Fundamentals of Neuroscience Neuroimaging in Cognitive Neuroscience

75
Fundamentals of Neuroscience Neuroimaging in Cognitive Neuroscience James Danckert PAS 4040 [email protected]

description

Fundamentals of Neuroscience Neuroimaging in Cognitive Neuroscience. James Danckert PAS 4040 [email protected]. Functional Neuroimaging. Electrical activity Event-related potentials (ERP), visual evoked potentials (VEP) all derivative from EEG Stimulation - PowerPoint PPT Presentation

Transcript of Fundamentals of Neuroscience Neuroimaging in Cognitive Neuroscience

Page 1: Fundamentals of Neuroscience Neuroimaging in Cognitive Neuroscience

Fundamentals of Neuroscience

Neuroimaging in Cognitive Neuroscience

James DanckertPAS 4040 [email protected]

Page 2: Fundamentals of Neuroscience Neuroimaging in Cognitive Neuroscience

Functional Neuroimaging

• Electrical activity– Event-related potentials (ERP), visual evoked potentials

(VEP) all derivative from EEG

• Stimulation– Trans-cranial magnetic stimulation – single vs. rapid

pulse TMS

• Metabolism– Positron Emission Tomography (PET) and Blood

Oxygenated Level Dependent (BOLD) functional MRI (fMRI)

Page 3: Fundamentals of Neuroscience Neuroimaging in Cognitive Neuroscience

EEG• Large populations of

neurons firing produce electrical potentials that can be measured at the scalp

• Signals are passively conducted through the skull and scalp and can be amplified and measured

• Difference between reference (ground) and recording electrodes are measured to give the electrical potential – electroencephalogram (EEG)

Page 4: Fundamentals of Neuroscience Neuroimaging in Cognitive Neuroscience

ERPs and VEPs• EEG tends to record global brain activity• ERPs (and VEPs) are a special case of EEG• average EEG trace from a large number of trials • align signal to onset of a stimulus or response – hence event-

related potential (ERP)

Page 5: Fundamentals of Neuroscience Neuroimaging in Cognitive Neuroscience

Pros and cons of ERPs.

• Good temporal resolution• Linked to specific physiological markers (e.g.,

N1, P3 etc. which in turn can be linked to known cognitive processes)

• Poor spatial resolution• Difficult to get at some brain regions (OFC,

temporal cortex)

Cons

Pros

Page 6: Fundamentals of Neuroscience Neuroimaging in Cognitive Neuroscience

Transcranial Magnetic Stimulation (TMS)

• Thompson (1910) placed head between two coils and stimulated at ~ 42 Hz• saw flashing lights – magnetophosphenes• was probably stimulating the retina and not the visual cortex

Cowey and Walsh, 2001

Page 7: Fundamentals of Neuroscience Neuroimaging in Cognitive Neuroscience

TMS

• TMS applies a magnetic pulse to a certain brain region to temporarily modulate the function of that region

Page 8: Fundamentals of Neuroscience Neuroimaging in Cognitive Neuroscience

TMS

• the induced current in the tissue is in the opposite direction to that of the coil

• the intensity of the signal drops off towards the centre and outside of the coil

circular coil induced current

Cowey and Walsh, 2001

Page 9: Fundamentals of Neuroscience Neuroimaging in Cognitive Neuroscience

TMS

• the flow of the current must cross the axon to cause stimulation or interruption of function (N3 will not be stimulated)

maximum depolarization

maximum hyperpolarization

Cowey and Walsh, 2001

little or no change

Page 10: Fundamentals of Neuroscience Neuroimaging in Cognitive Neuroscience

TMS

Page 11: Fundamentals of Neuroscience Neuroimaging in Cognitive Neuroscience

Spatial extent of TMS• spatial extent of induced electric field

– drops ~ 75% within 10 mm– affects 600 mm2 of neural tissue

Page 12: Fundamentals of Neuroscience Neuroimaging in Cognitive Neuroscience

Rapid vs. Single Pulse TMS

• for single pulse TMS duration of stimulation = 1 msec, but affects motor cortex for up to 100 msec

