThestudiesofSteinhaueretal.andKnöscheetal.pointthewaytocomparativeneuralstudiesofgroupinginlanguageandmusic.Thereismuchroomforfurtherwork,however.Forexample,intheKnöscheetal.studythesequenceswithphraseboundarieshaveinternalpauses,whereasthesequenceswithoutphraseboundariesdonot.Itwouldbepreferabletocomparesequenceswithandwithoutphraseboundariesbutwithidenticaltemporalstructure,forexample,usingharmonicstructuretoindicatephrasing(cf.Tanetal.,1981).Thisway,ERPsassociatedwithphraseboundariescannotbeattributedtosimpletemporaldifferencesinthestimuli.Itwouldalsobedesirabletoconductawithin-subjectsstudyofbrainresponsestophrasesinlanguageandmusic.Suchcomparativeworkshouldattendtotheabsolutedurationofmusicalversuslinguisticphrases,astheneuralprocessesinvolvedingroupingmaybeinfluencedbythesizeofthetemporalunitoverwhichinformationisintegrated(Elbertetal.,1991;vonSteinbüchel,1998).

Turningtothequestionofperiodicity,ifspeechrhythmsandperiodicmusicalrhythmsareservedbydifferentneuralmechanisms,thenonewouldpredictneuraldissociationsbetweenlinguisticrhythmicabilityandtheabilitytokeeporfollowabeatinmusic.Theneuropsychologicalliteraturecontainsdescriptionsofindividualswithmusicalrhythmicdisturbanceafterbraindamage,or(p.175) “acquiredarrhythmia”(e.g.,Mavlov,1980;Fries&Swihart,1990;Peretz,1990;Liégeois-Chauveletal.,1998;Schuppertetal.,2000;Wilsonetal.,2002;DiPietroetal.,2003).Twonotablefindingsfromthisliteraturearethatrhythmicabilitiescanbeselectivelydisrupted,leavingpitchprocessingskillsrelativelyintact,andthattherearedissociationsbetweenrhythmictasksrequiringsimplediscriminationoftemporalpatternsandthoserequiringtheevaluationorproductionofperiodicpatterns(e.g.,Peretz,1990).Forexample,Liégeois-Chauveletal.(1998)foundthatpatientswithlesionsintheanteriorpartoftheleftorrightsuperiortemporalgyrusweremuchmoreimpairedonametricaltaskthanonatemporaldiscriminationtask.Themetricaltaskinvolvedidentifyingapassageasawaltzoramarch,whereasthetemporaldiscriminationtaskinvolvedasamedifferentjudgmentonshortmelodicsequencesthatdifferedonlyintermsoftheirdurationpattern.Inthemetricaltask,patientswereencouragedtotapalongwiththeperceivedbeatofthemusictohelpthemintheirdecision.Wilsonetal.(2002)describeacasestudyofamusicianwitharighttemporoparietalstrokewhocoulddiscriminatenonmetricalrhythmsbutwhocouldnotdiscriminatemetricalpatternsorproduceasteadypulse.

Unfortunately,noneofthesestudiesexplicitlysetouttocomparerhythmicabilitiesinspeechandmusic.Thusthefieldiswideopenforcomparativestudiesthatemploy

Page 72: University Press Scholarship Online Oxford Scholarship ...

Rhythm

Page 72 of 79

PRINTED FROM OXFORD SCHOLARSHIP ONLINE (www.oxfordscholarship.com). (c) Copyright Oxford University Press, 2015.All Rights Reserved. Under the terms of the l icence agreement, an individual user may print out a PDF of a single chapter of amonograph in OSO for personal use (for details see http://www.oxfordscholarship.com/page/privacy-policy). Subscriber: UC - SantaCruz; date: 02 May 2015

quantitativemeasuresofbothspeechandmusicalrhythmafterbraindamage.Itwouldbeparticularlyinterestingtostudyindividualswhowereknowntohavegoodmusicalrhythmicabilitiesandnormalspeechbeforebraindamage,andtoexaminewhetherdisruptionsofspeechrhythmareassociatedwithimpairedtemporalpatterndiscrimination,impairedmetricalabilities,orboth.

