Personalidad y Depresion

download Personalidad y Depresion

of 29

Transcript of Personalidad y Depresion

  • 8/2/2019 Personalidad y Depresion

    1/29

    Personality and Depression:Explanatory Models andReview of the Evidence

    Daniel N. Klein, Roman Kotov, and Sara J. Buffe

    Departments of Psychology and Psychiatry, State University of New York at Stony BStony Brook, New York 11794-2500; email: [email protected]

    Annu. Rev. Clin. Psychol. 2011. 7:26995

    First published online as a Review in Advance on

    December 6, 2010

    The Annual Review of Clinical Psychology is onlineat clinpsy.annualreviews.org

    This articles doi:10.1146/annurev-clinpsy-032210-104540

    Copyright c 2011 by Annual Reviews.All rights reserved

    1548-5943/11/0427-0269$20.00

    Keywords

    traits, temperament, mood disorders, neuroticism, extraversion

    Abstract

    Understanding the association between personality and depressioimplications for elucidating etiology and comorbidity, identifyin

    risk individuals, andtailoring treatment. We discuss seven major mthat have been proposed to explain the relation between personality

    depression, and we review key methodological issues, including sdesign, the heterogeneity of mood disorders, and the assessment of

    sonality. We then selectively review the extensive empirical literaon the role of personality traits in depression in adults and child

    Current evidence suggests that depression is linked to traits su

    neuroticism/negative emotionality, extraversion/positive emotionand conscientiousness. Moreover, personality characteristics appecontribute to the onset and course of depression through a varie

    pathways. Implications for prevention and prediction of treatmen

    sponse are discussed, as well as specific considerations to guide furesearch on the relation between personality and depression.

    269

    Click here for quick links to

    Annual Reviews content online,

    including:

    Other articles in this volume

    Top cited articles

    Top downloaded articles

    Our comprehensive search

    FurtherANNUAL

    REVIEWS

  • 8/2/2019 Personalidad y Depresion

    2/29

    Temperament:generally used todescribe personality inchildhood; largelyinterchangeable withthe term personality

    Neuroticism:a tendency to copepoorly with stress andto experience feelingsof sadness, anxiety, andanger

    Contents

    INTRODUCTION . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 270THE CONSTRUCT OF

    PERSONALITY . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 270MODELS OF PERSONALITY

    AND DEPRESSION . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 271

    Classical Models ofPersonality-DepressionRelations . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 271

    Dynamic Models ofPersonality-Depression

    Relations . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 274METHODOLOGICAL ISSUES . . . . . 275

    Study Design . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 275

    Heterogeneity of DepressiveD i s o r d e r s . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 2 7 6

    Assessment . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 276

    AFFECTIVE TEMPERAMENTS . . . . 277PERSONALITY TRAIT

    DIMENSIONS . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 279

    The Five-Factor Model . . . . . . . . . . . . 279Psychobiological Models . . . . . . . . . . . 283

    Clinical Traits . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 284CHILD TEMPERAMENT . . . . . . . . . . 285

    CLINICAL IMPLICATIONS. . . . . . . . 286

    Prevention . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 286Treatment Response . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 286

    CONCLUSIONS AND FUTURE

    DIRECTIONS . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 287

    INTRODUCTION

    The hypothesis that depression is linked to per-sonality can be traced to antiquity, when Hip-

    pocrates, and laterGalen, argued thatparticularhumors were responsible for specific per-

    sonality types and forms of psychopathology.In this article, we discuss the major conceptual

    models that have been proposed to explain theassociationbetweenpersonality and depression,

    comment on some important methodological

    issues, and selectively review the empiricalliterature. Due to space limitations, we limit

    our review to nonbipolar forms of depression.This literature has developed along several

    distinct lines: (a) early clinical psychiatrists

    descriptions of affective temperamen

    (b) research on the structure and neurobioloof personality; (c) psychoanalytic and cognitiv

    behavioral theory and observations; a(d) developmental psychologists work

    temperament. In recent years, there has be

    substantial convergence between these lin

    of work, and it is increasingly possible to viethem within a single integrative frameworUnderstanding the associations between pe

    sonality and depression has a number of potetially important implications for research an

    practice. First, personality traits associated wiemotional experience, expression, and regul

    tion may be intermediate phenotypes that prvide more tractable targets for genetic and ne

    robiological research than depressive diagnos

    (Canli 2008). Second, personality may be usefin identifying more homogeneous subgrou

    of depressive disorders that differ in develomental trajectories and etiological influenc

    (e.g., Beck 1983). Third, tracing the pathwabetween personality and depressive disorde

    can help elucidate more proximal processinvolved in the development of mood disorde

    (Compas et al. 2004, Klein et al. 2008a, Lah2009). Fourth, personality may be useful in ta

    loring treatment (Zinbarg et al. 2008) and prdicting treatment response (Quilty et al. 2008

    Fifth, temperament/personality may provi

    a means to identify at-risk individuals wcould benefit from prevention and ear

    intervention efforts (Kovacs & Lopez-Dur2010). Finally, there is substantial comorbidi

    between depressive disorders and other formof psychopathology. Some personality trai

    such as neuroticism, are associated with mutiple psychiatric conditions. Thus, personali

    could help explain patterns of comorbidand point toward more etiologically releva

    classification systems (Brown & Barlow 200Kotov et al. 2007, Watson 2009).

    THE CONSTRUCT OFPERSONALITY

    Before addressing the relation between pe

    sonality and depression, several conceptu

    270 Klein Kotov Bufferd

  • 8/2/2019 Personalidad y Depresion

    3/29

    issues regarding the construct of personality

    should be considered. First, personality hastraditionally been conceptualized as having

    two components: temperament, which refersto biologically based, early-emerging, stable

    individual differences in emotion and its regu-

    lation, and character, which refers to individual

    differences due to socialization. However,the distinctions between these constructs arequestionable, as a large body of evidence has

    accumulated indicating that personality traitshave all the characteristics of temperament, in-

    cluding strong genetic and biological bases andsubstantial stability over the lifespan (Krueger

    & Johnson 2008, Watson et al. 2006). Hence,the terms personality and temperament

    are now often used interchangeably (Caspi

    & Shiner 2006, Clark & Watson 1999). Asmost research on personality in childhood

    has been conducted under the temperamentrubric, in this review we refer to this work

    using the term temperament and reserve theterm personality for discussing the literature

    on adolescents and adults. However, this isintended to reflect traditional usage rather

    than a conceptually meaningful distinction.Second, a variety of personality classifica-

    tions have been proposed over the past century,but in the 1980s they were integrated in a

    consensus taxonomy, the Five-Factor Model

    (FFM). The FFM recognized that personalityis ordered hierarchically from a large number

    of specific traits to five general characteristics(Digman 1994, Goldberg 1993, Markon et al.

    2005). These Big Five traits are neuroticism,extraversion, conscientiousness, agreeableness,

    and openness to experience. Importantly,the FFM can be further reduced to three

    dimensions of negative emotionality, positiveemotionality, and disinhibition versus con-

    straintthatformthenextlevelofthepersonalityhierarchy (Clark & Watson 1999, Markon

    et al. 2005). This Big Three model is used in

    studies of temperament as well as personality,although disinhibition is often labeled as

    effortful control in the child literature (Caspi &Shiner 2006, Rothbart & Bates 2006). The Big

    Five and Big Three schemes are closely related,

    Five-Factor M(FFM): hierarcpersonality taxowith five generaat its apex:neuroticism,

    extraversion,conscientiousneagreeableness, aopenness to exp

    Extraversion:tendency to engenvironment anpeople with vigoenthusiasm

    Conscientioustendency to apptasks in a planfu

    deliberate mannto be reliable anself-disciplined

    N/NE: neurotinegative emotio

    E/PE: extraverpositive emotion

    with neuroticism being essentially identical to

    negative emotionality and extraversion cor-responding to positive emotionality (Clark &

    Watson 1999, Markon et al. 2005); we refer tothese two dimensions as neuroticism/negative

    emotionality (N/NE) and extraversion/positive

    emotionality (E/PE), respectively. Disinhi-

    bition does not have an exact counterpart inthe FFM but instead reflects a combination oflow conscientiousness and low agreeableness.

    Finally, openness to experience is outside theterritory covered by the Big Three.

    Third, there is increasing recognitionthat temperament and personality are not a

    fixed, static set of characteristics, but ratherare dynamic constructs that develop over the

    lifespan and change in response to maturation

    and life circumstances (Fraley & Roberts 2005,Rothbart & Bates 2006). For example, although

    the rank-order stability of most personalitytraits is in the moderate range, it increases

    over the course of development (Roberts &DelVecchio 2000). In addition, mean levels

    of conscientiousness and some facets of E/PEincrease, and levels of N/NE decrease, over

    time, particularly in young adulthood (Robertset al. 2006). A number of processes contribute

    to stability and change of personality. For ex-ample, genes are a major influence on stability

    (Krueger & Johnson 2008, Kandler et al. 2010).

