AAlr - Virginia Tech

204
Observation, Description, and Prediction of Long-Term Learning on a Keyboarding Task by Mark L. McMulkin Thesis Submitted to the Faculty of the Virginia Polytechnic Institute and State University in Partial Fulfillment of the Requirements for the Degree of Masters of Science in Industrial and Systems Engineering APPROVED: —— AAlr / CHE. Kroginer, Chairman FT - NY \ i. % \ fo \ \ Hl \ oN vs | \ \ a “ye ' ( }\ wy pM? \ Te . SN SS \ Ry ¥ \ NX , MS “TRC. Wiliges | .C. Woldstad Blacksburg, Virginia January, 1992

Transcript of AAlr - Virginia Tech

Page 1: AAlr - Virginia Tech

Observation, Description, and Prediction of Long-Term Learning on a Keyboarding Task

by

Mark L. McMulkin

Thesis Submitted to the Faculty of the Virginia Polytechnic Institute and State University

in Partial Fulfillment of the Requirements for the Degree of

Masters of Science

in

Industrial and Systems Engineering

APPROVED:

——

AAlr / CHE. Kroginer, Chairman

FT - NY

\ i. % \ fo \ ‘

\ Hl ‘ \ oN vs | \ \ a ‘

“ye ' ( }\ wy pM?

\ Te . SN SS \ Ry ¥ \ NX , MS

“TRC. Wiliges |

.C. Woldstad

Blacksburg, Virginia

January, 1992

Page 2: AAlr - Virginia Tech

Lv

SoS >

VAST

J 912

[226 cy

Page 3: AAlr - Virginia Tech

Observation, Description, and Prediction of Long-Term

Learning on a Keyboarding Task

by

Mark Lee McMulkin

Committee Chairman: Karl H. E. Kroemer

Industrial and Systems Engineering

(ABSTRACT)

Three major principles of learning a chord keyboarding task were

investigated. Five subjects were taught 18 characters on a chord keyboard,

then practiced improving their keying speed for about 60 hours.

The first objective of the study was to observe long-term learning on a

keyboarding task. The performance, in characters typed per minute, was

recorded over the entire range of the experiment. Typing skill improved quickly

in the beginning and then slowed, but performance had not reached a stable

peak by the end of the experiment.

The second objective of this study was to determine a function that

describes performance progress from initial training to a high keying speed.

Five functions were evaluated; a function which predicts the logarithm of the

dependent variable (characters per minute) from the logarithm of the regressor

variable provided a good fit to the actual data. The final form of the equation

was CPM; = ePo7 Br where CPM; = performance in characters per minute on

the i-th interval, T; = the i-th interval of practice, and By and B, are fitted

coefficients.

The second objective also considered the form that T; (from the above

equation) should take. Performance can be predicted from number of

Page 4: AAlr - Virginia Tech

repetitions such as trials, or from amount of practice such as hours. Both trials

and time were used as predictor variables and both provided equally accurate

predictions of typing speed. Both also provided excellent fits in conjunction with

the Log-Log equation. Thus, it appears the Log-Log function is fairly robust in

predicting performance from different variables.

The third objective was to investigate how many trials of performance are

needed before the entire learning function can be reasonably determined. In

this experiment, subjects practiced for an extended period of time (about 60

hours) so a fairly complete progression of performance could be gathered. Yet,

it would be more convenient to collect data for only a few hours and deduce the

ensuing performance of the subject. The coefficients of the Log-Log function

were determined using only the first 25, 50, 100, 150, and 200 of the initial

performance points (out of about 550 total actual data points). The mean

squared error (MSE) was calculated for each of these fits and compared to the

MSE of the fit using all points. It appears that at least 50 performance data

points are required to reduce the error to a reasonably acceptable level.

Page 5: AAlr - Virginia Tech

ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS

This work was possible due to the funds and facilities provided by the

Center for Innovative Technology (CIT) of Virginia, the Department of Industrial

and Systems Engineering, in particular, the Industrial Ergonomics Laboratory,

and Vatell Corporation.

| would like to extend my appreciation and thanks to my committee chair,

Dr. Karl Kroemer, for his encouragement, assistance, time, and support

throughout this project and my entire Masters Degree education. | would also

like to thank my other committee members, Dr. Jeff Woldstad and Dr. Robert

Williges, for their advice and guidance on this project which were instrumental

to the completion of this thesis.

Finally, | would like to thank my fellow graduate students for their help

during my thesis, particularly Chris Rockwell for his assistance in

troubleshooting the experimental set-up.

Page 6: AAlr - Virginia Tech

TABLE OF CONTENTS

ADSHACE cee eeessssecssssccsesnescsccvsnsecesssscsensecsssascesssesesnensucensesensnaseesnansetsoeeseseasenenanaeesenes il

ACKNOWlECQMENMS ..........:cccesecessceceteesseceeseesesecesseeestececseccssneeseaeeeeseecueneessaeeesseesseneceenens iv

List Of ADPONdICES ou... eee eesseseeesssecceecncessceeesetseseessensnsesneecaaeseseseeesesesessteneessneeeness vill

List Of T€ADIOS oo... cceccecssccessensessceessseeceeeseeesecesessseaecesaaeesneeesessaeesesaeesenenseesnnseenseenes ix

List Of FIQUIES oo... cetecseeeeseessecseesseeeessecsseeeseeeeesacseecseceeseeceseeaeeeaeeeetuaesnaesnaseagens X

Chapter Page

1. INTRODUCTION .............:cccccscsecsssssceseseacecensneeeeetsnsecesssnseceesaseeseseaeeeeseseeeeessaeees 1

1.1 The One-Hand Ternary Chord Keyboard ............cccccsscseessescesseesseeens 1

1.2 Rationale .0........eeeeeccccscssscssececeeecesssnonsccecceseessssneeescesseeseesaaenenscessessanaasenees 3

1.3 Experimental Purpose and ODjeCtiIVES .............eeeeeseseceeecssesssenenaceneeeees 5

2. LITERATURE REVIEW . 0.0.0... cccccccccecsssseeeecessnaceecesssseeeeeeeeesscaeeecessesaneeees 6

2.1 Overview of Keyboard DeSIQN ...........cssssssssecsscesneecesseessecessseesseeeceeees 6

2.2 The QWERTY Keyboard... ecscessscsssscssscsssccesesecsaeeesssseaseseesaseaeesasenses 7 2.2.1 CHAPMAN (1919) oo... ccesseecesssecesessceecesseecesssseesessesecesesseeeeess 8 2.2.2 THUPStONE (1919) eee eccctecssssseesecseesssesseeseecseesseeeeesnsesneeeeees 10 2.2.3 GreON (1940) cee ceecssccseececessessseeseeseeeseecseeeseeeaecaeteeeeeessneensees 13

2.3 Alternative Layouts of the Standard Keyboard ..............ccccsssssssseeeeees 14 2.3.1 StrONG(1956) ee ceccsecseeesssseseesseerseseseeceeesseeessuesseesseeessnees 16

2.4 Binary Chord Keyboard ou... ccecsesssesssesscssecsesenseseessesseeeecessecsaeceeeceaseas 16 2.4.1 Kl@MMEr (1958) oo. ccceesessseecscseeccsseceessesessesseeetsneeesseeseseeens 17 2.4.2 Lockhead and KlemmMer (1959) ou... ccc cesssssceeeseessseeseeeseeeeees 19 2.4.3 Ratz and Ritchie(1961) and Seibel (1962) 0... 20 2.4.4 Cornog, Hockman, and Craig (1963) ..........c ce ececesseseeeeeeseees 20 2.4.5 Conrad and Longman (1965) .0.........cccceceeseseeeeeesesssneecereeseees 22 2.4.6 Bowen and Guinness (1965) .........cccesssscccesssseecessssseeeeeeseees 24 2.4.7 SidOrsky (1974) ooo... ccceccecscsccesssecssseeesseeeessceceeeeeeeeneeseceneesssneenss 25 2.4.8 Bequaert and Rochester (1977) and Rochester,

Bequaert, and Sharp (1978) ...........cccececsessennececceeeeeceeeeeeseeees 26 2.4.9 Gopher and Eilam (1979) ..........ccccecssseseceeeeseneseteessssneeeeeesees 28 2.4.10 Gopher, Hilsernath, and Raij (1985); Gopher (1986);

and Gopher and Raij (1988) ....... cc eeeeeeceeeseeseessesneeees 29

Page 7: AAlr - Virginia Tech

2.4.11 Richardson, Telson, Koch, and Chrysler (1987) ..............

2.5 Ternary Chord Keyboard Experiments .........eeeceeeecesceeceseeeeeceeeeeaeens 2.5.1 Fathallah (1988) - TOK #1... cccccccsccesseccesssseeeeesseesesssneeeees 2.5.2 Kroemer (1990) - TOK #2 oe cecccscsscceceeenseeseceesneceeeenersennees 2.5.3 Kroemer (1990) - TCK #3 uc ccecssessesecesseeseesssessesaneseeeseens 2.5.4 McMulkin and Kroemer (1991) and

Lee and Kroemer (1991) oe eeeseescessessssssesseesseeseeseeeeenens

2.6 Length of Training S@SSION ........ eee eeecesecececeeeteeeeeeecesaeeeeseeeneseneeaneernans

2.7 Format of Text for TYPiIng ooo... cee ccsccecssecsseeeesteeseeeeeseeecssesessaeeseeenesseseeas

2.8 Sequences of Finger Movements that Yield Fast Typing Rates ....

2.9 Which Hand to Use on a One-Hand Keyboard... eeeecessesereeecees

2.10 Literature SUMMALY ........cccccesscccsssssseeeesesssceeeeeesssceeesesscseneetesesssseneass

3. METHOD 1... 0... cee cccccccccssscessssesecesseeeeeesssseeeesseeeenesssaeesensauseeseesnsuueccasseseeceesensseneans

3.1 SUDJOCES ooo. ceecsecsseessssesseeesseeeesecsensecseneeensecsnsecsaeeseneetsessecseseseseesesuessaes

3.2 APPArAatUS oo. ceecesccseccesscesececnseseecseesssescseeeeecesseeesaeesaessatessteeseesaeeees 3.2.1 Dimensions of the One-Hand TCK uuu... ccc cccseenecssesseseeenees 3.2.2 CHOP COIN ......eccecceccceseesseesseeseecnesseeeseceseeesessecseessaneanecseeses

3.3 PLOCECUIES o.oo... eecestcecceesssccesseecssecesseneesacecsancessesessecesseecesesessanesuaeeneneeteagens 3.3.1 Experimental De@SIQN ...........ccccsssccsssssseecsssseceecesssnseneessesseesenes 3.3.2 Schedule Of Practice 0... ee eeecssscsseeseeseeesetsceesnsssreeeecsseeeeeeas

3.3.2.1 Breakdown Of SESSIONS ou... eesessesecsseeseeeesenens 3.3.2 2 Training Program .........ccssesceessteessseeeesseetsneesseeeenees 3.3.2.3 Makeup Of Trials oo... cece eeseeeseeeeeneceeseeeteeeeenes

3.3.3 Determining Peak Performance ...........:ccccccccsssssssssseneneeeeeeeees 3.3.4 SUBJECTS’ OPINION uo... eee cccessseessecesssecsecessneesseecseseecseseeneecases

3.4 Experimental Protocolo... cece ceseceessecseneecsecesssecseceesacesenseesseesenesetaaes

4. RESULTS. 0000... ccccceccsesccessececssnceseesaneesensneevsneeeesesueeseesseesessaeeesesseseueessaneeenasseees

4.1 Learning ReESUItS oo... sesseesensesesesssecesssnessssecseneseneeeesnssesaeeseeneeseaeenans

4.2 Speed - Accuracy Performance .0.......ececeecceseeeeeseceeeesseceaecseseeseaeeneeaes

4.3 Using Trials to Predict Performance oo... eee eeeseeecseeececeeeeeereeeeatecens

4.4 Using Time to Predict Performance ou... eee ceeeeeesceeeseseeeesteeeeenees

vi

Page 8: AAlr - Virginia Tech

4.5 Subjective Ratings of Performance... ccccccccecsecstectseertasenren 83

4.6 Fastest Speed Attained by the Subjects 2000.00... ce ccc cece eee 86

4.7 Prediction of Learning with Less Than All data Points «0.20.0... 86

4.8 Chord AnalyS@s oo... cccccccccceccesceeecceececseeessseneeeeecvstecsectiteneeecerseeess 102

4.9 Analysis of Average Performance of All Subjects ....0000...00 ee. 111 4.9.1 Prediction of All Subjects’ Performance oo... eee 111 4.9.2 Prediction of Average Performance... cecceeeeceeeees 114

Loam Bho} Os USS) | @] ee 116

5.1 Speed - Accuracy Performance 20.0.0... cece cece ecce sete seeceensetteeeneeas 116

5.2 Using Trials to Predict Performance 0.0... ccccccccccceeseeeetssseeeeeeees 117 5.2.1 Coefficients of the Log-Log Equation ............ cece 118 5.2.2 Prediction of Peak Keying Speed oo... cece ccc cece sees 119 5.2.3 Combined and Average Performance of All the

SUDJOCHS. 200. ee cece ecececesscceeecetseeesceceeessueseesenerseuereeeestnnaees 124

5.3 Predicting Performance from TIME o....... ccc ccc ccccceceeessceecessteentseeeesees 124

5.4 Subjective Ratings 0... cec cece cesseeessescesseceeeesseeecseesessaeeesseeneasees 125

5.5 Evaluation of Criteria to Predict Performance. .....0......0.cccceecc cece 126

5.6 The Concept of Peak Performance. ........000 occ cece cee cc ceee cee e ese ecenees 127

5.7 Prediction of Entire Curve from the Initial Data Points ...0.00000... 128

5.8 Chord AnalySiS oo... ccc cccccecesseseecesecensectseseseseeeeecssseseeseeesesenteensaess 129

5.9 Further RESCAIrCH o.oo. ec ccccceccccesececeeesstsaeecesersaseeseeeesaeeceessteeeesetstseeeceeeas 130

6. REFERENCES ooiniiicccccccccccccecsccceessetecssecesscccseesetseecesseecesessssseeeiteeeesees 132

ADDON GICES ooo cecccccceccccccceccccceccesenesesaeeeececesessssseeeceeeesenessaseeesscceseeetssssssseeseeneaas 137

MV ccc een e nea E Eee cece en DETER EAE EEE A UEEEAEE ESSE EcELLGAEGEEEEEEEEEEAEEe ES et ts bs adEaEOEEEEE Sets 191

vii

Page 9: AAlr - Virginia Tech

List of Appendices

Appendix Page

A Example of a Test Trial oo... escecsessseeseeesesrecceseseeeesseeseseeeeesneseeeees 137

B Experimental INStructions ...........ccccsssssecssscsseesescessseeseessseesasesseessseessreeees 139

C Informed Consent Form 2.0... ecsececeeseeceseesseceesececscseecsacecsceeesaeenseeeeeeees 141

D Derivation of CPMj = CT Bt i eeccssssssssssssssseesseessccesessssssssssnsnnesessseete 144

E Graphs of All Time Increments and Subjects for CPM; = Bo + ByP;

+ Bo/P; + Ba/Pi2 and CPM, = CBP Bt oc ceccsssssneeseeen 146

F Average Response Times for Each Chord by Trial .........eeceeeeeeee: 172

viii

Page 10: AAlr - Virginia Tech

List of Tables

Table Number Page

Table 1. Summary of Important Variables in Keyboarding Studies. .......0..... 48

Table 2. Average Accuracy Across Trials for Each Subject... 64

Table 3. Number of Trials Completed by Each Subject... eee 65

Table 4. Summary of R-Squared Values for All Functions Considered When Trial is the Regressor Variable. 0... ccc eeceeeeeeeeeeeereen 68

Table 5. The Fitted Log-Log Equation for Each Subject. oo... ee 78

Table 6. Statistical Comparison of Coefficients Bo and B1. 0.0... 80

Table 7. Average Length (and Standard Deviation) of 30, 40, 50, and 60 Minute Time Increments Used in the Regressions. ..............0..ccee 82

Table 8. Summary of R-Squared Values for All Functions Considered When Time Increment is the Regressor Variable. 30, 40, 50, and 60 Minute Increments and Sessions are Used. oo... 84

Table 9. Ratings Ranging From 0 to 9 Given by the Subjects Each Week Assessing Their Progress and Corresponding Performance. ......... 87

Table 10. Fastest Speed Attained on Randomized and Repetitive Trials. ..... 88

Table 11. Mean Squared Error for Log-Log Functions Fit Using Only Some of the Initial Performance Data Points. ........... eee 100

Table 12. Mean Squared Error for Four Equations Fit Using Only Some of the Initial Performance Data Points. For Subject 2 Only. ....0...... 109

Table 13. Chord Response Times (in ms) Averaged Over Trials 468 to 487, the Last 20 Trials Performed by All Subjects. «0.0.0... 110

Table 14. Summary of R-Squared Values for All Functions Considered for Average of the Subjects and All Subjects’ Performance. .......... 112

Table 15. Values of R@ and Peak Performance when Fitting the Equation

CPNj = Bo + B;/T; + Bo/T2 Without the First 100 Performance POINS. occ ccccccccccececcesseeeee sees e na nsseeeeetesecetsaeeeeeseeeesessieeeeseseeenesteiieeeeeas 123

Page 11: AAlr - Virginia Tech

List of Figures

Figure Number Page

Figure 1. View of Binary and Ternary KeyS.. ou... eseesceceesseeeceseeseeeeetneeeeeseeeneees 2

Figure 2. Learning Curve fora QWERTY Keyboard Found by Chapman (1919). eee eeecsesescseeeeecseesneeeeeeaecnaecsaesnseeseeseceseceeeseaecuseneesaseneeseeseseeanees 9

Figure 3. Learning Curve fora QWERTY Keyboard Found by Thurstone (1919) Predicted by Number of Pages Typed. ........cseeseneeeeeee 11

Figure 4. Learning Curve fora QWERTY Keyboard Found by Thurstone (1919) Predicted by Number of Weeks of Practice. oe 12

Figure 5. Median Speed for Four Semesters of Typewriting on a QWERTY Keyboard. Adapted from Green (1940). wu. 15

Figure 6. Typing Speed on a 10-key Chord Keyboard as a Function of Practice. Taken from Klemmer (1958). ou... ccsccssssecsssseesseeereesseeseens 18

Figure 7. Comparison of Typing Speeds for Chord and QWERTY Keyboards. Taken from Conrad and Longman (1965)... 23

Figure 8. View of a Chord Keyboard Developed by Rochester et al. (1978). Taken from Rochester et al. (1978). oo. eeeeseeeseeeseeneee 27

Figure 9. Typing Speed for Three Keyboards as a Function of Practice. Taken from Gopher and Raj (1988). oo. ceeeeeceteeeeeeeseeeeeeees 31

Figure 10. Ternary Chord Keyboard Model #1 oo... ccc eeceeeceseeeecereeeceaeeeeeeetenes 34

Figure 11. Ternary Chord Keyboard Model #2. oo. ee eeseseeceseeeesseeeeseaeees 36

Figure 12. Learning Curve for the Ternary Chord Keyboard #2. owe 38

Figure 13. Ternary Chord Keyboard Model #3. oo. eccceesesessesseneesereeennenes 39

Figure 14. Learning Curve for the Ternary Chord Keyboard #3. owe 40

Figure 15. Learning Curve for the One-Hand Ternary Chord Keyboard. ....... 43

Figure 16. Effect of Training Schedule on Typing Performance. Taken from Baddeley and Longman (1978). ...........cccssssscceeeesseesesnsnsesereeerees 45

Figure 17. Dimensions of the One-Hand Ternary Chord Keyboard. ............... 53

Page 12: AAlr - Virginia Tech

Figure Number

Figure 18.

Figure 19.

Figure 20.

Figure 21.

Figure 22.

Figure 23.

Figure 24.

Figure 25.

Figure 26.

Figure 27.

Figure 28.

Figure 29.

Figure 30.

Figure 31.

Figure 32.

Figure 33.

Figure 34.

Page

Chord Assignments for the Numeric One-Hand Ternary Chord KOYDOAIC. oo... ee ecesecsrseseeecnscereneeesenecsaaesenceesneetsvsessasesneeeesenecsaesenanersaeeesess 55

Scale Used by Subjects to Rate Their Performance Each WEEK. once. cccsccccsssecesscecssscecssscecescaeceesaeseesensecseacecsssaeeeeseeseesesseceesesaeessseesenees 61

Subject 2, Prediction of CPMi = Bo + B1*Trial and Actual PErfOrMaNnce. ou... ceecesscsssccesecescceesecssecsceesaeesessseeessensrseeaeecssssasecsasenseenaes 69

Subject 2, Prediction of CPMi = Bo + B1/Trial + B2/Trial*2 and Actual Performance. .0......cccccesssecsssssecceeceecceneessessssseeaeeeeaeseceeeeseeeceseesseees 70

Subject 2, Prediction of CPMi = Bo + B1i*Trial + B2/Trial + B3/Trial*2 and Actual Performance. oe. eecseessecesecseeeeceessesseees 71

Subject 2, Prediction of CPMi = Bo + B1*Ln(Trial) and Actual POrfOrMAN Ce. ce cecssccesssccsssseceesseeecesseecesseeeseeaeeeceneeeceeatecsusueesenueesseaeeeenees 72

Log-Log Prediction and Actual Performance, Subject 1. .............. 73

Log-Log Prediction and Actual Performance, Subject 2. .............. 74

Log-Log Prediction and Actual Performance, Subject 3. .............. 75

Log-Log Prediction and Actual Performance, Subject 4. ............... 76

Log-Log Prediction and Actual Performance, Subject 5. .......0....... 77

Log-Log Prediction for Each Subject. 0... eecssscecsssssecesessseeeceeseees 79

Log-Log Regressions Using Only the First 25, 50, 100, 150, and 200 Points, SUDJOCt 1. ou... eee esececesseceeseeseceeeseneeneecesseeeeeeseseeeees 89

MSE for Log-Log Regressions Using Only a Few Initial Points, SUDJOCE 1. oo. eeecessesssccvsseecseeceseesssecesceecssceecsneesseeecssesecceeseeeeessstecserseesees 91

Log-Log Regressions Using Only the First 25, 50, 100, 150, and 200 Points, SuDjeCt 2. oo... eee eceecceeeeeceeeeeeeeteeteeeeaeeneeesnaeaaaeeneess 92

MSE for Log-Log Regressions Using Only a Few Initial Points, SUDJOCH 2. cee cccccecscssscssescesscesecseecsaecseeenseseseeeesseceasensecseesseseesseesensseetseeeees 93

Log-Log Regressions Using Only the First 25, 50, 100, 150, ANd 200 POINTS, SUDJECt 3... eee scccsseeeeeseceessneeseseetenseeseseseeeeetsaneessnes 94

xi

Page 13: AAlr - Virginia Tech

Figure Number

Figure 35.

Figure 36.

Figure 37.

Figure 38.

Figure 39.

Figure 40.

Figure 41.

Figure 42.

Figure 43.

Figure 44.

Figure 45.

Figure 46.

Figure 47.

Figure 48.

Figure 49.