Anotherpopulationofindividualswhowouldbeinterestingtostudywithregardtospeechandmusicalrhythmareindividualswith“foreignaccentsyndrome”(Takayamaetal.,1993).Inthisraredisorder,braindamageresultsinchangesinspeechprosodythatgivetheimpressionthatthespeakerhasacquiredaforeignaccent.Itremainstobedeterminedifthisdisorderisassociatedwithsystematicchangesinspeechrhythm,butifso,onecouldexamineifsuchindividualshaveanyabnormalitiesintheirmusicalrhythmicskills.

Ofcourse,adifficultyinstudyingacquiredarrhythmiaandforeignaccentsyndromeisthatsuchcasesarequiterare.Thusitwouldbepreferabletofindlargerpopulationsinwhicheitherspeechrhythmormusicalrhythmicabilitieswereimpaired,inordertoconductcomparativeresearch.Onepopulationthatholdspromiseforcomparativestudiesaretone-deafor“congenitalamusic”individualswhohaveseveredifficultieswithmusicperceptionandproductionwhichcannotbeattributedtohearingloss,lackofexposuretomusic,oranyobviousnonmusicalsocial/cognitiveimpairments(Ayotteetal.,2002).Oneadvantageofworkingwithsuchindividualsisthattheycaneasilybefoundinanylargecommunitythroughaprocessofadvertisingandcareful(p.176) screening(Ayotteetal.,2002;Foxtonetal.,2004).Suchindividualsappeartohaveproblemswithbasicaspectsofpitchprocessing,suchasdiscriminatingsmallpitchchangesordeterminingthedirectionofsmallpitchchanges(i.e.,whetherpitchgoesupordown)(Peretz&Hyde,2003;Foxtonetal.,2004).Interestingly,theydonotseemtobeimpairedindiscriminatingsimpletemporalpatternsandcansynchronizesuccessfullytoasimplemetronome.However,theydohavedifficultysynchronizingtothebeatofmusic(DallaBella&Peretz,2003).Ofcourse,itcouldbethatthedifficultyinsynchronizingwithmusicissimplyduetothedistractioncausedbyastimuluswithpitchvariation,duetodeficitsinpitchprocessing(cf.Foxtonetal.,2006).Thusfuturestudiesofbeatperceptionincongenitalamusiashouldusecomplexrhythmicsequenceswithnopitchvariation,suchasthoseusedinthestudyofPatel,Iversen,etal.(2005)describedinsection3.2.1above(cf.SoundExamples3.3.and3.4).Ifmusicallytone-deafindividualscannotsynchronizetothebeatofsuchsequences,thiswouldsuggestthatthemechanismsinvolvedinkeepingabeatinmusichavenothingtodowithspeechrhythm(becausethespeechofmusicallytone-deafindividualssoundsperfectlynormal).29

Isuspectthatfutureresearchwillreveallittlerelationshipbetweenspeechrhythmabilitiesineitherproductionorperceptionandmusicalrhythmabilitiesinvolvingperiodicity(suchasmetricaldiscriminationorbeatperceptionandsynchronization).Thiswouldsupportthepointthatperiodicitydoesnotplayaroleinspeechrhythm.

3.6ConclusionSpeechandmusicinvolvethesystematictemporal,accentual,andphrasalpatterningof

Page 73: University Press Scholarship Online Oxford Scholarship ...

Rhythm

Page 73 of 79

PRINTED FROM OXFORD SCHOLARSHIP ONLINE (www.oxfordscholarship.com). (c) Copyright Oxford University Press, 2015.All Rights Reserved. Under the terms of the l icence agreement, an individual user may print out a PDF of a single chapter of amonograph in OSO for personal use (for details see http://www.oxfordscholarship.com/page/privacy-policy). Subscriber: UC - SantaCruz; date: 02 May 2015

