New UC Berkeley Phonology Lab Annual Report: ABC...

1
Vowel subsegments and surface correspondence Matthew Faytak — University of California, Berkeley Overview Distinct sets of vowels and consonants are usually the subject of studies on correspondence: but what if the distinction is not so clear? Vocoids are not always static in constriction degree, and they may in fact involve consonant-like “inter- ruptions,” in theory allowing for non-local interac- tions with “marginal” (onset, coda) consonants. These have been handled as OCP effects: a fea- ture [ϕ] is specified in some structural domain, with alignment (potentially mid-vowel) determined by phonetic implementation. Multiple assignments of the same [ϕ] in a local unit may lead to unlicensing or removal of one at a “distance.” ABC, with recent expansions to account for segment-internal structure (Shih and Inkelas, 2014), can similarly model these outcomes: unstable sur- face correspondence compels constricted mid-vowel subsegments to become more consonant-like; this might disagree with a language’s phonotactics and dissimilation may result (Bennett, 2013). Here, we investigate two putative cases of this phe- nomenon, both of which involve vowel interruptions C V with feature(s) [ϕ] that are “uncomfortably” sim- ilar to their onsets. Repositioning correspondence ABC is most often used to model long-distance interaction between consonants (Rose and Walker, 2004) or, more recently, vowels (Rhodes, 2010); these two sets are distinct from one another to where VC correspondences are perhaps not expected. However, a particularly consonant-like portion of a vowel could well correspond with a nearby consonant or set of consonants; this possibility is explored below for Huautla Mazatec and Aghem. Note same feature [ϕ] in Corr-[ϕ] and Ident-[ϕ]: given approximate similarity, identity is optimal. Phonological categories are known to exhibit this “magnetic” effect generally (Kuhl, 1991) Satisfying Corr is made costly by a high-ranked constraint that penalizes candidates that satisfy Corr and thus Ident (Bennett, 2013); multiple [ϕ] in close proximity might present suboptimal levels of articulatory difficulty The effect (below): dissimilation in C V , not in any V—see especially Aghem’s [+rd] diphthongs, which do not unround but lose their C V Aghem velars Two falling diphthongs [1(î)a] and [u(î w )o] may have velar interruption (Hyman, 1979); these are unit vocoids at some level of the phonological grammar (evidence from high tone spreading) Velars not realized (plain diphthongs) in certain en- vironments, depending on onset place and rounding of diphthong (note contextually rounded w ]): Plain Velarized lab *** -f´ ı î` a ‘plantain’ cor *** s` ı î` amb` ı î` a ‘seven’ vel -k´ ı ´a ‘headpad’ *** lab -bˆ uo ‘to be tired’ *** cor *** - n w ´ o ‘house’ vel -kˆ uo ‘belt’ *** Dissimilation pattern: to avoid Corr, [î] does not appear if preceded by an onset of the same [lab] and [vel] Mazatec laryngeals? Huautla Mazatec has both creaky [+cg] and breathy [+sg] vowels; non-modal phonation is variably timed and can occur separated from the onset by some brief modal voicing (Golston and Kehrein (1998, 315), but cf. also Steriade (1994)) No creaky/breathy vowels following aspirated onsets C ¨ , i.e. *[lar lar] σ (Golston and Kehrein, 1998), but the restriction only applies to breathy vowels in closely re- lated Jalapa de Díaz (Garellek and Keating, 2011) PMaz Jalapa Huautla *k h a 4 ı 21 ‘different’ k h ai ˜ 21 k h ı *t jh a 3 P ´ ˜ u 2 ‘fifteen’ t jh ´ ˜ o ˜ 2 h ´ ˜o *S h e 4 e 4 ‘corn’ e ˜ e From (Kirk, 1966). Caveat: data is surprisingly fragmentary Dissimilation pattern: to avoid Corr and Id, which lead to violation of *[lar lar] σ , the vowel’s [lar] is not realized Vocoid-internal structure ABC+Q (Shih and Inkelas, 2014): segments (Q) consist of at most three subsegments (q n ); a com- plex vocoid’s C V is q 2 in the diagrams below Dissimilation occurs to avoid correspondence with an onset; accomplished by changing features of q 2 (Bennett, 2013) — see below left for details Aghem: Huautla Mazatec: Constraint grammar After Bennett (2013); Shih and Inkelas (2014): *ϕ:v :ϕ is OCP-like—no identical specification for ϕ at some short distance Corr is optimally avoided by changing [ϕ] on a non-onset, here the C V For a particular language, replace ϕ throughout with its dissimilating feature Huautla Mazatec: [lar] ([±sg]/[±cg]) Aghem: [±cons], and Corr only goes into force if C and C V use the same articulators ([αpoa]) References and Thanks Bennett, W. (2013). Dissimilation, Consonant Harmony, and Surface Correspondence. PhD thesis, Rutgers University. Garellek, M. and Keating, P. (2011). The acoustic consequences of phonation and tone interactions in Jalapa Mazatec. JIPA, 41(2):185–205. Golston, C. and Kehrein, W. (1998). Mazatec onsets and nuclei. IJAL, 64(4):311–337. Hyman, L. M., editor (1979). Aghem Grammatical Structure. Number 7 in SCOIL. U. Southern California. Kirk, P. L. (1966). Proto-Mazatec Phonology. PhD thesis, Uni- versity of Washington. Kuhl, P. (1991). Human adults and human infants show a “per- ceptual magnet effect” for the prototypes of speech categories, monkeys do not. Perception & Psychophysics, 50(2):93–107. Rhodes, R. (2010). Vowel harmony as Agreement by Correspon- dence. Unpublished ms., University of California, Berkeley. Rose, S. and Walker, R. (2004). A typology of consonant agree- ment as correspondence. Language, 80(3):475–531. Shih, S. and Inkelas, S. (2014). A subsegmental approach to con- tour tone (dis)harmony patterns. Proc. of Phonology 2013. Steriade, D. (1994). Complex onsets as single segments: the Mazateco pattern. Perspectives in phonology, pages 203–291. Thanks to Stephanie Shih, Florian Lionnet, John Sylak- Glassman, and Sharon Inkelas for their useful comments. Ad- ditional thanks are owed to Erin Donnelly for Mesoamerican references. The usual disclaimers apply. UC Berkeley Phonology Lab Annual Report: ABCConference 456

