Mastering Metacompetence - Wiley

14
How to Become a More Effective CBT Therapist: Mastering Metacompetence in Clinical Practice, First Edition. Edited by Adrian Whittington and Nick Grey. © 2014 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Published 2014 by John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. 1 Mastering Metacompetence The Science and Art of Cognitive Behavioural Therapy Adrian Whittington and Nick Grey In a professional kitchen, recipes are essential to creating consistent food, so that everyone takes the same path to the same place. But cooks who rely only on strictly codi- fied formulas miss out on what is really important. Are the carrots more or less sweet, more or less tender? Is the ginger very strong, so that less should be used, or too weak for the amount specified? Or the thorniest problem: How long does it take something to cook, in a specific oven, on a specific day, with a certain set of ingredients? Daniel Patterson, Head Chef, Coi, San Francisco 1 Introduction Cognitive Behavioural Therapy (CBT) has grown up in a scientific tradition, which has been highly productive in the development of effective therapy. Research trials have given us a firm foundation for expanding the delivery of CBT, with the approaches delivered in the trials being reproduced in routine care to help a lot of people a lot of the time. However, these trials can seem a long way from the consulting room when as a therapist you sit down with a unique client who has a unique set of difficulties and strengths. As a therapist you face a seemingly infinite range of options in your moment to moment decision making about what to do next as you try to deliver CBT in the most helpful way with this client at this point in time. A lot of the time you probably cannot be sure of the best options and have to proceed in the hope and faith that by working collaboratively with your client you will be able to negotiate a helpful way forward. This can feel more like an art than a science. Abilities required to apply therapy artfully, in a flexible and individually tailored way, have been named “metacompetences” in Roth and Pilling’s competence framework for CBT (Roth & Pilling, 2007). COPYRIGHTED MATERIAL

Transcript of Mastering Metacompetence - Wiley

Page 1: Mastering Metacompetence - Wiley

How to Become a More Effective CBT Therapist: Mastering Metacompetence in Clinical Practice, First Edition. Edited by Adrian Whittington and Nick Grey. © 2014 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Published 2014 by John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.

1

Mastering MetacompetenceThe Science and Art of

Cognitive Behavioural Therapy

Adrian Whittington and Nick Grey

In a professional kitchen, recipes are essential to creating consistent food, so that everyone takes the same path to the same place. But cooks who rely only on strictly codi-fied formulas miss out on what is really important. Are the carrots more or less sweet, more or less tender? Is the ginger very strong, so that less should be used, or too weak for the amount specified? Or the thorniest problem: How long does it take something to cook, in a specific oven, on a specific day, with a certain set of ingredients?

Daniel Patterson, Head Chef, Coi, San Francisco1

Introduction

Cognitive Behavioural Therapy (CBT) has grown up in a scientific tradition, which has been highly productive in the development of effective therapy. Research trials have given us a firm foundation for expanding the delivery of CBT, with the approaches delivered in the trials being reproduced in routine care to help a lot of people a lot of the time.

However, these trials can seem a long way from the consulting room when as a therapist you sit down with a unique client who has a unique set of difficulties and strengths. As a therapist you face a seemingly infinite range of options in your moment to moment decision making about what to do next as you try to deliver CBT in the most helpful way with this client at this point in time. A lot of the time you probably cannot be sure of the best options and have to proceed in the hope and faith that by working collaboratively with your client you will be able to negotiate a helpful way forward. This can feel more like an art than a science. Abilities required to apply therapy artfully, in a flexible and individually tailored way, have been named “metacompetences” in Roth and Pilling’s competence framework for CBT (Roth & Pilling, 2007).

0002092982.INDD 3 3/17/2014 12:04:21 PM

COPYRIG

HTED M

ATERIAL

Page 2: Mastering Metacompetence - Wiley

4 Adrian Whittington and Nick Grey

There are significant pitfalls on the path between the science of CBT and its artful delivery. These include the risk of rejecting the research base because of a sometimes imperfect fit with routine practice, the risk of drifting away from effective methods in the belief that you are being helpfully flexible, and the risk of being overly rigid in your approach in an attempt to adhere to protocol.

We believe that the science and art of CBT can and should be brought closer together to help avoid these risks, and that the concept of metacompetent adherence gives us a framework for bridging this gap. Metacompetent adherence means making your ther-apy decisions based on evidence that clearly supports the practice and on a sound theo-retical rationale where the evidence is less clear. Mastering metacompetence is a process of making explicit and enacting the if–then procedural rules of therapy adaptation and where possible drawing on the evidence base. These rules will not take away all uncer-tainty, however. The experience of not knowing is inevitable and perhaps desirable for therapists – human experiences require you to respond with humility, compassion and openness to learning as you deliver the best evidence-based intervention that you can.

The Science of CBT: Efficacy, Effectiveness and Evidence-Based Practice

Thousands of research trials have been conducted to address the question of whether and for whom CBT is useful, and how it can be most effective. The research base of CBT includes efficacy studies that test treatment in carefully controlled experimental conditions and effectiveness studies that test the interventions in routine care settings, as well as a plethora of other approaches including dissemination trials, single case research, dismantling studies and experimental designs. Of these approaches rand-omized control trial (RCT) evidence has traditionally been viewed as the “gold stand-ard” methodology for establishing whether an intervention works (Kaptchuck, 2001).

This scientific effort has been more intensive than for any other form of psycho-therapy. Analysis of “what works for whom” clearly indicates CBT’s wide utility (Roth & Fonagy, 2005). As a consequence of this evidence, CBT has been recommended in evidence-based treatment guidance for a wide range of psychological difficulties (e.g., National Institute for Health and Clinical Excellence, 2009, 2011) and has become more widely available. In the United Kingdom, a national programme to increase access to psychological therapies for depression and anxiety disorders has seen an unprecedented expansion in the provision of CBT (Clark, 2011).

