Bullying y psicopatologias

download Bullying y psicopatologias

of 14

Transcript of Bullying y psicopatologias

  • 8/10/2019 Bullying y psicopatologias

    1/14

    Articles

    Peer Victimization: Cause or Consequence

    of School Maladjustment?

    Becky J. Kochenderfer and Gary W. Ladd

    University ofIllinois atUrbana-Charapaign

    KocHENDEftFER, EECKYJ., and LADD. GARY W . Peer Victim,ization: Cause orC onsequenceof

    School Maladjustment? CHIL D DETO LO PM ENT, 1996,67,130 5-1317. P ast research has shown tha

    peer victimization and school maladjustment are related, but it is unclear whether victimization is

    a cause or conseq uenc e of such difficulties. T his study exam ined whe ther (a) peer victimization

    ISa precursor of school maiadjus tment, (b) the effects are limited to the period of victimization

    and (c) stable peer victimization experiences compo und adjustment difficulties. T oward this en d

    daia were collected

    on

    200 5- and 6-year-old children (105 males, 95 females)

    m

    the fall an

    spring of kindergarten. Findings supported the hypothesis that victimizationis a precursor o

    children's loneliness and school avoidance. Whereas children's feelings of loneliness were more

    pronounced while victimization was occurring, delayed effects were found for school avoidance

    Furthermore, the duration of children's victimization experiences was related to the magnitude

    of their school adjustment problems.

    C oncern about children who are victim-

    ized by their peers has increased as

    a

    result

    of research conducted on childhood aggres-

    sion

    (see

    P atterson, L it tman,

    &

    Bricker,

    1967;

    Perrj ' ,

    Kusel, &

    Perrj-,

    1988) and bul-

    lying behavior (Boulton & U i d e r w o o d ,

    1992.,

    O lweus, 1991). How ever, peer victim-

    ization has not been as well defined nor as

    well investigated relative

    to its

    counter-

    partsaggressive

    and

    bullying behaviors

    (see Perr>'el al., 1988). W hereas somere-

    searchers have construed peer victimization

    asanoutcom e of bully ing behaviors (i .e .,a

    subtype of aggression that is unprovoked,

    occurs repeatedly and over time, .and is per-

    petrated by a stronger child against a weaker

    one; see O lwe us, 1991). others have framed

    the concept of victimization more hroadly as

    a role or position that children occupy in ag-

    gressive en coun ters (i.e. . , the targ et of pe ers '

    aggressive behaviors: see Perry elai., 1988).

    C onsistent witb P erry, Kusei, and P erry,we

    define peer victimization more brcadly, that

    is, as aform of pe er abu seinwhichachild

    is frequently the target of peer aggression.

    Recent investigations have shown that a

    substantial number

    of

    children

    are

    victira-

    ized by their peers at school (Boulton & Un-

    onaregular basis. T her e is also eviden ceto

    suggest that the prevalence of peer victim

    ization

    is

    greater am ong youn ger

    as

    com

    pared to older school-age po pulation s.Ol

    weus (1991) found that

    the

    proportion

    o

    students victimized by peers in secon

    through sixth grade (11.6%) was twice that o

    students in grades 7-9 (5.4%). Kochenderfe

    and L add (in press) found eve n high er rate

    of victimization among kindergarten chil

    dren; 22.6% of their sample of 56-year-oIds

    reported moderate to high levels of peer vic

    timization.

