Learning Material for Concept and Conceptual Change

download Learning Material for Concept and Conceptual Change

of 28

Transcript of Learning Material for Concept and Conceptual Change

  • 7/27/2019 Learning Material for Concept and Conceptual Change

    1/28

    1

    Learning Material for concept and conceptual change

    Introduction

    The concept of concepts is difficult to define, but no one doubts that concepts are fundamental tomental life and human communication. Cognitive scientists generally agree that a concept is a

    mental representation that picks out a set of entities, or a category. That is, concepts refer, and

    what they refer to are categories. It is also commonly assumed that category membership is not

    arbitrary but rather a principled matter. What goes into a category belongs there by virtue of

    some law-like regularities. But beyond these sparse facts, the concept CONCEPT is up for grabs.

    As an example, suppose you have the concept TRIANGLE represented as a closed geometric

    form having three sides. In this case, the concept is a definition. But it is unclear what else

    might be in your triangle concept. Does it include the fact that geometry books discuss them

    (though some dont) or that they have 180 degrees (though in hyperbolic geometry none do)? It

    is also unclear how many concepts have definitions or what substitutes for definitions in ones

    those dont.

    In metaphysics, and especially ontology, a concept is a fundamental category of existence.

    In contemporary philosophy, there are at least three prevailing ways to understand what a

    concept is:

    Concepts as mental representations, where concepts are entities that exist in the brain. Concepts as abilities, where concepts are abilities peculiar to cognitive agents. Concepts as abstract objects, where objects are the constituents of propositions that

    mediate between thought, language, and referents.

    Eggen and Kauchak (2004) defined concepts as ideas, objects, or events that help us understand

    the world around us.

    Functions of concepts

    For purposes of this review, we will collapse the many ways people can use concepts into two

    broad functions: categorization and communication. The conceptual function that most research

    has targeted is categorization, the process by which mental representations (concepts) determinewhether or not some entity is a member of a category. Categorization enables a wide variety of

    subordinate functions because classifying something as a category member allows people to

    bring their knowledge of the category to bear on the new instance. Once people categorize some

    novel entity, for example, they can use relevant knowledge forunderstandingandprediction.

    Recognizing a cylindrical object as a flashlight allows you to understand its parts, trace its

    functions, and predict its behavior. For example, you can confidently infer that the flashlight will

  • 7/27/2019 Learning Material for Concept and Conceptual Change

    2/28

    2

    have one or more batteries, will have some sort of switch, and will normally produce a beam of

    light when the switch is pressed. Not only do people categorize in order to understand new

    entities, they also use the new entities to modify and update their concepts. In other words,

    categorization supports learning.

    Concepts are also centrally involved in communication. Many of our concepts correspond tolexical entries, such as the English word flashlight. In order for people to avoid

    misunderstanding each other, they must have comparable concepts in mind. If As concept of

    cell phone corresponds with Bs concept of flashlight, it wont go well if A asks B to make a

    call. An important part of the function of concepts in communication is their ability to combine

    in order to create an unlimited number of new concepts.

    These functions and associated subfunctions are important to bear in mind because studying any

    one in isolation can lead to misleading conclusions about conceptual structure.

    Childrens Learning ofConcepts and Names

    Perhaps the most dramatic example of concept learning is the performance of young children,

    who can learn up to 15,000 new words for things by age six (Carey, 1978). Of course, learning a

    new word and learning a new concept are not the same, but they are closely related (Clark,

    1983). For example, a childs knowing the word dog and having the concept of dog are two

    different achievements. Knowing a concept might precede learning its name or alternately,

    hearing a name for an object might lead to further investigation of the concept (e.g., Waxman,

    Shipley, & Shepperson, 1991). Early concept learning by children appears to be guided by rather

    general principles or knowledge structures. Given the large number of concepts learned by

    children and the systematic biases that are apparent in this learning, it is plausible that thechildren are being influenced by general knowledge rather than by specific knowledge about

    other categories. Markman (1989, 1990) suggested, and reviewed evidence for, certain

    constraints that would guide category learning by children. First, according to the whole object

    assumption, a novel category label is more likely to refer to a whole object than to its parts. Upon

    hearing a category label such as dog for the first time, a child would assume that this label

    refers to a dog rather than to some part of a dog such as its wagging tail. Second, according to the

    taxonomic assumption, learners will tend to use new words as taxonomic category labels rather

    than as ways to group things by other relations. For example, after a child has learned about his

    or her first dog, the child would extend this label to other animals that appear to be in the same

    taxonomic categoryother dogs--rather than extending the label to objects that are otherwise

    associated with the dog. That is, the child would not call the dogs leash a dog, or call the

    dogs owner a dog. Third, the mutual exclusivity assumption would provide further guidance

    in early category learning. In following this assumption, a child would favor associating

    particular objects with just one category label. Thus, when learning a new category label, the

    child would look for some object for which he or she does not already know a label. For

    example, say that a child already knows the word dog, and sees a dog being pulled on a leash.