Both efficacy and effectiveness research shows that CBT works for many people with many types of difficulties and that research-based interventions can be applied in routine practice without dramatic reduction in effect. However, trial-based evidence will never resolve all of your dilemmas as a therapist about exactly what works for whom in which situ-ations. This has led to a movement towards evidence-based practice (EBP) as an approach to guide clinical decision making, drawing on a combination of research evidence, clinical expertise and client preferences (Lillienfield, Ritschel, Lynn, Cautin, & Latzman, 2013).

Efficacy of CBT

Efficacy studies are those in which the treatment is carefully studied under “ideal” experimental conditions in a randomized controlled trial (RCT). Most reviews and meta-analyses have examined how CBT treatments have performed in efficacy studies.

0002092982.INDD 4 3/17/2014 12:04:21 PM

Page 3: Mastering Metacompetence - Wiley

Mastering Metacompetence: The Science and Art of CBT 5

These have themselves been examined in a larger review of meta-analyses of CBT RCTs across a wide range of disorders (Butler, Chapman, Forman, & Beck, 2006). Overall large effect sizes for CBT were seen for unipolar depression, generalized anxiety disor-der, panic disorder, social anxiety disorder and post-traumatic stress disorder; moderate effect sizes for working with pain and anger; and CBT was as effective as behaviour therapy for obsessive compulsive disorder.

The degree to which results from RCTs translate into routine practice is a conten-tious issue (e.g., Westen, Novotny and Thompson-Brenner, 2004). RCTs typically have a single therapeutic focus (i.e., a particular psychiatric diagnosis/disorder), have an associated treatment manual, and are usually of a relatively brief fixed duration. This all makes sense scientifically, maximizing internal validity of the study, but has led to critiques of the evidence, suggesting that RCT conditions are too divorced from the realities of routine practice.

Effectiveness of CBT

Effectiveness studies measure the outcome of interventions provided in “routine” care settings. Effectiveness research indicates that it is possible to reproduce CBT RCT interventions in routine care settings with fewer controls and without greatly reducing their effects, although this is not guaranteed. A meta-analytic review of effectiveness trials of CBT for anxiety disorders showed that mean effect sizes were comparable to those in benchmarked RCTs (Stewart & Chambless, 2009). A similar meta-analytic review of effectiveness trials of CBT for depression showed a dilution of mean effect size in routine care, although the effect remained large (Hans & Hiller, 2013). Some effectiveness trials have even shown larger effects than in comparable RCTs (Ost, 2013). At one-year follow-up CBT for anxiety disorders in routine care has produced results almost equal to those of RCTs, whereas CBT for depression has not (DiMauro, Domigues, Fernandez, & Tolin, 2013; Gibbons et al., 2010).

The differences in effect of CBT in RCTs and routine care are not uniform and are likely to differ across the variety of treatments badged as CBT. Reasons for dilution of effect are not clear where these have been observed. Possible differences between RCTs and routine care include client characteristics, therapist and therapy character-istics. All may be relevant, but there is evidence that poorer quality control of therapy in routine settings may be at least as important in reducing effects as differences in the clients seen (Stewart & Chambless, 2009; Stirman, DeRubeis, Crits-Cristoph, & Rothman, 2005).

Despite the demonstrable value of CBT in routine settings as well as in RCTs, the evidence is currently insufficient to provide a comprehensive guide to the flexible, indi-vidually adapted delivery of CBT. The areas where research cannot be the only guide include numerous areas of complexity such as how best to intervene with co-morbid conditions (Shafran et al., 2009) and how best to deliver “flexibility within fidelity” (Kendall, Gosch, Furr, & Sood, 2008).

Evidence-based practice

There will never be enough research to tell you definitively what will work best for any particular individual client, and there will always be those who seek therapy from you who are “beyond the guidelines” developed during RCT trials. This situation leaves you unable to rely solely on RCT evidence to guide your practice as a therapist.

0002092982.INDD 5 3/17/2014 12:04:21 PM

Page 4: Mastering Metacompetence - Wiley

6 Adrian Whittington and Nick Grey

Evidence-based practice (EBP) offers the beginnings of a solution. EBP has been proposed in the United States as an approach to clinical decision-making, drawing on the “three-leg stool” of research evidence, clinical expertise, and client preferences (Spring, 2007). EBP has been distinguished from empirically supported treatments (ESTs) based on RCT evidence, which do not offer explicit specific guidance on adap-tation and flexibility (Lilienfield et al., 2013). In the United Kingdom the concept of empirically grounded clinical interventions (EGCIs) also highlights the need for a broader approach to evidence-based practice than can be derived from RCT evidence alone; EGCIs are said to be derived from the sequence of clinical observation, experi-mental study and theory development, followed by treatment efficacy and effectiveness trials (Salkovskis, 2002). This approach values the role of experimentally derived theory as part of an evidence-based approach to intervention in the absence of specific evi-dence for what to do next.

As a therapist, EBP and EGCIs offer you a more comprehensive framework for mak-ing clinical decisions than trial evidence alone. However, both stop short of defining in detail the nature of the clinical expertise that you will need to draw upon and how you should put this into action.

The Art of CBT: Metacompetence

To be an evidence-based practitioner does not mean that you will always find your-self following a defined course of action or sequence of steps. In fact this is likely to feel like the exception rather than the rule in your therapy sessions. Much of the time you will base your actions on a combination of fundamental CBT therapy com-petences, your knowledge of specific CBT techniques and models, and an informed negotiation with your client about a way forward. The competences to enact this combination of factors into a coherent and effective therapy for anxiety disorders or depression have been defined very helpfully, drawing on an expert reference group and the manuals used in RCT trials that showed CBT to have a positive effect (Roth & Pilling, 2007).

