Rev neurocogniciòn en TB II

download Rev neurocogniciòn en TB II

of 10

Transcript of Rev neurocogniciòn en TB II

  • 8/8/2019 Rev neurocognicin en TB II

    1/10

    Review

    Meta-analytic review of neurocognition

    in bipolar II disorder

    Introduction

    The clinical profile of bipolar II disorder andits relationship with bipolar I disorder and uni-polar disorder remains an area of active explo-ration. Endophenotypes, such as cognition, area potentially valuable differentiating marker.A substantial number of studies have found

    cognitive dysfunction in bipolar disorder (BD)(16). As confirmed by several meta-analyses,cognitive deficits in BD persist even in euthymicpatients (79). In addition to findings fromstudies in remitted patients, evidence regardingcognitive deficits in relatives of affected patientssuggests that cognitive deficits could be traitcharacteristics of BD (8, 10, 11).

    Bora E, Yu cel M, Pantelis C, Berk M. Meta-analytic review ofneurocognition in bipolar II disorder.

    Objective: The clinical distinction between bipolar II disorder (BD II)and bipolar I disorder (BD I) is not clear-cut. Cognitive functioningoffers the potential to explore objective markers to help delineate thisboundary. To examine this issue, we conducted a quantitative reviewof the cognitive profile of clinically stable patients with BD II incomparison with both patients with BD I and healthy controls.

    Method: Meta-analytical methods were used to compare cognitivefunctioning of BD II disorder with both BD I disorder and healthycontrols.Results: Individuals with BD II were less impaired than those with BDI on verbal memory. There were also small but significant difference invisual memory and semantic fluency. There were no significantdifferences in global cognition or in other cognitive domains. Patientswith BD II performed poorer than controls in all cognitive domains.Conclusion: Our findings suggest that with the exception of memoryand semantic fluency, cognitive impairment in BD II is as severe as inBD I. Further studies are needed to investigate whether more severedeficits in BD I are related to neurotoxic effects of severe manicepisodes on medial temporal structures or neurobiological differencesfrom the onset of the illness.

    E. Bora1

    , M. Ycel1,2

    , C. Pantelis1

    ,

    M. Berk2,3,4

    1Melbourne Neuropsychiatry Centre, Department of

    Psychiatry, The University of Melbourne and Melbourne

    Health, Carlton, 2Orygen Research Centre, Melbourne,3Department of Clinical and Biomedical Sciences,

    Barwon Health, The University of Melbourne, Geelong

    and4Mental Health Research Institute, Melbourne and

    Deakin University, Geelong, Vic., Australia

    Key words: bipolar disorder; type II; cognition;

    neuropsychology; meta-analysis

    Emre Bora, Melbourne Neuropsychiatry Centre,

    Department of Psychiatry, The University of Melbourne

    and Melbourne Health, Alan Gilbert Building NNF level

    3, Carlton, Vic. 3053, Australia.

    E-mail: [email protected]; [email protected]

    Accepted for publication October 25, 2010

    Summations

    The most important cognitive difference between BD I and BD II is poorer memory performancein BD I.

    Clinical, demographic, and antipsychotic use do not explain these cognitive differences. Individuals with BD II are also cognitively impaired outside depressive episodes.

    Considerations

    Some studies do not report data for meta-regression analyses.

    More studies are needed to make a more definitive neurocognitive comparison between BD I and BDII.

    Acta Psychiatr Scand 2010: 110All rights reservedDOI: 10.1111/j.1600-0447.2010.01638.x

    2010 John Wiley & Sons A/S

    ACTA PSYCHIATRICASCANDINAVICA

    1

  • 8/8/2019 Rev neurocognicin en TB II

    2/10

    One of the weaknesses in neurobiologicalresearch in a psychiatric context is the neglect ofheterogeneity within the various syndromes. This isalso the case with regard to the investigation intocognitive dysfunction in BD, because the majorityof neuropsychological studies have examined BDas a homogeneous category. In fact, heterogeneity

    of BD has been acknowledged in DSM-IV bydividing the disorder into subtypes: bipolar Idisorder I (BD I), bipolar II disorder (BD II),bipolar NOS, and cyclothymia. The main diagnos-tic difference between BD I and BD II is theseverity of elevated mood; while BD II involves lesssevere hypomanic episodes, BD I involves moresevere manic episodes with greater functionalimpairment. Depression, which is the majorburden in both BP I and BP II, is equally severe.Neglecting subtypes of BD might be relativelyunimportant if BD II is just a milder form of BD.

    In that case, we would expect to find less pro-nounced but qualitatively similar neurobiologicalfindings in BD II. However, BD II cannot beunderstood simply as a milder form of BD I,because there are other clinical differences betweenthese syndromes. BD II is a more chronic conditioncharacterized by more frequent depressive episodeswith shorter euthymic periods than BD I (1216).The clinical profile of BD II is spectrally related tounipolar depression, because BD II is characterizedby recurrent depressions and relatively mild epi-sodes with elation in mood. Moreover, familystudies also suggest that BD II is the most common

    diagnosis among the relatives of patients with BDII (13, 1719).