Page

MSE for Log-Log Regressions Using Only a Few Initial Points, SUDJSCE Oo icc cccccecesececenssececsseceseseeeesseeeecsateserseseseeseseseaesssieaeeeeses 95

Log-Log Regressions Using Only the First 25, 50, 100, 150, and 200 Points, Subject 4. occ rere teneteteeettnereetteeeees 96

MSE for Log-Log Regressions Using Only a Few Initial Points, SUDECE 4. cece ccc cecseccssccessecsesseecsseeecsesenseeeseseeecsseeeesesrtseeetseseneeees Q/

Log-Log Regressions Using Only the First 25, 50, 100, 150, and 200 Points, SUDjECt DO. ieee ce ceeceeeeeeeeeetsseettttnteeeseaeees 98

MSE for Log-Log Regressions Using Only a Few Initial Points, SUDJSCH ccc ccc ccccsecccnteceeecsaeceeaeseeseseesessseeeesegessssesesseeeesiiseeeecsas 99

Mean Squared Error as a Function of Number of Points Used in Regression for All SUBJECTS. ooo ce eect cee ce ects cece eeeereeeeseeneey 101

Subject 2, Bo+B1/Ti+B2/Ti*2 Regressions Using Only the First 25, 50, 100, 150, and 200 PointS. 2.0... cece eeeeeeeeeeetttneeaes 103

Subject 2, MSE for Bo+B1/Ti+B2/Ti*2 Regressions Using Only @ Few Initial PONS. ooo eeccccccsssseccessecesaseeesseeeeesssseesesseecesseseeees 104

Subject 2, Bo+B1*Ti+B2/Ti+B3/Ti42 Regressions Using Only the First 25, 50, 100, 150, and 200 Points. oer creee 105

Subject 2, MSE for Bo+B1*Ti+B2/Ti+B3/Ti*2 Regressions Using Only a Few Initial POInts. oo. ccecsecccceeeeeesseeeeeseseeeaees 106

Subject 2, Bo+B1*Ln(Ti) Regressions Using Only the First 25, 50, 100, 150, and 200 POINtS. ooo. cc ccccccccsteeeeesestseeeesentsteseeeeeas 107

Subject 2, MSE for Bo+B1*Ln(Ti) Regressions Using Only a Few Initial POINS. oo... ccc cc cceeceeeeeeeeceeceeeeceseeseeeeeeestttsssssseeeaeaees 108

Log-Log Prediction and Actual Performance, All Subjects’ Data. ioc ccc eccceececsceccssesenesececcssecesaseseessseseesesecsseseesseeensiveserteasereranens 113

Log-Log Prediction, Actual Average Performance, and + and - 1 Standard Deviation of the Average of All Subjects. 0... 115

Estimate of Peak Speed Using the Eqn CPM = B+B1/Ti+B2/TiA2, Without the First 100 Trials, Sub 3. ow... 121

xii

Page 14: AAlr - Virginia Tech

1. INTRODUCTION

1.1 The One-Hand Ternary Chord Keyboard.

The keyboard is currently the primary device which people use for data

input to, and interaction with, a computer. The accepted keyboard standard is

the QWERTY keyboard (named for the first six keys on the left side of the

second row). Chord keyboards have been proposed as an alternative device to

the QWERTY keyboard. A chord is defined as the simultaneous activation of

two or more keys to produce one input. Although many differences exist

between various chord and sequential keyboards, almost all use binary keys.

The binary key has only two states: on and off. An example of a binary key is

the type of key used on the standard QWERTY keyboard.

A new type of chord keyboard has been design by VATELL Corporation,

Christiansburg, Virginia (U.S. Patent 4,775,255 of October, 1988). This

keyboard, called the Ternary Chord Keyboard (TCK), uses keys which have

three states: two on and one off. Figure 1 shows a comparison of the binary and

ternary keys. The TCK uses the three-position ternary keys, whereas all

previous keyboards have used the two-position binary keys. The TCK,

discussed in detail in section 2.5, is intended to be used by two hands (eight

keys one for each finger) for data entry of the alphabet, numbers, punctuation,

symbols, and cursor keys. Each of 64 "simple" chords is generated by

activating one key with the left hand and one key with the right hand. Several

authors have analyzed the TCK for performance compared to the QWERTY

keyboard, learning times, and performance capabilities (Fathallah, 1988;

Page 15: AAlr - Virginia Tech

Key Activation (finger pushes downward)

-— 7 49

LL ~ ul

(State 1, no input) (State 2, input 1)

Neutral Downward

(a) Binary Key

Key Activation

(finger pushes forward or pulls backward)

7 Lt Vi\ &

| |

(State 1, no input) (State 2, input 1) (State 3, input 2)

Neutral e.g., backward e.g., Forward

(b) Ternary key

Figure 1. View of Binary and Ternary Keys

Page 16: AAlr - Virginia Tech

Callaghan, 1989; Kroemer, 1990). All of these experiments investigated the

use of a two-hand TCK as an alternative to the QWERTY keyboard.

Many data computation and entry tasks only require the use of a

numerical keypad. Numerical keypads require only 18 distinct characters (i.e.,

the numerals 0 to 9, "plus", "minus", "divide", "multiply", "decimal point",

"backspace", "subtotal", and "total"). A chord on the one-hand TCK is defined as

the activation of two keys by two different fingers on the same hand. (This is

different from the two-hand TCK in which one finger is used from both hands to

generate a chord.)

A total of 24 simple chords (those generated by two fingers) are possible

on the one-hand TCK. McMulkin and Kroemer (1991) and Lee and Kroemer

(1991) determined subjective ratings for ease of activation, response times, and

fastest possible keying capabilities for all 24 chords on the one-hand TCK. The

characters (numbers and functions) used for a numeric keypad were then

assigned to the 18 "best" chords of the one-hand TCK.

1.2 Rationale.

When considering how people learn to use keyboards, several questions

arise. First, what is the “learning curve” to reach peak performance? In other

words, how does performance progress, how long does it take to reach

maximum keying rate, and how fast does a person type after a certain number

of hours using a keyboard? Second, what is the maximum speed a person can

type using a particular keyboard (maximum speed being the point where the

rate of typing stops to increase significantly)? Finally, how can it be judged

when peak performance has been reached, and what criteria should be used to

Page 17: AAlr - Virginia Tech

assess performance/skill? Prior research on keyboard use has concentrated

primarily on finding the maximal performance and ignored the rate of progress

to reach final speed and the criteria on which to predict speed.

The term “learning curve" is often used when discussing keyboarding

tasks. For the purposes of this study, learning curve is defined as the way in

which a subject's performance progresses or improves over a period of time or

number of trials. The term learning curve is not intended to mean that there is

one equation that describes the way in which people learn all skills.

The QWERTY keyboard has been in use for over 100 years, yet

surprisingly few studies have been conducted to establish learning curves.

Naturally, with the number of people that use the QWERTY keyboard, maximal

typing speeds have been reported, but the criteria (time or number of characters

typed) used to indicate performance progress to top speed have not been

studied.

As alternative keyboard designs are investigated, the main thrust of

experimentation is to compare the performance of the new design with the

QWERTY design. In most reported experiments, subjects using the new

keyboard did not achieve peak performance (peak performance being the

fastest attainable keying speed); however, the experimenters compared

performance between the new keyboard and QWERTY keyboard at equivalent

points in learning. For example, Bowen and Guinness (1965), in evaluating a

mail sorting task, stated that after 25 hours of training, people using a chord

keyboard sorted 30 letters per minute while people using a QWERTY typewriter

sorted 33 letters per minute. There was no discussion of how performance

progressed during those 25 hours. No indication was given that “hours of

Page 18: AAlr - Virginia Tech

training” was the best criterion to evaluate speed; perhaps, "number of

characters typed" would be a better measure.

This study examined the performance progress of subjects on the one-

hand TCK. The subjects were taught 18 chords. After learning the chords, the

subjects practiced using the one-hand TCK for an extended period of time, 60

hours, so that they approached maximal keying speed.

1.3 Experimental Purpose and Objectives.

This study had the following primary experimental objectives:

1. Investigate the performance progress of people on the one-hand Ternary

Chord Keyboard as they attain peak keying speed.

2. Find a function which accurately predicts the learning or progress of the

subjects. Evaluate a set of criteria to find which can be used to best

predict a subject's performance in keyboarding. The criteria sets are:

1) The number of hours a person spends practicing.

2) The number of trials or characters typed.

3) Subjects’ opinion of when they have reached peak performance.

3. Determine the number of initial performance data points that are needed to

predict the entire learning function.

Page 19: AAlr - Virginia Tech

2. LITERATURE REVIEW

2.1 Overview of Keyboard Design.

The first keyboard that used the QWERTY layout was designed and

produced in 1874. Several attempts have been made to improve this design.

Many of the alternative designs have been concerned with rearranging the

keys. The most famous of these are the Dvorak keyboard (named for the

inventor August Dvorak, not for the arrangement of the keys) and the alphabetic

layout (Noyes, 1983b).

Other alternative designs have employed chords (simultaneous

activation of two or more keys to produce one input) to enter data. Colombo

was the first to attempt to develop a mechanical chord keyboard in 1942 when

he requested a patent (Noyes, 1983a). Since that time, many other chord

keyboards have been proposed and tested (Gopher and Raij, 1988;

Richardson, Telson, Koch, and Chrysler, 1987; Gopher, 1986; Kirschenbaum,

Friedman, and Melnik, 1986; Gopher, Hilsernath, and Raij, 1985; Bartram and

Feggou, 1985; Mussin, 1980; Gopher and Eilam, 1979; Rochester, Bequaert,

and Sharp, 1978; Sidorsky, 1974; Bowen and Guinness, 1965; Conrad and

Longman, 1965; Cornog, Hockman, and Craig, 1963; Seibel, 1962; Ratz and

Ritchie, 1961; Lockhead and Klemmer, 1959; Klemmer, 1958). These

keyboards involved different numbers of keys, required the use of one hand or

both hands, and used different numbers of fingers to activate a chord.

The literature on various keyboards will be reviewed for information

relevant to the objectives of this study. Emphasis will be given to studies

evaluating performance progress during a keyboarding task and the criteria

Page 20: AAlr - Virginia Tech

used to evaluate learning. Several studies have been conducted in which only

final performance on the keyboard is listed with little regard for the way it was

attained (e.g. performance at hourly increments). These will be reviewed

because they provide some insight to the objectives of this experiment. The

important variables that needed to be considered for this study will be

discussed.

2.2 The QWERTY Keyboard.

The typewriter has changed significantly from its first design in 1874. It

began as a manual entry device in which the human fingers generated the force

to move the typebars which struck the paper. The next major evolutionary

advancement in typing was the advent of the electric typewriter. The fingers still

activated the keys, but an electric motor generated the force to actuate the type

bars (or some other device to print characters on paper). Now, computer word

processing is replacing the conventional typewriter; keys are still activated by

the fingers, but electrical impulses generate the characters on a CRT to be

printed later on paper. Through all these mechanical and electrical changes

one element of data entry has remained essentially the same - the layout of the

characters on the keyboard.

Since the layout of the keys has not changed much since its invention,

the ability of people to learn the location of the characters and attain peak

performance should not have changed much either. However learning curves

for the QWERTY keyboard are not well documented. In the early 1900's,

several schools existed that taught students to type. Three of these schools

Page 21: AAlr - Virginia Tech

documented speed progress to improve teaching methods. These three studies

will be reviewed (Chapman, 1919: Thurstone, 1919; and Green, 1940).

2.2.1 Chapman (1919).

The purpose of Chapman's (1919) study was to observe the rate of

typing improvement of high school students. The number of characters taught

to the subjects was approximately 65. Although this was not explicitly stated,

subjects were taught to use the typewriter; the early typewriters probably

contained between 60 and 70 characters. He administered a five-minute typing

test to 20 students once a week; the tests were not begun until the students had

completed 20 hours of practice time to learn the keyboard. Every week, for

each student, the author recorded hours practiced (both in and out of school),

gross number of words typed during the five-minute test, and number of errors.

The author then plotted average net words per minute typed (gross words per

minute minus the number of words with errors) versus practice time for 20 to

180 hours. Chapman (1919) found the following results: 1) an initial linear

learning relationship existed for the first 75 hours, 2) the slope of the learning

curve abruptly changed at 90 hours of practice; the rate of learning was six

times as large between 20 to 90 hours as it was between 90 to 180 hours, 3) at

180 hours of practice the curve was nearly flat which indicates the students

were at nearly peak performance (48 words per minute or 240 characters per

minute were actually reached). Figure 2 shows the learning curve that

Chapman (1919) obtained. Note that the curve shown is the average over all

20 students; it is not for a single subject.

Page 22: AAlr - Virginia Tech

(6161) uwewdeuy

Aq punoy pueoghdy ALHAMDO

B& 40}

ening Bulusea

“Zz aunbi4

SINOH

O8l O91

Ov l Ocl

OO| O8

09 Ov

0¢ 0

0 OV

08

Ocl

ys

Sa]NuIW eAl4

x

ul padA| Spon,

Ps 091

00¢

al

_-

:- Ove

08¢

Page 23: AAlr - Virginia Tech

One problem with this study is that the practice time between tests for

each student was not kept constant. For example, subject A may have practiced

eight hours in week five, while subject B practiced only three hours during that

week; yet, the same test was administered to both subjects at the same time.

This problem left unequal sample sizes at each interval of time, as well as

different subjects’ data at each time.

An interesting result of this study was that subjects reached peak

performance in about 180 hours (although practice on each character is not

equivalent). Another important note is that the author related performance to

hours of practice. No other practice criterion was measured, such as pages

typed.

2.2.2 Thurstone (1919).

Fifty-one students learning to type at a business school were tested for

typing speed once a week for 28 consecutive weeks. Students practiced 10

hours a week (2 hours daily). At the end of each week, Thurstone (1919) gave a

test to measure the number of words typed over four minutes. Words typed per

four minutes were plotted as a function of total practice pages typed. The author

fit a curve to the data which yielded the following equation: y = 216(x +19)/(x +

148), where y was average words typed in four minutes and x was number of

pages of practice. The above equation predicted a maximum typing speed of

54 words per minute (WPM) or 216 words in four minutes (as shown in Figures

3 and 4) after over 1000 pages of practice. Subjects actually reached an

average of 41 WPM (205 characters per minute, CPM) after about 250 pages of

10

Page 24: AAlr - Virginia Tech

OO€ O8c

09¢ Ove

Occ O0Z

O8l O9I

OVI Ol

OO!

padA| sebey

j0 waqUuINN

Aq payaipaid

(6161) suojsuinyy,

Aq punoy

pueoghkay ALHAMD

B® 10}

BAIND Bulwee]

‘¢ eunbi4

pedd| seabed

jo vaquiny

jejo,

08 09

Ov O02

O

/

Jf

oT

ar

a"

_

a?”

.

at een

nt

—orre

Ole

yur BOB

Pslpipsaird

lenjoy .

paypal ww]

0 0c

Ov

09

08

001

Ocl

Or |

091

08 |

00¢

Occ 4

11

seinul~— sno4

ul padA|

SPJOAA

Page 25: AAlr - Virginia Tech

DONOCI

JO SHBOAA

JO OQUUNN

Aq pajyoIpald

(6161) euojsunyy

Aq punoy

pueoghsy ALHAMDO

& 10}

avINy Buiwea]

“py aunbi4

BOIIDBId JO

SYB9OM

ce O€

82 92

ve 22

OF BI

9 VE

Zt OL

8 9

vw @

O

nt

UGHBIO|BISY- OANSOd —

eo

we

a

A

uolqiajaooy enefon

a

> a

S$

ae “

¢

ow wg

¢

9L¢ ‘ytht]

Ss1e1_

PSssipaig

jemoy }

payipald yur)

0 02

OV

09

08

OOl

Oct

Ov |

091

081

00¢

Occ

Ove

seynuiw ino4

ul pedA|

SPlO/AA

12

Page 26: AAlr - Virginia Tech

practice. The entire learning curve for performance versus pages typed is

shown in Figure 3. This curve is the average over all subjects.

Thurstone (1919) then plotted words typed in four minutes versus weeks

of practice (about 10 hours of practice per week for 28 weeks), see Figure 4.

This plot yielded a curve which was treated as a third order polynomial,

whereas the plot of WPM versus pages of practice was described as a second

order polynomial by Thurstone (1919).

This study suffers from the same constraints as Chapman's (1919). The

tests were administered to all subjects at the same time, but the subjects were at

different levels of practice (number of pages typed). It is interesting to note that

the shape of the learning curve changes from second to third order when the

criterion to judge performance was changed from pages typed to weeks (hours)

of practice. This might be due to the fact that the pages typed per hour

increased over the 28 weeks.

2.2.3 Green (1940).

Green (1940) studied the typing progress of students over one semester

of classes. There were four groups of students: those in first semester typing,

those in second semester typing (who had already taken typing in the first

semester), those in third semester typing (who had already taken typing in the

first and second semesters), and those in fourth semester typing (who had

already taken typing in the first, second, and third semesters). Progress was

only recorded for one semester; therefore, for example, the learning curve for

the first three semesters of the fourth semester students was not known. Speed

tests were administered to the subjects once a week for 18 weeks.

13

Page 27: AAlr - Virginia Tech

Students in the fourth semester reached a peak speed of about 47 WPM

at week 18. This was presumably after a total of about 360 hours of practice (18

weeks X 5 hours/week X 4 semesters). The rate of learning (slope of the

learning curve) became flatter from the first to the fourth semester, see Figure 5.

A concern with this study is that four different groups of people are used

for different sections of the learning curve. This made it difficult to track

performance progress from start to finish. Also, WPM was plotted as a function

of weeks of practice; this was a fairly coarse measure, considering how much

practice time could vary between subjects in a week's time.

2.3 Alternative Layouts of the Standard Keyboard.

The most famous attempts to change the QWERTY layout have been the

Dvorak keyboard named for its inventor, August Dvorak, and the alphabetic

layout (Noyes, 1983b); the most common alphabetic layout had the letters

arranged with ABCDEFGHIJ on the second row, KLMNOPQRST on the third

row, and UVWXYZ on the bottom row, although other alphabetic layouts existed

(Norman and Fisher, 1982). While neither of these designs has experienced

commercial success, studies have been conducted to compare the performance

of alternative designs to the QWERTY layout. The majority of the papers

published on alternative layouts present theoretical arguments for the

advantages of one layout over the other. The one paper found that employed

experimentation to assess performance of subjects using an alternative

keyboard will be discussed.

14

Page 28: AAlr - Virginia Tech

50

4th Semester

Progress 46

44

42

40

38

36

34

32

30

28

26

24

22

20

18

16

14

12

10

3rd Semester

Progress

2nd Semester

Progress

1st Semester

< Progress

Spee

d (in

Words

per

Minu

te)

mo NN

ff

DMD

®

012 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 15 16 17 18

Weeks

Figure 5. Median Speed for Four Semesters of Typewriting on a QWERTY Keyboard. Adapted from Green (1940).

15

Page 29: AAlr - Virginia Tech

2.3.1 Strong (1956).

In an experiment to evaluate the Dvorak layout, Strong (1956) retrained

ten typists to use the Dvorak keyboard. They were trained on the new keyboard

until they had reached the original speed they had possessed on the QWERTY

keyboard. The time to retrain the subjects was recorded in number of hours and

was largely a function of original speed; the faster the original speed the longer

the retraining. Once the subjects had been retrained on the Dvorak keyboard,

ten additional subjects using the QWERTY keyboard were added to the

experiment who had roughly the same typing speed as the Dvorak group. Both

groups were then given an additional 100 hours of training. Over the final 100

hours of training the QWERTY group improved from an average of 61.7 WPM to

81.7 WPM (on a five minute typing test), a 20 WPM improvement. The Dvorak

group improved from an average of 54.9 WPM to 66.2 WPM (on a five minute

typing test), an 11.3 WPM improvement. Overall the QWERTY group improved

8.7 WPM more (20 WPM improvement minus 11.3 WPM improvement) than the

Dvorak group. No further conclusions could be drawn from this experiment.

Although the Dvorak keyboard has been a popular alternative to the QWERTY

keyboard, no other experiments were found which compared it to the QWERTY

keyboard. Therefore, no further conclusions can be drawn for this study.

2.4 Binary Chord Keyboards. |

A variety of chord keyboards have been investigated which differ in

aspects such as number of keys used, number of fingers used to generate a

chord, and character set size. For each of the following studies, how the

16

Page 30: AAlr - Virginia Tech

keyboard worked will be briefly described, as well as performance results

obtained.

2.4.1 Klemmer (1958).

One of the first documented experiments on a chord keyboard was

performed by Klemmer (1958) on a ten-key chord keyboard (one key for each

digit). Two subjects were taught a set of 30 characters (26 letters, "space",

"return", "period", and "comma"). Ten characters were generated with one

finger, nine with two fingers of the same hand, and eleven with one finger from

each hand. Klemmer (1958) had the subjects repeatedly type 17 different text

sets which consisted of 60 words drawn from a list of the 1000 most frequent

words in English. The subjects typed the word sets for approximately 35 hours

(50 minutes a day, 3 times a week for 12 weeks); one subject improved from 20

WPM to 60 WPM (went 19 times through the group of 1020 words), while the

other went from 17 WPM to 37 WPM (12 times through the 1020 words).

Learning curves, Figure 6, were generated as WPM versus number of times

through the group of 1020 words. Trends in the curves are difficult to extract.

For an additional six hours, subjects typed samples of text containing about 670

characters. The first subject consistently averaged about 47 WPM while the

second averaged about 30 WPM.

A relation between size of character set and amount of practice time to

high performance can be inferred by comparing this study to the studies by

Chapman (1919), Thurstone (1919), and Green (1940). Klemmer (1958) taught

30 characters and reached around 45 WPM (225 CPM) in approximately 35

17

Page 31: AAlr - Virginia Tech

(9961) JowWal|y

WO

UeYe] ‘aOOBI

JO UONOUNY

e Se

Preoghay ProYyD

Aey-01 B

UO paads

BudAy “9g

aunbl4

SVVINL

SAISSZIONS

LX31

430 SQHOM

Gt6

AO

SdNOHD

SQYOM

WOONVY

OzZOi 4O

SdNodYo

be

zdgii 6}

8! 2)

Of Sl

bl fi

fio 01

6 82

9 GC

¢ ¢

2 !

O

TT

TT

1 ITT

TT TTT

1 T7712?!

(if)

LO3BrAaNsS o

(gy) LOB¢aNsS

VY

SNOYUM S3MOYULS

4O %=

LNIOd TWINL

HOVZ 3A08V

S3YNdIZ

vor

20 >-

roo.

20

OS

Q3dAL BLNNIW U3d ,SOYOM, Y3LIVYVHO-S SSOYD

09

18

Page 32: AAlr - Virginia Tech

hours. Chapman (1919), Thurstone (1919), and Green (1940) taught about 65

characters and did not reach 45 WPM (225 CPM) until 160 to 300 hours.

2.4.2 Lockhead and Klemmer (1959).

A chord keyboard using eight keys, one for each finger, was developed

by Lockhead and Klemmer (1959). Chords were generated by simultaneous

activation of one to eight keys. Lockhead and Klemmer began by teaching four

subjects the chord patterns for 100 words. On the average, subjects took 20

hours to learn the 100 word patterns. Subjects then learned the 26 letters of the

alphabet and the 10 numerals; it took an additional six hours to learn the 36

new characters and integrate them with the original 100 word patterns. The

subjects then typed text (for about two hours) which contained approximately an

equal number of words which were in the 100 word pattern chords and words

which had to be typed one letter at a time. Subjects averaged about 15 WPM

(or 45 chords entered per minute, [15/2]X2 + 15/2, as estimated by the author of

this paper).

This study used a considerably larger chord/character set than the other

experiments that are reviewed. Long training times and low chords per minute

are expected with such a large set of chords. The chords entered per minute

are difficult to interpret because all the data are reported as WPM. Lockhead

and Klemmer (1959) guessed that the upper limit of speed for chords using

three fingers or more is 120 chords per minute. Single finger key activations

were estimated to have an upper speed of 300 key entries per minute.

However, they gave no indication as to the length of training time required to

reach these limits.

19

Page 33: AAlr - Virginia Tech

2.4.3 Z Ritchie (1961) an ibel (1962

Ratz and Ritchie (1961) and Seibel (1962) used the same one-hand five-

key chord keyboard. A total of 31 chords were possible using 1, 2, 3, 4, or 5-

finger combinations. The chord to be activated was presented on a display of

five lights that corresponded to the five keys. Both studies measured the

response time of all 31 chords (time from onset of light pattern to activation of all

of the corresponding keys). Ratz and Ritchie obtained an average response

time of 1.2 seconds after 2,660 measured response times. Seibel found an

average response time of about 0.32 seconds after 11,000 response times. The

large difference in response times was assumably due to more practice in

Seibel's experiment. In fact, Seibel (1962) performed the study to show that

Ratz and Ritchie (1961) had not attained the shortest response times possible.

These response times do not indicate training periods to reach maximum

performance on a typing task, but they indicate the approximately fastest keying

rate attainable when there are 31 choices.

2.4.4 Cornog, Hockman, and Craig (1963),

Cornog et al. (1963) experimented with a chord keyboard for sorting mail

that had 12 keys (2 rows of 6) for each hand, two keys for each finger and four

keys for the thumb. Each hand, on its own, could generate all 26 letters and 10

numbers (each hand operated an independent keyboard). Twelve characters

could be entered by activating one key while the remaining 24 required the

depression of two keys (from the same hand) simultaneously. The authors

trained subjects for one-and-a-half hours a day for 11 weeks, a total of about 80

20

Page 34: AAlr - Virginia Tech

hours until they reached "moderate" proficiency; the authors do not elaborate on

how proficiency was determined or what "moderate" means. For the next 15

weeks, the subjects performed sorting on an outgoing mail task. No learning

curves of progress performance were reported; however average performance

at the end of 15 weeks was 232 characters per minute (CPM). For weeks 16 to

30 the subjects performed an incoming mail sorting task. Typing speed at the

end of week 30 was an average of 180 CPM. Finally, during weeks 31 to 36,

subjects performed an originating mail sorting task. At the completion of week

36, average typing speed was 170 CPM.

