Potential defence from herbivory by ‘dazzle effects’ and ‘trickery coloration’ of leaf...

6
COMMENT Potential defence from herbivory by ‘dazzle effects’ and ‘trickery coloration’ of leaf variegation SIMCHA LEV-YADUN* Department of Biology & Environment, Faculty of Natural Sciences, University of Haifa – Oranim, Tivon 36006, Israel Received 3 November 2013; revised 12 December 2013; accepted for publication 12 December 2013 The very conspicuous dazzle coloration invented for naval defence during World War I was used in pre-radar days to mislead attackers of naval units about vessel size, type, speed, and direction. Among several potential types of defences, it is proposed that zebra-like white leaf variegation may defend leaves and other plant organs from herbivory as a result of dazzle effects. Two different dazzle effects may be involved in defending plants from herbivory, making it hard for herbivores (1) to decide where, in a three-dimensional space, to bite the leaves (large herbivores) and (2) to land on them (insects). In addition, the related types of leaf coloration described in the present study, comprising parallels of military defensive trickery naval painting, may also deceive herbivores about the actual shape, location, and identity of leaves. Some of these visual defences may operate at the same time as other visual defences, such as aposematism, or serve various physiological functions. © 2014 The Linnean Society of London, Biological Journal of the Linnean Society, 2014, 111, 692–697. ADDITIONAL KEYWORDS: aposematic – camouflage – deception – ecology – evolution – mimicry – visual. INTRODUCTION Dazzle and trickery coloration were developed to defend navies when the range of artillery and torpe- does surpassed the range of several kilometers, and when aiming by visual means ruled the naval battlefield. Despite the huge scale of use of dazzle and trickery coloration during World Wars I and II (Wilkinson, 1969; Stanley, 1998; Williams, 2001; Forbes, 2009), and the apparent visual parallels of these patterns of coloration in animals and plants, very little attention was given to these potential types of defence in biology. Defensive dazzle plant coloration received only a very limited attention, without dis- cussing the theoretical aspects (Lev-Yadun, 2003, 2006, 2009a), even when compared with the limited attention that it received in zoology. Four different major types of visual deception were involved in trickery naval painting (painted trickery sensu Stanley, 1998). The first type is simple: painting the fore and aft sectors of naval units lighter. The second type of painted trickery involves painting false bow waves. This can be achieved in two ways: (1) painting on the bow side, giving the impression of a higher speed than the actual speed, or (2) painting also or only on the stern side, causing confusion about the direction in which the vessel is heading (Stanley, 1998; Williams, 2001). A similar deceptive defence tactic in animals is known as a ‘false head’ or ‘deflec- tion marks’ and these are assumed to divert predator attacks towards less critical body parts (Cott, 1940; Wickler, 1968; Edmunds, 1974; Robbins, 1980, 1981; Ruxton, Sherratt & Speed, 2004). The third type involves painting shapes of other vessels as if posi- tioned alongside the hull, creating the illusion that either another vessel is blocking the way for artillery shells, torpedoes, and aerial bombs to the target, or that there is an escort vessel present that might attack (Stanley, 1998; Williams, 2001). The fourth type of trickery naval painting is visually and theo- retically the most complicated: dazzle painting. It was proposed to the British Admiralty as a visual defen- sive measure during the critical German submarine attack crisis in the year 1917 by the professional *E-mail: [email protected] Biological Journal of the Linnean Society, 2014, 111, 692–697. With 2 figures © 2014 The Linnean Society of London, Biological Journal of the Linnean Society, 2014, 111, 692–697 692

Transcript of Potential defence from herbivory by ‘dazzle effects’ and ‘trickery coloration’ of leaf...

