Fundamentals of Neuroscience Neuroimaging in Cognitive Neuroscience
Embed Size (px)
description
Transcript of Fundamentals of Neuroscience Neuroimaging in Cognitive Neuroscience

Fundamentals of Neuroscience
Neuroimaging in Cognitive Neuroscience
James DanckertPAS 4040 [email protected]

Functional Neuroimaging
• Electrical activity– Event-related potentials (ERP), visual evoked potentials
(VEP) all derivative from EEG
• Stimulation– Trans-cranial magnetic stimulation – single vs. rapid
pulse TMS
• Metabolism– Positron Emission Tomography (PET) and Blood
Oxygenated Level Dependent (BOLD) functional MRI (fMRI)

EEG• Large populations of
neurons firing produce electrical potentials that can be measured at the scalp
• Signals are passively conducted through the skull and scalp and can be amplified and measured
• Difference between reference (ground) and recording electrodes are measured to give the electrical potential – electroencephalogram (EEG)

ERPs and VEPs• EEG tends to record global brain activity• ERPs (and VEPs) are a special case of EEG• average EEG trace from a large number of trials • align signal to onset of a stimulus or response – hence event-
related potential (ERP)

Pros and cons of ERPs.
• Good temporal resolution• Linked to specific physiological markers (e.g.,
N1, P3 etc. which in turn can be linked to known cognitive processes)
• Poor spatial resolution• Difficult to get at some brain regions (OFC,
temporal cortex)
Cons
Pros

Transcranial Magnetic Stimulation (TMS)
• Thompson (1910) placed head between two coils and stimulated at ~ 42 Hz• saw flashing lights – magnetophosphenes• was probably stimulating the retina and not the visual cortex
Cowey and Walsh, 2001

TMS
• TMS applies a magnetic pulse to a certain brain region to temporarily modulate the function of that region

TMS
• the induced current in the tissue is in the opposite direction to that of the coil
• the intensity of the signal drops off towards the centre and outside of the coil
circular coil induced current
Cowey and Walsh, 2001

TMS
• the flow of the current must cross the axon to cause stimulation or interruption of function (N3 will not be stimulated)
maximum depolarization
maximum hyperpolarization
Cowey and Walsh, 2001
little or no change

TMS

Spatial extent of TMS• spatial extent of induced electric field
– drops ~ 75% within 10 mm– affects 600 mm2 of neural tissue

Rapid vs. Single Pulse TMS
• for single pulse TMS duration of stimulation = 1 msec, but affects motor cortex for up to 100 msec
• for rapid or repetitive pulse TMS stimuli are delivered in trains with frequencies from 1 to 25 Hz (1 – 25 times per second)
• duration of after-effects for rapid pulse TMS anywhere from msec to several seconds

• excitatory or inhibitory reversible effects depending on site and parameters of stimulation (e.g. frequency of pulses) -> facilitates or slows down cognitive process/behavior
• when inhibitory, referred to as ‘virtual lesion technique’
• can give precise timing information (msec level) due to transient nature of effects
• rTMS is beginning to be used as a treatment for depression (focus is on DLPFC)
Transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS / rTMS)

TMS
• Poor spatial localisation – how focal is the stimulation?
• Can’t stimulate certain areas (e.g., temporal lobe) and can only stimulate cortical surface
• Good temporal resolution• Can presumably disrupt individual processes
within a task.• Distance effects – changed interactions due
to stimulation• Can induce seizures (particularly rTMS)

Frameless stereotaxy and fMRI
• areas can be identified functionally and then used to position the coil in a TMS study using the frameless stereotaxy method
• Paus is attempting to directly combine fMRI and TMS – with TMS pulses delivered in between fMRI runs

Metabolic Imaging
• Two main techniques – positron emission tomography (PET) and functional MRI (fMRI)
• Activity in cells requires energy (oxygen and glucose)
• Increased neural activity will lead to changes in cerebral blood volume (CBV), cerebral blood flow (CBF) and the rate of metabolism of glucose and oxygen (CRMGl and CRMO)
• These changes in blood flow and metabolism can be measured using PET and fMRI