Roth and Pilling (2007) identified five specific aspects of competence (see Figure 1.1). The first four outline competences of increasing levels of specificity to CBT and to CBT for particular problems, as follows:

Generic therapeutic competences: Required for the delivery of any psychological therapy, which include knowledge about mental health, ability to engage and assess clients, manage a therapeutic relationship and make use of supervision.

Basic CBT competences: The foundations of all CBT interventions, including knowl-edge of core CBT principles and abilities to agree goals collaboratively, jointly man-age session structure and introduce a basic formulation using a cognitive-behavioural maintenance cycle.

Specific CBT techniques: A set of core cognitive and behavioural technical interventions, delivered within the context of Socratic dialogue and including, for example, the use of thought records, behavioural experiments, exposure and activity scheduling.

Problem-specific competences: The competences to deliver specific CBT intervention packages for particular disorders, for example the Clark intervention for panic disor-der (Clark, 1986) or the Jacobson behavioural activation intervention for depression (Jacobson, Dobson, Truax, Addis, et al., 1996).

0002092982.INDD 6 3/17/2014 12:04:21 PM

Page 5: Mastering Metacompetence - Wiley

App

lied

rela

xatio

n &

app

lied

tens

ion

Pro

blem

sol

ving

Cog

nitiv

e T

hera

py –

Bec

k

Beh

avio

ural

Act

ivat

ion

–C

acob

son

Exp

osur

e te

chni

ques

Spe

cific

pho

bias

Sp

ecif

ic b

ehav

iou

ral a

nd

cog

nit

ive

ther

apy

Pro

ble

m s

pec

ific

co

mp

eten

ces

GA

D –

Bor

kove

cG

AD

– D

ugas

/Lad

ouce

urG

AD

– Z

inba

rg/C

rask

e/B

arlo

w

Soc

ial P

hobi

a –

Hei

mbe

rgS

ocia

l Pho

bia

– C

lark

OC

D –

Ste

kete

eO

CD

– K

ozac

Act

ivity

mon

itorin

g &

sch

edul

ing

Bas

ic C

BT

co

mp

eten

ces

Sha

ring

resp

onsi

bilit

y fo

r se

ssio

nst

ruct

ure

& c

onte

nt

Abi

lity

to a

gree

goa

ls fo

r th

ein

terv

entio

n

Abi

lity

to p

lan

and

to r

evie

w p

ract

ice

assi

gnm

ents

(‘h

omew

ork’

)

Usi

ng s

umm

arie

s an

d fe

edba

ckto

str

uctu

re th

e se

ssio

n

Abi

lity

to d

evis

e a

mai

nten

ance

cycl

e an

d us

e th

is to

set

targ

ets

Gui

ded

disc

over

y &

Soc

ratic

ques

tioni

ng

Abi

lity

to e

licit

key

cogn

ition

s/im

ages

Abi

lity

to u

se th

ough

t rec

ords

Abi

lity

to d

etec

t, ex

amin

e an

dhe

lp c

lient

rea

lity

test

auto

mat

ic th

ough

ts/im

ages

Abi

lity

to id

entif

y an

d he

lp c

lient

mod

ify a

ssum

ptio

ns, a

ttitu

des

and

rule

s

PT

SD

– F

oa &

Rot

hbau

mP

TS

D –

Res

ick

PT

SD

– E

hler

s

Beh

avio

ural

Act

ivat

ion

Abi

lity

to p

lan

and

cond

uct

beha

viou

ral e

xper

imen

ts

Abi

lity

to d

evel

op fo

rmul

atio

n an

dus

e th

is to

dev

elop

trea

tmen

t pla

n/ca

se c

once

ptua

lisat

ion

Pan

ic D

isor

der

(with

or

with

out

agor

apho

bia)

– C

lark

Pan

ic D

isor

der

(with

or

with

out

agor

apho

bia)