  • 7/27/2019 Learning Material for Concept and Conceptual Change

    3/28

    3

    Upon hearing the word leash for the first time, the child might hypothesize that this term refers

    to the leash rather than to the dog, because the dog already has a known category label.

    These three constraints might seem obvious to an adult who has already learned a language. Yet

    imagine a child trying to learning thousands of category labels without these assumptions

    (Quine, 1960). In a relatively simple situation of a girl walking in a park with a dog on a leash,the category label dog might refer to the girl, the park, the dog, the leash, some part of the girl,

    the park, the dog, or the leash, or some relation between any of these things. It appears that some

    application of general knowledge to this potentially confusing situation would be extremely

    helpful and indeed necessary.

  • 7/27/2019 Learning Material for Concept and Conceptual Change

    4/28

    4

    ALTERNATIVE CONCEPTS AND MISCONCEPTIONS

    Roth (as cited in Fraser, 1995) describes three important approaches to science education:

    1) The inquiry approach - in 1960s and 1970s new science programmes were developed to focus

    upon the nature and processes of science. The students were to act like scientists and scientific

    method was a pivot. Science content was used for learning science process skills;

    2) The societal approach - in the late 1970s and 1980s science programmes were used to

    inculcate the skills of decision making and problem solving in relation to the societal problems in

    order to make the students good citizens;

    3) The conceptual change approach - from the mid-1980s science programmes stressed

    conceptual change in learning process. A learner has a world view based on personal

    observations. The starting point is the childrens prior conceptions. Instructional activities should

    be based on the reasoning explicit.Wandersee, Mintzes and Novak (1994) mention that the research on students alternative

    conceptions in various content domains rapidly expanded during the 1980s. As surveyed by Duit

    (2007) there are over 8000 studies in science education literature that reported the existence of

    misconceptions or alternative conceptions.

    Numerous studies in recent years have shown proof that many students do not understand

    concepts in science in the same way as experts and scientists.

    Students incorrect understanding of scientific concepts and natural phenomena affects their

    performances.

    Smith, DiSessa, and Roschelle (1993) observed that novice interpretations of scientific concepts

    and expert perceptions of scientific knowledge are very different.

    Tomita (2008) argued:

    When students enter science classrooms, they often hold deeply rooted prior knowledge or

    conceptions about the natural world. These conceptions will influence how they come to

    understand their formal science experiences in school. Some of this prior knowledge provides a

    good foundation for further formal schooling, while other conceptions may be incompatible with

    currently accepted scientific knowledge. The importance of prior knowledge and the struggle toreplace that knowledge with or help that knowledge evolve into scientifically-sound knowledge

    has spurred a large tradition of research in developmental and instructional psychology and

    science education (p. 9).

    Three examples of science concepts and their associated misconceptions

  • 7/27/2019 Learning Material for Concept and Conceptual Change

    5/28

    5

    Scientific Concepts Associated Misconceptions

    Whether something sinks or floats depends

    on a combination of its density, buoyancy,

    and effect on surface tension.

    Things float if they are light and sink if

    they are heavy.

    Clouds contain very small particles of

    water or ice that are held up in the air by

    the lifting action of air currents, wind and

    convection. These particles can become

    bigger through condensation and when they

    become too heavy to be held up in the air

    they fall to the earth as rain, hail or snow.

    Clouds contain water that leaks out as rain

    An animal is a multicellular organism thatis capable of independent movement.

    An animal is a land mammal other than ahuman being. Insects, birds and fish are not

    animals.