Positron Emission Tomography (PET)
• Measures local changes in cerebral blood flow (CBF) or volume and can also be used to trace certain neurotransmitters (but can only do one of these at a time)
• Radioactive isotopes are used as tracers
• The isotopes rapidly decay emitting positrons
• When the positrons collide with electrons two photons (or gamma rays) are emitted
• The two photons travel in opposite directions allowing the location of the collision to be determined

Positron Emission Tomography (PET)

PET and subtraction• Run two conditions
– stimulation (e.g., look at visual images) vs. control (e.g., look at blank screen)
• Measure the difference in activation between the two images (i.e., subtract control from stimulation)
• This provides a picture of regional cerebral blood flow relative to visual stimulation.

Motion vs. colour.• Subject views
coloured screen (left) vs. moving random black and white dots (right)
• Both task activate early visual areas (V1 and V2)
• Subtracting the two images reveals different brain areas for colour (V4) vs. motion (V5) processing

PET vs. fMRI• PET allows you to track multiple metabolic
processes so long as the emitted photon can be detected – allows imaging of some neurotransmitters
• PET is invasive – radioactive isotopes can only be administered (at experimental levels) every 4 – 5 years
• fMRI has much greater spatial resolution (~ mms)
• fMRI has greater temporal resolution – can detect activation to stimuli appearing for less than a second (PET is limited by the half life of the isotope used)

fMRI

Magnet safety
• very strong magnetic fields – even large and heavy objects can ‘fly’ into the magnet bore

Cerebral blood supply.• Arterioles
– Y=95% at rest.– Y=100% during activation.– 25 m diameter.– <15% blood volume of cortical
tissue.• Venules
– Y=60% at rest.– Y=90% during activation.– 25-50 m diameter.– 40% blood volume of cortical
tissue.• Red blood cell
– 6 m wide and 1-2 m thick.– Delivers O2 in form of
oxyhemoglobin.
• Capillaries– Y=80% at rest.– Y=90% during activation.– 8 m diameter.– 40% blood volume of cortical
tissue.
– Primary site of O2 exchange with tissue.
Artery Vein
Arterioles Veneoles
Capillaries
1 - 2 cm
Neurons
Transit Time = 2-3 s

Cerebral blood supply.

fMRI
• Deoxyhaemoglobin is paramagnetic• When neural activity increases more oxygenated
blood than is needed is delivered to the site• This leads to an imbalance in oxyhaemoglobin
and deoxyhaemoglobin – more oxy than deoxy• fMRI is able to measure this difference due to the
different magnetic properties of oxy and deoxyhaemoglobin

fMRI and BOLD
• blood oxygenated level dependent (BOLD) signal is actually a complex combination of:– rate of glucose
and oxygen metabolism
– CBV– CBF
• same subtraction logic used in PET is used in fMRI

fMRI – block design• fMRI (like PET) began examining brain activity using block designs
colours
rest
motion
restrestrest
colours

fMRI – event-related design
• allows randomization of stimuli (not possible in PET)

fMRI – event-related design• BOLD response has a predictable form
• In rapid event-related designs the signal to a given trial type is deconvolved using models of the BOLD response

Linearity of BOLD responseDale & Buckner, 1997
Linearity:“Do things really add up?”
red = 2 - 1
green = 3 - 2
Sync each trial response to start of trial
Not quite linear but good enough !