    T h e r eisalso growing evide nce to indi

    cate that victimized children are at risk for

    a

    variety

    of

    adjustment prob lems. Studie

    have shown that victimized childrenare

    more anxious and insecure (O lweus, 1993)

    hav e lower self-esteem (A lsaker,1993

    Bjorkqvist, Ek man , & L agerspetz, 1982; L a

    gerspetz, Bjorkqvist, Berts,

    &

    King, 1982

    O lweu s, 1993), and are mo re likely to be de

    pressed (Bjorkqvist

    et al.,

    1982; O l w e us

    1992) than non victimized age mates. A ddi

    tionally, victimized children appear to have

    greater difficulty adjusting

    to

    school. C om

    pared

    to

    nonv ictimized classmates, victim

    ized children arelon elier (Boulton &U n

    &

    L a dd ,

    m

  • 8/10/2019 Bullying y psicopatologias

    2/14

    1306 Child Development

    higher levels

    of

    school av oidan ce (Kochen-

    derfer & L add;inpress).

    atel}^,

    the linkage betw een

    peer victimization and children's adjustment

    problems

    has not

    oeen well iisvestigated.

    Because most reseajrclieTS have reliedpri-

    marili--

    on

    coacurrent assessments

    to

    exam-

    ine the relation be^tween these constructs,

    it

    remains uncJesr astowh ether pe er victim-

    ization

    is a

    cause

    or a

    qonseq-uence

    of

    cinfi--

    dre ii's adjustoieiif: prob.iem.. M oreov er,

    m uc h

    of

    tne data

    on

    the reJation beti^'ee-n

    pe er victimizatioi . and child ren 's adjustoient

    has beert gatbered clming tbe middle grade

    school )-eai-s when v-ict.imizatiori patterns

    have been shown to be pelatively stable (see

    Bouitou & IJfiderwood. 1992). By this point

    in children's school careers,

    ft

    raavhedifB-

    cult to disentongie tTrie linkage bete'een vic-

    timization

    and

    child ren's adjustment,

    as

    these relations mav already

    be

    la-el] estab-

    listed..

    I D

    order to obtain clues about the causa.

    priority of these variables, thereis ane e dto

    conckicl lon gitud inal stad ias witii sannpies of

    relatively unacquainted childrenbefore

    aggressor/victim palteras have been estab-

    lished. By identifj/ing such samples arid as-

    sessing botli victimization and adjustment at

    u ltiple poJnts in tim e, it Y /ou)d be possible

    or investigators

    to

    de t e r m i ne w he t he r

    it is

    in

    Kindergarten provides a natural v/indow

    as

    chil-

    are

    tjr'pically

    in

    new peer groups

    and

    know rela-

    few, ifany , of the ir classmates (L add

    in

    that, upon enterin g grade school,

    n ew de m a nds ,

    a

    ttovei school env iron

    a new

    peer group,

    and

    (L add & P rice, 1987).Orone hand,it

    may have

    Hence, one objective

    of

    the present

    in-

    vestigation was to examine rnore closely the

    link between children's victimization expe-

    riences

    and

    their school adjustment

    in

    kin-

    dergarten. Specifically, it wasofinterestto

    explore

    tbe

    hyp oth esis "that early pe er vic-

    timization

    has a

    negative impact

    OD

    chil-

    dren's school adjustment and,in particular,

    leads to the formation of negative seliool atti-

    tudes,

    perceptions, and feelings. T oward

    tJiis end, vre ideotified a .sampleofkinder-

    gajten children and assessed botii peer vie-

    tin-iization and school adjustm ent tv/ice dur-

    ing file school year (i.e.. fall, spring). Several

    school adjustment indices (see L add, 1980;

    L add i& P rice, 1987} we re assessedat each

    dine

    rjf

    measavement, including

    (a)

    s c t ool

    attitudes {i.e., how rnnch children like

    school),

    (it)

    school affect (i.e., lonelin ess

    in

    school),

    (c)

    school invo lvem ent (i.e .,

    ex-

    pressed desire to avoid school), and (d) aca-

    de m i c a c M e ve m e nt D a t a pr oduc ed

    by

    this

    iongitudiiiai design vrere analysed to deter-

    mine vA ether early victimizaiion experi-

    ences predtcfced changes in children's school

    aclju.sfcme.ri(- or whether early adjusfcrnent dii~

    ficiJiaes forecasted cbanges

    in

    children's

    victimiz ation status. C onsi.stent with

    our

    bypotheses, it wasanticipated that the re-

    suits oftiiese analyses would support the

    in-

    terpretation tliat peer victimization isapre-

    cuj-sior of children 's school adjustraent

    prohlems.