    In addition,people often select, create, andcon-strue environments in ways that reinforce and

    maintain their initial trait dispositions (Caspi &Shiner 2006). However, life stressors and major

    shifts in social roles and relationships can con-tribute to personality change (Fraley & Roberts

    2005, Kandler et al. 2010). We consider theimplications of these processes for the relation

    between personality and depression below.

    MODELS OF PERSONALITYAND DEPRESSION

    Classical Models ofPersonality-Depression Relations

    A variety of models of the relation between

    personality and mood disorders have been

    www.annualreviews.org Personality and Depression 271

  • 8/2/2019 Personalidad y Depresion

    4/29

    Table 1 Summary of key predictions of the classic models

    Model Predictions about a target trait and its relation to depression

    Common cause Shared etiology accounts for the observed association

    Continuum/spectrum Similar etiology; association is fairly specific and nonlinear

    Precursor Similar etiology; predicts depression onset

    Predisposition Predicts depression onset; other variables mediate or moderate this link

    Pathoplasticity Predicts variation in presentation or outcome of depression above and beyond other baseline

    characteristicsConcomitants Is altered during a depressive episode but returns to premorbid level after

    Consequences/scars Is altered during and after a depressive episode

    proposed (e.g., Akiskal et al. 1983, M.H. Klein

    et al. 1993, Krueger & Tackett 2003). Theseproposed relations include: (a) personality and

    depressive disorders have common causes;

    (b) personality and depressive disorders forma continuous spectrum; (c) personality is a pre-

    cursor of depressive disorders; (d) personalitypredisposes to developing depressive disor-

    ders; (e) personality has pathoplastic effectson depression; (f) personality features are

    state-dependent concomitants of depressiveepisodes; and (g) personality features are

    consequences (or scars) of depressive episodes.The distinctions between some of these ac-

    counts are subtle (cf. Kendler & Neale 2010),and other models, as well as combinations

    of these scenarios, are plausible. However,

    these seven models provide a useful conceptualframework for approaching the issue.

    These models can be divided into threegroups. The first three models (common cause,

    continuum/spectrum, and precursor) view per-sonality and depression as having similar causal

    influences but do not see one as having acausal influence on the other. The fourth and

    fifth models (predisposition and pathoplastic-ity) hold that personality has causal effects on

    the onset or maintenance of depression.Finally,

    the sixth and seventh models (concomitantsand consequences) view depression as having a

    causal influence on personality. These models,and their unique predictions, are summarized

    in Table 1.The common cause model views personality

    and depressive disorders as distinct entities

    that arise from the same, or at least an ove

    lapping, set of etiological processes. From thperspective, personality and depression are n

    directly related; rather, the association is du

    to a shared third variable. The common caumodel would be supported by evidence th

    personality traits and depression have sharetiological influences.

    The continuum/spectrum model emphasizes the conceptual overlap betwe

    depressive disorders and certain personaltraits and argues for a fundamental continui

    between them. A depressive diagnosis thought to simply identify individuals w

    have the most extreme scores on a releva

    trait. Like the common cause model, tcontinuum/spectrum model assumes th

    personality and depression arise from a similif not identical, set of causal factors. Howeve

    the continuum/spectrum model goes furtherpositing that the association between the tr

    and disorder should be fairly specific becauthey are on the same continuum.1 Moreov

    this association is expected to be nonlineso that almost nobody below the definition

    threshold on the trait has the diagnosis bnearly everyone above the threshold meets t

    criteria. Thus, the continuum/spectrum mod

    1There may not be complete specificity owing to diagnosheterogeneity. As discussed below, depression is a hetegeneous disorder with multiple etiological pathways (eqfinality). A personality trait may be part of only one of tpathways. In contrast, multifinality, in which the trait is sociated with multiple disorders, is less consistent with tcontinuum/spectrum view.

    272 Klein Kotov Bufferd

  • 8/2/2019 Personalidad y Depresion

    5/29

    would be supported by evidence that the trait

    and depression are associated with the same eti-ological influences and that the trait-disorder

    relationship is fairly specific and nonlinear.The precursor model views personality as

    an early manifestation or forme fruste of de-

    pressive disorder. Like the common cause and

    continuum/spectrum accounts, the precursormodel posits that personality and depressivedisorders are caused by similar etiologic factors.

    Also like the continuum/spectrum account, itimplies considerable phenomenologic similar-

    ity between the relevant trait and depression.However, the precursor model differs from

    both of these other models in that it assumesa particular developmental sequence, with the

    personality traits being evident prior to the on-

    set of depressive disorder. In other words, boththe common cause and continuum/spectrum

    models assume a fixed clinical expression astraits or disorder, whereas the precursor model

    implies escalation from traits to disorder withinindividuals over time. Support for theprecursor

    model would come from evidence that the traitand depression are associated with the same

    etiological influences and that individuals withhigh levels of the trait are at increased risk for

    developing the disorder over time.2

    The common cause, continuum/spectrum,

    and precursor models do not posit causalrelations between personality and depression.

    In contrast, the predisposition model holds that

    personality plays a causal role in the onset ofdepression. However, the predisposition model

    overlaps with the precursor model in that bothpropose that the relevant traits are evident prior

    to the onset of depressive disorder. The majordifference between these two accounts is that

    2

    Applicationof the continuum/spectrumand precursor mod-els to depressive disorders is not straightforward. Person-ality traits are relatively stable, whereas depression is oftenepisodic. Existing formulations of the continuum/spectrummodel have not explained how stable trait characteristicsmanifest as an episodic illness. Similarly, the precursor modeldoes not account for why a stable trait would subsequentlydevelop into a nonstable depressive state. Thus, the contin-uum/spectrum and precursor models may provide a betterexplanation for chronic than episodic forms of depression.

    Precursor modconsiders personan early manifesof the disorder

    Predispositionmodel: posits tpersonality is difrom psychopatand plays a causin its developme

    Pathoplasticitymodel: posits tpersonality influthe expression odisorder after on

    the precursor model assumes that personality

    and depression derive from the same set of etio-logical processes, but the predisposition model

    posits that the processes that underlie person-ality differ from those that lead to depression.

    Thus, the predisposition account implies a

    complex interplay among risk factors involving

    moderation and/or mediation, and this is whatdistinguishes it from the precursor model.3

    The most common examplethe diathesis-

    stress modelconceptualizes personality asthe diathesis and stress as a moderator that

    precipitates the onset of depressive disorder.Alternatively, stress may be a mediator, so

    that personality vulnerability leads to negativeexperiences (e.g., interpersonal rejection, job

    loss), which in turn increase the probability

    of a depressive episode. A second differencebetween these models is that the predisposition

    model does not assume any phenomenologicallinks between personality traits and depressive

    symptoms. Consequently, the predisposingtrait may not have any phenotypic similarity to

    depression. Thus, the two most critical sourcesof support for the predisposition model would

    involve demonstrating that individuals withthe trait are at increased risk for subsequently

    developing depression, and that other variablesplay a role in mediating or moderating this

    transition.

    The pathoplasticity model is similar tothe predisposition model in that it also views

    personality as having a causal influence ondepressive disorder. However, rather than con-

    tributing to the onset of depression, the patho-plasticity model posits that personality influ-

    ences the expression of the disorder after onset.This influence can include the severity or pat-

    tern of symptomatology, course, and responseto treatment. The pathoplasticity model would

    be supported by evidence that personality

    3Although moderating and mediating variables play an ex-plicit and central role in the predisposition model, it shouldbe acknowledged that they are not incompatible with theprecursor account. That is, the escalation from personalitytraits to depressive disorders in the precursor model impliesthat additional variables (e.g., maturational or environmentalfactors) must be involved to precipitate the change.

    www.annualreviews.org Personality and Depression 273

  • 8/2/2019 Personalidad y Depresion

    6/29

    explains variation among depressed individuals

    in their clinical presentation or outcome.The final two models also assume that there

    is a causal relation between personality anddepression. However, these models reverse

    the direction of causality. In the concomitants

    (or state-dependent) model, assessments of

    personality are colored, or distorted, by theindividuals mood state. This model impliesthat personality returns to its baseline form

    after recovery from the episode. In contrast,the consequences (or scar) model holds that

    depressive episodes have an enduring effect onpersonality, such that changes in personality

    persist after recovery. These models would besupported by evidence that depression alters

    levels of personality traits, either concurrently

    (concomitants model) or over the longer-term(consequences model).

    Dynamic Models ofPersonality-Depression Relations

    The models above consider traits to be perfectlystable. As noted earlier, there is now extensive

    evidence indicating that personality shows plas-ticity in childhood, with long-term test-retest

    correlations of r 0.35, and continues tochange across the lifespan, although personality

    consistency gradually increases up to r 0.75after the age of 50 (Roberts & DelVecchio

    2000). Models of personality-psychopathologyrelations can be expanded to recognize the

    malleability of traits (e.g., Ormel et al. 2001).