– B

arlo

w

Abi

lity

to a

dher

e to

an

agre

edag

enda

Kno

wle

dge

of c

omm

on c

ogni

tive

bias

es r

elev

ant t

o C

BT

Abi

lity

to id

entif

y an

d w

ork

with

safe

ty b

ehav

iour

s

Abi

lity

to e

mpl

oy im

ager

yte

chni

ques

Abi

lity

to id

entif

y an

d he

lp c

lient

mod

ify c

ore

belie

fs

Kno

wle

dge

of b

asic

prin

cipl

es o

fC

BT

and

ratio

nale

for

trea

tmen

t

Abi

lity

to e

xpla

in a

nd d

emon

stra

tera

tiona

le fo

r C

BT

to c

lient

Kno

wle

dge

of th

e ro

le o

f saf

ety–

seek

ing

beha

viou

rs

Abi

lity

to s

truc

ture

ses

sion

s

Abi

lity

to u

nder

stan

d cl

ient

’s in

ner

wor

ld a

nd r

espo

nse

to th

erap

y

Gui

ded

CB

T s

elf h

elp

Dep

ress

ion

– Lo

win

tens

ity in

terv

entio

ns

Dep

ress

ion

– H

igh

inte

nsity

inte

rven

tions

Abi

lity

to e

nd th

erap

y in

a p

lann

edm

anne

r, a

nd to

pla

n fo

r lo

ng–t

erm

mai

nten

ance

of g

ains

afte

r tr

eatm

ent

Abi

lity

to u

se m

easu

res

and

self

mon

itorin

g to

gui

de th

erap

y an

d to

mon

itor

outc

ome

Ab

ility

to

imp

lem

ent

CB

T u

sin

g a

colla

bo

rati

ve a

pp

roac

h

Gen

eric

th

erap

euti

cco

mp

eten

ces

Kno

wle

dge

and

unde

rsta

ndin

gof

men

tal h

ealth

pro

blem

s

Kno

wle

dge

of, a

nd a

bilit

y to

oper

ate

with

in, p

rofe

ssio

nal

and

ethi

cal g

uide

lines

Kno

wle

dge

of a

mod

el o

fth

erap

y, a

nd th

e ab

ility

toun

ders

tand

and

em

ploy

the

mod

el in

pra

ctic

e

Abi

lity

to e

ngag

e cl

ient

Abi

lity

to fo

ster

and

mai

ntai

n a

good

ther

apeu

tic a

llian

ce, a

ndto

gra

sp th

e cl

ient

’spe

rspe

ctiv

e an

d ‘w

orld

vie

w’

Abi

lity

to d

eal w

ith e

mot

iona

lco

nten

t of s

essi

ons

Abi

lity

to m

anag

e en

ding

s

Abi

lity

to u

nder

take

gen

eric

asse

ssm

ent (

rele

vant

his

tory

and

iden

tifyi

ng s

uita

bilit

y fo

rin

terv

entio

n)

Abi

lity

to m

ake

use

ofsu

perv

isio

n

Met

aco

mp

eten

ces

Cap

acity

to im

plem

ent

CB

T in

a m

anne

rco

nson

ant w

ith it

sun

derly

ing

philo

soph

y

Cap

acity

to m

anag

eob

stac

les

to C

BT

ther

apy

Cap

acity

to u

se c

linic

alju

dgm

ent w

hen

impl

emen

ting

trea

tmen

t mod

els

Cap

acity

to a

dapt

inte

rven

tions

in r

espo

nse

to c

lient

feed

back

Cap

acity

to fo

rmul

ate

and

to a

pply

CB

T m

odel

s to

the

indi

vidu

al c

lient

Cap

acity

to s

elec

t and

appl

y m

ost a

ppro

pria

teB

T &

CB

T m

etho

d

CB

T s

peci

fic m

etac

ompe

nten

cies

Cap

acity

to s

truc

ture

sess

ions

and

mai

ntai

nap

prop

riate

pac

ing

Cap

acity

to u

se a

nd r

espo

ndto

hum

our

Gen

eric

met

acom

pete

nces

Figu

re 1

.1

Com

pete

nces

for

the

eff

ectiv

e de

liver

y of

CB

T f

or d

epre

ssio

n an

d an

xiet

y di

sord

ers

(Rot

h &

Pill

ing,

200

7).

© C

row

n C

opyr

ight

(20

07).

0002092982.INDD 7 3/17/2014 12:04:21 PM

Page 6: Mastering Metacompetence - Wiley

8 Adrian Whittington and Nick Grey

In addition, recognizing that skilled psychological therapy must be more than a combination of technical procedures, Roth and Pilling (2007) identified a fifth category:

Metacompetences: These are defined as a set of higher order competences that “focus on the ability to implement models in a manner that is flexible and tailored to the needs of the individual client” (p. 9). Most of their list was derived from RCT therapy manuals, with some based on expert consensus and some on research evidence. Metacompetences were generated in two areas: generic metacompetences, said to be employed in all therapies, and CBT-specific metacompetences. The listed categories of metacompetences are as follows, with a number of specific metacompetences in each category:Generic metacompetences:

1. capacity to use clinical judgement when implementing treatment models2. capacity to adapt interventions in response to client feedback, and3. capacity to use and respond to humour.

CBT specific metacompetences:1. capacity to implement CBT in a manner consonant with its underlying

philosophy,2. capacity to formulate and to apply CBT models to the individual client,3. capacity to select and apply skilfully the most appropriate CBT intervention

method,4. capacity to structure sessions and maintain appropriate pacing, and5. capacity to manage obstacles to carrying out CBT.

Roth and Pilling suggested that metacompetences may be thought of as procedural rules by which therapists can apply the methods of therapy in a theoretically coher-ent, but appropriately adapted and individually tailored way, as a good cook may use but adapt a recipe. For example, one generic metacompetence procedural rule is listed as:

[to] maintain adherence to a therapy without inappropriate switching between modalities in response to minor difficulties (i.e., difficulties which can be readily accommodated by the model being applied).

As with the rest of the competency framework the list of metacompetences is not pre-sented as exhaustive or permanent. Metacompetences define your art as a therapist in implementing evidence-based practice, adapting empirically grounded clinical inter-ventions to the circumstances that are presented to you. They encompass the process of translating research findings into practice. We think there is value in building on and expanding the list provided by the Roth and Pilling (2007) framework so that proce-dural rules for the art of therapy can be made more explicit.

Three Risks to Effective Practice

There are risks in leaving the art of therapy as an implicit skill, assumed to be developed through experience or supervision, rather than something that is at least worth trying to define and make explicit. Without definition, lots of different forms of unhelpful

0002092982.INDD 8 3/17/2014 12:04:21 PM

Page 7: Mastering Metacompetence - Wiley

Mastering Metacompetence: The Science and Art of CBT 9

practice can be labelled as appropriate flexibility or adaptation. Therapist resistance to research-based evidence can provide fertile ground in which such anomalous practice can grow. Even those committed to evidence-based practice may drift away from effec-tive approaches or become overly rigid in their approach and not recognize that what is happening is far from artful.