Subjects received a considerable amount of practice using the chord

keyboard (estimated at least 300 hours). However, they were learning a very

difficult task. First, they had to learn 36 character chords for each hand.

Second, they had to learn the coding schemes to sort mail, i.e., what letters and

numbers to enter for each address. The task of the subjects was to read the

address on the letter, determine the code that needed to be entered on the

keyboard, recall the chords needed to produce the code, and then enter those

chords. Therefore, it is difficult to separate the portion of practice attributable to

learning the keyboard from the portion of time attributed to learning to read and

code addresses. Also, the average CPM at the end of each section of practice

was confounded with the type of task - outgoing, incoming, or originating mail.

However, it appears that highest performance, 232 CPM, was found before the

task was changed at the end of 11 weeks of training and an additional 15

weeks of practice - a total of about 190 hours.

21

Page 35: AAlr - Virginia Tech

2.4.5 Conrad and Longman (1965),

An experiment to compare the standard typewriter with a chord keyboard

was conducted by Conrad and Longman (1965) for the British post office. The

chord keyboard was comprised of 12 keys, six for each hand. Letters and

numbers, a total of 36 characters, were generated by pressing two keys, one by

each hand. An additional five keys were used for the functions of enter, cancel,

and coding of local addresses. Subjects participated in the experiment for 3 1/2

hours a day, 5 days a week for 7 weeks. One group of 24 subjects learned the

36 characters on the standard typewriter, while another group of 22 subjects

was taught the chord keyboard.

The intent of the study was to compare relative performance of the two

keyboards at stages along the learning curve, not to find the maximum

performance possible in seven weeks of practice. However, the learning curves

and method are pertinent to the objectives of this study.

The group trained on the chord keyboard became "operational" in an

average of 12 days, while the typewriter group needed an average time of 21

days; "operational" was defined as the subjects being able to type all 36

characters accurately as determined by the instructor. The typewriter group

became "operational" an average of 9 days slower than the chord group. After

learning the characters, the two groups practiced to increase speed for the

remainder of the 35 days. Figure 7 shows the learning curves for both groups

as a function of days. Performance on each day was an average of four speed

tests across all subjects that were considered operational by that day. It is

important to note that the performance curves begin earlier for the chord group

because its members were operational earlier. When the typewriter group was

22

Page 36: AAlr - Virginia Tech

Os

O8

06

ool

(G96) uewHu07

pue peiu0y

wo

uaye] ‘spueogheay

ALHAMO

puke puoyy

410} speeds

BHuldA| jo

uosuedwoy

‘7 ainbi4

BINIWDIg pO

SADG 3A1199)I9

ee ce

oe sz

9e ve

zz 0%

gi 91

vi zl

Ol 6

a

GG

Mi i

YILIUMSdAL

{

r AVO

SIHL NO

SLD3PENS JO

oN ‘

t ‘

‘ t

t '

§ ‘

. ‘

‘ '

61— O2-

O2— 12-2

zz ~-61—

6i— 8I—

81 — SI

—C1— a

— ZI

— zi—2r— zl

OS

O9

oe

O06

OOo!

ww/do 32g s3902)sh7y

23

Page 37: AAlr - Virginia Tech

added to the graph, it was consistently slower than the chord group. It should

also be pointed out that at the end of training, after 120 total hours, typing speed

had reached almost 100 CPM for the chord group and was still increasing

approximately linearly. However, each day represented three hours of training

So it is difficult to interpret the utility of each hour.

An editorial note: the training schedules in the studies differed

significantly. For example, Klemmer (1958) trained subjects for one hour a day

while Conrad and Longman (1965) had subjects practice nearly four hours a

day. Massed or distributed training can have profoundly different effects on

performance (Goldstein, 1986); this aspect will be covered in greater detail later

(section 2.6). In reviewing each of the studies, caution should be used in

relating total hours of practice to final performance because the method to reach

that total number of hours may drastically affect performance.

2.4.6 Bowen and Guinness (1965),

Bowen and Guinness (1965) designed another experiment investigating

the differences between chord and standard keyboards in a mail sorting task.

Three keyboards were investigated: a standard typewriter, a large chord

keyboard (12 keys for each hand), and a small chord keyboard (6 keys for each

hand). Subjects learned 11 keying patterns corresponding to 11 3-digit

numbers displayed. For the typewriter, two keys were depressed sequentially

for each 3-digit pattern; for the large chord keyboard, 1, 2, or 3 keys were

depressed simultaneously for one 3-digit pattern, while for the small chord

keyboard 1, 2, 3, or 4 keys were activated at the same time.

24

Page 38: AAlr - Virginia Tech

After practicing 20 hours, six subjects on each keyboard performed 800

experimental encodings. The typewriter group performed an average of 40

sorts per minute (this is actually 80 key presses per minute), while the large

chord keyboard group and small chord keyboard groups averaged 49 and 55

sorts (or chords) per minute, respectively. No learning curves over the 20 hours

of practice were provided. The training schedule, e.g. hours per day, number of

days, etc., was not indicated.

2.4.7 Sidorsky (1974),

Sidorsky (1974) experimented with a one hand combined chord and

sequential keyboard for use in battlefield data entry. The keyboard consisted of

five keys, one for each digit of the right hand. Single characters were generated

by two consecutive inputs of single keys or chords. Each input was either the

thumb, little finger, or combinations (chords) of the index, middle, and ring

fingers. For example, the letter F was produced by simultaneously pressing the

keys under the index, middle, and ring fingers followed by depression of the

index finger key. Ten subjects were taught about 40 characters (letters,

numbers, and punctuation). Subjects were then given 20 text tests (each 255

characters long, consisting of typical sentences) each day to type. The training

and testing process was conducted in 10 sessions of 1/2 hour over 10 to 15

days, a total of 5 hours. Considering only each subject's fastest trial, the

average speed achieved was 63 CPM.

Sidorsky (1974) provided no information on performance progress from

day to day. He only counted the fastest trial a subject obtained on any day.

Considering the size of the character set, 40, and the length of practice, the

25

Page 39: AAlr - Virginia Tech

subjects attained fast data entry, because 63 CPM actually corresponded to

twice as many chord activations.

2.4.8 Bequaert and Rochester (1977) and Rochester, Bequaert, and Sharp

(1978),

A chord keyboard consisting of two rows of five keys was developed by

Rochester et al. (1978) for use with one-hand. Figure 8 shows the layout of the

keys and the patterns of keys required to generate characters; the bottom part of

the figure shows the symbols activated by the shift function. Four subjects

learned about 54 characters (letters, punctuation, and symbols, but no

numbers).

Bequaert and Rochester (1977) and Rochester et al. (1978) appear to

have reported on the same experiments. Subjects were trained for one 50-

minute session a day, 5 days a week. After an average of 49 sessions or 37

hours, subjects could touch-type all 54 characters. Their average speed at this

time was 18 WPM (90 CPM), however, this was achieved during repeated

practice of one text file. The subjects then strived to increase their tying speed.

They did this by repeatedly practicing a short text passage, alternated about half

of the time with new typing material. The results are difficult to understand. For

example, Rochester et al. (1978) stated that three subjects achieved typing

speeds of over 40 WPM (200 CPM); the fourth gained a speed of 25 WPM (125

CPM) after a total of about 90 hours of practice. These speeds were reported as

achieved on previously unpracticed material. Whereas, Bequaert and

Rochester (1977) reported typing speeds of 46, 47, 32, and 45 WPM for

previously unpracticed material. They also provided performance progress

26

Page 40: AAlr - Virginia Tech

DIMPLES PRESSED WITH

| INDEX | MIDDLE | RING | | FINGER | FINGER | FINGER |

| | | | D 8 F Ss N K ! J U A

| | | ~<| T Ww P Vv H x Zz

{ i

| G c M | L R a | Y | O |

J J | !

SPECIAL

NUMBER

UPPER—LOW

SPACE

SPACE— UP

PER

SPACE—

REVERSE

REVERSE

I ' ‘ ! | INDEX | MIDDLE | RING | | FINGER | FINGER | FINGER |

4 | T O “Vel i(Vr “x¥s]i;¥N =VK/ t:¥s «Vu /-VYa @

T 7Y¥w P vi l/V¥rH - ¥x V7 é E { ‘ '

VA >Ycl],¥M SVL] I<V¥R }YVal]]- Ik aViil |7Vo | |

| l |

Figure 8. View of Chord Keyboard Developed by Rochester et al. (1978). Taken from Rochester et al. (1978).

2/

Page 41: AAlr - Virginia Tech

curves as a function of one-hour sessions. It appears that the maximum typing

performance occurred at the final session. It is assumed the tests, given (once

every four sessions) to judge typing speed, were new (unpracticed) material,

but it was not explicitly stated.

It is difficult to determine any trends in performance progress from the

learning curves. First, the tests were only given every four sessions. Second, it

is not known if new or old material was used, how long the tests were, or what

the distribution of characters in the test was. Therefore, it is hard to determine if

a “maximum” performance was approached.

2.4.9 Gopher and Eilam (1979),

A one-hand chord keyboard similar to that used by Sidorsky (1974) was

tested by Gopher and Eilam (1979). The keyboard consisted of only four keys,

one for each digit of the right hand except the little finger. Two consecutive

chords by combinations of the three fingers were required to create one

(Hebrew) character. The thumb was pressed sequentially with one of the three-

finger chords to activate an editing function.

In the first experiment by Gopher and Eilam (1979), subjects were taught

30 letters and functions. The training occurred over seven one-hour sessions.

Three one-hour sessions separated by two hours were administered on day

one, two one-hour sessions on day two, and two one-hour sessions six weeks

later (to test retention). At the end of the seventh session subjects typed an

average of 65 CPM (130 strokes). There was linear increase in speed over the

seven sessions, even with six weeks off after five sessions.

28

Page 42: AAlr - Virginia Tech

In the second experiment, Gopher and Eilam (1979) taught subjects 40

letters and functions in six one-hour sessions spread over three to five weeks.

For half of each session, subjects practiced text from a newspaper while the

other half they practiced nonsensical material. Subjects reached an average

speed of 56 CPM on text and 41 CPM on nonsense trials. Their performance

progress on both sets of material was close to linear.

For the same amounts of practice, the subjects in experiment #1

consistently typed 10 CPM faster then subjects in experiment #2. One

explanation of this phenomenon may be the larger character set in the second

experiment. The increase in number of choices of chords may have caused

reaction times to be longer and typing speed to be lower. This effect may mean

that peak performance is attained faster with smaller character sets. It is difficult

to interpret if maximal speed might be different or if time to reach top speed

varies by character set size.

2.4.10 Gopher, Hilsernath, and Raij (1985): Gopher (1986): and Gopher and

Raij (1

In a set of related studies, Gopher et al. (1985), Gopher (1986), and

Gopher and Raij (1988) performed experiments on a one-hand five-key chord

keyboard, one key for each digit. A total of 31 chords were possible considering

1, 2, 3, 4, or 5 finger combinations. The most recent and comprehensive of

these studies, reported by Gopher and Raij (1988), will be discussed in detail.

Three groups of subjects were assigned to one of three keyboards: the

QWERTY type keyboard, a one-hand chord keyboard, and a two-hand chord

keyboard (each hand could produce all characters). Gopher and Raij (1988)

29

Page 43: AAlr - Virginia Tech

taught all subjects 22 Hebrew letters and the space character. All subjects in

the three groups were able to memorize the patterns for the characters in 30-45

minutes. The subjects then participated in 35 one-hour sessions; three to four

sessions were held a week. Each session consisted of 15 three-minute typing

trials: each trial was followed by a one-minute rest break. The average CPM for

each session (mean of 15 trials) was then plotted versus session to obtain a

learning curve (Figure 9). The characters per minute plotted on the chart is an

average over all subjects.

At the end of the thirty-fifth session the performance of the one-hand

chord keyboard group was 178 CPM; for the two-hand chord keyboard group

performance was 208 CPM, while the QWERTY group had a speed of 118

CPM. The results of the one-hand chord keyboard are most applicable to this

study. The rate of learning between sessions 20-24 was 4.36 letters per

session for the one-hand chord keyboard; between sessions 26-35, the rate of

learning had dropped to 1.86 letters per session. This indicates a flattening of

the learning curve beyond 30 hours of training. While apparently peak

performance had not been obtained, progress was clearly slowing for the one-

hand chord keyboard group after 30 sessions.

2.4.11 Richardson, Telson, Koch

Richardson et al. (1987) compared three types of keyboards for a mail

encoding task: a one-hand conventional calculator layout, a two-hand 10-key

sequential keyboard like the top row of a QWERTY layout, and a two-hand 10-

key chord keyboard with 5 keys for each hand. Richardson et al. were

interested in data entry of ZIP codes so the subjects were taught only the ten

30

Page 44: AAlr - Virginia Tech

220

200 4

180

160 4

140 4

120 4

100 4

80 -

Choracters

/ Minute

60 -

+>

40 + °

Two hands chord

Single hand chord

Standard QWERTY

0 i T TT T

0 10 20

Session Number

Figure 9. Typing Speed for Three Keyboards as a Function of Practice. Taken from Gopher and Raij (1988).

31

30

Page 45: AAlr - Virginia Tech

numerals. On the chord keyboard, each of the hands could input all ten

numbers. Subjects were presented single numbers and required to press the

correct key or chord. They had to encode an average of 100 numbers per

minute with 95% accuracy for three consecutive tests (300 numbers per test).

Next, two digit strings were presented with a passing criterion of 70 strings per

minute with 95% accuracy. Finally, training was completed with subjects

entering three digit strings at a rate of 65 strings per minute with 95% accuracy.

Training was conducted in sessions that were three hours and ten

minutes long. Within each session, subjects practiced for 30 minutes, followed

by a 10 minute break, followed by 30 minutes of practice, and so on. The mean

time for training of the calculator and sequential keyboards to the criterion of two

digit strings (70 strings per minute or 140 CPM) was estimated at 22 hours; for

the chord keyboard, it was 97 hours. Therefore, the time to reach 140 CPM fora

character set size of 10 was 22 hours for the two sequential keyboards and 97

hours for the chord keyboard. However, the training methods used to teach the

subjects were peculiar. First, the subjects were not given any feedback on their

accuracy during a trial. It is also not known if the subjects knew the final

criterion needed to advance to the next stage. Subjects may have had to

randomly set their speed and accuracy levels until they hit the correct criterion.

Second, although the chord keyboard group could enter the strings

independently with each hand they were not given specific instructions such as

enter the first number with the left hand and the second with the right hand.

32

Page 46: AAlr - Virginia Tech

2.5 Ternary Chord Keyboard Experiments.

As stated earlier, the ternary chord keyboard differs from all chord

keyboards discussed so far in that its keys have three states instead of just two

(Figure 1). Therefore, previous studies on the ternary chord keyboard (TCK)

provided much of the foundation for this study .

2.5.1 Fathall - TCK #1

A study to compare performance on the TCK versus the QWERTY

keyboard was conducted by Fathallah (1988). Figure 10 shows the Ternary

Chord Keyboard Model #1. Four subjects were trained to use the TCK, and

another four learned the QWERTY keyboard. Both groups of subjects were

taught a total of 17 characters composed of 15 letters, space, and help. All

subjects were taught the characters in the same manner. For the TCK group,

chords were generated by using only two fingers, one finger from each hand.

Practice sessions (and al! other sessions) lasted about 1 1/2 hours broken into

15 minute sessions of typing followed by 5 minute breaks. Five characters were

learned per session and then reviewed on the fourth and final practice session.

Then, in what was called training sessions, the subjects were given text files to

type which were made up of words which used only the 15 letters. The files

were approximately 880 characters long. Subjects were trained until they met

the following criteria: 97% accuracy per character, and a mean response time of

800 msec per character. The two keyboards were evaluated in terms of number

of 15-minute sessions needed to meet the criteria: the average number of

sessions were 10 for the QWERTY group and 12 for the TCK group (these were

not significantly different).

33

Page 47: AAlr - Virginia Tech

"(afeos 0}

Jou ‘WO

U! SUO!SUBUUIP)

L# [EPO

PAeOghey ProYyD

AWeUsa! ‘O}

euNbI4

A

< 0'Sz

. Ws

G6

34

Page 48: AAlr - Virginia Tech

After training was completed, subjects participated in what was called

test sessions. Four 15-minute test sessions were given per day for five days.

On the fifth day average speed was 545 msec/character (110 CPM) for the

QWERTY group and 571 msec/character (105 CPM) for the TCK group; the

difference was not significant. In summary, subjects attained an approximate

typing speed of 107 CPM with a character set size of 16 after an average of 20

total hours of practice on either keyboard.

2.5.2 Kroemer (1990) - TCK #2.

Kroemer (1990) reported on the first training of subjects on TCK #2

(called #2 because it was modified from the one used by Fathallah) with a full

character set. Figure 11 shows the TCK #2. A total of 64 characters consisting

of 26 letters, 10 numbers, 4 cursor functions, and 24 punctuation marks,

symbols, and editing features were taught. Twelve subjects were trained in two

one-hour sessions per day. Characters were presented in "blocks" of six to ten.

Subjects were taught one block of characters and tested. Then another block of

characters was added until all 64 characters were learned. Subjects were

considered to have learned the keyboard once they could pass a test including

all the characters (presented four times each in random order) with 97%

accuracy two times consecutively. On the average, subjects learned all

characters in approximately four hours.

The next stage in the experiment was aimed at increasing the subjects’

speed. They were first given 20 text files that were 406 characters long, each

called a "trial". These were called "equal distribution trials" because each of 58

characters (the 4 cursor functions, shift, and backspace were not incorporated in

35

Page 49: AAlr - Virginia Tech

Figure 11. Ternary Chord Keyboard Model #2.

36

Page 50: AAlr - Virginia Tech

these trials) occurred 7 times, though randomly presented. By the twentieth

trial, subjects were averaging 54 CPM. Next, 20 text trials, 390 to 411

characters long, were typed by the subjects; the text, consisted of meaningful

sentences which attempted to capture the majority of the characters. At the end

of the fortieth trial, average speed was 78 CPM. The performance progress

over all 40 trials is shown in Figure 12.

In this experiment, subjects completed four trials in each one-hour

session. Since some subjects were faster than others, not all needed a full hour

to type the trials. Therefore, it is difficult to relate performance to hours of

practice. However, the total time spent using the keyboard averaged

approximately 12 hours.

2.5.3 Kroemer (1990) - TCK #3.

Kroemer (1990) reported on experiments with a modified keyboard, TCK

#3, as shown in Figure 13. The training procedure for TCK #3 was essentially

the same as TCK #2, except each subject was trained in one two-hour session

per day. Ten subjects learned all 64 characters in an average of about three

hours.

After training, subjects worked on improving speed, continuing the same

schedule as in the learning phase, i.e. one session per day lasting two hours.

The subject typed 40 text "trials" of meaningful sentences; each trial was 391 to

513 characters long. (No “equal distribution" trials were presented.) On the

average, subjects began trial number one keying 36 CPM; by trial 40 they

reached an average of 70 CPM. The performance progress for the 40 trials is

shown in Figure 14. Total time spent on the keyboard can not be directly

37

Page 51: AAlr - Virginia Tech

‘ZH pueoghsy

psouy Aveusa]

uy} JO}

BAUND Burwee

“z} aunbig

JEquNN jeu

Ov 8&

9E VE

cE OF

BC 92

He Ceo

OS BSL

OL FL

Gh OL

8 9

F @

as | -

——_o—__

SAV ——™——_

as |

+ —_-)——

Ol

02

0€

Or

0S

09

OZ

08

06

001

alnul—; sed syajoeseyuy

38

Page 52: AAlr - Virginia Tech

‘C# lepO-

Pueoghey

Puoyo Aueuwsal

“E4 eunbi4

39

Page 53: AAlr - Virginia Tech

"e# preoghey

puoyyD Ayeusay

ay} 10}

BAIND Buiwsee7

“p} eunBi4

JOQUINN Jeu

Op 8&

9E VE

ce OF

B82 92

He ce

OC BL

OE FPL

2h OL

8 9

FY @

O

foo }

| {

{ {

{|

0 FE

] 1

I tT]

I J

Tt |

] Pt

as | - —

—o——

SAV ——-—

+

0S

as |

+ —_-)—_

— OO0L

40

elnull sed suajoeieyuD

Page 54: AAlr - Virginia Tech

expressed by the number of trials since time to type each trial was not constant.

However, the total hours spent practicing on the keyboard was approximately

10 hours.

Two subjects continued practicing past the first 40 trials; each subject

typed an additional 120 trials, for a total of 15 to 17 hours. Their highest inputs

per trial were 134 and 183 CPM, respectively. Yet, until the end both subjects’

performance still increased in a seemingly linear fashion.

2.5.4 McMulkin and Kroemer (1991) and L nd Kroemer (1

A pilot study was conducted on a one-hand TCK by McMulkin and

Kroemer (1991) and Lee and Kroemer (1991). The apparatus used for the

experiment was the right-hand side of the keyboard used by Kroemer (1990),

TCK#2 shown in Figure 11. A total of 24 two-finger chords can be produced on

the one-hand TCK. The method in which chords were generated on the one-

hand TCK is different from the method used with the two-hand TCK. For the

two-hand TCK, chords were always produced by using one finger from each

hand; on the one-hand TCK, chords were produced by using two fingers from

the right hand.

Twelve subjects were taught 24 letters of the alphabet (V and Q not

included). Chords were grouped into blocks of eight letters. In teaching a block

of eight finger patterns, care was taken to make sure each chord was practiced

an equal number of times, which is different from the training program used in

the earlier experiments (Kroemer, 1990). The subjects learned all 24 chords in

an average of one hour.

41

Page 55: AAlr - Virginia Tech

The subjects returned on four consecutive days for one-hour testing

sessions. In each session, they completed five "trials". Each trial consisted of

the 24 letters randomly presented 10 times for a total of 240 characters. The

average typing speed began at 33 CPM and reached 63 CPM by the twentieth

trial. The performance progress is shown in Figure 15. The main purpose was

to collect response times for all 24 chords, so that the 18 "fastest" could be

selected.

2.6 Length of Training Session.

As alluded to earlier, the schedule of training can have a profound effect

on progress of speed acquisition. Glencross and Bluhm (1986) trained 242

persons on the QWERTY keyboard. Two hundred thirty-three people were

trained using ten 45-minute sessions spread over two weeks, one lesson per

weekday. Nine subjects received the same lessons, except that two sessions

were given per day for five days. At the end of the training, the two-week group

had an average keying speed of about 14 WPM as compared to the one-week

group's speed of about 11.2 WPM, a significant difference (p=0.012).

Baddeley and Longman (1978) conducted the most comprehensive

experiment on the effect of training session length on acquisition of typing

speed. Four levels of training schedule were established: one one-hour

session per day (1 X 1h); two one-hour sessions per day (2 X 1h); one two-hour

session per day (1 X 2h); and two two-hour sessions per day (2 X 2h). Multiple

sessions were separated by at least two hours of break. All groups were trained

for at least 60 hours; this means the 1 X 1h group had a total training period of

12 weeks, the 1 X 2h and 2 X 1h groups trained for 6 weeks, and the 2 X 2h

42

Page 56: AAlr - Virginia Tech

‘pueoghey puoyy

Areusla] PUBH-8UO

Oy} JO}

BAIND Buiusee]

“S} eunbi4

JOQUINN [UL

Oc 6|

St ZL

OF StL

VI EF

cl LL

OO 6

8

Jo {of

| |

| |

{of

| —|

FY

gs |

- ——o——_

SAV ——a-—_

as |

+ ——_)»—_

Ol

0c

0€

Ov

OS

09

OZ

08

06

anu Jed suajpoeieyD

43

Page 57: AAlr - Virginia Tech

group trained for 3 weeks. The training time until the groups became

operational was listed as follows: 1 X 1h - 35 hours, 2 X 1h - 42.6 hours, 1 X 2h-

43.2 hours, and 2 X 2h - 50 hours. This means the latter groups were operating

less efficiently per hour, but completed training much faster in practical terms of

weeks.

After becoming operational, the remaining portion of the 60 hours was

spent increasing speed. Typing tests were given once an hour. At the end of

60 hours each of the groups obtained the following speeds: 1 X 1h - 79 CPM, 2

X 1h- 73 CPM, 1 X 2h- 71 CPM, and 2 X 2h-65 CPM. The speed of the 1 X th

group was significantly faster than the 1 X 2h and 2 X 2h groups. The 2 X th

group was significantly faster than the 2 X 2h group. The performance progress

by hour is shown in Figure 16. An important finding was that at every hour of

practice, the rank order of groups from fastest to slowest was 1 X 1h, 2 X 1h, 1 X

2h, then 2 X 2h.

