Wink of an Eye

2
T he bird brain, though much maligned, can perform feats while sleeping of which we can only dreamnamely, it can stay awake. Although it’s long been known that birds can sleep with one eye open, researchers at Indiana State University have determined that ducks can care- fully choreograph sleeping and waking states simultaneously in different re- gions of the brain. Earlier studies had shown that birds achieve these two states of conscious- ness at once by splitting the tasks be- tween the brain: one hemisphere falls asleep while the other stays awake and responsive. This half-brain, or unihemi- spheric, sleeping manifests itself as something like a prolonged wink; the eye connected to the wakeful half of the brain remains open, whereas that wired to the dozing half droops. The coexistence of two states of con- sciousness is impressive enough, “but the kicker,” Indiana State researcher Niels Rattenborg says, “is the control.” Because the hemispheres of birds’ brains process some information inde- pendently while awake, “it was con- ceivable that unihemispheric sleeping simply occurred when the hemispheres fell asleep or awakened out of phase by chance,” explains Charles Amlaner, the sleep specialist of the team. To rule out this possibility, the inves- tigators set about finding a situation in which ducks would want control over their sleep. They knew that some aquat- ic mammalsthe only other group be- sides birds that displays unihemispheric sleepkeep half of the brain awake to ensure that they keep one flipper pad- dling and so don’t drown while sleep- ing at sea. Sleeping in the water is ex- tremely specialized, however, and be- cause almost all birds demonstrate unihemispheric sleeping, the impetus behind the evolution of this behavior had to be a more common threatnamely, predation. Much of animal be- havior “is influenced by the great risk of being killed,” notes team member Steven Lima. The researchers simulated a risky sleep situation by lining up four ducks in a row. Whereas the two ducks in the central positions could presumably feel buffered by their neighbors from threats, those situated on either end were more likely to feel vulnerable to attack on their exposed sides. “Animals in general perceive more risk at the edge of a group,” Rattenborg explainsa News and Analysis 26 Scientific American May 1999 world and represents what is termed a mature epidemic. According to some es- timates, up to one third of the women in the main trading centers are HIV-posi- tive, and the infection rate for the gener- al community is 16 percent. In contrast, Mwanza had a community HIV preva- lence of only 4 percent and in 1992 was still experiencing a relatively early HIV epidemic. The presence of STDs may have a far greater impact on HIV trans- mission during the early stages of an epidemic, rather than one where the virus is already well established. An additional factor may be Rakai’s baseline rates of resistant and untreat- able STDs. Of reported genital ulcers, a large percentage tested positive for her- pes, which is incurable. Bacterial vagi- nosis, a difficult-to-treat condition that has been linked to increased risk for HIV infection, existed in 50 percent of the population at the beginning of the study. Though observed in East Africa, it was not reported in the Mwanza study. A third factor may be the treatment approach at Rakai. Periodic mass inter- vention did not specifically target symp- tomatic STDs, as was the case in Mwan- za. Wawer hypothesizes that at the popu- lation level, symptomatic STDs may have a greater effect on HIV transmission. Data from Rakai, however, suggest that a sizable portion of symptomatic cases was not associated with a treatable STD. “The whole issue of STD control for HIV prevention is, unfortunately, a lot more complex than we hoped,” Wawer says, adding that there is a great need to do more research into different popula- tions with different STD and HIV back- grounds. There is one more communi- ty-based, randomized study currently in place in Africa, and investigators hope to present their results sometime next year. Roxanne Nelson ROXANNE NELSON, based in Seattle, described bloodless insulin mon- itors in the October 1998 issue. Y2Hot Global warming has topped itself again: 1998 has set the record for the past mil- lennium. As reported in the March 15 Geophysical Research Letters, Malcolm Hughes of the University of Arizona and his colleagues analyzed tree rings and ice cores to reconstruct temperature trends over the past 10 centuries. Most alarming is the past century’s abrupt re- versal of a 1,000-year-long cooling trend, culminating in the 1990s being the warmest decade yet. One result of the warmth: early spring activity. Univer- sity of Munich researchers note in the February 25 Nature that spring blooms now take place six days earlier than in the 1960s and that the growing season lasts nearly 11 days longer. —J.N. An Arm and a Leg The cost of an arm and a leg may be high, but the two limbs are eerily inter- changeable, as reported in the March 12 Science. Harvard University researchers made leg structures grow where a wing should have been. Three genes are in- volved in limb development, Pitx1, Tbx4 and Tbx5. Moving active Pitx1 from the leg bud to the wing bud, where the gene is normally in- active, began leg formation there. Wings did not com- plete their new path toward legginess, but they did be- come distinctly leg- like: feathers were lost, muscle structure changed, and digits and claws appeared (photograph). J.N. Temper, Temper David J. Green of Pennsylvania State Uni- versity and his colleagues have devised a way for tempered glass to crack without shattering. Tempering strengthens glass by compressing the molecules of the outer surface; if that surface cracks, the pent-up stress is released, exploding the rest of the glass. The researchers de- scribe in Science a chemical method to make deeper layers in the glass denser by substituting sodium atoms with larg- er potassium atoms. Cracks could appear on the less dense outer layer but fail to reach any deeper. The method could lead to thinner glasses for scanners, pho- tocopy machines and displays. —P.Y. In Brief, continued from page 24 SA Extra-leggy chicken REPRINTED FROM SCIENCE WINK OF AN EYE Half-asleep ducks can control which hemisphere gets to snooze BEHAVIOR Copyright 1999 Scientific American, Inc.

