Regiao Prelimbica Cocaina Preferencia Lugar

download Regiao Prelimbica Cocaina Preferencia Lugar

of 8

Transcript of Regiao Prelimbica Cocaina Preferencia Lugar

  • 7/27/2019 Regiao Prelimbica Cocaina Preferencia Lugar

    1/8

    Research report

    Role of the prelimbic subregion of the medial prefrontal

    cortex in acquisition, extinction, and reinstatement of

    cocaine-conditioned place preference

    Arturo R. Zavala, Suzanne M. Weber, Heather J. Rice,Andrea T. Alleweireldt, Janet L. Neisewander*

    Department of Psychology, Arizona State University, PO Box 871104, Tempe, AZ 85287-1104, USA

    Accepted 30 July 2003

    Abstract

    Previous research suggests that the prelimbic subregion of the medial prefrontal cortex (mPFC) is necessary for acquisition of cocaine-

    conditioned place preference (CPP). Recently, it has been shown that extinguished cocaine-CPP can be reinstated by cocaine priming

    injections, and that this effect reflects the incentive motivational effects of the cocaine prime. To determine whether the prelimbic cortex is

    necessary for cocaine-reinstated CPP, rats received bilateral infusions of quinolinic acid (lesion group) or vehicle (sham group) into the

    prelimbic cortex and were later tested for acquisition, extinction, and reinstatement of cocaine-CPP. Both sham and lesion rats exhibited

    robust CPP established by systemic injections of cocaine (15 mg/kg, ip) following either one or three drug-environment pairings. Following

    repeated exposure to the cocaine- and saline-paired environments, sham and lesion rats showed similar rates of extinction of cocaine-CPP. In

    contrast, reinstatement of cocaine-CPP by cocaine priming injections (5 and 10 mg/kg, ip) was attenuated in rats with prelimbic cortex

    lesions relative to sham controls. This finding suggests that the prelimbic cortex is involved in the incentive motivational effects of cocaine

    priming.

    D

    2003 Elsevier B.V. All rights reserved.

    Theme: Neural basis of behavior

    Topic: Drugs of abuse: cocaine

    Keywords: Cocaine; Conditioned place preference; Prelimbic cortex; Prefrontal cortex; Reinstatement; Incentive motivation

    1. Introduction

    Conditioned place preference (CPP) is a model common-

    ly used to study the rewarding and incentive motivational

    effects of drugs and drug-paired stimuli [23,36]. During

    conditioning, animals receive a drug paired with a distinct

    environment and saline paired with another environment.

    On the test day, animals are given free access to both

    environments in a drug-free state and their preferences for

    drug- versus saline-paired environments are assessed. CPP

    is evident as an increase in preference for the drug-paired

    environment.

    Recently, it has been demonstrated that preference for the

    drug-paired environment can be extinguished and subse-

    quently reinstated by drug priming injections [25,29,39].

    Drug-primed reinstatement of CPP is thought to reflect

    renewed incentive value of the environmental stimuli via

    the incentive motivational effects of the prime [25]. A

    growing number of studies have demonstrated reinstatement

    of CPP using a variety of drugs. For instance, drug priming

    injections have been shown to reinstate CPP in animals

    conditioned with cocaine [13,22,25,31,32] , morphine

    [19,21,26,39,40], methamphetamine [14,18], and ethanol

    [16]. CPP reinstatement is not limited to drug primes,

    however, since presentation of intermittent footshock

    [20,22,39], conditioned fear stimuli [31], or immobilization

    stress [32] are also effective in reinstating CPP.

    Although the neuronal circuitry mediating drug-primed

    and stress-induced reinstatement of CPP has yet to be

    determined, there is evidence suggesting that they are

    mediated by different neuronal circuitries. For example,

    electrolytic lesions of either the nucleus accumbens or

    0006-8993/$ - see front matterD 2003 Elsevier B.V. All rights reserved.

    doi:10.1016/S0006-8993(03)03452-8

    * Corresponding author. Tel.: +1-480-965-0209; fax: +1-480-965-

    8544.