• for rapid or repetitive pulse TMS stimuli are delivered in trains with frequencies from 1 to 25 Hz (1 – 25 times per second)

• duration of after-effects for rapid pulse TMS anywhere from msec to several seconds

Page 13: Fundamentals of Neuroscience Neuroimaging in Cognitive Neuroscience

• excitatory or inhibitory reversible effects depending on site and parameters of stimulation (e.g. frequency of pulses) -> facilitates or slows down cognitive process/behavior

• when inhibitory, referred to as ‘virtual lesion technique’

• can give precise timing information (msec level) due to transient nature of effects

• rTMS is beginning to be used as a treatment for depression (focus is on DLPFC)

Transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS / rTMS)

Page 14: Fundamentals of Neuroscience Neuroimaging in Cognitive Neuroscience

TMS

• Poor spatial localisation – how focal is the stimulation?

• Can’t stimulate certain areas (e.g., temporal lobe) and can only stimulate cortical surface

• Good temporal resolution• Can presumably disrupt individual processes

within a task.• Distance effects – changed interactions due

to stimulation• Can induce seizures (particularly rTMS)

Page 15: Fundamentals of Neuroscience Neuroimaging in Cognitive Neuroscience

Frameless stereotaxy and fMRI

• areas can be identified functionally and then used to position the coil in a TMS study using the frameless stereotaxy method

• Paus is attempting to directly combine fMRI and TMS – with TMS pulses delivered in between fMRI runs

Page 16: Fundamentals of Neuroscience Neuroimaging in Cognitive Neuroscience

Metabolic Imaging

• Two main techniques – positron emission tomography (PET) and functional MRI (fMRI)

• Activity in cells requires energy (oxygen and glucose)

• Increased neural activity will lead to changes in cerebral blood volume (CBV), cerebral blood flow (CBF) and the rate of metabolism of glucose and oxygen (CRMGl and CRMO)

• These changes in blood flow and metabolism can be measured using PET and fMRI

Page 17: Fundamentals of Neuroscience Neuroimaging in Cognitive Neuroscience

Positron Emission Tomography (PET)

• Measures local changes in cerebral blood flow (CBF) or volume and can also be used to trace certain neurotransmitters (but can only do one of these at a time)

• Radioactive isotopes are used as tracers

• The isotopes rapidly decay emitting positrons

• When the positrons collide with electrons two photons (or gamma rays) are emitted

• The two photons travel in opposite directions allowing the location of the collision to be determined

Page 18: Fundamentals of Neuroscience Neuroimaging in Cognitive Neuroscience

Positron Emission Tomography (PET)

Page 19: Fundamentals of Neuroscience Neuroimaging in Cognitive Neuroscience

PET and subtraction• Run two conditions

– stimulation (e.g., look at visual images) vs. control (e.g., look at blank screen)

• Measure the difference in activation between the two images (i.e., subtract control from stimulation)

• This provides a picture of regional cerebral blood flow relative to visual stimulation.

Page 20: Fundamentals of Neuroscience Neuroimaging in Cognitive Neuroscience

Motion vs. colour.• Subject views

coloured screen (left) vs. moving random black and white dots (right)

• Both task activate early visual areas (V1 and V2)

• Subtracting the two images reveals different brain areas for colour (V4) vs. motion (V5) processing

Page 21: Fundamentals of Neuroscience Neuroimaging in Cognitive Neuroscience

PET vs. fMRI• PET allows you to track multiple metabolic

processes so long as the emitted photon can be detected – allows imaging of some neurotransmitters

• PET is invasive – radioactive isotopes can only be administered (at experimental levels) every 4 – 5 years

• fMRI has much greater spatial resolution (~ mms)

