Creative thinking & insight tests measure the same ... · ine creative thinking. For instance, the...

7
20 | Inkblot • Vol. 5 • May 2016 Introduction e denition of creativity has eluded people for decades, largely because its multifaceted nature makes it seemingly impossible to settle on one, all-encompass- ing explanation (Pier, 2012). Rhodes’ (1961) deni- tion seems to encapsulate most elements of creativity, such as the creative person, the process, the product, and the press, of which the process refers to the cog- nitive mechanisms underlying creativity. Likewise, one of the most intensively studied phenomena in the cog- nitive psychology of problem solving is insight, com- monly understood as the ‘aha!’ feeling when one unex- pectedly stumbles upon a solution to a problem (Burns & Ellis, 1970; Davidson & Sternberg, 1995; Weisberg, 2010). Insight constitutes a set of cognitive processes for solving a problem that requires an individual to shiperspective. Insight problems require restructur- ing of the way the problem is perceived and attended to, such that when the restructuring occurs, the solution becomes immediately apparent and evokes the ‘aha!’ feeling. Creative thinking and the processes underlying insight are remarkably alike, as they both involve redi- recting or restructuring attention and they both require increasing awareness of the relationships between ele- ments of a problem. Creativity and insight tests mea- sure the same underlying cognitive processes of diver- gent thinking (DT), convergent thinking (CT), and restructuring via attention redirection, which suggests that creative thinking is simply an example of insight. Creativity Prior to explaining creative thinking, it is important to understand the concept of creativity. In attempting to dene creativity, many end up segregating the cre- ative process and the creative product (Gardner, 1988; Hanson, 2013; Ivcevic, 2009; Zeng, Proctor, & Salvendy, 2011), but rarely do they include both aspects. Pier (2012) focuses on the product in dening creativity in terms of the usefulness and originality of the product. Conversely, Mumford and Whetzel (1996) outline the various mental processes of creativity, such as how one organizes and attends to the problem, and how one can reach the solution. In trying to nd a balance between the two denitions, Zeng and colleagues (2011) dened creativity as the cognitive processes employed to cre- ate a novel and useful product. Although the cognitive process is mentioned, the product and its commercial- ity are emphasized as the dening factors of creativity (Zeng et al., 2011), even though creative products do not always have commercial value. Despite the vast range of denitions available for creativity, most denitions converge on four main de- ning characteristics of creativity as they relate to the product: originality, novelty, appropriateness, and val- ue (Kilgour, 2006). Although these characteristics do not allude to the creative process, they are oen evalu- ated in creativity tests. is suggests that unlike study- ing insight, creative thinking can hardly be studied in isolation from the sociocultural factors attached to it. Creative thinking & insight tests measure the same processes: Is creative thinking simply insight? Shireen Parimoo Abstract Creativity is a multidimensional concept that has been studied extensively over the last few decades (Kilgour, 2006). Two central aspects of creativity are the product and the process; the product refers to an item or an idea produced by an indi- vidual, while the process refers to the cognitive steps during creative thinking that give rise to the product. Insight is another well-studied phenomenon that results from a combination of cognitive processes such as divergent thinking, convergent thinking, and attention redirection (Ansburg, 2000; DeYoung, Flanders, & Peterson, 2008). is paper examines the various denitions of creativity and compares the creative process to the processes underlying insight. Furthermore, the cognitive processes measured in various tests of creative thinking, such as the Remote Associates Test (RAT) and the Torrance Test of Creative inking (TTCT), are examined and argued to also underlie insight. Creative thinking and insight are essentially the same phenomena, and creativity is simply the product of insight.

Transcript of Creative thinking & insight tests measure the same ... · ine creative thinking. For instance, the...

Page 1: Creative thinking & insight tests measure the same ... · ine creative thinking. For instance, the Remote Asso-ciates Task (RAT), which measures processes such as convergent thinking