    E-mail address: [email protected] (J.L. Neisewander).

    www.elsevier.com/locate/brainres

    Brain Research 990 (2003) 157 164

  • 7/27/2019 Regiao Prelimbica Cocaina Preferencia Lugar

    2/8

    ventral tegmental area, but not the central amygdala, block

    reinstatement of morphine-CPP by a drug prime, whereas

    only electrolytic lesions of the central amygdala block

    stress-induced reinstatement of morphine-CPP [40]. To date,

    the neuroanatomical basis of cocaine-reinstated CPP has not

    been investigated and, therefore, is the focus of the present

    study.It is likely that the medial prefrontal cortex (mPFC)

    plays a role in reinstatement of cocaine-CPP because

    several lines of evidence suggest that this region is in-

    volved in the reinforcing and incentive motivational effects

    of cocaine. First, preconditioning lesions of the mPFC

    disrupt acquisition of cocaine-CPP [12,38]. Second, rats

    will readily self-administer cocaine directly into the mPFC

    [7,9,10]. Moreover, lesions of the mPFC enhance acquisi-

    tion and maintenance of self-administration with low doses

    of cocaine [41], implicating this region in the reinforcing

    effects of cocaine. Third, intracranial injections of cocaine

    directly into the mPFC reinstate cocaine-seeking behavior

    using the extinction/reinstatement model [28], whereas

    inactivation of the dorsal region of the prefrontal cortex

    by intracranial injections of GABA agonists attenuate

    reinstatement of cocaine-seeking behavior elicited by a

    systemic cocaine injection [24]. These latter findings sug-

    gest that the mPFC is necessary for the response-reinstating

    effects of cocaine.

    Anatomical and functional studies have revealed that the

    mPFC is not a homogeneous structure [15]. For instance,

    recent evidence suggests that a specific subregion of the

    mPFC mediates acquisition of cocaine-CPP. Specifically,

    Tzschentke and Schmidt [37,38] have demonstrated that

    discrete excitotoxic lesions of the prelimbic subregion of themPFC, but not the infralimbic or anterior cingulate sub-

    regions, can disrupt acquisition of cocaine-CPP. The role of

    the prelimbic cortex in the reinstatement of cocaine-CPP has

    yet to be determined. In the present study, we hypothesized

    that the prelimbic cortex plays a role in the rewarding and

    incentive motivational effects of cocaine and predicted that

    excitotoxic lesions of this region would disrupt acquisition

    and reinstatement of cocaine-CPP.

    2. Methods

    2.1. Animals

    Male SpragueDawley rats weighing 225275 g at the

    start of the experiment were housed individually in a

    temperature-controlled colony with a 12-h reversed light/

    dark cycle (lights on at 6:00 PM). Behavioral testing was

    conducted during the rats dark cycle. Food and water

    were available ad libitum. Housing facilities and animal

    care were in accordance with the conditions set forth in

    the National Institute of Health Guide for the Care and

    Use of Laboratory Animals (NIH Publications No. 80-23,

    1996).

    2.2. Apparatus

    CPP was assessed in rectangular Plexiglas chambers that

    had two equal-sized compartments (362430 cm each)

    separated by a removable solid partition. One compartment

    had three walls painted black, cedar bedding beneath a bar

    grid floor, and a fluorescent light located 32 cm above thetop of compartment. The other compartment had three walls

    painted white and pine bedding beneath a wire mesh floor.

    The front wall of the chamber was constructed from clear

    Plexiglas to allow observation of the rats. The tops of the

    chambers were enclosed by a clear Plexiglas cover. A

    second removable partition, which had a small opening

    (812 cm), allowed rats to move freely between compart-

    ments during preference tests.

    2.3. Surgery

    After 5 days of handling, rats were pretreated with

    atropine (10 mg/kg, ip) and anesthetized using sodium

    pentobarbital (50 mg/kg, ip). A standard Kopf stereotaxic

    instrument was then used to localize infusions of quinolinic

    acid (45 nmol, Sigma, St. Louis, MO) or phosphate-buffered

    saline (PBS) into the prelimbic cortex using 30-gauge

    injector cannulae attached to 10-Al Hamilton syringes

    mounted on an infusion pump via PE 10 tubing. Quino-

    linic acid was dissolved in 0.1 M PBS and adjusted to a

    pH of 7.4. Bilateral infusions were made at two sites per

    side with coordinates used previously by Tzschentke and

    Schmidt [37]: +3.2 anteroposterior (AP), F0.8 medio-

    lateral (ML), 4.4 and 3.8 dorsoventral (DV) from

    bregma. For each infusion, the injector was lowered tothe appropriate coordinate and left in place for 1 min

    prior to infusing 0.25 Al of quinolinic acid or PBS over 1

    min. The injector was then left in place for an additional

    5 min following the infusion. After surgery, rats were

    allowed to recover for 1 week, during which they were

    handled daily.