The risk of rejecting research evidence

Many therapists hold research evidence as a highly valued guide in practice, and CBT therapists are likely to value it fairly highly. However, this is by no means uni-versal. A qualitative study of US practicing psychologists found that most ranked research evidence as lower than clinical experience and intuition in guiding their practice (Stewart, Stirman, & Chambless, 2012). In the United Kingdom, therapist disquiet about applying research-based evidence is highlighted by the active debate on the subject in the UK Clinical Psychology professional literature (e.g., Smail, 2006).

Scott Lilienfield and colleagues (2013) have provided a thoughtful and constructive review and commentary on the “resistance” shown by some therapy practitioners to evidence-based practice. They highlight the risks of rejecting research evidence, citing harmful medical practices in mental health such as the prefrontal lobotomy, which gained currency through the reliance on clinical expertise in the absence of research data. Without research evidence, as a therapist you are unable to tell the difference between therapeutic effectiveness and placebo effect or spontaneous remission. This opens the way to a proliferation of spurious treatments.

There are numerous therapist beliefs that may interfere with the delivery of EBP, including a belief that what seems to be the evidence of your own eyes is more valuable than the evidence of RCTs (Lilienfield et al., 2013). Beliefs that we have found to be particularly relevant that could impede the dissemination of effective CBT treatments include beliefs that the clients who enter RCTs are not representative of the population seen in routine care and that treatments derived from RCT manuals are prescriptive and inflexible (Shafran et al., 2009). These beliefs are examined in the light of the following evidence.

Belief 1: Clients entering RCTs are not representative of those seen in routine care, where there are more severe or co-morbid presentationsCo-morbidity is very common, with axis 1 conditions co-morbid with other axis 1 or axis 2 disorders in the range of 50 to 90 per cent (e.g., Kessler, Nelso, McGonagle, & Liu, 1996). It is true that RCTs do sometimes exclude participants with co-morbidity or as a result of severity, for example, if the client is actively suicidal. However, analyses of clients that are seen in routine care suggest that only 5 per cent would have been excluded from an RCT (Stirman et al., 2005). The most common reasons for exclusion in this routine care population would not have been more severe or complex presenta-tions, but the clients in routine care not meeting minimum severity or duration criteria. More recent trials allow extensive co-morbidity without great reduction in outcome (DeRubies et al., 2005; Duffy, Gillespie, & Clark, 2007). Furthermore, a recent study of CBT for PTSD in routine care suggests that the majority of client characteristics that would have led to exclusion from an RCT made no difference to outcome of therapy. Large effect sizes were demonstrated even for clients that would have been excluded from an RCT (Ehlers et al., 2013).

0002092982.INDD 9 3/17/2014 12:04:21 PM

Page 8: Mastering Metacompetence - Wiley

10 Adrian Whittington and Nick Grey

Belief 2: Interventions delivered in RCT manuals are prescriptive and inflexibleClinical guidelines used in RCTs have been developed for diagnostic categories, which can be limiting in complex cases. However, interventions within RCTs are usually based on an individualized formulation, based tightly on a specific model for the problem that is the focus of the trial. Flexibility is in fact inherent in RCT treatments using manuals and the use of manuals must always involve “flexibility within fidelity” (Kendall and Beidas, 2007). Even in trials participants will “strain the paradigm” (Markowitz et al., 2012).

In reality there are likely to be significant differences between trial conditions and routine care, and also some differences on dimensions of resourcing, therapist expertise and quality control (Roth, Pilling, & Turner, 2010; Stewart and Chambless, 2009) (see Table 1.1).

These differences in context, therapy and therapist factors suggest that rather than RCTs needing to be more like routine practice in order to provide realistic outcomes, we should endeavour to make routine practice become more like the conditions estab-lished in RCTs in order to achieve the best outcomes.

The risk of therapist drift

In a similar vein, there is evidence that therapist “drift” from adherence to evidence-based protocols can lead to poorer responses to treatment. Glenn Waller has observed that therapists commonly “drift” away from pushing for behavioural change (a core element in component analyses of successful treatment) to a more discursive approach (Waller, 2009). This drift may be driven by a number of factors including therapists’ own beliefs, emotional reactions and safety behaviours. Waller proposed that the same factors may lead clinicians to rush to implement newer “third wave” therapies even when the best evidence-based therapy has never been tried.

A detailed analysis of video recordings of CBT for anxiety, confirmed that therapists frequently switched away from core methods such as exposure (Schulte and Eifert,

Table 1.1 Likely differences between trials and routine practice

RCTs Routine practice

Resources Usually better resourced Resources restricted

Assessment More structured, detailed and regular

Procedures to identify focal problems and diagnoses less common

Therapists More likely to be expert in administration of a particular treatment

Covering a wider range of main problems

Caseloads Usually smaller Usually larger

Therapy Protocol controls duration and number of sessions

Often service provider controls duration and number of sessions

Engagement Sometimes greater efforts made to maintain engagement

Sometimes less emphasis on reducing attrition rates given the often large numbers waiting for treatments

Quality control Adherence monitoring and high quality supervision

Adherence monitoring may be limited and supervision of less consistent quality

0002092982.INDD 10 3/17/2014 12:04:21 PM

Page 9: Mastering Metacompetence - Wiley

Mastering Metacompetence: The Science and Art of CBT 11

2002). Therapists explained that they perceived a need to do something different when they faced relational difficulties in the session. More frequent switches in this direction were associated with more negative outcomes of therapy, whereas more frequent switches of direction towards implementing core methods were associated with more positive outcomes. Changes in treatment direction were not triggered by a lack of progress, but by therapists feeling less positive about their relationship with their client. In light of these findings it will be important for you to distinguish metacompetent flexibility and adaptation in your practice from drift or unhelpful changes of direction.