20 | Inkblot • Vol. 5 • May 2016

Introduction

The definition of creativity has eluded people for decades, largely because its multifaceted nature makes it seemingly impossible to settle on one, all-encompass-ing explanation (Piffer, 2012). Rhodes’ (1961) defini-tion seems to encapsulate most elements of creativity, such as the creative person, the process, the product, and the press, of which the process refers to the cog-nitive mechanisms underlying creativity. Likewise, one of the most intensively studied phenomena in the cog-nitive psychology of problem solving is insight, com-monly understood as the ‘aha!’ feeling when one unex-pectedly stumbles upon a solution to a problem (Burns & Ellis, 1970; Davidson & Sternberg, 1995; Weisberg, 2010). Insight constitutes a set of cognitive processes for solving a problem that requires an individual to shift perspective. Insight problems require restructur-ing of the way the problem is perceived and attended to, such that when the restructuring occurs, the solution becomes immediately apparent and evokes the ‘aha!’ feeling. Creative thinking and the processes underlying insight are remarkably alike, as they both involve redi-recting or restructuring attention and they both require increasing awareness of the relationships between ele-ments of a problem. Creativity and insight tests mea-sure the same underlying cognitive processes of diver-gent thinking (DT), convergent thinking (CT), and restructuring via attention redirection, which suggests that creative thinking is simply an example of insight.

Creativity

Prior to explaining creative thinking, it is important to understand the concept of creativity. In attempting to define creativity, many end up segregating the cre-ative process and the creative product (Gardner, 1988; Hanson, 2013; Ivcevic, 2009; Zeng, Proctor, & Salvendy, 2011), but rarely do they include both aspects. Piffer (2012) focuses on the product in defining creativity in terms of the usefulness and originality of the product. Conversely, Mumford and Whetzel (1996) outline the various mental processes of creativity, such as how one organizes and attends to the problem, and how one can reach the solution. In trying to find a balance between the two definitions, Zeng and colleagues (2011) defined creativity as the cognitive processes employed to cre-ate a novel and useful product. Although the cognitive process is mentioned, the product and its commercial-ity are emphasized as the defining factors of creativity (Zeng et al., 2011), even though creative products do not always have commercial value.

Despite the vast range of definitions available for creativity, most definitions converge on four main de-fining characteristics of creativity as they relate to the product: originality, novelty, appropriateness, and val-ue (Kilgour, 2006). Although these characteristics do not allude to the creative process, they are often evalu-ated in creativity tests. This suggests that unlike study-ing insight, creative thinking can hardly be studied in isolation from the sociocultural factors attached to it.

Creative thinking & insight tests measure the same processes: Is

creative thinking simply insight?

Shireen Parimoo

Abstract

Creativity is a multidimensional concept that has been studied extensively over the last few decades (Kilgour, 2006). Two central aspects of creativity are the product and the process; the product refers to an item or an idea produced by an indi-vidual, while the process refers to the cognitive steps during creative thinking that give rise to the product. Insight is another well-studied phenomenon that results from a combination of cognitive processes such as divergent thinking, convergent thinking, and attention redirection (Ansburg, 2000; DeYoung, Flanders, & Peterson, 2008). This paper examines the various definitions of creativity and compares the creative process to the processes underlying insight. Furthermore, the cognitive processes measured in various tests of creative thinking, such as the Remote Associates Test (RAT) and the Torrance Test of Creative Thinking (TTCT), are examined and argued to also underlie insight. Creative thinking and insight are essentially the same phenomena, and creativity is simply the product of insight.

Page 2: Creative thinking & insight tests measure the same ... · ine creative thinking. For instance, the Remote Asso-ciates Task (RAT), which measures processes such as convergent thinking

Inkblot • Vol. 5 • May 2016 | 21

The creative process is not expected to be novel, origi-nal, or useful; rather, the outcome of the process is what earns the label of being creative. Accordingly, if one in-stance were more novel, original, or functional than an-other, it would be considered more creative. A strength of product-oriented creativity tests is that ideas or products from any field, from science to art, can be an instance of creativity due to the breadth of the criteria. Unfortunately, the breadth becomes vague in attempts to determine definitive, standardized criteria for an idea or product to be labelled novel. The continuum of creativity does not have clear boundaries that allow us to distinguish between what is minimally creative and what is not creative. The problem with settling on a def-inition of a creative product is that the criteria used to label the product creative are subjective. For example, does art have to be aesthetically pleasing in order to be creative? If so, opinions of what is aesthetically pleasing may differ between people. Creative products reflect the sociocultural views of a society at any given point in time. When examining creative thinking in isolation from sociocultural variables, such as the originality or the novelty of a product, there is overlap between the tests used to examine insight and those used to exam-ine creative thinking. For instance, the Remote Asso-ciates Task (RAT), which measures processes such as convergent thinking and divergent thinking, is used as a measure of both creativity and insight. Thus, the pro-cess-oriented definition of creativity is most relevant to creative thinking, as it outlines the mental processes that result in a creative product and can be compared to those underlying insight.