sound.Thatis,botharerhythmic,andtheirrhythmsshowbothimportantsimilaritiesanddifferences.Onesimilarityisgroupingstructure:Inbothdomains,elements(suchastonesandwords)aregroupedintohigher(p.177) levelunitssuchasphrases.Akeydifferenceistemporalperiodicity,whichiswidespreadinmusicalrhythmbutlackinginspeechrhythm.Ironically,theideathatspeechhasperiodictemporalstructuredrovemuchoftheearlyresearchonspeechrhythm,andwasthebasisforarhythmictypologyoflanguageswhichpersiststoday(stress-timedvs.syllable-timedlanguages).Itisquiteevident,however,thatthenotionofisochronyinspeechisnotempiricallysupported.Fortunately,muchrecentempiricalresearchonspeechrhythmhasabandonedthenotionofisochrony,andismovingtowardarichernotionofspeechrhythmbasedonhowlanguagesdifferinthetemporalpatterningofvowels,consonants,andsyllables.Akeyideathatmotivatesthisresearchisthatlinguisticrhythmistheproductofavarietyofinteractingphonologicalphenomena,andnotanorganizingprinciple,unlikethecaseofmusic.

Itmayseemthatbreakingthe“periodicitylink”betweenspeechandmusicwoulddiminishthechanceoffindinginterestingrhythmicrelationsbetweenthedomains.Infact,theconverseistrue.Changingthefocusofcomparativeworkfromperiodictononperiodicaspectsofrhythmrevealsnumerousinterestingconnectionsbetweenthedomains,suchasthereflectionofspeechtimingpatternsinmusic,andtheinfluenceofspeechrhythmsonnonlinguisticrhythmicgroupingpreferences.Althoughmanymoreconnectionsawaitexploration,itseemsclearthatsomeofthekeyprocessesthatextractrhythmicstructurefromcomplexacousticsignalsaresharedbymusicandlanguage.

Appendix1:ThenPVIEquation

Thisisanappendixforsection3.3.1,subsection“DurationandTypology.”ThenPVIequationis:

Inthisequation,misthenumberofdurationsinthesequence(e.g.,voweldurationsinasentence)anddkisthedurationofthekthelement.ThenPVIcomputestheabsolutevalueofthedifferencebetweeneachsuccessivepairofdurationsinasequence,normalizedbythemeanofthesetwodurations(thisnormalizationwasoriginallyintroducedtocontrolforfluctuationsinspeechrate).Thisconvertsasequenceofmdurationstoasequenceofm−1contrastivenessscores.Eachofthesescoresrangesbetween0(whenthetwodurationsareidentical)and2(formaximumdurationalcontrast,i.e.,whenoneofthedurationsapproacheszero).Themeanofthesescores,multipliedby100,yieldsthenPVIofthesequence.ThenPVIvalueforasequenceisthusboundedbylowerandupperlimitsof0and200,withhighernumbersindicatingagreaterdegreeofdurationalcontrastbetweenneighboringelements.

(p.178) Appendix2:MusicalnPVIValuesofDifferentNations

Page 74: University Press Scholarship Online Oxford Scholarship ...

Rhythm

Page 74 of 79

PRINTED FROM OXFORD SCHOLARSHIP ONLINE (www.oxfordscholarship.com). (c) Copyright Oxford University Press, 2015.All Rights Reserved. Under the terms of the l icence agreement, an individual user may print out a PDF of a single chapter of amonograph in OSO for personal use (for details see http://www.oxfordscholarship.com/page/privacy-policy). Subscriber: UC - SantaCruz; date: 02 May 2015

ThisisanappendixforChapter3,section3.5.1,Table3.1.DatakindlyprovidedbyDavidHuron.

Inthetablesbelow,#C=numberofcomposers,sd=standarddeviation.