  • 7/27/2019 Learning Material for Concept and Conceptual Change

    6/28

    6

    Importance of weeding out misconceptions

    For the authenticity of the knowledge, correct concepts need to be developed and the

    misconceptions are to be weeded out. Ausubel (as cited in Joyce, 1996) mentions that a person's

    existing cognitive structure is the foremost factor governing whether new material will be

    meaningful and how well it can be acquired and retained. Before we present new materialeffectively, we must increase the stability and clarity of our students' structures.

    To acquire knowledge of misconceptions of students about certain topics to be taught is

    important. Generally, misconceptions are the cause of low achievement. If the number of

    misconceptions increases, the students face difficulty in understanding the concept being taught.

    Low understanding results in hampering the concept understanding. In some cases alternative

    concepts hamper to generalize. As a consequence, meaning of the concept is restricted. In other

    cases, students are unable to discriminate among closely related concepts causing them to

    mislabel or misclassify.

    The history of science has shown that conceptual change is a lengthy process accompanied by astruggle with different types of misconceptions and that it includes alternating stages of advance

    and retreat. It is not reasonable to expect our students to internalize the model in a meaningful

    manner after it is presented by several statements of knowledge over the course of one or two

    class sessions.

    Misconceptions may have a positive role. Historical research has shown that even great scientists who

    broke new ground in any given area still held misconceptions in other areas or aspects. These

    misconceptions may be viewed as essential steps in the evolution of innovative ideas. Prior

    misconceptions are the raw materials for critical research which then leads us to higher levels

    of understanding.It is also necessary to apply this view to the classroom. Despite the fact that teachers want to

    help their students reach the desired conceptions, it is necessary to be tolerant and relate

    positively to their misconceptions. These misconceptions may be analogous to childhood

    illnesses or problems of adolescence. True, they are not welcomed and we may wish that they

    would not exist, but we know that ultimately they have a positive function in human develop-

    ment.

    The development of the history of science can be perceived as an incomplete dialectic process

    (thesisantithesissynthesis; today's conception is tomorrow's misconception). We should

    understand the function of misconceptions in student intellectual development in a similarmanner. For this reason, educators should not try to ignore or to declare war on misconceptions,

    but should rather focus the class on them. Only in this way can they initiate the desired process

    of conceptual change.

    "For example, Pascal's and Boyle's comparison of air to wool or to a spring in order to

    explain its compressibility or Torricelli's comparison of the atmosphere to an ocean in order to

    explain differences of pressure at different altitudes.

  • 7/27/2019 Learning Material for Concept and Conceptual Change

    7/28

    7

    Definitions of Misconceptions

    Driver, Guesne and Tiberghien (1985) mention that children try to interpret prevailing

    phenomenon with previously acquired ideas. An extensive literature has been built up in recent

    years, which indicates that children develop ideas about natural phenomenon before they are

    taught science in school. Fraser (1995) mentions that every child has ideas to explain thesurrounding world. In many cases these explanations are not the same as given by today's

    scientists. Such type of explanation is termed as misconceptions or alternative conceptions.

    Misconceptions can be described as ideas that provide an incorrect understanding of such ideas,

    objects orevents that are constructed based on a persons experience including such things as

    preconceived notions, nonscientific beliefs, nave theories, mixed conceptions or conceptual

    misunderstandings.

    Misconceptions are the interpretations and understandings that dont support the scientific

    reasons or provide footing for the logic.

    Despite the fact that the term ofstudent misconceptions is widely used in scientific literature, not

    all researchers agree to define students prior knowledge as misconceptions. The term

    misconception has many synonyms.

    Tomita (2008) summarized synonyms existing in the literature for this term. Primarily referred to

    as misconceptions, these conceptions also are called naive conceptions, nonscientific beliefs,pre-

    instructional beliefs, intuitive knowledge, phenomenological primitives or p-prims, facets, or

    alternative frameworks.

    Regardless of terminology, the point is to recognize that a students' prior knowledge is

    embedded in a system of logic and justification, although one that may be incompatible with

    accepted scientific understanding (Tomita, 2008, p. 10).

    Smith, diSessa, and Roschelle (1993) argued that clarification of the terms misconceptions,

    alternative beliefs, and preconceptions is necessary:

    The prefix to the most common term - misconception - emphasizes the mistaken quality of

    students ideas.

    Terms that include the qualifier alternative - indicate a more relativist epistemological

    perspective.