Page 1: Potential defence from herbivory by ‘dazzle effects’ and ‘trickery coloration’ of leaf variegation

COMMENT

Potential defence from herbivory by ‘dazzle effects’ and‘trickery coloration’ of leaf variegation

SIMCHA LEV-YADUN*

Department of Biology & Environment, Faculty of Natural Sciences, University of Haifa – Oranim,Tivon 36006, Israel

Received 3 November 2013; revised 12 December 2013; accepted for publication 12 December 2013

The very conspicuous dazzle coloration invented for naval defence during World War I was used in pre-radar daysto mislead attackers of naval units about vessel size, type, speed, and direction. Among several potential types ofdefences, it is proposed that zebra-like white leaf variegation may defend leaves and other plant organs fromherbivory as a result of dazzle effects. Two different dazzle effects may be involved in defending plants fromherbivory, making it hard for herbivores (1) to decide where, in a three-dimensional space, to bite the leaves (largeherbivores) and (2) to land on them (insects). In addition, the related types of leaf coloration described in thepresent study, comprising parallels of military defensive trickery naval painting, may also deceive herbivores aboutthe actual shape, location, and identity of leaves. Some of these visual defences may operate at the same time asother visual defences, such as aposematism, or serve various physiological functions. © 2014 The Linnean Societyof London, Biological Journal of the Linnean Society, 2014, 111, 692–697.

ADDITIONAL KEYWORDS: aposematic – camouflage – deception – ecology – evolution – mimicry – visual.

INTRODUCTION

Dazzle and trickery coloration were developed todefend navies when the range of artillery and torpe-does surpassed the range of several kilometers,and when aiming by visual means ruled the navalbattlefield. Despite the huge scale of use of dazzleand trickery coloration during World Wars I andII (Wilkinson, 1969; Stanley, 1998; Williams, 2001;Forbes, 2009), and the apparent visual parallels ofthese patterns of coloration in animals and plants,very little attention was given to these potential typesof defence in biology. Defensive dazzle plant colorationreceived only a very limited attention, without dis-cussing the theoretical aspects (Lev-Yadun, 2003,2006, 2009a), even when compared with the limitedattention that it received in zoology.

Four different major types of visual deception wereinvolved in trickery naval painting (painted trickerysensu Stanley, 1998). The first type is simple: paintingthe fore and aft sectors of naval units lighter. The

second type of painted trickery involves painting falsebow waves. This can be achieved in two ways: (1)painting on the bow side, giving the impression of ahigher speed than the actual speed, or (2) paintingalso or only on the stern side, causing confusion aboutthe direction in which the vessel is heading (Stanley,1998; Williams, 2001). A similar deceptive defencetactic in animals is known as a ‘false head’ or ‘deflec-tion marks’ and these are assumed to divert predatorattacks towards less critical body parts (Cott, 1940;Wickler, 1968; Edmunds, 1974; Robbins, 1980, 1981;Ruxton, Sherratt & Speed, 2004). The third typeinvolves painting shapes of other vessels as if posi-tioned alongside the hull, creating the illusion thateither another vessel is blocking the way for artilleryshells, torpedoes, and aerial bombs to the target, orthat there is an escort vessel present that mightattack (Stanley, 1998; Williams, 2001). The fourthtype of trickery naval painting is visually and theo-retically the most complicated: dazzle painting. It wasproposed to the British Admiralty as a visual defen-sive measure during the critical German submarineattack crisis in the year 1917 by the professional*E-mail: [email protected]

bs_bs_banner

Biological Journal of the Linnean Society, 2014, 111, 692–697. With 2 figures

© 2014 The Linnean Society of London, Biological Journal of the Linnean Society, 2014, 111, 692–697692

Page 2: Potential defence from herbivory by ‘dazzle effects’ and ‘trickery coloration’ of leaf variegation

marine painter Norman Wilkinson, who served as anofficer in the Royal Navy during World War I andrealized, during his vessel’s patrols at sea, that, undermany types of illumination, regular naval camouflagenot only failed to conceal vessels, but also sometimeseven made them more conspicuous (Wilkinson, 1969).He proposed to the British Admiralty that, instead ofusing the non-efficient camouflaging of navy units,they should be conspicuously painted with strongcolours applied in a way that would make it hard toidentify the vessel type, as well as its distance, speed,and direction.

Herbivores, which are usually not well defendedagainst fierce carnivores at close range, operate undervarious fears (Rothschild, 1984; Brown, 1999; Sheriff,Krebs & Boonstra, 2009; Janzen, Hallwachs & Burns,2010). Thus, part of the attention of herbivorousanimals is always drawn away from their prey (i.e.the plants) and this helps the defending plantsthat have evolved characters adding various visualdifficulties to prolong the time needed for decisionmaking, thus exploiting for defence the herbivore’slimited attention (Jones, Krebs & Whittingham,2006).