The risk of rigid application of technique

Lack of flexibility is another risk. Whereas therapists may sometimes switch away from core strategies when therapeutic alliance starts to deteriorate, therapists may equally sometimes be tempted to push even harder on rigid application of technique at these times, with negative effects on outcome (Castonguay, Goldfreid, Wiser, Raue, & Hayes, 1996). There is emerging evidence that flexing the structure of interventions in a planned way (including providing more sessions) and the focus of sessions (including “stressor sessions” when the focus of session is primarily on current life events) can help to maintain good outcomes in effectiveness studies (Galovski, Blain, Mott, Elwood, & Houle, 2012). However, there is little data on whether particular variations on the structure of therapy (e.g., duration or frequency of sessions) are superior to others (Haaga, 2004; Westen et al., 2004). You will need to walk a tightrope between adher-ence and flexible adaptation and avoid straying into drift, or overly rigid practice.

Bringing the Science and Art Together: Metacompetent Adherence

Telling the difference between unhelpful drift and helpful flexibility or between unhelp-ful rigidity and helpful adherence is highly challenging. This book aims to help with this. It does not propose an “anything goes” approach, but rather a way of introducing appropriate adaptation and flexibility in a measured and evidence-based way, when faced with particular circumstances. Metacompetence is not all about flexing or chang-ing “normal” CBT. It is about knowing when and what to flex within the principles and theoretical underpinnings of CBT. Often metacompetence will guide a practitioner to stay on course with the usual evidence-based method rather than divert into less well-tested methods.

We make a distinction between (1) principles of CBT, including its conceptual under-pinning and core methods such as formulation and collaborative empiricism, (2) tactics such as deciding how to set up opportunities for cognitive change or which problem to tackle first, and (3) techniques such as the use of thought records or behavioural experi-ments. A sporting analogy can be helpful here. In tennis, for example, principles are the rules of the game, tactics refer to how different game situations are approached when responding to a particular opponent, and techniques include the application of fore-hand, backhand, volley and serve.

In considering how to apply CBT to each unique circumstance you should consider these as a hierarchy of levels for adaptation. Technique can be adapted in a myriad of unique ways to suit individual needs and it will be appropriate to do this in every session with every client. Tactics may be reconsidered in the light of evidence that the usual

0002092982.INDD 11 3/17/2014 12:04:21 PM

Page 10: Mastering Metacompetence - Wiley

12 Adrian Whittington and Nick Grey

tactics are unsuitable, but principles should be held firm in all cases, except where there is evidence that another form of therapy or intervention may be more helpful.

How closely we hold to each of these aspects of CBT may be “tight” (more rigidly held) or “loose” (more flexibly held). These ideas are partly inspired by Cory Newman’s excellent book on core competencies in CBT (Newman, 2012). Various degrees of tight/loose alignment to principles, tactics and technique can occur and can explain different styles of practice commonly encountered (see Figure 1.2).

According to this framework therapists aim to provide competent CBT, and then further to adapt and flex it as appropriate, by employing metacompetences. Some ther-apists, particularly when inexperienced, may fall into the trap of rigid practice, and oth-ers, however experienced, can drift into unfocused practice, sometimes mistaking this for metacompetence.

Many commonly occurring complexities add to the challenge of providing CBT (e.g. Westbrook, Mueller, Kennerley, & McManus, 2010). These include:

1. A mismatch between how client and therapist understand the problem (e.g., a cli-ent who assures you they do not have a drink problem despite objective signs that they are alcohol dependent).

2. Co-morbid Axis I problems (e.g., PTSD with depression)3. Co-morbid Axis I and Axis II problems (e.g., Depression in the context of

Borderline Personality Disorder)

Tactics and technique: Tight Tactics and technique: Loose

Principles: Tight Competent Adherence

Tactics and techniques applied in the standard way as in RCTs. Suited to learning CBT and to specific problem types with specific protocols.

Likely to be effective if there is a good fit between the specific approach and the specific problem/case. Practitioners with less experience should aim to practice in this manner.

Metacompetent Adherence

Appropriate adaptation of tactics and techniques using fundamental CBT principles. Suited to CBT for cases that don’t fit the usual guidelines. Different to therapist “drift”; as motivation to adapt is theory and evidence-based. May be effective, more likely to be needed with complex cases and easier with greater clinical experience.

Principles: Loose Rigid Practice

Tactics and techniques applied rigidly without recourse to principles. When first learning CBT treatments practitioners may find themselves practicing in this manner – just thinking about what to do next without properly basing this on a formulation. Can be aversive for clients and have negative impact on outcome.

Unfocused Practice

Unfocused tactics and erratic use of technique, without recourse to principles. This is the culmination of therapist “drift”. Does not resemble CBT and likely to beless effective.

Figure 1.2 Different styles of CBT practice associated with loose/tight application of CBT principles, tactics and technique. © Adrian Whittington and Nick Grey (2013).

0002092982.INDD 12 3/17/2014 12:04:22 PM

Page 11: Mastering Metacompetence - Wiley

Mastering Metacompetence: The Science and Art of CBT 13

4. Client factors (e.g., memory problems associated with learning disability or physi-cal health problems associated with older age)

5. Client context factors (e.g., client’s relatives inadvertently act in ways that maintain the problem, or social circumstances such a loss of employment)

6. Therapist–client interaction factors (e.g., client acts in ways that triggers counter-productive cognitions and behaviours in the therapist)

7. Therapist factors (e.g., therapist holds beliefs about CBT that do not reflect the evidence, for example, that straightforward application of CBT cannot provide long–term resolution of difficulties)

8. Therapist context factors (e.g., the provider rations the “dose” of CBT to below the level where effectiveness would be expected)

9. Therapy is not helping (e.g.,“usual” protocol is leading to no change, deteriora-tion or client disengagement)

Metacompetence will be especially important in addressing these complexities, which often take us beyond the typical textbook application of CBT.