    Students prior ideas are not always criticized as mistaken notions that need repair or

    replacement but are understood as understandings that are simply different from the views of

    experts

    Studentsalternative conceptions are incommensurable with expert concepts in a manner parallel

    to scientific theories from different historical periods

    Preconceptions and nave beliefs emphasize the existence of student ideas prior to instruction

    without any clear indication of their validity or usefulness in learning expert concepts.

  • 7/27/2019 Learning Material for Concept and Conceptual Change

    8/28

    8

    However, researchers who have used them have tended to emphasize their negative aspects. This

    epistemological dimension emphasizes differences in content. The content of student

    conceptions (whether mistaken, preexisting, or alternative) is judged in contrast to the content of

    expert concepts (p. 159).

    The word misconceptions as used throughout this document may be taken to mean alternativeframeworks or alternative conceptions.) A misconception can be defined as a view that does

    not fully coincide with the scientific view.

    Categories of misconceptions

    National Academy Press (1997) quotes five categories of misconceptions:

    1) preconceived notions - many people preconceive that underground water flows in the form of

    streams as they have preconceived this idea from their everyday experiences. Such type of

    misconceptions is called preconceived notions;2) non-scientific beliefs - some students learn through religious/mythical belief about the origin

    of life. The disparity between the religious belief and the scientific belief create misconception;

    3) conceptual misunderstanding - students when taught in a way, which does not conflict with

    their misconceptions, creates faulty models;

    4) vernacular misconception, when words mean scientifically different and in everyday life as

    well e.g. work;

    5) factual misconceptions - falsities learnt at early ages are not challenged in adulthood. For

    example, an idea that lightning never strikes the same area twice if not challenged may be

    present in your belief system.

  • 7/27/2019 Learning Material for Concept and Conceptual Change

    9/28

    9

    UNDERSTANDING AND CONCEPTUAL CHANGE

    IN SCIENCE

    To date, science educators have published over 3500 studies on students' understandings of

    scientific concepts.

    In contrast to the assumptions of many science teachers, it is now clear that learnersdevelop a set of well-defined ideas about natural objects and events even before they arrive

    at the classroom door. These ideas span the range of the formal scientific disciplines and

    seem to be found in equal frequencies among males and females, learners of all ages and

    ability levels, as well as cultural backgrounds and ethnic origins.

    Often these notions conflict with accepted scientific explanations and, most significantly,

    because they serve a useful function in everyday life, they tend to resist the efforts of even

    our finest teachers and most thoughtful textbook authors and curriculum developers.

    Unfortunately students' ideas often interact with knowledge presented in formal science

    lessons resulting in a diverse set of unintended learning outcomes.

    Assumptions about Misconceptions

    1.Learners are not "empty vessels" or "blank slates"; they bring with them to their formalstudy of science concepts; a finite but diverse set of ideas about natural objects and

    events; often these ideas are incompatible with those offered by science teachers and

    textbooks.

    2.Many alternative conceptions are robust with respect to age, ability, gender, andcultural boundaries; they are characteristic of all formal science disciplines including biology,chemistry, physics, and the earth and space sciences; they typically serve a useful function

    in the everyday l ives of individuals.

    3.The ideas that learners bring with them to formal science instruction are oftentenacious and resistant to change by conventional teaching strategies.

    4.As learners construct meanings, the knowledge they bring interacts with knowledgepresented in formal instruction; the result is a diverse set of unintended learning outcomes;

    because of limitations in formal assessment strategies, these unintended outcomes may

    remain hidden from teachers and students themselves.

    5.The explanations that learners cling to often resemble those of previous generationsof scientists and natural philosophers.

    6.Alternative conceptions are products of a diverse set of personal experiences, includingdirect observation of natural objects and events, peer culture, everyday language, and the

    mass media as well as formal instructional intervention.

    7.Classroom teachers often subscribe to the same alternative conceptions as their

  • 7/27/2019 Learning Material for Concept and Conceptual Change

    10/28

    10

    students.

    8.Successful science learners possess a strongly hierarchical, cohesive framework ofrelated concepts and they represent those concepts at a deeper, more principled level.

    9.Understanding and conceptual change are epistemological outcomes of the consciousattempt by learners to make meanings; successful science learners make meanings byrestructuring their existing knowledge frameworks through an orderly set of cognitive events

    (i.e., sub-sumption, super ordination, integration, and differentiation).

