The Extended Genotype: The Heritability of Personality ...

26
The Extended Genotype: The Heritability of Personality Accounts for the Heritability of Recalled Family Environments in Twins Reared Apart Robert F. Krueger Kristian E. Markon Thomas J. Bouchard, Jr. University of Minnesota—Twin Cities Campus ABSTRACT How are retrospective accounts of family rearing envir- onments linked to adult personality? We addressed this question by measuring both domains in a sample of 180 reared-apart twins. Twins completed extensive measures of rearing environments (the Minnesota- Briggs History Record, the Block Environmental Questionnaire, the Family Environment Scale, and the Physical Facilities Questionnaire) and an omnibus measure of adult personality (the Multidimensional Personality Questionnaire). Retrospective accounts of family environ- ments were partially heritable and all the heritable variance in environmental measures could be accounted for by heritable variance in personality. In addition, differences between twins in their accounts of their rearing environments (nonshared environmental factors) were not significantly linked to differences between twins in their personalities. Hence, family environmental measures appear to be heritable because Data collection for this project was supported by contributions of the Pioneer Fund, the Koch Charitable Foundation, and the Seaver Institute to the Minnesota Study of Twins Reared Apart. Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Robert F. Krueger, Department of Psychology, University of Minnesota, N414 Elliott Hall, 75 East River Road, Minneapolis, MN 55455-0344. Electronic mail may be sent to krueg038@um- n.edu. Journal of Personality 71:5, October 2003. Blackwell Publishing 2003

Transcript of The Extended Genotype: The Heritability of Personality ...

The Extended Genotype: The Heritability of

Personality Accounts for the Heritability of

Recalled Family Environments in Twins

Reared Apart

Robert F. Krueger

Kristian E. Markon

Thomas J. Bouchard, Jr.

University of Minnesota—Twin Cities Campus

ABSTRACT How are retrospective accounts of family rearing envir-onments linked to adult personality? We addressed this question bymeasuring both domains in a sample of 180 reared-apart twins. Twinscompleted extensive measures of rearing environments (the Minnesota-Briggs History Record, the Block Environmental Questionnaire, theFamily Environment Scale, and the Physical Facilities Questionnaire)and an omnibus measure of adult personality (the MultidimensionalPersonality Questionnaire). Retrospective accounts of family environ-ments were partially heritable and all the heritable variance inenvironmental measures could be accounted for by heritable variancein personality. In addition, differences between twins in their accounts oftheir rearing environments (nonshared environmental factors) were notsignificantly linked to differences between twins in their personalities.Hence, family environmental measures appear to be heritable because

Data collection for this project was supported by contributions of the Pioneer Fund,

the Koch Charitable Foundation, and the Seaver Institute to the Minnesota Study of

Twins Reared Apart.

Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Robert F. Krueger,

Department of Psychology, University of Minnesota, N414 Elliott Hall, 75 East River

Road, Minneapolis, MN 55455-0344. Electronic mail may be sent to krueg038@um-

n.edu.

Journal of Personality 71:5, October 2003.

Blackwell Publishing 2003

personality genes influence the way people shape and recall their rearingenvironments. In addition, differences in reared-apart twins’ retro-spectively recalled rearing environments appear to have little impact ondifferences in their personalities in adulthood.

How are childhood family environments and personality related?Classical answers to this question focused on the potential causal

role of the childhood family environment in shaping personality.For example, parenting style was often described as a family-wide

environment with important implications for personality develop-ment (Halverson & Wampler, 1997). More recently, however,findings from behavior genetic research have provided a different

perspective. Such research indicates that personality can be tracedprimarily to genetic influences, and to nonshared environments—

environments that make persons growing up in the same familydifferent, rather than similar (for a quantitative review, see Loehlin,

1992). Still, behavior genetic research has been criticized for oftenneglecting to measure the environment directly (Collins, Maccoby,

Steinberg, Hetherington & Bornstein, 2000; Hoffman, 1991; Wachs,1993). In many behavior genetic studies, environmental effects areinferred, rather than being linked to specific, measured environ-

mental variables.In the research reported herein, our goal was to better understand

the relationship between retrospectively recalled childhood familyenvironments and adult personality by linking measures of

environments and personality in a study of twins reared apart. Webegin by reviewing what genetically informative studies have taught

us about the family environment.

What Have We Learned From Genetically Informative Studies of

Family Environmental Measures?

One of the more fundamental findings from genetically informativeresearch is that measures of family environments are often heritable

(Plomin & Bergeman, 1991; Plomin, 1994). That is, a portion of thevariance in such ‘‘environmental’’ measures is actually genetic,

rather than environmental. Rowe (1981, 1983) reported the seminalwork in this area, showing that adolescent twins’ reports of

acceptance versus rejection by their parents were under geneticinfluence, whereas twins’ reports of parental control showed no

810 Krueger et al.

genetic influence. These results were extended to older adult twins

reared apart and together by Plomin and his colleagues (Plomin,McClearn, Pedersen, Nesselroade, & Bergeman, 1988). Plomin et al.

(1988) reported on data collected on twins’ retrospective accounts ofthe family environments in which they were reared. Similar to

Rowe’s (1981, 1983) findings, reports of warmth and personalgrowth showed greater genetic influence than reports of parental

control. Bouchard and colleagues (Bouchard & McGue, 1990; Hur& Bouchard, 1995) have also reported similar results. In a sample of

adult twins reared apart, Bouchard and McGue (1990) reportedsignificant genetic contributions to retrospectively recalled reports offamily cohesion and encouragement of individual growth, but not

positive constraint (family organization and moral-religious orienta-tion). Hur and Bouchard (1995) extended the Bouchard and McGue

(1990) report to a larger reared-apart twin sample assessed on awider array of environmental measures. Hur and Bouchard (1995)

found greater genetic influence on retrospectively recalled reports ofsupport in twins’ rearing families, whereas genetic influences on an

organization and cultural orientation factor were smaller. Thus,evidence for genetic contributions to the family environment is notlimited to only a few specific measures. This was also illustrated by

Plomin, Reiss, Hetherington, and Howe (1994), who extended thefinding of significant heritability of family environments to a very

wide variety of measures taken on participants in the NonsharedEnvironment in Adolescent Development (NEAD) project. Fifteen

of 18 composite measures showed significant genetic influence, withan average heritability of 27%.

Adult twins’ accounts of their current family environments (asopposed to the family environments in which they were raised) are

also heritable. In a sample of adult twins reared apart and together,Plomin, McClearn, Pedersen, Nesselroade, and Bergeman (1989)reported that, on average, genetic influences accounted for 24% of

the variance in eight indices of current family environment. Adulttwins’ parenting practices—an aspect of their current family

environments—are also heritable (Kendler, 1996; Losoya, Callor,Rowe, & Goldsmith, 1997; Perusse, Neale, Heath, & Eaves, 1994).

