This content downloaded from 75.166.117.246 on …...If the protective cases of H. borealis larvae...

3
Evolution, 33(1), 1979, pp. 511-513 NOTES AND COMMENTS CONVERGENT EVOLUTION BETWEEN PHYLA: A TEST CASE OF MIMICRY BETWEEN CADDISFLY LARVAE (HELICOPSYCHE BOREALIS) AND AQUATIC SNAILS (PHYSA INTEGRA) JOEL BERGER1 AND JERRY KASTER2 Department of Environmental, Population, and Organismic Biology, University of Colorado, Boulder, Colorado 80309 Received May 5, 1978. Revised July 19, 1978 Many cases of potential mimicry are reported sim- ply because the species in question are quite similar in external appearance. However, neither data nor testable hypotheses are advanced to determine if mimicry actually exists (e.g., Eaton, 1976; Ginger- ich, 1975). We have observed morphological simi- larity in the structure of the protective body cases of caddisfly larvae (Helicopsyche borealis: Insecta; Tri- choptera) and aquatic snails (Physa integra: Gastro- poda). The resemblance of body coverings between lar- vae and snails appears analogous to Batesian mim- icry (see Wickler, 1968), whereby the apparent model (i.e., the snail) might be undesirable, but because of a protective body covering rather than a noxious taste. A possible mimic (i.e., the caddisfly larvae) could then enhance its survival probability, if less predation resulted (because of its resemblance to the model) and if a predator could not discriminate be- tween the two species. The protective cases of caddisfly larvae and aquat- ic snails are similar in coloration and characterized by a prominent whorl (Fig. 1B, C). Originally, H. borealis was described as a snail based on the case only (Lea, 1834); and, although early taxonomists placed them in the same family (Denning, pers. comm.), fundamental differences exist in shell con- struction. The cases of caddisfly larvae are formed by cementing sand grains together with excretions from silk glands. In contrast, the shells of aquatic snails are formed by calcium carbonate excretions from the mantle. Since these species occur in sym- patry (Pennak, 1953; Cummins, pers. comm.), we tentatively hypothesized that caddisfly larvae mimic the external morphology of aquatic snails. In nature, fishes are a known predator of caddisfly larvae (Pen- nak, 1953). However, further systematic study of the specific circumstances regarding their predation is necessary. If our tentative hypothesis were correct, the fol- lowing statements could be made: 1) piscine preda- tors learn through reinforcement and conditioning 1 Center for Conservation and Research, Smith- sonian Institution, Front Royal, Virginia 22630. 2 Department of Zoology, University of Wiscon- sin, Milwaukee, Wisconsin 53201. not to attack shells they cannot successfully "crack" open; 2) the protective body cases of snails and cad- disfly larvae are indistinguishable to predators; and 3) the cases are conferring a protective advantage to the caddisfly larvae. In this note we formulate a number of hypotheses which are experimentally tested to determine wheth- er the cases of caddisfly larvae mimic those of aquatic snails. METHODS Naive hatchery raised brook trout (Salvelinus fon- tinalus) were used as predators in all experiments. All trout were starved for four days prior to exposure to caddisfly larvae and aquatic snails. Larvae and snails were collected most often from the same rocks in the St. Vrain Drainage in Boulder County, Col- orado. All experiments were performed in individual three-gallon tanks stored in a controlled environ- mental room. Water temperatures in the tanks ranged from 13-17 C. RESULTS AND DISCUSSION Experiment 1.-Do trout discriminate between larvae and snails? If trout cannot distinguish between larvae and snails, several results should follow. First, if a group of trout learns that snails are inedible, they should then avoid both snails and larvae. Second, a differ- ent group of trout that learns to prey on larvae should, when exposed to snails, attempt to prey on them. Conversely, if trout learn to discriminate, the order of presentation of snails and larvae will be unimportant. Procedure.-Twelve trout were individually housed in tanks. Six were then exposed individually to five larvae, each for six days. The other six trout were presented individually to five snails, each for a six-day period. Results. -In all trials larvae were never preyed upon and all available snails were consumed. How- ever, eight snails avoided predation by climbing walls. These data illustrate that snails are not gain- ing a protective advantage. In fact, they were the only source of trout predation. Also, since snails were preyed upon, mimicry (as exemplified by a hard and protective shell) no longer appears a plausible hy- 511 This content downloaded from 75.166.117.246 on Fri, 10 Nov 2017 06:41:07 UTC All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms

Transcript of This content downloaded from 75.166.117.246 on …...If the protective cases of H. borealis larvae...