    10.The differential ability to solve problems in novel, real-world settings is attributableprimarily to the advantages conferred on individuals possess ing a highly integrated, well-

    differentiated framework of domain-specific knowledge which is activated through

    concentrated attention to and sustained reflection on related objects and events.

    11.Learners who excel in the natural sciences habitually employ a set of metacognitivestrategies enabling them to plan, monitor, regulate, and control their own learning.

    12.Instructional strategies that focus on understanding and conceptual change may beeffective classroom tools.

    Research-based claims relating to authentic alternative conceptions

    Claim 1: Learners come to formal science instruction with a diverse set of alternative

    conceptions concerning natural objects and events. Alternative conceptions span the fields from

    physics and earth & space science to biology, chemistry, and environmental science. Each

    associated subfield within the disciplines seems to have its alternative conceptions.

    Claim 2: The alternative conceptions that learners bring to formal science instruction cut across

    age, ability, gender, and cultural boundaries. No matter how gifted a group of students

    concerned, each group will have students with alternative conceptions regardless of background.

    Claim 3: Alternative conceptions are tenacious and resistant to extinction by conventional

    teaching strategies. Students alternative conceptions are very difficult to change; only very

    specific teaching approaches have shown promise of getting students to accept new explanations.

    Claim 4: Alternative conceptions often parallel explanations of natural phenomena offered by

    previous generations of scientists and philosophers. Students often hold to the same views as

    those held by very early scientists that are frequently referred to as Aristotelian in nature.Claim 5: Alternative conceptions have their origins in a diverse set of personal experiences

    including direct observation and perception, peer culture, and language, as well as in teachers

    explanations and instructional materials. The many sources of alternative conceptions are at best

    speculative, but research and inference suggest that a students worldview is strongly influenced

    by his or her social environment.

  • 7/27/2019 Learning Material for Concept and Conceptual Change

    11/28

    11

    Claim 6: Teachers often subscribe to the same alternative conceptions as their students. It is not

    at all uncommon for science teacher educators to see alternative conceptions in their teacher

    candidates; likewise, even experienced science teachers and scientists with advanced degrees

    will sometimes cling to alternative conceptions that are held by their students.

    Claim 7: Learners prior knowledge interacts with knowledge presented in formal instruction,resulting in a diverse variety of unintended learning outcomes. Not only can alternative

    conceptions be a hindrance to new learning; they can also interact with new learning resulting in

    mixed outcomes. It is not unusual to see different students draw different conclusions from the

    same experiences and observations.

    Claim 8: Instructional approaches that facilitate conceptual change can be effective classroom

    tools. Several conceptual change approaches have been developed to identify, confront, and

    resolve problems associated with alternative conceptions.

    Some features of misconceptions are that they:

    nd are usually adequate for everyday life;

    ht in schools;

  • 7/27/2019 Learning Material for Concept and Conceptual Change

    12/28

    12

    Sources of students misconceptions

    Students develop certain misconceptions since science is being taught as a historical account.

    Demonstrations, observation and experimentation find a little space in the teaching of science.

    The content teaching is not supported with the practical experimentation.

    Driver (1985) mentions that the possible sources of students misconceptions are: schoolteaching, outside school teaching, everyday experiences, social environment, and intuition.

    Nonetheless, there is general evidence that pre-instructional knowledge structures have their

    origins in the following sources: (1) experiences and perceptions that extend as far back as early

    infancy, (2) a wide variety of cultural values and ideas, and (3) language factors.

    Experiences and perceptions: Hawkins and Pea (1987) argue that children construct knowledge

    structures for scientific understanding on a domain by domain basis prior to formal instruction.

    It is therefore important to view children as active constructors of knowledge through their

    interactions with the physical world and their social and cultural environments. Even when they

    are only toddlers, children are actively engaged in asking for explanations and giving reasons

    about the way things are. A functional reason for developing these explanations is to gain more

    predictive control over the world. This allows the child to avoid undesirable events and to

    perpetuate desirable ones. The child learns what to expect by his own actions, by the actions of

    others, and by events in the physical world. In this way, children construct non-scientific

    understandings of natural phenomena as they are encountered, and they create frames for

    interpreting natural and social events. Further insights are provided by McClelland (1984):

    Phenomena are not the content of science but the vehicle for learning it, that is, for learning

    theories. Children in all societies meet a wide range of phenomena but a glance at history and

    anthropology is enough to remind us that interpretations in terms or reproducible, explicable,causally related events are not automatic features of human thought.