The present study describes the theoretical basisof the dazzle issue and the related types of trickeryleaf coloration as potential visual defences of plantsfrom herbivory. These issues were never discussed indepth by botanists or zoologists with respect to plant–herbivore interactions.

DAZZLE COLORATION IN ANIMALS

Because coloration has been ignored as a significantdefensive mechanism in plants for a long time, thestate of art in zoology (although also very far frombeing sufficient) can be used as a standard in manycases, whereas dazzle coloration has received, untilrecently, only limited attention even in animals and,for many years, it remained untested and the defini-tions were also partly misleading because of thecommon confusion with disruptive coloration (Forbes,2009). In both the military and nature, the bordersbetween these two types of defensive coloration arenot always clear, especially because certain colourpatterns may serve as camouflage when distant andfor dazzling or trickery at close range. Dazzle colora-tion consists of high contrast patterns that make theprey conspicuous, although it results in difficultiesin estimating the speed and trajectory of prey andin identifying its specific limbs. The best commonlyknown suggestion for the operation of dazzle asan anti-predator defence is for the zebra’s stripes(Kruuk, 1972; Kingdon, 1984; Morris, 1990), althoughthis hypothesis, which was not supported by carefullyexamined predation statistics, was basically rejected

(Ruxton, 2002). The recent detailed experimentalstudy (Egri et al., 2012) of the role of zebra stripes inhorsefly (tabanids) repellence appears to give verystrong indications that such patterns indeed functionat least in insect repellence, even if the mechanismis unknown. After decades of some theoretical dis-cussions (Stevens, 2007; Stevens & Merilaita, 2009),only recently was some experimentation of the poten-tial for defensive dazzle effects by animals conducted,although still with human subjects as hunters ofimages on computer screens rather than in nature(Stevens, Yule & Ruxton, 2008; Scott-Samuel et al.,2011; Stevens et al., 2011; von Helversen, Schooler& Czienskowski, 2013). A study of the neuronalresponse in locusts indicated that dazzle effectsby high-contrast patterns indeed weaken move-ment detection (Santer, 2013). The results of allthese experiments illuminate both the possibilitiesand limitations of the defensive potential of dazzlecoloration.

ACTUAL AND RELATIVE PLANTMOVEMENT AND ITS POSSIBLE ROLE

IN DAZZLE EFFECTS

As discussed above, the operation of naval dazzlepainting is related to giving the wrong impression ofidentity, size, speed, and direction. When this type ofvisual defensive tactic is discussed with respect toplants, it immediately raises the question of themovement component in such a defence. The role ofplant movement as defence against herbivory hasbeen almost overlooked. The classic examples includethe escape from herbivory by folding of Mimosapudica leaves (Jensen, Dill & Cahill, 2011) or theexposure of thorns by leaflet folding (Eisner, 1981).Lev-Yadun & Inbar (2002) proposed that the blackanthers of Paspalum, which move with a light wind,may look like swivelling aphids. Later, Lev-Yadun(2009a) proposed that the swaying of leaves, stems orbranches in the wind may help in ant mimicry byplants by giving the illusion that the pigmentationpatterns proposed to mimic ‘ants’ (Lev-Yadun & Inbar,2002) are moving. Yamazaki (2011), in the first broad,innovative and intriguing theoretical treatment ofdefensive plant movement, advocated the hypothesisof considering the possible roles of passive leaf move-ment as a common anti-herbivory defence, althoughnot in the connotation of dazzle effects.

The visual aspect of plant movement as a way ofcommunication with (and deception of) animals hasalready been studied in two cases of pollinator attrac-tion. In the first, some orchid species belonging to thegenus Oncidium induce attacks by territorial maleCentris bees when the inflorescences move in thewind because the male bees mistake them for rival

DAZZLE EFFECTS OF LEAF VARIEGATION 693

© 2014 The Linnean Society of London, Biological Journal of the Linnean Society, 2014, 111, 692–697

Page 3: Potential defence from herbivory by ‘dazzle effects’ and ‘trickery coloration’ of leaf variegation

male bees. During the attacks of the pseudo rivals,the male bees transfer the pollen (Dodson & Frymire,1961; Wiens, 1978). In the other case, Warren &James (2008) showed that movements (‘waving’) ofthe inflorescences of Silene maritima increase polli-nation success. Thus, following such functional exam-ples from pollination biology, there is no theoreticalreason to dismiss the possibility that actual and rela-tive plant movements take part in visual defence fromherbivory (Lev-Yadun & Ne’eman, 2012).