    For example, one can posit a dynamic precursormodel4 in which early temperament defines the

    baseline level of risk but subsequentexperiencesmodify personality liability to depression. This

    model explains variability in disorder onset as afunction of the initial level of risk and steepness

    of the trait trajectory over time. Given the evi-dence on patterns of personality continuity and

    change (Roberts & DelVecchio 2000), it ap-

    4This could also be called the dynamic continuum modelbecause once the dynamic element is introduced, it becomes

    virtually impossible to distinguish the continuum/spectrumand precursor models.

    pears likely that trait vulnerability is more m

    leable early in life, but significant life events calter its trajectory even in old age. A depressi

    disorder is thought to emerge when personalliability crosses the threshold. Thus, individ

    als who are born with an elevated personali

    liability or those with a rapidly increasing tr

    trajectory would have a childhood onset of tdisorder, whereas those with a more slowincreasing trait trajectory would not cross th

    threshold until much later, if ever. Moreovea pathological trait trajectory may be check

    or reversed by positive experiences (Ormel de Jong 1999). In fact, personality genera

    tends to change in a more adaptive directiowith age (Roberts et al. 2006), although th

    pattern is not universal ( Johnson et al. 200

    This may help to explain why the probabilityfirst-episode depression peaks in adolescenc

    as trait deviance is more common at that ageSimilarly, the predisposition model can

    expanded to recognizepersonality change. Thdynamic predisposition model (Ormel &

    Jong, 1999, Ormel et al. 2001) acknowledgtransactions between personality and the env

    ronment and integrates them with the enviromental moderation and mediation mechanism

    of the classic predisposition model. In the envronmental moderation version of this accoun

    negative life experiences influence not only d

    pression onset but also levels of trait vulnerbility (Middledorp et al. 2008). This increase

    personality liability may then lead to additionlife stress. If this vicious cycle is perpetuat

    unchecked, personality liability would continto increase, and at some point, a negative li

    event could overwhelm coping capabilities anelicit a depressive disorder. Importantly, and

    contrast to thedynamic precursor model, in thaccount maladaptive traits alone are not suf

    cient to cause depression, andan environmentrigger is necessary.

    The vicious cycle of increasing trait vulne

    ability and stress exposure does not necessarindicate that personality per se influenc

    depression onset. Indeed, certain traits mincrease stress exposure but have no effect o

    depression otherwise (e.g., it is possible that lo

    274 Klein Kotov Bufferd

  • 8/2/2019 Personalidad y Depresion

    7/29

    conscientiousness does not cause depression

    directly but leads to depressogenic experiences,such as academic difficulties, job loss, and

    relationship problems; Roberts et al. 2007),consistent with the environmental-mediation

    pathway.

    Dynamic models offerricher andmore com-

    plete accounts of the role of personality in theonset of depression. Moreover, it is importantto recognizethatdepressive disorders havebeen

    linked to multiple traits (as reviewed below),and it is likely that different personality charac-

    teristics contribute through different pathways.

    METHODOLOGICAL ISSUES

    A number of methodological issues must beconsidered in evaluating the relation between

    personality and mood disorders, including

    (a) study design, (b) heterogeneity of depressivedisorders, and (c) assessment of personality.

    Study Design

    A number of research designs can be useful

    in studying the relation between personalityand depressive disorders. The common cause,

    continuum/spectrum, precursor, and predispo-sition models would all be supported by family

    studies demonstrating personality differencesbetween nonaffected relatives of probands

    with and without a history of depression. The

    common cause, continuum/spectrum, and pre-cursor models would be supported by twin and

    genetic association studies demonstrating thatthe same genes predispose to both personality

    and depressive disorders. The precursor andpredisposition models posit that personality ab-

    normalities are trait markers and hence shouldbe present prior to the onset, and after recovery

    from, depressive episodes. Hence, these modelscan be tested by comparing individuals with a

    history of depression that is currently in remis-

    sion to persons with no history of depressionon relevant personality traits. An even stronger

    approach to testing the precursor and predispo-sition models is to use prospective longitudinal

    studies of persons with no prior history of

    mood disorder to determine whether particular

    personality traits predict the later onset ofdepressive disorder. Although no single design

    can distinguish among these four models, thecombination of designs can bolster the case

    for particular accounts. For example, finding

    substantial common genetic variance in twin

    studies, but no evidence of developmental se-quencing in longitudinal studies, wouldsupportthe common cause and continuum/spectrum

    models. In turn, these two models could becompared by examining the specificity of the

    association between trait and disorder andwhether there is a nonlinear relation between

    trait level and probability of disorder. On theother hand, if there were evidence of develop-

    mental sequencing in longitudinal studies as

    well as substantial common genetic variancein twin studies (or overlap of other etiological

    factors in other designs), it would support theprecursor model (particularly if the trait was

    also phenomenologically similar to depression).In contrast, developmental sequencing but less

    shared genetic (or other etiological) variancewould support the predisposition model. Also

    crucial for the predisposition model is evidencefrom longitudinal studies demonstrating that

    other variables (e.g., life stress) moderate ormediate the association between personality

    and subsequent depression.

    The pathoplasticity model can be evaluatedin longitudinal studies of persons with depres-

    sive disorders by examining the associationsamong personality traits and clinical features,

    course, and treatment response. Specifically,the pathoplasticity model posits that the trait

    would predict these outcomes even aftercontrolling for initial illness severity and other

    prognostic factors. Of note, an alternative ex-planation of such results is that the personality

    trait is a marker for a more severe, chronic,

    or etiologically distinct subgroup, rather thanhaving a causal influence on the expression

    of the disorder. A multiwave follow-up ofindividuals with a depressive disorder could be

    helpful in ruling out this possibility. If the traitinfluences the disorder course directly, rather

    than because it is an indicator of a latent

    www.annualreviews.org Personality and Depression 275

  • 8/2/2019 Personalidad y Depresion

    8/29

    Dysthymic disorder:a condition defined bychronic but relativelymild feelings ofdepression lasting atleast two years

    disorder class, changes in personality

    scores should predict subsequent changes inoutcomes.

    The concomitants model can be testedthrough cross-sectional studies comparing per-

    sons who are currently depressed, persons who

    have recovered from depressive episodes, and

    healthy controls. An even better approach isto conduct longitudinal studies assessing indi-viduals when they are in a depressive episode

    and again after they have recovered. If person-ality measures are abnormal during depressive

    episodes but not after recovery, it would sug-gest that they are concomitantsof thedepressed

    state. Multilevel analyses can also be used toseparate personality variance into trait and state

    components and to test whether state variance

    is associated with concurrent measures of de-pression (e.g., Duncan-Jones et al. 1990).

    The consequences (or scar) hypothesis canbe evaluated by assessing persons before and

    after a first depressive episode. If personalitydeviance is much greater after the episode has

    remitted, it would suggest that scarring hasoccurred.

    Testing dynamic theories requires longitu-dinal data with at least three assessment points.

    These assessments should measure relevantcontextual factors (e.g., life stress) in addition

    to depression and personality to allow the ex-

    amination of dynamic and transactional effects.Multilevel modeling and structural equation

    modeling offer powerful approaches to evalu-ating such effects with longitudinal data.

    Heterogeneity of DepressiveDisorders

    The depressive disorders are almost certainlyetiologically heterogeneous, reflecting the con-

    vergence of multiple developmental pathways.Hence, it is likely that the role of personality

    factors and, as suggested above, the applicabil-

    ity of different models of the relation betweenpersonality and depression differ for different

    forms of depression. The current classificationsystem for depressive disorders is based on

    clinical features and is a poor approximation of

    etiological distinctions. Nonetheless, it is im

    portant to consider whether the role of persoality varies as a function of the specific depre

    sive diagnosis (e.g., major depressive disordedysthymic disorder), subtype (e.g., psychot

    melancholic, atypical), and clinical characte

    istics such as age of onset, recurrence, an

    chronicity. Failure to take heterogeneity inaccount may obscure important personalitdepression associations. Conversely, perso

    ality may provide a basis for identifying mohomogeneous subgroups within the depressi

    disorders. Unfortunately,few studies of persoality and depressive disorders have attempt

    to take this heterogeneity into account.It is important to note, however, th

    associations between personality and speci

    subtypes and clinical characteristics do nnecessarily indicate etiological heterogenei

    Instead, they could reflect pathoplasticiin which personality influences sympto

    presentation and/or course, but the primaetiological process is the same, or they could r

    flect differential severity of subtypes that resuin quantitative differences in their trait profil

    Finally, a significant source of heterogenein depression is comorbidity with other form

    of psychopathology. Given the high rates comorbidity, particularly with the anxie

    disorders, associations between personal

    and depressive disorders may actually reflethe relation of personality with a co-occurrin

    nonmood disorder. Indeed, personality may a third variable that explains broad patterns

    comorbidity among many disorders. For example, recent hierarchical models of classificatio

    posit that trait dispositions such as N/Naccount for much of the comorbidity betwe

    depression and other disorders (Griffith et 2010, Kotov et al. 2007). Thus, it is importa

    for researchers to consider whether trahave specific relations with depression ov

    and above more general associations with t

    broader group of internalizing disorders.

    Assessment

    Temperament/personality can be assessed u

    ing a variety of methods, including self-repo

    276 Klein Kotov Bufferd

  • 8/2/2019 Personalidad y Depresion

    9/29

    inventories, semistructured interviews, infor-

    mants reports, and observations in naturalis-tic settings and the laboratory. Unfortunately,

    most of the literature examining the associa-tion between personality and depressive dis-

    orders has assessed personality via self-report.