Nevertheless, an exception to the general finding of geneticcontributions to measures of the family environment was provided

by Vernon, Jang, Harris, and McCarthy (1997). Vernon et al. (1997)reported no genetic influence on measures of the family environment

Environment and Personality 811

in twins reared together; in their best-fitting models for factors

derived from the Family Environment Scale (FES), heritabilitieswere estimated at zero. Still, the finding of significant genetic

contributions to measures of the family environment is generallyrobust. Indeed, such evidence is not limited to the studies of self-

report measures in twins described above. Observational indices ofthe family environments of adolescents also show genetic influence

(O’Connor, Hetherington, Reiss, & Plomin, 1995), as do adoptionstudies of measures of family environments in early adolescence

(Deater-Deckard, Fulker, & Plomin, 1999).

Can the Heritability of Personality Help Explain the Heritability

of Measures of the Family Environment?

Although a number of methodologically diverse studies point togenetic influences on perceptions of the family environment, the

current literature is missing a clear explanation of why measures ofthe family environment are heritable. One key possibility is that

genetic influences on measures of family environments reflect geneticinfluences on personality. Personality genes may influence more than

characteristic styles of affect, behavior, and cognition because thespecific affects, behaviors, and cognitions that uniquely characterizea person have implications for that person’s impact on and

understanding of his or her family environment. Consider theexample of a person who is generally calm and tends to control her

impulses. Such a person might have a positive impact on her familyenvironment, increasing cohesion among family members because

she is pleasant and easy to get along with.Little research has investigated the possibility that genetic effects

on adult personality account for genetic effects on family environ-mental measures. However, a pioneering study along these lines was

reported by Chipuer, Plomin, Pedersen, McClearn, & Nesselroade(1993). Chipuer et al. (1993) investigated the extent to which geneticinfluences on the personality traits of extraversion and neuroticism

could account for genetic influences on reports of current familyenvironments made by older adult twins. Although genetic

influences on extraversion and neuroticism accounted for some ofthe genetic influence on environmental reports, a significant portion

of the genetic influence on reports of the environment wasindependent of personality. Nevertheless, an important shortcoming

812 Krueger et al.

of this study was its limitation to only two personality traits, which

may explain why Chipuer et al. (1993) were only partially successfulin accounting for the heritability of environmental measures using

personality measures. Similarly, Jang, Vernon, and Livesley (2000)present moderate genetic correlations between indices of personality

pathology and factors derived from the family environment scales(FES), but their analytic approach was limited to estimating

bivariate correlations between specific FES factors and specificpersonality pathology indices, rather than simultaneous multivariate

modeling of multiple FES variables along with multiple personalitytraits.

Along these lines, Saudino, Pedersen, Lichtenstein, McClearn, and

Plomin (1997) presented multivariate analyses showing that geneticvariance in neuroticism, extraversion, and openness to experience

accounted for all the genetic variance in life events in a sample offemale twins. Although life events measures are not measures of the

family environment (cf. Billig, Hershberger, Iacono, &McGue, 1996),the Saudino et al. (1997) study suggests the possibility that

multivariate modeling of more extensive measures of personalitymay be useful in studies attempting to understand the heritability offamily environmental measures using personality measures.

Hence, in reviewing research on genetic contributions toenvironmental measures, Rowe (1997) suggested further examina-

tion of the question of ‘‘whether genetic variation in measures of‘environment’ represents gene effects shared with known personality

traits’’ (p. 383). The research reported herein represents such aninvestigation. We investigated the genetic overlap between measures

of recalled rearing environments and personality in the MinnesotaStudy of Twins Reared Apart (MISTRA), a comprehensive

investigation of adult twins separated very early in life (at anaverage age of one year old) and reunited as adults. The currentreport extends previous reports on environmental measures from

MISTRA (Bouchard & McGue, 1990; Hur & Bouchard, 1995) bymodeling the full range of environmental measures available in

MISTRA and personality simultaneously. In addition to investigat-ing a possible genetic account of the connection between measures of

the family environment and personality, we also investigated thepossibility of a nonshared environmental connection between family

environment measures and personality measures, a topic to whichwe now turn.

Environment and Personality 813

Do Differences Between Twins’ Accounts of Their Rearing

Environments Predict Personality Differences in Adulthood?

As noted above, our twin participants were reared apart, having

been separated at a very early age. Hence, they were reared indifferent family environments, and they reported on these different

rearing environments as adults. The design of our study thereforeallowed us to evaluate the possibility that differences in the family-rearing environments of our twins might predict differences between

them in their personalities as adults. In most twin studies, theparticipants are reared together, and their early rearing environ-

ments therefore fall into the category of ‘‘shared family environ-ments.’’ However, because our twins were reared apart, their rearing

environments are not shared; in behavior genetic parlance, theirrearing environments would be characterized as ‘‘nonshared

environments’’ (see also Turkheimer & Waldron, 2000, for a reviewof the concept of nonshared environment).

Our reared-apart twins, therefore, provide a very uniqueopportunity to examine the link between distinctive family rearingenvironments and personality in adulthood. Genetically informative

studies from other research groups have attempted to identifymeasured environments that predict differences between family

members’ personalities (Baker & Daniels, 1990; Vernon et al., 1997),but these studies are distinct from our study because they involved

participants reared together, that is, participants who shared theirfamily rearing environments. That is, these studies are unable to

address the possibility that adults’ perceptions of the different familyenvironments in which they were reared might explain differences intheir personalities, taking into account genetic influences on

personality. Hence, we examined not only the possibility of geneticconnections between recalled family environments and personality,

but also the possibility that differences in the rearing environmentsof our twins, as reported in adulthood, might predict differences in

their personalities as adults.

METHODS

Research Participants

Participants in the present study were adult (mean age5 42, SD 5 13)monozygotic and dizygotic twin pairs from the Minnesota Study of

814 Krueger et al.

Twins Reared Apart (MISTRA) who had been tested by July 2000. Datafrom other members of the twins’ families and their friends were also usedin some analyses, where noted. Participants were ascertained from anumber of countries, through media accounts, professional referral, andvarious other methods. Zygosity was determined by blood and serumassays and physical similarity measures (estimated probability ofmisclassification o.001). Procedures regarding ascertainment and screen-ing have been described in a number of other sources (e.g., Bouchard,1994; DiLalla, Carey, Gottesman, & Bouchard, 1996; Tellegen et al.,1988).

