NEUROSCIENCE Spatial representations of self and other in ... · NEUROSCIENCE Spatial...

7
NEUROSCIENCE Spatial representations of self and other in the hippocampus Teruko Danjo, 1 Taro Toyoizumi, 2 Shigeyoshi Fujisawa 1 * An animals awareness of its location in space depends on the activity of place cells in the hippocampus. How the brain encodes the spatial position of others has not yet been identified. We investigated neuronal representations of other animalslocations in the dorsal CA1 region of the hippocampus with an observational T-maze task in which one rat was required to observe another rats trajectory to successfully retrieve a reward. Information reflecting the spatial location of both the self and the other was jointly and discretely encoded by CA1 pyramidal cells in the observer rat. A subset of CA1 pyramidal cells exhibited spatial receptive fields that were identical for the self and the other.These findings demonstrate that hippocampal spatial representations include dimensions for both self and nonself. S patial navigation requires the hippocampus (1, 2). The cognitive map theory states that spatial recognition in the hippocampus is allocentric (35). Place cells in the hippo- campus are the physiological correlate of this representation because they are highly sensi- tive to changes in landmarks and contexts (613). The characterization of additional types of navi- gational representations, including head-direction cells and grid cells, has promoted our understand- ing of the neural mechanisms of allocentric spatial representations in the hippocampal-entorhinal cortex network (1419). The studies of these neural maps have focused on the animals own position in space. It still remains unclear whether and how spatial information of nonself, such as the position of conspecifics, landmarks, and moving objects, is represented in the hippocampus. We designed an observational T-maze task using a pair of rats (hereafter termed selfand other) and investigated how the others position is represented in the selfs hippocampus. The self was required to make a left/right choice to re- trieve a reward based on the location of the other. We used two versions of the task, an opposite- side ruleversion in which the self rat had to choose the side opposite to the others location (Fig. 1A, fig. S1, and movie S1) and a same-side rulein which the self rat had to choose the same side as the other rat (fig. S1 and movie S2). During the T-maze task, neuronal activity in the selfs hippocampus (dorsal CA1) was recorded extra- cellularly (n = 3 pairs of rats; 88 ± 8.1% and 84 ± 11% correct performance with opposite-side and same-side rules, respectively). All analyses were performed on single units with pyramidal cell features and place fields in the task area (n = 1298 and 1205 units with opposite-side and same- side rules, respectively) (fig. S2) [see the supple- mentary materials (SM)]. We first examined how the location of the other rat was represented in the opposite-side rule observational task. Firing-rate maps of the selfs positions (selfs rate map) and the others positions (others rate map, which were obtained by replacing the selfs positional data with the others) revealed that in addition to the expected coding of the observers own position in space, the majority of units also displayed obvious place fields for the other (Fig. 1, C to F, top). To under- stand the positional relationship of the rat pair at the time of firing, we constructed joint rate maps by linearizing the ratstrajectories and entering them into two-dimensional x-y axes (fig. S3). Here, the selfs and others place fields composed from identical spikes were combined into a sin- gle joint place field (Fig. 1, C to F, bottom, and fig. S4). We then examined whether these joint place fields were truly modulated by the others positions and not a mere consequence of the constraints in the positional relationships of the two rats. The null hypothesis was that the firing only depended on the selfs position and was independent of the others. By computing the surrogate firing rates that followed this null hypothesis (see the SM) (20), we identified areas with significantly higher firing rates for the other (P < 0.05) (Fig. 1, C to F, third from top, and fig. S5). A significant firing dependency on the others positions was detected for 85% of units (Fig. 1, G to I), indicating that the others spatial information was generally encoded by the place cells in this T-maze task. Importantly, the sig- nificant areas were not dependent on the selfs specific behavior or the others positions (Fig. 1, C to I). Moreover, the firing activities of most units were not dependent on the time periods in the trial (fig. S6). Similar results were also observed when we analyzed the same-side rule version of the task (fig. S7). In the hippocampus, spatial information is en- coded not only by the average firing rates of the neurons but also by the timing of the spikes with respect to the phase of the underlying theta os- cillations (7 to 11 Hz) (2124). We next examined this temporal coding in the others spatial repre- sentations. To control for confounds of the mo- tion of the observer, we focused on units whose rate peaks were located in the selfs starting po- sitions in the opposite-side rule and analyzed the relationship between the others positions and the theta phases of the spikes (n = 78 units). Although the self was not running during the time course of this analysis, the local field po- tential was dominated by theta oscillations (fig. S8 and SM). In contrast to the lack of theta- phase precession relative to the selfs location, 75% of the units displayed significant theta- phase precession as a function of the others location (P < 0.01) (Fig. 1, J to M). The observed theta-phase precession to the others location was consistent with that to the selfs location, as reported previously (2124), demonstrating that the others spatial information was also temporally encoded in the hippocampal pyram- idal cells. We next sought to dissociate two types of unitsthose preferentially modulated by the others position (other-preferred cells) and those preferentially modulated by the selfs goal [goal-preferred cells,also previously described as prospective cells(25, 26)]. We took advan- tage of the two versions of our observational T-maze task (Fig. 2A). Other-preferred cells fire when the other is located on the same side of the side arms (left or right), irrespective of the rule during a given trial and, as a result, these neurons fire when the selfs goal is on a different side across the two conditions (Fig. 2B). This should further support the allocentric representation of the others place. In contrast, goal-preferred cells increase their firing rate only according to the future choice of the observer. By statistically comparing the others rate maps between the two versions, we defined other-preferred cells and goal-preferred cells based on these criteria (see the SM). We identified 58 other-preferred cells (Fig. 2, C to G, and fig. S9) and 252 goal-preferred cells (Fig. 2, H and I). Although the number of other-preferred cells was smaller than that of goal-preferred cells, it represented 13% of the place-responsive units examined in this analysis (Fig. 2F and SM), and the firing-rate peaks of both unit groups distributed all along the side arms in the others rate maps, constituting a full spatial map based on the position of the other rat (Fig. 2, G and I). Furthermore, the analysis of comparing the correct and the error trials in the opposite- side rule resulted in similar percentages of other- preferred and goal-preferred cells (fig. S10). We next looked for another type of spatial representation, a common place fieldfor the self and other, for which a unit fires when either the other or the self is in a specific spatial loca- tion. We found 45 units that had common place fields in both the others and the selfs rate maps under the opposite-side rule (Fig. 3, A to E, and SM). We further tested the trajectory specificity of common place fields by separating left- and right-side targeting trials. Notably, we identified 51 of 1244 units in the opposite-side rule task RESEARCH Danjo et al., Science 359, 213218 (2018) 12 January 2018 1 of 6 1 Laboratory for Systems Neurophysiology, RIKEN Brain Science Institute, 2-1 Hirosawa, Wako, Saitama, 351-0198, Japan. 2 Laboratory for Neural Computation and Adaptation, RIKEN Brain Science Institute, 2-1 Hirosawa, Wako, Saitama, 351-0198, Japan. *Corresponding author. Email: [email protected] on May 21, 2020 http://science.sciencemag.org/ Downloaded from