    This is potentially problematic because self-

    reports of personality can be complicated bycurrent mood state, limited insight, responsestyles, and the difficulty of distinguishing traits

    from the effects of stable environmental con-texts (Chmielewski & Watson 2009). In addi-

    tion, when the same individual provides infor-mation on both personality and depression, as

    has been the case in almost all studies in thisarea, common method variance can inflate as-

    sociations. Hence, there is a need for greater

    use of informant report and observationalmeasures.

    A second issue concerns the overlapbetween some personality constructs and

    psychopathology (Lahey 2004). For example,many items on N/NE scales are similar to de-

    pressive symptoms (Ormel et al. 2004b). Thiscan inflate associations between measures of

    personality and depression. On the other hand,personality and symptom assessments usually

    have different time frames, with trait scalesreflecting long-standing patterns and depres-

    sion measures tapping more recent experiences

    (e.g., past week, past month). This trait versusstate distinction parallels that between person-

    ality and other related constructs. For example,measures of N/NE and negative affect have

    nearly identical content but are distinguishedby their time frames (Watson 2000). Thus, the

    degree to which this content overlap threatensthe validity of personality-psychopathology

    research depends, at least in part, on theduration/chronicity of the disorders of interest.

    The extent to which this is a concern also de-pendson ones modelof personality-depression

    relations. From the continuum/spectrum per-

    spective,personality and depression are variantsof the same phenomenon, so the two constructs

    should overlap. In contrast, the predispositionmodel views personality and depression as

    distinct domains, so from this perspective it

    DSM-IV: Diagand Statistical Mof Mental Disord

    Fourth Edition

    is important to define and assess these sets of

    constructs as independently as possible and tojudiciously delineate their time frames.

    AFFECTIVE TEMPERAMENTS

    The classical European descriptive psy-

    chopathologists in the late-nineteenth andearly-twentieth centuries observed that manypatients with mood disorders, as well as

    their relatives, exhibited particular patternsof premorbid personalities that appeared to

    be attenuated versions of their illnesses. Forexample, Kraepelin (1921) described four

    patterns of personality that he consideredthe fundamental states underlying manic-

    depressive illness: depressive, manic, irritable,

    and cyclothymic temperament. He believedthat these were precursors or rudimentary

    forms of the major mood disorders. Schneider(1958) described similar types; however, he

    viewed them as personality disorders that werenot necessarily related to the mood disorders.

    Two variants of these types, cyclothymicdisorder and dysthymic disorder, are included

    as mood disorder diagnoses in the Diagnostic

    and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders,

    Fourth Edition (DSM-IV; Am. Psychiatr. Assoc.1994). However, these disorders are defined

    as fairly severe conditions, with the criteria

    emphasizing symptomatology rather thanpersonality traits. As a result, these categories

    appear to be limited to the more severe,symptomatic manifestations of the affective

    temperaments described by Kraepelin andSchneider (Akiskal 1989).

    On the basis of Kraepelins and Schneidersdescriptions, Akiskal (1989) proposed formal

    criteria for the affective temperament types,and he and his colleagues developed interview

    and self-report measures of these constructsthat have been applied in a number of settings

    and cultures (e.g., Akiskal et al. 2005). Akiskals

    work also provided the basis for including de-pressive temperament as a personality disorder

    in the DSM-IV appendix. Of the four affectivetemperament types, depressive temperament

    has been the most systematically studied in

    www.annualreviews.org Personality and Depression 277

  • 8/2/2019 Personalidad y Depresion

    10/29

    Major depressivedisorder (MDD):a conditioncharacterized byepisodes of depressedmood or loss of

    interest or pleasurelasting at least twoweeks

    relation to nonbipolar depressive disorders.

    The terms depressive temperament, depres-sive personality, and depressive personality

    disorder have been used interchangeablyin the literature to refer to the following

    constellation of traits: introversion, passivity,

    and nonassertiveness; gloominess, cheer-

    lessness, and joylessness; self-reproach andself-criticism; pessimism, guilt, and remorse;being critical and judgmental of others; consci-

    entiousness and self-discipline; brooding andgiven to worry; and feelings of inadequacy and

    low self-esteem.Data on the nature of the relation between

    depressive personality and depressive disordersare consistent with most of the causal models

    described above, illustrating the complexity

    of the associations between personality anddepression. The strongest support for the com-

    mon cause and continuum/spectrum modelsderives from twin and family studies. In a large

    twin study, rstavik et al. (2007) found that de-pressive personality and majordepressive disor-

    der (MDD) shared substantial genetic variance,although there was evidence for unique genetic

    factors as well. Family studies indicate thatindividuals with depressive personality have an

    increased rate of mood disorders in their first-degree relatives (e.g., Klein & Miller 1993).

    In addition, patients with MDD, particularly

    those with chronic forms of depression, haveelevated levels of depressive personality traits

    in their first-degree relatives (Klein 1999).Consistent with Kraepelins (1921) retro-

    spective observations, prospective longitudinaldata indicate that depressive personality traits

    precede theonsetof depressive disorders.Kwonet al. (2000) found that young women with de-

    pressive personality and no comorbid Axis Iand II disorders had a significantly increased

    risk of developing dysthymic disorder (but notMDD) over the course of a three-year follow-

    up. Taken together with the twin and family

    studies, these findings provide compelling sup-port for the precursor model. In addition, in

    light of the conceptual issue regarding traitsandstates raised above for the continuum/spectrum

    and precursor models, it is noteworthy that

    depressive personality is most closely associat

    with chronic forms of depression at both tfamily and individual levels.

    Evidence also supports the predispositio

    pathoplasticity, and consequences modeRudolph & Klein (2009) reported that you

    with elevated levels of depressive personal

    traits experienced a significant increase depressive symptoms 12 months later. Whconsistent with the precursor model, it is not

    worthy that this association was moderatby pubertal status and timing. Thus, you

    with elevated levels of depressive personaltraits and more advanced pubertal stat

    and earlier pubertal timing experienced tgreatest increase in depressive symptoms. Th

    supports the predisposition model, suggesti

    that depressive personality traits confer vulneability to depression in the presence of oth

    maturational and psychosocial processes.Depressive personality also appears to ha

    a pathoplastic effect on the course of depresive disorders, predicting poorer outcomes an

    response to treatment (Laptook et al. 200Ryder et al. 2010). Moreover, Rudolph & Kle

    (2009) recently reported preliminary suppofor the consequences model, at least in yout

    They found that in a sample of early adlescents, higher levels of depressive symptom

    predicted an increase in depressive personali

    traits 12 months later. Finally, the limited eidence available suggests that semi-structur

    interview assessments of depressive personalitraits are not influenced by a depressive episo

    (Klein 1990), arguing against the concomitanmodel.

    Although the work on affective tempeaments is important in understanding t

    development of depressive disorders, it unlikely that these types actually reflect bas

    temperamental processes that originate

    early childhood, as their defining featurinclude a number of developmentally compl

    cognitive and interpersonal characteristiInstead, these temperament types are mo

    likely to be intermediate outcomes that reflethe interaction of more basic temperame

    traits that are elaborated over development

    278 Klein Kotov Bufferd

  • 8/2/2019 Personalidad y Depresion

    11/29

    conjunction with early socialization and other

    environment influences.In recent years, considerable evidence

    has accumulated indicating that depressivepersonality is associated with several of the

    basic personality trait dimensions discussed

    below, particularly high N/NE and low E/PE

    and a number of their facets (e.g., Huprich2003, Vachon et al. 2009).

    PERSONALITY TRAITDIMENSIONS

    The affective temperaments are conceptualized

    within a categorical framework. In contrast,most of the other work on personality and

    depression views personality in dimensionalterms. In this section, we focus on the FFM,

    but we also briefly consider Grays (1994) and

    Cloninger and colleagues (1993) psychobi-ological models and several additional traits

    from the clinical literature (e.g., self-criticism,dependency, and rumination). This section

    focuses primarily on studies of adults andadolescents using self-report measures of

    personality. Studies of younger children usingobservational measures of temperament are

    reviewed in a later section.

    The Five-Factor Model

    Cross-sectional associations. In their influ-

    ential theory of personality and depression,Clark & Watson (1999, Clark et al. 1994)

    posited that depressive disorders are character-ized by high levels of N/NE and low levels of

    E/PE. A large number of cross-sectional stud-ies have evaluated these relations as well as the

    links between depression and the other FFMdimensions. Kotov et al. (2010) recently con-

    ducted a meta-analysis of this literature, whichrevealed that MDD is associated with very high

    N/NE (Cohens d = 1.33) and low conscien-

    tiousness (d = 0.90). The link to low E/PEwas more modest (d = 0.62) and inconsis-

    tent, with some studies finding positive effects.The associations with the other two traits were

    weak and unremarkable. The N/NE finding is

    consistent with expectations, but the effect for

    E/PE was smaller and that for conscientious-nesswas larger thananticipated. Dysthymicdis-

    order exhibited a more extreme profile with re-markably strong and consistent links to E/PE

    (d = 1.47), N/NE (d = 1.93), and conscien-

    tiousness (d = 1.24). This is not surprising

    as dysthymic disorder is thought to be moretrait-like than MDD, and a greater contribu-tion from personality might be expected.