Conclusion

Providing CBT is inevitably both a science and an art. Metacompetence offers a frame-work for defining and helping to make explicit some aspects of the artistry of CBT. Metacompetent adherence is a way of bringing science and artistry together to deliver more effective therapy. This approach can reduce the risk of drift or unhelpful rigidity in practice and help you to resolve dilemmas that you will face in delivering CBT every day.

Metacompetence consists of numerous procedural rules for applying CBT in differ-ent specific circumstances. Roth and Pilling (2007) identified a number of desired outcomes from implementing these rules (e.g., “formulate and apply CBT models to the individual client”). In this book we attempt to uncover some of the specific rules by which these outcomes might be achieved, based on best evidence and the experience of experts in the field. The book reviews the foundations of CBT competence on which metacompetence must be built, establishes how you should adapt and flex the delivery of CBT in the face of complex presentations of anxiety and depression, and serves as an introduction to ways of meeting the additional needs of other specific client groups. It also guides you in how you might continue to learn additional procedural rules as you go forward in your practice.

Procedural Rules•  Always look first to whether you are competently delivering evidence-based

approaches before you attempt variations on these.•  Do not get thrown off course by myths that evidence-based practice and

evidence from randomized controlled trials is not relevant. Use this evidence as a starting point for competent practice.

•  Beware the risks of therapist “drift” from proven approaches. Where you adapt or flex CBT, do so based on the principles of CBT and evidence-based practice.

0002092982.INDD 13 3/17/2014 12:04:22 PM

Page 12: Mastering Metacompetence - Wiley

14 Adrian Whittington and Nick Grey

Acknowledgements

Many thanks to Helen Curr, Ben Smith and Kerry Young for comments on an earlier version of this chapter.

Note

1 Reproduced courtesy of www.foodandwine.com, by permission of Daniel Patterson

References

Butler, A.C., Chapman, J.E., Forman, E.M., & Beck, A.T. (2006). The empirical status of cog-nitive behaviour therapy: a review of meta-analyses. Clinical Psychology Review, 26, 17–31. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2005.07.003

Castonguay, L.G., Goldfried, M.R., Wiser, S., Raue, P.J., & Hayes, A.M. (1996). Predicting the effect of cognitive therapy for depression: A study of unique and common factors. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 64, 497–504.

Clark, D.M. (1986). A cognitive approach to panic. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 24, 461–470.Clark, D.M. (2011). Implementing NICE guidelines for the psychological treatment of depres-

sion and anxiety disorders: The IAPT experience. International Review of Psychiatry, 23, 375–384. doi: 10.3109/09540261.2011.606803

DeRubies, R.J., Hollon, S.D., Amsterdam, J.D., Shelton, R.C., Young, P.R., Salomon, R.M., . . . Gallop, R. (2005). Cognitive therapy vs. medications in the treatment of moderate to severe depression. Archives of General Psychiatry, 62, 409–416.

DiMauro, J., Domingues, J., Fernandez, G., & Tolin, D. (2013). Long-term effectiveness of CBT for anxiety disorders in an adult outpatient clinic sample: a follow-up study. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 51, 82–86. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2012.10.003

Duffy, M., Gillespie, K., & Clark, D.M. (2007). Posttraumatic Stress Disorder in the context of terrorism and other civil conflict in Northern Ireland: randomized controlled trial. British Medical Journal, 334, 1147–1150.

Ehlers, A., Grey, N., Wild, J., Stott, R., Liness, S., Deale, A., . . . Clark, D.M. (2013). Dissemina-tion of cognitive therapy for PTSD in routine clinical care: Effectiveness and moderators of outcome in a consecutive sample. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 51(11), 742–752. doi: 10.1016/j/brat.2013.08.006

Galovski, T.E., Blain, L.M., Mott, J.M., Elwood, L., & Houle, T. (2012). Manualized therapy for PTSD: flexing the structure of cognitive processing therapy. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 80, 968–981. doi: 10.1037/a0030600

•  Avoid overly rigid adherence to particular techniques in the face of difficulties.

•  Aim for metacompetent adherence, adhering to the principles and conceptual underpinnings of CBT while allowing appropriate flexibility in tactics and techniques of therapy.

•  Set about collecting explicit “procedural rules” for metacompetent practice. You can start by reading this book and attempting to put the rules you read about into practice.

0002092982.INDD 14 3/17/2014 12:04:22 PM

Page 13: Mastering Metacompetence - Wiley

Mastering Metacompetence: The Science and Art of CBT 15

Gibbons, C.J., Fournier, J.C., Stirman, S.W., DeRubeis, R.J., Crits-Cristoph, P., & Beck, A.T. (2010). The clinical effectiveness of cognitive therapy for depression in an outpatient clinic. Journal of Affective Disorders, 125, 169–176. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2009.12.030

Haaga, D.A.F. (2004). A healthy dose of criticism for randomized trials: comment on Westen, Novotny and Thompson-Brenner (2004). Psychological Bulletin, 130, 674–676. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.130.4.674

Hands, E., & Hiller, W. (2013). Effectiveness of and dropout from outpatient cognitive behav-ioral therapy for adult unipolar depression: a meta-analysis of nonrandomized effective-ness studies. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 81(1), 75–88. doi: 10.1037/a0031080.

Jacobson, N.S., Dobson, K.S., Truax, P.A., Addis, M.E., Koerner, K., . . . Prince, S.E. (1996). A component analysis of cognitive-behavioral treatment for depression. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 64(2), 295–304.