Nationality Mean #Themes #C sdAmerican 46.7 478 32 22.4Armenian 43.1 33 1 22.2Austrian 45.1 1,636 22 23.7Austro-Hung 45.9 14 1 18.7Belgian 48.7 41 6 19.4Bohemian 44.4 30 1 22.9Brazilian 41.5 5 1 18.3Catalan 48.9 12 1 16.8Cuban 36.0 17 2 17.9Czech 46.9 266 6 24.3Danish 51.0 7 1 24.6English 45.6 741 27 24.4Finnish 44.9 169 2 25.3Flemish 25.2 3 1 3.8French 43.4 1,343 52 25.2German 43.2 2,379 39 26.8Hungarian 45.4 244 8 25.4Irish 44.1 16 3 25.7Italian 41.4 572 46 23.9Mexican 28.4 13 2 19.3Norwegian 45.2 122 2 20.3Polish 50.2 60 8 18.7Romanian 42.4 26 2 21.3Russian 41.3 853 25 23.4Spanish 42.5 108 8 19.3Swedish 50.0 12 3 29.3

(p.179) Datafromtheabovetableregroupedbylanguage:English=American,English,Irish;French=French,Belgian;German=German,Austrian,Austro-Hungarian;Slavic=Russian,Czech,Polish,Bohemian;Spanish=Spanish,Catalan,Cuban,Mexican;Scandinavian=Danish,Norwegian,Swedish(notFinnish)

Page 75: University Press Scholarship Online Oxford Scholarship ...

Rhythm

Page 75 of 79

PRINTED FROM OXFORD SCHOLARSHIP ONLINE (www.oxfordscholarship.com). (c) Copyright Oxford University Press, 2015.All Rights Reserved. Under the terms of the l icence agreement, an individual user may print out a PDF of a single chapter of amonograph in OSO for personal use (for details see http://www.oxfordscholarship.com/page/privacy-policy). Subscriber: UC - SantaCruz; date: 02 May 2015

Language Mean #Themes #C sdEnglish 46.0 1,235 62 23.6French 43.6 1,384 58 25.0German 44.0 4,029 62 25.6Slavic 43.1 1,209 40 23.5Spanish 41.0 150 13 19.4Scandinavian 45.9 141 6 21.3

Notes:

(1)Thiscorrespondstoatimesignatureof6/8,incontrasttoawaltz,whichhasatimesignatureof3/4.Asonecansee,although6/8=3/4inmathematicalterms,theseratiosrefertoratherdifferentformsoforganizationinamusicalcontext.

(2)AnapparentexceptionoccursinlongrhythmiccyclesofIndianclassicalmusic(e.g.,cyclesof16beatsormore),inwhichthestrongaccentonthefirstbeatofeachcyclecanbeseparatedby10secondsormore,yetplaysanimportantperceptualroleinthemusic.However,thismaybeanexceptionthatprovestherule,asexplicitcountingofthebeatsbytheaudienceispartofthelisteningtraditioninthismusic.Thatis,consciouseffortisexpendedinordertokeeptrackofwherethemusicisinitslongmetricalcycle.

(3)Neuralactivityinthebetafrequencybandhasbeenassociatedwiththemotorsystem,raisingthepossibilitythatmeterperceptioninthebraininvolvessomesortofcouplingbetweentheauditoryandmotorsystem,evenintheabsenceofovertmovement.

(4)Schulkindetal.(2003)conductedaninterestinganalysisofmelodicstructurethathelpssuggestwhyphraseboundariesmightbeimportantlandmarksinmelodyrecognition.Theyexaminedthetemporaldistributionofpitchandtemporalaccentsinmelodies,inwhichpitchaccentsweredefinedasnotesthatweremembersofthetonictriadorpointsofcontourchange,andtemporalaccentsweredefinedasrelativelylongnotesandmetricallyaccentednotes.Theyfoundthataccentdensitywashigheratphraseboundariesthanwithinphrases.(Forthoseunfamiliarwiththeconceptofatonictriad,itisexplainedinChapter5).Thustheedgesofphrasesarestructuralslotsthatattractaccentsofvariouskinds.

(5)Inasurveyof21typologicallydifferentlanguages,Jun(2005)foundthatalllanguageshadatleastonegroupinglevelabovetheword,andmosthadtwo.

(6)Morespecifically,thelengtheningwasconfinedtothesyllabicrime(thevowelandfollowingconsonants).Interestingly,thisasymmetricexpansionofthesyllableduetoprosodicboundariesdiffersfromtemporalchangesinasyllableduetostress(Beckmanetal.,1992).