• fMRI has greater temporal resolution – can detect activation to stimuli appearing for less than a second (PET is limited by the half life of the isotope used)

Page 22: Fundamentals of Neuroscience Neuroimaging in Cognitive Neuroscience

fMRI

Page 23: Fundamentals of Neuroscience Neuroimaging in Cognitive Neuroscience

Magnet safety

• very strong magnetic fields – even large and heavy objects can ‘fly’ into the magnet bore

Page 24: Fundamentals of Neuroscience Neuroimaging in Cognitive Neuroscience

Cerebral blood supply.• Arterioles

– Y=95% at rest.– Y=100% during activation.– 25 m diameter.– <15% blood volume of cortical

tissue.• Venules

– Y=60% at rest.– Y=90% during activation.– 25-50 m diameter.– 40% blood volume of cortical

tissue.• Red blood cell

– 6 m wide and 1-2 m thick.– Delivers O2 in form of

oxyhemoglobin.

• Capillaries– Y=80% at rest.– Y=90% during activation.– 8 m diameter.– 40% blood volume of cortical

tissue.

– Primary site of O2 exchange with tissue.

Artery Vein

Arterioles Veneoles

Capillaries

1 - 2 cm

Neurons

Transit Time = 2-3 s

Page 25: Fundamentals of Neuroscience Neuroimaging in Cognitive Neuroscience

Cerebral blood supply.

Page 26: Fundamentals of Neuroscience Neuroimaging in Cognitive Neuroscience

fMRI

• Deoxyhaemoglobin is paramagnetic• When neural activity increases more oxygenated

blood than is needed is delivered to the site• This leads to an imbalance in oxyhaemoglobin

and deoxyhaemoglobin – more oxy than deoxy• fMRI is able to measure this difference due to the

different magnetic properties of oxy and deoxyhaemoglobin

Page 27: Fundamentals of Neuroscience Neuroimaging in Cognitive Neuroscience

fMRI and BOLD

• blood oxygenated level dependent (BOLD) signal is actually a complex combination of:– rate of glucose

and oxygen metabolism

– CBV– CBF

• same subtraction logic used in PET is used in fMRI

Page 28: Fundamentals of Neuroscience Neuroimaging in Cognitive Neuroscience

fMRI – block design• fMRI (like PET) began examining brain activity using block designs

colours

rest

motion

restrestrest

colours

Page 29: Fundamentals of Neuroscience Neuroimaging in Cognitive Neuroscience

fMRI – event-related design

• allows randomization of stimuli (not possible in PET)

Page 30: Fundamentals of Neuroscience Neuroimaging in Cognitive Neuroscience

fMRI – event-related design• BOLD response has a predictable form

• In rapid event-related designs the signal to a given trial type is deconvolved using models of the BOLD response

Page 31: Fundamentals of Neuroscience Neuroimaging in Cognitive Neuroscience

Linearity of BOLD responseDale & Buckner, 1997

Linearity:“Do things really add up?”

red = 2 - 1

green = 3 - 2

Sync each trial response to start of trial

Not quite linear but good enough !

Page 32: Fundamentals of Neuroscience Neuroimaging in Cognitive Neuroscience

Fixed vs. Random Intervals If trials are jittered, ITI power

Source: Burock et al., 1998

Page 33: Fundamentals of Neuroscience Neuroimaging in Cognitive Neuroscience

fMRI spatial resolution• images can be co-registered to the subject’s own brain

(not an average brain as in PET)

PET fMRI

Page 34: Fundamentals of Neuroscience Neuroimaging in Cognitive Neuroscience

fMRI and topologies• Using fMRI to “map” different brain functions

Penfield’s maps

Servos et al., 1998red = wrist; orange = shoulder

Page 35: Fundamentals of Neuroscience Neuroimaging in Cognitive Neuroscience

EXPANDING RINGS

Retintopy

• 8 Hz flicker (checks reverse contrast 8X/sec)• good stimulus for driving visual areas• subjects must maintain fixation (on red dot)