There are two ubiquitous possibilities for plantmovement. The first is the actual swaying ofleaves, stems, branches or flower parts in thewind (Lev-Yadun & Inbar, 2002; Lev-Yadun, 2009a;Yamazaki, 2011). When the plants move, their mor-phological and colour patterns move with them. Aftermaking many attempts myself to photograph varioustypes of defensive coloration under windy conditionsin the field, it became apparent that the movement,especially of leaves, may be significant. The othertype of movement is relative: the outcome of themovement of the head of large herbivores as theyapproach plants, or that of the whole body of insectsthat fly towards the plants. This is not true forcaterpillars, aphids or other small herbivores thatalready inhabit leaves or walk slowly on them. There-fore, although plants are commonly considered to bemotionless targets for foraging herbivores, this is notthe case in many instances.

POTENTIAL DEFENCE BY DAZZLEEFFECTS AND OTHER TRICKY TYPES OFLEAF VARIEGATION AND COLORATION

Two types of leaf variegation and coloration indicatethe possibility that variegated plants indeed use var-iegation and coloration as a visual defence to trick thesensory system of the herbivores with repect to leafsize and location, such as with naval trick coloration.It is suggested that this is not aposematism and,instead, a simultaneous aposematic function mayoperate in certain cases when the variegated leavesare spiny or toxic. The variegation types are classifiedin accordance with the naval terminology: trickeryand dazzle coloration. The basic principle of suchdefence is its bearing on the trade-off between speedof decision/operation and accuracy when biting theplant. A similar trade-off appears to operate in imper-fect mimicry based defence systems (Chittka &Osorio, 2007; Janzen et al., 2010). The longer ittakes a herbivore to decide where to bite or land, thehigher are the risks of attacks by predators (Lima &Dill, 1990; Brown, 1999; Ripple & Beschta, 2004;Nersesian, Banks & McArthur, 2011), along with thedecrease in feeding efficiency.

Certain plant species have white or colourful zoneson their leaves formed parallel to their contours,which create a visual illusion of several leaf layersinstead of one. This pattern is typical of several wildtaxa common in Israel: Amaranthus sp., Lamiummoschatum, and Cyclamen persicum, all with whitemarkings; Polygonum lapathifolium, with dark mark-ings; and Trifolium spp. and Medicago spp., witheither white or various similar colourful markings.The genus Cyclamen is especially characterized bysuch white variegated leaves (Grey-Wilson, 1988). Ina much more limited search of the flora of Switzer-land, the same phenomenon was found in Lamiumgaleobdolon, Trifolium repens and Trifolium pratense(with white markings), as well as Medicago arabicaand Polygonum lapathifolium (with dark markings).Except for Cyclamen species that occupy both openand forested habitats, all other taxa occupy open andwell illuminated habitats.

It is proposed that, in certain plant leaves, colora-tion patterns formed parallel to their contours (Fig. 1)may cause the visual illusion of a fake leaf shape,size, distance, and perspective, blurring actual leafshapes, sizes, and distances, making it difficult forherbivores to estimate the distance to the leaf andaim their bites or landing. When there are severalsuch parallel markings, the leaf may look as if it ismade of several leaf layers that occupy differentplanes. Leaves of such taxa use the vegetal parallel tothe third type of defensive naval painting (i.e. paint-ing shapes of smaller vessels as if positioned along-side the hull) (Stanley, 1998; Williams, 2001). As withseveral other types of defensive plant coloration, addi-tional physiological or defensive functions of suchcoloration should also be considered specifically foreach species and ecology.

Figure 1. Coloration patterns formed parallel to leafcontour in Cyclamen persicum giving the visual impres-sion of several leaf layers when only one exists.