Data from a total of 90 twin pairs were available for the present study:52 monozygotic pairs (35 female, 17 male) and 38 dizygotic pairs (19female same-sex DZ pairs, 9 male same-sex DZ pairs, and 10 opposite-sexDZ pairs). Opposite-sex and same-sex DZ pairs were included together inanalyses, given the relatively small number of opposite-sex DZ pairs. Thisis consistent with previously published analyses of MISTRA data, whichsuggest that any bias introduced by combining opposite-sex and same-sextwin pairs is small to negligible (DiLalla et al., 1996).

Measures

Personality. Personality was measured using the MultidimensionalPersonality Questionnaire (MPQ; Tellegen, 2000). The MPQ is a self-report inventory, designed through iterative factor analysis and itemselection as a comprehensive measure of normal personality functioning.The MPQ comprises 11 primary scales which can be scored as threefactor-analytically derived higher-order scales: Positive Emotionality,Negative Emotionality, and Constraint (Krueger, 2000; Tellegen, 1985).Only the higher-order scales were included in the present analyses. Theexternal and internal validity of the MPQ has been demonstrated in anumber of settings (e.g., Caspi et al., 1997; Harkness, Tellegen, & Waller,1995; Krueger et al., 1996; Markon, Krueger, Bouchard, & Gottesman,2002; Waller, Benet, & Farney, 1994; White & Depue, 1999); detailedcharacteristics of the scales and their constituent items are provided byTellegen and Waller (in press).

Family environment. Retrospectively recalled childhood family environ-ment was measured using four sets of self-report measures. The first setcomprised the 6 Family Scales of the Minnesota-Briggs History Record(MBHR). The MBHR is an inventory developed by Briggs, Rouzer,Hamberg, and Holman (1972) to record life histories of researchparticipants efficiently. The Family Scales, based on factor analyses in

Environment and Personality 815

the MISTRA sample, are comparable to those published by Briggs et al.(1972) and include those items relating to parental behavior andcharacteristics. The Family Scales include Parental Conflict (e.g., ‘‘Inmy childhood home, there were daily quarrels between my parents’’;a5 .83), Parental Control (e.g., ‘‘As a child and adolescent my parentswere very strict with me, and I was frequently punished’’; a5 .67),Parental Acceptance-Rejection (e.g., ‘‘I was much wanted by myparents’’; a5 .71), Family Cohesion (e.g., ‘‘My parents never, to myknowledge, considered divorce’’; a5 .63), Parental Drinking (e.g., ‘‘Mymother drank alcoholic beverages almost daily and always to excess’’;a5 .65), and Family Income (e.g., ‘‘The earned income in my parents’family was or has been steady and enough’’; a5 .75).

The second set of environmental measures included the BlockEnvironmental Questionnaire (BEQ), a 101-item inventory measuringmaternal and paternal behavioral characteristics and family environment.The BEQ was derived from Block’s (1971) 92-item Environmental Q-Sortby changing the response format to a 9-point Likert rating scale, addingnine items reflecting issues important to adoptees, and rewording someitems for clarity. The BEQ comprises six primary scales, developedthrough factor analysis and a reliability considerations: MaternalAcceptance-Rejection (a5 .88), Paternal Acceptance-Rejection(a5 .85), Family Cohesion (a5 .89), Maternal Intellectual-CulturalOrientation (a5 .68), Paternal Intellectual-Cultural Orientation(a5 .70), and Family Organization (a5 .73). Constituent items anddetails regarding scale construction are described in Hur and Bouchard(1995).

The Family Environment Scale (FES; Moos and Moos, 1986) is awidely used measure of general family environment. The 90 items of theFES are scored as 10 scales: Cohesion (a5 .82), Expressiveness (a5 .68),Conflict (a5 .83), Independence (a5 .55), Active-Recreational Orienta-tion (a5 .70), Achievement Orientation (a5 .66), Intellectual-CulturalOrientation (a5 .74), Moral-Religious Orientation (a5 .72), Organiza-tion (a5 .70), and Control (a5 .67).

The final set of family environmental measures included the scales ofthe Physical Facilities Questionnaire (PFQ), a 41-item retrospectivemeasure of material possessions present in the home. Individualscompleting the PFQ were asked to indicate whether or not they couldrecall each of 41 objects being present in the home. Items of the PFQ canbe organized into four scales indicated by factor analyses: MaterialPossessions (e.g., stereo, tape recorder; a5 .88), Cultural Possessions(e.g., art reproductions, world atlas; a5 .75), Mechanical Possessions(e.g., automotive tools, carpentry tools; a5 .66), and Scientific-TechnicalPossessions (e.g., electronic or chemical lab equipment; a5 .71).

816 Krueger et al.

Statistical Analyses

To correct for potential biases in model fitting, environmental and MPQhigher-order factor scores were first adjusted for effects of age and sex(McGue & Bouchard, 1984). Each scale was regressed on age, sex, theinteraction between age and sex, and age squared, using data from familymembers in addition to twin data when available (N for environmentalmeasures5 294; for the MPQ, N5 363). Standardized residuals fromthese regressions were used in subsequent analyses. For biometricanalyses, data were then double-entered, with df in all modeling analysesadjusted appropriately.

P-values were calculated for phenotypic correlations between MPQand environmental factors by means of bias-corrected and acceleratedbootstrap confidence intervals (Efron & Tibshirani, 1998). Eachconfidence interval was based on 10,000 bootstrap samples with twinpair taken as the sampling unit to correct for dependent observationswithin pairs. The p-value for a correlation estimate was calculated byfinding a such that the (1-a)% confidence interval did not contain zero.

MZ and DZ covariance matrices were estimated using the em.normroutine of the R statistical language (Ihaka & Gentleman, 1996). Thisroutine performs maximum-likelihood EM estimation of multivariatenormal parameters in the presence of randomly missing values (7 MZ and10 DZ pairs had at least one member with a missing value on one of themultiple variables involved in our analyses). In the EM algorithm,missing values, the covariance matrix, and other parameters of thepopulation multivariate distribution are estimated jointly, in an iterativeprocedure whereby the expected likelihood of the observed data—givenestimated parameters—is calculated (E-step) and maximized (M-step).EM estimation of covariance matrices is less biased and more efficientthan other methods, such as pairwise or listwise deletion (Dempster,Laird, & Rubin, 1977). Nevertheless, in our sample, the EM-estimatedcovariance matrices did not differ significantly from estimates based onlistwise deletion (w25 2.99, df5 10, p5 .98).