Transcript of NEUROSCIENCE Spatial representations of self and other in ... · NEUROSCIENCE Spatial...

Page 1: NEUROSCIENCE Spatial representations of self and other in ... · NEUROSCIENCE Spatial representations of self and other in the hippocampus Teruko Danjo,1 Taro Toyoizumi,2 Shigeyoshi

NEUROSCIENCE

Spatial representations of self andother in the hippocampusTeruko Danjo,1 Taro Toyoizumi,2 Shigeyoshi Fujisawa1*

An animal’s awareness of its location in space depends on the activity of place cells inthe hippocampus. How the brain encodes the spatial position of others has not yet beenidentified. We investigated neuronal representations of other animals’ locations in thedorsal CA1 region of the hippocampus with an observational T-maze task in which onerat was required to observe another rat’s trajectory to successfully retrieve a reward.Information reflecting the spatial location of both the self and the other was jointly anddiscretely encoded by CA1 pyramidal cells in the observer rat. A subset of CA1 pyramidalcells exhibited spatial receptive fields that were identical for the self and the other. Thesefindings demonstrate that hippocampal spatial representations include dimensions forboth self and nonself.

Spatial navigation requires the hippocampus(1, 2). The cognitivemap theory states thatspatial recognition in the hippocampus isallocentric (3–5). Place cells in the hippo-campus are the physiological correlate of

this representation because they are highly sensi-tive to changes in landmarks and contexts (6–13).The characterization of additional types of navi-gational representations, includinghead-directioncells and grid cells, has promoted our understand-ing of the neuralmechanisms of allocentric spatialrepresentations in the hippocampal-entorhinalcortex network (14–19). The studies of these neuralmaps have focused on the animal’s own positionin space. It still remains unclear whether andhow spatial information of nonself, such as theposition of conspecifics, landmarks, and movingobjects, is represented in the hippocampus.We designed an observational T-maze task