694 S. LEV-YADUN

© 2014 The Linnean Society of London, Biological Journal of the Linnean Society, 2014, 111, 692–697

Page 4: Potential defence from herbivory by ‘dazzle effects’ and ‘trickery coloration’ of leaf variegation

Theoretically, coloration that produces an illusion ofa different leaf shape may also cause identificationproblems for insects that search for specific leaf typesor leaf developmental stages (Prokopy & Owens,1983; Brown, Lawton & Grubb, 1991; Reeves, 2011).Moreover, because some of the plants with leavesvariegated in white (or with other colours) (e.g. Cycla-men spp. and certain Trifolium spp. and Medicagospp.) may be poisonous in that there are cyano-genic types in Trifolium spp. and Medicago spp.(Crawford-Sidebotham, 1972; Dirzo & Harper, 1982)and Cyclamen species known to be rich with alkaloidsand saponins (Hornell, 1941; Reznicek et al., 1989),the visual illusion may thus not be the sole visualdefensive mode and these patterns may also serveas aposematic coloration. Müllerian and Batesianmimicry rings are expected to exist in such cases.

To date, zebra-like white leaf variegation has beenvery briefly proposed to serve as defensive dazzlecoloration (sensu Wilkinson, 1969) only for the threespecies of open, well-illuminated Mediterranean habi-tats Silybum marianum, Notobasis syriaca, andScolymus maculatus (Lev-Yadun, 2003, 2006, 2009a,b) (Fig. 2). It is proposed that this hypothesis can beextended to various species belonging to the generaHaworthia, Gasteria, and Sansevieria, which alsohave zebra-like white leaf variegation, as well as toany other plant taxa with such coloration. Interest-ingly, all these three genera are common in openhabitats in arid and semi-arid districts of SouthAfrica, habitats resembling those occupied by thethree Mediterranean species listed above. This coin-cidence may indicate a similar function(s) and similarevolutionary pressures. Such plants actually haveclassic dazzle coloration very similar to that appliedto naval vessels [compare photographs of the French

cruiser Gloire in Williams (2001) or on the Internet,and other photographs in Wilkinson (1969), andStanley (1998), with those in Lev-Yadun (2003)]. Theswaying of the leaves in the wind, and the relativemovements of plants with respect to flying insects orthe head movements of large herbivores approachingthe plants, increase the potential to form the desireddazzle effects on the visual nerve systems of theherbivores, making it difficult for them to land on orbite the leaf because of problems in locating its actualposition in space.

The putative dazzle effects of such variegation areprobably not the sole visual defensive function insome of these plants. It has been proposed that theconspicuous zebra-like leaf variegation of the veryspiny species S. marianum, N. syriaca, and S.maculatus serves mainly as visual aposematism(Lev-Yadun, 2003, 2006, 2009a, b). Lev-Yadun (2003)proposed that mimicking of tunnelling by flies belong-ing to the Agromyzidae to deter female Agromyzidaeflies from laying eggs, and that such conspicuousstripes reduce insect landing on the leaves in general,as was proposed for the evolution of zebra stripesshould also not be ruled out.

In conclusion, in accordance with the data obtainedfrom recent experiments indicating that dazzle effectsmay defend animals from predation (Stevens et al.,2008, 2011; Scott-Samuel et al., 2011; von Helversenet al., 2013) and from the morphological descriptionsof potential dazzle patterns in leaves of various taxa(Lev-Yadun, 2003, 2009b) and those described in thepresent study, defence from herbivory by dazzle andtrickery coloration should be considered as a workinghypothesis with respect to the hundreds, if not thou-sands, of plant species that express such types of leafcoloration.

ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS

I thank Professor John A. Allen and two anonymousreviewers for their important suggestions.

REFERENCES

Brown JS. 1999. Vigilance, patch use and habitat selection:foraging under predation risk. Evolutionary EcologyResearch 1: 49–71.

Brown VK, Lawton JH, Grubb PJ. 1991. Herbivory andthe evolution of leaf size and shape. Philosophical Transac-tions of the Royal Society of London Series B, BiologicalSciences 333: 265–272.

Chittka L, Osorio D. 2007. Cognitive dimensions of predatorresponses to imperfect mimicry? PLoS Biology 5: 2754–2758e339.

Cott HB. 1940. Adaptive coloration in animals. London:Methuen & Co.

Figure 2. Zebra-like leaf variegation in Silybummarianum that may serve as dazzle coloration. This plantlook like the classic dazzle painted French cruiser Gloire(Williams, 2001).