Transcript of New UC Berkeley Phonology Lab Annual Report: ABC...

Page 1: New UC Berkeley Phonology Lab Annual Report: ABC …linguistics.berkeley.edu/phonlab/documents/2014/ABCC/... · 2016. 9. 27. · Huautla Mazatec: Constraint grammar AfterBennett(2013);ShihandInkelas(2014):

VowelsubsegmentsandsurfacecorrespondenceMatthew Faytak — University of California, Berkeley

OverviewDistinct sets of vowels and consonants are usuallythe subject of studies on correspondence: but whatif the distinction is not so clear?Vocoids are not always static in constriction degree,and they may in fact involve consonant-like “inter-ruptions,” in theory allowing for non-local interac-tions with “marginal” (onset, coda) consonants.These have been handled as OCP effects: a fea-ture [ϕ] is specified in some structural domain, withalignment (potentially mid-vowel) determined byphonetic implementation. Multiple assignments ofthe same [ϕ] in a local unit may lead to unlicensingor removal of one at a “distance.”ABC, with recent expansions to account forsegment-internal structure (Shih and Inkelas, 2014),can similarly model these outcomes: unstable sur-face correspondence compels constricted mid-vowelsubsegments to become more consonant-like; thismight disagree with a language’s phonotactics anddissimilation may result (Bennett, 2013).Here, we investigate two putative cases of this phe-nomenon, both of which involve vowel interruptionsCV with feature(s) [ϕ] that are “uncomfortably” sim-ilar to their onsets.

Repositioning correspondenceABC is most often used to model long-distance interaction between consonants (Rose and Walker, 2004)or, more recently, vowels (Rhodes, 2010); these two sets are distinct from one another to where V↔Ccorrespondences are perhaps not expected.However, a particularly consonant-like portion of a vowel could well correspond with a nearby consonant orset of consonants; this possibility is explored below for Huautla Mazatec and Aghem.