Baddeley and Longman's (1978) results indicated where the optimum

efficiency for training period length lies. Although less efficient, longer sessions

decreased the total number of weeks needed to perform an experiment. To

further illustrate this point, the 2 X 1h and 1 X 2h groups reached 80 CPM (the

60 hour level of the 1 X 1h group) in approximately 75 hours or 7.5 weeks. At

that time, the 1 X 1 group had barely become operational.

2.7 Format of Text for Testing.

The format of the text used to test keying performance can affect the

measures of speed. The main variable in text is whether it is nonsensical, such

as in a random presentation of letters, or whether it makes sense such as by

44

Page 58: AAlr - Virginia Tech

z Ss %F n w x

2 80F

g - 707 UO

Ecol YU q

50 ‘ . . . 40 50 60 70 BO

HOURS OF PRACTICE

Figure 16. Effect of Training Schedule on Typing Performance. Taken from Baddeley and Longman (1978).

45

Page 59: AAlr - Virginia Tech

having random words, phrases, or sentences. Gopher and Eilam (1979),

Sidorsky (1974), Alden, Daniels, and Kanarick (1972), and Seibel (1972) all

indicated that faster performance will be obtained if the text material makes

some sense. However, in this study, data were entered in the form of numbers

and mathematical operators; there seems to be little redundancy or predictable

order in numerical data entry (Seibel, 1972). Because of the lack of

redundancy in numerical data, this present study probably did not find keying

speeds comparable to a study which might have used sentences.

2.8 Sequences of Finger Movements that Yield Fast Typing Rates.

The time between keystrokes on the QWERTY keyboard has been

measured by several investigators (Salthouse, 1986; Salthouse, 1984; Gentner,

1983; Evey, 1980; and Kinkead, 1975). There has been general agreement

that the categories of consecutive keystrokes that rank from fastest to slowest

are as follows: 1) successive strokes from different hands, e.g. "th" , 2)

successive strokes from different fingers of the same hand, e.g. “er" , 3) same

key struck twice by the same finger, e.g. "ss" , and 4) different keys struck by the

same finger, e.g. "ed". Theses rules hold for skilled typists, not beginners.

Again, these rules do not seem to be helpful for numerical entry. Since

numerical entry has no order or redundancy, it is difficult to develop design

guidelines for assigning the numbers to particular chords based on the four

rules above. In addition, since the experiment conducted was constrained to

one-hand use of a keyboard, the fastest variety of successive keystrokes

(between hands) could not be implemented.

46

Page 60: AAlr - Virginia Tech

2.9 Which Hand to Use on a One-Hand Keyboard.

There is some disagreement on the issue of which hand should be used

in a one-hand keyboard task. Morgan, Drake, Heck, and Long (1981) trained

two groups of people on a 10-key calculator, a left-handed group and a right-

handed group. They found the left hand group to be significantly faster.

However, Dvorak (1943) and Dvorak, Merrick, Dealey, and Ford (1936) argue

that the right hand should be used more frequently in keyboard tasks. Since

there is no clear evidence of an advantage for either hand, the right hand was

assumed to be appropriate for the completed experiment. Also, using two

fingers of the same hand to generate a chord requires greater dexterity than a

Strict tapping task. The preferred hand probably possesses greater dexterity.

2.10 Literature Summary.

The ability of people to learn keyboard operation depends (among other

variables) on the number of characters taught, type of keyboard used

(sequential or chord), and criterion employed to establish proficiency. Total

amount of training time, number of trials or sessions, and total inputs have been

used to predict and analyze performance progress. It does not appear that any

of these criteria have been investigated to determine the most effective one.

Table 1 provides a summary of the findings, as they pertain to the objectives of

the present study.

The first experimental objective is to investigate the performance

progress of subjects on a keyboard. Few studies have evaluated keyboard

performance from initial learning to peak performance; thus, performance

progress on keyboards is not well Known. The training time to maximal

47

Page 61: AAlr - Virginia Tech

Table 1. Summary of Important Variables in Keyboarding Studies

Total Provide

Type of | Character| Training Total Char- CPM Learning

Author | Keyboard| Set Size | Schedule Hours acters at end Curve?

Chapman QWERTY 65 n/a 180 n/a 240 yes (1919)

Thurstone QWERTY 65 2 hrs/day 280 250 pages 205 yes

(1919) 5 days/wk

Green QWERTY 65 1 hr/day 360 n/a 235 yes (1940) 5 days/wk

Klemmer Chord 30 1 hr/day 35 15,000 225 yes

(1958) 3 days/wk words

Lockhead Chord 136 1 hr/day 45 na 40 no

& Klemmer 50 days

(1959)

Cornog Chord 36 1.5 hr/day 190 n/a 232 no

et al. (1963) 11 weeks

Conrad & QWERTY 36 3.5 hrs/day 120 n/a 92 yes Longman Chord 36 5 days/wk 120 n/a 100 yes

(1965)

Bowen& QWERTY 11 n/a 20 n/a 80 no

Guinness Chord 11 n/a 20 n/a 50 no (1965)

Sidorsky Chord 40 0.5 hr/day 5 n/a 63 no

(1974) 10 days

Rochester Chord 54 50 min/day 90 n/a 200 yes

et al. (1978) 5 days/wk

Gopher & Chord 30 7, 1hr sess 7 n/a 65 yes

Ejlam Chord 40 6, 1hr sess 6 n/a 56 yes

(1979)

Gopher et Chord 23 1 hr/day 35 na 208 (2-hand) yes

al. 85, 86, Chord 23 3-4 day/wk 35 n/a 178 (1-hand) yes

and 88 QWERTY 23 35 sess 35 n/a 118 yes

Richardson Chord 10 3 hr/day 97 n/a 140 no

et al. 30 days (1987)

Fathallah Chord 17 1.5 hrs/day 20 n/a 105 yes

(1988) QWERTY 17 12 days 20 na 110 yes

TCK #2 Chord 64 2, 1hr sess 12 16,000 78 yes (1990) per day

TCK #3 Chord 64 2 hrs/day 10 18,000 70 yes

(1990)

One-Hand Chord 24 45 min/day 3 2,400 63

TCK (1991) 5 days

48

Page 62: AAlr - Virginia Tech

performance seems to be largely dependent on character set size, see Table 1.

Since only 18 chords are used for this study, the training time is expected to be

considerably shorter then for a set size of 64. The training schedule has a large

effect on the learning curve; this study uses a schedule that is as efficient as

possible to obtain results rapidly.

The second experimental objective is to find a function which predicts

learning and evaluate three different criteria to assess which criterion is most

effective at predicting performance progress. Only three of the reviewed studies

attempted to find a function which predicted learning, Thurstone (1919),

Kroemer (1990), and McMulkin and Kroemer (1991). The conclusions that can

be drawn about each criterion from the literature may be summarized as

follows:

1) Number of hours of practice

Fourteen of the seventeen studies summarized in Table 1 used practice

time to describe performance progress. In none of these studies was it

investigated whether this was the best predictor. The assumption that hours of

practice was indeed the best criterion should be tested against other predictors,

which is one goal of this study.

The amount of practice to reach maximal performance with an 18-chord

set was estimated from the studies at 50 hours. This was based on the

assumption that the subjects would reach a peak speed of close to 200 CPM

(this is an interpolation of the results listed in Table 1). With character set sizes

of 50 or more, 200 CPM was reached after about 200 hours (Chapman, 1919;

Thurstone, 1919; and Green, 1940). Therefore, with a character set size of only

49

Page 63: AAlr - Virginia Tech

18 the time was expected to be about a fourth as long. However, due to the

differences in training schedules and method of testing, this remains a guess.

2) Number of trials or characters

Only five of the studies reviewed in Table 1 recorded the number of

characters practiced as a function of performance. In none of these studies

were subjects able to attain peak performance, so an estimate of characters

practiced until peak performance was almost impossible. There was no

discussion that number-of-characters-typed was the best predictor in any of the

five studies. Only Thurstone (1919) provided learning curves for both hours of

practice and pages typed. However, he did no analysis to show which criterion

provided a better prediction.

3) Subjects’ Opinion

None of the studies considered the opinion of the subject as a method to

assess how close or far the subject was from maximal performance.

50

Page 64: AAlr - Virginia Tech

3. METHOD

3.1 Subjects.

Five subjects participated in this experiment. The only criteria used to

select subjects was that they write with their right hand and have no history of

wrist problems (such as carpal tunnel syndrome). Four of the subjects were

female, and one was male. The subjects’ ages ranged from 20 to 29 years.

The total time commitment of each subject was 60 hours. Subjects were

compensated at a rate of $5.00 per hour, plus a $50 bonus for participating for

all 60 hours, for a total of $350. None of the subjects dropped out of the

experiment.

3.2 Apparatus.

The one-hand Ternary Chord Keyboard, shown previously in Figure 11,

was used for this study. Activation of a key was detected by microswitches. The

microswitches were connected to an Input/Output carrier on an IBM (AT)

computer via a bus cable. A software program (written by VATELL Corporation)

interpreted and manipulated inputs from the TCK.

For each trial the following data were recorded: subject number, trial

number, total time to complete the trial, and the number of errors committed by

the subject. Also, the following data were collected on every chord within each

trial: subject number, trial number, chord number, and response time for the

chord. If the chord was not keyed correctly, the incorrectly entered (actual)

chord was recorded.

51

Page 65: AAlr - Virginia Tech

3.2.1 Dimensions of the One-Hand TCK.

The physical dimensions and positions of the keys for the one-hand TCK

are shown in Figure 17. A palm rest was provided for the subjects to use if they

wished. The wooden palm rest was shaped approximately like a half cone,

11.5 cm long, sloped from a radius of 2.7 cm at the thumb side of the palm toa

radius of 1.6 cm at the little finger side of the palm. The palm rest was

adjustable longitudinally (4.5 to 11 cm from the center of the keys to the center

of the palm rest) and laterally (1 cm in either direction from the center). It was

fastened to the base of the keyboard by velcro strips. The entire keyboard base

could be rotated in roll, pitch or yaw. The keyboard (rotated as preferred by the

subject, no specific pattern) and chair height were adjusted so that the subject

felt comfortable, the wrist was straight, and the elbow formed about a 90 degree

angle.

3.2.2 Chord Coding.

The method to assign the numbers and functions to the chords was

determined largely from the experiments conducted by McMulkin and Kroemer

(1991) and Lee and Kroemer (1991). Of the 24 possible chords, 6 were

eliminated, based on user preference and response times. The remaining 18

chords were assigned by VATELL Corporation to the numbers and functions.

On an adding machine, it was assumed the ten numbers, “decimal point", and

"plus", "minus", "multiply", "divide", "subtotal", and "total" functions are all used

with about equal frequency. No literature could be found which ranked the

frequency of these 18 numbers and functions. Therefore, some of the

assignments were chosen based on ease of learning. All the "odd" numbers

52

Page 66: AAlr - Virginia Tech

“othe

2)

D a

® a

>

OE

O'@ oo

$s

Page 67: AAlr - Virginia Tech

were assigned to chords in which both the fingers moved backward. All the

“even” numbers were assigned to chords in which the fingers moved forward.

The "backspace" chord was assigned to the chord in which the index finger

moves backward and middle finger forward; this is like turning a wheel where

the top of the wheel moves to the left. No learning principle existed for the

remaining seven functions, so they were matched with the remaining seven

chords. Because each of the chords was to appear equally often in the text

trials, the chord assignments should not have had an effect on performance.

Figure 18 shows how the chords were assigned to the characters. The letter S

was used to represent "Subtotal", the letter T represented "Total", and an arrow,

© , represented "Backspace".

3.3 Procedures.

3.3.1 Experimental Design.

One of the objectives of the experiment was to determine a function

which effectively predicts learning and evaluate a set of criteria for predicting

performance progress and peak keying speed. Therefore, the regressor

variables used to predict performance were number of hours spent practicing,

and number of repetitions (trials). The dependent measures recorded were

time to complete a trial/test, number of errors during a trial, and opinion of the

subjects about their performance at the end of each week. From the first two

dependent measures, characters per minute were computed for each trial and

for each time increment of practice.

54

Page 68: AAlr - Virginia Tech

Finger: Index

Middle

Ring

Little

(The chord numbers describe which fingers were used to generate a chord.

The first number of the chord represents the first finger's direction while the

second number represents the second finger and its movement direction.

For example, the chord number 36 represents the middle finger moving

backward and the ring finger moving forward.)

Character

i,

+o OOn oar

|] DY

*

/

. (Decimal Point)

S (Subtotal)

T (Total)

€ (Backspace)

Chord

15

26

17

28

35

46

37

48

57

68

24

23

45

36

13

25

16

14

Figure 18. Chord Assignments for the Numeric One-Hand Ternary

Chord Keyboard.

55

Page 69: AAlr - Virginia Tech

3.3.2 Schedule of Practice.

Subjects were scheduled for one one-and-a-half hour session per day,

five days a week. Subjects participated for eight weeks for a total of 60 hours.

(As pointed out in the literature review this may not have led to optimum

efficiency for each hour of practice, but the study was to be completed in

approximately eight weeks.) Only one keyboard was available for

experimentation; this meant one-and-a-half hour sessions required a chunk of

7.5 hours per day be allotted to testing (this was close to the upper limit of the

time available by the experimenter). Subjects were scheduled for the same

times each day. Having the sessions the same time each day was the most

convenient for the subjects and greatly reduced the probability of them dropping

out. Daily sessions also allowed the subjects to participate in the experiment

before, after, or between classes, and avoided conflicts with other activities.

Overall, 1.5-hour sessions offered the best compromise between utility of each

hour of practice and convenience for the subjects.

Due to a variety of reasons, each subject missed some of the sessions.

Any session that was missed was rescheduled for another day either at the end

of the eight weeks or on a weekend. Subjects never participated in two

sessions in one day.

3.3.2.1 Breakdown of Sessions.

The first session was dedicated to teaching the subjects the 18

characters (training program described in detail in the next section). The

second through the fortieth sessions had the same format. The subjects were

given text consisting of 504 characters to input. Typing each such text was

96

Page 70: AAlr - Virginia Tech

called a “trial” (the terms text and trial are used interchangeable from this point

on). During each session the subject was presented a trial to type. The subject

was instructed to input the text as rapidly as possible maintaining 97%

accuracy. The subjects took approximately 15 minutes to type the first trial.

After the end of the trial, subjects were given a break of two to three minutes and

then started another trial. As the subjects typed the trials faster, they were given

shorter breaks, of about one minute to maintain a fairly constant ratio between

practice and rest. The experimenter determined how long the rest breaks would

be so that the ratio of practice to rest was approximately constant (about two to

three minutes practice to every one minute of rest). The session continued in

this way until the end, after one-and-a-half hours.

3.3 2.2 Training Program.

During the first session the subjects were taught the 18 chords. The

characters were taught in two “blocks”: first, the numbers 1 to 9, and 0; second,

the functions "plus", "minus", "multiply", "divide", "decimal point", "backspace",

"subtotal", and "total". Within each block, the first character was presented

along with the finger pattern corresponding to that character. The subject then

practiced the character five times. The second through fifth characters were

presented in the same manner. A "mini-quiz" was then given over the first five

characters in the number block, each character presented in random order four

times. During the "mini-quiz", no finger patterns were displayed for the

characters unless the subject entered a chord incorrectly. The sixth through

tenth characters were then presented five times each. A "mini-quiz" was then

given over the final five characters in the number block. After the first learning

a7

Page 71: AAlr - Virginia Tech

block, a quiz was given over all the characters in that block; each was

presented in random order a total of three times. The subject had to pass the

quiz two consecutive times with 97% accuracy; there was no time limit. An

accuracy of 97% was selected so that subjects could not incorrectly enter one

chord all three times it was presented and still pass. If the subject did not pass

the quizzes, the learning block was repeated and the quizzes repeated at the

end of that learning block.

After passing the quizzes, the next block of characters was presented in

the same manner. Quizzes were given over the second block to ensure the

subject had learned the eight symbols. Upon completion of the second learning

block quizzes, a cumulative quiz over all characters was given. Each character

was presented randomly three times. The subject had to pass the quiz two

consecutive times with 97% accuracy (again subjects could not miss 1 of the 18

chords all three times and still pass). If the quiz was failed, the subject had the

option of reviewing a block or retaking the final quiz. After passing two

cumulative quizzes, the subject was considered to have learned the 18

characters.

The number of keystrokes performed during training as well as the time

to complete the training session was recorded for all subjects.

It might be argued that this training program had not been experimentally

verified as being the most effective method of teaching the chords. This is true,

but the same training program has been used successfully in the experiments

conducted by Kroemer (1990), and McMulkin and Kroemer (1991). There a

total of 64 chords were taught in three to four hours, and 24 chords were taught

08

Page 72: AAlr - Virginia Tech

in an hour. These results indicated that the training program brought about a

short training time.

3.3.2.3 Mak { Trial

Each trial contained 504 characters; therefore, each of the characters

was presented 28 times. The trials were generated using a PASCAL program

which randomly selected the order of the characters. Each trial presented to the

subjects was a different trial created by the program. However, all the subjects

were presented with the same trials in the same order. As subjects’ keying

speed became faster it took the subjects less time to complete the trials. Due to

the variable keying speed of the subjects, some typed trials that were never

presented to others. Appendix A shows an example of a trial.

3.3.3 Determining Peak Performance.

A problem in analyzing the experimental results is to establish a subject’s

peak performance, i.e. the fastest keying speed possible. Bittner (personal

communication, April 11, 1991) suggested having the subjects perform a series

of characters over and over; he assumed that this allows them to develop a fast

motor skill pattern, thus give a measure of peak performance.

Following Bittner’s suggestions, at the end of the final session, subjects

continuously typed a single series of characters. Five different character strings

were used: 6.7-3S, 4/0<2T, 584+1*9, 142-3. , and 123456. Each trial was 504

characters long, consisting of 84 repetitions of one of these five character

strings.

99

Page 73: AAlr - Virginia Tech

3.3.4 Subjects' Opinion.

Every subject knew that an objective of the study was to reach maximal

input speed. It was hypothesized that the subjects’ opinion of when they were

typing as fast as they could may be a good measure of when peak performance

was obtained. Once a week the subjects were asked to rate their performance

on a0 to 9 scale, 9 representing the anchor for peak performance; Figure 19

shows the scale that was used.

3.4 Experimental Protocol.

The experiment was conducted in 40 one-and-a-half hour sessions, 5

days a week, for 8 weeks. In the first session, the subject was given the written

experimental instructions to read (Appendix B). The experimenter then

reviewed the instructions and answered any questions. The subject was given

the informed consent form to sign (Appendix C). The subject was then seated in

front of the keyboard; the seat height, the keyboard, and palm rest were

adjusted until the subject was comfortable. The subject was not required to use

the palm rest. Two subjects used the palm rest throughout the entire

experiment. Two subjects never used the palm rest, and one subject started

using it about halfway through the experiment. The subject was presented

introductory information on the one-hand TCK and started with the training

program. After learning the 18 chords, the subject was dismissed for the day.

At the start of the second session the subject was again required to take

a cumulative quiz over all 18 characters until it was passed. The subject was

then presented with the first trial to type. Additional trials were presented until

the end of the session. As the session approached the end of one-and-a-half

60

Page 74: AAlr - Virginia Tech

1 __ Beginning Keying Speed

2 __ Very Slow Keying Speed

3 __ Slow Keying Speed

4 _ Below Moderate Keying Speed

5 _ Moderate Keying Speed

6 __ Above Moderate Keying Speed

7 _ Fast Keying Speed

8 __ Very Fast Keying Speed

9 _ Fastest Possible Keying Speed

Figure 19. Scale Used by Subjects to Rate Their Performance Each Week.

61

Page 75: AAlr - Virginia Tech

hours, the experimenter decided if enough time remained for an additional trial.

The third through fifth sessions paralleled the second; the subject was required

to pass a quiz and then practiced keying text trials. After the fifth session (after

about 7.5 hours of practice), the subject was no longer required to take the

cumulative quiz. During the sixth through the fortieth sessions, the subjects

practiced keying text. At the end of the fortieth session, the subject was paid,

thanked, and dismissed.

For all trials, the subject was instructed to increase input speed while

maintaining at least a 97% accuracy level; 97% was selected to match the

accuracy stressed in the training program. To help control the speed versus

accuracy tradeoff, feedback was given to the subjects at the end of every trial.

As stated before, for each trial the following data were recorded: subject

number, trial number, total time to complete the trial, and the number of errors

committed by the subject. Immediately after the completion of each trial, the

characters per minute and accuracy were calculated from these data and

displayed on the screen along with total time to complete each trial and number

of errors. The subjects received immediate feedback on their speed and

accuracy. They could then make adjustments in their performance to increase

input speed, but maintain their accuracy levels. No specific rewards were given

to the subjects to maintain 97% accuracy or increase their speed; presenting

feedback of their performance to them was considered an effective way to

control the variability due to speed versus accuracy.

62

Page 76: AAlr - Virginia Tech

4. RESULTS

4.1 Learning Results.

All five subjects passed the final training test within the first one-and-a-

half hour session; the average learning time was 35 minutes (including quizzes)

with a standard deviation of 8.3 minutes to achieve the required minimum of

97% accuracy on all 18 chords. The number of times a subject had to go

through each training block ranged from two to four times and the number of

quizzes per block taken to pass ranged from two to six.

4.2 Speed - Accuracy Performance.

The subjects were instructed to increase speed while maintaining a 97%

accuracy level. Table 2 shows, for each subject, the average accuracy and

standard deviation of the trials typed over the entire experiment. All the subjects

had an average accuracy greater than 97%.

4.3 Using Number of Trials to Predict Performance.

The five subjects each completed different numbers of trials over the 60-

hour experimental! period, see Table 3. Analyses were done using individual

subjects’ data, except for chord and average performance analyses. This

means that regressions for individual subjects include different numbers of data

points, e.g. 487 points for subject 1 and 636 points for subject 3.

One of the objectives of this experiment was to find an equation that

accurately describes the performance progress of each subject. The dependent

variable being predicted is characters per minute. The keying speed of the

63

Page 77: AAlr - Virginia Tech

Table 2. Average Accuracy Across Trials for Each Subject.

Average Standard Subject Accuracy Deviation

(in %) (in %)

1 97.39 0.76

2 98.51 0.61

3 98.20 0.80

4 97.75 0.85

5 97.19 0.94

64

Page 78: AAlr - Virginia Tech

Table 3. Number of Trials Completed by Each Subject

Number of Trials Completed

Subject 1 487

Subject 2 622

Subject 3 636

Subject 4 509

Subject 5 516

65

Page 79: AAlr - Virginia Tech

subjects in characters per minute was calculated by dividing the correct entries

(total number of characters minus number of errors) typed during a trial by the

total time needed to complete the trial.

The major criterion used to determine which equations should be used

was that they predict performance in a monotonically increasing fashion.

Equations which would predict increasing and then decreasing performance

were not considered, such as polynomial equations (e.g. By + Bi*X + Bo*X2).

Five candidate functions were considered to fit the data. First, a simple

linear relationship between characters per minute (CPM) and trials was used:

CPM; = Bo + By*T; (Equation 1)

where CPM; is the characters per minute typed on the i-th trial

(dependent variable), Tj is the i-th trial, and Bp and B; are fitted

coefficients.

Second, a relationship suggested by Bittner (1991) was used:

CPM; = Bo + By/T; + Bo/Ti2 (Equation 2)

The first two equations were combined to generate the third function:

CPM; = Bo + By*T; + Bo/T; + Ba/Ti2 (Equation 3)

Fourth, a logarithmic function was tried of the form:

CPN; = Bo + By*Ln(T}) (Equation 4)

The final attempt to fit the data was by a Log-Log function:

Ln(CPM;) = Bo+ By*Ln(T;), this reduces to

CPM; = C*T;81 (Equation 5)

66

Page 80: AAlr - Virginia Tech

where C = eBo: see Appendix D for the derivation.

The subjects’ performance was analyzed individually, not collectively.

Each of the five functions above was fit to each of the subject’s actual data. The

statistical package SYSTAT was used to calculate the coefficients using the

method of least squares. The criterion used to judge goodness of fit of the

functions is the coefficient of determination, R2, also calculated by SYSTAT.

Table 4 summarizes the R2 values obtained for each formula for each subject.