    2.4. Habituation and baseline preference

    Following recovery from surgery, rats were tested for

    baseline preference on three consecutive days. During

    preference tests, rats were given 15 min free access to

    both compartments and the amount of time spent in each

    compartment across the 3 days was computed. The side in

    which animals spent the least amount of time (i.e.,

  • 7/27/2019 Regiao Prelimbica Cocaina Preferencia Lugar

    3/8

    particular side of our apparatus [27]. During conditioning,

    cocaine injections (RTI International, Research Triangle

    Park, NC) were always paired with confinement to the

    rats nonpreferred side in order to enhance sensitivity for

    detecting a shift in preference and for detecting extinction

    of the preference.

    2.5. Conditioned place preference procedure

    The day after baseline preference tests were completed,

    rats underwent conditioning during which they were ex-

    posed to each of the following conditions in 3-day cycles,

    with one condition per day in a counterbalanced order: (1)

    a cocaine injection (15 mg/kg, ip) immediately followed

    by confinement to the nonpreferred compartment; (2) a

    saline injection immediately followed by confinement to

    the preferred compartment; and (3) a saline injection

    immediately followed by confinement to an alternative

    environment. Rats remained in the respective environments

    for 30 min. Exposure to the alternative environment was

    used because follow-up neurochemical experiments are

    planned that will require controls to be placed in an

    alternative environment. The alternative environment was

    an opaque plastic box (363430 cm) that had a solid

    floor and corn cob bedding. Rats in Experiment 1 under-

    went three conditioning cycles, whereas rats in Experiment

    2 underwent only one conditioning cycle. CPP was

    assessed the day after conditioning was completed by

    allowing rats free access to both compartments for 15

    min. Cocaine-CPP was evident when rats exhibited an

    increase in time spent in the cocaine-paired side (i.e., the

    initially nonpreferred compartment) relative to the averagetime spent in that side during the three baseline preference

    tests.

    2.6. Extinction and reinstatement of conditioned place

    preference

    Beginning the day after the test for CPP, rats in

    Experiment 2 underwent 6-day cycles of extinction train-

    ing consisting of daily 30-min exposures to the saline- and

    cocaine-paired compartments without any injections on

    alternating days (i.e., three exposures to each compart-

    ment). Order of exposure to the compartments was coun-

    terbalanced. Tests for extinction of CPP were conducted

    following each extinction cycle (i.e., every 7 days) by

    allowing rats free access to both compartments for 15

    min. Extinction of CPP was operationally defined as a

    decrease in time spent in the cocaine-paired side to within

    40 s of each rats preconditioning baseline. Rats who

    failed to meet this criterion continued extinction training

    for a maximum of 8 weeks. Rats meeting the criterion

    were given a saline priming injection and immediately

    tested for reinstatement of extinguished cocaine-CPP the

    following day. Rats that failed to maintain extinction of

    cocaine-CPP after receiving the saline prime (5 out of 18)

    underwent additional extinction training. Rats that

    remained extinguished after the saline prime were then

    tested the next day for reinstatement of CPP immediately

    after an injection of 5 mg/kg cocaine, ip, and then again

    after 10 mg/kg cocaine, ip, with five rest days in between

    each test.

    2.7. Histology

    After the last test day for each given rat (i.e., after 2

    8 weeks of extinction depending on the rat), the rats were

    deeply anesthetized using sodium pentobarbital (100 mg/kg,

    ip). Their brains were then removed and rapidly frozen in

    20 jC 2-methylbutane. Brains were then stored at70 jC

    until they were later sectioned in a 15 jC cryostat.