Source: Jody Culham

ROTATING WEDGES

Page 36: Fundamentals of Neuroscience Neuroimaging in Cognitive Neuroscience

Source: Jody Culham

time = 0

time = 20 sec

time = 60 sec

time = 40 sec

0 20 40 60

TIME STIMULUS

EXPECTED RESPONSE PROFILE OF AREA RESPONDING TO STIMULUS To analyze

retinotopic data:

Analyze the data with a set of functions with the same profile but different phase offsets.

For any voxels that show a significant response to any of the functions, color code the activation by the phase offset that yielded maximum activation (e.g., maximum response to foveal stimulus = red, maximum response to peripheral stimulus = pink)

Page 37: Fundamentals of Neuroscience Neuroimaging in Cognitive Neuroscience

Retintopy: Eccentricity

calcarinesulcus

left occipitallobe

right occipitallobe

• foveal area represented at occipital pole• peripheral regions represented more anteriorly

Page 38: Fundamentals of Neuroscience Neuroimaging in Cognitive Neuroscience

Retinotopy

Source: Sereno et al., 1995

Page 39: Fundamentals of Neuroscience Neuroimaging in Cognitive Neuroscience

Other Sensory “-topies”

Audition: Tonotopy

cochlea

Sylvian fissure

temporal lobe

Page 40: Fundamentals of Neuroscience Neuroimaging in Cognitive Neuroscience

Saccadotopy

Source: Sereno et al., 2001

•delayed saccades

•move saccadic target systematically around the clock

http://kamares.ucsd.edu/~sereno/LIP/both-closeup+stim.mpgMarty Sereno’s web page

Page 41: Fundamentals of Neuroscience Neuroimaging in Cognitive Neuroscience

Break

Page 42: Fundamentals of Neuroscience Neuroimaging in Cognitive Neuroscience

Finding the human homologue of monkey area X!

• recent research has used monkey neurophys to guide fMRI in humans

Dukelow et al. 2001

Page 43: Fundamentals of Neuroscience Neuroimaging in Cognitive Neuroscience

Problems with the search for homologues

• Absence of activation doesn’t mean the absence of function• Presence of activation doesn’t imply sole locus of function• But our brains are different!• Confirmatory hypotheses

Dukelow et al. 2001

Page 44: Fundamentals of Neuroscience Neuroimaging in Cognitive Neuroscience

fMRI and diagnosis

• fMRI is starting to be used in patients with epilepsy

• one goal is to use this as a tool to localise language, memory etc. prior to surgery

• another goal would be to use fMRI to study the propogation of seizures

• in stroke patients fMRI can be used to chart recovery of function

Page 45: Fundamentals of Neuroscience Neuroimaging in Cognitive Neuroscience

Patient SP – congenital porencephalic cystPatient SP – congenital porencephalic cyst

Left Right

Page 46: Fundamentals of Neuroscience Neuroimaging in Cognitive Neuroscience

im a g e s

1 2 1 4 1

% s

igna

l cha

nge

0

1

2

3

-1

SP - motor stripSP - motor strip

sequential tapping

alternating tapping

motor strip

Page 47: Fundamentals of Neuroscience Neuroimaging in Cognitive Neuroscience

im a g e s

1 2 1 4 1

% s

igna

l cha

nge

0

1

2

3

-1

SP – somatosensory stripSP – somatosensory strip

sequential tapping

alternating tapping

somatosensorystrip

Page 48: Fundamentals of Neuroscience Neuroimaging in Cognitive Neuroscience

6.0

4.0

2.0

0.0

-2.0% s

ignal

chan

ge

1 21 41images

6

4

2

0

-1

1

3

5

% s

ignal

chang

eimages

sequential tapping

alternating tapping

superiorparietal

Page 49: Fundamentals of Neuroscience Neuroimaging in Cognitive Neuroscience

% s

ign

al c

ha

ng

e

images1 21 41

2

1

0

-1

name animals

name objects

non-word sounds (‘ba’)

Broca’s area?