    To date, most cognitive studies have focused onBD I, and in some of these studies, patients withBD II have been included together with patientswith BD I. However, a number of recent studieshave compared the cognitive profiles of BD I andBD II. The findings from these studies have beenlargely contradictory; some found better cognitiveabilities in BD II (20, 21), while others foundgreater impairment in BD II (22, 23). Thesecontradictory findings might be related to small

    samples sizes and differences in the clinical char-acteristics of cohorts. Clinical state is particularlyimportant, because unlike the studies in stablepatients, studies in depressive episodes reportedmore severe cognitive impairment in BD II. Meta-analytical methods offer the means to help over-come these limitations.

    Aims of the study

    Our primary aim was to compare the neurocogni-tive profiles of clinically stable bipolar I disorder

    and bipolar II disorder (BD II) using meta-analyt-ical methods. We also systematically reviewedstudies comparing the cognitive abilities of clini-cally stable individuals with BD II with healthycontrols.

    Material and methods

    Study selection

    Potential articles were identified through a litera-ture search using Pubmed, Scopus, and Psychinfoduring the period between 1987 and July 2009. Forthe literature search, combinations of the followingkeywords were used: bipolar disorder, manic-depress*, mania, cognit*, neuropsycholog*. Thereference lists of the published articles were alsocross-referenced and reviewed.

    The following criteria were used to select studiesfor review: i) articles assessed the cognitive abilitiesof patients with BD II using reliable neuropsycho-logical testing methods and were published inEnglish peer-reviewed journals; ii) included BD Ior healthy subjects as controls; iii) reported testscores (mean and SD) of both groups or effect sizesof group comparisons, and iv) patients are clini-cally stable (sample is entirely in remission orincludes stable patients with minimal symptoms).While our initial literature search revealed over1000 articles, there were only 19 studies (2038)that met the first three inclusion criteria (Table 1),and five of these studies were excluded because

    assessment was conducted in depressive phase (22,23, 25, 36, 37) (criterion 4). Furthermore, anothertwo of these studies were not used in the meta-analytic calculations because they were based onoverlapping samples with other studies (35, 38)leaving 12 studies in the current meta-analysis.

    In addition to cognitive data, we recordedinformation concerning the sample characteristicsand potential moderator variables. The clinicalvariables included depression and mania scales,history of psychosis, percentage of antipsychoticuse, age of onset, and duration of illness. We also

    coded studies into two groups depending onwhether they used strict criteria for euthymia todefine clinical stability. Demographic variablesincluded mean and SD of age and years ofeducation for both groups. Gender was coded aspercentage of males. These variables are used in themeta-regression and subgroup analyses describedlater.

    Similar to other meta-analyses in BD, wecombined data across very similar tests in someinstances. For example, we combined differenttasks of list learning such as the Rey Auditory

    Bora et al.

    2

  • 8/8/2019 Rev neurocognicin en TB II

    3/10

    Table1.

    CharacteristicsofstudiesthatcomparecognitiveabilitiesofBDIIwithBDIandorhealth

    ycontrols

    Sample

    Clinicalfeatures

    Historyofpsychosis

    Antipsychotics

    Cogn

    itivemeasures

    Studiesincludedin

    meta-analyses

    Glahnetal.(24)

    21BDI

    10BDII

    17HC

    Pediatricsample

    Clinicallystableout-patientslivingwithpa

    rents

    Mixtureofpatientsinremissionandpatie

    ntswith

    depressiveandmanicsymptoms

    Noinformation

    57%

    BDI

    10%

    BDIIareon

    atypicals

    CVLT,

    PM

    Torrentetal.(20)

    38BDI

    33BDII

    35HC

    Euthymic

    81%

    BDI

    18%

    BDIIpsychosis+

    50%

    BDI

    27%

    BDIIareon

    atypicals

    WCST,Stroop,

    TMT-A

    ,TMT-B,

    Digitsforward,

    digits

    backward,semantic

    fluency,phoneticfluency,

    CVLT

    Anderssonetal.(26)

    25BDII

    28HC

    Mixtureofpatientsinremissionandpatie

    ntswith

    mildtomoderatedepressivesymptoms

    Noinformation

    8%

    BDIIareonatypicals

    PASAT,Stroop,symb

    olcoding,

    CVLT,

    Reycomplex

    figurerecall,phone

    ticfluency

    Dittmannetal.(27)

    65BDI

    38BDII

    62HC

    Euthymic

    72%

    BDI

    26%

    BDIIpsychosis+

    51%

    BDI

    24%

    BDIIareon

    atypicals

    TMT-A,symbolcodin

    g,

    digitsforward,

    figurerecall,

    storyrecall,

    listrecall,

    LNS,

    TMT,semanticfluency

    Savitzetal.(29)

    49BDI

    19BDII

    44UD

    Euthymic

    Noinformation

    N

    oinformation

    Digitsforward,

    digits

    backward,

    Stroop,phonetic

    fluency,

    RAVLT,

    WCST,Reycomplexfigurerecall

    Simonsenetal.(21)

    42BDI

    31BDII

    124HC

    Mixtureofpatientsinremissionandpatie

    ntswith

    mildtomoderatedepressivesymptoms

    62%

    BDI

    32%

    BDIIpsychosis+

    81%

    BDI

    16%

    BDIIareon

    atypicals

    WMSlogicalmemory,CVLT,

    2-back,

    digitsforward,

    digitsbackward,Stroop,phoneticfluency,

    semanticfluency

    Derntletal.(30)