Page 1: This content downloaded from 75.166.117.246 on …...If the protective cases of H. borealis larvae mimic those of snails, then H. borealis should be subject to less predation than

Evolution, 33(1), 1979, pp. 511-513

NOTES AND COMMENTS

CONVERGENT EVOLUTION BETWEEN PHYLA: A TEST CASE OF MIMICRY BETWEEN CADDISFLY LARVAE (HELICOPSYCHE BOREALIS)

AND AQUATIC SNAILS (PHYSA INTEGRA)

JOEL BERGER1 AND JERRY KASTER2

Department of Environmental, Population, and Organismic Biology, University of Colorado, Boulder, Colorado 80309

Received May 5, 1978. Revised July 19, 1978

Many cases of potential mimicry are reported sim- ply because the species in question are quite similar in external appearance. However, neither data nor

testable hypotheses are advanced to determine if mimicry actually exists (e.g., Eaton, 1976; Ginger- ich, 1975). We have observed morphological simi-

larity in the structure of the protective body cases of caddisfly larvae (Helicopsyche borealis: Insecta; Tri- choptera) and aquatic snails (Physa integra: Gastro- poda).

The resemblance of body coverings between lar-

vae and snails appears analogous to Batesian mim- icry (see Wickler, 1968), whereby the apparent model

(i.e., the snail) might be undesirable, but because of

a protective body covering rather than a noxious taste. A possible mimic (i.e., the caddisfly larvae) could then enhance its survival probability, if less predation resulted (because of its resemblance to the model) and if a predator could not discriminate be-

tween the two species. The protective cases of caddisfly larvae and aquat-

ic snails are similar in coloration and characterized by a prominent whorl (Fig. 1B, C). Originally, H. borealis was described as a snail based on the case only (Lea, 1834); and, although early taxonomists placed them in the same family (Denning, pers.

comm.), fundamental differences exist in shell con- struction. The cases of caddisfly larvae are formed by cementing sand grains together with excretions

from silk glands. In contrast, the shells of aquatic snails are formed by calcium carbonate excretions from the mantle. Since these species occur in sym- patry (Pennak, 1953; Cummins, pers. comm.), we tentatively hypothesized that caddisfly larvae mimic the external morphology of aquatic snails. In nature, fishes are a known predator of caddisfly larvae (Pen- nak, 1953). However, further systematic study of the specific circumstances regarding their predation is necessary.

If our tentative hypothesis were correct, the fol- lowing statements could be made: 1) piscine preda-

tors learn through reinforcement and conditioning

1 Center for Conservation and Research, Smith- sonian Institution, Front Royal, Virginia 22630.

2 Department of Zoology, University of Wiscon- sin, Milwaukee, Wisconsin 53201.

not to attack shells they cannot successfully "crack" open; 2) the protective body cases of snails and cad-

disfly larvae are indistinguishable to predators; and 3) the cases are conferring a protective advantage to the caddisfly larvae.

In this note we formulate a number of hypotheses

which are experimentally tested to determine wheth- er the cases of caddisfly larvae mimic those of aquatic snails.

METHODS

Naive hatchery raised brook trout (Salvelinus fon- tinalus) were used as predators in all experiments. All trout were starved for four days prior to exposure

to caddisfly larvae and aquatic snails. Larvae and snails were collected most often from the same rocks in the St. Vrain Drainage in Boulder County, Col-

orado. All experiments were performed in individual three-gallon tanks stored in a controlled environ- mental room. Water temperatures in the tanks ranged from 13-17 C.

RESULTS AND DISCUSSION

Experiment 1.-Do trout discriminate between larvae and snails?

If trout cannot distinguish between larvae and

snails, several results should follow. First, if a group of trout learns that snails are inedible, they should then avoid both snails and larvae. Second, a differ- ent group of trout that learns to prey on larvae should, when exposed to snails, attempt to prey on them. Conversely, if trout learn to discriminate, the order of presentation of snails and larvae will be unimportant.

Procedure.-Twelve trout were individually

housed in tanks. Six were then exposed individually

to five larvae, each for six days. The other six trout were presented individually to five snails, each for a six-day period.