Fixed vs. Random Intervals If trials are jittered, ITI power
Source: Burock et al., 1998

fMRI spatial resolution• images can be co-registered to the subject’s own brain
(not an average brain as in PET)
PET fMRI

fMRI and topologies• Using fMRI to “map” different brain functions
Penfield’s maps
Servos et al., 1998red = wrist; orange = shoulder

EXPANDING RINGS
Retintopy
• 8 Hz flicker (checks reverse contrast 8X/sec)• good stimulus for driving visual areas• subjects must maintain fixation (on red dot)
Source: Jody Culham
ROTATING WEDGES

Source: Jody Culham
time = 0
time = 20 sec
time = 60 sec
time = 40 sec
0 20 40 60
TIME STIMULUS
EXPECTED RESPONSE PROFILE OF AREA RESPONDING TO STIMULUS To analyze
retinotopic data:
Analyze the data with a set of functions with the same profile but different phase offsets.
For any voxels that show a significant response to any of the functions, color code the activation by the phase offset that yielded maximum activation (e.g., maximum response to foveal stimulus = red, maximum response to peripheral stimulus = pink)

Retintopy: Eccentricity
calcarinesulcus
left occipitallobe
right occipitallobe
• foveal area represented at occipital pole• peripheral regions represented more anteriorly

Retinotopy
Source: Sereno et al., 1995

Other Sensory “-topies”
Audition: Tonotopy
cochlea
Sylvian fissure
temporal lobe

Saccadotopy
Source: Sereno et al., 2001
•delayed saccades
•move saccadic target systematically around the clock
http://kamares.ucsd.edu/~sereno/LIP/both-closeup+stim.mpgMarty Sereno’s web page

Break

Finding the human homologue of monkey area X!
• recent research has used monkey neurophys to guide fMRI in humans
Dukelow et al. 2001

Problems with the search for homologues
• Absence of activation doesn’t mean the absence of function• Presence of activation doesn’t imply sole locus of function• But our brains are different!• Confirmatory hypotheses
Dukelow et al. 2001

fMRI and diagnosis
• fMRI is starting to be used in patients with epilepsy
• one goal is to use this as a tool to localise language, memory etc. prior to surgery
• another goal would be to use fMRI to study the propogation of seizures
• in stroke patients fMRI can be used to chart recovery of function

Patient SP – congenital porencephalic cystPatient SP – congenital porencephalic cyst
Left Right

im a g e s
1 2 1 4 1
% s
igna
l cha
nge
0
1
2
3
-1
SP - motor stripSP - motor strip
sequential tapping
alternating tapping
motor strip

im a g e s
1 2 1 4 1
% s
igna
l cha
nge
0
1
2
3
-1
SP – somatosensory stripSP – somatosensory strip
sequential tapping
alternating tapping
somatosensorystrip

6.0
4.0
2.0
0.0
-2.0% s
ignal
chan
ge
1 21 41images
6
4
2
0
-1
1
3
5
% s
ignal
chang
eimages
sequential tapping
alternating tapping
superiorparietal

% s
ign
al c
ha
ng
e
images1 21 41
2
1
0
-1
name animals
name objects
non-word sounds (‘ba’)
Broca’s area?

% s
ign
al c
ha
ng
e
images1 21 41
2
1
0
-1
name animals
name objects
non-word sounds (‘ba’)
left occipital

fMRI and cognition
• What not to do – poorly designed tasks!
• What is the right inferior parietal lobe’s contribution to movement control?
– spatial component of movements
– compare imagined movements with only a spatial component vs. movements with a sequential component

spatial only complex sequence

Bilateral FEFBilateral FEF
1 21 41 61 81 101
1.0
0
-1.0
% s
igna
l cha
nge
im a g e s

Supplementary Motor Area (SMA)Supplementary Motor Area (SMA)
1 21 41 61 81 101
1.0
0
-1.0
% s
igna
l cha
nge
im a g e s

Bilateral superior parietalBilateral superior parietal
1 21 41 61 81 101
1.0
0
-1.0
% s
igna
l cha
nge
im a g e s

Inferior parietal cortexInferior parietal cortex
Left Right
Anterior
Posterior
L
R
R
alternation only
complex sequence only
both

Design Problems
Boy, you must be rich then!!!
• Why is right parietal more active for less difficult tasks then?
• So did you just test task difficulty?
• Were the tasks really different in the intended way?
• What could the right vs. left parietal difference be due to?
– Attention? – possibly!– Differences in eye movements? – maybe!
– Perhaps – both tasks were spatial in nature and both tasks had a sequential component, so…
– Maybe, what of it?
– I don’t know and I don’t care, piss off! I’m gonna start again!