    26BDI

    36BDII

    62HC

    Euthymic

    58%

    BDI

    28%

    BDIIpsychosis+

    58%

    BDI

    42%

    BDIIareon

    atypicals

    PM,

    facerecognition

    memory

    Haetal.(31)

    22BDI

    29BDII

    29HC

    Mixtureofpatientsinremissionandpatie

    ntswith

    mildtomoderatedepressivesymptoms

    Noinformation

    N

    oinformation

    CVLT,

    WMSlogicalm

    emory,

    Reyfigurerecall,

    CPT,

    phoneticfluency,se

    manticfluency,

    WCST,TMT-B,

    ToL

    Hsiaoetal.(28)

    30BDI

    37BDII

    22HC

    Euthymic

    73%

    BDI

    16%

    BDIIpsychosis+

    N

    oinformation

    TMT-A,

    TMT-B,

    digit

    symbol,WMS-III:logical

    memory,

    VPA,

    listrecognition,

    faceandpicture

    delayedrecall,spatialspan,

    digitspan

    Kungetal.(32)

    22BDI

    29BDII

    20HC

    Remission

    Unclear

    Unclear

    Conners

    CPT

    Ancinetal.(33)

    115BDI

    28BDII

    Euthymic

    Unclear

    Unclear

    VisualCPT

    Morieraetal.(34)

    36BDI

    24BDII

    60HC

    Euthymic

    Unclear

    Unclear

    Reycomplexfigurer

    ecall

    Studiesexcludedfrom

    meta-analyses

    Martinez-Aranetal.(35)

    108BD

    30HCpost-hoc

    analysis

    forBDIvs.B

    D

    II

    Manic,

    depressedandeuthymicpatients

    Unclearforsubgroups

    Unclearforsubgroups

    Post-hoc

    analysisfor

    CVLT

    Cognition and BD II

    3

  • 8/8/2019 Rev neurocognicin en TB II

    4/10

    Table1.

    Continued

    Sample

    Clinicalfeatures

    Historyofpsychosis

    Antipsychotics

    Cogn

    itivemeasures

    HarvakyFriedmanetal.(23)

    32BDI

    19BDII

    58HC

    Suicideattempters,allwereindepresse

    depisode

    Noinformation

    N

    oinformation

    Symbolcoding,

    TMT-

    A,

    TMT-B,

    CPT,Stroop,

    Nback,

    Buschke,

    BentonVR

    T,phoneticfluency,

    finger

    tapping,

    AnotBre

    actiontimetask,reactiontime

    tests,GoNogo,tim

    eestimation

    Summersetal.(22)

    25BDI

    11BDII

    Comparedw

    ith

    Normatived

    ata

    MoredepressedpatientsinBDII(4565%)thanBD

    I(2032%)

    Noinformation

    N

    oinformation

    Facerecognition,

    PAL

    ,Reyfigurerecall,shapestest,

    TMT-B,

    SWM,

    Nod

    ataforothertestsinthestudy

    TaylorTavaresetal.(25)

    17BDII

    22UD

    25HC

    Depression

    Noinformation

    U

    nmedicated

    CANTABtests:PRM,

    SRM,spatialspan,

    SWM,

    DMTS,

    IDED

    Holmesetal.(36)

    65BD(5265BD

    II)

    52HC

    Depressed

    Noinformation

    3

    2unmedicated0%

    anti-

    p

    sychotics

    CANTABtests:PRM,

    SWM,

    RVIP

    Roiseretal.(37)

    49BD(3849BD

    II)

    55HC

    Depressed

    2%

    historyofpsychosisin

    mania,

    fordepressionno

    information

    U

    nmedicated

    CANTABtests:PRM,

    SRM,spatialspan,

    SWM,

    DMTS,

    IDED,

    RVIP,

    reversalLearning

    Simonsenetal.(38)

    80BDI

    61BDII

    280HC

    Mixtureofpatientsinremissionandpatientswith

    mildtomoderatedepressivesymptoms

    80%

    BDI

    30%

    BDIIpsychosis+

    U

    nclearforsubgroups

    WMSlogicalmemory,CVLT,

    2-back,

    digitsforward,

    digitsbackward,Stroop,phoneticfluency,

    semanticfluency

    CVLT,

    Californiaverballearningtest;TMT,trailmakingtest;PAL,pairedassociateslearningtest;LN,

    letternumbersequencingtest;PM,progressivematrices;ToL,towerofLondontest;PRM,patternrecognitionmemory;SRM,spatial

    recognitionmemory;SWM,spatialworkingmemory;DMTS,

    delayedmatchingtosample;VPA,verbalpairassociates;BDI,bipolarIdisorder;BDII,

    bipo

    larIIdisorder;UD,unipolardepression.

    Bora et al.

    4

  • 8/8/2019 Rev neurocognicin en TB II

    5/10

    Verbal Learning Test and the California VerbalLearning Test. In addition to meta-analysesfor individual tasks, we also combined indi-vidual tasks under six cognitive domains ofthe MATRICS (Measurement and TreatmentResearch to Improve Cognition in Schizophrenia)because there were not a sufficient number of

    studies for some individual tasks (3940).Domain scores were calculated by averagingeffect sizes of individual tasks under each cate-gory. The cognitive tasks that were not part ofMATRICS were categorized on relevant domainbased on agreement between authors. We alsocalculated a summary measure, global cognition,by averaging the effect sizes from each cognitivedomain. This measure was only calculated forstudies that at least included two domains. Inaddition to MATRICS domains and summaryscore, we conducted meta-analyses for individual

    tasks when there were three or more studies thatexamined these tasks. In Table 2, classification ofindividual cognitive tasks used in the reviewedstudies according to MATRICS domains isshown (4153).