The regressions of the data of subject 2 are selected as a typical

example of each of the five function’s predictions plotted with the actual

performance in Figures 20 to 23 and 25. Comparing the Figures 20 to 23 and

25, it becomes apparent that the Log-Log function most accurately follows the

actual performance progress. This impression is supported by the statistical

findings that the Log-Log function accounts for the largest portion of variation,

i.e. has the highest R2.

The plot of actual performance and predicted performance from the Log-

Log equation (in Characters Per Minute typed) for each subject is shown in

Figures 24 to 28. The Log-Log equation which best fit actual performance of

each subject is listed in Table 5. Figure 29 shows the predictions for all

individual subjects.

The Log-Log equation which was used to fit the actual performance had

the form: Ln(CPMj) = Bo + By*Ln(Tj). The coefficients Bp and B; which have a

different value for each subject are listed in Table 6. A t-test can be used to

indicate if any of the Bo or By; coefficients for each subject are statistically

different from one another (Neter and Wasserman, 1974). All Bo coefficients

67

Page 81: AAlr - Virginia Tech

Table 4. Summary of R-Squared Values for All Functions

Considered When Trial is the Regressor Variable.

Equation

(487 to 636 observations for each fit)

CPMi = Bo+|CPMi = Bo+| CPMi=Bo+ |CPMi =Bo+| CPMi =

B1*Ti Bi*(1/Ti) + | B1*Ti+B2*(1/Ti)} Bi*Ln(Ti) | C*(Ti*B1)

B2*(1/Ti*2) | +B3*(1/Ti*2) {log-log}

Subject 1 0.899 0.361 0.927 0.896 0.961

Subject 2 0.825 0.430 0.903 0.921 0.953

Subject 3 0.817 0.427 0.897 0.924 0.955

Subject 4 0.819 0.498 0.920 0.953 0.973

Subject 5 0.802 0.440 0.879 0.898 0.939

Average 0.832 0.431 0.905 0.918 0.956

CPMi = Characters per Minute on the i-th trial

Bo, B1, B2, and B3 are fitted coefficients

Ti = Trial

C = Exp(B)

68

Page 82: AAlr - Virginia Tech

OS9 009

“QOURWUOLO ENO

pue feUL,+g

+ Og = INO

JO UOHIpald ‘Z alang

“og eunbiy

JaquNny jeuL

OSS 00S

OSb O00F

OSE O0€

OS¢ 002

i

W

| |

J. {

| |

| a

{ i

I I

l |

ai

it

\

HNN

if nae

i ‘i

|

ID a a

air Ny

[PHL.SELO +

SV O0L = Wdd

OSt OOF

0S

| L

| |

I t

+H fo

ay

Wl

Oc

Ov

09

08

OO!

Ocl

Ori

O9L

O8l

00¢

eynull sed suajpoeeyyD

69

Page 83: AAlr - Virginia Tech

“BOUBLUIOPSd jenjoy

pue Zyjeul/Zgt+

|EUL/LG +

OG =

INWdd JO

UOHdIPald ‘Z

y~alqns "1Z

eunbi4

JOquNnN jeu

0S9 009

OSS 00S

OSr O0v

OSE O0€

OS2 002

OSt OO!

0S

CvIEHI/8°9EL +

[EL L/9'B6Z - ES

6HL =IWdd

OV

08

Ocl

O91

00¢

anu sed suajoeseyD

70

Page 84: AAlr - Virginia Tech

‘OURO

emoy puke

Zyfeu L/eg + IEUL/ZA

+ IEUL.La + 0g

= INdd JO UONAIpeld

‘Z yalqns ‘zz

eunbi4 JOqUNN

jeul

0S9 O09

OSS 00S

OSb O0v

ose O00&

O0S¢ 00¢

£OS!I

| |

| |

| |

| |

| !

| I

| 1

I {

{ 1

I ]

| |

tea he

bi, A

Healt

\ \

| fl

i

hu af

\ Wl

Ae aN

A A

ny ry

ovIeH L/P ole

+ IEUL/C

C9E - IELL.VLL'O

+ 80°LOL

= Ndd

OOl OS

0c

OV

09

08

0O0-

Oct

Ort

O91

081

00¢

eInulpy Jed suapeseyy

71

Page 85: AAlr - Virginia Tech

0S9

“QOUBWUOHA jenjoy

pue (jeu

)UT.1@ +

OG = INdd

JO UOHIpPald

‘2 jalqns ‘Ez

eunbl4

JOQUNN

[BULL

009 OSS

00S OSr

OO0v OSE

O00€ ose

002 oS!

ool os

oO

! !

oS yy

| an

i My - }

mag I"

| yn

| art

M hhh

|

| Kal

Al i

fh

Vi ha

At hat h

y

A :

|

cH Te

(jeu )U7,91°9¢

+ 961°0

=INdOD

0d

OV

09

08

OOL

Oct

Ovl

O91

08}

00¢

eInuly Jed syaj~eieyD

72

Page 86: AAlr - Virginia Tech

", palqns

‘eoueWOLaY

jenjoy puke

uO!oIpal 4 607-607]

‘pz eunbi4

JOQUUNN Jeu

069 009

OSS 00S

OSr OOv

OSE O0€

OS¢ 000

OSl OO!

0S 0

[+ +}

| :

: I—

+ |

if yr

y

yr pe

| Pe

mT iV

T

r ot

NA} a

1 ML

deat Mr

1 di my

Rv aul

qi’ i

4 aL

(pSEOvieUL). LSB

= NdO

Oc

Ov

09

08

001

Ocl

Ort

091

08 |

00¢

e]null Jed srajoeueyy

73

Page 87: AAlr - Virginia Tech

0S9

‘2 yalqns ‘aoueBWOLaY

jenjoy pue

uoNoIpesg 607-607

“Sg aunbi4

JOQUINN Jeu,

009 OSS

006 OS

O0v OSE

oO0& OS¢

002 £OS/I

OOl 0S

0

+ +

/

) per?

|

wath ;

ul | fc

| {al

tk!

iy An

PG Ih

"a

NOH

Hl I

MW |

i ft

HR

i |

lh le ea

TTP (222 OvieUL).L2Z

kv = NdO

Oe

Ov

09

08

001

Oct

Ort

O9l

O8l

00¢

ENUF, Jed suajoeseyy

74

Page 88: AAlr - Virginia Tech

0S9 T

‘€ joalqns

‘eoueWOLad

jenjoy pue

uoloipesg 607-607

‘92g eunbi4

JEquINN |euL

009. OSS

00S OSh

O00b oOSt

O00€ OSe¢

000 OSI

O00! 0S

| {

| |

I |

I |

{

nah ft

Mi

!\ A

iin 4

iW IW

yen

Hd

nm Vi

|

(661 OvIGHL),

Ea 8h =

NWdO

r Oe

r OV

- 09

- 08

r OOL

r OSh

- Ol

r O9-

- O8t

L 00¢

elnull Jed suejpeseuyD

75

Page 89: AAlr - Virginia Tech

0S9 009

‘p yoalqns

‘eoueWOLad

jenjoy pue

uoloipald 607-607

“72 eunbi4

JOQUINN Jeu

OSS 00S

OSh O0v

OSE O0€&

O0S¢ 000

OS! OO!

0S

| |

| _t

| |

| |

| |

I I

I t

J t-

{ I

1 1

(Zya OvieUL),ZEL' CE = NdO

0d

OV

09

08

001

Ocl

a

O91

O8l

00¢

elnull—y Jed suajoeeyyD

76

Page 90: AAlr - Virginia Tech

0S9

‘g oalqns

‘eoueWWOLeY jenjoy

pue UO!OIpaig

607-607 “gz

aunbi

JEquUNN jeu

009 OSS

00S OSr

O0v OSE

O0€ OG2

002 OSt

OOl OS

0

t+— {

+ —t

4

J+ Vr

De +

Len] er

M up

\ hs Wh

i iy a7

| y en

hy |

(ZL 7 OvleuL),6Z6°9€ = WdO

0c

OV

09

08

OOl

Oc |

Sinull Jed suajoeeyD

OVl

091

081

002

v7

Page 91: AAlr - Virginia Tech

Table 5. The Fitted Log-Log Equation for Each Subject.

Subject Log-Log Equation Re

1 CPM; = 18.541*T;0.394 0.961

2 CPM; = 41.721*T,0-222 0.953

3 CPM; = 48.231*T,9-199 0.955

4 CPM; = 32.137°T;0-242 0.973

5 CPN; = 36.379°T;0-217 0.939

where CPM j is the characters per minute typed on the i-th trial

(dependent variable) and Tjis the i-th trial of practice

78

Page 92: AAlr - Virginia Tech

‘palqns yoeg

10} uonoipeig

607-607 “62

aunbi4

Jaquunu jeuy

0S9 009

0SS 00S

OSr O00v

OSE O0€

OSe 002

OS! 00}

0S

| t—

: |

. |

: [+

§ ang yang

Lang

LaviPHL.O = Nd

OV

09

08

OOl

Och

OVl

091

081

00¢

elnulp Jed syajoeieyy

79

Page 93: AAlr - Virginia Tech

Table 6. Statistical Comparison of Coefficients Bo and B1.

Comparison Between Bo Coefficients

Subject 1 2 3 4 Bo Value 2.9197 3.7310 3.8761 3.4698

1 2.9197 Difference Between Coefficients

2 3.7310 0.8113 *

3 3.8761 0.9564 * | 0.1451 *

4 3.4698 0.5501 * | 0.2612 * | 0.4063 *

5 3.5942 0.6745 * | 0.1368 * | 0.2819* | 0.1244 *

Comparison Between B1 Coefficients

Subject 1 2 3 4 B1 Value 0.3545 0.2218 0.1995 0.2422

1 0.3545 Difference Between Coefficients

2 0.2218 0.1327 *

3 0.1995 0.1550 * | 0.0223 *

4 0.2422 0.1123 * | 0.0204* | 0.0427 *

5 0.2166 0.1378* | 0.0051 0.0172 * | 0.0256 * |

* indicates a significant difference, at p < 0.001

80

Page 94: AAlr - Virginia Tech

were found to be significantly different from each other (p < 0.001), see Table 6.

The By, coefficients for subjects 2 and 5 were not found to be significantly

different each other (p = 0.10), but all other comparisons between B,

coefficients were found to be significantly different (p < 0.001), see Table 6.

4.4 Using Time to Predict Performance.

Another method of regressing performance is to predict the dependent

variable, characters per minute, from practice time. For example, performance

could be predicted from the equation CPM; = Bog + By*Log(Pj) where P;

represents the i-th time increment of practice, for example 30 minute increments

(Pj = 1, 2, 3, ..... ; not 30, 60, 90, .... ). During a given time increment of practice,

each subject typed a different number of trials.

All the data were collected in terms of time needed to complete a trial. To

create 30-minute time periods, the times to complete trials were added until the

total reached at least 30 minutes. Therefore, none of the periods were exactly

30 minutes, but none were less than 30 minutes. The time periods also

overlapped the sessions of practice. Session one might include the first 30-

minute period and half of the second 30-minute period; session two might cover

the other haif of the second period, all of the third, and then part of the fourth.

The overlap of the periods is not constant across subjects either. The

procedure to create periods was repeated for 40, 50, and 60-minute time

periods. The average length of the 30, 40, 50, and 60-minute time increments

for each subject is shown in Table 7, along with the standard deviation. The

characters per minute typed for a period was calculated by averaging the

characters per minute of the trials that constituted a given time period.

81

Page 95: AAlr - Virginia Tech

Table 7. Average Length (and Standard Deviation) of 30, 40, 50,

and 60 Minute Time Increments Used in the Regressions.

30 Minute Period Subject Average (in Min) | Std Dev (in Min) N

1 32.7 2.01 67 2 32.2 1.43 70

3 31.9 1.22 70 4 32.4 1.99 70

5 32.2 1.52 71

40 Minute Period Subject Average (in Min) | Std Dev (in Min) N

1 42.5 2.11 5 1

2 42.0 1.47 54

3 41.4 0.93 54

4 42.5 1.54 53

5 42.3 1.48 54

50 Minute Period Subject Average (in Min) | Std Dev (in Min) N

1 52.6 2.44 41

2 51.9 1.20 43

3 51.6 1.11 43

4 52.4 1.61 43

5 52.3 1.33 43

60 Minute Period Subject Average (in Min) | Std Dev (in Min) N

1 62.8 2.13 35

2 62.1 1.24 36

3 62.0 1.01 36

4 62.5 1.79 36

5 62.2 1.55 36

82

Page 96: AAlr - Virginia Tech

The average characters per minute typed was also calculated by

session. Although the sessions were one-and-a-half hours long, not all of that

time was spent typing because of rest breaks after each trial. Calculating

characters per minute by session does not indicate any specific time period, but

maintains the continuity of an entire day of practice.

The same five equations used to fit trials were tried with time periods:

CPM; = Bo + ByP; (Equation 1a)

CPM; = Bo + Bi/P; + Bo/P;2 (Equation 2a)

CPM; = Bo + ByP; + Bo/P; + Ba/Pi2 (Equation 3a)

CPMj = Bo + ByLn(P)) (Equation 4a)

CPM; = eBo p;B1 (Equation 5a)

where CPM; is the characters per minute typed on the i-th time

period (dependent variable), Pj is the i-th time period (30, 40,

50 or 60 minute periods or days) and B's are fitted coefficients.

Table 8 lists the R@ values found for all equations for each time period by

subject. The equations CPM; = Bo + ByP; + Bo/P; + B3/Pi2 and CPM; = eBo p;Bt

(Log-Log) provided the best fit for every time increment considered. Their R2

values are equal to at least 0.96 in every case. Appendix E contains the graphs

of actual performance and predictions using both CPM; = Bo + By;P; + Bo/P; +

Ba/Pj2 and CPM; = eo P;®1 for all time increments and subjects.

4.5 Subjective Ratings of Performance.

At the end of the every fifth session the subjects were asked to rate their

performance on a 0 to 9 scale (shown previously in Figure 19). The ratings for

83

Page 97: AAlr - Virginia Tech

Table 8. Summary of R-Squared Values for All Functions Considered

30 Minute Period

(approximately 70 observations

When Time Increment is the Regressor Variable. 30, 40, 50, and

60 Minute Increments and Sessions are Used.

CPMi = Bo + | CPMi= Bo+}| CPMi=Bo+ | CPMi=Bo+ CPMi =

B1*Pi B1/Pi + Bo*Pi+B1/Pi+ | B1*Ln(Pi) | e*Bo*(Pi*B1)

Ba/Pi*2 Ba/Pi*2 {log-log}

Subject 1 0.978 0.624 0.984 0.868 0.965

Subject 2 0.893 0.802 0.982 0.966 0.987

Subject 3 0.896 0.778 0.978 0.966 0.989

Subject 4 0.880 0.812 0.979 0.973 0.989

Subject 5 0.892 0.780 0.973 0.960 0.986

Average 0.908 0.759 0.979 0.947 0.983

40 Minute Period

(approximately 54 observations

CPMi = Bo +; CPMi=Bo+] CPMi=Bo+ | CPMi=Bo+ CPMi =

B1*Pi B1/Pi + Bo*Pi+B1/Pi+ | B1*Ln(Pi) | e*Bo*(Pi*B1)

Ba/Pi*2 B2/Pi*2 {log-log}

Subject 1 0.980 0.672 0.985 0.871 0.964

Subject 2 0.896 0.842 0.986 0.971 0.988

Subject 3 0.894 0.824 0.981 0.973 0.991

Subject 4 0.878 0.859 0.983 0.979 0.987

Subject 5 0.893 0.823 0.975 0.962 0.986

Average 0.908 0.804 0.982 0.951 0.983

50 Minute Period

(approximately 43 observations

CPMi = Bo + | CPMi = Bo+] CPMi=Bo+ | CPMi=Bo+ CPMi =

B1*Pi B1/Pi + Bo*Pi+B1/Pi+ | B1*Ln(Pi) | e*Bo*(Pi*B1)

B2/Pik2 Ba/Pi*2 {log-log}

Subject 1 0.981 0.716 0.986 0.878 0.968

Subject 2 0.898 0.873 0.989 0.975 0.989

Subject 3 0.896 0.850 0.981 0.975 0.989

Subject 4 0.882 0.883 0.986 0.982 0.986

Subject 5 0.894 0.861 0.979 0.986 0.987

Average 0.910 0.837 0.984 0.959 0.984

84

Page 98: AAlr - Virginia Tech

Table 8. (continued)

60 Minute Period

(approximately 36 observations

CPMi = Bo +] CPMi=Bo+] CPMi=Bo+ | CPMi=Bo+ CPMi =

B1*Pi B1/Pi + Bo*Pi+B1/Pi+ | Bi*Ln(Pi) | e*Bo*(Pi*B1)

B2/Pi*2 B2/Pi*2 {log-log}

Subject 1 0.982 0.749 0.987 0.885 0.973

Subject 2 0.897 0.896 0.991 0.980 0.988

Subject 3 0.897 0.874 0.984 0.979 0.990

Subject 4 0.884 0.900 0.987 0.984 0.984

Subject 5 0.897 0.885 0.983 0.974 0.989

Average 0.911 0.861 0.986 0.960 0.985

By Day

(39 observations)

CPMi = Bo +} CPMi=Bo+ | CPMi=Bo+ | CPMi=Bo+ CPMi =

B1*day Bi/day+ |B1*day+B2/day| Bi*Ln(day) |e*Bo*(day*B1)

Ba/day*2 +B3/day*2 {log-log}

Subject 1 0.954 0.765 0.983 0.931 0.988

Subject 2 0.817 0.892 0.986 0.979 0.976

Subject 3 0.778 0.910 0.984 0.974 0.975

Subject 4 0.821 0.918 0.990 0.993 0.964

Subject 5 0.814 0.889 0.982 0.977 0.980

Average 0.837 0.875 0.985 0.971 0.977

Average over Subjects

CPMi = Bo + | CPMi = Bo+|] CPMi=Bo+ | CPMi=Bo+ CPMi =

Average R*2 B1*Pi B1/Pi + Bo*Pi+B1/Pi+ | B1*Ln(Pi) | e*Bo*(Pi*B1)

Be/Pi*2 B2/Pi*2 {log-log}

30 Min 0.908 0.759 0.979 0.947 0.983

40 Min 0.908 0.804 0.982 0.951 0.983

50 Min 0.910 0.837 0.984 0.959 0.984

60 Min 0.911 0.861 0.986 0.960 0.985

By Day 0.837 0.875 0.985 0.971 0.977

Avg of Avg 0.895 0.827 0.983 0.958 0.982

CPMi = Characters per Minute on the i-th time period

Bo, B1, B2, and B3 are fitted coefficients

Pi = Time Period

85

Page 99: AAlr - Virginia Tech

all the subjects are shown in Table 9. The average characters per minute typed

on every fifth day is matched with the subjective ratings. No further analysis

was performed on the subjective ratings.

4.6 Fastest Speed Attained by the Subjects.

For most of the experiment, the subjects were presented trials which

were composed of randomly ordered characters. However, during the final

session, subjects were presented trials which were composed of a string of six

characters repeated 84 times (see Section 3.3.3). In Table 10, the fastest typing

speed on the repetition trials is compared with the fastest speed attained on the

randomized trials. The mean of the fastest speed attained on the random trials

(170.1 CPM) was compared with the mean of the fastest speed on the repeated

character string trial (175.3) using a t-test; the difference between the two

means was not significant (p > 0.20).

4.7 Prediction of Learning with Less Than all Data Points.

The analysis to this point has focused on the prediction of learning for

individuals where all actual performance points were used in the regression. lt

is desirable to be able to predict an accurate performance curve of long term

learning using only a few initial performance points.

The coefficients of the equation CPM; = eBoT,B1 (1; = trials} were

determined for each subject when only the performance in either the first 5, 10,

15, 20, 25, 50, 75, 100, 125, 150, 175, and 200 trials was used in the

regression. Figure 30 shows the actual performance by trial and the predictions

when either 25, 50, 100, 150, 200, or all points were used in the regression for

86

Page 100: AAlr - Virginia Tech

Table 9. Ratings Ranging From 0 to 9 Given by the Subjects Each Week

Assessing Their Progress and Corresponding Performance.

Subject 1 | Subject2 | Subject3 | Subject4 | Subject 5 Avg. Avg. Avg. Avg. Avg.

Day |Rating) CPM |Rating] CPM |Rating] CPM |Rating! CPM |Rating; CPM

5 4 53.3 4 84.7| 4.5 | 96.8) 5.5 | 67.6 4 71.4

10 5 | 74.2} 5 |118.8! 6 |122.4 89.0) 5 |101.4

15 5.5 |89.5]} 5 |124.8] 6.5 |127.8 106.2} 4 | 96.9

20 6.75 ]113.8] 6 |136.3] 6.5 |143.8 118.3} 5 |112.1

25 8 |133.0) 6 |147.2] 7 |156.8] 8.3 |128.1] 5.5 [126.6

30 8.5 |144.9) 7 |162.9) 7.5 |167.5| 9 |130.6 119.9

35 8.8 1156.0} 7 |165.8] 7.5 |170.4 136.0 136.8

40 8.9 {170.9} 8 |174.8] 8 |173.1 9 |144.2; 7.5 1147.3

Rating ranges from 0 to 9

Avg. CPM = The average characters per minute typed over the

entire day

87

Page 101: AAlr - Virginia Tech

Table 10. Fastest Speed Attained on Randomized and Repetitive Trials.

Peak Keying Speed

(Characters per Minute)

Fastest attained in Fastest attained in

Randomized Trials Repetitive Trials

Subject 1 177.9 198.6

Subject 2 186.3 173.0

Subject 3 180.6 173.0

Subject 4 150.6 166.0

Subject 5 155.2 166.0

88

Page 102: AAlr - Virginia Tech

Sld S¢

Sid OS

Sid 00}

Sid OS|

Sld 00¢

Sid IIV

| joalqns ‘sjulod

OO Pur

‘OSI ‘OO!

‘OG ‘Gz

Isuy 84}

AjUO Bulsq

SuoIssesHay 607-607]

‘og asnbi4

009 00S

OOP

JOQUINN [eu L

00€ | |

00¢ 001

0c

OV

09

08

00L

Ocl

Ovl

091

O8l

00¢

alnul sed suejoeseyyD

89

Page 103: AAlr - Virginia Tech

subject 1. (Regressions with 5, 10, 15, 20, 75, 125, and 175 points were also

calculated, but are not shown due to space limitations.) To determine how

accurately each equation fits the actual data, the mean squared error (MSE)

was calculated for all regressions considered. The squared error (predicted

CPM minus actual CPM, squared) was calculated for all the trials that each

subject completed. All squared errors (for example 487 squared errors for

subject 1) were averaged to obtain the MSE. The smaller the MSE, the more

closely the equation predicts the actual performance over the entire range

considered. Figure 31 shows the mean squared error as a function of the

number of initial points used to calculate the regression for Subject 1. Figures

32, 34, 36, and 38 show the prediction equations for subjects 2, 3, 4, and 5

respectively. Figures 33, 35, 37, and 39 plot the mean squared error as a

function of initial points used to calculate the regression for subjects 2, 3, 4, and

5 respectively.

The mean squared errors are shown in Table 11 for all subjects and the

number of initial points used to fit the Log-Log equation. The data from Table

11 (MSE'’s for all subjects) are graphed in Figure 40.

It is possible that one of the other original functions considered (e.g.,

CPM; = Bo + ByLn[Tj]) would be effective in predicting final performance from

only a few initial points even though it did not provide good fits using all the data

points. For subject 2, the following three equations were also used to predict

performance when only the first 25, 50, 100, 150, and 200 data points were

included to calculate the coefficients.

CPM; = Bo + B4/T; + Bo/T 2

CPM; = Bo + ByT; + Bo/T; + Ba/Ti2

90

Page 104: AAlr - Virginia Tech

Mean Sq

uare

d Error

(for

all points)

1800

1600

1400

1200

1000

800

600

400

200 0 50 100 150 200 250 300 350 400 450 500

Number of Points Used in Regression

Figure 31. MSE for Log-Log Regressions Using Only a Few Initial

Points, Subject 1.

91

Page 105: AAlr - Virginia Tech

oOo wh

a & oO ”

2 8 a AN ~ oF 4

100

Pts

40 +

enul—; sed suajoeieyy

92

20

+ 10

0 15

0 20

0 25

0 30

0 35

0 40

0 45

0 50

0 55

0 600

650

50

Tria

l Number

Figure

32.

Log-

Log

Regressions

Usin

g only th

e first

25,

50,

100,

15

0, an

d 20

0 Points,

Subj

ect

2.