Page 76: University Press Scholarship Online Oxford Scholarship ...

Rhythm

Page 76 of 79

PRINTED FROM OXFORD SCHOLARSHIP ONLINE (www.oxfordscholarship.com). (c) Copyright Oxford University Press, 2015.All Rights Reserved. Under the terms of the l icence agreement, an individual user may print out a PDF of a single chapter of amonograph in OSO for personal use (for details see http://www.oxfordscholarship.com/page/privacy-policy). Subscriber: UC - SantaCruz; date: 02 May 2015

(7)Indoingthisresearch,itwouldbeimportanttobeawareofdurationallengtheninginthevicinityofphraseboundariesinbothspeechandmusic:Iftherearedifferentdegreesofpreboundarylengtheninginthetwodomains,theneventsnearboundariesshouldbeexcludedfromtheanalysisasthiswouldbeconfoundedwithvariabilitymeasures.

(8)IamgratefultoBrunoReppforprovidingmewiththisdata.

(9)Perceivedloudnessincorporatesbothphysicalintensityandthedistributionofenergyacrossdifferentfrequencybands,inotherwords,“spectralbalance.”Thelattermaybeamoresalientandreliablecuethantheformer(Sluijter&vanHeuven,1996,Sluijteretal.,1997).

(10)Inapitch-accentlanguage,awordcanhaveentirelydifferentmeaningdependingonitspitchpattern.Thedifferencebetweenatonelanguageandapitch-accentlanguageisthatintheformerthereisaprescribedpitchforeachsyllable,whereasinpitch-accentlanguagesacertainsyllableofawordmayhavelexicalspecificationforpitch(Jun,2005).

(11)Asanexample,Ladefogedpointsoutthatthewordkakemono(scroll)takesaboutthesameamountoftimetosayas“nippon”(Japan),andattributesthistothefactthatbothwordscontainfourmorae:[kakemono]and[nippon].

(12)Abercrombie’stheoryofsyllablesasrootedinchestpulseshasalsobeenfalsified.ItshouldbenotedthatAbercrombiewasapioneeringscientistwhoestablishedoneofthefirstlaboratoriesdevotedtobasicresearchinphonetics(inEdinburgh).Hisideasaboutspeechrhythmarebutatinysliceofhiswork,andthoughwrong,stimulatedagreatdealofresearch.

(13)Syllablesaregenerallyrecognizedashavingthreestructuralslots:theonsetconsonant(s),thenucleus(usuallyoccupiedbyavowel),andthefollowingconsonants(referredtoasthecoda).AsyllablewithoneconsonantintheonsetandnoneinthecodaisrepresentedbyCV,whereasCCVCmeanstwoconsonantsintheonsetandoneinthecoda,andsoon.

(14)Daueralsosuggestedthatstress-andsyllable-timedlanguageshadadifferentrelationshipbetweenstressandintonation:Intheformer,stressedsyllablesserveasturningpointsintheintonationcontour,whereasinthelatter,intonationandstressaremoreindependent.Asintonationisnotdiscussedinthischapter,thisideawillnotbepursuedhere.

(15)AlthoughRamusetal.(1999)relateddifferencesinΔCand%Vtosyllablestructure,FrotaandVigário(2001)pointoutthatintheBP/EPcase,differencesinthesevariablesaredrivenbyvowelreduction,becausesyllablestructuresaresimilarinthetwovarieties.SeeFrotaandVigário(2001)fordetails.FrotaandVigárioalsoprovideaveryusefuldiscussionoftheΔCparameterandtheneedtonormalizethisvariableforoverallsentenceduration/speechrate(seealsoRamus,2002a).

Page 77: University Press Scholarship Online Oxford Scholarship ...