    Meta-analytical procedure

    For each cognitive test, an effect size and standarderror was calculated. Effect sizes for each cognitivevariable from each study were calculated as themean difference between two groups divided by thepooled standard deviation (Cohens d). Effect sizes

    were weighted using the inverse variance method.We used a random effects model. Homogeneity ofthe resulting mean weighted effect sizes was testedusing the Q-test. The Q-test is computed bysumming the squared deviations of each studyseffect estimate from the overall effect estimate,weighting the contribution of each study by itsinverse variance. Publication bias was assessed byEggers test. Meta-analyses were performed usingmix software (54). Effects of moderator variables on

    observed between-group differences were analyzedby meta-regression analyses. Meta-regression anal-yses were conducted using spss 11.0 (SPSS Inc.,Chicago, IL, USA). These weighted generalizedleast squares regressions were performed using themacros written by David B. Wilson (http://mason.gmu.edu/~dwilsonb/ma.html). Meta-regres-

    sion analyses with random effects modelling wereperformed using the restricted-information maxi-mum likelihood method with a significance level setat P < 0.05.

    Results

    BD I vs. BD II

    Eleven studies that met our inclusion criteria inves-tigated cognitive differences between BD I and BDII. These studies included 444patientswith BD I and285 patients with BD II. The BD I and BD II groupswere well matched for age (d =

    )0.01), duration of

    education (d = )0.05), duration of illness(d = )0.03), and gender (RR = 1.04). Patientswith BD I tended to have a younger onset of illness;however, the between-group difference was notsignificant (d = 0.15, CI = )0.18 to 0.47.z = 0.89, P = 0.37). There were no significantbetween-group differences in depressive (d = 0.12,CI = )0.23 to 0.47, z = 0.69, P = 0.49, k = 8)and manic symptoms (d = )0.14, CI = )0.34 to0.05, z = 1.44, P = 0.15, k = 6). More patientswith BD I were using antipsychotics than patients

    with BD II (RR = 1.80, CI = 1.362.39,Z = 4.05, P < 0.001, k = 5). Five studiesreported data regarding history of psychosis andfound more patients with a history of psychosis inBD I (RR = 3.33, CI = 2.384.67, Z = 6.99,P < 0.001, k = 5).

    Global cognition

    There was a small (d = 0.26) but significantdifference in global cognition between the BD Iand BD II groups: patients with BD II performed

    better than patients with BD I (Table 3). Excludingthe only study with a pediatric sample (20) did notchange the observed findings (d = 0.27). Therewas no evidence of publication bias or heteroge-neous distribution of effect sizes.

    Processing speed

    There was a small but significant difference inprocessing speed, with BD II being better than BDI. The distribution of effect sizes was homoge-neous. Eggers test did not show evidence for

    Table 2. NIMH-MATRICS cognitive domains

    Cognitive domain Cognitive tests

    Processing speed Phonetic fluency (41), semantic fluency (41),

    TMT (42), Stroop (41), symbol coding (43)

    Attention Continuos performance test (44)

    Working memory Digit span (43), LNS (43), spatial span

    (41), PASAT (45)

    Reasoning and problem solving WCST (46), ToL (47), progressive matrices (48)

    Verbal learning and memory WMS logical memory (49), CVLT (50), RAVLT (51)

    Visual learning and memory Rey complex figure (52), shapes test (53), face

    memory (41)

    CVLT, California verbal l earning test; RAVLT, Rey auditory verbal learning test; TMT,

    trail making test; PRM, pattern recognition memory; SRM, spatial recognition

    memory; SWM, spatial working memory; DMTS, delayed matching to sample.

    Cognition and BD II

    5

  • 8/8/2019 Rev neurocognicin en TB II

    6/10

    publication bias. Individual analyses were con-ducted for TMT-A, TMT-B, phonetic fluency,semantic fluency, and the Stroop interference task.Patients with BD I performed significantly poorerthan patients with BD II only in semantic fluency(d = 0.31). There was no evidence for publicationbias or heterogeneity of effect size distributions forthe individual tests.

    Attention

    There was no significant difference in sustainedattention abilities of people with BD I or BD II.Individual task analyses for omission and commis-sion errors were not significantly different either.There was no evidence for significant heterogeneityor publication bias for any of sustained attentionmeasures.

    Visual memory

    Individuals with BD II performed significantlybetter than BD I (d = 0.38) on visual memory.The distribution of effect sizes for visual memorywas heterogeneous. Patients with BD II performedbetter on the complex figure recall task (d = 0.66).There was no evidence for publication bias for thevisual memory domain score and figure recall task.

    Verbal memory

    Patients with BD II performed significantly betterthan BD I in the verbal memory domain. The

    between-group difference had a medium effectsize (d = 0.52). The distribution of effect sizesbecame homogenous (Fig. 1). Individual taskanalyses for list learning, recall, and recognitiontasks showed significantly worse performance forindividuals with BD I (d = 0.480.53). Distribu-tions of effect sizes were homogenous for indi-vidual tasks. There was no evidence forpublication bias for verbal memory domain and

    any individual tasks.