Page 106: AAlr - Virginia Tech

Mean Squared

Erro

r (for

all po

ints

)

1800

1600

1400

1200

oma

©

© ©

800

600

400

200

| | | | | | | | | I | | y l T T I I I } I } T t l

O 50 100 150 200 250 300 350 400 450 500 550 600 650

Number of Points Used in Regression

Figure 33. MSE for Log-Log Regressions Using Only a Few Initial Points, Subject 2

93

Page 107: AAlr - Virginia Tech

25

Pts

SS S— °

SS =<

| | | | | { | | I | T T 1 J I ] i

oO © oO Oo © oO oS oO oO eo ite) wt © © © wT NI Tr r r r” 20

0

ejNul ed SyajoOBIeUyD

94

600

500

400

300

200

100

Tria

l Number

Figure

34.

Log-Log

Regressions

Usin

g on

ly the

firs

t 25,

50,

100,

150, and

200

Poin

ts,

Subj

ect

3

Page 108: AAlr - Virginia Tech

Mean

Squared

Error

(for

all po

ints

)

1800

1600

1400

1200

1000

800

600

400

200

O 50 100 150 200 250 300 350 400 450 500 550 600 650

Number of Points Used in Regression

Figure 35. MSE for Log-Log Regressions Using Only a Few Initial

Points, Subject 3

95

Page 109: AAlr - Virginia Tech

py yalgngs ‘siUulod

OOZ PUB

‘OSI ‘ODL

‘OS ‘GZ

ISU aU

AjUO Bulsq

suoIssesBay 607-607

‘9¢ eunbi4

009 00¢

0Ol

Sid IIV

sJaulo Sld

SZ Sid

0S

00S oor

nos

|

; -

l

|

|

nl

Pr if

[ht

‘| (4 NM

Ae gr

0c

Ov

09

08

001

Ocl

Ovi

09}

08 |

00¢

a]null\ Jed suajoeeyy

96

Page 110: AAlr - Virginia Tech

Mean

Sq

uare

d Er

ror

(for

all points)

1800

1600

1400

1200

1000

800

600

400

200

|

0 50 100 150 200 250 300 350 400 450 500 550 600 650

Number of Points Used in Regression

Figure 37. MSE for Log-Log Regressions Using Only a Few Initial Points, Subject 4

97

Page 111: AAlr - Virginia Tech

Sld 0S

Sid S¢

SIBUIO

G algns ‘s}UlIOg

0OZ PU

‘OSI ‘OO!

‘OSG ‘Gz

Isuy @Y}

AjuUO Hulsq

SuoIssesBey 607-607

‘ge eun6i4

009 00S

007

JEquINN jeu L

00€ 002

001

0d

Ov

09

08

001

Ocl

Ovl

091

08 |

00¢

ainuly Jad syajoeieuy

98

Page 112: AAlr - Virginia Tech

Mean

Sq

uare

d Er

ror

(for

all points)

1800

1600

1400

1200

1000

800

600

400

200 h woo 0 jl Fl l l (°

T T T “T t i qT i I i T T 1

0 50 100 150 200 250 300 350 400 450 500 550 600 650

Number of Points Used in Regression

Figure 39. MSE for Log-Log Regressions Using Only a Few Initial

Points, Subject 5

99

Page 113: AAlr - Virginia Tech

Table 11. Mean Squared Error for Log-Log Functions Fit Using Only Some

of the Initial Performance Data Points.

Number of

Initial

Points Used Mean Squared Error

in Regression

Subject 1| Subject 2} Subject 3} Subject 4) Subject 5] Average

25 1471.4 857.3 125.9 88.7 66.1 521.9

50 1130.6 43.1 36.3 184.9 118.9 302.8

75 1147.2 49.4 34.8 105.0 38.1 274.9

100 639.9 80.3 34.8 41.7 38.4 167.0

125 515.4 75.2 37.0 35.3 38.2 140.2

150 337.4 57.1 35.1 42.8 40.0 102.5

175 224.9 42.9 35.6 42.8 38.3 76.9

200 133.0 49.3 36.6 47.1 38.5 60.9

487 43.9

509 19.6

N

516 38.2] “ 35.5

622 40.9

636 34.8

100

Page 114: AAlr - Virginia Tech

"soalqns ji'y 40}

uoissesHay ul

pesn S]UIOd

JO JOQUINN

JO UO}OUNY

eB Se 10

Pauenbs ues

‘Or aunbl4

uoissalBay ul

pasn siulod

jo Jaquinyy

0S9 009

OSS 00S

OSh OOF

OSE ODE

OSG2e 002

OSGt OOl

OS O

re | eT

SBE

=F

0 TW NY

\ +

+ 002

g alqng —_»

7 00%

p palqnsg

—_4

+ 009

€ pelqng —_»+—

7 008

Z palqns —___»

T OOOL

| jelqnsg

—__

+ O01

7 00}

1 0091

101

JO pesenbs ueayy

Page 115: AAlr - Virginia Tech

CPM; = Bo + ByLn(T})

Figure 41 shows the regression equations obtained for the function CPM; = Bo +

B1/Tj + Bo/Tj2; the mean squared error for each equation is graphed in Figure

42. The predictions from CPM; = Bo + By Tj + Bo/T; + B3/Ti2 are shown in Figure

43 and the mean squared error for each fit in Figure 44. Finally, the fit of CPM; =

Bo + ByLn(Tj) using only some of the initial data points is graphed in Figure 45

and the mean squared error in Figure 46. For subject 2, the mean squared

errors for the four equations considered are listed in Table 12 by number of

points used to fit the coefficients. It is clear from Table 12 and Figures 42, 44,

and 46 that only the Log-Log function can accurately predict the entire learning

curve and final performance from a few initial data points.

4.8 Chord Analyses.

Response times for each of the 18 chords were recorded for all trials for

every subject. The average response times for each chord by trial were plotted

for all chords for the first 487 trials -- see Appendix F. The first 487 trials are

used because these were completed by all five subjects.

An analysis of variance was performed on the chord data for the last 20

trials, i.e. the trials 468 to 487. Response time was modeled on a within-subject

design of trial and chord. The chord effect and trial X chord effect were both

significant at p < 0.0001. A post-hoc analysis (Student-Newman-Keuls test)

was performed on the chord effect to determine which chords were significantly

fastest. Table 13 lists each chord, its average response time, and its relative

statistical significance. No post-hoc analysis was performed on the trial X chord

data, owing to the large number of comparisons (360).

102

Page 116: AAlr - Virginia Tech

‘SJUlOd 00Z

PUL ‘OSI

‘OOL‘OS ‘Sz

IS4 Oy

AlUO Bus;

suo|ssesBey ZvLL/ZatiL/La+og

‘z yelqns “Lp

aunbiy

JOQUINN [eu

0S9 009

OSG 00S

OSr O0h

OSE O0€

OS¢ 006

OSt OO

0S 0

L |

| t

4 —F

| |

| |

0

| 02

| | OV

09 j

} Sld

SZ fl y

+

08

“™ y

| OO}

Sid 001

pe

Sid OSI

Choc

Sid 002

| i

(Md if +

Od

Sid IIV po

Ay |

IN +

Ol

cal

i" im +

O8t

— 00¢

ejnuly sed suygjoeieyy

103

Page 117: AAlr - Virginia Tech

Mean

Sq

uare

d Er

ror

(for

all

points)

5000

4500

4000

3500

3000

2500

2000

1500

1000

500 | | | | | | | | | | | | | y LJ T 1 i T q 7 i r I 1

50 100 150 200 250 300 350 400 450 500 550 600 650

Number of Points Used to Predict

Figure 42. Subject 2, MSE for Bo+B1/Ti+B2/Ti*2 Regressions

Using Only a Few Initial Points.

104

Page 118: AAlr - Virginia Tech

"SIUIOd OO

PUB ‘OSL

‘OO! ‘OS

‘Gz ISI4

OY} AjUG

Buisp suoissalBey

?viL/eg+l/2gtiL, Ldt+og

‘z yelqns ‘ep

eunbiy JOquINN

jeuL

0S9 009

OSS 00S

OSt 00h

OSE O0€

OS2 002

OSI O01

OS 0

|

l |

l I

—T 0 08

001

Oc |

e]null Jed suejOeBeyy

Ovt raat

IK +

O91

7 O8t

Wf

Std IV

Sld 002

Sid 0S1 Sld 001

“Sid Sz Sid 0

~~ 002

105

Page 119: AAlr - Virginia Tech

5000 +

4000 +

® oO Oo

© | !

(for

all points)

Mena

Sq

uare

d Er

ror

N

© © Oo

1000 + 0 { + {— t——1—

O 50 100 150 200 250 300 350 400 450 500 550 600 650

Number of Points Used to Predict

Figure 44. Subject 2, MSE for Bo+B1*Ti+B2/Ti+B3/Ti*2 Using

Only a Few Initial Points.

106

Page 120: AAlr - Virginia Tech

SJUIOd OOZ

PUL ‘OG|

‘OOL ‘OG

‘Gz ISUIY

AY} AjUD

Buisq suoissasBay (11)u7,

La+og ‘2 oalqns

“Gp eunbi4

JOqUINN [eu

0S9 009

OSS 00S

OSr O0v

OSE OOF

OG2 O00¢

OSt OO

OS 0

I- |

4 |

7

| M iy

i

a

T

a

Sid SZ

Ls

i py

Sld 0S ant

me

¥ in

L Sid

0S4‘00L weer

——

gn

Sld 002

7

Sid IV \ My i pica

i. Hi

Oc

OV

09

08

001

Oc |

Ori

091

O81

00¢

alnull\ Jed suajoeIeyD

107

Page 121: AAlr - Virginia Tech

Mean

Sq

uare

d Er

ror

2000

1800

1600

1400

1200

800 (for

all

points)

° ©

©

600

400

200 0 50 100 150 200 250 300 350 400 450 500 550 600 650

Number of Points Used to Predict

Figure 46. Subject 2, MSE for Bo+B1*Ln(Ti) Regressions Using

Only a Few Initial Points.

108

Page 122: AAlr - Virginia Tech

Table 12. Mean Squared Error for Four Equations Fit Using Only Some of the Initial Performance Data Points. For Subject 2 Only.

Mean Squared Error for

Number of Points Used in the Regression

25 50 100 150 200 All

CPMi=Bo+B1*Ti+ 21860.1} 71309.7| 14519.7) 2904.1) 1284.4 68.8 B2/Ti+B3/Ti*2

CPMi=Bo+B1/Ti+ 4815.6] 3056.1) 1580.5] 1376.6] 1063.7) 405.5 B2/Ti*2

CPMi=Bo+B1*In(Ti) 1989.3 547.8 170.9) 174.1] 117.8 44.1

CPMi=(e*Bo)*(Ti*B1) 875.3 43.1 80.3 57.3 49.3 40.9 CPMi = Characters per Minute

Bo, B1, B2, and B3 are fitted coeffiecients

Ti = Trials

109

Page 123: AAlr - Virginia Tech

Table 13. Chord Response Times (in ms) Averaged Over Trials 468 to 487,

the Last 20 Trials Performed by All Subjects. (Means with the same letters are not significantly different)

Chord Mean Std. Dev. SNK Grouping

13 325.0 139.7 A

24 329.0 125.7 A B

14 350.5 144.5 A B C

36 359.2 138.0 A B C

35 361.8 148.4 A B C

28 364.6 150.3 A B C

26 368.3 148.3 A B CC OD

45 368.9 145.8 A B C OD

46 371.0 160.6 A B Cc OD

68 371.4 176.2 A B CC OD

23 372.9 140.7 B cC OD

15 375.5 168.9 B Cc OD

17 378.5 160.0 Cc OD

57 380.2 171.4 Cc oD

16 381.0 168.1 C D

25 399.4 177.7 Cc D

37 413.4 167.2 D

48 414.3 175.3 D

110

Page 124: AAlr - Virginia Tech

4.9 Analysis of Average Performance of All Subjects.

To this point the analysis was focused on predicting performance of

individual subjects. It is also of interest to evaluate the ability of the functions to

predict the combined subjects’ data as well as the average typing speed.

4.9.1 Prediction of All Subjects Performance.

Using regressions to predict combined data of the subjects differs from

predicting average performance; each trial has five values (one from each

subject) whereas an average prediction has only one point for each trial. The

five functions considered for individual data were also used to predict all

subjects’ data:

CPM; = Bo + By Tj (Equation 1)

CPM; = Bo + By/T; + Bo/T)2 (Equation 2)

CPM; = Bo + Bi7; + Bo/T; + Ba/Ti2 (Equation 3)

CPM; = Bo + ByLn(T}) (Equation 4)

CPM; = eBo 7;B: (Equation 5)

These equations were only fit on the first 487 trials - those that included all

subjects. The R2 values for each function are listed in Table 14. The following

Log-Log equation provided the best fit (R2 = 0.843):

CPM; = 33.115*T,9-291

The actual data points of all subjects and the plotted equation are shown in

Figure 47.

Page 125: AAlr - Virginia Tech

Table 14. Summary of R-Squared Values for All Functions Considered for Average of the Subjects and All Subjects’ Performance

R-Squared Value

Equation All Subjects |Avg. of Subjects

CPMi=Bo+B1*Ti 0.747 0.883

CPMi=Bo+B1/Ti+B2*(1/Ti*2) 0.351 0.457

CPMi=Bo+B1*Ti+B2*(1/Ti)+B3*(1/Ti*2) 0.799 0.951

CPMi=Bo+B1*Ln(Ti) 0.791 0.956

CPMi=Exp(Bo)*(Ti*B1) 0.843 0.991

CPMi = Characters per Minute on the i-th Trial

Ti = Trial

Bo, B1, B2, and B3 are fitted coefficients

112

Page 126: AAlr - Virginia Tech

"eleq ,sjoalans

yy ‘souewJOLeg

jenjoy pue

uoioipasg 607-607

‘zp aunbi4

JOQqUNN JeUL

0S9 009

OSG 00S

OSb O00v

OSE O0€&

O0S¢ 000

OSt O01

| |

| |

| __|

| |

| |

| I

po 1

1 1

| |

1 I

\ |

| J

001

Ocl

Ov}

091

08}

- 002

(GZ OvIeWL).GLEES

=Wdd

e]nuIW sed srejoeseyy

113

Page 127: AAlr - Virginia Tech

4.9.2 Prediction of Average Performance.

The average typing speed and its standard deviation were calculated for

the trials completed by all five subjects (up to trial 487). The same five

equations used to predict individual performance were considered to fit average

performance. Table 14 lists the R@ values for each function. The following Log-

Log equation provided the best fit of the data (R2 = 0.991):

CPM; = 34.467+T,0-244

Figure 48 shows the average characters per minute typed, the prediction of the

average using the above equation, and the average plus and minus one

standard deviations.

114

Page 128: AAlr - Virginia Tech

“sjoalqns jy

jo abeJaay

aul jo

uOIeIANG

DIJEPURIS |

- pue

+ puke

‘BOUBWUOLAY abeieAy

jenjoy ‘uoHoIpaig

607-607 ‘gp

ainbi4

0S9 009

OSS 00S

A8q PIS

I -

ABQ PIS

I+

JOQUINN [eu L

OSp O0b

OSE O0€&

OSe O02

OSt OOF

OS

| |

| J

| |

| i

| |

I T

t I

I t

t t

(ppz OvieUl),Z9r

ve = NdO

0g

OV

09

08

00}

Oct

eynui Jed suajoBseYD

Ovl

091

08}

00¢

115

Page 129: AAlr - Virginia Tech

5. DISCUSSION

The goals of the experiment were to find answers to questions that have

not been fully addressed in the literature. This study provided information on

what function best predicts learning, the best predictors of performance, and

criterion of peak keying speed. The study did not address all the questions

raised in the literature review; for example, the relationship of character set size

versus time to maximal speed is important, but was not investigated.

5.1 Speed - Accuracy Performance.

Providing feedback to the subjects at the end of each trial seemed to be

very effective in controlling speed and accuracy tradeoffs. All the subjects had

an average accuracy greater than 97% over the entire experiment. In addition,

the subjects generally set goals for themselves such as finish a trial in less than

five minutes or reach 100 characters per minute. By providing feedback of

performance, there seemed to be natural performance goals throughout the

experiment for which the subjects aimed.

The subjects probably did not make adjustments in their speed and

accuracy criteria after every trial, but more likely made adjustments after

perceiving a trend in either speed or accuracy performance. For example, if a

subject's accuracy level for a particular trial fell below 97%, that person

probably did not immediately slow down on the next trial. However, if accuracy

performance on many consecutive trials was below 97%, the subject probably

maintained speed but tried to increase accuracy. Overall, the method of

providing immediate feedback to the subjects on speed and accuracy appeared

116

Page 130: AAlr - Virginia Tech

effective in motivating the subjects and controlling some of the error associated

with speed accuracy tradeoffs.

5.2 Using Trials to Predict Performance.

One goal of this study was to find an equation which described the

learning or performance progress on a keyboarding task. The following five

candidate functions were fit to individual performance curves.

CPM; = Bo + ByT; (Equation 1)

CPM, = Bo + By/T; + Bo/T 2 (Equation 2)

CPM, = Bg + ByT; + Bo/T; + B3/T;2 (Equation 3)

CPM; = Bo + ByLn(T}) (Equation 4)

CPM; = eBo T;81 (Equation 5)

where CPM, is the characters per minute typed on the i-th trial

(dependent variable), Tj is the i-th trial, and Bo and B, are fitted

coefficients.

Only equations that predicted performance in a monotonically increasing

fashion were considered. Actual performance on a keyboarding task is not

always increasing with each additional trial of practice; however, there is a

general trend of improvement with practice in learning a skill. A Log-Log

equation of the form CPN; = eBo TB 1 (where CPM, is performance on the i-th

trial or hour, Tj is the i-th trial or hour of practice, and Bo and By, are fitted

coefficients) described each subject’s performance most accurately. The Log-

Log equation had an average R2 of 0.956 when using trials as the regressor

variable. An important characteristic of this type of equation is that it does not

117

Page 131: AAlr - Virginia Tech

reach an asymptote, and therefore does not predict a maximal keying speed.

The only equation considered which predicts an asymptote (CPM; = Bo + Bi/Tj +

B2/T;2) did not even account for 50% of the variance (average R@ = 0.431).

Overall, the Log-Log function provided the best description of subject

performance on the keyboarding task up to the first 60 hours of practice when

trials are the predictor variables. The ability of the equation to predict beyond

that point is unknown because there are no actual! performance data for

comparison with the predictions. However, from Figures 24 to 28 it appears that

the subjects did not reach peak performance (i.e., a stable, non-increasing

value) in this experiment.

5.2.1 Coefficients of the Log-Log Equation.

It appears that the shapes of the Log-Log curves (performance

predictions) for subjects 2 to 5 are very similar, as shown previously in Figure

29. However, the performance prediction of subject 1 varies significantly from

the predictions for the other four subjects. The predictions for subjects 2 and 3

are nearly identical, as are the predictions for subjects 4 and 5. Subjects’ 2 and

3 performance predictions seem to differ from subjects 4 and 5 by only a

constant. It was hypothesized that one or both of the coefficients for subject 1

were significantly different from the other subjects causing the different shape of

the prediction curve. However, after using a t-test to compare the values of the

coefficients, it was found that all the Bo coefficients were significantly different

from each other, and all B; comparisons except one were significantly different,

as shown previously in Table 6. The reason for so many significant differences

118

Page 132: AAlr - Virginia Tech

in the coefficients is that the t-tests were so powerful because of the large

number of observations for each subject.

5.2.2 Prediction of Peak Keying Speed.

While the Log-Log function predicts the actual progress of the subjects

very well, by its nature it does not approach a peak value asymptote. Four

techniques can be used to estimate each subject’s peak performance. First, a

large number of trials chosen to represent the upper limit of practice can be

entered into the Log-Log equation. For example, 5,000 trials might be assumed

to be a long enough training period for a person to reach a peak keying speed.

For subject 3, this leads to a speed of 265 characters per minute (CPMsooo =

48.231+50009-199 = 265).

The second method uses the slope of the Log-Log function to determine

peak keying speed. Although the Log-Log function never approaches an

asymptote, its slope decreases as trial numbers increase. Slope is equal to the

first derivative of the performance function, CPM; = 48.231*T,9-199. The first

derivative of this equation is as follows:

d(CPM) / dT; [slope] = (0.199)(48.231)*T;0-801 = 9.6 «7,;0-801

Thus, a given slope could be associated with top speed, the trial number

determined when this slope occurs, and finally that trial number could be used

to calculate maximal performance. For example, it may be assumed that when

the slope decreases to 0.025 CPM/Trial, a person has effectively reached peak

performance. For subject 3, a slope of 0.025 CPM/Trial occurs at trial 1684

(solving for T; when 0.025 = 9.6 «7, 9-891) at trial 1684, the Log-Log equation

119

Page 133: AAlr - Virginia Tech

predicts a speed of 212 characters per minute (CPMy¢94 = 48.231*16849-199 —

212). |

The third technique to predict top speed involves using the function CPM;

= Bo + B,/T; + Bo/T|2 as suggested by Bittner (1991). He assumed that the

equation Y; = Bo + By/T; + B2/T;2 can predict peak performance, where Y; is the i-

th trial performance, Tj is the i-th trial, and Bo, By, and Bo are fitted coefficients.

As the number of trials increases the magnitude of the last two terms decreases

very rapidly ( B,/T; and B2/T;2) and Bg estimates peak performance; it is the only

function of all functions considered that will reach an asymptote. However, this

formula does not predict the performance progress of the subjects very well (all

R2 are less than 0.5 as shown in Table 4); thus, Bp can not be considered an

accurate prediction of the fastest keying speed.

The function appears not to predict final performance well because of the

steepness of the initial learning curve; thus, it underestimates peak

performance, see Figure 21 for subject 2. If several of the early points are

deleted from the regression, this function might predict a more accurate

asymptote. As an example, the formula CPM; = Bo + B;/T; + Bo/T;2 was fit to the

data of subject 3 ignoring the first 100 data points. It appears that most of the

steep portion of the learning curve is eliminated by ignoring the first 100 points,

see Figure 49. The fit of the remaining 536 points yields the equation CPM; =

195.7 - 16,199(1/T;) + 919,625(1/T;2) which has an R2 of 0.863 and predicts a

peak performance of 195.7 characters per minute.

The fourth technique used to predict peak performance employs only the

final trials. These trials were composed of a string of six characters repeated 84

times (see Section 3.3.3). It was felt that if keying the string became a highly

120

Page 134: AAlr - Virginia Tech

'E ANS ‘SIEUL

OOL ISUl4

SUL INOYWM

‘ZvIL/ZAtLL/ +a

= Wd

uby ay}

Guisq peeds

yead jo

ayewisy “Gy

aunb4 JOQUINN

jeu

069 009

OSS 00S

OSr O0r

ose o00€

ose 002

Ost ool

og 0

| I

} |

{ |

| |

| |

| |

| I

I {

| {

t 1

1 I

] I

| i

0

+ OV

I 09

+ O01

+ Oct

ainulyy Jad suajoeseuy

ry +

OVI Ph

TY

Nf yr

oF Wi

+

O91

+ O8t

— 00¢

ovil/ed+ W/lG@+@

--------- enjoy

121

Page 135: AAlr - Virginia Tech

automated motor program (with minimal response times), the performance on

these trials would represent peak keying speed. However, two of the subjects

typed faster in the random trials than in the repeated character string trial, as

shown previously in Table 10. In addition, the means of the fastest speeds

attained on randomized and repetitive trials were not significantly different

which indicates that the repetitive trials, in fact, were not a good indication of

peak keying performance.

Of the four techniques, using the equation CPM; = Bo + By/T; + Bo/Ti2

(without initial trials) might provide the best method for predicting peak

performance. This equation provides a much better predictor of actual

performance if initial performance points are ignored; the R2 values and

predicted peak performance for each subject are shown in Table 15. This

equation, without the 100 initial trials, predicts peak (asymptotic) performance

which was not attained by any of the subjects on the randomized trials

(comparing Table 15 with previously shown Table 10). The major difficulty with

this approach is to determine how many of the original points should be left out

of the regression.

Assuming a large amount of practice time, assuming a minimal slope, or

using the repeated string trials do not seem to be suitable methods to use to

predict peak performance. By what criteria could the number of trials be chosen

and considered the upper limit of practice; should it be 2000, 5000, 10000, or

more trials? The same dilemma arises when trying to assume a minimal slope

as the stopping point of improvement.