Kaptchuck, T.J. (2001). The double-blind, randomized, placebo-controlled trial:Gold standard or golden calf? Journal of Clinical Epidemiology, 54, 541–549.

Kendall, P.C., & Beidas, R.S. (2007). Smoothing the trail for dissemination of evidence based practice for youth: flexibility within fidelity. Professional Psychology: Research and Practice, 38, 13–19. doi: 10.1016/j.cbpra.2009.11.002

Kendall, P.C., Gosch, E., Furr, J.M., & Sood, E. (2008). Flexibility within fidelity. Journal of the Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 47, 987–993. doi: 10.1097/CHI.0b013 e31817eed2f

Kessler, R.C., Nelson, C.B., McGonagle, K.A., & Liu, J. (1996). Comorbidity of DSM-III-R major depressive disorder in the general population: results from the US National Comor-bidity Survey. British Journal of Psychiatry, 168, 17–30.

Lillienfield, S.O., Ritschel, L.A., Lynn, S.J., Cautin, R.L., & Latzman, R.D. (2013). Why many clinical psychologists are resistant to evidence-based practice: root causes and constructive remedies. Clinical Psychology Review, 33(7), 883–900. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2012.09.008

Markowitz, J.C., Kaplowitz, M., Eun-Jung, S., Meehan, K.B., Neria, Y., Jonker, H., . . . Lovell, K. (2012). Treating patients who strain the research psychotherapy paradigm. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 200, 594–597. doi: 10.1097/NMD.0b013e31825bfaf4

National Institute for Health and Clinical Excellence. (2009). The treatment and management of depression in adults. NICE clinical guideline 90. Retrieved from http://guidance.nice.org.uk/cg90

National Institute for Health and Clinical Excellence. (2011). Generalised anxiety disorder and panic disorder (with or without agoraphobia) in adults. Management in primary, secondary and community care. NICE clinical guideline 113. Retrieved from http://guidance.nice.org.uk/CG113

Newman, C.F. (2012). Core competencies in cognitive behavioural therapy: becoming a highly effec-tive and competent cognitive behavioural therapist. New York: Routledge.

Ost, L.-G. (2013, July). One-session treatment, ACT, and implementation of research findings in clinical practice. Paper presented at British Association for Behavioural and Cognitive Psychotherapies 41st Annual Conference, London.

Roth, A., & Fonagy, P. (2005). What works for whom? A critical review of psychotherapy research (2nd ed.). New York: Guilford Press.

Roth, A., & Pilling, S. (2007). The competences required to deliver effective cognitive and be-havioural therapy for people with depression and with anxiety disorders. Improving Access to Psychological Therapies (IAPT) Programme. Retrieved from www.ucl.ac.uk/clinical-psychology/CORE/CBT_Competences/CBT_Competence_List.pdf

Roth, A., Pilling, S., & Turner, J. (2010). Therapist training and supervision in clinical trials: implications for clinical practice. Behavioural and Cognitive Psychotherapy, 38, 291–302.

Salkovskis, P.M. (2002). Empirically grounded clinical interventions: Cognitive-behavioural therapy progresses through a multi-dimensional approach to clinical science. Behavioural and Cognitive Psychotherapy, 30, 3–9. doi: 10.1017/S1352465802001029

0002092982.INDD 15 3/17/2014 12:04:22 PM

Page 14: Mastering Metacompetence - Wiley

16 Adrian Whittington and Nick Grey

Schulte, D., & Eifert, G.H. (2002). What to do when the manuals fail? The dual model of psy-chotherapy. Clinical Psychology Science and Practice, 9, 312–328.

Shafran, R., Clark, D.M., Fairburn, C.G., Arntz, A., Barlow, D.H., Ehlers, A., . . . Wilson, G.T. (2009). Mind the gap: improving the dissemination of CBT. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 47, 902–909. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2009.07.003

Smail, D. (2006). Is clinical psychology selling its soul (again)? Clinical Psychology Forum, 168, 17–20.

Spring, B. (2007). Evidence-based practice in clinical psychology: what it is; why it matters; what you need to know. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 63, 611–631. doi: 10.1002/jclp.20373

Stewart, R.E., & Chambless, D.L. (2009). Cognitive-behavioral therapy for adult anxiety disor-ders in clinical practice: a meta-analysis of effectiveness studies. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 77, 595–606. doi: 10.1037/a0016032

Stewart, R.E., Stirman, S.W., & Chambless, D.L. (2012). A qualitative investigation of practic-ing psychologists’ attitudes toward research-informed practice: Implications for dissemina-tion strategies. Professional Psychology: Research and Practice, 43, 100–109. doi: 10.1037/a0025694

Stirman, S.W., DeRubeis, R.J., Crits-Cristoph, P., & Rothman, A. (2005). Can the randomized controlled trial literature generalize to non-randomized clients? Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 73, 127–145. doi: 10.1037/0022-006X.73.1.127

Waller, G. (2009). Evidence-based treatment and therapist drift. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 47, 119–127. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2008.10.018

Westbrook, D., Mueller, M., Kennerley, H., & McManus, F. (2010). Common problems in therapy. In edited by M. Mueller, H. Kennerley, F. McManus & D. Westbrook (Eds.), Oxford Guide to Surviving as a CBT Therapist (pp. 1–40). Oxford: Oxford University Press.

Westen, D., Novotny, C.M., &Thompson-Brenner, H. (2004). The empirical status of empiri-cally supported psychotherapies: assumptions, findings and reporting in controlled clinical trials. Psychological Bulletin, 130, 631–663. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.130.4.631

0002092982.INDD 16 3/17/2014 12:04:22 PM