Rhythm

Page 77 of 79

PRINTED FROM OXFORD SCHOLARSHIP ONLINE (www.oxfordscholarship.com). (c) Copyright Oxford University Press, 2015.All Rights Reserved. Under the terms of the l icence agreement, an individual user may print out a PDF of a single chapter of amonograph in OSO for personal use (for details see http://www.oxfordscholarship.com/page/privacy-policy). Subscriber: UC - SantaCruz; date: 02 May 2015

(16)Individualresearcherswhoarecomparingsyllabledurationpatternsacrosstwolanguagescanalsohandletheproblemofsyllableboundaryidentificationbymakingallsuchdecisionsinamannerthatisconservativewithregardtothehypothesisathand.Forexample,ifcomparinglanguagesAandBwiththehypothesisthatsyllablesaremorevariableindurationinthesentencesoflanguageA,thenanyjudgmentcallsaboutsyllableboundariesshouldbemadeinsuchawayastoworkagainstthishypothesis.

(17)ThecorrelationbetweennPVIandrhythmclassisnotperfect,however.GrabeandLow(2002)foundahighvowelnPVIvalueforTamil,alanguagewhichhasbeenclassifiedassyllable-timed(cf.Keane,2006).ItshouldbenotedthatGrabeandLow’s(2002)resultsshouldbeconsideredpreliminarybecauseonlyonespeakerperlanguagewasstudied.SubsequentworkhasappliedthenPVItocross-linguisticcorporawithfewerlanguagesbutmorespeakersperlanguage(e.g.,Ramus,2002b;Lee&Todd,2004;Dellwo,2004).

(18)ItshouldbenotedthatbothGrabeandLow(2002)andRamus(2002b)measuredthenPVIofvocalicintervals,definedasvowelsandsequencesofconsecutivevowelsirrespectiveofsyllableandwordboundaries,whereasBolinger’sargumentsarefocusedonindividualvowels.Thisisnotaseriousproblembecausemostvocalicintervalsareindividualvowelsduetothestrongtendencyforvowelstobeseparatedbyconsonantsinspeech.Forexample,inthedatabaseusedbyRamus(eightlanguages,160sentences),thereare2,725vowels,outofwhich2,475(91%)aresingletons,inotherwords,asinglevowelflankedbyaconsonantoneitherside(orifthevowelisthefirstorlastphonemeofthesentence,asinglevowelflankedbyafollowingorprecedingconsonant,respectively).ThusitislikelythatnPVImeasurementsbasedonindividualvowelswouldproducequalitativelysimilarresultsasthosebasedonvocalicintervals(cf.Pateletal.,2006).

(19)PolishalsohasthelowestvocalicnPVIofalllanguagesintheRamusetal.(1999)database(Ramus,2002a;cf.Figure3.10).

(20)Itwouldbedesirableforfutureresearchonrhythmicclassestosuggestnewnamesforrhythmicclasses,asthecurrentnames(stress-timed,syllable-timed,andmora-timed)areimplicitlyboundupwiththe(failed)notionofisochrony.

(21)OnepossibleconfoundintheelegantstudyofNazzietal.(1998)isthepresenceofintonation,whichmayhaveplayedaroleintheinfants’discrimination.Indeed,Ramusetal.(2000)foundthatFrenchnewbornscoulddistinguishDutchfromJapaneseusingresynthesizedsaltanajspeech,butthattheirdiscriminationabilitywasmuchweakerwhentheoriginalF0contoursofthesentenceswerereplacedbythesameartificialcontours(Ramus,2002b).Healsonotesthatintonationcanberemovedentirelyusingflatsasasaresynthesis,butthattheresultingsoundpatternsareproblematicforusewithnewbornsandinfants,whomayfindthemboringordistressing.

(22)AnearlyconceptuallinkbetweenhierarchicaltheoriesoflinguisticrhythmandtheoriesofmusicalstructurewasmadebyJackendoff(1989),whonotedastructural

Page 78: University Press Scholarship Online Oxford Scholarship ...