    Coronal sections were made at a thickness of 40 Am and

    collected across the range of the prelimbic cortex. Sections

    were thaw mounted on gelatin-coated slides and stained

    with cresyl violet. The extent and placement of the lesion

    was determined by an observer unaware of the rats CPP

    data.

    2.8. Statistical analyses

    Time (s) spent in the cocaine-paired side was analyzed

    using a 22 mixed factor ANOVA with Group (sham/

    lesion) as a between-subjects factor, and Day (baseline/test)

    as a within-subjects factor. For Experiment 2, time spent in

    the cocaine-paired side during the extinction tests was

    analyzed using a 23 mixed factor ANOVA with Group

    (sham/lesion) as a between subjects factor, and Day (CPP

    test/extinction test 1/ extinction test 2) as a within-subjectsfactor. Only data from the first two extinction tests were

    analyzed because all rats were tested at least twice, with

    individual rats extinguishing at different time points there-

    after. Reinstatement data was analyzed using a 23 mixed

    factor ANOVA with Group (sham/lesion) as a between-

    subjects factor, and Prime (saline/5 mg/kg cocaine/10 mg/kg

    cocaine) as a within-subjects factor. When appropriate, post

    hoc analyses were made using Tukey HSD tests (P

  • 7/27/2019 Regiao Prelimbica Cocaina Preferencia Lugar

    4/8

    3.2. Experiment 1: effects of preconditioning prelimbic

    cortex lesions on acquisition of cocaine-conditioned place

    preference with three pairings

    Contrary to our expectations, bilateral lesions of the

    prelimbic cortex failed to alter cocaine-CPP. Both sham

    (n=7) and lesion (n=11) groups demonstrated robust co-

    caine-CPP (see Fig. 3A), evident as an increase in time

    spent in the cocaine-paired side during the test day

    compared to the baseline preference test [Day main effect,

    F(1,16)=24.38; P50% of the test time in the drug-paired

    environment, indicating that conditioning shifted the rats

    preference rather than simply decreasing aversion to their

    initially nonpreferred side. The lack of a lesion effect was

    surprising since lesions of the prelimbic cortex have been

    shown to disrupt cocaine-CPP [37,38]. Thus, in order to

    enhance sensitivity for detecting a lesion effect, Experi-

    ment 2 was conducted using a single drug environment

    pairing based on the rationale that a weaker conditioned

    preference may be more susceptible to lesion-induced

    disruption.

    Fig. 2. Schematic representation of prelimbic cortex lesions. The extent of

    damage is shown on coronal sections from the most posterior to anterior

    portions of the observed lesion. The shaded areas indicate the smallest

    (black) and largest (gray) areas of damage observed. The numbers indicate

    distance from Bregma in millimeters. Schematic figures were adapted from

    Paxinos and Watson [30] with permission from Elsevier.

    Fig. 1. Photomicrographs of coronal sections (Nissl stain) showing repre-

    sentative sections from sham (A) and lesion (B) animals. Arrows point to the

    prelimbic region of the medial prefrontal cortex. Numbers indicate

    approximate distance relative to Bregma.

    A.R. Zavala et al. / Brain Research 990 (2003) 157164160

  • 7/27/2019 Regiao Prelimbica Cocaina Preferencia Lugar

    5/8

    3.3. Experiment 2: effects of preconditioning prelimbic

    cortex lesions on acquisition with a single pairing, extinction,

    and reinstatement of cocaine-conditioned place preference

    Prelimbic cortex lesions again failed to alter cocaine-

    CPP. Both sham (n=8) and lesion (n=10) groups exhibited a

    similar increase in time spent on the cocaine-paired side on

    the CPP test day relative to baseline [Day main effect,

    F(1,16)=72.67; P

  • 7/27/2019 Regiao Prelimbica Cocaina Preferencia Lugar

    6/8

    tion, F(2,28)=3.51; P

  • 7/27/2019 Regiao Prelimbica Cocaina Preferencia Lugar

    7/8

    conducted the experiments during the light phase and

    counterbalanced the side paired with cocaine. Future studies

    will need to determine the significance of these procedural

    differences when assessing the effects of prelimbic cortex

    lesions on the acquisition of cocaine-CPP.