    Fnrthermore, the timing and duration of

    peer victimization iri.ay play

    a

    crucial role

    in

    the wil-astment outcomes experienced by

    young ' I 'ictimized children . A lthough there

    is evidence to sugge st that pee r victimiza-

    tion is a relatively stable phen om eno nas

    early as secortd g rade (b)' S or 9 years of

    age;

    B'O uiton S: U nde r w ood, 1992; O l w eu s ,

    1902), little

    IS

    known about

    the

    onset

    and

    stability

    of

    pieer victimiz.ation

    at

    younger

    grade levels.Jt is conceivable that, young

    children's exposure

    to

    pe er victimization

    may be eidier an acute or chronic ex perience

    and may emerge

    at

    different tim es d urin g

    the .school year.Forinstance, P eTiy, P err;/,

    an d B oldizar (1990) liave argued that, as chil-

  • 8/10/2019 Bullying y psicopatologias

    3/14

  • 8/10/2019 Bullying y psicopatologias

    4/14

    130 8 C hild Development

    compound children's schooi adjiistment dil^

    ficulties. It was anticipated that children

    who continued to hs victimized over the

    school year (i.e. . Stable V ictims) v/ouJd sho w

    significant increases in school makdjust-

    ment from fall to spring of kindergarten.

    M e t h o d

    Subjects

    T he sam ple consisted of 200 kindergar-

    ten ch ildr en -vidio w ere r ec rui ted from th re e

    schooi_ systems in the mid we stem Un ited

    States. ' T here w ere 105 males and 95 fe-

    males whose mean age was 5.50 at the be-

    ginning of the school year. All subjects were

    participants in a larger 5-year longitudinal

    project, and informed parental consent \as

    obtained for all children as they enrered kin-

    dergai-ten. O nly classroom s for wh ich p er-

    mission rates exceeded 80% were included

    in the project {n = 16). Paiideipants came

    from di'verse socioeconomie backgrourtds:

    36.3% were lo/er- to lovk'er-iniddle income

    (less tiian 820,000); 30.5% were middle in-

    come (821,000-340,000;: and the remainder

    were upper-middle to high-income families

    (above S41,000;. T he e thnic com position of

    tlie sampie was representative of that found

    in the participating schooJ systems.- 20,0%

    Miicaik A m e ri ca n, 2 .0% L a t ino; 73 . 5% C a u-

    casian; and 4.5% mixed race and other.

    Measures

    Self-reports of peer vict-imization.

    T he P erceptions of P eer Support Scale

    ( P P S S ; see L add, Kochenderfer, & C oleman,

    1998; was administered to children during

    indi's'idual interviews to obtain information

    about children's perceptions of classroom

    peers. r''oiir of the items on this scale al-

    lowed children to self-report the extent to

    which "they had experienced peer aggres-

    sion. T hese i tems loaded

    C Q

    a factor that was

    separate from the remaining items cii the

    scale find produced a relatively consistent

    attern of responses from children (alpha of

    our-item subscale = .74). Du ring m ter-

    iei'i/s, the items ivere presented to children

    as follows: Does anyone in your class

    e-*'er;

    (1) pick on you at school? (2) hit ycraaE

    interviewers trained children to use a 'three-

    point response format (scale): no (1), some-

    times (2}, a lot (3). VictimizatioK scores were

    calculated by averaging the four items

    (scores ranged from 1.00 to 3.00).

    In order to identify a group of children

    who had experienboed^ .high levels of peer ag-

    gression, children were cla.ssified as victims

    if; across the four items, they received scores

    tiat an/eraged above thie scale's midpoint of

    "soT O etirfies" (i.e., 2.25 on tlie thr ee-p oin t

    scale).