122

Page 136: AAlr - Virginia Tech

Table 15. Values of R2 and Peak Performance when Fitting the Equation CPM;

= Bo + By/T; + Bo/Ti2 Without the First 100 Performance Points.

Subject R2 Predicted Peak (in CPM)

1 0.940 196.8

2 0.854 197.7

3 0.863 195.7

4 0.870 158.8

5 0.759 161.1

where CPM j is the characters per minute typed on the i-th trial

(dependent variable) and Tjis the i-th trial of practice

123

Page 137: AAlr - Virginia Tech

5.2.3 Combin nd Aver Performan f All th

The Log-Log equation fit the combined and average performance of the

subjects best. It was expected to fit best because it was the best at predicting

each individual’s performance. The equation fit to the combined subjects’ data

probably had a high R@ (0.843) because there were not more than five subjects’

data. Kroemer (1990) and McMulkin and Kroemer (1991) did not find an

equation which accurately fit combined subjects’ data. This could be due either

to the larger number of subjects (10 to 12) or the shorter training periods (3 to

12 hours).

5.3 Predicting Performance from Time

The average characters per minute entered by the subjects were

calculated for 30, 40, 50, and 60 minute time periods and for sessions.

Because the time periods were composed of entire trials, the average lengths of

the 30, 40, 50, and 60 minute time periods were closer to 32, 42, 52, and 62

minutes respectively, shown previously in Table 7. It is not so important that the

length of the time periods are factors of 10, but that the average length of the

time increments is close to equal for all subjects. Inspection of Table 7 reveals

that for all four time periods each subject had approximately the same average

amount of practice.

After establishing the performance of the subjects over the time intervals,

the same five equations used to predict performance from trials were fit to the

time period data. The equations CPM; = Bo + ByP; + Bo/P; + B3/Pi2 and CPM; =

eBo p,B1 (a Log-Log equation) provided the best fits. P; is equal to 1, 2, 3,.... not

124

Page 138: AAlr - Virginia Tech

30 minutes, 60 minutes, and so on. The general form of the Log-Log equation

(CPM; = eBo P;B1) provided good fits for both repetition and time data.

It does not appear that changing the length of time increments has much

effect on the ability to predict performance. Five different time increments were

used as regressor variables: 30 minutes, 40 minutes, 50 minutes, 60 minutes,

and total sessions (1.5 hours, not all this time was spent practicing). For the

Log-Log function, the largest difference in R2 values was only 0.008 (Table 8).

5.4 Subjective Ratings.

The subjective ratings could not be used in any analysis to predict

performance of the subjects. The subjects probably found it difficult to rate their

performance early in the experiment because they did not know what their final

performance would be. At the beginning of the experiment, subjects might have

simply chosen a middle rating and then gradually increased it until the end of

the experiment. For this reason, the ratings that the subjects provided are not of

an interval nature, but are instead at best an ordinal measure. The data of

subjects four and five illustrate some of the problems with the subjective rating

procedure, shown previously in Table 9. Subject four rated performance at a

nine by day 30 but was clearly still improving over the following two weeks.

Subject five rated performance at a four on day 5, then a five on day 10, and

then back to a four on day 15. This subject probably felt the ratings of the initial

two weeks were too high and then readjusted the scale for week three and

started the rating at four again. Overall, little information could be extracted from

the results of the subjective ratings.

125

Page 139: AAlr - Virginia Tech

5.5 Evaluation of Criteria to Predict Performance.

Part of the second goal of the experiment was to determine which

criterion of the following three criteria could be used to predict keying speed:

number of trials/repetitions, hours (or other time increments) of practice, and

subjective opinion. The Log-Log equation, CPMj = eBo T;B1, provided the most

accurate prediction of performance when considering two regressor variables,

time and repetition. The average R2 values using the Log-Log function for

these two criteria were as follows:

Trials (504 repetitions per trial) R2 = 0.956

Time of Practice (average for all increments) R2 = 0.982

This gives further credence to the assertion that the Log-Log function describes

the underlying progress of performance. It does not appear to matter much

whether repetition or time of practice is used to predict performance. Although

the equation CPM; = Bo + ByPj + Bo/P; + B3/P2 is effective when time increments

constitute the regressor variable, it did not predict performance as well as the

Log-Log equation when trials constitute the predictor variable. This indicates

that the equation to describe performance remains the same when the predictor

variable changes from trials to time. These findings contrast those of Thurstone

(1919); he asserted that the function changed when time instead of repetition

was used to describe performance progress. The Log-Log function continued

to show flexibility with different levels of the time predictor. The fits of the Log-

Log equation had fairly constant R@ values across the five time increments

considered.

126

Page 140: AAlr - Virginia Tech

Subjective opinion data, as elicited in this study, could not be used to

predict performance due to the ordinal nature of the ratings. See Section 5.4 for

further discussion.

5.6 The Concept of Peak Performance.

It is apparent from Figures 24 to 28 that the actual performance of the

subjects did not approach an asymptote which could be considered the peak

performance. The Log-Log equation, which provided a very good description of

the actual performance does not predict an asymptote; it presumes continual

improvement in performance which, however, becomes increasingly slow and

gradual.

Both the actual performance observed and the prediction of the Log-Log

equation seem to contradict the traditional presumption of learning - a slow

increase in performance until gradually a upper limit is reached that can not be

exceeded. It might be that there is no limit to performance improvement.

The response times of the keyboarding task of this study can be broken

down into their components. First, the stimulus must be sensed by the subject,

for example the number 8 is to be typed. Second, the sensed information must

be transmitted through the (afferent) peripheral nervous system to the brain.

Third, a decision must be made in the brain as to what response to make to the

incoming stimulus. Fourth, the response signal must be transmitted along the

(efferent) peripheral nervous system to muscles. Fifth, the ensuing movement of

the fingers determines the "response time" and a character is typed. The third

and fifth steps of the response are probably becoming faster with practice with

most of the improvement likely to occur in the third step, the decision making

127

Page 141: AAlr - Virginia Tech

process. There may be a limitation to how quickly response decisions can

become. However, that minimal decision time may never actually be reached.

5.7 Prediction of an Entire Curve from the Initial Data Points.

From a practical standpoint, to predict the performance of subjects, it is

costly to gather over 500 data points. It is, therefore, of great interest to find out

how many initial performance points are needed before an accurate learning

curve can be predicted.

For subject 2, four different equations were fit to the actual data when

only 25, 50, 100, 150, and 200 of the initial performance points were used in the

regression. The mean squared error between the actual and predicted

performance for all 20 cases (4 equations and 5 quantities of initial number of

points used in the regression) was shown previously in Table 12. By perusing

the information in Table 12, it becomes clear that the only function capable of

accurately predicting the entire learning curve from the initial data points is a

Log-Log function.

The remaining functions do not closely approximate learning or final

performance. The Log-Log equation has a mean squared error which is at least

an order of magnitude smaller that the other three functions when only 25, 50,

100, 150, or 200 initial data points are used in the regressions. Inspection of

previously shown Figures 32, 41, 43 and 45 clearly indicates that only the Log-

Log function predicts final performance effectively from regressions including

only a few initial performance points. The other functions either predict too high

a final performance (CPMj = Bo + By,Tj + Bo/T; + B3/Ti2) or too low a final

performance (CPM, = Bo + By/T; + Bo/Tj2 and CPM; = Bo + ByLn[TjJ).

128

Page 142: AAlr - Virginia Tech

It is difficult from a small sample size of five subjects to draw a definite

conclusion about how many points are sufficient to accurately predict the entire

performance curve using the Log-Log equation. The regression using all points

is assumed to have the lowest mean squared error. Thus, the MSE of

regressions using less points should be compared to the MSE of the all points

regressions to determine its relative accuracy. For four of the subjects (2

through 5), the MSE is drastically reduced when the number of points used

reaches 50, see Figure 40 and Table 11 shown previously. The MSE resulting

from including more than 50 points is a fairly flat function for these four subjects.

In contrast, subject 1 exhibited a much more modest decrease in MSE as the

number of points included in the regression increased.

Overall, the Log-Log function proved to be the only equation able to

predict the entire learning curve from a limited number of initial data points.

Due to the small number of subjects, it is difficult to make assertions about the

number of points needed with the Log-Log equation to capture the entire

performance progress with a reasonably small mean squared error: as few as

50 initial points were needed for four of the five subjects (out of about 550 total

points). However, when applying these findings, the amount of uncertainty that

is acceptable needs to be determined which then points to the number of initial

performance points needed.

5.8 Chord Analyses.

The average response times for each chord were calculated for the last

20 trials including all subjects, i.e., trials 468 to 487. Table 13, shown

previously, lists the chords’ average response times and relative ranking. The

129

Page 143: AAlr - Virginia Tech

major result of the chord analysis is that after over 400 trials of practice there are

still chords that are significantly faster than others. However, significant

differences found could be due to the large power of the tests. Each chord

average response time consisted of 2800 observations (28 chords per trial X 20

trials X 5 subjects).

The chord analysis was based on practice due to repetition not amount of

time because response times from trials 468 to 487 were used. For subject 1,

these trials were the last ones completed, while for subjects 2 to 5 they were

completed before the end of the experiment. The number of final trials to

include in the analysis was limited to 20 for two reasons. First, chord

performance was of interest after a large amount of practice. It was of interest to

find if differential chord performance existed after a large amount of practice.

Second, the last 20 trials include enough observations to obtain a

representative average and standard deviation. It is possible that fewer number

of final trials could be used for the analysis.

5.9 Further Research.

The results of this study point to several areas for future research and

analysis. First, the trials were held constant at 504 characters. It would be

interesting to investigate the effect of different trial lengths on the ability of the

Log-Log function to predict performance. In the same way that variations of time

increments were used with the Log-Log equation, number of repetitions per trial

could be varied and accuracy of the Log-Log function analyzed.

Second, the performance points seem to have some autocorrelation.

Performances of the subjects “bounced” about the Log-Log curve. Apparently,

130

Page 144: AAlr - Virginia Tech

series of points alternated between being higher and lower than the previous

point. It might be possible to include an autocorrelation factor in the model of

performance and then account for more of the variation.

Third, as mentioned in the literature review, there appears to be a

relationship between number of characters that are learned and the steepness

of performance progress. Research is needed on how the values of the

coefficients in the Log-Log equation change with character set size. For

example, faster keying speeds might have been obtained more quickly if this

study had used 10 characters instead of 18.

Fourth, the subjective opinions were that were obtained in this study

could not be correlated with performance. Perhaps other techniques of rating

could be used to find a relationship between performance and subjective

opinion about that performance.

131

Page 145: AAlr - Virginia Tech

6. REFERENCES

Alden, D.G., Daniels, R.W., and Kanarick, A.F. (1972). Keyboard design and operation: a review of the major issues. Human Factors, 14(4), 275-293.

Baddeley, A.D, and Longman, D.J.A. (1978). The influence of length and frequency of training session on the rate of learning to type. Ergonomics, 21(8), 627-635.

Bartram, D., and Feggou, O. (1985). An evaluation of four different keyboard designs for foreign destination coding desks. Report ERG/Z6545/85/4c. Hull: University of Hull, Ergonomics Research Group.

Bequaert, F.C., and Rochester, N. (1977). Teaching typing on a chord keyboard. IBM Report TR 00.2918. Cambridge, MA: SPD - Cambridge Systems Group.

Bittner, A.C. (1991). Analysis of learning-curve asymptotes: ergonomic applications. In W. Karwowski and J.W. Yates (eds.), Advances in industrial ergonomics and safety Ill. London, England: Taylor and Francis Ltd.

Bowen, H.M., and Guinness, G.V. (1965). Preliminary experiments on keyboard design for semiautomatic mail sorting. Journal of Applied Psychology, 49(3), 194-198.

Callaghan, T.F. (1989). The utility of a technique for testing the difference in ease of chords on the ternary chord keyboard. Unpublished masters thesis, Virginia Polytechnic Institute and State University, Blacksburg, VA.

Chapman, J.C. (1919). The learning curve in typewriting. Journal of Applied Psychology, 3, 252-268.

Conrad R., and Longman, D.J.A. (1965). Standard typewriter versus chord keyboard - an experimental comparison. Ergonomics, 8, 77-88.

Cornog, J.R., Hockman, J.F., and Craig, J.C. (1963). Address encoding - a study of the double-binary keyboard as a link in the machine-sorting of mail (ASME Report Number 63-WA-338). New York, NY: American Society of Mechanical Engineers.

Dvorak, A. (1943). There is a better typewriter keyboard. National Business Education Quarterly, 12(2), 51-66.

Dvorak, A., Merrick, N.L., Dealey, W.L., and Ford, G.C. (1936). Typewriting behavior. New York, NY: American Book Company.

132

Page 146: AAlr - Virginia Tech

Evey, R.J. (1980). How typists type. IBM Report Number HFC-35. San Jose, CA: IBM General Products Division.

Fathallah, F.A. (1988). An experimental comparison of a ternary chord keyboard with the QWERTY keyboard. Unpublished masters thesis, Virginia

Polytechnic Institute and State University, Blacksburg, VA.

Gentner, D.R. (1983). The acquisition of typewriting skill. Acta Psychologica, 54, 233-248.

Glencross, D., and Bluhm, N. (1986). Intensive computer keyboard training programmes. Applied Ergonomics, 17(3), 191-194.

Goldstein, |.L. (1986). Training in organizations: needs assessment, development, and evaluation. Pacific Grove, CA: Brooks/Cole.

Gopher, D. (1986). Experiments with a two hand chord keyboard - the structure and acquisition process of a complex transcription skill. Report Number HEIS-86-5. Arlington, VA: Office of Naval Research, Personne! and Training Research Programs.

Gopher, D. and Eilam, Z. (1979). Development of the letter-shape keyboard: a new approach to the design of data entry devices. /n Proceedings of the Human Factors Society 23rd Annual Meeting (pp.40-44). Santa Monica, CA: Human Factors Society.

Gopher, D., Hilsernath, H., and Raij, D. (1985). Steps in the development of a new data entry device based upon two hand chord keyboard. /n Proceedings of the Human Factors Society 29th Annual Meeting (pp.132-136). Santa Monica, CA: Human Factors Society.

Gopher, D. and Raij, D. (1988). Typing with a two-hand chord keyboard: will the QWERTY become obsolete. /EEE Transactions on Systems, man, and cybernetics, 18(4), 601-609.

Green, L. (1940). A study of typewriting achievement in three high schools. Journal of Educational Research, 34(3), 209-217.

Lee, S.S., and Kroemer, K.H.E. (1991). Report on subjectively preferred one- hand chords on TCK #2. Blacksburg, VA: Industrial Ergonomics Laboratory, Virginia Polytechnic Institute and State University.

Leonard, J.A., and Newman, R.C. (1965). On the acquisition and maintenance of high speed and high accuracy in a keyboard task. Ergonomics, 8, 281- 304.

133

Page 147: AAlr - Virginia Tech

Lockhead, G.R., and Klemmer, E.T. (1959). An evaluation of an 8-key word- writing typewriter. IBM Report Number RC-180. Yorktown Heights, NY: IBM Research Center.

Killeen, P.R. (1978). Stability Criteria. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 19, 17-25.

Kinkead, R. (1975). Typing speed, keying rated, and optimal keyboard layouts. In Proceedings of the Human Factors Society 19th Annual Meeting (pp.159-161). Santa Monica, CA: Human Factors Society.

Kirschenbaum, A., Friedman, Z., and Melnik, A. (1986). Performance of disabled persons on a chordic keyboard. Human Factors, 28(2), 187-194.

Klemmer, E.T. (1958). A ten-key typewriter. IBM Report Number RC-65. Yorktown Heights, NY: IBM Research Center.

Kroemer, K.H.E. (1990). Final report on the experiments performed on VATELL Corporation's ternary chord keyboard research project 43716 (230-1 1- 110F-106-801189-1). Blacksburg, VA: Industrial Ergonomics Laboratory, Virginia Polytechnic Institute and State University.

McMulkin, M.L., and Kroemer, K.H.E. (1991). Report on the experiments performed on one-hand TCK #2. Blacksburg, VA: Industrial Ergonomics Laboratory, Virginia Polytechnic Institute and State University.

Morgan, A.S., Drake, J.B., Heck, S.K., and Long, C.H. (1981). Experimental study of effects on speed and accuracy of teaching alternate-hand method on a ten-key electronic calculator. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 52, 695-700.

Mussin, E.H. (1980). The microwriter. In Excerpt from JERE Electronic Office Conference Proceeding, (129-134). London, UK: Institute of Electronic and Radio Engineers.

Neter, J., and Wasserman, W. (1974). Applied Linear Statistical Models. Homewood, IL: Irwin Inc.

Norman, D.A., and Fisher, D. (1982). Why alphabetic keyboards are not easy to use: keyboard layout doesn't much matter. Human Factors, 24(5), 509- 519.

Noyes, J. (1983a). Chord keyboards. Applied Ergonomics, 14(1), 55-59.

Noyes, J. (1983b). The QWERTY keyboard: a review. International Journal of Man-Machine Studies, 18, 265-281.

134

Page 148: AAlr - Virginia Tech

Ratz, H.C., and Ritchie, D.L. (1961). Operator performance on a chord keyboard. Journal of Applied Psychology, 45(5), 303-308.

Richardson, R.M.M., Telson, R. U., Koch, C.G., and Chrysler, $.T. (1987). Evaluation of conventional, serial, and chord keyboard options for mail encoding. In Proceedings of the Human Factors Society 31st Annual Meeting (pp.911-915). Santa Monica, CA: Human Factors Society.

Rochester, N., Bequaert, F.C., and Sharp, E.M. (1978). The chord keyboard. Computer, 11(12), 57-63.

Salthouse, T.A. (1984). The skill of typing. Scientific American, 250(2), 128-135.

Salthouse, T.A. (1986). Effects of practice on a typing-like keying task. Acta Psychologica, 62, 189-198.

SAS Institute Inc. (1985). SAS user's guide: statistics. Cary, NC: SAS Institute Inc.

Seibel, R. (1962). Performance on a five-finger chord keyboard. Journal of Applied Psychology, 46(3), 165-169.

Seibel, R. (1964). Levels of practice, learning curves, and system values for human performance on complex tasks. Human Factors, 6, 293-298.

Seibel, R. (1972). Data entry devices and procedures. In Van Cott, H., and

Kinkade, R. (Eds.), Human engineering guide to equipment design (pp.311-345). Washington D.C.: American Institute for Research.

Showel, M. (1974). A comparison of alternative media for teaching beginning typists. The Journal of Educational Research, 67(6), 279-285.

Sidorsky, R.C. (1974). ALPHA-DOT: a new approach to direct computer entry of battlefield data (Technical Report Number 249). Arlington, VA: U.S. Army Research Institute for Behavioral and Social Sciences.

Strong, E.P. (1956) A comparative experiment in simplified keyboard retraining and standard keyboard supplementary training. Washington D.C.: General Services Administration.

Thurstone, L.L. (1919). The learning equation. Psychological Monographs, 26, 1-52.

Towill, D.R. (1976). Transfer functions and learning curves. Ergonomics, 19(5), 623-638.

135

Page 149: AAlr - Virginia Tech

Towill, D.R. (1982). How complex a learning curve model need we use? The Radio and Electronic Engineer, 52(7), 331-338.

West. L.J. (1969). Acquisition of typewriting skills. Belmont, CA: Pitman Publishing.

136

Page 150: AAlr - Virginia Tech

Appendix A

Example of a Test Trial

137

Page 151: AAlr - Virginia Tech

Q/728+*0474*-S/S9 . 88¢*08 ¢.¢ -876/8148T 3T118S¢7244S04+467-T7742

1609/T931677*+/29SS .12T+88S¢286877¢++9T/-7T+. 69366831929610/5

*7926480 .€€9072¢21S04/1T 7801/26. -**42/42+108555 ¢. 5312S00+78*

203 -*5S%¢€6S*348-962--T+OT *8-S/-@. 8101/1268 -7T*71T .4 T-*/1*8

711+/-694+4/8*/9947/S¢ -84935¢45T/8/S0- . 5¢1T+060-8*%++-2T+698/20

T3/TOS¢+S3T2*+-52S893+4+714245S. *, &. 79S83S¢32*. +6TO450ST--/41T6

34. -23-T*301¢ -4/T2T48.1/¢ 83ST9226S/7544. /-62469++1+7-*-S914.

4S6. *€17-5644 -134919*/*765937/36967 . +5530. G@9910+*9364+5 +343

5**5*55333..3..5..555555

S represents "Subtotal"

T represents "Total"

represents "decimal point"

€ represents "backspace"

138

Page 152: AAlr - Virginia Tech

Appendix B

Experimental Instructions

139

Page 153: AAlr - Virginia Tech

EXPERIMENTAL INSTRUCTIONS

The purpose of the study is to gather information about a newly designed keyboard, called the One-Hand Ternary Chord Keyboard (TCK). You are expected to participate for 40 one-and-a-half hour sessions. There will be one session a day, five days a week for eight weeks; sessions will be scheduled for the same time each day.

The first session will be the "learning phase" and the following 39 will be the "performance phase”.

Learning Phase. In the first session, you will learn general facts about the One-Hand TCK and how to type characters with it. You should proceed through this section at your own pace, follow directions on the computer screen carefully, and consult the experimenter when suggested by the computer screen and also if you have any questions or problems.

You will learn to type the 10 numbers and 8 mathematical symbols during this session. To help you learn these characters, quizzes will be given at times to test your knowledge. Please, keep in mind that you are expected to memorize how to type these characters, and to type them with not more than 3% errors.

Performance Phase. In each one-and-a-half hour sessions, you will be given text files to type which will increase your speed. Follow the instructions displayed on the computer screen. Please keep errors down at 3%, but - - go for speed!

Keep in mind that the purpose of this experiment is to determine the highest typing speed that you can attain on the One-Hand TCK. During the sessions, please work to increase your speed and give the fastest performance you can. It is expected that you will have a general trend to increase your keying speed over the 40 sessions.

Once a week you will be asked to rate your performance on a 0 to 9 scale. You will still finish the full 40 sessions regardless of your ratings, but it is important to the experimenter how you feel you are progressing.

The computer program will explain a great deal about the TCK, so wait until you have proceeded though the INTRODUCTION to the learning section before asking any detailed questions you might have. Consult the experimenter at any time if there are any questions or problems.

Initials

140

Page 154: AAlr - Virginia Tech

Appendix C

Informed Consent Form

141

Page 155: AAlr - Virginia Tech

CONSENT FORM

l, am participating in this research study because | want to participate. The decision to participate is completely voluntarily on my part. No one has coerced or intimidated me to participate.

The experimenters have adequately answered any and all questions | have asked about this study, my participation, and the procedures involved, which are described in the attached "EXPERIMENT INSTRUCTIONS," which | have initialed.

| recognize the research team as Mark McMulkin, Graduate Research Assistant (231-5359) and Dr. K.H.E. Kroemer, Principal Investigator (231-5677).

| understand that they will be available to answer any questions concerning procedures throughout this study. | understand that if significant new findings develop during the course of this research which may relate to my decision to continue participation, | will be informed. | further understand that | may withdraw this consent at any time and discontinue further participation in this study without prejudice to my entitlements. | also understand that the Principal Investigator, his assistants, or medical consultants for this study may terminate my participation in this study if he or she feels this to be in my best interest. | may be required to undergo certain further examinations, if they are necessary for my health or well being.

| do not have any disorders of my cardiovascular system, of my spinal column (particularly in the lower back), or any other disorders or deficiencies, which make it unadvisable for me to participate in this experiment.

| understand that in the case of physical injury, no medical treatment or compensation are offered under the research program, of by VA Tech-VPI.

| understand that | shall receive payment in the amount of $5 per hour plus a $50 bonus for showing a general trend of improvement in keying speed over all 40 sessions. However, | further understand that if | withdraw from the experiment before it is completed, | will be paid only for the time | actually spent performing in the experiment at $5 per hour. If for some reason the equipment used for the experiment malfunctions before the completion of the experiment, | understand that | will only be paid for the time | actually spent performing in the experiment at $5 per hour.

| understand that the results of my efforts will be recorded and that | may be photographed, filmed, or audio/videotaped. | consent to the use of this information for scientific or training purposes, and | understand that any records of my participation in this study may be disclosed only according to federal law, including the Federal Privacy Act, and its implementing regulations. This

142

Page 156: AAlr - Virginia Tech

means that personal information will not be released to an unauthorized third party without my permission. |

| understand that if | have any further questions about my rights as a participant, | may contact Dr. Ernest R. Stoudt, Chairman of the Institutional Review Board at VPI&SU, at 231-5281.

| FULLY UNDERSTAND THAT | AM MAKING A DECISION WHETHER OR NOT TO PARTICIPATE. MY SIGNATURE INDICATES THAT | HAVE DECIDED TO PARTICIPATE UNDER THE CONDITIONS DESCRIBED ABOVE.