    Prelimbic cortex lesions did not affect the rates of

    extinction of cocaine-CPP (see Fig. 4A). Previous researchfrom our laboratory has found that postconditioning lesions

    of the basolateral amygdala increase resistance to extinction

    of cocaine-CPP, suggesting that lesion animals may be

    unable to process changes in previously learned stimulus

    reward associations [6]. The finding that no change in the

    rate of extinction was observed in the present study is

    surprising given that there are reciprocal connections be-

    tween the basolateral amygdala and prelimbic cortex [11]. In

    fact, a nonsignificant trend toward the opposite effect was

    evident in rats with prelimbic cortex lesions. If anything,

    prelimbic cortex lesions may decrease resistance to (i.e.,

    facilitate) extinction of cocaine-CPP, perhaps reflecting a

    more weakly established CPP relative to controls. Collec-

    tively, these findings suggest that extinction of cocaine-CPP

    is influenced differently by the prelimbic cortex and baso-

    lateral amygdala despite the fact that these regions are

    interconnected.

    In summary, the present findings implicate the prelimbic

    subregion of the mPFC in the reinstatement of extinguished

    cocaine-CPP by priming injections of cocaine. Prelimbic

    cortex lesions likely diminish the incentive motivational

    effects of cocaine priming. Previous research suggests that

    the prelimbic cortex is involved in acquisition of cocaine-

    CPP [37,38]; however, the present findings suggest its role

    may be nonessential.

    Acknowledgements

    We thank Ryan Meyers, Jeffrey Burmeister, Natalie

    Krok, and Kenneth Kirshner for their expert technical

    assistance and Dr. Brock Schroeder for his comments on an

    earlier version of this manuscript. This study was supported

    by the Minority Access to Research Careers program, the

    Howard Hughes Medical Institute Undergraduate Biology

    Enrichment Program, and NIDA Grants DA11064 and

    DA13649.

    References

    [1] S.M. Bell, R.B. Stewart, S.C. Thompson, R.A. Meisch, Food-depri-

    vation increases cocaine-induced conditioned place preference and

    locomotor activity in rats, Psychopharmacology 131 (1997) 18.

    [2] G.N. Brito, L.S. Brito, Septohippocampal system and the prelimbic

    sector of frontal cortex: a neuropsychological battery analysis in the

    rat, Behav. Brain Res. 36 (1990) 127 146.

    [3] J.N. Carlson, K.F. Herrick, J.L. Baird, S.D. Glick, Selective enhance-

    ment of dopamine utilization in the rat prefrontal cortex by food

    deprivation, Brain Res. 400 (1987) 200203.

    [4] J.N. Carlson, S.D. Glick, P.A. Hinds, J.L. Baird, Food deprivation

    alters dopamine utilization in the rat prefrontal cortex and asymmetri-

    cally alters amphetamine-induced rotational behavior, Brain Res. 454

    (1988) 373 377.

    [5] R. Ciccocioppo, P.P. Sanna, F. Weiss, Cocaine-predictive stimulus

    induces drug-seeking behavior and neural activation in limbic brain

    regions after multiple months of abstinence: reversal by D1 antago-

    nists, Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U. S. A. 98 (2001) 19761981.

    [6] R.A. Fuchs, S.M. Weber, H.J. Rice, J.L. Neisewander, Effects of

    excitotoxic lesions of the basolateral amygdala on cocaine-seeking

    behavior and cocaine conditioned place preference in rats, Brain

    Res. 929 (2002) 1525.

    [7] N.E. Goeders, J.E. Smith, Cortical dopaminergic involvement in co-

    caine reinforcement, Science 221 (1983) 773775.

    [8] N.E. Goeders, J.E. Smith, Reinforcing properties of cocaine in the

    medical prefrontal cortex: primary action on presynaptic dopaminer-

    gic terminals, Pharmacol. Biochem. Behav. 25 (1986) 191199.

    [9] N.E. Goeders, J.E. Smith, Intracranial cocaine self-administration into

    the medial prefrontal cortex increases dopamine turnover in the nu-

    cleus accumbens, J. Pharmacol. Exp. Ther. 265 (1993) 592600.