    To determine whether the observed per-sonality links are specific to depression, Kotov

    et al. (2010) also examined personality profilesof anxiety disorders. They found that with the

    exception of specific phobia, which had rela-tively weak associations with all five traits, all

    anxiety disorders showed stronger effects on

    N/NE, E/PE, and conscientiousness (averaged = 1.91, 1.05, 1.02, respectively) than did

    MDD. Several also scored above dysthymic dis-order on N/NE. Dysthymia had stronger asso-

    ciations than anxiety disorders on the other twotraits, but the differences were slight.

    It is conceivable that more specific associ-ations were not evident because these analyses

    focused on broad personality dimensions.Narrow traits that comprise the general di-

    mensions may have stronger associations withdepressive disorders. Indeed, self-harma

    component of N/NE that reflects propensity to

    self-deprecation and self-injurywas found tocontribute to depression even after controlling

    for the broad traits, and this effect was specificrelative to other common mental disorders

    (Watson et al. 2006). With regard to E/PE,evidence is emerging that the positive affec-

    tivity facet, but not the sociability/extraversionfacet, is related to depression (Durbin et al.

    2005, Naragon-Gainey et al. 2009). This mayexplain the surprisingly modest association

    between MDD and E/PE, if this general

    trait includes much variance not relevant todepression. Thus, facet-level research promises

    to yield stronger and more specific evidence ofpersonality-depression links.

    Evidence bearing on causal models. Be-

    cause most attempts to tease apart explanatory

    www.annualreviews.org Personality and Depression 279

  • 8/2/2019 Personalidad y Depresion

    12/29

    models of the association between depression

    and personality have focused on N/NE andE/PE, we consider only these two traits in this

    section. The section is organized by the type ofresearch design used to address the models.

    Personality during and after a depres-

    sive episode. Studies of personality and psy-chopathology may be complicated by the in-fluence of participants mood states on reports

    of their personalities (the concomitants model).For example, many studies have found that in-

    dividuals with MDD report higher levels ofN/NE when they are depressed than when

    they are not depressed (Hirschfeld et al. 1983b,Kendler et al. 1993, Ormel et al. 2004a). In

    contrast, the evidence for mood state effects on

    E/PE is weaker and less consistent (de Fruytet al. 2006, Kendler et al. 1993, Morey et al.

    2010). However, the influence of mood stateon personality should not be overstated. Even

    though levels of N/NE decline significantly af-ter remission from a depressive episode (i.e.,

    absolute stability), individuals relative posi-tions with respect to levels of N/NE (i.e., rank-

    order stability) tend to be moderately well pre-served (de Fruyt et al. 2006, Morey et al. 2010).

    Moreover, clinical trials suggest that changesin depressive symptoms are not necessarily ac-

    companied by changes in personality (Quilty

    et al. 2008b, Tang et al. 2009).

    Cross-sectional comparisons of remitted pa-

    tients and controls. A number of early stud-

    ies used remission designs, comparing patientswho had recovered from a depressive episode to

    never-depressed controls or population normson self-rated personality traits. These studies

    found that E/PE is significantly lower in for-merly depressed patients than in healthy con-

    trols (Hirschfeld et al. 1983a, Reich et al. 1987),arguing against the concomitants model and in

    favor of the precursor, predisposition, and/or

    consequences models. However, the results forN/NE were less consistent (Hirschfeld et al.

    1983a, Reich et al. 1987). This inconsistencymay be due to a number of factors, includ-

    ing insufficiently stringent criteria for recovery,

    thereby possibly confounding personality an

    residual symptoms; using normative data colected by other investigators, which may intr

    duce demographic and sociocultural differenc

    between the formerly depressed and comparson samples; and selection effects, as N/NE

    associated with a poorer course (discussed b

    low) and thus samples of remitted depressivmay include a disproportionate number wilow levels of this trait.

    Personality before and after a depressi

    episode. Several studies have tested the consquences (or scar) hypothesis by comparing pe

    sonality measures in depressed individuals bfore and after a MDD episode. The results

    these studies have been inconsistent. Kendl

    andcolleagues reported increasesin N/NE (bnot E/PE) after a depressive episode in tw

    separate samples (Fanous et al. 2007, Kendlet al. 1993); however, other studies have foun

    that N/NE and E/PE do not change from bfore to after a MDD episode (e.g., Ormel et

    2004a, Shea et al. 1996). Importantly, the stuies reportingscarring used less stringentcriter

    for recovery and shorter follow-ups, suggestithat the findings may be due to residual sym

    toms and/or that the scars dissipate over tim

    Personality in relatives of depressed ind

    viduals. A number of studies have tested tcommon cause, continuum/spectrum, precu

    sor, and predisposition models by comparinpersonality traits in the never-depressed rel

    tives of patients with mood disordersand nevedepressed controls (e.g., Farmer et al. 200

    Hecht et al. 1998, Ouimette et al. 1996). Thresults have been mixed, with some studies r

    porting higher N/NE and/or lower E/PE the never-depressed relatives of probands wi

    mood disorders, and other studies reporting n

    differences. However, interpretation of thestudies is complicated by two factors. First, pe

    sonality traits may not play the same role risk for depression among familial as nonf

    milial forms of depression. Second, there mbe selection biases in samples using well rel

    tives who are already partly through the ri

    280 Klein Kotov Bufferd

  • 8/2/2019 Personalidad y Depresion

    13/29

    period for mood disorder. Thus, those rela-

    tives with the strongest personality vulnerabil-ities may have already developed the disorder

    and be excluded from the study.

    Twin studies. As discussed above, a valu-able approach to testing the common cause,

    continuum/spectrum, and precursor models isthrough twin studies. These studies indicate

    that there are substantial associations between

    the liabilities for N/NE and MDD, but onlyweak associations between the genetic liabili-

    ties for E/PE and MDD (Fanous et al. 2007;Kendler et al. 1993, 2006).

    Prospective longitudinal studies. The most di-

    rect approach to testing the precursor and pre-disposition models is to conduct prospective

    studies of personality in never-depressed partic-ipants to determine whether personality char-

    acteristics predict the subsequent onset of de-pressive disorders. Several studies using large

    community samples have reported that higherlevels of N/NE predict theonset of first lifetime

    MDD episodes (de Graaf et al. 2002; Fanous

    et al. 2007; Kendler et al. 1993, 2006; Ormelet al. 2004a). In addition, several studies us-

    ing measures of other traits that overlap withN/NE or its facets have reported similar find-

    ings (Hirschfeld et al. 1989, Rorsman et al.1993). Although there is some evidence that

    E/PE predicts the first onset of MDD (Kendleret al. 2006, Rorsman et al. 1993), it is much

    weaker, and several studies have failed to findan association (Fanous et al. 2007, Hirschfeld

    et al. 1989, Kendler et al. 1993).

    Personality and the subsequent course of de-

    pression. Finally, there is evidence that both

    N/NE and E/PE have pathoplastic influenceson the course of depression after the onset of

    the disorder. For example, many studies have

    reported that higher N/NE and lower E/PEpredict a poorer course and response to treat-

    ment, although thefindings regardingE/PE areslightly less consistent (de Fruyt et al. 2006,

    Duggan et al. 1990, Morris et al. 2009, Quilty

    et al. 2008a, Tang et al. 2009). As noted above,

    however, these findings are also consistent withdiagnostic heterogeneity, such that personal-

    ity dysfunction is a marker for a more severeor etiologically distinct group. Indeed, there is

    evidence that the nonmelancholic subtype is

    characterized by more vulnerable personality

    styles than is melancholia and that chronic de-pressions are associated with higher N/NE andlower E/PE than is nonchronic MDD (Klein

    2008, Kotov et al. 2010).

    Evidence relevant to dynamic models.

    Transactions between N/NE and environmen-

    tal contexts have received the most attentionin the literature (Ormel & de Jong 1999, van

    Os & Jones 1999). N/NE shows reciprocal

    relations with a range of significant life expe-riences, such as initiation and break-up of a

    committed relationship, relationship quality,occupational attainment, and financial security

    (Neyer & Lehnart 2007, Roberts et al. 2003,Scollon & Diener 2006). Furthermore, N/NE

    has been repeatedly implicated in the gen-eration of stressful life events (Kercher et al.

    2009, Lahey 2009, Middeldorp et al. 2008),which suggests an environmentally mediated

    relationship between this trait and depression.The environmentally moderated mechanism

    has also received support, as several studies

    found that N/NE interacts with stressful lifeevents to predict first onset of major depression

    (Kendler et al. 2004, Ormel et al. 2001, van Os& Jones 1999).

    E/PE has demonstrated bidirectional effectswith many significant social and occupational

    experiences (Neyer & Lehnart 2007, Robertset al. 2003, Scollon & Diener 2006). In addi-

    tion, a decrease in E/PE over time was foundto predict future internalizing problems (Van

    den Akker et al. 2010). However, little atten-

    tion has been given to mechanisms underlyingthe association between this trait and depres-

    sion. Support for an environmentally mediatedeffect is limited and mixed (Middeldorp et al.