Page 50: Fundamentals of Neuroscience Neuroimaging in Cognitive Neuroscience

% s

ign

al c

ha

ng

e

images1 21 41

2

1

0

-1

name animals

name objects

non-word sounds (‘ba’)

left occipital

Page 51: Fundamentals of Neuroscience Neuroimaging in Cognitive Neuroscience

fMRI and cognition

• What not to do – poorly designed tasks!

• What is the right inferior parietal lobe’s contribution to movement control?

– spatial component of movements

– compare imagined movements with only a spatial component vs. movements with a sequential component

Page 52: Fundamentals of Neuroscience Neuroimaging in Cognitive Neuroscience

spatial only complex sequence

Page 53: Fundamentals of Neuroscience Neuroimaging in Cognitive Neuroscience

Bilateral FEFBilateral FEF

1 21 41 61 81 101

1.0

0

-1.0

% s

igna

l cha

nge

im a g e s

Page 54: Fundamentals of Neuroscience Neuroimaging in Cognitive Neuroscience

Supplementary Motor Area (SMA)Supplementary Motor Area (SMA)

1 21 41 61 81 101

1.0

0

-1.0

% s

igna

l cha

nge

im a g e s

Page 55: Fundamentals of Neuroscience Neuroimaging in Cognitive Neuroscience

Bilateral superior parietalBilateral superior parietal

1 21 41 61 81 101

1.0

0

-1.0

% s

igna

l cha

nge

im a g e s

Page 56: Fundamentals of Neuroscience Neuroimaging in Cognitive Neuroscience

Inferior parietal cortexInferior parietal cortex

Left Right

Anterior

Posterior

L

R

R

alternation only

complex sequence only

both

Page 57: Fundamentals of Neuroscience Neuroimaging in Cognitive Neuroscience

Design Problems

Boy, you must be rich then!!!

• Why is right parietal more active for less difficult tasks then?

• So did you just test task difficulty?

• Were the tasks really different in the intended way?

• What could the right vs. left parietal difference be due to?

– Attention? – possibly!– Differences in eye movements? – maybe!

– Perhaps – both tasks were spatial in nature and both tasks had a sequential component, so…

– Maybe, what of it?

– I don’t know and I don’t care, piss off! I’m gonna start again!

Page 58: Fundamentals of Neuroscience Neuroimaging in Cognitive Neuroscience

Confirming modularity

• Nancy Kanwisher and the parahippocampal recliner region!

Page 59: Fundamentals of Neuroscience Neuroimaging in Cognitive Neuroscience

Is that all there is to it?• Alex Martin and co. suggest that the FFA responds

to other kinds of objects too• Isabelle Gauthier and co. suggest that it is

expertise with faces which drives the activation

Page 60: Fundamentals of Neuroscience Neuroimaging in Cognitive Neuroscience

Exploring behaviours

• Prism adaptation ameliorates neglect – how?

• First, explore the direct effects of prism adaptation in the healthy brain.

• Clower et al 1996 used PET to do this but reversed the direction of prismatic shift every 5 trials.

Page 61: Fundamentals of Neuroscience Neuroimaging in Cognitive Neuroscience

Prism Adaptation – Rossetti and colleagues

• prisms shift world further to the right (into the patient’s ‘good’ field)

• patient’s movements compensate for the prismatic shift – in the opposite direction

• after effects lead to better processing of previously neglected stimuli

Page 62: Fundamentals of Neuroscience Neuroimaging in Cognitive Neuroscience

Setup.Setup.

head coil

arm brace

gaze

scanner bed

fixationtargets

rightward shifting prism

flexible arm(mounted to scanner

bed on left side)

Subject moved prism in front of right eye (left eye was patched) prior to prisms run and moved the prism to the side at the end of the run.