    Working memory

    Working memory (WM) did not differentiatebetween BD I and BD II groups. Distribution ofeffect sizes was homogenous. Egger test was alsonon-significant, indicating that there was no evi-dence for publication bias. Individual task analysesfor digits backward and forward did not reveal anysignificant difference between BD I and BD II.

    Planning and reasoning

    There was no significant between-group differencein planning abilities. The distribution of effect sizeswas homogeneous, and there was no evidence forpublication bias. We were able to undertakeindividual task analysis for the WCST (persevera-tion and number of categories achieved scores),and these analyses similarly did not reveal anysignificant between-group differences. The distri-bution of effect sizes was homogeneous for theseindividual task analyses.

    Table 3. Mean weighted effect sizes for cognitive differences between BD I and BD II

    Test Study BD I BD II D 95% CI Z P Q-test P Bias

    Global cognition 8 293 233 0.26 0.080.44 2.88 0.004 0.85 0.90

    Processing speed 6 246 187 0.28 0.080.48 2.81 0.005 0.54 0.81

    Phonetic fluency 4 151 112 0 )0.25 to 0.25 0.01 0.99 0.86 0.87

    Stroop interference 3 129 83 0.26 )0.05 to 0.58 1.62 0.10 0.29 0.10

    TMT-A 3 130 98 0.08 )0.18 to 0.34 0.59 0.56 0.99 0.50

    TMT-B 4 152 131 0.25)

    0.05 to 0.53 1.65 0.10 0.21 0.69Semantic fluency 4 167 131 0.31 0.080.55 2.59 0.01 0.94 0.43

    Visual memory 6 218 183 0.38 0.080.68 2.45 0.01 0.05 0.12

    Complex figure recall 3 107 72 0.66 0.340.97 4.07

  • 8/8/2019 Rev neurocognicin en TB II

    7/10

    Moderator factor analyses

    Meta-regression analyses were conducted for theclinical (duration of illness, age of onset, between-group differences in depression mania symptoms,relative ratio for patients with psychotic of psy-chosis, and antipsychotic use) and demographic(age, education, and sex) variables. These analysesdid not find any significant association betweenconfounder variables and the magnitude of cogni-tive differences between BD I and BD II.

    BD II vs. controlsNine studies that compared the cognitive abilitiesof patients with BD II with healthy controls wereincluded into the meta-analysis. These studiescompared the cognitive performances of 263patients with BD II and 415 healthy controls. Inthese studies, there were no significant between-group differences in age, years of education, andgender composition.

    We were unable to conduct a meta-analysis forattention because less than three studies examinedcontrol vs. BD II differences. Meta-analysis of the

    other five cognitive domains (d = 0.290.55) andglobal cognition (d = 0.43) found significant cog-nitive deficits for all measures (Table 4). Excludingthe only study with a pediatric sample (20) did notchange the results (d = 0.45). According to meta-regression analyses, clinical and demographic vari-ables did not significantly influence the cognitivedeficits in BD II. The Q-test did not reveal anyheterogeneity for distribution of effect sizes for anyof these measures, nor was there any evidence forpublication bias. We were able to conduct individ-ual meta-analyses for processing speed (phonetic

    fluency, semantic fluency, symbol coding, Stroopinterference, TMT-A, and TMT-B), verbalmemory (list learning, list recall, and list recogni-tion), visual memory (figure recall), and WM(digits forward) measures. In all individual taskanalyses for processing speed (d = 0.460.72) andvisual memory (d = 0.60), patients with BD IIperformed significantly worse than healthy con-trols. There was no evidence for publication bias orheterogeneous distribution of effect sizes for thesedomains. Individual task analyses for three mea-sures of verbal memory showed less pronounceddifferences between patients with BD II and

    healthy controls (d = 0.220.39) and patientswith BD II were significantly more impaired thancontrol subjects only on tasks of list learning. Thedistribution of effect sizes was heterogeneous forlist recall.

    Discussion

    To our knowledge, this is the first meta-analyticstudy of cognition in BD II. In general, ourfindings suggest that BD II is associated withsimilar cognitive deficits as have been reported in

    BD I. However, verbal memory, visual memory,and semantic categorization deficits seem to bemore specifically associated with BD I.

    There could be several potential explanations fora relatively specific impairment of memory in BD I.First, diagnosis-associated secondary features likepsychosis, age of illness onset, and treatment ratherthan diagnoses itself might explain the between-group difference in this cognitive domain, becausethese factors are associated with more cognitivedeficits in BD (6, 5562). However, cognitivedifferences between the BD I and BD II were

    Fig. 1. Forest plot for showing verbal memory differences between BD I and BD II.

    Cognition and BD II

    7

  • 8/8/2019 Rev neurocognicin en TB II

    8/10

    more selective for memory, and the only study thatcontrolled for the effect of a history of psychosis stillfound more impaired verbal memory functioning inBD I (21). Our meta-regression analyses did notfind any relationship between the relative ratio ofhistory of psychosis, age of onset, antipsychotic use,and cognition in BD I and BD II. However, thesemeta-regression analyses are limited because only asmall number of studies reported these variables.Additional studies examining the effect of theseconfounding factors on cognitive deficits in BD I

    and BD II are necessary for further clarifying theseissues. Also, potential effect of other medicationslike lithium, antiepileptics, and antidepressantsshould also be further examined.