using a pair of rats (hereafter termed “self” and“other”) and investigated how the other’s positionis represented in the self’s hippocampus. The selfwas required to make a left/right choice to re-trieve a reward based on the location of the other.We used two versions of the task, an “opposite-side rule” version in which the self rat had tochoose the side opposite to the other’s location(Fig. 1A, fig. S1, and movie S1) and a “same-siderule” in which the self rat had to choose the sameside as the other rat (fig. S1 andmovie S2). Duringthe T-maze task, neuronal activity in the self’shippocampus (dorsal CA1) was recorded extra-cellularly (n = 3 pairs of rats; 88 ± 8.1% and 84 ±11% correct performance with opposite-side andsame-side rules, respectively). All analyses wereperformed on single units with pyramidal cellfeatures and place fields in the task area (n =1298 and 1205 unitswith opposite-side and same-

side rules, respectively) (fig. S2) [see the supple-mentary materials (SM)].We first examined how the location of the

other rat was represented in the opposite-siderule observational task. Firing-rate maps of theself’s positions (“self’s ratemap”) and the other’spositions (“other’s ratemap,”whichwere obtainedby replacing the self’s positional data with theother’s) revealed that in addition to the expectedcoding of the observer’s own position in space,themajority of units also displayed obvious placefields for the other (Fig. 1, C to F, top). To under-stand the positional relationship of the rat pair atthe time of firing, we constructed joint rate mapsby linearizing the rats’ trajectories and enteringthem into two-dimensional x-y axes (fig. S3).Here, the self’s and other’s place fields composedfrom identical spikes were combined into a sin-gle joint place field (Fig. 1, C to F, bottom, andfig. S4). We then examined whether these jointplace fields were truly modulated by the other’spositions and not a mere consequence of theconstraints in the positional relationships ofthe two rats. The null hypothesis was that thefiring only depended on the self’s position andwas independent of the other’s. By computingthe surrogate firing rates that followed this nullhypothesis (see the SM) (20), we identified areaswith significantly higher firing rates for theother (P < 0.05) (Fig. 1, C to F, third from top, andfig. S5). A significant firing dependency on theother’s positions was detected for 85% of units(Fig. 1, G to I), indicating that the other’s spatialinformationwas generally encoded by the placecells in this T-maze task. Importantly, the sig-nificant areas were not dependent on the self’sspecific behavior or the other’s positions (Fig. 1,C to I). Moreover, the firing activities of mostunits were not dependent on the time periodsin the trial (fig. S6). Similar results were alsoobserved when we analyzed the same-side ruleversion of the task (fig. S7).In the hippocampus, spatial information is en-

coded not only by the average firing rates of theneurons but also by the timing of the spikes withrespect to the phase of the underlying theta os-

cillations (7 to 11 Hz) (21–24). We next examinedthis temporal coding in the other’s spatial repre-sentations. To control for confounds of the mo-tion of the observer, we focused on units whoserate peaks were located in the self’s starting po-sitions in the opposite-side rule and analyzedthe relationship between the other’s positionsand the theta phases of the spikes (n = 78 units).Although the self was not running during thetime course of this analysis, the local field po-tential was dominated by theta oscillations (fig.S8 and SM). In contrast to the lack of theta-phase precession relative to the self’s location,75% of the units displayed significant theta-phase precession as a function of the other’slocation (P < 0.01) (Fig. 1, J to M). The observedtheta-phase precession to the other’s locationwas consistent with that to the self’s location,as reported previously (21–24), demonstratingthat the other’s spatial information was alsotemporally encoded in the hippocampal pyram-idal cells.We next sought to dissociate two types of

units—those preferentially modulated by theother’s position (“other-preferred cells”) andthose preferentiallymodulated by the self’s goal[“goal-preferred cells,” also previously describedas “prospective cells” (25, 26)]. We took advan-tage of the two versions of our observationalT-maze task (Fig. 2A). Other-preferred cells firewhen the other is located on the same side ofthe side arms (left or right), irrespective of therule during a given trial and, as a result, theseneurons fire when the self’s goal is on a differentside across the two conditions (Fig. 2B). This shouldfurther support the allocentric representationof the other’s place. In contrast, goal-preferredcells increase their firing rate only according tothe future choice of the observer. By statisticallycomparing the other’s rate maps between thetwo versions, we defined other-preferred cells andgoal-preferred cells based on these criteria (seethe SM). We identified 58 other-preferred cells(Fig. 2, C to G, and fig. S9) and 252 goal-preferredcells (Fig. 2, H and I). Although the number ofother-preferred cells was smaller than that ofgoal-preferred cells, it represented 13% of theplace-responsive units examined in this analysis(Fig. 2F and SM), and the firing-rate peaks of bothunit groups distributed all along the side arms inthe other’s rate maps, constituting a full spatialmap based on the position of the other rat (Fig. 2,G and I). Furthermore, the analysis of comparingthe correct and the error trials in the opposite-side rule resulted in similar percentages of other-preferred and goal-preferred cells (fig. S10).We next looked for another type of spatial