Transcript of Wink of an Eye

Page 1: Wink of an Eye

The bird brain, though muchmaligned, can perform featswhile sleeping of which we

can only dream—namely, it can stayawake. Although it’s long been knownthat birds can sleep with one eye open,researchers at Indiana State Universityhave determined that ducks can care-fully choreograph sleeping and wakingstates simultaneously in different re-gions of the brain.

Earlier studies had shown that birdsachieve these two states of conscious-ness at once by splitting the tasks be-tween the brain: one hemisphere fallsasleep while the other stays awake andresponsive. This half-brain, or unihemi-spheric, sleeping manifests itself assomething like a prolonged wink; theeye connected to the wakeful half of thebrain remains open, whereas that wiredto the dozing half droops.

The coexistence of two states of con-sciousness is impressive enough, “butthe kicker,” Indiana State researcherNiels Rattenborg says, “is the control.”Because the hemispheres of birds’

brains process some information inde-pendently while awake, “it was con-ceivable that unihemispheric sleepingsimply occurred when the hemispheresfell asleep or awakened out of phase bychance,” explains Charles Amlaner, thesleep specialist of the team.

To rule out this possibility, the inves-tigators set about finding a situation inwhich ducks would want control overtheir sleep. They knew that some aquat-ic mammals—the only other group be-sides birds that displays unihemisphericsleep—keep half of the brain awake toensure that they keep one flipper pad-dling and so don’t drown while sleep-ing at sea. Sleeping in the water is ex-tremely specialized, however, and be-cause almost all birds demonstrateunihemispheric sleeping, the impetusbehind the evolution of this behaviorhad to be a more common threat—namely, predation. Much of animal be-havior “is influenced by the great riskof being killed,” notes team memberSteven Lima.

The researchers simulated a riskysleep situation by lining up four ducksin a row. Whereas the two ducks in thecentral positions could presumably feelbuffered by their neighbors fromthreats, those situated on either endwere more likely to feel vulnerable toattack on their exposed sides. “Animalsin general perceive more risk at the edgeof a group,” Rattenborg explains—a

News and Analysis26 Scientific American May 1999

world and represents what is termed amature epidemic. According to some es-timates, up to one third of the women inthe main trading centers are HIV-posi-tive, and the infection rate for the gener-al community is 16 percent. In contrast,Mwanza had a community HIV preva-lence of only 4 percent and in 1992 wasstill experiencing a relatively early HIVepidemic. The presence of STDs mayhave a far greater impact on HIV trans-mission during the early stages of anepidemic, rather than one where thevirus is already well established.

An additional factor may be Rakai’sbaseline rates of resistant and untreat-able STDs. Of reported genital ulcers, alarge percentage tested positive for her-pes, which is incurable. Bacterial vagi-nosis, a difficult-to-treat condition thathas been linked to increased risk forHIV infection, existed in 50 percent ofthe population at the beginning of thestudy. Though observed in East Africa, itwas not reported in the Mwanza study.