Rhythm

Page 78 of 79

PRINTED FROM OXFORD SCHOLARSHIP ONLINE (www.oxfordscholarship.com). (c) Copyright Oxford University Press, 2015.All Rights Reserved. Under the terms of the l icence agreement, an individual user may print out a PDF of a single chapter of amonograph in OSO for personal use (for details see http://www.oxfordscholarship.com/page/privacy-policy). Subscriber: UC - SantaCruz; date: 02 May 2015

equivalencebetweenonetypeofprosodictreestructureusedtodepicthierarchicalprominencerelationsinlanguageandatypeoftreeusedbyLerdahlandJackendoff(1983)toindicatetherelativestructuralimportanceofeventsinaspanofmusicalnotes.Jackendoffspeculatedthatthecoincidenceofthesetwoformalismsmightreflectthefactthatlanguageandmusicusedifferentspecializationsofgeneral-purposementalprinciplesforassigningstructuretotemporalpatterns,inotherwords,principlesthatparsesoundsequencesintorecursivehierarchiesofbinaryoppositionsofstructuralimportance.AsnotedbyJackendoff,however,prosodictreeshavebeenlargelyabandonedintheoriesofspeechrhythm.

(23)Suchastudywillhavetobecarefultotryandmatchacousticpropertiesoftheonsetofthespokenandmusicalsound.Forexample,ifamusicalsoundwithasharpattackisused,suchasapianotone,thenaspeechsoundwithaplosiveonset(suchas/ta/)shouldbeusedratherthanonewithagradualonset(suchas/la/).

(24)IamgratefultoLauraDilleyformarkingstressedsyllablesinthesesentences.

(25)IamgratefultoGraemeBooneforbringingthiscorrespondencetomyattention.

(26)AlthoughthisistrueforEnglishandFrench,itshouldbenotedthatinJapaneseitisthemoraandnotthesyllablethatgetsmappedontoamusicalnote(Hayes,1995a).

(27)ThenPVIvaluesforEnglishandFrenchspeechshowninFigure3.14aretakenfromPateletal.(2006),ratherthanfromPatelandDaniele(2003a).Bothstudiesshowasignificantdifferencebetweenthetwolanguages(EnglishnPVI〉FrenchnPVI),butthe2006studyisbasedonmoreaccuratemeasurements.SeePateletal.(2006)formeasurementdetails,andforalistofallsentencesanalyzed.

(28)UnlikeGermanmusic,EnglishandFrenchmusicdonotshowasignificantincreaseinnPVIovertheequivalenttimeperiod,basedonthemesinBarlowandMorgenstern’sdictionary(Greig,2003).Thisraisesaninterestingmusicologicalpuzzle:WhydoGermanandAustrianmusicshowsuchastronghistoricalchangeinthismeasureofrhythm,whereasEnglishandFrenchmusicdonot?

(29)Ofcourse,beforeanyfirmconclusionscanbedrawn,thespeechrhythmoftone-deafindividualswouldneedtobequantitativelymeasuredtoshowthatitdidnotdifferfromnormalcontrols(forexample,usingthenPVI).Also,itispossiblethattone-deafindividualscannotkeepabeatbecausetheirpitchperceptionproblemhascausedanaversiontomusic,sothattheyhavenothadenoughexposuretomusictolearnhowtokeepabeat.Thusitwouldbepreferabletoworkwithindividualswhohavenormalpitchperceptionandwhoenjoymusic,butwhocannotkeepabeat.Theexistenceofsuch“rhythm-deaf”individualsisintuitivelyplausible,astherearecertainlypeoplewholikemusicbutclaimtohave“twoleftfeet”whenitcomestodancing,and/orwhocannotclapalongwithabeat.Itshouldbepossibletofindapopulationofsuchpeoplethroughaprocessofadvertisingandscreening,akintotheproceduresusedtofindcongenitalamusics.

Page 79: University Press Scholarship Online Oxford Scholarship ...

Rhythm

Page 79 of 79

PRINTED FROM OXFORD SCHOLARSHIP ONLINE (www.oxfordscholarship.com). (c) Copyright Oxford University Press, 2015.All Rights Reserved. Under the terms of the l icence agreement, an individual user may print out a PDF of a single chapter of amonograph in OSO for personal use (for details see http://www.oxfordscholarship.com/page/privacy-policy). Subscriber: UC - SantaCruz; date: 02 May 2015

Accessbroughttoyouby: UC-SantaCruz