    Second, more pronounced memory deficits inBD I could be related to differences in the course ofBD II and BD I. BD I is characterized by moresevere manic mood episodes, and previous studieshave found a relationship between the number ofmanic episodes and verbal memory impairment(61, 62). There is a suggestion that mania is moreneurotoxic than depression and associated with a

    more accelerated process of neuroprogression (63).Recurrent and severe manic episodes might there-fore be associated with progressive deficits inmedial temporal regions, and this could lead tomemory deficits specific to BD I. However, it isalso possible that there might be verbal memorydifferences from the onset of the illness. However,the available evidence is too limited to concludedefinitively whether pronounced verbal memorydeficits in BD I are a result of neurodevelopmentalor neurodegenerative differences between BD I andBD II.

    Outside of memory, a semantic fluency deficitwas the only other cognitive measure that differedbetween BD I and BD II, with worse performancein the former group. This difference is unlikely tobe related to slower processing speed in BD I,because there were no group differences in phoneticfluency. While processing speed is an importantfactor in semantic fluency performance, otherfactors like semantic knowledge and organizationalso contribute to semantic fluency performance.Because temporal lobe, including medial temporal

    lobe, function is important for semantic fluency(64, 65), there might be a common pathophysiol-ogy for verbal memory and semantic fluencydeficits in BD I.

    Our findings suggest that executive functions,attention, and WM might be potential endophe-notypes for both BD I and BD II. These deficitsmight be associated with structural abnormalitieslike anterior cingulate cortex (ACC) volume reduc-tion, which is a consistent finding in BD (66). Ourresults also suggest that memory deficits associatedwith medial temporal lobe abnormalities might be

    a potentially specific endophenotype for BD I.These findings are consistent with a recent studysuggesting that ventromedial frontal and anteriorlimbic gray matter abnormalities are sharedbetween BD I and BD II, and there are additionalgray matter abnormalities in other regions of brainincluding temporal and parahippocampal corticesin BD I (67).

    In conclusion, our findings revealed a differentprofile for certain cognitive deficits between BD Iand BD II, suggesting that there might be someneurobiological differences that underpin these

    Table 4. Mean weighted effect sizes for cognitive differences between controls and BD II

    Test Study HC BD II D 95% CI Z P Q-test P Bias

    Global cognition 8 379 239 0.43 0.250.60 4.87

  • 8/8/2019 Rev neurocognicin en TB II

    9/10

    subtypes of BD. More work is needed tounderstand the relationship between cognitivedeficits and depressive symptoms in BD II.Brain imaging studies might be relevant tofurther demonstrate neurobiological similaritiesand differences between BD I and BD II. Studiesin relatives of patients with BD II and follow-up

    studies are also important to ascertain whethercognitive dysfunction is a trait characteristic ofBD II.

    References

    1. van Gorp WG, Altshuler L, Theberge DC et al. Cognitive

    impairment in euthymic patients with and without prior

    alcohol dependence. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1988;55:4146.

    2. Bora E, Vahip S, Gonul AS et al. Evidence for theory of

    mind deficits in euthymic patients with bipolar disorder.

    Acta Psychiatr Scand 2005;112:110116.

    3. Martnez-Aran A, Vieta E, Colom F et al. Cognitive

    impairment in euthymic bipolar patients: implications for

    clinical and functional outcome. Bipolar Disord2004;6:224232.

    4. Smith DJ, Muir WJ, Blackwood DH. Neurocognitive

    impairment in euthymic young adults with bipolar spec-

    trum disorder and recurrent major depressive disorder.

    Bipolar Disord 2006;8:4046.

    5. Jamrozinski K, Gruber O, Kemmer C, Falkai P, Scherk H.

    Neurocognitive functions in euthymic bipolar patients.

    Acta Psychiatr Scand 2009;119:365374.

    6. Dias VV, Brissos S, Carita AI. Clinical and neurocognitive

    correlates of insight in patients with bipolar I disorder in

    remission. Acta Psychiatr Scand 2008;117:2834.

    7. Robinson LJ, Thompson JM, Gallagher P et al. A meta-

    analysis of cognitive deficits in euthymic patients with

    bipolar disorder. J Affect Disord 2006;93:105115.

    8. Bora E, Yucel M, Pantelis C. Cognitive endophenotypes ofbipolar disorder: a meta-analysis of neuropsychological

    deficits in euthymic patients and their first-degree relatives.

    J Affect Disord 2009;113:120.

    9. Torres IJ, Boudreau VG, Yatham LN. Neuropsychological

    functioning in euthymic bipolar disorder: a meta-analysis.

    Acta Psychiatr Scand Suppl 2007;434:1726.

    10. Quraishi S, Walshe M, Mcdonald C et al. Memory func-

    tioning in familial bipolar I disorder patients and their

    relatives. Bipolar Disord 2009;11:209214.

    11. Bora E, Vahip S, Akdeniz F et al. Executive and verbal

    working memory dysfunction in first-degree relatives of

    patients with bipolar disorder. Psychiatry Res

    2008;161:318324.

    12. Berk M, Dodd S. Bipolar II disorder: a review. Bipolar

    Disord 2005;7:1121.13. Vieta E, Suppes T. Bipolar II disorder: arguments for and

    against a distinct diagnostic entity. Bipolar Disord

    2008;10:163178.