Genetic and environmental relationships between the personality andenvironmental variables were estimated by fitting the multivariate modelshown in Figure 1 to the MZ and DZ phenotypic covariance matrices viamaximum likelihood using the computer program Mx (Neale, Boker,Xie, & Maes, 1999). The model shown in Figure 1 contains three types offactors: genetic factors linking the three personality and two environ-mental variables on the Figure, environmental factors linking thepersonality and environmental variables, and genetic and environmentalfactors unique to each of the environmental variables. Five geneticfactors and five environmental factors were modeled. Three of the geneticfactors each load on a personality factor and both of the environmental

Environment and Personality 817

factors: a factor representing genetic links between Positive Emotionality,Cohesion, and Status (G1), a factor representing genetic links betweenNegative Emotionality, Cohesion, and Status (G2), and a factorrepresenting genetic links between Constraint, Cohesion, and Status(G3). Analogously, three environmental factors, each representing

PEM NEM CON COH STA

G1 G2 G3 G4 G5

E1 E2 E3 E4 E5

g44g22 g33g11

e11 e22 e33 e44 e55

g55

g41

g51

g42

g52

g43

g53

e41

e51

e42

e52

e43

e53

Figure1Initial multivariate model of the association between factors ofpersonality and environmental factors. G5additive genetic effects,E5nonshared environmental effects. PEM5positive emotionality,NEM5negative emotionality, CON5 constraint, COH5 cohesion,

STA5 status.

818 Krueger et al.

environmental links between a personality variable and the twoenvironmental variables, were also modeled (E1, E2, and E3). Finally,four factors representing genetic (G4, G5) and environmental variance(E4, E5) unique to the two environmental variables were modeled.

Model fit was evaluated using Akaike’s Information Criterion (AIC;Akaike, 1987), in conjunction with the Root Mean Square Error ofApproximation (RMSEA; Browne & Cudeck, 1993). Our goal was tofind the model that best explained the observed data while invoking asfew unknown parameters as possible (i.e., the model that achieved thesmallest w2 while allowing the largest number of degrees of freedom). Assuch, we sought the model with the most negative AIC value(AIC5 w2� 2 df) that also maintained good fit in an absolute sense(i.e., an RMSEA o .05).

For simplicity, the model is shown only for one twin in Figure 1. Inactuality, each of the genetic and environmental factors shown in Figure1 were modeled for each of the twins in a pair. Genetic andenvironmental factors were modeled as correlated between twins to theextent that twins share effects represented by the factors. For example,the genetic factors shown were modeled as correlated between twins tothe extent that they share independently segregating genes—.5 for DZpairs, and 1.0 for MZ pairs (i.e., all genetic effects were modeled asadditive). Similarly, as all the twins were reared apart, environmentaleffects were modeled as nonshared, and, therefore, environmental factorswere uncorrelated between twins.

In addition to estimating model parameters, we also reexpressed thoseparameters as genetic and environmental correlations. Genetic andenvironmental correlations are particularly useful representations ofgenetic and environmental relationships between variables and werecalculated for the best-fitting model. The genetic correlation can bethought of as the correlation between genetic influences on two variables;the environmental correlation, similarly, can be thought of as thecorrelation between environmental influences on two variables. Geneticor environmental correlations can be derived by dividing the genetic orenvironmental covariance between two variables by the square root ofthe product of the genetic or environmental variances of the twovariables. Genetic or environmental covariances can be obtainedrelatively easily by tracing through relevant paths in a path diagram. InFigure 1, for example, the genetic covariance between NegativeEmotionality and Cohesion would be estimated as (g22)(g42). In addition,the statistical significance of the paths in our best-fitting model wasdetermined using the likelihood-based approach described by Neale andMiller (1997).

Environment and Personality 819

RESULTS

Factor Analysis of Environmental Measures

To facilitate interpretation of results, higher-order factors were

derived from scales of the four sets of environmental measures—theMBHR, BEQ, FES, and PFQ scales—by means of factor analysis.

Factor analysis was conducted by means of initial principalcomponents analysis, followed by maximum-likelihood estimationand VARIMAX rotation for estimation of factor loadings and

factor scores. Data from family members and friends of twins as wellas the twins themselves (N5 373) were used in factor analyses and

factor score estimation.Screeplots suggested that two or possibly three factors accounted

for much of the variance in the environmental scales. Two factorswere extracted because the two-factor solution resembles other

environmental factor models often published in the literature (e.g.,Fowler, 1981; Hur & Bouchard, 1995; Rowe, 1983), providing

theoretical and empirical consistency with a larger body of work.1

The maximum-likelihood estimated, varimax-rotated loadings forthe two-factor solution are displayed in Table 1. The first factor,

with large positive loadings on the BEQ and FES cohesion andacceptance scales and large negative loadings on the FES and

MBHR control and conflict scales, is readily interpretable as a factorreflecting perceived cohesion versus conflict in the family environ-

1. Biometric analyses also supported the choice of a two-factor over a three-

factor solution. When we examined genetic correlations among the estimated

factor scores in the twin data, we found little evidence that the three factors were

distinct at an etiologic level. These results were obtained in spite of the fact that

the scores used in these analyses were derived from orthogonal varimax-rotated

solutions, estimated on data from the entire sample, which included both twins

and other participants. Specifically, the genetic correlation between the first and

third factors of a three-factor solution was .8, and the genetic correlation between

the second and third factors was .4. However, the genetic correlation between the

first and second factors was only .1, and in a two-factor solution, the first and

second factors were also etiologically distinct, with an estimated genetic

correlation of .2. Thus, the two-factor phenotypic solution appears to converge

on etiologically distinct dimensions of variation, whereas the three-factor solution

does not. Along with considerations of consistency with the existing literature,

these findings also support adoption of a two-factor solution in an investigation

of the underlying etiologic sources of the relationship between personality and

recalled family environment.

820 Krueger et al.

ment. For this reason, we have labeled the first factor Cohesion. Thesecond factor, with large positive loadings on the PFQ material and

cultural possession scales and BEQ family organization, maternal,and paternal intellectual orientation scales, reflects organized,

intellectual, high-socioeconomic-status family environments. Welabeled this second factor Status, as a shorthand for socioeconomic

Table 1Loadings of Environmental Scales on Cohesion and Status Factors

Cohesion Status

BEQ Family Cohesion 0.90 0.20

BEQ Maternal Acceptance 0.81 0.14

FES Cohesion 0.76 0.17

BEQ Paternal Acceptance 0.70 0.16

MBHR Acceptance 0.70 0.28

FES Expressiveness 0.58 0.07

FES Independence 0.39 0.12

MBHR Cohesion 0.35 0.09

MBHR Drinking � 0.15 0.01

FES Control � 0.48 0.13

FES Conflict � 0.59 � 0.04

MBHR Conflict � 0.61 � 0.11

MBHR Control � 0.69 0.19

PFQ Cultural Possessions 0.04 0.68

PFQ Material Possessions 0.00 0.67

BEQ Family Organization 0.21 0.65

BEQ Maternal Intellectual-Cultural Orientation 0.19 0.64

BEQ Paternal Intellectual-Cultural Orientation 0.04 0.62

FES Intellectual-Cultural Orientation 0.20 0.59

FES Active-Recreational Orientation 0.29 0.49

PFQ Scientific Possessions � 0.04 0.48

MBHR Family Income 0.04 0.46

FES Organization 0.12 0.41

FES Achievement Orientation � 0.16 0.39

PFQ Mechanical Possessions 0.02 0.35

FES Moral-Religious Orientation 0.23 0.24

Note. MBHR5Minnesota Briggs History Record; BEQ5Block Environmental

Questionnaire; FES5Family Environment Scale; PFQ5Physical Facilities Ques-

tionnaire.