    [10] N.E. Goeders, S.I. Dworkin, J.E. Smith, Neuropharmacological as-

    sessment of cocaine self-administration into the medial prefrontal

    cortex, Pharmacol. Biochem. Behav. 24 (1986) 1429 1440.

    [11] H.J. Groenewegen, Organization of the afferent connections of the

    mediodorsal thalamic nucleus in the rat, related to the mediodorsal

    prefrontal topography, Neuroscience 24 (1988) 379 431.

    [12] W.L. Isaac, A.J. Nonneman, J. Neisewander, T. Landers, M.T. Bardo,

    Prefrontal cortex lesions differentially disrupt cocaine-reinforced con-

    ditioned place preference but not conditioned taste aversion, Behav.

    Neurosci. 103 (1989) 345 355.

    [13] Y. Itzhak, J.L. Martin, Cocaine-induced conditioned place preference

    in mice: induction, extinction and reinstatement by related psycho-

    stimulants, Neuropsychopharmacology 26 (2002) 130 134.

    [14] Y. Itzhak, J.L. Martin, S.F. Ali, Methamphetamine-induced dopami-

    nergic neurotoxicity in mice: long-lasting sensitization to the locomo-

    tor stimulation and desensitization to the rewarding effects of

    methamphetamine, Prog. Neuropsychopharmacol. Biol. Psychiatry

    26 (2002) 11771183.[15] B. Kolb, Functions of the frontal cortex of the rat: a comparative

    review, Brain Res. 320 (1984) 6598.

    [16] A. Kuzmin, J. Sandin, L. Terenius, S.O. Ogren, Acquisition, expres-

    sion, and reinstatement of ethanol-induced conditioned place prefer-

    ence in mice: effects of opioid receptor-like 1 receptor agonists and

    naloxone, J. Pharmacol. Exp. Ther. 304 (2003) 310318.

    [17] L. Li, J. Shao, Restricted lesions to ventral prefrontal subareas block

    reversal learning but not visual discrimination learning in rats, Phys-

    iol. Behav. 65 (1998) 371 379.

    [18] S.M. Li, Y.H. Ren, J.W. Zheng, Effect of 7-nitroindazole on drug-

    priming reinstatement of D-methamphetamine-induced conditioned

    place preference, Eur. J. Pharmacol. 443 (2002) 205 206.

    [19] L. Lu, M. Huang, L. Ma, J. Li, Different role of cholecystokinin

    (CCK)-A and CCK-B receptors in relapse to morphine dependence

    in rats, Behav. Brain Res. 120 (2001) 105 110.[20] L. Lu, D. Liu, X. Ceng, Corticotropin-releasing factor receptor type 1

    mediates stress-induced relapse to cocaine-conditioned place prefer-

    ence in rats, Eur. J. Pharmacol. 415 (2001) 203208.

    [21] L. Lu, N.J. Xu, X. Ge, W. Yue, W.J. Su, G. Pei, L. Ma, Reactivation

    of morphine conditioned place preference by drug priming: role of

    environmental cues and sensitization, Psychopharmacology 159

    (2002) 125 132.

    [22] L. Lu, B. Zhang, Z. Liu, Z. Zhang, Reactivation of cocaine condi-

    tioned place preference induced by stress is reversed by cholecysto-

    kinin-B receptors antagonist in rats, Brain Res. 954 (2002) 132 140.

    [23] A. Markou, F. Weiss, L.H. Gold, S.B. Caine, G. Schulteis, G.F. Koob,

    Animal models of drug craving, Psychopharmacology 112 (1993)

    163182.

    [24] K. McFarland, P.W. Kalivas, The circuitry mediating cocaine-induced

    A.R. Zavala et al. / Brain Research 990 (2003) 157164 163

  • 7/27/2019 Regiao Prelimbica Cocaina Preferencia Lugar

    8/8

    reinstatement of drug-seeking behavior, J. Neurosci. 21 (2001)

    86558663.

    [25] D. Mueller, J. Stewart, Cocaine-induced conditioned place preference:

    reinstatement by priming injections of cocaine after extinction, Behav.

    Brain Res. 115 (2000) 3947.