Insight

Insight, much like the creative process, constitutes the set of mental processes that leads to a realization, or the ‘aha!’ moment. Insight results from a dynami-cal and self-organizing process of shifting attention and restructuring the salient and relevant aspects of a prob-lem. At a specific moment during the process of solving an insight problem, an individual becomes aware of the associations between critical elements of the problem, which allows them to restructure their problem formu-lation and subsequently arrive upon a solution (Brock, 2015; Dominowski & Dallob, 1995; Metcalfe & Wiebe, 1987; Mumford & Whetzel, 1996; Necka, 2011). To

break out of the initially incorrect problem formula-tion, the problem solver must first scale down or focus their attention on the different aspects of a problem by breaking the problem down, and then scale up their at-tention by looking at the big picture in order to make the relevant associations clear and achieve the correct problem reformulation. This requires consciously ad-justing the opponent processes of scaling up and down at appropriate times during the task in order to facili-tate insight.

The main characteristics of insight include restruc-turing (Bowden & Jung-Beeman, 2003; Duch, 2007; Luo & Knoblich, 2007; Necka, 2011) and overcoming fixa-tion on the incorrect problem formulation (Mumford & Whetzel, 1996; Necka, 2011), as mentioned above, as well as cognitive leaping (Baker-Sennett & Ceci, 1996; Necka, 2011). Cognitive leaping, which is a subset of pat-tern completion (Baker-Sennett & Ceci, 1996), involves leaping from not knowing to knowing the solution. In this context, pattern completion refers to identifying a pattern as informative cues become more apparent. An example of a visual pattern completion task is when, upon seeing a blurred out picture of a couch, it is dif-ficult to determine what it is. As the picture becomes less blurry and more information is provided, even if it is not completely clear, it becomes easier to identify it as a couch. Likewise, with insight problems, as relevant cues to solve the problem become more apparent, it re-sults in a sudden insight to the solution. Often, relevant cues in the environment can trigger restructuring of the problem during the time spent away from the problem, which can lead to an insight (Dodds, Smith, & Ward, 2002; Necka, 2011; Seifert, Meyer, Davidson, Patalano, & Yaniv, 1995; Sio & Ormerod, 2014).

Many of the characteristics of insight are apparent in the nine-dot insight problem (NDP), as illustrated in Figure 1. The task is to draw four straight lines that pass through each dot without lifting the pen off the paper. This is an insight problem because it requires reformu-lation of the problem. Specifically, the solver must break out of the incorrect representation of the dots forming a square. Restructuring the representation of dots re-sults in the sudden insight that the NDP is not a con-nect-the-dots problem but rather, the solution requires a non-dot turn. The solver must surpass the perceived square frame created by the dots to solve the problem

Page 3: Creative thinking & insight tests measure the same ... · ine creative thinking. For instance, the Remote Asso-ciates Task (RAT), which measures processes such as convergent thinking

22 | Inkblot • Vol. 5 • May 2016

(Figure 2; Dominowski, 1981; Kershaw & Ohlsson, 2004). Due to fixation on the incorrect problem repre-sentation, in two separate experiments, none of the par-ticipants were able to solve this problem without exter-nal help (Chi & Snyder, 2012; Weisberg & Alba, 1981). In Weisberg and Alba’s (1981) study of 15 participants who were not given any clues or help, none of them was able to solve the NDP. Similarly, Chi and Snyder (2012) found that none of the 22 participants were able to solve the NDP prior to brain stimulation, and those who received sham stimulation could not solve it dur-ing or after stimulation. In this case, only those partici-pants with excitatory transcranial direct stimulation of the right anterior temporal lobe (ATL) and inhibition of the left ATL solved the problem. However, the rate of success for solving the NDP increases if appropri-ate clues or training are provided (Casselman & Maier, 1970; Kershaw & Ohlsson, 2004). As the low success rate indicates, people nearly always reach an impasse when trying to solve the nine-dot problem; clues can help them overcome fixation and help them restructure the problem appropriately.

Aside from brain stimulation, training people to scale up or down can also facilitate insight. McCaffrey (2012) gave participants various insight problems like the Duncker candle problem. In this problem, using only a matchbox, a box of thumbtacks, and a candle, the objective is to attach the candle to the wall while ensuring that the wax does not drip down to the floor. The solution is taking the thumbtacks out of the box, setting the candle into the box and attaching the box to the wall using the thumbtacks, and using the match-sticks to light the candle. The problem’s difficulty arises because people fixate on the conventional use of the ob-jects, whereas the key is to realize that the box can be used to hold the candle. Half of the participants were

taught the generic-parts technique, in which they were given an object and asked to break it down into its parts by scaling down their attention, such as a bell, and to think about whether the parts could be useful in any way. After training, all participants were given the in-sight problems. Those trained on the generic-parts tech-nique solved many more insight problems than those in the control group, largely because the technique helped them overcome fixation.