DAZZLE EFFECTS OF LEAF VARIEGATION 695

© 2014 The Linnean Society of London, Biological Journal of the Linnean Society, 2014, 111, 692–697

Page 5: Potential defence from herbivory by ‘dazzle effects’ and ‘trickery coloration’ of leaf variegation

Crawford-Sidebotham TJ. 1972. The role of slugs andsnails in the maintenance of the cyanogenesis polymor-phisms of Lotus corniculatus and Trifolium repens. Heredity28: 405–411.

Dirzo R, Harper JL. 1982. Experimental studies on slug–plant interactions IV. The performance of cyanogenic andacyanogenic morphs of Trifolium repens in the field. Journalof Ecology 70: 119–138.

Dodson C, Frymire G. 1961. Natural pollination of orchids.Missouri Botanical Garden Bulletin 49: 133–152.

Edmunds M. 1974. Defence in animals. A survey of anti-predator defences. Harlow: Longman.

Egri Á, Blahó M, Kriska G, Farkas R, Gyurkovszky M,Åkesson S, Horváth G. 2012. Polarotactic tabanids findstriped patterns with brightness and/or polarization modu-lation least attractive: an advantage of zebra stripes.Journal of Experimental Biology 215: 736–745.

Eisner T. 1981. Leaf folding in a sensitive plant: a defensivethorn-exposure mechanism? Proceedings of the NationalAcademy of Sciences of the United States of America 78:402–404.

Forbes P. 2009. Dazzled and deceived: mimicry and camou-flage. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press.

Grey-Wilson C. 1988. The genus Cyclamen. Kew: The RoyalBotanic Gardens, in association with Christopher Helm andTimber Press.

von Helversen B, Schooler LJ, Czienskowski U. 2013.Are stripes beneficial? Dazzle camouflage influences per-ceived speed and hit rates. PLoS ONE 8: eg1173.

Hornell J. 1941. Fishing-poisons. Man 41: 126–128.Janzen DH, Hallwachs W, Burns JM. 2010. A tropical

horde of counterfeit predator eyes. Proceedings of theNational Academy of Sciences of the United States ofAmerica 107: 11659–11665.

Jensen EL, Dill LM, Cahill JF Jr. 2011. Applyingbehavioral-ecological theory to plant defense: light-dependent movement in Mimosa pudica suggests a trade-offbetween predation risk and energetic reward. AmericanNaturalist 177: 377–381.

Jones KA, Krebs JR, Whittingham MJ. 2006. Interactionbetween seed crypsis and habitat structure influence patchchoice in a granivorous bird, the chaffinch Fringilla coelebs.Journal of Avian Biology 37: 413–418.

Kingdon J. 1984. The zebra’s stripes: an aid to group cohe-sion. In: MacDonald D, ed. The encyclopedia of mammals.Oxford: Equinox, 486–487.

Kruuk H. 1972. The spotted hyena. A study of predation andsocial behavior. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.

Lev-Yadun S. 2003. Why do some thorny plants resemblegreen zebras? Journal of Theoretical Biology 244: 483–489.

Lev-Yadun S. 2006. Defensive coloration in plants: a reviewof current ideas about anti-herbivore coloration strategies.In: Teixeira da Silva JA, ed. Floriculture, ornamental andplant biotechnology: advances and topical issues, Vol. IV.London: Global Science Books, 292–299.

Lev-Yadun S. 2009a. Aposematic (warning) coloration inplants. In: Baluska F, ed. Plant–environment interactions.

From sensory plant biology to active behavior, Vol. II. Berlin:Springer Verlag, 167–202.

Lev-Yadun S. 2009b. Müllerian and Batesian mimicry ringsof white-variegated aposematic spiny and thorny plants: ahypothesis. Israel Journal of Plant Sciences 57: 107–116.

Lev-Yadun S, Inbar M. 2002. Defensive ant, aphid andcaterpillar mimicry in plants. Biological Journal of theLinnean Society 77: 393–398.

Lev-Yadun S, Ne’eman G. 2012. Does bee or wasp mimicryby orchid flowers also deter herbivores? Arthropod–PlantInteractions 6: 327–332.

Lima SL, Dill LM. 1990. Behavioral decisions made underthe risk of predation: a review and prospectus. CanadianJournal of Zoology 68: 619–640.