Results. -In all trials larvae were never preyed upon and all available snails were consumed. How- ever, eight snails avoided predation by climbing walls. These data illustrate that snails are not gain- ing a protective advantage. In fact, they were the

only source of trout predation. Also, since snails were preyed upon, mimicry (as exemplified by a hard and

protective shell) no longer appears a plausible hy-

511

This content downloaded from 75.166.117.246 on Fri, 10 Nov 2017 06:41:07 UTCAll use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms

Page 2: This content downloaded from 75.166.117.246 on …...If the protective cases of H. borealis larvae mimic those of snails, then H. borealis should be subject to less predation than

512 NOTES AND COMMENTS

pothesis. We suspected however that some form of mimicry was still possible.

Experiment 2. -Predation and movement: Do larvae mimic dead or non-moving snails?

Since, in Experiment 1, some of the snails moved out of the range of predation we next examined the movements of snails and larvae. Marked locomotor differences existed. Larvae were sedentary and ex- hibited little movement whereas snails actively moved across tank bottoms and occasionally climbed the walls. Because larvae were not preyed upon in Experiment 1 and predatory behavior in some trout is elicited by prey movement (Ware, 1973), our working hypothesis was that larvae mimic either dead or non-moving (e.g., satiated) snails, or both, or other inanimate objects (e.g., sand grains, rocks, etc.).

If our hypothesis were correct, several results should follow. First, prey movement must be impli- cated in eliciting predatory behavior in naive brook trout. This behavior would be discernible by expos- ing naive trout to live and dead snails. Second, the lack of trout discriminatory ability between dead snails and larvae (which also were non-moving al- though they were alive) must be demonstrated. This step is also testable by introducing starved trout to both dead snails and live larvae.

Procedure.-Both of the above steps were com- bined. In ten separate tanks we individually exposed ten trout simultaneously (one fish per tank) to live snails, dead snails, and live larvae. An individual was considered to be preyed upon when either it was consumed by a trout (observed by us) or it no longer persisted in its original form (i.e., it was mutilated through mastication).

Results.-Trout demonstrated prey preferences by selecting moving forms more often than non-mov- ing forms (Table 1A). However, this experiment sim- ply illustrates that prey movement is important in eliciting predatory behavior in naive brook trout. It demonstrates nothing pertaining to the shape of prey. Perhaps the presence of moving prey detracts from predatory discrimination in non-moving types.

A

B 4C

FIG. 1. The three species tested for a mimetic complex. A) Leptocella albida, B) Physa integra, C) Helicopsyche borealis.

Experiment 3.-Is the whorled case or the lack of motion confering the

protective advantage?

If the protective cases of H. borealis larvae mimic those of snails, then H. borealis should be subject to less predation than other non-moving species of caddisfly larvae with different case designs. That is, a different species of non-moving caddisfly larvae with a case not resembling that of snails, should be preyed upon more often than H. borealis. Alterna- tively, if it is not the case which confers the protec- tive advantage but the lack of motion instead, then both species of larvae should experience little pre- dation. Our working hypothesis then, was that H. borealis mimics non-moving snails and will therefore experience less predation than other non-moving but differently shaped caddisfly larvae cases.

Procedure.-To test our hypothesis we exposed two species of non-moving larvae, H. borealis and Leptocella albida (Fig. 1A), to individually housed trout that had been previously exposed to snails. At this time, we also placed dead snails in each tank to determine further if differential predation was oc- curring. All three species were then exposed to trout predation for six days.

TABLE 1. (A) The percent of live caddisfly larvae (n = 50), live snails (n = 50), and dead snails (n = 30)

remaining after exposure to naive brook trout (Experiment 2). (B) The percent of live caddisfly larvae (n = 40), live Leptocella albida (n = 40), and dead snails (n = 20) after exposure to brook trout (Experiment 3).

Number of days after exposure

Organism 1 2 3 4 5 6

caddisfly larvae 100 100 100 100 100 95 (A) dead snails 100 77 67 67 67 67

live snails 100 27 0 0 0 0

caddisfly larvae 100 100 100 100 100 98 (B) L. albida 100 100 100 100 95 95

dead snails 100 100 95 95 95 95

This content downloaded from 75.166.117.246 on Fri, 10 Nov 2017 06:41:07 UTCAll use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms

Page 3: This content downloaded from 75.166.117.246 on …...If the protective cases of H. borealis larvae mimic those of snails, then H. borealis should be subject to less predation than

NOTES AND COMMENTS 513

Results.-In this experiment, prey preferences

were not demonstrated by trout (Table 1B). The lack of preference for both of the species of larvae indi- cates that shape is not the unique feature conferring an advantage to H. borealis. In view of the fact that the prey species were largely ignored by hungry trout, it seems reasonable to assume that the lack of movement was more important than shape. We therefore rejected our last hypothesis and concluded that the case of H. borealis does not mimic that of P. integra.