Another example of an insight problem is the mul-tiple marriages problem, which requires making remote associations, and in turn requires divergent thinking and convergent thinking in order to reach the solution (Sowden, Pringle, & Gabora, 2015). Remote associa-tions are relations between items that are not salient or immediately apparent. Convergent or analytical think-ing occurs from scaling down attention, while DT oc-curs from scaling up attention. The multiple marriages problem is:

“A man who lived in a small town married twenty different women of the same town. All are still living and he never divorced a single one of them. Yet, he broke no law. Can you explain?”

(Lockhart, Lamon, & Gick, 1988, p.36)People are quick to reach an impasse because they

fixate on the predominant meaning of the word “mar-ried” (Lockhart et al., 1988, p.36), which is to be wed to someone. However, to solve this problem successfully, people must overcome this fixation and shift their at-tention to the less common meaning of marry, which is to make two people into spouses. Once they restruc-ture their formulation of the problem and make this remote association, usually with the help of clues, they realize that the man in question is in fact a clergyman (Lockhart et al., 1988; Perfetto, Bransford, & Franks, 1983). It is evident from this problem that making re-mote associations between aspects of insight problems is critical to solving them, and often requires training or additional clues (Gick & Holyoak, 1980). Insight prob-lems measure the processes of overcoming fixation via DT, restructuring via attention redirection, and mak-ing remote associations via DT and evaluating them using CT (DeYoung et al., 2008). These processes are also present in creativity, and are measured by tests of creative thinking.

Figure 1. The Nine-Dot Problem

Figure 2. Solution to the Nine-Dot Problem

Page 4: Creative thinking & insight tests measure the same ... · ine creative thinking. For instance, the Remote Asso-ciates Task (RAT), which measures processes such as convergent thinking

Inkblot • Vol. 5 • May 2016 | 23

Tests of Creative Thinking

Similar to insight, the creative process consists of restructuring, problem reformulation, and a combi-nation of DT and CT (Okuda, Runco, & Berger, 1991; Sowden et al., 2015; Sternberg, 2001). The idea that creative thinking and insight potentially use the same cognitive processes is not new (Duch, 2007; Necka, 2011). Creativity requires dynamical opponent pro-cessing, such as shifting attention from a defocused to a concentrated state, and a combination of DT and CT (Chand & Runco, 1992; Gilhooly, Ball, & Macchi, 2015; Kilgour, 2006; Simonton, 2012; Sowden et al., 2015; Tasman, 1976), which occur in a recursive, dy-namic manner (Zeng et al., 2011). Creative thinking is not simply a matter of applying a process to a problem, but rather, it is the ability to shift between different cog-nitive processes depending on the context of the task. This opponent processing also occurs in insight fol-lowing de-automatization of cognitive processes. For instance, if DT is used to solve an insight problem, de-automatization of cognitive processes facilitates a shift to CT, which often results in insight. Alternatively, if CT is used ineffectively, the individual must also be able to switch to DT. Creative thinking tests that measure DT and CT via the ability to form remote associations and problem restructuring following attention redirec-tion lend support to the fact that the cognitive processes occurring in creativity and insight are the same.

Traditional tests of creative thinking largely mea-sure DT, and DT tests measure the extent to which people can think in unusual ways. The most common DT task is the Alternate Uses task, in which partici-pants have to generate alternative uses for an object. It is present in the Torrance Tests of Creative Thinking (TTCT) and the Wallach and Kogan Creativity Battery (WKCB), which are widely used tests to evaluate DT and CT (Crockenberg, 1972). The tasks in the TTCT and WKCB involve visual and verbal content, allowing for an estimation of creative thinking across domains. The TTCT is mostly composed of DT tasks, while the WKCB is mostly composed of tasks that involve CT in the form of making remote associations. The TTCT is scored on fluency, flexibility, originality, and elabora-tion (Crockenberg, 1972). Fluency is the number of responses given by the participant; flexibility is the di-versity of the responses and it implicitly measures the

number of remote associations the participant makes; originality is the uniqueness of the responses when compared to other participants; and elaboration is the amount of detail in a response (Crockenberg, 1972). As these tests measure DT comprehensively through dif-ferent tasks like the Alternate Uses task, and as these tests moderately predict creative potential (Crocken-berg, 1972; Piffer, 2012; Runco, 2004; Yoruk & Runco, 2014; Zeng et al., 2011), it is clear that DT is an impor-tant part of creative thinking.

Although the TTCT and WKCB suggest DT has a role in creativity, the role of CT cannot be overlooked. Some aspects of a task require DT and widespread at-tention (Kounios et al., 2008), while others entail CT and focused attention (Sowden et al., 2015). A DT task such as the Alternate Uses task requires multiple solu-tions, whereas a CT task would require analytical think-ing resulting in one correct solution. CT tasks can be mental arithmetic or logical thinking tasks. However, most insight and creative thinking tasks, such as the nine-dot problem, employ both CT and DT. Atten-tion redirection has been shown to facilitate the de-automatization of cognitive processes, which results in a shift from convergent to divergent thinking and vice versa (Gilhooly et al., 2015). Cropley (2006) argues that convergent thinking is necessarily required in cre-ative thinking because when ideas are generated, they must be further analyzed before they are accepted as a feasible solution or idea to further build on. CT is also involved in creative thinking, and the use of an appro-priate task is particularly important. That is, a straight-forward CT task that measures simple addition is not likely to be correlated with creativity, whereas a task that measures CT in the context of complex problem-solving requiring restructuring and appropriate atten-tional focus, such as a Remote Associates Task (RAT), is more highly correlated with creativity (Taft & Rossit-er, 1966). This is consistent with the importance of the appropriate scaling of attention to insight. Moreover, even in DT and insight tasks, CT is used because analyt-ical thinking is required to determine whether potential solutions are correct (Sowden et al., 2015). However, creative thinking tasks that measure CT, such as the RAT, typically also measure DT, rather than measuring CT independently of DT. This is one of the problems with measuring creative thinking, as the disproportion-

Page 5: Creative thinking & insight tests measure the same ... · ine creative thinking. For instance, the Remote Asso-ciates Task (RAT), which measures processes such as convergent thinking

24 | Inkblot • Vol. 5 • May 2016

ate focus on DT results in other cognitive processes be-ing overlooked.

The Remote Associates Task (RAT) is an insight and a creative thinking test based on both DT and CT. In the RAT, words are presented to the participant, and they are asked to identify one word that is related to each of the presented words. For example, given the words home, stomach, and sea, the expected answer is sick, as it relates to all three given words. In this task, recognizing associations requires DT while reaching the correct solution requires convergence (Benedek & Konen, 2012). It was initially considered a convergent thinking task because it was most highly correlated with measures of convergent thinking, such as verbal IQ and total IQ (Taft & Rossiter, 1966). However, newer re-search indicates that the RAT validly captures the ma-chinery of both DT and CT, which are involved in both insight and creativity (Kilgour, 2006; Morais, Almeida, Azevedo, & Necka, 2009; Yoruk & Runco, 2014; Zeng et al., 2011). Forming associations may require scaling up of attention to seek out potentially distant relationships, while convergent thinking is used to analyze potential solutions to determine the correct one (Cropley, 2006). Making easy associations on the RAT requires focused and narrowed attention, and not much DT, because the relationship between the words is salient, allowing one to reach the solution easily. On the other hand, difficult associations require widespread attention to all word meanings, because the relationship between the words is not immediately evident (Sio & Ormerod, 2014). More DT is needed to make those associations. Thus, the scaling up and down of attention, as well as know-ing when to do so, is equally important in both creative thinking and insight, as are CT and DT.

Despite the widespread use of DT and CT tasks as measures of creative thinking, they come with several criticisms. For instance, there is evidence against the use of TTCT tasks as measures of creative thinking. The lack of score consistency between the TTCT tasks is interpreted as evidence against their importance in the creative process, though restructuring and problem reformulation that occur in insight are important for creativity (Almeida, Prieto, Ferrando, Oliveira, & Fer-randiz, 2008). This is a valid concern, but this task in-consistency could occur because DT is measured in dif-ferent domains (Kilgour, 2006). That is, measuring DT

in the verbal Alternate Uses task is not the same as mea-suring it in the visual Picture Construction task, because the participants’ capabilities in each domain reflect in-dividual differences. This is not sufficient to conclude that DT and CT are not essential to the creative process. Creativity is also associated with DT rather than CT, because the novelty associated with a creative product is the result of combining information in a novel and unusual way (Almeida et al., 2008). However, as cre-ative thinking tasks like the RAT measure both DT and CT, and Almeida and colleagues (2008) only looked at the TTCT, this assertion is not supported. Though both DT and CT are implicated in creative thinking, DT may play a larger role. However, this is difficult to conclude as most tests of creative thinking, such as the TTCT, are biased towards measuring DT, and rarely measure CT as well.