    14. Judd LL, Akishal HS, Schettler PJ et al. A prospective

    investigation of the natural history of the long term weekly

    symptomatic status of bipolar II disorder. Arch Gen Psy-

    chiatry 2003;60:261269.

    15. Ayuso-Gutierrez JL, Ramos-Brieva JA. The course of

    manic-depressive illness. A comparative study of bipolar I

    and bipolar II patients. J Affect Disord 1982;4:914.

    16. Judd LL, Akishal HS, Schettler PJ et al. The comparative

    clinical phenotype and long term longitudinal episode

    course of bipolar I and bipolar II: a clinical spectrum or

    distinct disorder? J Affect Disord 2003;73:1932.

    17. Coryell W, Endicott J, Reich T, Andreasen N, Keller M.

    A family study of bipolar II disorder. Br J Psychiatry

    1984;145:4954.

    18. Andreasen NC, Rice J, Endicott J, Coryell W, Grove WM,

    Reich T. Familial rates of affective disorder. Report from

    the National Institute of Mental Health Collaborative

    Study. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1987;44:461469.

    19. Heun R, Maier W. The distinction of bipolar II disorder

    from bipolar I and recurrent unipolar depression: results of

    a controlled family study. Acta Psychiatr Scand

    1993;87:279284.

    20. Torrent C, Martinez-Aran A, Daban C et al. Cognitive

    impairment in bipolar II disorder. Br J Psychiatry

    2006;189:254259.

    21. Simonsen C, Sundet K, Vaskinn A et al. Neurocognitive

    profiles in bipolar I and bipolar II disorder: differences in

    pattern and magnitude of dysfunction. Bipolar Disord

    2008;10:245255.

    22. Summers M, Papadopoulou K, Bruno S, Cipolotti L, Ron MA.

    Bipolar I and II disorder: cognition and emotion process-

    ing. Psychol Med 2006;36:17991809.

    23. Harvaky-Friedman JM, Keilp JG, Grunebaum MF et al. AreBPI and BPII suicide attempters distinct neuropsycholog-

    ically? J Affect Disord 2006;94:255259.

    24. Glahn DC, Bearden CE, Caetano S et al. Declarative

    memory impairment in pediatric bipolar disorder. Bipolar

    Disord 2005;7:546554.

    25. Taylor-Tavares JV, Clark L, Cannon DM, Erickson K,

    Drevets WC, Sahakian BJ. Distinct profiles of neurocog-

    nitive function in unmedicated unipolar depression and

    bipolar II depression. Biol Psychiatry 2007;62:917924.

    26. Andersson S, Barder HE, Hellvin T, Lvdahl H, Malt UF.

    Neuropsychological and electrophysiological indices of

    neurocognitive dysfunction in bipolar II disorder. Bipolar

    Disord 2008;10:888899.

    27. Dittmann S, Hennig-Fast K, Gerber S et al. Cognitive

    functioning in euthymic bipolar I and bipolar II patients.Bipolar Disord 2008;10:877887.

    28. Hsiao YL, Wu YS, Wu JYW et al. Neuropsychological

    functions in patients with bipolar I and bipolar II disorder.

    Bipolar Disord 2009;11:547554.

    29. Savitz JB, van der Merwe L, Stein DJ, Solms M, Ramesar R.

    Neuropsychological task performance in bipolar spectrum

    illness: genetics, alcohol abuse, medication and childhood

    trauma. Bipolar Disord 2008;10:479494.

    30. Dernt B, Seidel EM, Kryspin-Exner I, Hasmann A, Dobmeier

    M. Facial emotion recognition in patients with bipolar

    I and bipolar II disorder. Br J Clin Psychol 2009;48:

    363375.

    31. Ha K, Choi J, Kim J, Cha B, Ha T. Cognitive impairment in

    patients with bipolar I and bipolar II disorder. Schizophr

    Res 2008;98:186.32. Kung CH, Lee SY, Chang YH et al. Poorer sustained

    attention in bipolar I than bipolar II disorder. Ann Gen

    Psychiatry 2010;9:8.

    33. Ancn I, Santos JL, Teijeira C et al. Sustained attention as a

    potential endophenotype for bipolar disorder. Acta Psy-

    chiatr Scand 2010;122:235245.

    34. Morieara L, Neves FS, Schlottfeldt CG et al. Visual and

    verbal memory in euthymic bipolar patients: impact of

    subtype, psychotic symptoms and suicide behaviour. Clin

    Neuropsychiatr 2010; (in press).

    35. Martnez-Aran A, Vieta E, Reinares M et al. Cognitive

    function across manic or hypomanic, depressed, and

    Cognition and BD II

    9

  • 8/8/2019 Rev neurocognicin en TB II

    10/10

    euthymic states in bipolar disorder. Am J Psychiatry

    2004;161:262270.

    36. Holmes MK, Erickson K, Luckenbaugh DA et al. A com-

    parison of cognitive functioning in medicated and

    unmedicated patients with bipolar depression. Bipolar

    Disord 2008;10:806815.

    37. Roiser JP, Cannon DM, Gandhi SK et al. Hot and cold

    cognition in unmedicated depressed subjects with bipolar

    disorder. Bipolar Disord 2009;11:178189.

    38. Simonsen C, Sundet K, Vaskinn A et al. Neurocognitive

    dysfunction in bipolar and schizophrenia spectrum disor-

    ders depends on history of psychosis rather than diagnostic

    group. Schizophr Bull 2009; (in press) doi:10.1093/schbul/

    sbp034.