CONCLUSIONS

The lack of differential predation or any predation in the last experiment supports the contention that the non-moving prey items were undesirable to brook trout. Hence, our conclusion that predation (or the lack of it) has not been a major selective force governing the structural resemblance of H. borealis to P. integra.

Ross (1956; see also Wiggins, 1977) discussed the evolution and taxonomy of H. borealis, but we are unaware of any statements or experiments that at- tempt to explain the evolutionary significance of lar- val case resmblance to snails. Our inability to im- plicate mimicry between caddisfly larvae and snails using brook trout does not rule out the possibility that other predators (e.g., birds or other fish) have been important in the evolution of these forms. How- ever, it is also likely that common selection pressures (e.g., hydrodynamics) have favored the evolution of similar morphologies between phylogenetically dis- tant groups. If so, the striking resemblance between cases and shells represents an extreme example of convergent evolution between phyla (see Kaster and Berger, 1977, for a further discussion of convergent evolution between phyla).

Although this study presents evidence contrary to our original supposition of mimicry, our results are

based on the testing of a number of hypotheses. Many of the existing examples of mimicry have not been carefully studied experimentally. It is therefore possible that many of these examples represent par-

allel or convergent evolution rather than mimicry.

ACKNOWLEDGMENTS

We thank the following people for comments on earlier versions of this paper: David Armstrong, Marc Bekoff, Ruth Bernstein, Kenneth Cummins,

Greg Snyder and Andrew Starrett. Sigma Xi and the University of Colorado contributed funds and facil- ities.

LITERATURE CITED

EATON, R. 1976. A possible case of mimicry in large mammals. Evolution 30:853-856.

GINGERICH, P. D. 1975. Is the aardwolf a mimic of the hyaena? Nature 253 (5488):191-192.

KASTER, J., AND J. BERGER. 1977. Convergent and

parallel evolution: a model illustrating selection, phylogeny, and phenetic similarity. Bio Systems 9:195-200.

LEA, I. 1834. Observations on the Naiades and de- scriptions of new species of that and other fam- ilies. Trans. Amer. Phil. Soc. 4:63-121.

PENNAK, R. W. 1953. Freshwater Invertebrates of the United States. Ronald Press, New York.

Ross, H. H. 1956. Evolution and Classification of the Mountain Caddisflies. Univ. Illinois Press, Urbana.

WARE, D. 1973. The predatory behavior of rainbow

trout (Salmo gairdneri). Ph.D. Dissertation, Univ. British Columbia, Vancouver.

WICKLER, W. 1968. Mimicry in Plants and Ani- mals. World Univ. Library, London.

WIGGINS, G. B. 1977. Larva of the North Ameri-

can Caddisfly Genera (Trichoptera). Univ. To- ronto Press, Toronto.

Evolution, 33(1), 1979, pp. 513-517

ONCE AGAIN, WHY 300 SPECIES OF HAWAIIAN DROSOPHILA?

ALAN R. TEMPLETON

Department of Biology, Washington University, St. Louis, Mo. 63130

Received April 20, 1978. Revised September 7, 1978

The startling number of endemic Drosophila species found on the Hawaiian Islands has attracted the attention of a large number of evolutionary bi- ologists. Recently, Ringo (1977) has reviewed the three primary hypotheses that have been proposed to explain this spectacular case of rapid speciation. The first is the "adaptive" model (Dobzhansky, 1972,

1976), which Ringo points out is inconsistent with the allozyme data and does not explain why Hawai- ian Drosophila evolution has been characterized by

striking morphological and behavioral modifications whereas continental Drosophila evolution has not. The second is Carsons' hypothesis (1970, 1975) that founder effects (Mayr, 1963) can lead to a "genetic

This content downloaded from 75.166.117.246 on Fri, 10 Nov 2017 06:41:07 UTCAll use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms