PSY 1950 Null Hypothesis Significance Testing September 29, 2008

21
PSY 1950 Null Hypothesis Significance Testing September 29, 2008

description

PSY 1950 Null Hypothesis Significance Testing September 29, 2008. vs. Finite Population Correction Factor. SEM and central limit theorem calculations are based on sampling with replacement from idealized, infinite populations - PowerPoint PPT Presentation

Transcript of PSY 1950 Null Hypothesis Significance Testing September 29, 2008

Page 1: PSY 1950 Null Hypothesis Significance Testing September 29, 2008

PSY 1950Null Hypothesis Significance

TestingSeptember 29, 2008

Page 2: PSY 1950 Null Hypothesis Significance Testing September 29, 2008

vs

Page 3: PSY 1950 Null Hypothesis Significance Testing September 29, 2008

0

2

4

6

8

10

12

2 12 22 32 42 52 62 72 82 92

sample N

mean sampling statistic

sample SQRT(SS/N) sample SQRT(SS)/Npopulation SQRT(SS/N) population SQRT(SS)/N

Page 4: PSY 1950 Null Hypothesis Significance Testing September 29, 2008

Finite Population Correction Factor

• SEM and central limit theorem calculations are based on sampling with replacement from idealized, infinite populations

• Real-life research involves sampling without replacement from actual, finite populations

• When n/N<.05, this doesn’t matter

• When n/N>.05, use a correction factor:

Page 5: PSY 1950 Null Hypothesis Significance Testing September 29, 2008

Controversy of NHST“backbone of psychological research”

– Gerrig & Zimbardo (2002, p. 42)

“a potent but sterile intellectual rake who leaves in his merry path a long train of ravished maidens but no viable scientific offspring”– Meehl (1967, p. 265)

“…surely the most bone-headedly misguided procedure ever institutionalized in the rote training of science students”– Rozeboom (1997, p. 335)

Page 6: PSY 1950 Null Hypothesis Significance Testing September 29, 2008
Page 7: PSY 1950 Null Hypothesis Significance Testing September 29, 2008

NHST example (z-test)1. State the null and alternative

hypotheses• H0: µinfant = 26 lbs• H1: µ infant 26 lbs

2. Set the criteria for a decision• = .05• |z| ≥ 1.96

• Collect data and compute sample statistics• Minfant = 30 lbs• with n = 16 and = 4• z = (M - µ)/M = (30 - 26)/1 = 4

• Make a decision• Reject H0

Page 8: PSY 1950 Null Hypothesis Significance Testing September 29, 2008

NHST Errors

Type III error?

Page 9: PSY 1950 Null Hypothesis Significance Testing September 29, 2008

Power• The probability of correctly rejecting a false null hypothesis = 1 -

• http://wise.cgu.edu/power/power_applet.html

Page 10: PSY 1950 Null Hypothesis Significance Testing September 29, 2008

= .05• “It is convenient to take this point as a limit in judging whether a deviation is to be considered significant or not. Deviations exceeding twice the standard deviation are thus formally regard as significant”– Fisher (1925, p. 47)

• Historical roots prior to Fisher’s definition

• Corresponds to subjective demarcation of chance from non-chance events

• "... surely, God loves the .06 nearly as much as the .05”– (Rosnow & Rosenthal, 1989)

Page 11: PSY 1950 Null Hypothesis Significance Testing September 29, 2008

NHST Rationale• Why try to reject null hypothesis?– Philosophical: Popperian falsifiability•accept H1: the projector occasionally malfunctions

•reject H0: the projector always works

– Practical: defining the sampling distribution•H1: the projector failure rate = ?

•H0: the projector failure rate = 0%

Page 12: PSY 1950 Null Hypothesis Significance Testing September 29, 2008

History of NHSTFisher’s (1925) NHST:

1. Set up null hypothesis (not necessarily)2. Report exact significance3. Only do this when you know little else

Neyman & Pearson (1950)1. Set up two competing hypothesis, H1 and H2,

and make a priori decisions about and 2. If data falls into rejection region of H1,

accept H2; otherwise accept H1. Acceptance belief.

3. Only do this when you have a disjunction of hypotheses (either H1 or H2 is true)

Current NHST (according to some):1. Set up null hypothesis as nil hypothesis2. Reject null at p<.05 and accept your

hypothesis3. Always do this

QuickTime™ and aTIFF (Uncompressed) decompressor

are needed to see this picture.

QuickTime™ and aTIFF (Uncompressed) decompressor

are needed to see this picture.

QuickTime™ and aTIFF (Uncompressed) decompressor

are needed to see this picture.

Page 13: PSY 1950 Null Hypothesis Significance Testing September 29, 2008

Criticisms of NHST• Affirming the consequent

– If P then Q. Q. Therefore P.

• The straw person argument– Tukey (1991): “It is foolish to ask ‘Are the effects of A and B different?’ They are always different—for some decimal place”(p. 100)

– “Statistical significance does not necessarily imply practical significance!”

• The replication fallacy– If you conduct an experiment that results in p = .05 (two-tailed), what is the chance that a replication of that experiment will produce a statistically significant (p<.05) effect?• 50% (see Cumming, 2008, Appendix B)

• “Confusion of the inverse”– “absence of proof is not proof of absence”– “presence of proof is not proof of presence”

Page 14: PSY 1950 Null Hypothesis Significance Testing September 29, 2008

Affirming the Consequent• NHST commits logical fallacy

– NHST: If the null hypothesis is correct, then these data are highly unlikely•These data have occurred•Therefore, the null hypothesis is highly unlikely

– Analog: If a person is an American, then he is probably not a member of Congress•This person is a member of Congress•Therefore, he is probably not an American

• Response: Science progresses through testing its predictions– Logic may be flawed, but success is hard to deny

Page 15: PSY 1950 Null Hypothesis Significance Testing September 29, 2008

The Straw Person Argument• Often null hypothesis = nil hypothesis– The nil hypothesis is always (or almost always) false•The “crud factor” in correlational research (Meehl, 1990)

•The “princess and the pea” effect in experimental research

– If the null hypothesis is always false, how does rejecting it increase knowledge?

• Response: effect size matters, statistical significance is not practical significance, test interactions

Page 16: PSY 1950 Null Hypothesis Significance Testing September 29, 2008

Replication Fallacy• p-values don’t say much about replicability, yet most everyone thinks they do– Replication is NOT 1 - (Tversky & Kahneman, 1971)

• Response: p-values inform replicability, just less than one might think– All else equal, the lower the p-value, the higher the replicability

Page 17: PSY 1950 Null Hypothesis Significance Testing September 29, 2008
Page 18: PSY 1950 Null Hypothesis Significance Testing September 29, 2008

“Confusion of the Inverse”• Criticism: NHST calculates the probability of obtaining the data given a hypothesis, p(D|H0), not the probability of obtaining a hypothesis given the data, p(H0|D)– A p-value of .05 does NOT necessarily indicate that the null hypothesis is unlikely to be true

• Response: logically faulty but productive inferences is better than nothing– p(D|H0) approximates p(H0|D) under typical experimental settings where p(H0) is low, i.e., p(H1) > p(H0)

– p(H0|D) varies monotonically with p(D|H0) p(H0|D)

– When p(H0) = .35, p(H0|D) = .35

– p(D|H0) and p(H0|D) are correlated (r = .38)

Page 19: PSY 1950 Null Hypothesis Significance Testing September 29, 2008

NHST gives p(D|H0) not p(H0|D)

Page 20: PSY 1950 Null Hypothesis Significance Testing September 29, 2008

Reconciliation• “Inductive inference cannot be logically justified, but they can be defended pragmatically” (Krueger, 2001)

• Use NHST mindfully– “There is no God-given rule about when and how to make up your mind in general.”•Hays (1973, p. 353)

• Don’t rely exclusively on p-values

Page 21: PSY 1950 Null Hypothesis Significance Testing September 29, 2008

Alternatives to p-values • Effect size

– Meta-analysis

• Confidence intervals

• prep