Confirming modularity
• Nancy Kanwisher and the parahippocampal recliner region!

Is that all there is to it?• Alex Martin and co. suggest that the FFA responds
to other kinds of objects too• Isabelle Gauthier and co. suggest that it is
expertise with faces which drives the activation

Exploring behaviours
• Prism adaptation ameliorates neglect – how?
• First, explore the direct effects of prism adaptation in the healthy brain.
• Clower et al 1996 used PET to do this but reversed the direction of prismatic shift every 5 trials.

Prism Adaptation – Rossetti and colleagues
• prisms shift world further to the right (into the patient’s ‘good’ field)
• patient’s movements compensate for the prismatic shift – in the opposite direction
• after effects lead to better processing of previously neglected stimuli

Setup.Setup.
head coil
arm brace
gaze
scanner bed
fixationtargets
rightward shifting prism
flexible arm(mounted to scanner
bed on left side)
Subject moved prism in front of right eye (left eye was patched) prior to prisms run and moved the prism to the side at the end of the run.

Protocol I.Protocol I.
2 sec
2 sec
11.5 sec
0.5 sec
2 sec
2 sec
5 runs with prisms (50 trials)
5 runs without prisms (50)

Protocol II.Protocol II.
fixation target
post-target fixation
2 sec volumes – so 2 sec for critical stimulus (the target) and 12 secfor post stimulus return to baseline (a la Bandettini).

Protocol III.Protocol III.
4T scanner at Robarts
17 pseudo-axial slices
5 mm thick
TR 2 sec
2-shot EPI sequence
Co-registered to 128 sliceanatomical

Adapting in the magnet.Adapting in the magnet.
PREPOST
-50 -40 -30 -20 -10 0 10 20 30 40 50
group meangroup mean
mean shift = 27.73 mm

Finding ROIs.Finding ROIs.
right sup parleft sup par
med frontal (SMA)

Modeling the peak activation across Modeling the peak activation across trials.trials.
Linear
Logarithmic

Left and right superior parietal Left and right superior parietal cortices.cortices.
-0.5
0
0.5
1
1.5
2
2.5
1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10
trial
max
% s
igna
l cha
nge
r2=0.13
-0.5
0
0.5
1
1.5
2
2.5
1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10
trial
max
% s
igna
l cha
nge
r2=0.15
Left Superior Parietal
Right Superior Parietal

Cerebellar ROIs.Cerebellar ROIs.
-0.5
0
0.5
1
1.5
2
2.5
1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10
trial
max
% s
igna
l cha
nge
r2=0.14
Medial Cerebellum
Right Lateral Cerebellum
-0.5
0
0.5
1
1.5
2
2.5
1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10
trial
max
% s
igna
l cha
nge
r2=0.42
trial

SMA.SMA.
transverse sagittal
0.40.50.60.70.80.911.11.2
trial
max
% s
igna
l cha
nge
(gro
up m
ean)
1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10
r2 = 0.37

Bottom line?Bottom line?
• Difficult to image the direct effects of adaptation in normals.
• Image “good adaptors” OR change protocol to look at after effects of adaptation – with all its problems…

Conclusions?• fMRI should be used for good and not evil!
I wonder if fMRI could be used to
cure cancer?

Acknowledgements
• fMRI of epilepsy patient– Stacey Danckert– Seyed Mirsitari – David Carey– Mel Goodale– Ravi Menon– Jody Culham
• fMRI of prism adaptation– Susanne Ferber – Stacey Danckert– Mel Goodale– Yves Rossetti
• fMRI of imagined movements– it was all my fault!

End of Lecture