representation, “a common place field” for theself and other, for which a unit fires when eitherthe other or the self is in a specific spatial loca-tion. We found 45 units that had common placefields in both the other’s and the self’s ratemapsunder the opposite-side rule (Fig. 3, A to E, andSM). We further tested the trajectory specificityof common place fields by separating left- andright-side targeting trials. Notably, we identified51 of 1244 units in the opposite-side rule task

RESEARCH

Danjo et al., Science 359, 213–218 (2018) 12 January 2018 1 of 6

1Laboratory for Systems Neurophysiology, RIKEN BrainScience Institute, 2-1 Hirosawa, Wako, Saitama, 351-0198,Japan. 2Laboratory for Neural Computation and Adaptation,RIKEN Brain Science Institute, 2-1 Hirosawa, Wako, Saitama,351-0198, Japan.*Corresponding author. Email: [email protected]

on May 21, 2020

http://science.sciencem

ag.org/D

ownloaded from

Page 2: NEUROSCIENCE Spatial representations of self and other in ... · NEUROSCIENCE Spatial representations of self and other in the hippocampus Teruko Danjo,1 Taro Toyoizumi,2 Shigeyoshi

Danjo et al., Science 359, 213–218 (2018) 12 January 2018 2 of 6

Center Arm Side ArmsStart L R L R

Other’s normalized rate map

Self’s normalized rate map

Significant area on the other’s positions

Sel

f’s p

osi

tio

n

Other

11.5 5.4

17.4 d0422_u95

Other Self

L L RR Centerstart arms side arms

LL

RR

Cen

ter

Other’s position

0

4

8

Fir

ing

rat

e (H

z)

Nu

mb

er o

f U

nit

s

Correlation of Theta Phase and Other’s Position

0 0.1 0.2 0.3 0.4 0.50

5

10

15p<0.01

Center Arm Side ArmsStart L R L R

16.9 21.1Other Self

0

5

10

15

43.9 d0222_u5

L L RR Centerstart arms side arms

LL

RR

Cen

ter

Other’s position

Other

13.4 26.4

17.9

Other Self

d0330_u740

4

8

L L RR Centerstart arms side arms

LL

RR

Cen

ter

Other’s position

Other

27.727.3

29.0 d1008_u13

Other Self

0

10

20

30

L L RR Centerstart arms side arms

LL

RR

Cen

ter

Other’s position

Other

Self

Other 25.4

26.7

Theta Phase (degree)

Firing Rate (Hz)

Theta Phase (degree)

Firing Rate (Hz)

0

180

360

0

180

360

020

0

6

start T-crossPosition of Rats

d1008_u13

14.8Self

Other 15.7

start T-crossPosition of Rats

0

180

360

0

180

360

Theta Phase (degree)

Firing Rate (Hz)

0

15

Theta Phase (degree)

Firing Rate (Hz)

0

2

d0215_u13

10.6Self

Other 12.5

Theta Phase (degree)

Firing Rate (Hz)

Theta Phase (degree)

Firing Rate (Hz)

0

180

360

0

180

360

0

10

0

3

start T-crossPosition of Rats

d0222_u28

112

98ce

ll n

um

ber

112

98ce

ll n

um

ber

112

98ce

ll n

um

ber

Cen

ter

arm

Right

RightLeft

Left

Start arms

Side arms

reward spouts

reward spouts

Self Other

Fig. 1. Joint spatial representations of self and other in theobservational task. (A) Schematic of the opposite-side rule. Seefig. S1A. (B) Schematic of linearizing the trajectories on the T-maze.See figs. S3 and S4. (C to F) Units whose firing rates significantlydepended on the other’s position. (Top) Other’s (left) and self’s (right)rate maps. (Second from top) Trajectory plots of other (left) andself (right). Gray lines, rats’ trajectories; red dots, spike positions.(Third from top) Linearized rate plots on other’s position (black lines).Red lines indicate statistically significant bands (P < 0.05) (see fig. S5).Blue thick line indicates the significant area. (Bottom) Joint ratemaps. Other’s and self’s linearized positions are coordinated in