A third factor may be the treatmentapproach at Rakai. Periodic mass inter-vention did not specifically target symp-tomatic STDs, as was the case in Mwan-za. Wawer hypothesizes that at the popu-lation level, symptomatic STDs may havea greater effect on HIV transmission.Data from Rakai, however, suggest that asizable portion of symptomatic cases wasnot associated with a treatable STD.

“The whole issue of STD control forHIV prevention is, unfortunately, a lotmore complex than we hoped,” Wawersays, adding that there is a great need todo more research into different popula-tions with different STD and HIV back-grounds. There is one more communi-ty-based, randomized study currently inplace in Africa, and investigators hopeto present their results sometime nextyear. —Roxanne Nelson

ROXANNE NELSON, based inSeattle, described bloodless insulin mon-itors in the October 1998 issue.

Y2HotGlobal warming has topped itself again:1998 has set the record for the past mil-lennium. As reported in the March 15Geophysical Research Letters, MalcolmHughes of the University of Arizona andhis colleagues analyzed tree rings andice cores to reconstruct temperaturetrends over the past 10 centuries. Mostalarming is the past century’s abrupt re-versal of a 1,000-year-long coolingtrend, culminating in the 1990s beingthe warmest decade yet. One result ofthe warmth: early spring activity. Univer-sity of Munich researchers note in theFebruary 25 Nature that spring bloomsnow take place six days earlier than inthe 1960s and that the growing seasonlasts nearly 11 days longer. —J.N.

An Arm and a LegThe cost of an arm and a leg may behigh, but the two limbs are eerily inter-changeable, as reported in the March 12Science. Harvard University researchersmade leg structures grow where a wingshould have been. Three genes are in-volved in limb development, Pitx1, Tbx4

and Tbx5. Movingactive Pitx1 from theleg bud to the wingbud, where thegene is normally in-active, began legformation there.Wings did not com-plete their new pathtoward legginess,but they did be-come distinctly leg-

like: feathers were lost, muscle structurechanged, and digits and claws appeared(photograph). —J.N.

Temper, TemperDavid J. Green of Pennsylvania State Uni-versity and his colleagues have devised away for tempered glass to crack withoutshattering. Tempering strengthens glassby compressing the molecules of theouter surface; if that surface cracks, thepent-up stress is released, exploding therest of the glass. The researchers de-scribe in Science a chemical method tomake deeper layers in the glass denserby substituting sodium atoms with larg-er potassium atoms. Cracks could appearon the less dense outer layer but fail toreach any deeper. The method couldlead to thinner glasses for scanners, pho-tocopy machines and displays. —P.Y.

In Brief, continued from page 24

SA

Extra-leggy chicken

REPR

INTE

D F

RO

M S

CIE

NC

E

WINK OF AN EYE

Half-asleep ducks can control which hemisphere gets to snooze

BEHAVIOR

Copyright 1999 Scientific American, Inc.

Page 2: Wink of an Eye

The long search for extraterres-trial intelligence has foundnone, but it has revealed some-

thing interesting about a certain formof terrestrial intelligence: namely, thatwhen scientists get on to an idea theybelieve is truly important, somethingthat could shake civilization to its cen-ter, failure no longer discourages—it mo-tivates. Some 30 years of listening todeep-space radio waves is culminating

with the conclusion next year of ProjectPhoenix, the best-funded, most ambi-tious search yet for signals from the1,000 neighboring star systems mostlikely to harbor life. There is not a sin-gle repeatable observation of an artifi-cial radio signal to show for all that ef-fort. Yet the SETI community is ener-gized, buzzing with new plans, includingone to build a giant telescope array de-voted to the search.

This is not the first time that the faith-ful have bounced back from adversity.Indeed, Project Phoenix rose from theashes of a National Aeronautics andSpace Administration program that wasterminated by Congress after less thantwo years. Phoenix was funded by theSETI Institute in Mountain View, Calif.,and backed by several computer indus-

try tycoons, yet it had to beg time onmajor radio observatories. “We get 10days a year at Arecibo in Puerto Rico;that’s not much,” complains William“Jack” Welch, who holds an endowedchair for SETI that the University ofCalifornia at Berkeley created in June,suggesting a growing acceptance of thefield among mainstream astronomers.