    [26] D. Mueller, D. Perdikaris, J. Stewart, Persistence and drug-induced

    reinstatement of a morphine-induced conditioned place preference,

    Behav. Brain Res. 136 (2002) 389397.

    [27] L.E. ODell, T.V. Khroyan, J.L. Neisewander, Dose-dependent char-

    acterization of the rewarding and stimulant properties of cocaine

    following intraperitoneal and intravenous administration in rats, Psy-

    chopharmacology 123 (1996) 144153.

    [28] W.K. Park, A.A. Bari, A.R. Jey, S.M. Anderson, R.D. Spealman, J.K.

    Rowlett, R.C. Pierce, Cocaine administered into the medial prefrontal

    cortex reinstates cocaine-seeking behavior by increasing AMPA re-

    ceptor-mediated glutamate transmission in the nucleus accumbens,

    J. Neurosci. 22 (2002) 29162925.

    [29] L.A. Parker, R.V. McDonald, Reinstatement of both a conditioned

    place preference and a conditioned place aversion with drug primes,

    Pharmacol. Biochem. Behav. 66 (2000) 559 561.

    [30] G. Paxinos, C. Watson, The Rat Brain in Stereotaxic Coordinates, 4th

    ed., Academic Press, San Diego, 1998.

    [31] C.J. Sanchez, B.A. Sorg, Conditioned fear stimuli reinstate cocaine-

    induced conditioned place preference, Brain Res. 908 (2001) 86 92.

    [32] C.J. Sanchez, T.M. Bailie, W. Wu, N. Li, B.A. Sorg, Manipulation of

    dopamine D1-like receptor activation in the rat medial prefrontal

    cortex alters stress- and cocaine-induced reinstatement of conditioned

    place preference behavior, Neuroscience 119 (2003) 497 505.

    [33] B.A. Sorg, P.W. Kalivas, Effects of cocaine and footshock stress on

    extracellular dopamine levels in the medial prefrontal cortex, Neuro-

    science 53 (1993) 695 703.

    [34] J.D. Steketee, Neurotransmitter systems of the medial prefrontal cor-

    tex: potential role in sensitization to psychostimulants, Brain Res.

    Rev. 41 (2003) 203 228.

    [35] G.D. Stuber, S.B. Evans, M.S. Higgins, Y. Pu, D.P. Figlewicz, Food

    restriction modulates amphetamine-conditioned place preference and

    nucleus accumbens dopamine release in the rat, Synapse 46 (2002)

    8390.

    [36] T.M. Tzschentke, Measuring reward with the conditioned place pref-

    erence paradigm: a comprehensive review of drug effects, recent

    progress and new issues, Prog. Neurobiol. 56 (1998) 613 672.

    [37] T.M. Tzschentke, W.J. Schmidt, Discrete quinolinic acid lesions of the

    rat prelimbic medial prefrontal cortex affect cocaine- and MK-801-,

    but not morphine- and amphetamine-induced reward and psychomo-

    tor activation as measured with the place preference conditioning

    paradigm, Behav. Brain Res. 97 (1998) 115 127.

    [38] T.M. Tzschentke, W.J. Schmidt, Functional heterogeneity of the rat

    medial prefrontal cortex: effects of discrete subarea-specific lesions

    on drug-induced conditioned place preference and behavioural sensi-

    tization, Eur. J. Neurosci. 11 (1999) 40994109.

    [39] B. Wang, F. Luo, W.T. Zhang, J.S. Han, Stress or drug priming

    induces reinstatement of extinguished conditioned place preference,

    NeuroReport 11 (2000) 2781 2784.

    [40] B. Wang, F. Luo, X.C. Ge, A.H. Fu, J.S. Han, Effects of lesions of

    various brain areas on drug priming or footshock-induced reactivation

    of extinguished conditioned place preference, Brain Res. 950 (2002)

    19.

    [41] R. Weissenborn, T.W. Robbins, B.J. Everitt, Effects of medial pre-

    frontal or anterior cingulate cortex lesions on responding for cocaine

    under fixed-ratio and second-order schedules of reinforcement in rats,

    Psychopharmacology 134 (1997) 242 257.

    A.R. Zavala et al. / Brain Research 990 (2003) 157164164