    39. Nuechterlein KH, Green MF, Kern RS et al. The MAT-

    RICS consensus cognitive battery, part 1: test selection,

    reliability and validity. Am J Psychiatry 2008;165:203213.

    40. Nuechterlein KH, Barch DM, Gold JM et al. Identification

    of separable cognitive factors in schizophrenia. Schizophr

    Res 2004;72:2939.

    41. Lezak MD. Neuropsychological assessment. New York:

    Oxford University Press, 1995.

    42. Reitan RM. Validity of trail making test as an indication

    of organic brain disease. Percept Mot Skills 1958;8:271276.

    43. Wechsler D. WAIS-III: Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale,

    administration and scoring manual, 3rd edn. San Antonio,

    TX: Psychological Corporation, 1997.

    44. Clark L, Goodwin GM. State and trait related deficits in

    suatained attention in bipolar disorder. Eur Arch Psychi-

    atry Clin Neurosci 2004;254:6168.

    45. Gronwall DM. Paced auditory serial-addition task: a

    measure of recovery from concussion. Percept Mot Skills

    1977;44:367373.

    46. Heaton RK. Wisconsin cart sorting test manual. Odessa,

    FL: Psychological Assessment Resources, Inc, 1981.

    47. Shallice T. Specific impairments of planning. Philos Trans

    R Soc Lond B Biol Sci 1982;298:199209.

    48. van der Ven AHGS, Ellis JL. A Rasch analysis of Ravensstandard progressive matrices. Pers Individ Dif

    2000;29:4564.

    49. Wechsler D. Wechsler memory scale revised manual. San

    Antonio, TX: Psychological Corporation, 1987.

    50. Delis DC, Kramer JH, Kaplan E, Ober BA. California verbal

    learning test: adult version. San Antonio, TX: The Psy-

    chological Corporation, 1987.

    51. Rey A. Lexamen clinique en psychologie. Paris: Presses

    Universitaires de France, 1964.

    52. Rey A. Psychological examination of traumatic encepha-

    lopathy. Arch Psychol 1941;28:286340.

    53. Baddeley A, Emslie H, Nimmo-Smith I. Doors and people test.

    Thames Bury St Edmunds: Valley Test Company, 1994.

    54. Bax L, Yu LM, Ikeda N, Tsuruta H, Moons KGM. Devel-

    opment and validation of MIX: comprehensive free soft-

    ware for meta-analysis of causal research data. BMC Med

    Res Methodol 2006;6:50.

    55. Bora E, Vahip S, Akdeniz F et al. The effect of previous

    psychotic mood episodes on cognitive impairment in eu-

    thymic bipolar patients. Bipolar Disord 2007;9:468477.

    56. Glahn DC, Bearden CE, Barguil M et al. The neurocogni-

    tive signature of psychotic bipolar disorder. Biol Psychia-

    try 2007;62:910916.

    57. Martinez-Aran A, Torrent C, Tabares-Seisdedos R et al.

    Neurocognitive impairment in bipolar patients with and

    without history of psychosis. J Clin Psychiatry 2008;

    69:233239.

    58. Bora E, Yucel M, Fornito A, Berk M, Pantelis C. Major

    psychoses with mixed psychotic and mood symptoms: are

    mixed psychoses associated with different neurobiological

    markers? Acta Psychiatr Scand 2008;118:172187.

    59. Bora E, Yucel M, Pantelis C. Neurocognitive markers of

    psychosis in bipolar disorder: a meta-analytical study. J

    Affect Disord 2010;127:19.

    60. Biffin F, Tahtalian S, Filia K et al. The impact of age atonset of bipolar I disorder on functioning and clinical

    presentation. Acta Neuropsychiatr 2006;18:242243.

    61. Robinson LJ, Ferrier IN. Evolution of cognitive impair-

    ments in bipolar disorder: a systemic review of cross-sec-

    tional evidence. Bipolar Disord 2006;8:103116.

    62. Bora E, Vahip S, Akdeniz F. The role and importance of

    cognitive symptoms in bipolar disorder. Turk Psikiyatri

    Derg 2008;19:8193.

    63. Berk M. Neuroprogression: pathways to progressive brain

    changes in bipolar disorder. Int J Neuropsychopharmacol

    2009;12:441445.

    64. Pihlajamaki M, Tanila H, Hanninen T et al. Verbal fluency

    activates the left medial temporal lobe: a functional mag-

    netic resonance imaging study. Ann Neurol 2000;47:470

    476.65. Rascovsky K, Salmon DP, Hansen LA, Thal LJ, Galasko D.

    Disparate letter and semantic category fluency deficits in

    autopsy-confirmed frontotemporal dementia and Alzhei-

    mers disease. Neuropsychology 2007;21:2030.

    66. Bora E, Fornito A, Yucel M, Pantelis C. Voxelwise meta-

    analysis of gray matter abnormalities in bipolar disorder.

    Biol Psychiatry 2010;67:10971105.

    67. Ha TH, Ha K, Kim JH, Choi JE. Regional brain gray matter

    abnormalities in patients with bipolar II disorder: a com-

    parison study with bipolar I patients and healthy controls.

    Neurosci Lett 2009;456:4448.

    Bora et al.

    10