That and perhaps the rapidly accumu-lating resources of its billionaire donorshave emboldened the SETI Institute todesign a 10,000-square-meter radio an-tenna—the One Hectare Telescope, or1HT—whose primary mission would beto search for aliens. “The idea is to buildan instrument that would have as muchcollecting area as the new [government-owned] 100-meter dish at Green Bank,W.Va., but cost only a third of its $75-million price,” Welch explains.

To save money, the 1HT would beconstructed from mass-produced satel-lite TV dishes, 500 to 1,000 of themspread across the countryside in north-ern California. To amplify the signalsreceived in a huge spectrum from 300megahertz to perhaps 10 gigahertz, ev-ery antenna would have a custom-de-signed microchip, no bigger than a sug-ar cube and costing only a few hundreddollars, Welch says. Filtering that riotof signals to pick out a single, all-im-portant anomalous spike will take seri-ous computing power. Welch concedesthat the processing is “an order of mag-nitude more challenging than what’sbeen done before.”

Although the 1HT would be used pri-marily for SETI surveys, Welch pointsout that it could also serve as a criticalstepping stone for radio astronomerswho hope to construct a mammoth in-strument with 100 times the collectingarea of the 1HT, at a cost of perhaps$600 million. The Chinese are advocat-ing up to 30 Arecibo-like dishes erectedin karst depressions in China. Australiansprefer arrays of flat panels in the out-back. But Welch argues that a few thou-sand consumer satellite dishes might per-form just as well at half the price.

Even as the traditionalist alien huntersplan to sift once more through the ra-dio spectrum with ever finer combs,several high-profile astronomers are be-ginning to look for optical beacons. Inone analysis, Harvard University physi-cist Paul Horowitz estimated that the$1-billion Helios laser being planned atLawrence Livermore National Labora-tory could send nanosecond-long pulsesthat would appear 3,000 times brighter

News and Analysis28 Scientific American May 1999

CH

ARL

ES A

MLA

NER

Indi

ana

Stat

e U

nive

rsit

y

FIELD OF DREAMS

Undeterred by failure, SETI researchers plan to build a telescope of their own

EXTRATERRESTRIAL INTELLIGENCE

phenomenon known as the edge effect.The ducks on the edge spent 2.5

times longer in unihemispheric sleepthan ducks in the central spots did.They also directed their open eye to-ward the unprotected flank during 86percent of their unihemispheric sleepingtime, whereas the central ducks indicat-ed no preference for which eye theykept open. Some edge ducks even didthis the entire time, periodically turningaround to switch eyes. Half-sleepingducks shown a video image simulatingan approaching predator roused in-stantly, indicating that the open eye wasalert. Far from being an accidental con-sequence of their sleepy state, theducks’ half-sleeping behavior demon-strated that they can direct the alerthemisphere and eye to stand guard.

But if unihemispheric sleeping is sovaluable for detecting predators’ at-tacks, why don’t more animals do it?The answer seems to be that they losttheir chance a long time ago. ChristianMathews, working in Amlaner’s lab,

recently found that lizards also sleepwith one eye open, especially after theyhave seen a predator. This behaviormay not technically qualify as unihemi-spheric sleep, because lizards’ sleepingbrain waves differ in general from thoseof birds and mammals. But the fact thatlizards have a similar behavior out-wardly indicates that this sleeping statemay have been shared by an early com-mon ancestor.

Early mammals probably spent muchof their day sleeping safely in their bur-rows, Amlaner reasons. As birds refinedvigilant sleeping, early mammals mayhave lost much of the ability, only rede-veloping unihemispheric sleep under themost extreme evolutionary circum-stances, such as becoming aquatic. In-terestingly, Rattenborg notes, humanswho have undergone a severe traumadisplay sleeping brain-wave patternsreminiscent of those of birds in unihemi-spheric sleep. Perhaps some very oldpart of their brain is telling them to keepan eye open for danger. —Jessa Netting

DUCKS AT THE ENDSsleep with one eye open(inset; eyelid is white)

more so than the twomiddle ducks, whichpresumably feel safer.

JOE

DIC

KSO

N

Copyright 1999 Scientific American, Inc.