    2008, Wetter & Hankin2009), andthe environ-mental moderation model is largely untested,

    although there is some evidence that positive

    www.annualreviews.org Personality and Depression 281

  • 8/2/2019 Personalidad y Depresion

    14/29

    affect moderates the effects of daily stressors on

    depressive symptoms (Wichers et al. 2007).Finally, conscientiousness may play an im-

    portant role in dynamic models of personality-depression relations. It has reciprocal associa-

    tions with familysupport, divorce, occupational

    attainment, and job involvement (Roberts et al.

    2003, Roberts & Bogg 2004). Conscientious-ness is hypothesized to influence depressionby increasing exposure to negative life events

    (Anderson & McLean 1997, Compas et al.2004), but mediation and moderation effects

    have not been tested.

    Interactions between temperament dimen-

    sions. Finally, personality-depression rela-

    tions may be multivariate, rather than bivariate,

    with multiple traits interacting to influ-ence depression. Indeed, in their influential

    model of personality and depression, Clark &Watson (1999, Clark et al. 1994) hypothesized

    that depression is characterized by high N/NEand low E/PE, raising the possibility that it is

    the combination of thetwo traits that is particu-larly important in depressive disorders. A grow-

    ing number of studies have reported that the in-teraction of high N/NE and low E/PE predicts

    subsequent depressive symptoms or disordersin adults and youth (Gershuny & Sher 1998,

    Joiner & Lonigan 2000, Wetter & Hankin

    2009), although several studies have not foundsuch an interaction (Jorm et al. 2000, Kendler

    et al. 2006, Verstraeten et al. 2009). The inter-action between N/NE and conscientiousness is

    also of interest, as the latter construct includesaspects of self-regulation and effortful control

    (Rothbart & Bates 2006) and may therefore re-flect the ability to modulate ones affective reac-

    tivity. Indeed, there is cross-sectional evidencethat effortful control moderates the association

    between N/NE and depressive symptoms inadolescents (Verstraeten et al. 2009).

    Summary and discussion. Cross-sectionalstudies have documented strong links of

    depressive disorders to N/NE, conscien-tiousness, and E/PE, although the latter

    effect is substantial in dysthymic disorder but

    only moderate in MDD. In fact, personali

    generally appears to play a greater role dysthymia. None of these relations are specifi

    however, as anxiety disorders have very simil

    trait profiles. This observation argues againthe continuum/spectrum model at least wi

    regard to these broad dimensions. It may

    possible to find traits that are specific to dpression by targeting lower-order personalidimensions. Narrower traits may also expla

    the surprisingly modest link between E/PE aMDD, as some, but not other, facets of th

    general dimension are relevant to depressionThe nature of relations between the

    personality traits and depression is compleand our understanding is still limited. N/N

    the most widely studied personality trait

    depression, raises challenging conceptual anmethodological issues due to the overl

    between some of its features and depressisymptoms (Ormel et al. 2004b). Nonethele

    this cannot completely explain the associatibetween these constructs (Tang et al. 2009

    N/NE is moderately influenced by clinical sta(the concomitants model), shares comm

    etiological influences with MDD (commcause, continuum/spectrum, and precurs

    models), predicts the subsequent onset MDD (precursor and predisposition model

    and influences the course of depression (path

    plasticity model). In addition, N/NE appeato contribute to subsequent stress and adversi

    and increases the risk of depression in the faof negative life events (predisposition mode

    Finally, it may also be changed by experienceMDD episodes (consequences model), but t

    evidence for this is weaker and less consistenThe role played by E/PE in depression is le

    clear. Its cross-sectional association with dythymia is substantial, but its relation to MDD

    more modest. E/PE is not influenced by clini

    state or changed by the experience of depresive episodes. It appears to be abnormally lo

    even during remission, which is consistent withe continuation of trait deviance from the pr

    morbid stage (precursor or predisposition acounts). Moreover, low E/PE tends to predi

    a poorer course of depression. However, t

    282 Klein Kotov Bufferd

  • 8/2/2019 Personalidad y Depresion

    15/29

    degree of shared etiological influences between

    E/PE and MDD is low, and the evidence thatE/PE predicts theonset of MDD in prospective

    longitudinal studies is weak. As noted above,three possible reasons for the weaker and less

    consistent findings regarding E/PE are (a) it

    plays a greater role in some forms of depres-

    sion than others (e.g., chronic depressions);(b) only some facets of the broader trait (e.g.,low positive affective and approach motivation)

    are related to depression; and (c) E/PE maymake a greater contribution to depression by

    moderating N/NE than as a main effect (Olinoet al. 2010).

    Finally, there appears to be a strong negativeassociation between conscientiousness and de-

    pression, at least in cross-sectional studies. This

    may appear surprising in light of the positiveassociations discussed below between depres-

    sion and a number of other constructs thatare thought to be related to conscientiousness,

    such as behavioral inhibition system sensitivity,harm avoidance, perfectionism, and temper-

    amental behavioral inhibition. It is importantto note, however, that these latter constructs

    are more strongly associated with high N/NE,and in some cases, low E/PE, than with consci-

    entiousness (de Fruyt et al. 2000, Muris et al.2009a, Smits & Boeck 2006). Hence, it is likely

    that their positive correlations with depression

    are driven by their shared variance with highN/NE. Unfortunately, few studies testing

    causal models of personality and depressionhave considered conscientiousness. However,

    evidence indicating that this trait may mod-erate the effects of N/NE on depression and

    that it increases the likelihood of subsequentadversity that could then, in turn, produce

    depression suggests that further research onthe role of conscientiousness is warranted.

    Psychobiological Models

    Grays model. Grays (e.g., Gray 1994) influ-ential theory proposes that there are two major

    neurobehavioral systems that underlie behav-ior: the behavioral activation system (BAS),

    which responds to signals of reward, and the be-

    BAS: behavioraactivation system

    BIS: behaviorainhibition system

    havioral inhibition system (BIS), which is sensi-

    tive to cues for punishment. Although BAS andBIS differ conceptually and empirically from

    E/PE and N/NE, their relations with depres-sion are thought to be similar. Thus, it has been

    hypothesized that depression is associated with

    reduced BAS and/or heightened BIS sensitivity

    (Depue & Iacono 1989, Gray 1994). Althoughmuch of this work has focused on bipolar disor-der (e.g., Alloy et al. 2008, Johnson et al. 2008),

    several recent studies have examined self-reportmeasures of BAS and BIS sensitivity in MDD.

    Consistent with Grays model, compared withhealthy controls, currently depressed patients

    report lower levels of BAS and higher levels ofBIS, and patients with a past history of MDD

    report lower levels of BAS (Pinto-Meza et al.

    2006). In addition, lower BAS sensitivity, butnot higher BIS sensitivity, is associated with a

    poorer course of MDD (e.g., Kasch et al. 2002,McFarland et al. 2006), suggesting that BAS

    may have a pathoplastic effect on depression.

    Cloningers model. Cloninger (e.g.,Cloninger et al. 1993) has proposed a

    model of personality that includes four tem-perament and three character dimensions.

    The temperament dimensions include noveltyseeking (an appetitive/approach system), harm

    avoidance (an inhibition/avoidance system),

    reward dependency (a system that is responsiveto signals of social approval and attachment),

    and persistence. The character dimensions areself-directedness (responsible, goal-directed),

    cooperativeness (helpful, empathic versushostile and alienated), and self-transcendence

    (imaginative, unconventional). Harm avoid-ance is conceptually and empirically associated

    with BIS, and novelty seeking and persis-tence are associated with BAS. Similarly,

    harm avoidance is positively correlated withN/NE and negatively associated with E/PE,

    self-directedness is negatively correlated with

    N/NE, and novelty seeking and persistenceare associated with E/PE (e.g., de Fruyt et al.

    2000).A number of studies have reported that pa-

    tients with MDD report higher levels of harm

    www.annualreviews.org Personality and Depression 283

  • 8/2/2019 Personalidad y Depresion

    16/29

    avoidance and lower levels of self-directedness

    than do healthy controls (e.g., Celikel et al.2009). Most of the traits in Cloningers system

    are influenced by the respondents mood state(e.g., Farmer et al. 2003); however, abnormal

    levels of harm avoidance and self-directedness

    are present even after remission (e.g., Smith

    et al. 2005). Increased harm avoidance andlower self-directedness are also characteristicof most anxiety disorders, indicating that these

    effects are not specific to MDD (Ongur et al.2005).

    Few studies have explicitly tested the com-mon cause, precursor, predisposition, and con-

    sequences hypotheses for Cloningers model.Farmer et al. (2003) found that the never-

    depressed siblings of patients with MDD re-

    ported significantly greater harm avoidanceand less self-directedness than did the never-

    depressed siblings of healthy controls. In addi-tion, Cloninger et al. (2006) reported that in a

    large community sample, high harm avoidanceand persistence and low self-directedness pre-

    dicted an increase in self-reported depressivesymptoms 12 months later. A larger number of

    studies have addressed the pathoplasticity hy-pothesis, albeit with mixed results. Low harm

    avoidance, self-directedness, and reward de-pendency have predicted a poorer response to

    treatmentin some, but not all, studies; theother

    dimensions have generally not been associatedwith course and treatmentoutcome ( Joyce et al.