    T his criterion is consisten t with the

    conceptualization of peer victimization as a

    form of abuse in vt^hich a child is frequently

    ta.rgeted for peer aggression. For instance,

    O lweus (1991) consid ered children to be

    victims if diey repo^rted being tscrgeted for

    aggressi've be hav iors rep ea ted )' aDid over

    time. Tbu.s., our cut-off is an attempt to dis-

    tiiigutsh betvi/een those kiridergarmers who

    are occasionally tbe recipieots of peers' ag-

    gressive O 'i'ertures and tbo se child ren w ho

    ar epe rsistently tEtrgeted for agg ression . Fu r-

    tb^jToore, this cut-off criterion was chosen

    to be compar,able to the "now and tlien, or

    moie often" sel^report ciiterloQ utilized by

    O 'lweus to idendfi,' 'i ' ictimized child ren.

    M oreov er, the propori:ion of childre n ide nti-

    fied using tbis criterion ('20.5%) app.rosi-

    mates A at found itt other samples of Idnder-

    garteiL chi ldre n (18%; Kodbenderfer, 1995)

    and O 'lweus's y oung est sample of children

    (17%; second d

    T he validity of

    t be

    self-report m easu re

    ot peer victimization 'if s examined with a

    separate sample of kindergarten cbildren

    (see Koefeenderfer & L add,

    1996}.

    Based on

    observational data, tliese investigEitors con-

    cluded that tbe self-report measure pro-

    duced sufficiently reliable and valid data for

    identiiyiiig young victims. T bat is . children

    virbo were identifieda>svictims based onself-

    reports were aiso rated by observers as expe-

    riencing significantly higher ievels of peer

    aggnessiori in their classrooms compared to

    children who were classified as nonvictims.

    A i^diL tiooai )', evi de nc e of conc iirienf valid-

    ity foi bo'lii: self-re po rts arid obseT ver ratin gs

    of i,':ictimization i^Tis oblained. SpeciBcall)',

  • 8/10/2019 Bullying y psicopatologias

    5/14

    Kochenderfer and L add 1309

    with pee r acceptance (see A lsaker, 1993;

    Boulton & Un derw ood , 1992; P erry, Kusel,

    & P erry, 1988).

    O ver 75% of kindergarten ch ildren re-

    ported experiencing some level of peer ag-

    gression at each assessment period (i.e., re-

    ceived an a^'erase score that was greater

    than 1.0.3: Fa ll: M = 1.66, SD = .57; Spring:

    M = 1.61, SD = .56). How ever, as men -

    tioned above, in order to be classified as a

    "victim" a child had to receive a victimisa-

    tion score which indicated that she or he w as

    frequently targeted for pe er aggression.

    Based on this criterion, victimization groups

    were created hy identifying children who

    had scores abo'^'e 2.25 only in the fall (Fall

    O nly Victim) and only in the spring (Spring

    O nly Victim). C hildren who reported this

    relatively high level of victimization in both

    fall and spring were assigned to a Stable Vic-

    tim group, and children whose victimization

    scores fell below the cut-off criterion at both

    points in time were assigned to the Konvic-

    tim group.^

    School liking and avoidance. A r e -

    vised version of the School L iking and

    A voidance Questionnaire (L add & P rice,

    19S7) was also administered to children dur-

    ing individual interviews to tap differences

    in their school attitudes (e.g., how much

    they like school) and involvement (i.e., ex-

    pressed desire to avoid school). T his 14-item

    scale factored into a nine-item school liking

    subscale (e.g., "Do you like being in

    school?" alpha = .91) and a five-it'm school

    avoidance .subscale (e.g., '"Do you wish you

    didn't have to come to school?" alpha = .81)

    C hildren w ere trained to respond to a three-

    point respotise format (scale): yes (3), some-

    times (2), or no (1). Scores were obtained for

    each Eubscale by averaging childreti's scores

    across items.