Signature Date

Printed Name SS Number

Address Phone Number

Industrial Ergonomics Lab ISE Department VPI&SU Blacksburg, VA 24061

Protocol ERGLAB 1987 Date: Jan. 1987, renewed February 12, 1989 for two

143

Page 157: AAlr - Virginia Tech

Appendix D

Derivation of CPM; = C+T|B1

144

Page 158: AAlr - Virginia Tech

Derivation of the Log-Log function

Log(CPM)) = Bo + ByLn(T})

CPM, = eBo + BiLn(Ti)

CPM; = eBo eBiLn{T))

CPM; = eBo eb n(TiP1)

CPM; = eBo 7,81

Since eBo is constant let C = eBo

CPM; = C+T;1

145

Page 159: AAlr - Virginia Tech

Appendix E

Graphs of All Time Increments and Subjects for CPM; = Bp + ByP; + Bo/Pj +

Bs/P2 and CPM; = eBo P;B

146

Page 160: AAlr - Virginia Tech

LavieQll.dve

ovd/edtd/ldtd.d

lenpy

POLO, INU]

O€ 40}

uoIssasHay ‘|

yoalqns *1°9

eunBbi4

Poa

aINUIW OF

02 S9

09 SG

OS Sr

Ov Se

OF Sz

OZ SI

OL

|

| _ {

{ |__|

| |

| |

{ |

| I

t 1

1 I

| }

1 i

] !

0c

OV

09

08

OOL

Ocl

OVI

O91

O8l

002

elnuily Jed suajoeeyy

147

Page 161: AAlr - Virginia Tech

Lavibul,. ave

ovd/cdtd/ldtd.d

lenjpy

GS

POUedY a}NUI

Op 40}

UoIssesBayY ‘|

Joalqns °2z°9

aunbi4

OS

Poudd

aINUIW OF

Sb Ob

GSE O&

Ge O06

Gt _

Ol S

0

Ov

09

08

001

0c}

OVI

091

O8l

002

o]nulyy sed syajoeseyyg

148

Page 162: AAlr - Virginia Tech

POS

S}NuUIPy OS

40} uoISsaiHay

‘| yoolqns

‘Ey eunbi

POHSd aiNuUIl|

OS

GY Ov

GE O€

Ge 0¢

Sl Ol

S 0

| |

| |

| ]

l |—

| +

0 Ov

09

LavibQll,dyve ———-—~—

08

ovd/cdtd/l dtd.

--------- 001

jenjoy Ocl

SINUIW Jed syajoeeYyD

Or |

O91

+ O81

—— 002

149

Page 163: AAlr - Virginia Tech

bavielt .ave

ovd/cdtd/tdtd.d

Jenjov

[EAUalU] AINUI|

O9 40)

SuoisseuBay ‘|

yalqns ‘py

aunbi4

POHad aINUIW

O9

OV Ge

0€ Se

0¢ Sl

Ol S

| |

—__| |

L {

| |

I 1

| {

|

+

0c

OV

09

08

00}

0c |

e]nul Jed suajoeieyD

Ovl

O91

O81

00¢

150

Page 164: AAlr - Virginia Tech

jeAsaju| Aeq

10} uoissaiBHay

‘1 yoelaqns

*s°y eunbi4

Keq

OV Ge

0€ Ge

02 Gl

Ol G

0

| |

| |

| |

{ |

I t

I |

i |

| |

0

LavAed,dve ——-———

cevAeq/zqtAeq/LatAed,d ---------

+ O01

leny +

Ob

alnulj Jad syejoeieyy

7 OVt

+ O91

7 O8t

—- 00¢

151

Page 165: AAlr - Virginia Tech

bavibl1, ave

ovd/edtd/l dtd.

jenpy

OZ

POU,

SINUIW] OE

40} uoISsesBeyY

‘Zz yoalqns

-9°9 aunbi4

POuad SyNuUI

OF

S9 09

GS OS

Sh OF

GE OF

Ge 02

StL OF

GS O

| |

| {st

| {

| |

| |

{

| i

! ]

J |

I |

{ I

| 1

I

0c

Ov

09

08

OOt

Oct

OVI

O9t

O8l

00¢

e}Null\ 49d sugjOeBeYyy

152

Page 166: AAlr - Virginia Tech

bavieHl.dve

ovd/edtd/|atd.d

lenjoy

GS

POudY s}NUI-

OP 10}

uoIsseuBay ‘2

yalqns “7°

eunbi4

POUdd aINUIN

OF

0S Sb

Ov Sse

O0& Ge

O06 Gt

Ol S

| |

| |

jt |

| |

| {

I |

I |

| |

T |

1 1

OV

09

08

00}

Ocl

Ovi

O91

081

00¢

anu Jad syajoeseuy

153

Page 167: AAlr - Virginia Tech

POMS

a}NUIW OG

40} uoIssasHayY

‘Zz elgns

-g°y eunbi4

poued aINUIW!

OS

Sy CO Oh:Ssi‘<i«sSC“‘<«té‘i:SC“‘wSSC‘iSCC(‘éiR(SSCSG

0 |

| l

a |

| |

i 0 09

LdvieWil.dve8 —————

08

ovd/cdtd/ldtd.d ---------

OO}

jenjoy Oc}

Ori

O91

081

— 00¢

154

ejnull sed syajoeeuy

Page 168: AAlr - Virginia Tech

bavieut.dve

ovd/cdtd/ldtd.d

jenjpoy

[EAIBIU] S}NUII|

OQ 410}

SUOISSasHay ‘zg

yoalqns ‘6°9

eunbi4

POUSd 9}NUI

09

OV GE

O& Se

0c Sl

Ol G

0

0c

OV

09

08

00l

Ocl

OV lL

091

O8t

00¢

eINUIW sed SuBjORIeUD

155

Page 169: AAlr - Virginia Tech

LavAeqd, dave

ovAeq/za+Aeq/1g+Aed,

fenjoy

jeAraju Aeg

40} uoissaiBay

‘g yelqns

‘o1°y sunbi4

Keq

Ov GE

O¢ Ge

0d Gl

Ol G

| |

| |

| {_

{ |

| }

| i

I |

Ov

09

08

001

Ocl

e]nuIw 16d suajoeeyyD

Ovl

O91

O8l

00¢

156

Page 170: AAlr - Virginia Tech

LaviGlt.dva

ovd/edt+d/tdatd.d

Jenjoy

DOO

SINUI

OF 10}

UoIssalBay ‘¢E

yalqns “1

1°9 eunbi4

poua aynuiW

O€

0Z S9

09 SG

0G Sb

Ov SE

O€ GZ

O02 St

OL ¢

O |

| |

| |

{|

| |

| |

i |

| |

1 |

j |

1 |

1 I

t |

| 1

1

Od

OV

09

08

001

0c}

Ovi

091

O8l

00¢

alnui sed syajoeueyy

157

Page 171: AAlr - Virginia Tech

POWdd aINUIW

OF 40}

UoIssesHay ‘E

yoelqns *z1°Q

eunbi4

Poued aINUIW

OF

S¢ 0S

Sr Or

Se of

Sze of

St OL

¢ oO

|

| |

| |

I 1

I IF

0 09

havieQHl.dve —————

08

évd/cdtd/ldtd.d

--------- 0Ol

yenjoy Oct

OF |

091

O8l

— 00¢

158

e]nul~ 4ad syejoeseyD

Page 172: AAlr - Virginia Tech

POUSAY aINUIP]

OG 404

UOIssalHaY ‘€

yelqns ‘e1°4

aunbig

pouad ainui~w

Og

Sp Ohsti‘<iésESC“<«ié‘iOE:C“‘sSSSC‘i

SC C:t‘éisék

S 0

|

| |

| |

| |

) 0 09

bdvieHl,dye —-—-———

08

Cvd/cdtd/ldtd.d ---------

00}

jenjoy Och

Ort

O91

O8 |

— 00¢

159

enul, 18d suapeueyyD

Page 173: AAlr - Virginia Tech

baviebll.dve

ovd/cdtd/|dtd.d

renyoy

jeAlaju] aINUIWY

OO 40}

SuOIssasBayY ‘E

yoaelqns ‘p1"g

eunbi

poued eINuIW

09

Op Ge

O€ GZ

02 Sh

Ol S

Ov

09

08

001

Ocl

orl

091

O81

00¢

einul~— Jed suajoeieuy

160

Page 174: AAlr - Virginia Tech

jeAlaju| Aeg

10} uoissaiHay

‘¢ yoalqns

“¢4°y aunbi4

Keg

OV Ge

O€ Se

02 Gt

Ol G

0

| —t

| |

| |

| 0

| |

| 4

| i

] |

T Od

09

LavAed,a@ye ————~—

08

evAeq/edtAeq/Lg+Aed,g ---------

001

jenpy

OZL

a

091

08}

—- 00¢

161

ainul Jed syajoeeuy

Page 175: AAlr - Virginia Tech

Laviel1l.dye

ovd/cdtd/tatd.d

lenjoy

POHdd ajNuIWy

O€ 10}

UoISSauBayY ‘p

yoalqns ‘91°9

aunbi4

pouad eInuIW

OF

02 G9

09 SG

0G Sr

Or SE

Of Gz

Of Gt

OL

0

02

OV

09

08

001

Ocl

Ovl

091

O8l

00¢

Slnull\ Jed syajpeweyy

162

Page 176: AAlr - Virginia Tech

POW,

BINUIW OF

10} UoISsalBayY

‘p yoelqns

"74°94 aunbi4

pouad aInuIW

OF

cS 0S

Gr Orv

se oe

G2 of

St oO

¢

O |

| 4

| |

| |

| ]

| |

0

OV

09

LdvieHl,dve —————

08

Cvd/cdtd/ld+d.d ---------

00}

jenjoy Och

alnuljy Jad srajoeseuy

OVI

+ O91

+ O8t

—- 002

163

Page 177: AAlr - Virginia Tech

baviell. ave

ovd/cdtd/l dtd.

lenjpy

Sv OV

POS

SINUIW] OG

JO) UdISsaiBay

‘p joalqns

-g1°5 eunbi4

pouad anu]

OS

sé o€

G2 02

st ors

0

0c

Ov

09

08

001

Ocl

Or |

O91

081

00¢

SINuIW 16d syejoeieUy

164

Page 178: AAlr - Virginia Tech

havieul.dve

ovd/cdtd/ld*d.d

lenjoy

|EAJ}U] SINUIW

OQ 40}

SUOIssaJBayY ‘p

yoelans “61°9

aunbi

POUdd a}NUlW

O9

OV Se

O€ Go

0¢ Sl

OL S

| {.

| I

1 i

1 \

| |

|

Oc

Ov

09

08

001

Ocl

Ovl

091

08 |

00¢

anu Jad sr9joeieyyD

165

Page 179: AAlr - Virginia Tech

LavAeqd, ave

evAeq/zatheq/1g+Aeq,g

enjoy

OV

GE O€

Sd

Aeq

0c Gl ! ft

jealajuy Aeg

40} uoissaiBay

‘p yoalqns

‘ogy eunbi4

Oe

OV

09

08

00!

Ocl

OV l

O9l

O8l

00¢

alnulw sad suajoeieyyD

166

Page 180: AAlr - Virginia Tech

bavi. ave

ovd/cdtd/ld+d.d

fenjpy

POUdd aINuUIP

OE 10}

UOISsalHayY ‘s

oalqns *12°9

eunbi4

POLS, 9INUI|

OF

OL S9

09 GS

OS Sb

OF SE

OF G2

O02 Gt

OL G

O

| |

| I

{ |

| |

| |

| {

{ {

I t

I {

1 {

i |

I j

| |

I i

0c

OV

09

08

00!

Oc |

Ovl

091

08}

002

eynully Jed suajoeueyy

167

Page 181: AAlr - Virginia Tech

bavibl lave

ovd/cdtd/ldtd.d

enjoy

POS

SINUIW Ov

40} uoIssalBaY

‘sg yalqns

-zg"yQ eunbi4

OS Gv

OV

POWdd 9INUIW

OV

Ge SE

0€ 0c | __

St | {——

Ol

OV

09

08

OOL

Ocl

Ovi

O91

O8l

00¢

enully 18d susjoeeYyyD

168

Page 182: AAlr - Virginia Tech

baviGW1.dve

ovd/cdt+d/ldtd.d

lenjoy

POUAd SINUIW]

OS 40}

UdISsalBaYy ‘G

yoalqns ‘Eez’y

eunbi4

SY OV

+

GE

pousd einuiy

OS

06 Gz

oc St

or gs

0

| |

| |

1 |

\ I

J I

I

0c

OV

09

08

O0Ol

Ocl

Ort

O91

081

002

elnul~-, Jad syajoeseyy

169

Page 183: AAlr - Virginia Tech

[eAJa}U] SINUI,

OY 40};

SuoIssaiBayY ‘sg

yoalqns “pz°y

eunbi4

pousd elnuiyy

09

OP ce

o¢€ Sz

02 SI

Ol c

0

, |

| |

l l

L |

| 0

LavilGWl,dva

—_-——

Cvd/cdtd/ldtd.d

--------- 7

OO-

lenjoy +

OZt

+ OvVl

+ O91

+ O8t

— 00¢

170

ejnul sed susjoeeYyD

Page 184: AAlr - Virginia Tech

jeAraju Aeg

10} uoissaiBay

‘g yoalqns

°sz°y aunbi4

Keg

OV Ge

O€ Ge

0¢ GI

Ol S

0

LavAed,@ve —————

ovAeq/zat+Aeqg/lgt+Aed,g ---------

+ OOL

Gans

+ Oct

+ Ov

7 O9t

+ O81

— 002

171

einuljy Jed syajoeeyy

Page 185: AAlr - Virginia Tech

Appendix F

Average Response Times of Each Chord by Trial

172

Page 186: AAlr - Virginia Tech

EL puoyy

seul Aq

ow) asuodsay ebesaay

“1°44 ounbi4

JaquUNN jeu

00S OST

OO0r OSE

O00€ 0S¢

00¢ OSI

001 OS

0

[| |

| |

| |

| |

| )

t |

| |

| I

I |

I

+ O01

+ Q0¢

7 OO€&

+ O00

+ 00S

+ 009

+ O0OZL

+ 008

+ 006

- OOOL

- OOLL

- O0cl

+- OO€1

+ OOF1

+ OOSI

+ 0091

+ OOZI

7 0081

+ OO6L

+ 000¢

+ OOLe

— 00¢¢

173

(DeSswW ul) awl] esuodsey abeusay

Page 187: AAlr - Virginia Tech

00S OSL

OOP OSE

OO0€

Pl Puoyy

‘yeu Aq

euly asuodsay ebelaay

‘2°'4 eunbi4

JEQUINN jeuL

0S¢ |

00¢ | I

OSt { i

001 | I

0S | l

001

00¢

OO0€

OOF

00S

009

002

008

006

0001

OO-L

00¢1

OO€L

0O0r |

00S |

0091

OOZ 1

0081

0061

000¢

0012

00¢¢

(Oesw ul) aul, asuOodsey sbeusay

174

Page 188: AAlr - Virginia Tech

00S OSP

007 OSE

00€ | |

G| puoyy

:yeu Aq

eu, asuodsay

ebesany ‘¢'4

ounbi4

JEqUINN Jeu,

0S¢ | |

00¢ OS|

0Ol 0S

OOL

00¢

OO0€&

00v

00S

009

002

008

006

0001

OOLL

00c1

OOE |

00V |

00S1

0091

0021

0081

0061

000¢

00Le

00c¢

(O@sw ul) euly esuodsey obesoAy

175

Page 189: AAlr - Virginia Tech

00S OSt

00r OSE

O00€

Q| puoyy

seus AQ

auly asuodsay

ebeleny “p’4

aunbi4

JEquUNN jeuL

0S¢ | I

00¢ OSI | |

001 { I

OS {| I

OOl

00¢

OO&

00V

00S

009

002

008

006

0001

OOLt

001

00€ |

007+ |

00SI

0091

0021

008 |

0061

000¢

0Ole

00¢e¢

(Oesw ul) sully esuodsey abeisay

176

Page 190: AAlr - Virginia Tech

00S OST

0O0V OSE

00€

Z| puoyy

‘yeuy Aq

awl, asuodsay ebeaay

*s’4 aunbi4

JEQuINN jeu lL

0S¢ | i

00¢ | T

OS 1 | |

OOL 0S

001

00¢

00€

00V

00S

009

002

008

006

0001

OOL |

00c1

OO€ 1

007 |

OOS 1

009 |

OOZ1

0081

0061

0002

0Ol¢e

00¢¢

(Oesw ul) euly asuOdsay sbeusay

177

Page 191: AAlr - Virginia Tech

00S OSt

00v OSE

O0€ | i

Eg puoyy

‘jeu Aq

awl, asuodsay

ebessay -9°+4

aunbi4

JEqUINN jeu

0S¢ 00¢

0S |

001 0S | |

001

00¢

00€

007

00S

009

002

008

006

0001

0011

00¢ |

00€ |

00r |

00S 1

0091

OOZ1

OO08t

0061

000¢

001d

00c¢e

(osu ul) Swi, BsuOdseY eHbeuaay

178

Page 192: AAlr - Virginia Tech

00S OSP +

00v OSE

O0€

pz psouy

:jeuL Aq

euiy asuodsay

sbeyany °7°4

aunbiy

JOQUINN jeuL

0S¢ | |

00¢ OSI

O0Ol OS

OOl

00¢

O0O0€

007

00S

009

002

008

006

0001

001}

00¢ |

OOE |

0O0r |

00S |

0091

OO |

008 |

0061

0002

0012

00¢¢

(O@sw ul) swly asuodsay aBeusay

179

Page 193: AAlr - Virginia Tech

00S OST

OOP OGE

00€

Gz puoyuy

yeu

Aq awiy

asuodsay ebesaay

“g°+4 aunbi-4

JOQUNN jeu]

0S¢ | i

00¢ OS| | ]

001 | I

OS

001

00¢

00€

00V

00S

009

002

008

006

0001

0011

00c1

OOEL

OOVl

00SI

0091

OOZI

0081

0061

000¢

0Ole

00c¢

(O@sw ul) euuly eBsuOdsey sHbeieAy

180

Page 194: AAlr - Virginia Tech

00S OS?

OOP OSE

OO0€

{ |

QZ ps0yy

‘yeu Aq

owl asuodsay ebeuaay

*6°4 aunbi4

JOquny seul

0S¢ 00¢ | |

OSI | |

OOL { |

0S

001

00¢

O0€

00V

00S

009

002

008

006

0001

OOLL

00cl

OOEL

Or

00S |

0091

OOZ1

0081

0061

000¢

0OLe

00é¢

(Oasw ul) dw] ssuOdseYy ebessaay

181

Page 195: AAlr - Virginia Tech

00S OS?

00P OSE

00€ | |

8g PuoUy

:jeuy Aq

aul) asuodsay sbeisay

‘O|'+ aunbi4

JOquNN |e

0S¢ | I

00¢ |

OSI OO}

OS

OOL

00¢

O0O0€

00V

00S

009

002

008

006

0001

OOL |

00cl

OO0€ L

0Ovl

00S1

0091

0021

0081

0061

000¢

0O0Le2

00¢c2

(OesSw ul) eluly eBSUOdseY BbeisAy

182

Page 196: AAlr - Virginia Tech

GE puoyy

:jeuL Aq

oul asuodsey sHesany

*11°4 aunbis

Jaquinn jeu

00S OST

00” OSE

OO0€ 0S¢

00¢ OSI

O0Ol OS

0

| |

| |

| |

| 4

| 0

+ 001

+ 002

+ 00€

+ 00¢

+ 00S

+ 009

+ 004

+ 008

- 006 OOO}

i OO}

+ 001

+ O0€1

+ 00v1

+ 00S}

+ 0091

+ OOLI

+ 0081

+ 0061

+ 0002

+ 0012

+ 00z2

183

(Oesw ul) sul) asuodsey sbeisAy

Page 197: AAlr - Virginia Tech

00S OSV

00r OSE

00€

9€ POY

jeu, AQ

owt, esuodsey ebeyaAy

‘Z1°4 eunbi4

JOqUINN [eu

0S¢ 00¢ | |

OSI 001

00}

00¢

00€

007

00S

009

002

008

006

OO00L

OOl |

00cl

00€ |

007 |

00S1

0091

OOZ1

0081

0061

000¢

00l¢e

00¢¢

(Desw ul) ewuly eSuodseyY ebeieay

184

Page 198: AAlr - Virginia Tech

00S + OSP

OO0V OSE

00€ { |

Z€ puouy

yeuy Aq

ewiy asuodsey sbeseny

“eE1°4 eunbi4

JOQUINN JeuL

0S¢e

| I

00¢ OS |

OOL OS

OOl

00¢

00€

OOP

00S

009

002

008

006

0001

OOl}

00ct

OOEl

OOrl

00S |

0091

OOZL

0081

OO6l

000¢

001¢e

00¢¢d

(Oesw ul) awl, esuodsey sBbeusay

185

Page 199: AAlr - Virginia Tech

00S OSt

0O0r OSE

O00€

Gp Puouy

:jeuy Aq

euly esuodsay

ebeieay ‘p1'4

aunbi

JOQuUNN jeu,

OSe {| l

00¢ OS}

001 | —|—

OS | t—

O01

00¢

OO0&

00v

00S

009

002

008

006

000t

OOl tL

00cl

OOEL

OOF |

00S I

0091

OOZ1

008 |

0061

000¢

O0l¢e

00e¢

(DeSuw ul) eu] easuodsey eBbeieay

186

Page 200: AAlr - Virginia Tech

00S OST

OOV OSE

OO0&

9p puoyy

‘jeu, Aq

ewly asuodsay ebeiaay

“s1°4 aunbi4

JOQUINN jeu

0S¢ 00¢

OS | 001

OS | | : i

001

00¢

O00€

OOP

00S

009

002

008

006

0001

OO} |

00c |

O00 |

OOr

00S |

0091

OOZI

0081

0061

000¢

0OLe

00c¢

(Dasw ul) sul] esuodsey ebeusay

187

Page 201: AAlr - Virginia Tech

00S OS

OOr OSE

O0€ | |

8p Psoyy

:jeu, Aq

aul asuodsay

ebeuany ‘91°44

eunbi4

JEQuNN [el

0Ge 002 |

OS | | {

OOL OS

001

00¢

OO0€

007

00S

009

002

008

006

000l

OOl |

00d}

O0€ |

00r I

00S |

0091

OOZI

008 t

0061

000¢

001d

00¢¢

(oasw ul) eu, esuodsey sBessAy

188

Page 202: AAlr - Virginia Tech

00S OSV

00V OSE

00€ | |

ZG psouy

:jeuy Aq

au) asuodsay

ebesny

*/}°4 eunbi

JequnN jeuL

OS¢ { i

00¢ OS 1

OOL

002

00&

007

00S

009

002

008

006

0001

OOLL

00cl

OOE L

ooVl

OOSt

0091

OOZI

O08 t

OO6I

000¢

00l¢e

00c¢

(Oaswi ul) sul, esuodsaey aBbeisay

189

Page 203: AAlr - Virginia Tech

89 POU

‘yeu Aq

euly asuodsay sbeieay

“g}°4 eunbi-y

Jaqunn

seul

00S OSY

OOP OSE

OO0€ 0S¢

00¢ OSI

00} OS

0

| |

| |

| |

| 0

| i

| |

i |

| ]

| I

OOL

7 00¢

+ OO0€

+ OOD

+ 00S

+ 009

+ 002

+ 008

+ 006

- O00}

r OOLL

- O0Zc!

7 OOEl

qt O01

y OOS1

y~ OO9L

+ OOZt

+ 0081

+ OO6I

+ 000¢

+ O0l2

—— 00¢¢

190

(DeSwW ul) su] esuodsey eHbeusay

Page 204: AAlr - Virginia Tech

VITA

Mark Lee McMulkin was born on May 17, 1966 in Great Bend, Kansas.

He received a B.S. degree in Mechanical Engineering from the University of

Idaho in May 1989. While pursuing his Master Degree at Virginia Polytechnic

Institute and State University (Virginia Tech), he worked as a Graduate

Research Assistant for two years performing research on the Ternary Chord

Keyboard and finger speed. He is a member of Tau Beta Pi and the Human

Factors Society. He will be working on his Doctoral Degree in Human Factors

at Virginia Tech.

Muck. Mb Mark L. McMulkin

191