horizontal and vertical axes, respectively. The maximum firing rate isindicated at the top of each map. (G to I) Linearized and normalized ratemaps based on the other’s position (G), the self’s position (H), and thesignificant area on the other’s position (I), sorted by maximum ratepositions on the other’s map (n = 1298 units). (J to L) Units withtheta-phase precession based on the other’s position. (Top) Self’s ratemaps (left), linearized rate plots, and theta phases of the spikes as afunction of self’s position (right). (Bottom) Other’s rate maps (left),linearized rate plots, and theta phases of the spikes as a function of other’sposition (right). (M) Correlation between theta phase and the other’sposition. Color bars indicate significant correlation (P < 0.01; n = 59 units).

RESEARCH | REPORTon M

ay 21, 2020

http://science.sciencemag.org/

Dow

nloaded from

Page 3: NEUROSCIENCE Spatial representations of self and other in ... · NEUROSCIENCE Spatial representations of self and other in the hippocampus Teruko Danjo,1 Taro Toyoizumi,2 Shigeyoshi

that had trajectory-specific common place fields(Fig. 3, F and G; fig. S11; and SM), which wassignificantly higher than the number expectedby chance (P < 0.05) (see SM).Because the allocentric representation of self’s

place was also demonstrated by reliable recon-struction of self’s positions only from the spikesof place-cell ensembles (27, 28), we further testedwhether the spikes of joint-place-cell ensemblescould also reconstruct the other’s positions. First,we performed Bayesian decoding analysis witha leave-one-out strategy in each rule (SM). The

reconstructed other’s trajectories revealed thatthe spikes of joint-place-cell ensembles containedsufficient information regarding the other’s po-sitions (Fig. 4A and fig. S12A). Although the er-ror distances between the actual and decodedpositions were larger for the other’s decodingthan those for the self, the differences in theerror distances were as small as 5 cm per timewindow (200 ms) in both rules (other’s versusself’s error distances, 20.3 versus 16.1 cm in theopposite-side rule, 20.1 versus 15.1 cm in the same-side rule) (Fig. 4B). We then examined whether

these spikes contained more information aboutthe other’s place than that obtained by distribu-tions of time spent in the positional relationshipsof the self and the other. We computed a controlestimation of the other’s positions by reconstruct-ing the self’s positions and then accordinglyreferring to the probability distributions of thepositional relationships (fig. S12B and SM). Forthe Bayesian reconstructions in this analysis,the prior templates were computed from trialsincluding both rules, and results were examinedbased on error distances in a block-wisemanner

Danjo et al., Science 359, 213–218 (2018) 12 January 2018 3 of 6

Fig. 2. Spatial representa-tions of other-preferredand goal-preferred cells.(A) Schematic of the relation-ship of the other’s positionand the self’s goal in opposite-side and same-side rules.(B) Schematic explainingother-preferred cells (left)and goal-preferred cells(right). Other-preferredcells fire preferentially whenthe other is on the same side,regardless of the rule. Simi-larly, goal-preferredcells fire when the self’sgoal is on the same side.(C to E) Rate maps ofother-preferred cells.(Top) Other’s rate mapsin the opposite-side rule(left) and same-side rule(right). (Bottom) Self’s ratemaps in the opposite-siderule (left) and the same-siderule (right). See fig. S9for statistical methods.(F) Summary of other-preferred cells and goal-preferred cells (n = total434 units). (G) Linearizedand normalized ratemaps of other-referredcells, sorted by peak ratepositions. (H) Other’s (top)and self’s (bottom) rate mapsof goal-preferred cells in theopposite-side rule (left)and the same-side rule(right). (I) Linearized andnormalized other’s ratemaps of goal-preferred cellsin the opposite-side task(left) and same-side task(right), sorted by themaximum rate positions.

Center Arm Side ArmsStart L R L R

Center Arm Side ArmsStart L R L R

Opposite-side rule Same-side rule

Other

Self

Opposite-side rule Same-side rule

d0215_u35

14.610.4

14.2 17.9

32.5 6.1

39.2 12.4

Opposite-side rule Same-side rule

d0330_u70

40.629.4

53.640.5

Opposite-side rule Same-side rule

d0401_u77

Opposite-side rule Same-side rule

124 (29%)

252 (58%)

58 (13%)Other-preferred cells

Goal-preferred cells

1ce

ll nu

mbe

r58

The rest

Self Self

Other Other

Other-preferred cells(Prospective Cells)