Environment and Personality 821

and intellectual-cultural status. Estimated factor scores for each ofthese higher-order environmental factors were estimated by the

regression method and used in the biometric modeling describedbelow.

Phenotypic Correlations Between MPQ and Environmental

Factors

Phenotypic correlations between the MPQ and environmentalhigher-order factors are shown in Table 2. Of the six correlations,

three were zero or near zero: the correlations between PositiveEmotionality and Cohesion (r5 0.00, bootstrap derived p5 .980),between Negative Emotionality and Status (r5 0.00, p5 .974), and

between Constraint and Status (r5 � 0.03, p5 .658). The remainingthree correlations were larger: a negative correlation between

Negative Emotionality and Cohesion (r5 � 0.17, p5 .020), apositive correlation between Constraint and Cohesion (r5 0.16,

p5 .062), and a positive correlation between Positive Emotionalityand Status (r5 0.16, p5 .034).

Model-Fitting Analyses

To determine the most parsimonious model of genetic and

environmental relationships between personality and recalled familyenvironment, a series of multivariate models, all variants of the

Table 2Phenotypic Correlations and Best-Fitting Model Estimates of Genetic

and Environmental Correlations

Environmental Factor

Cohesion Status

MPQ Factor rp rg re rp rg re

Positive Emotionality 0.00 0 0 .16 1.00 0

Negative Emotionality � 0.17 � .77 0 0.00 0 0

Constraint 0.16 .64 0 � 0.03 0 0

Note. Values given in single digits were fixed at zero in the best fitting model;

rp5phenotypic correlation; rg5 genetic correlation; re5 nonshared environmental

correlation.

822 Krueger et al.

model in Figure 1, were fit. These models all derived from the initial

model in that one or more paths were set to zero. In this sense, themodel in Figure 1 provided a baseline measure of fit (w2 (38)5 62.1,

p5 .01; AIC5 � 13.9; RMSEA5 .02), which we sought to improveupon by maintaining fit while removing parameters from the model.

That is, we sought the model with the most negative AIC value thatalso maintained an RMSEA o .05.

The first two models tested reflected the general hypotheses thatrelationships between personality and recalled environment are

mediated entirely genetically or entirely environmentally. In the firstmodel, all genetic paths linking personality and environmentalvariables (i.e., g41,g51,g42,g52,g43, and g53) were set equal to zero.

This model, reflecting the hypothesis that relationships betweenpersonality and family environment are mediated entirely by

nonshared environment, resulted in a poorer fit than the baseline(w2 (44)5 79.55, p5 .001; AIC5 � 8.4; RMSEA5 .03). In the

second model all environmental paths linking personality and environ-mental variables (i.e., e41,e51,e42,e52,e43, and e53) were set equal to

zero. This model, reflecting the hypothesis that relationshipsbetween personality and family environment are mediated entirelyby genetic effects, had an improved fit relative to baseline (w2

(44)5 67.9, p5 .012; AIC5 � 20.1; RMSEA5 .02).To further improve parsimony, additional paths reflecting small

or nonexistent effects were removed. All paths in the best-fittingmodel not significant at p o .05 were set equal to zero. Eliminated

were those paths reflecting unique genetic effects on Cohesion andStatus (g44 and g55), paths reflecting genetic variance in common

between Constraint and Status (g53), paths reflecting geneticvariance in common between Negative Emotionality and Status

(g52), and paths reflecting genetic variance in common betweenPositive Emotionality and Cohesion (g41). Retained were all uniquenonshared environmental effects (e11, e22, e33, e44, and e55), genetic

effects in common between Negative Emotionality and Cohesion,genetic effects in common between Constraint and Cohesion, and

genetic effects in common between Positive Emotionality andStatus.

This final model, shown in Figure 2, provided the best fit of anytested (w2 (49)5 73.7, p5 .013; AIC5 � 24.3; RMSEA5 .02). All

parameter estimates reported on Figure 2 were significant atpo.05. In the model in Figure 2, three relationships between

Environment and Personality 823

personality and family environment were identified, each mediated

entirely genetically: a negative relationship between NegativeEmotionality and Cohesion, a positive relationship between Con-straint and Cohesion, and a positive relationship between Positive

PEM NEM CON COH STA

G1 G2 G3

E1 E2 E3 E4 E5

0.690.61

0.79 0.72 0.63 0.92 0.96

0.28

-0.31 0.26

0.78

Figure2Best-fitting, multivariate model of the association between factors ofpersonality and environmental factors. Coefficients on the diagramare standardized and significant at p o .05. G5additive geneticeffects, E5nonshared environmental effects. PEM5positive emotion-ality, NEM5negative emotionality, CON5constraint, COH5cohe-sion, STA5 status. The percentage of variance a given variableaccounts for in another variable can be determined by squaring thepath coefficient on the path connecting the first to the second

variable.

824 Krueger et al.

Emotionality and Status. Table 2 expresses these relationships

between MPQ and environmental factors from the model in Figure 2as genetic and environmental correlations.

Most of the variance in each of the two environmental variables(.9225 84% of the variance in Cohesion, and .9625 92% of the

variance in Status) was due to unique nonshared environmentalfactors, the remaining variance being due to genetic factors shared

with the personality scales (16% for cohesion, and 8% for Status).Approximately 60% (� .312/.16) of the heritable variance of

Cohesion was shared with Negative Emotionality (genetic correla-tion, or rg5 � .77), the other approximately 40% (.262/.16) of theheritable variance being shared with Constraint (rg5 .64). All of the

heritable variance in Status was shared with Positive Emotionality(rg5 1.00). The estimated heritability of Positive Emotionality was

37% (.612), the estimated heritability of Negative Emotionalitywas 48% (.692), and the estimated heritability of Constraint was

61% (.782). Nonshared environmental variance in each of thepersonality factors was unique to that factor.

DISCUSSION

One fundamental finding from behavior genetic research has been

that measures of family environments are heritable (Plomin &Bergeman, 1991; Plomin, 1994). However, the meaning of this

finding has been less clear. What does the observation that genesinfluence the way people describe their family environments mean?