    2007, Kennedy et al. 2005, Morris et al. 2009).

    Clinical Traits

    Independent of the traditional personality field,clinical researchers have developed a number of

    trait-like constructs to describe dispositions todepression. These clinical traits are similar in

    scope to personality facets, and their stability iscomparable to that of a typical personality di-

    mension (e.g., Kasch et al. 2001, Zuroff et al.

    2004). Also, factor analytic studies have shownthat most of these clinical traits can be success-

    fully incorporated in the personality taxonomyas components of neuroticism (Watson et al.

    2006). Next, we brieflydiscuss threeof themost

    studied constructs: ruminative response sty

    self-criticism, and dependency.Ruminative response style, a tendency

    dwell on sad mood and thoughts (NoleHoeksema 1991), is correlated with concurre

    depressive symptoms and predicts future sym

    toms as well as increases in symptoms over tim

    (Rood et al. 2009). Also, one study reportthat ruminative response style prospectivepredicts onset of MDD (Nolen-Hoeksem

    2000). The trait has also been linked to anxiedisorders, but the association with depressio

    is appreciably stronger (Cox et al. 200Nolen-Hoeksema et al. 2008).

    Blatts (1974, 1991) theory of depressiofocuses on two trait vulnerabilities: se

    criticism (an inclination to feelings of gu

    and failure stemming from unrealistically hiexpectations for oneself) and dependency

    disposition to feelings of helplessness and feaof abandonment resulting from a preoccup

    tion with relationships). These constructs asimilar, although not identical, to Becks (198

    constructs of autonomy and sociotropy. Studindicate that the link between dependen

    and depressive disorders is relatively weak annonspecific, whereas self-criticism has be

    established as an important and specific factin these conditions (Zuroff et al. 2004). Bo

    traits have been conceptualized as dynam

    predispositions to depressive disorders, athere is some support for this view, includin

    evidence of transactions with life stress as was environmental mediation and moderati

    of personality effects (Zuroff et al. 2004). Secriticism, and to a lesser extent dependenc

    have also been found to predict future increasin depressive symptoms. In addition, there

    evidence that dependency predicts the subsquent onset of major depression in older, b

    notyounger, individuals (Hirschfeld et al. 198Rohde et al. 1990). The concomitants a

    pathoplasty models have also received empir

    cal support (Zuroff et al. 2004). Finally, theresome research indicating that dependency m

    increase as a function of depressive episod(consequences model) in youth but not adu

    (Rohde et al. 1990, 1994; Shea et al. 1996).

    284 Klein Kotov Bufferd

  • 8/2/2019 Personalidad y Depresion

    17/29

    As noted above, all of these constructs are

    stronglylinkedtoN/NE(Coxetal.2001,Kaschet al. 2001), and some (particularly rumination

    and self-criticism) can be considered facets ofthis broader trait (Watson et al. 2006). Lower-

    order facets can account for variance over and

    above that of higher-order traits (Paunonen

    & Ashton 2001), and several cross-sectionalstudies have supported the incremental validityof ruminative response style and self-criticism

    (Cox et al. 2004, Muris et al. 2009b) in associa-tions with depressive symptoms. However, this

    issue requires more research, particularly usinglongitudinal designs.

    CHILD TEMPERAMENT

    Most of the literature on personality and de-

    pression has focused on adolescents and adults.

    Research that is grounded in the child temper-ament literature in developmental psychology

    has the potential to extend existing work onpersonality in depression by (a) providing the

    strongest test of the precursor and predisposi-tion models; (b) more precisely delineating the

    behavioral manifestations of temperamentalvulnerabilities to mood disorders in young

    children; (c) tracing the development and conti-nuity of trait vulnerabilities across the lifespan;

    and (d) examining the neurobiological, cog-nitive, and interpersonal processes that may

    mediate the association between early tempera-

    ment traits and the subsequent development ofdepressive disorder (Compas et al. 2004, Klein

    et al. 2008a, Kovacs & Lopez-Duran 2010).The early childhood temperament dimen-

    sions that have received the greatest attentionwith respect to depression are N/NE, E/PE,

    andbehavioral inhibition (BI). BI refersto wari-ness, fear, and low exploration in novel situa-

    tions (Kagan et al. 1987). It combines aspectsof N/NE (fear and anxiety), E/PE (low ap-

    proach), and conscientiousness (constraint/ ef-

    fortful control) that do not have a direct analogin most models of adult personality.

    Cross-sectional and longitudinal studies ofolder children and adolescents using self-report

    measures have generally reported associations

    BI: behavioralinhibition

    of low E/PE and high N/NE with depression

    similar to those in the adult literature (e.g.,Lonigan et al. 2003).5 Observational studies of

    younger children of depressed mothers also in-dicate that these traits may be associated with

    risk for depression (Kovacs & Lopez-Duran

    2010). For example, in a community sample of

    100 three-year-olds, Durbin et al. (2005) re-ported that children of mothers with a historyof mood disorder exhibited low PE in emotion-

    eliciting laboratory tasks. Importantly, this ef-fect was limited to the affective (positive af-

    fect) and motivational (approach/engagement),rather than the interpersonal (sociability), com-

    ponents of PE. Furthermore, low PE at age3 predicted depressotypic cognition and mem-

    ory biases at age 7 (Hayden et al. 2006) and

    parent-reported depressive symptoms at age 10(Dougherty et al. 2010).

    Subsequently, using a larger communitysample (N = 543), Olino et al. (2010) found

    that preschool-aged children of parents with ahistory of depression had higher levels of NE

    and BI. However, both main effects were qual-ified by interactions with child PE. At high

    and moderate (but not low) levels of child PE,higher levels of NE and BI were each associ-

    ated with higher rates of parental depression.Conversely, at low (but not high and moder-

    ate) levels of child NE, low PE was associated

    with higher rates of parental depression. Takentogether, these results suggest that children of

    depressed parents may exhibit diminished PEor elevated NE and BI. In this latter sample,

    low PE was also associated with elevated levelsof cortisol shortly after awakening, an index of

    hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal axis dysregula-tion that has been shown to predict MDD in

    adolescents and adults (Dougherty et al. 2009).In both the Durbin et al. (2005) and Olino

    et al. (2010) studies, the child temperamentparental psychopathology associations were

    5Few studies have examined the association of conscientious-ness/effortful control with depression in youth, but analo-gous to the adult literature, there is cross-sectional evidencethat effortful control is negatively correlated with depression(Verstraeten et al. 2009).

    www.annualreviews.org Personality and Depression 285

  • 8/2/2019 Personalidad y Depresion

    18/29

    specific to depression. However, other work

    suggests that children of parents with anxietydisorders may also exhibit elevated BI. For ex-

    ample, Rosenbaum et al. (2000) assessed BI us-inglaboratorymeasuresin2-to6-year-oldchil-

    dren of parents with a history of MDD and/or

    panic disorder and parents with no history of

    mood or anxiety disorders. Children of patientswith both MDD and panic disorder exhibitedsignificantlygreater BI than did children of par-

    ents with no history of mood or anxiety dis-order. Children of parents with panic disor-

    der alone and children of parents with MDDalone had intermediate levels of BI that did

    not differ significantly from children of par-ents in the comorbid and no-psychopathology

    groups.

    Finally, there is some direct evidence thatpersonality traits assessed in childhood predict

    the development of depressive disorders inadults. Caspi et al. (1996)reported that children

    who were rated as socially reticent, inhibited,and easily upset at age 3 had elevated rates of

    depressive (but not anxiety or substance use)disorders at age 21. Moreover, van Os et al.

    (1997) found that physicians ratings of behav-ioral apathy at ages 6, 7, and 11 were predictive

    of both adolescent mood disorder and chronicdepression in middle adulthood. However, BI

    appears to predict the development of anxiety

    disorders at least as strongly as depression(Hirshfeld-Becker et al. 2008).

    CLINICAL IMPLICATIONS

    Prevention

    Personality researchhas importantimplications

    for the prevention of depression. Meta-analyticevidence indicates that existing preventive

    interventions can reduce the incidence of de-pressive disordersby 25%(Cuijpers et al. 2008).

    However, the available strategies are a mix of

    universal (intervention is administered to theentire population), selective (to a well-defined

    at-risk group), and indicated (to those withsubthreshold disorder) approaches. Universal

    interventions are costly, lack a personalized

    focus, and require very large samples to yie

    detectable effects, whereas indicated intervetions may be better described as treatment th

    prevention (Munoz et al. 2010). In contraselective interventions are true preventi

    measures that are cost effective and can

    tailored to a specific mechanism of risk. How

    ever, implementation of selective strategrequires knowledge of risk factors and causprocesses that lead from the vulnerability to t

    disorder.The majority of established risk factors f

    depressive disorders are either immutable (e.demographic characteristics, family history)

    predict onset only in theshort term (e.g., streful lifeevents). In contrast, personality is at le

    somewhat malleable, especially in youth, b

    may forecast the onset of depression years advance, which makes traitsa potentially attra

    tive means of identifying individuals at risk aninforming selection of interventions. Differe

    trait-disorder pathways wouldpoint to differepreventive strategies; hence, further resear

    on the nature of personality-depression reltions can significantly facilitate developme

    of preventive interventions. Another advantaof traits is that they can be assessed relative

    easily and efficiently and thus are ideal fscreening.