Oth

er’

plac

e fie

ldS

elf’

goa

l

Opposite-siderule

Same-siderule

Opposite-siderule

Same-siderule

Other Other

Self Self

Goal-preferred cells

Other

Self

Opposite-side rule Same-side rule

22.8 8.4

27.5 9.7d0215_u32

Opposite-side rule Same-side rule

125

2ce

ll nu

mbe

r

Center Arm Side ArmsStart L R L R

Center Arm Side ArmsStart L R L R

Other-preferred cells

Goal-preferred cells

Other-preferred cells

ss

RESEARCH | REPORTon M

ay 21, 2020

http://science.sciencemag.org/

Dow

nloaded from

Page 4: NEUROSCIENCE Spatial representations of self and other in ... · NEUROSCIENCE Spatial representations of self and other in the hippocampus Teruko Danjo,1 Taro Toyoizumi,2 Shigeyoshi

(Fig. 4, C and D, and SM). Reconstructing trajec-torieswithout rule information apparentlymadethe error distances of the other’s decoded posi-tions larger, but they were significantly smallerthan the control estimate of the other’s positions(Fig. 4D). The overall averages of the error dis-tances of the other’s decoding were less than40 cm and also were significantly smaller thanthose of the control estimation in both rules(Fig. 4E).

We propose an extended model of hippocam-pal spatial representations that can include di-mensions for both self and other (fig. S13). Ourmodel, encompassing various types of spatialrepresentations, can categorize spatial represen-tations into four types: own place fields, jointplace fields, other’s place fields, and commonplace fields (fig. S13, A to D). In particular, thecommon place field could be hypothesized to beamirror representation of place (29, 30). Combi-

natorial representations of spatial informationof self and nonself would open the door to exam-ining whether this allocentric spatial represen-tation extendsmore generally to other nonlivingmoving objects (31–33) and how it is generatedin the hippocampal-entorhinal cortex network.Our data indicate that the place cells in the hip-pocampus encode sufficient spatial informationfor organizing the recognition of other animals,which is essential for social behavior (34, 35).

Danjo et al., Science 359, 213–218 (2018) 12 January 2018 4 of 6

Oth

erS

elf

Right trials Left trials 2D productsWhole

24.1

17.2

0.68

0.29d0214_58

Oth

erS

elf

Right trials Left trials 2D productsWhole

17.3

13.8

d0923_32

0.36

0.89

SelfOther 27.2 33.6

start T-cross

Normalized firing rate 1D Products

0

0

1 1

0

0.5SelfOther

2D products

0.76

start T-cross

SelfOther 16.3 16.0 2D products

SelfOther 19.3 23.4 2D products

start T-cross

d0925_u13

d0323_u1

0.64d0213_u26

0.84

0

1

0

1

0

1

0

1

0

0.5

0

0.5

X

X

X

145

Cel

l num

ber

Self’s rate maps

Center SideStart L R L R

Other’s rate maps

Center SideStart L R L R

1D products

Center SideStart L R L R

Nor

mal

ized

firin

g ra

te (

Hz)

1D P

rodu

ct

Fig. 3. Common place fields in self’s and other’s rate maps. (A to C)Common place fields of representative units in the other’s (left) andthe self’s (middle) rate maps. (Right) The products (element-wisemultiplications) of normalized other’s and self’s firing rates in each pixel.(D) Linearized other’s (left) and self’s (middle) firing rates and theirproducts (right). The products are the element-wise multiplications of thenormalized firing rates of the self and the other at each position, of theunits shown in (A) to (C). This value was used to identify units withcommon place fields. (E) Linearized and normalized rate maps of units

with common place fields aligned by the position with the maximumvalue of the products (n = 45 of 908 units). (F and G) Trajectory-specificcommon place fields of two representative units. (Left) Other’s (top) andself’s (bottom) rate maps of whole trials. (Middle) Rate maps with the left andright trials separated. (Right) The products of normalized other’s and self’sfiring rates of right (top) and left (bottom) targeting trials in each pixel.Arrows indicate the centers of common place fields. The maximum colorvalue of the product map was set as 0.5, and the values in the product mapindicate the maximum of the product.