Additionally, the RAT scores’ are not related to other product-oriented measures of creativity, in that the in-consistency is a result of association between presented information rather than an individual’s own associative production (Kilgour, 2006). While this is a strong criti-cism of creativity per se, it is not relevant when compar-ing the RAT to insight problems, which also require the formation of novel associations amongst the presented information. DT tests receive criticisms because they measure cognitive strategy selection rather than inher-ent associative ability (Kilgour, 2006). However, this is based on the assumption that creative thinking abilities are different from general cognitive processes such as attention and working memory. Yet if creative thinking is viewed as a set of cognitive processes that result in in-sight, the cognitive processing strategy becomes synon-ymous with creative thinking because creative thinking is neither automatic nor unique from other cognitive processes (Dietrich & Kanso, 2010; Morais et al., 2009; de Sousa, 2008). Hence, in the absence of these poten-tially weak assumptions, creative thinking and insight can employ the same cognitive processes.

In addition to traditional creativity tests, insight problems themselves are often used as measures of creative thinking based on the premise that solving an insight problem itself is a form of creativity (Cos-melli & Preiss, 2014; Dietrich & Kanso, 2010; Kounios et al., 2008; Kounios & Beeman, 2009; Runco, 2004). In assuming that solving insight problems is a form of

Page 6: Creative thinking & insight tests measure the same ... · ine creative thinking. For instance, the Remote Asso-ciates Task (RAT), which measures processes such as convergent thinking

Inkblot • Vol. 5 • May 2016 | 25

creativity, insight problems provide strong evidence re-garding the relationship between insight and creative thinking. The cognitive aspect of creativity therefore appears to be reliant on or simply a series of insights. The most commonly used tasks are the RAT and the compound remote associates (CRA) tasks, which have been used to study insight in the past (Ansburg & Hill, 2003; Bowden et al., 2005; Bowden & Jung-Beeman, 2003; Dodds et al., 2002). The CRA task is a version of the RAT in which the answer forms a compound word when joined to the presented words, rather than just being related to them. For example, if the presented words were magic, board, and hole, the answer black would yield a compound word when combined with the presented words, such as black magic. Performance on different insight tasks was correlated with the RAT, indicating that making associations between aspects of a problem is crucial in both creativity and insight (Ansburg, 2000). Moreover, when insight and diver-gent thinking tasks are used as measures of individual differences in creativity, success on various verbal in-sight problems is a distinguishing factor in the degree to which participants’ products are considered creative (Morais et al., 2009). There is an important relation-ship between insight problem solving and DT, and DT is central to creativity. Although insight is related to creative thinking, the majority of these experiments use the same type of task to measure this relationship – the RAT – which is not representative of all aspects of creative thinking. In particular, the RAT demonstrates how CT and DT are used to form remote associations, but the RAT fails to account for appropriate restructur-ing processes seen in problems such as the NDP that are also essential for creative thinking and insight. Cre-ativity and insight use various cognitive processes, not all of which are studied within the same experiment. As such, the processes underlying how creativity and insight are reported and conceptualized is inconsistent, with a disproportionate focus on DT. This creates an avenue for future research for exploring and compar-ing the concepts of creativity and insight in detail. Even though they both use the same cognitive processes, they are not the same phenomenon. Insight is a cognitive phenomenon, whereas creativity involves sociocultural factors in addition to the cognitive processes of creative thinking. Given the overlap between the processes in-

volved in creative thinking and insight, they appear to be largely the same, albeit with varying outcomes. That is, insight seems to be a precursor to creativity, as it may be needed to generate a creative product. However, not every insight results in creativity or a creative product. Creative thinking may be an instance of insight, with the outcome of generating a creative product. It is un-clear whether DT or CT contribute more to creative thinking, as DT has been studied more extensively in this context. Next, researchers could develop tasks that measure CT in the context of creative thinking, to de-termine whether CT significantly contributes to cre-ative thinking independently of DT.

Conclusion

Creative thinking tests such as the TTCT and RAT, as well as insight problems like the NDP, the multiple marriages problem, and the RAT measure the same cognitive processes; namely divergent thinking, con-vergent thinking, and attentional focus. Currently, re-searchers believe both insight and creative thinking use the same cognitive processes (Gilhooly et al., 2015), and insight can be facilitated (Chi & Snyder, 2012; McCaf-frey, 2012). However, most studies have not investi-gated the similarities between creative thinking and in-sight processes directly. They have not examined all of these processes in one study; most studies focus on di-vergent thinking exclusively. While divergent thinking is required to solve insight problems, other processes such as CT and attentional focus should be studied fur-ther, as these tests do not fully measure these other pro-cesses. When evaluating an individual’s performance during an insight or during a creative thinking task, it is important to specify which cognitive processes are being studied. This will determine whether the same processes underlie both creative thinking and insight. Future research could measure all the processes under-lying insight and creative thinking, and an increased understanding of these processes could allow us to train creativity by facilitating insight.

ReferencesAlmeida, L. S., Prieto, L. P., Ferrando, M., Oliveira, E., & Ferrandiz, C. (2008). Torrance test of creative thinking: the question of its construct validity. Thinking Skills and Creativity, 3, 53-58.Ansburg, P.I. (2000). Individual differences in problem solving via insight. Current Psychology, 19(2), 143-146.Baker-Sennett, J. & Ceci, S. J. (1996). Clue-efficiency and insight: Unveiling the mystery of inductive leaps. Journal of Creative Behavior, 30(3), 153-172.

Page 7: Creative thinking & insight tests measure the same ... · ine creative thinking. For instance, the Remote Asso-ciates Task (RAT), which measures processes such as convergent thinking

26 | Inkblot • Vol. 5 • May 2016

Benedek, M. & Konen, T. (2012). Associative abilities underlying creativity. Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts, 6(3), 273-281.Bowden, E.M. & Jung-Beeman, M. (2003). Aha! Insight experience correlates with solution activation in the right hemisphere. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 10(3), 730-737.Bowden, E. M., Jung-Beeman, M., Fleck, J., & Kounios, J. (2005). New approaches to demystify-ing insight. Trends in Cognitive Science, 9(7), 322-328.Brock, R. (2015). Intuition and insight: two concepts that illuminate the tacit in science educa-tion. Studies in Science Education, 51(2), 127-167.Burns, R. W. & Ellis, B. M. (1970). What IS creativity? School Science and Mathematics, 70(3), 204-206.Chand, I. & Runco, M. A. (1992). Problem finding skills as components in the creative process. Personality and Individual Differences, 14, 155-162. Chi, R. P. & Snyder, A. W. (2012). Brain stimulation enables the solution of an inherently dif-ficult problem. Neuroscience Letters, 515(2), 121-124.Cosmelli, D., & Preiss, D. D. (2014). On the temporality of creative insight: a psychological and phenomenological perspective. Frontiers in Psychology, 5, 1184.Crockenberg, S. B. (1972). Creativity tests: a boon or boondoggle for education? Review of Educational Research, 42, 27-45. Dietrich, A. & Kanso, R. (2010). A review of EEG, ERP, and neuroimaging studies of creativity and insight. Psychological Bulletin, 136(5), 822-848.DeYoung, C. G., Flanders, J. L., & Peterson, J. B. (2008). Cognitive abilities involved in insight problem solving: An individual differences model. Creativity Research Journal, 20(3), 278-290.Dodds, R. A., Smith, S. M., & Ward, T. B. (2002). The use of environmental clues during incuba-tion. Creativity Research Journal, 14(3), 287-304. Dominowski, R. L. (1981). Comment on “An examination of the alleged role of ‘fixation’ in the solution of several ‘insight’ problems” by Weisberg and Alba. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 110(2), 199-203. Dominowski, R. L. & Dallob, P. (1995). Insight and problem solving. In R. J. Sternberg & J. E. Davidson (Eds.), The Nature of Insight (pp. 33-62). Duch, W. (2007). Intuition, insight, imagination and creativity. IEEE Computational Intelligence Magazine, 2(3), 40-52.Gardner, H. (1988). Creativity: an interdisciplinary perspective. Creativity Research Journal, 1, 8-26.Gick, M. L. & Holyoak, K. J. (1980). Analogical problem solving. Cognitive Psychology, 12, 306-355. Gilhooly, K. J., Ball, L. J., & Macchi, L. (2015). Insight and creative thinking processes: Routine and special. Thinking & Reasoning, 21, 1-4.Hanson, M. H. (2013). Author, self, monster: Using Foucault to examine functions of creativity. Journal of Theoretical and Philosophical Psychology, 33(1), 18-31. Ivcevic, Z. (2009). Creativity map: toward the next generation of theories of creativity. Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts, 3, 17-21.Kershaw, T. C. & Ohlsson, S. (2004). Multiple causes of difficulty in insight: The case of the nine-dot problem. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 30, 3-13.Kilgour, M. (2006). Improving the creative process: analysis of the effects of divergent thinking techniques and domain specific knowledge on creativity. International Journal of Business and Society, 7(2), 79-102. Kounios, J. & Beeman, M. (2009). The aha! moment: the cognitive neuroscience of insight. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 18(4), 210-216. Kounios, J., Fleck, J. I., Green, D. L., Payne, L., Stevenson, J. L., Bowden, E. M., & Jung-Beeman, M. (2008). The origins of insight in resting-state brain activity. Neuropsychologia, 46, 281-291.Lockhart, R. S., Lamon, M., & Gick, M. L. (1988). Conceptual transfer in simple insight prob-lems. Memory and Cognition, 16, 36-44. Luo, J. & Knoblich, G. (2007). Studying insight problem solving with neuroscientific methods. Methods, 42, 77-86.McCaffrey, T. (2012). Innovation relies on the obscure: A key to overcoming the classic problem of functional fixedness. Psychological Science, 23(3), 215-218. Metcalfe, J. & Wiebe, D. (1987). Intuition in insight and noninsight problem solving. Memory and Cognition, 15(3), 238-246. Morais, M. F., Almeida, L. S., Azevedo, M. I., & Necka, E. (2009). Cognitive dimensions of creativity: what makes the difference between creative and non-creative university students? Polish Psychological Bulletin, 40(2), 55-61.Mumford, M. D. & Whetzel, D. L. (1996). Insight, creativity, and cognition: On Sternberg and Davidson’s The Nature of Insight. Creativity Research, 9, 103-107. Necka, E. (2011). Insight. In M. A. Runco & S. R. Pritzker (Eds.), Encyclopedia of Creativity (pp. 667-672).Okuda, S. M., Runco, M. A., & Berger, D. E. (1991). Creativity and the finding and solving of real-world problems. Journal of Psychoeducational Assessment, 9, 45-53.Perfetto, G. A., Bransford, J. D., & Franks, J. J. (1983). Constraints on access in a problem solving context. Memory and Cognition, 11, 24-31. Piffer, D. (2012). Can creativity be measured? An attempt to clarify the notion of creativity and general directions for future research. Thinking Skills and Creativity, 7, 258-264.Rhodes, M. (1961). An analysis of creativity. Phi Delta Kappa International, 42(7), 305-310.Runco, M. A. (2004). Creativity. Annual Review of Psychology, 55, 657-687.

Seifert, C. M., Meyer, D. E., Davidson, N., Patalano, A. L., & Yaniv, I. (1995). In R. K. Sternberg & J. E. Davidson (Eds.), The Nature of Insight (65-124). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press, 1995.Simonton, D. K. (2012). Quantifying creativity: can measures span the spectrum? Dialogues in Clinical Neuroscience, 14, 100-104.Sio, U. N. & Ormerod, T. C. (2014). Incubation and cueing effects in problem-solving: set aside the difficult problems but focus on the easy ones. Thinking & Reasoning, 21, 113-129.de Sousa, F. C. (2008). Still the elusive definition of creativity. International Journal of Psychology: A Biopsychosocial Approach, 2, 55-82. Sowden, P. T., Pringle, A., & Gabora, L. (2015). The shifting sands of creative thinking: Connections to dual-process theory. Thinking and Reasoning, 21, 40-60.Sternberg, R. J. (2001). What is the common thread of creativity? Its dialectical relation to intel-ligence and wisdom. American Psychologist, 56(4), 360-362.Taft, R. & Rossiter, J. R. (1966). The remote associates test: divergent or convergent thinking? Psychological Reports, 19, 1313-1314. Tasman, A. (1976). Creativity, the creative process, and cognitive style and state. Comprehensive Psychiatry, 17, 259-269.Weisberg, R. (2010). The study of creativity: from genius to cognitive science. International Journal of Cultural Policy, 16(3), 235-253.Weisberg, R. W. & Alba, J. W. (1981). An examination of the alleged role of “fixation” in the solution of several “insight” problems. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 110(2), 169-192. Yoruk, S. & Runco, M. A. (2014). The neuroscience of divergent thinking. Journal for Neurocognitive Research, 56(1-2), 1-16.Zeng, L., Proctor, R. W., & Salvendy, G. (2011). Can traditional divergent thinking tests be trust-ed in measuring and predicting real-world creativity? Creativity Research Journal, 23, 24-37.