• Note same feature [ϕ] in Corr-[ϕ] and Ident-[ϕ]: given approximate similarity, identity is optimal.Phonological categories are known to exhibit this “magnetic” effect generally (Kuhl, 1991)• Satisfying Corr is made costly by a high-ranked constraint that penalizes candidates that satisfy

Corr and thus Ident (Bennett, 2013); multiple [ϕ] in close proximity might present suboptimallevels of articulatory difficulty• The effect (below): dissimilation in CV , not in any V—see especially Aghem’s [+rd] diphthongs, which

do not unround but lose their CV

Aghem velarsTwo falling diphthongs [1(î)a] and [u(îw)o] may havevelar interruption (Hyman, 1979); these are unit vocoidsat some level of the phonological grammar (evidencefrom high tone spreading)

Velars not realized (→ plain diphthongs) in certain en-vironments, depending on onset place and rounding ofdiphthong (note contextually rounded [îw]):

Plain Velarizedlab *** -fıîa ‘plantain’cor *** sıîambıîa ‘seven’vel -kıa ‘headpad’ ***lab -buo ‘to be tired’ ***cor *** -nduîwo ‘house’vel -kuo ‘belt’ ***

Dissimilation pattern:to avoid Corr, [î] doesnot appear if preceded byan onset of the same [lab]and [vel]

Mazatec laryngeals?Huautla Mazatec has both creaky [+cg] and breathy[+sg] vowels; non-modal phonation is variably timed andcan occur separated from the onset by some brief modalvoicing (Golston and Kehrein (1998, 315), but cf. alsoSteriade (1994))

No creaky/breathy vowels following aspirated onsets C¨,

i.e. *[lar lar]σ (Golston and Kehrein, 1998), but therestriction only applies to breathy vowels in closely re-lated Jalapa de Díaz (Garellek and Keating, 2011)

PMaz Jalapa Huautla*kha4Pı21 ‘different’ khai

˜21 khaı

*tjha3P´u2 ‘fifteen’ tjh´o˜2 tùh´o

*She4Pe4 ‘corn’ he˜

he

From (Kirk, 1966). Caveat: data is surprisingly fragmentary

Dissimilation pattern:to avoid Corr and Id,which lead to violation of*[lar lar]σ, the vowel’s[lar] is not realized

Vocoid-internal structureABC+Q (Shih and Inkelas, 2014): segments (Q)consist of at most three subsegments (qn); a com-plex vocoid’s CV is q2 in the diagrams belowDissimilation occurs to avoid correspondence withan onset; accomplished by changing features of q2(Bennett, 2013) — see below left for details

Aghem:

Huautla Mazatec:

Constraint grammarAfter Bennett (2013); Shih and Inkelas (2014):

• *ϕ:v“:ϕ is OCP-like—no identical specification

for ϕ at some short distance• Corr is optimally avoided by changing [ϕ] on

a non-onset, here the CV

For a particular language, replace ϕ throughoutwith its dissimilating featureHuautla Mazatec: [lar] ([±sg]/[±cg])Aghem: [±cons], and Corr only goes into force ifC and CV use the same articulators ([αpoa])

References and ThanksBennett, W. (2013). Dissimilation, Consonant Harmony, and

Surface Correspondence. PhD thesis, Rutgers University.Garellek, M. and Keating, P. (2011). The acoustic consequences

of phonation and tone interactions in Jalapa Mazatec. JIPA,41(2):185–205.

Golston, C. and Kehrein, W. (1998). Mazatec onsets and nuclei.IJAL, 64(4):311–337.

Hyman, L. M., editor (1979). Aghem Grammatical Structure.Number 7 in SCOIL. U. Southern California.

Kirk, P. L. (1966). Proto-Mazatec Phonology. PhD thesis, Uni-versity of Washington.

Kuhl, P. (1991). Human adults and human infants show a “per-ceptual magnet effect” for the prototypes of speech categories,monkeys do not. Perception & Psychophysics, 50(2):93–107.

Rhodes, R. (2010). Vowel harmony as Agreement by Correspon-dence. Unpublished ms., University of California, Berkeley.

Rose, S. and Walker, R. (2004). A typology of consonant agree-ment as correspondence. Language, 80(3):475–531.

Shih, S. and Inkelas, S. (2014). A subsegmental approach to con-tour tone (dis)harmony patterns. Proc. of Phonology 2013.

Steriade, D. (1994). Complex onsets as single segments: theMazateco pattern. Perspectives in phonology, pages 203–291.

Thanks to Stephanie Shih, Florian Lionnet, John Sylak-Glassman, and Sharon Inkelas for their useful comments. Ad-ditional thanks are owed to Erin Donnelly for Mesoamericanreferences. The usual disclaimers apply.

UC Berkeley Phonology Lab Annual Report: ABC↔Conference

456