    Loneliness. T he C a ss idy a nd A s her

    (1992) L on eline ss and So cial Dissatisfaction

    Questionnaire (LSDQ) was also individually

    administered to children. To obtain a mea-

    sure of loneliness that was distinct from so-

    cial dissatisfaction, a subscaJe was created

    that included th ree L SD Q items ( those that

    refer directly to loneliness) and Uvo addi-

    you sad and alone at school?"). Scores were

    created by averaging children's yes (3)

    sometimes (2), or no (1) responses over these

    five items (alpha = .78).

    Academic achievement. T h e M e tr o

    politan Readiness T est (M RT ; Nurss &

    M cG auvran, 1986) was used to assess chil

    dren's academic progress at two points in

    time.

    For the fall assessmen t. L evel 1 wa

    administered during individual intervievvs

    A fall ach ievem ent score was calculated by

    averaging the verbal and language stanine

    scores . During the spring semester . L evel 2

    of the M RT was administered in groups o

    about 15 children. Spring achievemen

    scores were computed by averaging the stan

    ine scores for the verbal, language, and

    quantitative subtests.

    Procedure

    As part of the larger longitudinal study

    children were individually interviewed dur

    ing two 40-min sessions during both the fal

    and sp ring of the kinderg arten year. With the

    exception of the M RT , which was group

    administered m the spring, the same mea

    sures were given in both semesters and v/er

    administered in a counterbalanced orde

    across sessions. Before each interview, chil

    dren were told that their participation wa

    voluntary and that their responses were con

    fidential. A fter each session, child ren re

    ceived colorful stickers and pencils for thei

    participation.

    Results

    Relations among School

    Adjustment Measures

    C orrelations am ong the fall and sp rin

    scbool adjustment indices were low to mod

    erate in magnitude (see T able 1). T he mag

    nitude of these correlations suggests that th

    subscales tap different aspects of school ad

    justment and, consequently, all subscale

    were retained for the following analyses.

    Prevalence and Stability

    of Peer Victimization

    In the fall, 20.5% of the kindergartner

    reported being victimized, on the average

    more than "sometimes" (i .e ., scores abov

  • 8/10/2019 Bullying y psicopatologias

    6/14

    131 0 C hild Development

    T A B L E

    1

    COEHELATIONS AMONG

    F A L L AXD S P H I N G S C H O C L - V D J US T M E N T

    INDICES

    L o n e l in e ss ...

    School liking .. ....

    School avoidance ....... ..

    L o n e li n e ss

    School likmg

    School avoidance

    ' p 4.75, p

  • 8/10/2019 Bullying y psicopatologias

    9/14

    Kochenderfer and L add 13 13

    TA B LE 4

    MEAN SCOBES(Standard Deviations in Parentheses) FO H SCHOO L ADJUSTM ENT

    M EA SUBES FOH EAC H VICT IM IZA TIO N G HOUP A CROSS ASSESSM ENT PERIO DS

    Fall Spring C hange from

    A ssessment A ssessment Fall to Spring

    L oneliness:

    Nonvictims 1.37 (.48)" 1.41 (.48)" N. T .

    Fall only victims 1.81 (.66)*' 1.64 (.60) N. S.

    Spring onlv victims 1.54 (.54) 1.70 (.68) p < .001

    Stable victims 1.68 (.61)'' 1.98 (.66)'' p

  • 8/10/2019 Bullying y psicopatologias

    10/14

    3 4 Child Development

    school less

    (t=

    2.64,

    p ', L . G . (1988)

    Victims of peer aggression.

    Developmenta

    Psychology, 2 4 , 8 0 7 - 8 1 4 .

    P erry, D. G . , P erry, L . G . , & Boldizar, J . P . (1990)

    L earning of aggression. I n M . L ewis & S

    M iller (Eds.) , Handb ook of developmenta

    psychopathology (pp. 135-146). New York

    P l e n u m .

    Rosenthal, R., & Rosnow, R. L . (1988). Contra

    analysis. Focused comparisons in the analy

    sis of variance.

    New Y ork: G ambridge Uni

    versity' P ress.

  • 8/10/2019 Bullying y psicopatologias

    14/14