In our study, reared-apart twins completed extensive measures oftheir recall of the family environments in which they were raised, as

well as an omnibus measure of adult personality (the MPQ).According to our results, the connections between these measures

were genetically mediated, thereby explaining the meaning of theheritability of measures of the family environment. In other words,the heritability of family environmental measures was due to the

heritability of personality. Genotypes that led to individualdifferences in adult personality extended beyond traditional

personality measures, into indices of the way people remembertheir family rearing environments.

More specifically, genetic influences on recall of a cohesive familyenvironment were accounted for by genetic influences on constraint

Environment and Personality 825

and a lack of negative emotionality. Genetic effects on both

constraint and a lack of negative emotionality made similarcontributions to explaining the heritability of cohesion, accounting

for 40% and 60% of the heritable variance in cohesion, respectively.In addition, the personality factor of positive emotionality

accounted for 100% of the genetic influence on recall of familyenvironments high in socioeconomic, intellectual, and cultural

status. In sum, the genetic factors that lead adults to constraintheir expression of affect and impulse and to lack a propensity

toward experiencing negative emotions appear to be the samegenetic factors that lead adults to recall a cohesive family rearingenvironment. Similarly, the genetic factors that lead adults to

experience frequent positive emotions appear to be the same geneticfactors that lead adults to recall a family rearing environment high

in socioeconomic, intellectual, and cultural status.In considering these results, some key issues regarding the design

of the study should be borne in mind. The reared-apart status of ourtwins is one such issue. One potential weakness of the reared-apart

twin design concerns limitations on tractable sample sizes, due to therareness of twins being reared apart. Owing to our modest samplesize, we limited our analyses to the three higher-order factors of the

MPQ, rather than the eleven primary MPQ scales. Future researchcould endeavor to document genetic and environmental connections

between personality and recalled family environments at a morefine-grained level of the personality trait hierarchy in larger, reared-

together twin samples.Nevertheless, the reared-apart status of our twins is also a

strength of our design in terms of conceptual clarity. That is, ourtwins reported on the different family environments in which they

were raised. Along these lines, it is interesting to note that themajority of the variance in our ‘‘environmental’’ measures was, infact, traceable to the nonshared environments of the twins (see

Figure 2). This means that, to a large extent, twins reported to usthat they were raised in different family environments, a point we

will return to later. Nevertheless, to the extent that twins’ reportswere similar, within pairs, the degree of similarity was predicted by

the genetic relationship between the twins (i.e., family environmentalreports were heritable).

This raises another issue that must also be carefully considered:The retrospective nature of our twins’ accounts of their family

826 Krueger et al.

rearing environments. Clearly, the precise veridicality of retro-

spective recall of family environments can be questioned (Halverson,1988). It is likely that what our participants reported about their

family-rearing circumstances contained a mixture of both objectiveand subjective elements. Hence, our results are consistent with two

interpretations. First, if twins’ reports of their rearing circumstancesare more subjective, the ability of genetic effects on personality to

account for genetic effects on family environment measures mightreflect the way adult personality colors the way we remember how

we were raised. Second, to the extent that twins’ reports of theirrearing circumstances are more objective, the ability of geneticeffects on personality to account for genetic effects on family

environment measures might reflect genotype–environment correla-tions. From this perspective, the actual, experienced environments of

the twins might have been influenced by their personalities. Forexample, persons who are genetically predisposed towards having

calm, controlled personalities may promote cohesion among familymembers, thereby explaining why genetic effects on negative

emotionality and constraint account for genetic effects on recalledfamily cohesion.

The ideal experiment to compare these two interpretations is not

ethical to conduct; one cannot rear infant twins apart in system-atically different circumstances, measure those circumstances as they

occur, and follow the twins to adulthood. Nevertheless, our resultsdo advance the current literature because, to date, it has remained

unclear why measures of the family environment are heritable. Ourresults solve this puzzle by indicating that genetic influences on

measures of recalled family environment can be accounted for bygenetic influences on personality. Yet, as alluded to above, the

results leave another parallel puzzle unsolved: What is the meaningof the nonshared environmental variance in personality? Our twinswere reared in different family environments, and the majority of the

variance in our environmental measures was, in fact, nonshared.That is, the twins reported significant differences in their rearing

family environments. Yet the nonshared environmental variance inthe environmental measures did not predict nonshared environ-

mental variance in personality. The differences in the twins’accounts of their rearing environments did not predict differences

in their personalities. So where, then, do these apparent personalitydifferences come from?

Environment and Personality 827

This is a fundamental question, but answers to this question

remain elusive. In addition to the current research, other studieshave also attempted to identify the source of the nonshared

environmental variance in outcomes such as personality. Yet arecent meta-analytic review of these studies concluded that the

median effect size for studies attempting to link measured nonsharedenvironmental variables to measured outcomes was extremely small

(Turkheimer & Waldron, 2000). It may be that a permanent causalimpact of objectively non-shared environmental events on person-

ality is weak or nonexistent; this possibility is compatible with thefindings reported here.

In interpreting the null findings of their meta-analysis, Turkhei-

mer and Waldron (2000) offer the explanation that complex,nonlinear processes may be responsible for the differences in

outcome currently categorized as nonshared environment. Certainly,this is a possibility that should be considered, but other possibilities

also exist. For example, nonshared environmental effects arereduced, and genetic effects increased, when data from multiple

reporters (e.g., both self and peers) are modeled simultaneously(Bouchard & Loehlin, 2001; Heath, Neale, Kessler, Eaves, &Kendler, 1992; Reiss, Neiderhiser, Hetherington, & Plomin, 2000;

Riemann, Angleitner, & Strelau, 1997). It may be that the nonsharedenvironment reflects specificity of viewpoint, rather than persistent

individual differences in personality.We suggest another related possibility that might be considered in

future research on the link between environments and personality.Perhaps, in addition to specificity of viewpoint, the nonshared

environmental variance in personality reflects temporary fluctuationin personality around a heritable set-point (cf. Dickens & Flynn,

2001). From this perspective, differences between twins’ personal-ities measured at a specific point in time might reflect the impact ofuncontrollable aspects of proximal environments on personality.