RESEARCH | REPORTon M

ay 21, 2020

http://science.sciencemag.org/

Dow

nloaded from

Page 5: NEUROSCIENCE Spatial representations of self and other in ... · NEUROSCIENCE Spatial representations of self and other in the hippocampus Teruko Danjo,1 Taro Toyoizumi,2 Shigeyoshi

REFERENCES AND NOTES

1. N. Burgess, E. A. Maguire, J. O’Keefe, Neuron 35, 625–641(2002).

2. A. D. Ekstrom et al., Nature 425, 184–188 (2003).3. E. C. Tolman, Psychol. Rev. 55, 189–208 (1948).4. J. O’Keefe, L. Nadel, The Hippocampus as a Cognitive Map

(Clarendon Press, Oxford, 1978).5. G. Buzsáki, E. I. Moser, Nat. Neurosci. 16, 130–138 (2013).6. J. O’Keefe, J. Dostrovsky, Brain Res. 34, 171–175 (1971).7. E. R. Wood, P. A. Dudchenko, H. Eichenbaum, Nature 397,

613–616 (1999).8. I. Lee, D. Yoganarasimha, G. Rao, J. J. Knierim, Nature 430,

456–459 (2004).

9. S. Leutgeb et al., Science 309, 619–623 (2005).10. T. J. Wills, C. Lever, F. Cacucci, N. Burgess, J. O’Keefe, Science

308, 873–876 (2005).11. S. A. Ho et al., Neuroscience 157, 254–270 (2008).12. S. S. Deshmukh, J. J. Knierim, Hippocampus 23, 253–267

(2013).13. X. Mou, D. Ji, eLife 5, e18022 (2016).14. J. S. Taube, R. U. Muller, J. B. Ranck Jr., J. Neurosci. 10,

420–435 (1990).15. T. Hafting, M. Fyhn, S. Molden, M. B. Moser, E. I. Moser, Nature

436, 801–806 (2005).16. D. J. Foster, M. A. Wilson, Nature 440, 680–683 (2006).17. P. Ravassard et al., Science 340, 1342–1346 (2013).

18. E. Kropff, J. E. Carmichael, M. B. Moser, E. I. Moser, Nature523, 419–424 (2015).

19. B. L. McNaughton, F. P. Battaglia, O. Jensen, E. I. Moser,M. B. Moser, Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 7, 663–678 (2006).

20. S. Fujisawa, A. Amarasingham, M. T. Harrison, G. Buzsáki,Nat. Neurosci. 11, 823–833 (2008).

21. J. O’Keefe, M. L. Recce, Hippocampus 3, 317–330(1993).

22. M. R. Mehta, A. K. Lee, M. A. Wilson, Nature 417, 741–746(2002).

23. G. Dragoi, G. Buzsáki, Neuron 50, 145–157 (2006).24. C. D. Harvey, F. Collman, D. A. Dombeck, D. W. Tank, Nature

461, 941–946 (2009).

Danjo et al., Science 359, 213–218 (2018) 12 January 2018 5 of 6

100

80

40

60

20

01 2 3 4 5 61 2 3 4 5 6

Same-side ruleOpposite-side rule

Same-side

ruleOpposite-side

rule

40

60

20

0

1 2 34

5

6

1 2 34

5

6

Opposite-side rule

Same-side rule

trial 41 trial 42 trial 46

trial 47 trial 48 trial 49 trial 50

trial 43 trial 44 trial 45

trial 41 trial 42 trial 46

trial 47 trial 48 trial 49 trial 50

trial 43 trial 44 trial 45

0

5

10

15

20

25

OtherSelf

OtherSelf

0

5

10

15

20

25

Other’sPosition

Self’sPosition

Opposite-siderule

Same-siderule

Other’sPosition

Self’sPosition

Block Block

Opposite-side rule

Err

or d

ista

nce

/ Blo

ck (

cm)

Err

or d

ista

nce

(cm

)

Err

or d

ista

nce

(cm

)

Other’s decoding

Control estimation

decodeactual

decodeactual

Fig. 4. Decoding other’s and self’s positions from spikes of hippocampalcells. (A) Reconstructed other’s and self’s positions in the opposite-siderule. Red and blue lines, reconstructed other’s and self’s positions,respectively. Black lines, actual trajectories of the other or self. (B) Meanerror distances between actual and decoded positions (31 and 28 sessionsfrom the opposite-side and same-side rules, respectively). (C to E) Thereconstructions of other’s positions by using prior templates calculated fromtrials including both rules. (C) Schematic of the division of other’s trajectories

into six blocks. (D) Error distances of reconstructed other’s positions in eachblock. Red bars, distance between the actual and decoded other’s positions.Gray bars, distance between the actual and estimated other’s positions(n = 22 sessions) (two-way repeated measures analysis of variance: P < 0.001,F5 = 83.95 for the opposite-side rule; P < 0.001, F5 = 72.6 for the same-siderule). (E) Averaged error distances per block (two-tailed paired t test: P <0.001, t21 = –10.70 for the opposite-side rule; P < 0.001, t21 = –10.95 for thesame-side rule). Data are mean ± SEM.