    Treatment Response

    Personality also can inform treatment depressive disorders post onset. In particula

    traits can predict response to treatmenSubstantial evidence has accumulated th

    individuals with lower N/NE have better treament outcomes across modalities (Kenne

    et al. 2005, Mulder 2002, Tang et al. 2009Other Big Five traits have been studied less a

    their role is not yet certain. However, a recelarge investigation of a combination interve

    tion (medication plus psychotherapy) foun

    that low N/NE and high conscientiousnepredicted who would respond to treatme

    and although high E/PE did not contribudirectly, it amplified the effect of high consc

    entiousness (Quilty et al. 2008a). As discuss

    286 Klein Kotov Bufferd

  • 8/2/2019 Personalidad y Depresion

    19/29

    above, investigations of Cloningers traits have

    produced inconsistent results ( Joyce et al.2007, Kennedy et al. 2005, Mulder 2002). Few

    studies have examined personality facets, butpreliminary evidence suggests that lower-order

    traits can add substantially to the prediction of

    treatment response (Bagby et al. 2008). Among

    clinical traits, self-criticism, but not depen-dency, was found to forecast poor treatmentoutcomes (Blatt et al. 1995). Furthermore, per-

    sonality may be useful in matching patients tointerventions. For instance, Bagby et al. (2008)

    reported that patients high on N/NE or low onsome agreeableness facets respond better to an-

    tidepressant medication than to psychotherapy.The processes underlying these predictive

    associations are not entirely clear. One hypoth-

    esis is that personality change mediates the ef-fect of treatment on depression. Indeed, there

    is a fair amount of evidence that depressiontreatment reduces N/NE and increases E/PE

    (Zinbarg et al. 2008) and that this effect isnot due to confounding by the depressive state

    (Tang et al. 2009). Quilty et al. (2008b) tested amediation model and found direct support for

    this hypothesis. Other possibilities need to beruled out, however, particularly the hypotheses

    that traits predict poorer response because theyindicate a more severe form of depression or

    that they interfere with treatment compliance

    and the therapeutic relationship, thus reducingthe efficacy of the intervention.

    CONCLUSIONS AND FUTUREDIRECTIONS

    The literature on the relation between person-ality and depression is large, but it has many

    gaps and inconsistent findings. Nonetheless,it is possible to draw a number of conclu-

    sions. First, there are moderate-to-large cross-sectional associations between depression and

    three general personality traitsN/NE, E/PE,

    and conscientiousnessas well as with a vari-ety of related traits (e.g., harm avoidance, ru-

    mination, and self-criticism) and personalitytypes (depressive personality). Second, most of

    the personality traits associated with depression

    also are related to other forms of psychopathol-

    ogy,particularly anxiety disorders.Thismay re-flect the phenomenon of multifinality, in which

    variables early in the causal chain lead to multi-ple outcomes depending on subsequent events

    in the causal pathway. On the other hand, many

    of the disorders that are currently classified as

    distinct conditions are closely related; hence,research on personality-psychopathology asso-ciations can provide important information for

    revising our nosological system. Third, reportsof some traits (e.g., N/NE and harm avoidance)

    are influenced by clinical state, whereas othertraits (e.g., E/PE) appear to be independent of

    mood state. However, state effects cannot fullyaccount for the associations between person-

    ality and depression. Fourth, shared etiologi-

    cal factors (e.g., genes) account for a portion ofthe association between N/NE and depression.

    Fifth, depressive personality and some traits,particularly N/NE, predict the subsequent on-

    set of depressive disorders. However, it is un-clear at this point whether they are best con-

    ceptualized as precursors or predispositions, asit is difficult to tease these models apart, and

    there is evidence supporting both accounts.In either case, there is growing evidence that

    temperamental risk factors are evident at anearly age, suggesting a promising approach to

    identifying young children at risk for depres-

    sion. Sixth, there is evidence suggesting thatother traits, such as low E/PE and low con-

    scientiousness/effortful control, may moderatethe relationship between N/NE and depres-

    sion. Seventh, it appears unlikely that depres-sive episodes produce enduringchanges in most

    personality traits. Finally, personality traits pre-dict, and may in fact influence, the course and

    treatment response of depression.To make further progress in elucidating the

    relation between personality and mood disor-ders, future studies should be guided by six

    broad considerations. First, most of the litera-

    ture on personality and depression has focusedon the broad traits of N/NE and E/PE. There

    is a need for further work on conscientiousnessand on lower levels in the trait hierarchy (i.e.,

    facets). It is important to determine whether a

    www.annualreviews.org Personality and Depression 287

  • 8/2/2019 Personalidad y Depresion

    20/29

    more specific level of analysis will yield more

    powerful effects and increase the specificity ofassociations between personality constructs and

    particular forms of psychopathology. Clinicaltraits, such as ruminative response style and

    self-criticism, need to be included in these stud-

    ies and evaluated jointly with traditional per-

    sonality dimensions. Finally, it is importantto continue to explore interactions betweentraits.

    Second, there is a critical need for prospec-tive, longitudinal studies. Most existing longi-

    tudinal studies have begun in late adolescenceor adulthood. However, a substantial propor-

    tion of mood disorders have already developedby mid-adolescence. Therefore, in order to fur-

    ther test the precursor and predisposition mod-

    els, and to trace the developmental pathwaysbetween personality and depression, it is neces-

    sary to conduct longitudinal studies that start asearly as possible in order to obtain a sufficient

    number of first-onset cases and avoid selectionbiases caused by excluding participants who al-

    ready have a history of mood disorder at initialassessment.

    Third, depression researchers have treatedpersonality as static. However, personality

    changes over the course of development.Future work must begin to consider the

    complex personality-environment transactions

    that can strengthen or attenuate personalitytrajectories and predispositions for depressive

    disorder. In addition, as understanding ofepigenetics increases, it will be important to

    explore epigenetic influences on personal

    change and their relation to depression.Fourth, if personality is a precursor of,

    predisposes to, the development of depressidisorders, it is critical to identify the modera

    ing factors and mediating processes involved

    these pathways. There is some evidence su

    gesting that moderators may include gendeearly adversity, and life stress, and mediatomay include interpersonal deficits, depress

    typic cognitions, maladaptive coping, and bhavioral and neurobiological stress reactivi

    (Klein et al. 2008a). There is a need for mosystematic research examining these modertors and mediators in a longitudinal framewor

    Fifth, self-reports have borne the brunt most research in this area andhave made impo

    tant contributions. However, like all methodthey have limitations and cannot be applied

    all contexts (e.g., young children). Thus, theis a need for further work using complementa

    methods such as informant reports and obsevations in naturalistic and laboratory settings

    Finally, therole of personality/temperamemay differ for different forms of depressive d

    order. Personality appears to play an especia

    important role in early-onset, chronic, and rcurrent depressive conditions (e.g., Klein 200

    Kotov et al. 2010, van Os et al. 1997). Focusinon broad diagnostic categories such as MD

    mayobscure importantassociations withpartiular forms of depression; hence, future studi

    need to give greater consideration to the heerogeneity of depressive disorders.

    SUMMARY POINTS

    1. There are moderate-to-large cross-sectional associations between depression and threegeneral personality traitsN/NE, E/PE, and conscientiousnessas well as with a variety

    of related traits (e.g., harm avoidance, rumination, and self-criticism) and personality

    types (e.g., depressive personality).

    2. Most of the personality traits associated with depression also are related to other forms

    of psychopathology, particularly anxiety disorders. This may reflect the phenomenonof multifinality, in which variables early in the causal chain lead to multiple outcomes

    depending on subsequent events in the causal pathway. On the other hand, many of the

    288 Klein Kotov Bufferd

  • 8/2/2019 Personalidad y Depresion

    21/29

    disorders that are currently classified as distinct conditions are closely related; hence,research on personality-psychopathology associations can also provide important infor-

    mation for revising our nosological system.

    3. Reports of some traits (e.g., N/NE and harm avoidance) are influenced by clinical state,

    whereas other traits (e.g., E/PE) appear to be independent of mood state. However, stateeffects cannot fully account for the associations between personality and depression.

    4. Shared etiological factors (e.g., genes) account for a portion of the association between

    N/NE and depression.

    5. Depressive personality and some traits, particularly N/NE, predict the subsequent

    onset of depressive disorders. However, it is unclear at this point whether they are

    best conceptualized as precursors or predispositions, as it is difficult to tease thesemodels apart, and there is evidence supporting both accounts. In either case, there is

    growing evidence that temperamental risk factors are evident at an early age, suggestinga promising approach to identifying young children at risk for depression.

    6. There is evidence suggesting that other traits, such as low E/PE and