Along these lines, although measures of more controllable life eventsare heritable, measures of less controllable life events are less

heritable (Kendler, Neale, Kessler, Heath, & Eaves, 1993; McGue,Bouchard, Lykken, & Finkel, 1991; Plomin, Lichtenstein, Pedersen,

McClearn, & Nesselroade, 1990). Instead, measures of lesscontrollable life events consist primarily of nonshared environ-

mental variance (i.e., unlike genes and shared family environments,they are unique to specific persons). Future research might examine

828 Krueger et al.

the extent to which the nonshared environmental variance in recent

and less controllable life events is correlated with the nonsharedenvironmental variance in personality measured at a specific point in

time.In sum, psychologists have often conceived of the environment as

having an indelible impact on people. The finding that many adultoutcomes are weakly influenced by environmental factors shared

among persons reared in the same families represents a fundamentalchallenge to this viewpoint (Rowe, 1994). This finding has led to a

search for other kinds of environmental factors—those not sharedamong family members—that are also presumed to have relativelyindelible impacts on individual differences. Yet, as described above,

this research has produced little in the way of specific, measured,nonshared environmental events that predict the nonshared

environmental part of personality.Perhaps these findings can be understood by conceiving of people

as more flexible and more responsive to the proximal environmentthan traditionally thought. Although the neural systems that

undergird personality are built using the information encoded inour genes (Krueger & Markon, 2002), these systems are not designedto produce behavioral stereotypy. Indeed, behavioral inflexibility

suggests that something is wrong with the way these systems arefunctioning (i.e., that a person’s personality has become disordered;

cf. Depue & Lenzenweger, 2001). Future research might thereforefocus on a conception of the environment as proximal and

temporary in its effects in attempting to solve the puzzle ofnonshared environmental variance in adult personality.

REFERENCES

Akaike, H. (1987). Factor analysis and AIC. Psychometrika, 52, 317–332.

Baker, L. A., & Daniels, D. (1990). Nonshared environmental influences and

personality differences in adult twins. Journal of Personality and Social

Psychology, 58, 103–110.

Billig, J. P., Hershberger, S. L., Iacono, W. G., & McGue, M. (1996). Life events

and personality in late adolescence: Genetic and environmental relations.

Behavior Genetics, 26, 543–554.

Block, J. (1971). Lives through time. Berkeley: Bancroft.

Bouchard, T. J., Jr. (1994). Genes, environment, and personality. Science, 264,

1700–1701.

Environment and Personality 829

Bouchard, T. J., Jr., & Loehlin, J. C. (2001). Genes, evolution, and personality.

Behavior Genetics, 31, 243–273.

Bouchard, T. J., Jr., & McGue, M. (1990). Genetic and rearing environmental

influences on adult personality: An analysis of adopted twins reared apart.

Journal of Personality, 58, 263–292.

Briggs, P. F., Rouzer, D. L., Hamberg, R. L., & Holman, T. R. (1972). Seven

scales for the Minnesota-Briggs History Record with reference group data.

Journal of Clinical Psychology, 28, 431–448.

Browne, M. W., & Cudeck, R. (1993). Alternative ways of assessing model fit. In

K. A. Bollen & J. S. Long (Eds.), Testing structural equation models (pp. 136–

162). Newbury Park, CA: Sage.

Caspi, A., Begg, D., Dickson, N., Harrington, H., Langley, J., Moffitt, T. E., &

Silva, P. A. (1997). Personality differences predict health-risk behaviors in

young adulthood: Evidence from a longitudinal study. Journal of Personality

and Social Psychology, 73, 1052–1063.

Chipuer, H. M., Plomin, R., Pedersen, N. L., McClearn, G. E., & Nesselroade, J. R.

(1993). Genetic influence on family environment: The role of personality.

Developmental Psychology, 29, 110–118.

Collins, W. A., Maccoby, E. E., Steinberg, L., Hetherington, E. M., & Bornstein,

M. H. (2000). Contemporary research on parenting: The case for nature and

nurture. American Psychologist, 55, 218–232.

Deater-Deckard, K., Fulker, D. W., & Plomin, R. (1999). A genetic study of the

family environment in the transition to early adolescence. Journal of Child

Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines, 40, 769–775.

Dempster, A. P., Laird, N. M., & Rubin, D. B. (1977). Maximum likelihood from

incomplete data via the EM algorithm. Journal of the Royal Statistical Society:

Series B, 39, 1–38.

Depue, R. A., & Lenzenweger, M. F. (2001). A neurobehavioral dimensional

model. In W. J. Livesley (Ed.), Handbook of Personality Disorders (pp. 136–

176). New York: Guilford.

Dickens, W. T., & Flynn, J. R. (2001). Heritability estimates versus large

environmental effects: The IQ paradox resolved. Psychological Review, 108,

346–369.

DiLalla, D. L., Carey, G., Gottesman, I. I., & Bouchard, T. J. (1996). Heritability

of MMPI personality indicators of psychopathology in twins reared apart.

Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 105, 491–499.

Efron, B., & Tibshirani, R. J. (1998). An introduction to the bootstrap. Boca

Raton, FL: Chapman & Hall.

Fowler, P. C. (1981). Maximum likelihood factor structure of the Family

Environment Scale. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 37, 160–164.

Halverson, C. F., Jr. (1988). Remembering your parents: Reflections on the

retrospective method. Journal of Personality, 56, 435–443.

Halverson, C. F., Jr., & Wampler, K. S. (1997). Family influences on personality

development. In R. Hogan, J. Johnson, & S. Briggs (Eds.), Handbook of

Personality Psychology (pp. 241–267). New York: Academic Press.

Harkness, A. R., Tellegen, A., & Waller, N. (1995). Differential converg-

ence of self-report and informant data for Multidimensional Personality

830 Krueger et al.

Questionnaire traits: Implications for the construct of Negative Emotionality.

Journal of Personality Assessment, 64, 185–204.

Heath, A. C., Neale, M. C., Kessler, R. C., Eaves, L. J., & Kendler, K. S. (1992).

Evidence for genetic influences on personality from self-reports and informant

ratings. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 63, 85–96.

Hoffman, L. (1991). The influence of the family environment on personality:

Accounting for sibling differences. Psychological Bulletin, 110, 187–203.

Hur, Y.-M., & Bouchard, T. J., Jr. (1995). Genetic influences on perceptions of

childhood family environment: A reared apart twin study. Child Development,

66, 330–345.

Ihaka, R., & Gentleman, R. (1996). R: A language for data analysis and graphics.

Journal of Computational and Graphical Statistics, 5, 299–314.

Jang, K. L., Vernon, P. A., & Livesley, W. J. (2000). Personality disorder traits,

family environment, and alcohol misuse: A multivariate behavioural genetic

analysis. Addiction, 95, 873–888.

Kendler, K. S. (1996). Parenting: A genetic-epidemiologic perspective. American

Journal of Psychiatry, 153, 11–20.

Kendler, K. S., Neale, M., Kessler, R., Heath, A., & Eaves, L. (1993). A twin

study of recent life events and difficulties. Archives of General Psychiatry, 50,

789–796.

Krueger, R. F. (2000). Phenotypic, genetic, and non-shared environmental

parallels in the structure of personality: A view from the Multidimensional

Personality Questionnaire. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 79,

1057–1067.

Krueger, R. F., Caspi, A., Moffitt, T. E., Silva, P. A., & McGee, R. (1996).