Page 63: Fundamentals of Neuroscience Neuroimaging in Cognitive Neuroscience

Protocol I.Protocol I.

2 sec

2 sec

11.5 sec

0.5 sec

2 sec

2 sec

5 runs with prisms (50 trials)

5 runs without prisms (50)

Page 64: Fundamentals of Neuroscience Neuroimaging in Cognitive Neuroscience

Protocol II.Protocol II.

fixation target

post-target fixation

2 sec volumes – so 2 sec for critical stimulus (the target) and 12 secfor post stimulus return to baseline (a la Bandettini).

Page 65: Fundamentals of Neuroscience Neuroimaging in Cognitive Neuroscience

Protocol III.Protocol III.

4T scanner at Robarts

17 pseudo-axial slices

5 mm thick

TR 2 sec

2-shot EPI sequence

Co-registered to 128 sliceanatomical

Page 66: Fundamentals of Neuroscience Neuroimaging in Cognitive Neuroscience

Adapting in the magnet.Adapting in the magnet.

PREPOST

-50 -40 -30 -20 -10 0 10 20 30 40 50

group meangroup mean

mean shift = 27.73 mm

Page 67: Fundamentals of Neuroscience Neuroimaging in Cognitive Neuroscience

Finding ROIs.Finding ROIs.

right sup parleft sup par

med frontal (SMA)

Page 68: Fundamentals of Neuroscience Neuroimaging in Cognitive Neuroscience

Modeling the peak activation across Modeling the peak activation across trials.trials.

Linear

Logarithmic

Page 69: Fundamentals of Neuroscience Neuroimaging in Cognitive Neuroscience

Left and right superior parietal Left and right superior parietal cortices.cortices.

-0.5

0

0.5

1

1.5

2

2.5

1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10

trial

max

% s

igna

l cha

nge

r2=0.13

-0.5

0

0.5

1

1.5

2

2.5

1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10

trial

max

% s

igna

l cha

nge

r2=0.15

Left Superior Parietal

Right Superior Parietal

Page 70: Fundamentals of Neuroscience Neuroimaging in Cognitive Neuroscience

Cerebellar ROIs.Cerebellar ROIs.

-0.5

0

0.5

1

1.5

2

2.5

1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10

trial

max

% s

igna

l cha

nge

r2=0.14

Medial Cerebellum

Right Lateral Cerebellum

-0.5

0

0.5

1

1.5

2

2.5

1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10

trial

max

% s

igna

l cha

nge

r2=0.42

trial

Page 71: Fundamentals of Neuroscience Neuroimaging in Cognitive Neuroscience

SMA.SMA.

transverse sagittal

0.40.50.60.70.80.911.11.2

trial

max

% s

igna

l cha

nge

(gro

up m

ean)

1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10

r2 = 0.37

Page 72: Fundamentals of Neuroscience Neuroimaging in Cognitive Neuroscience

Bottom line?Bottom line?

• Difficult to image the direct effects of adaptation in normals.

• Image “good adaptors” OR change protocol to look at after effects of adaptation – with all its problems…

Page 73: Fundamentals of Neuroscience Neuroimaging in Cognitive Neuroscience

Conclusions?• fMRI should be used for good and not evil!

I wonder if fMRI could be used to

cure cancer?

Page 74: Fundamentals of Neuroscience Neuroimaging in Cognitive Neuroscience

Acknowledgements

• fMRI of epilepsy patient– Stacey Danckert– Seyed Mirsitari – David Carey– Mel Goodale– Ravi Menon– Jody Culham

• fMRI of prism adaptation– Susanne Ferber – Stacey Danckert– Mel Goodale– Yves Rossetti

• fMRI of imagined movements– it was all my fault!

Page 75: Fundamentals of Neuroscience Neuroimaging in Cognitive Neuroscience

End of Lecture