Morris D. 1990. Animal watching. A field guide to animalbehaviour. London: Jonathan Cape.

Nersesian CL, Banks PB, McArthur C. 2011. Titratingthe cost of plant toxins against predators: determining thetipping point for foraging herbivores. Journal of AnimalEcology 80: 753–760.

Prokopy RJ, Owens ED. 1983. Visual detection of plants byherbivorous insects. Annual Review of Entomology 28: 337–364.

Reeves JL. 2011. Vision should not be overlooked as animportant sensory modality for finding host plants. Envi-ronmental Entomology 40: 855–863.

Reznicek G, Jurenitsch J, Robien W, Kubelka W. 1989.Saponins in Cyclamen species: configuration of cyclamiretinC and structure of isocyclamin. Phytochemistry 28: 825–828.

Ripple WJ, Beschta RL. 2004. Wolves and the ecology offear: can predation risk structure ecosystems? BioScience54: 755–766.

Robbins RK. 1980. The lycaened ‘false head’ hypothesis:historical review and quantitative analysis. Journal of theLepidopteran Society 34: 194–208.

Robbins RK. 1981. The ‘false head’ hypothesis: predation andwing pattern variation of lycaenid butterflies. AmericanNaturalist 118: 770–775.

Rothschild M. 1984. Aide mémoire mimicry. Ecological Ento-mology 9: 311–319.

Ruxton GD. 2002. The possible fitness benefits of striped coatcoloration for zebra. Mammalian Review 32: 237–244.

Ruxton GD, Sherratt TN, Speed MP. 2004. Avoidingattack. The evolutionary ecology of crypsis, warning signals& mimicry. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

Santer RD. 2013. Motion dazzle: a locust’s eye view. BiologyLetters 9: 20130811.

Scott-Samuel NE, Baddeley R, Palmer CE, Cuthill IC.2011. Dazzle camouflage affects speed perception. PLoSONE 6: e20233.

Sheriff MJ, Krebs CJ, Boonstra R. 2009. The sensitivehare: sublethal effects of predator stress on reproductionin snowshoe hares. Journal of Animal Ecology 78: 1249–1258.

Stanley RM II. 1998. To fool a glass eye. Camouflage versusphotoreconnaissance in World War II. Washington, DC:Smithsonian Institute Press.

696 S. LEV-YADUN

© 2014 The Linnean Society of London, Biological Journal of the Linnean Society, 2014, 111, 692–697

Page 6: Potential defence from herbivory by ‘dazzle effects’ and ‘trickery coloration’ of leaf variegation

Stevens M. 2007. Predator perception and the interrelationbetween different forms of protective coloration. Proceedingsof the Royal Society of London Series B, Biological Sciences274: 1457–1464.

Stevens M, Merilaita S. 2009. Animal camouflage: currentissues and new perspectives. Philosophical Transactions ofthe Royal Society of London Series B, Biological Sciences364: 423–427.

Stevens M, Searle WTL, Seymour JE, Marshall KLA,Ruxton GD. 2011. Motion dazzle and camouflage as dis-tinct anti-predator defenses. BMC Biology 9: 81.

Stevens M, Yule DH, Ruxton GD. 2008. Dazzle colorationand prey movement. Proceedings of the Royal Society ofLondon Series B, Biological Sciences 275: 2639–2643.

Warren J, James P. 2008. Do flowers wave to attract polli-nators? A case study with Silene maritima. Journal ofEvolutionary Biology 21: 1024–1029.

Wickler W. 1968. Mimicry in plants and animals. London:Weidenfeld and Nicolson.

Wiens D. 1978. Mimicry in plants. Evolutionary Biology 11:365–403.

Wilkinson N. 1969. A brush with life. London: Seeley Service& Co Ltd.

Williams D. 2001. Naval camouflage 1914–1945. A completevisual reference. Annapolis, MD: Naval Institute Press.

Yamazaki K. 2011. Gone with the wind: trembling leavesmay deter herbivory. Biological Journal of the LinneanSociety 104: 738–747.

DAZZLE EFFECTS OF LEAF VARIEGATION 697

© 2014 The Linnean Society of London, Biological Journal of the Linnean Society, 2014, 111, 692–697