Personality traits are differentially linked to mental disorders: A multitrait-

multidiagnosis study of an adolescent birth cohort. Journal of Abnormal

Psychology, 105, 299–312.

Krueger, R. F., & Markon, K. E. (2002). Behavior genetic perspectives on clinical

personality assessment. In J. N. Butcher (Ed.), Clinical Personality Assessment

(pp. 40–55). New York: Oxford University Press.

Loehlin, J. C. (1992). Genes and environment in personality development. Newbury

Park, CA: Sage.

Losoya, S. H., Callor, S., Rowe, D. C., & Goldsmith, H. H. (1997). Origins of

familial similarity in parenting: A study of twins and adoptive siblings.

Developmental Psychology, 33, 1012–1023.

Markon, K. E., Krueger, R. F., Bouchard, T. J., Jr., & Gottesman, I. I. (2002).

Normal and abnormal personality traits: Evidence for genetic and environ-

mental relationships in the Minnesota Study of Twins Reared Apart. Journal

of Personality, 70, 661–693.

McGue, M., & Bouchard, T. J., Jr. (1984). Adjustment of twin data for the effects

of age and sex. Behavior Genetics, 14, 325–343.

McGue, M., Bouchard, T. J., Jr., Lykken, D. T., & Finkel, D. (1991). On

genes, environment, and experience. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 14,

400–401.

Moos, R. H., & Moos, B. S. (1986). Family Environment Scale Manual. Palo Alto,

CA: Consulting Psychologists Press.

Environment and Personality 831

Neale, M. C., Boker, S. M., Xie, G., & Maes, H.H (1999). Mx: Statistical

modeling (5th ed.). Box 126 MCV, Richmond, VA 23298: Department of

Psychiatry.

Neale, M. C., & Miller, M. B. (1997). The use of likelihood-based confidence

intervals in genetic models. Behavior Genetics, 27, 113–120.

O’Connor, T. G., Hetherington, E. M., Reiss, D., & Plomin, R. (1995). A twin-

sibling study of observed parent-adolescent interactions. Child Development,

66, 812–829.

Perusse, D., Neale, M. C., Heath, A. C., & Eaves, L. J. (1994). Human parental

behavior: Evidence for genetic influence and potential implications for gene-

culture transmission. Behavior Genetics, 24, 327–335.

Plomin, R. (1994). Genetics and experience: The interplay between nature and

nurture. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.

Plomin, R., & Bergeman, C. S. (1991). The nature of nurture: Genetic influence

on ‘‘environmental’’ measures. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 14, 373–427.

Plomin, R., Lichtenstein, P., Pedersen, N. L., McClearn, G. E., & Nesselroade, J.

R. (1990). Genetic influence on life events during the last half of the life span.

Psychology and Aging, 5, 25–30.

Plomin, R., McClearn, G. E., Pedersen, N.L, Nesselroade, J. R., & Bergeman, C.

S. (1988). Genetic influence on childhood family environment perceived

retrospectively from the last half of the life span. Developmental Psychology,

24, 738–745.

Plomin, R., McClearn, G. E., Pedersen, N.L, Nesselroade, J. R., & Bergeman, C.

S. (1989). Genetic influence on adults’ ratings of their current family

environment. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 51, 791–803.

Plomin, R., Reiss, D., Hetherington, E. M., & Howe, G. W. (1994). Nature and

nurture: Genetic contributions to measures of the family environment.

Developmental Psychology, 30, 32–43.

Reiss, D., Neiderhiser, J. M., Hetherington, E. M., & Plomin, R. (2000). The

relationship code: Deciphering genetic and social influences on adolescent

development. Cambridge MA: Harvard University Press.

Riemann, R., Angleitner, A., & Strelau, J. (1997). Genetic and environmental

influences on personality: A study of twins reared together using the self- and

peer report NEO-FFI scales. Journal of Personality, 65, 449–475.

Rowe, D. C. (1981). Environmental and genetic influences on dimensions of

perceived parenting: A twin study. Developmental Psychology, 17, 203–208.

Rowe, D. C. (1983). A biometrical analysis of perceptions of family environment:

A study of twin and singleton sibling kinships. Child Development, 54, 416–

423.

Rowe, D. C. (1994). The limits of family influence: Genes, experience, and behavior.

New York: Guilford.

Rowe, D. C. (1997). Genetics, temperament, and personality. In R. Hogan, J.

Johnson, & S. Briggs (Eds.), Handbook of personality psychology (pp. 367–

386). New York: Academic Press.

Saudino, K. J., Pedersen, N. L., Lichtenstein, P., McClearn, G. E., & Plomin, R.

(1997). Can personality explain genetic influences on life events? Journal of

Personality and Social Psychology, 72, 196–206.

832 Krueger et al.

Tellegen, A. (1985). Structures of mood and personality and their relevance to

assessing anxiety with an emphasis on self-report. In A. H. Tuma & J. D.

Maser (Eds.), Anxiety and the anxiety disorders (pp. 681–706). Hillsdale, NJ:

Erlbaum.

Tellegen, A. (2000). Manual of the Multidimensional Personality Questionnaire.

University of Minnesota Press: Minneapolis, MN.

Tellegen, A., Lykken, D. T., Bouchard, T. J., Wilcox, K. J., Segal, N. L., & Rich,

S. (1988). Personality similarity in twins reared apart and together. Journal of

Personality and Social Psychology, 54, 1031–1039.

Tellegen, A., & Waller, N. (in press). Exploring personality through test

construction: Development of the Multidimensional Personality Question-

naire. In S. R. Briggs & J. M. Cheek (Eds.), Personality measures: Development

and evaluation (Vol. 1). Greenwich, CT: JAI Press.

Turkheimer, E., & Waldron, M. (2000). Nonshared environment: A theoretical,

methodological, and quantitative review. Psychological Bulletin, 126, 78–108.

Vernon, P. A., Jang, K. L., Harris, J. A., & McCarthy, J. M. (1997).

Environmental predictors of personality differences: A twin and sibling study.

Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 72, 177–183.

Wachs, T. D. (1993). The nature-nurture gap: What we have here is a failure to

collaborate. In R. Plomin & G. E. McClearn (Eds.), Nature, Nurture, and

Psychology (pp. 375–391). Washington, DC: American Psychological Associa-

tion.

Waller, N. G., Benet, V., & Farney, D. L. (1994). Modeling person-situation

correspondence over time: A study of 103 evangelical disciple-makers. Journal

of Personality, 62, 177–197.

White, T. L., & Depue, R. A. (1999). Differential association of traits of fear and

anxiety with norepinephrine- and dark-induced pupil reactivity. Journal of

Personality and Social Psychology, 77, 863–877.

Environment and Personality 833

834