RESEARCH | REPORTon M

ay 21, 2020

http://science.sciencemag.org/

Dow

nloaded from

Page 6: NEUROSCIENCE Spatial representations of self and other in ... · NEUROSCIENCE Spatial representations of self and other in the hippocampus Teruko Danjo,1 Taro Toyoizumi,2 Shigeyoshi

25. L. M. Frank, E. N. Brown, M. Wilson, Neuron 27, 169–178(2000).

26. E. R. Wood, P. A. Dudchenko, R. J. Robitsek, H. Eichenbaum,Neuron 27, 623–633 (2000).

27. M. A. Wilson, B. L. McNaughton, Science 261, 1055–1058(1993).

28. K. Zhang, I. Ginzburg, B. L. McNaughton, T. J. Sejnowski,J. Neurophysiol. 79, 1017–1044 (1998).

29. V. Gallese, L. Fadiga, L. Fogassi, G. Rizzolatti, Brain 119,593–609 (1996).

30. G. Rizzolatti, L. Craighero, Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 27, 169–192(2004).

31. S. Wirth et al., Science 300, 1578–1581 (2003).

32. R. Q. Quiroga, L. Reddy, G. Kreiman, C. Koch, I. Fried, Nature435, 1102–1107 (2005).

33. N. T. M. Robinson et al., Neuron 94, 677–688.e6 (2017).34. F. L. Hitti, S. A. Siegelbaum, Nature 508, 88–92 (2014).35. T. Okuyama, T. Kitamura, D. S. Roy, S. Itohara, S. Tonegawa,

Science 353, 1536–1541 (2016).

ACKNOWLEDGMENTS

We thank A. Amarasingham for helpful suggestions on dataanalysis and T. J. McHugh and C. Yokoyama for comments on themanuscript. This work was supported by Japan Society for thePromotion of Science Grants-in-Aid for Scientific Research(KAKENHI) nos. 16K14561 and 16H01290 to T.D. and 15H05876 and

16H01519 to S.F. All data necessary to support the paper’sconclusions are present in the paper and the supplementary materials.

SUPPLEMENTARY MATERIALS

www.sciencemag.org/content/359/6372/213/suppl/DC1Materials and MethodsFigs. S1 to S13Movies S1 and S2References (36–41)

14 July 2017; accepted 7 December 201710.1126/science.aao3898

Danjo et al., Science 359, 213–218 (2018) 12 January 2018 6 of 6

RESEARCH | REPORTon M

ay 21, 2020

http://science.sciencemag.org/

Dow

nloaded from

Page 7: NEUROSCIENCE Spatial representations of self and other in ... · NEUROSCIENCE Spatial representations of self and other in the hippocampus Teruko Danjo,1 Taro Toyoizumi,2 Shigeyoshi

Spatial representations of self and other in the hippocampusTeruko Danjo, Taro Toyoizumi and Shigeyoshi Fujisawa

DOI: 10.1126/science.aao3898 (6372), 213-218.359Science 

, this issue p. 213, p. 218Scienceconspecifics in rat and bat brains, respectively.

now report the discovery of a subgroup of neurons in hippocampal area CA1 that encodes the presence ofet al.Omer andet al.Surprisingly, very little is known about how the position of other animals is represented in the brain. Danjo

need to know the position of other individuals for social interactions, observational learning, and group navigation. Different sets of neurons encode the spatial position and orientation of an organism. However, social animals

The representation of others in space

ARTICLE TOOLS http://science.sciencemag.org/content/359/6372/213

MATERIALSSUPPLEMENTARY http://science.sciencemag.org/content/suppl/2018/01/11/359.6372.213.DC1

CONTENTRELATED http://science.sciencemag.org/content/sci/359/6372/218.full

REFERENCES

http://science.sciencemag.org/content/359/6372/213#BIBLThis article cites 40 articles, 9 of which you can access for free

PERMISSIONS http://www.sciencemag.org/help/reprints-and-permissions

Terms of ServiceUse of this article is subject to the

is a registered trademark of AAAS.ScienceScience, 1200 New York Avenue NW, Washington, DC 20005. The title (print ISSN 0036-8075; online ISSN 1095-9203) is published by the American Association for the Advancement ofScience

Science. No claim to original U.S. Government WorksCopyright © 2018 The Authors, some rights reserved; exclusive licensee American Association for the Advancement of

on May 21, 2020

http://science.sciencem

ag.org/D

ownloaded from