    Pre-instructional understandings are therefore quite adequate to interpret and guide daily life

    (Driver, 1994), but may significantly hinder learning in the context of the science classroom.

    Culture: Conceptions can also have their origin within the overall culture that students are

    participants in. Solomon (1987) states that the social scene makes an essential difference as to

    how a particular task is perceived in the learning environment.

    He therefore asks, Do entirely personal ideas ever exist? When a child holds some private

    evaluation about a scientific happening, is it ever unaffected by culture?

    Solomon argues that even if a student has a truly eccentric idea, this idea will probably will not

    survive for very long. Too different of a viewpoint from the accepted notion will generally be

    excluded from social intercourse, and many children may not have the ability to withstand this

    kind of pressure. The human desire to be accepted will cause many individual ideas to fade

    away. The chief effect of social interaction, therefore, is to

  • 7/27/2019 Learning Material for Concept and Conceptual Change

    13/28

    13

    smooth out differences within the culture and to produce consensus. This is not to say that

    change cannot take place; even majority views change with time. However, it is to say that the

    influence of the overall culture on students understandings is incredibly powerful and cannot be

    ignored by instructors.

    An example of how cultural influences can effect understanding is found in a study thatexamined folk biological taxonomies among the Itzaj (a people native to the Americas) and

    among North American college students (Lopez 1997). Of special interest was the way that the

    Itzaj subjects categorized bats. While the American group tended to group bats with insectivores

    and rodents (thus preserving scientific formalisms to a significant degree), the Itzaj left them

    unaffiliated with any general category, or they classified them as birds. When asked, the Itzaj

    acknowledged that bats do indeed seem to more closely resemble shrews and small rodents.

    They did not classify them as mammals, however, because they knew that bats are birds!

    Cultural influences caused the Itzaj subjects to deem the relationship of bats to mammals as

    superficial. The influence of scientific understanding on the culture of the United States,

    however, helped the North American college students to avoid this stumbling block.

    Language: Word meaning and usage can also be a significant source for alternative conceptions.

    An example of how this can happen is presented by Strike and Posner (1992). They describe a

    hypothetical learner named Fred who is asked to choose between two views of motion. Fred is

    asked to think about what will happen if a force is applied to a particular object. He is presented

    with two views:

    (1) Force is transferred to the object and erodes, causing the object to gradually slow and

    eventually come to a rest.

    (2) Application of force to the object imparts some motion to the object that continuesindefinitely until it is acted upon by another force.

    Fred is a baseball fan and he notes that there are numerous cases where forces are applied to

    baseballs. The subsequent motion of the balls leads him to accept the first view. This seems

    logical to Fred because he can detect no other forces being applied to the baseballs.

    Thus begins the language game. Strike and Posner explain it in the following way:

    Fred may have learned to talk about force in a way that requires force to have an agent. Hitting

    balls with bats thus counts as applying force. Also, force-talk may be associated with fatigue.

    Ones ability to apply force is limited by stamina. Or sometimes in ordinary speech force is

    associated with coercion. Normally, when people are coerced, they cease doing what they arecoerced to do as soon as the coercion is withdrawn. Fred thus has ways of talking about force

    that lead to and reinforce a way of seeing.

    Fred thus decides that forces are transferred to objects and erode during motion.

    This story may only be hypothetical, but it is a very realistic illustration. Indeed, studies

    involving both grade school and college level students have demonstrated that students often do

  • 7/27/2019 Learning Material for Concept and Conceptual Change

    14/28

    14

    not have the same definitions for scientific terms as those that are held by their instructors. For

    example, a general characterization of nave knowledge of motion has been described as follows

    (Champagne 1983):

    (1) Concepts are poorly differentiated. For example, students use the terms speed, velocity and

    acceleration interchangeably. As a result, the typical student does not perceive any differencesbetween two propositions such as these: (a) The speed of an object is proportional to the [net]

    force on the object; (b) The acceleration of an object is proportional to the [net] force on the

    object.

    (2) Meanings physicists attribute to terms are different from the everyday meanings attributed to

    the terms by the students. For example, students generally define acceleration as speeding up,

    while physicists define acceleration as any change in velocity.

    According to some psychologist, there are some other possible sources for the development of

    misconceptions. First, not all experiences lead to correct conclusions or result in students seeing

    all possible outcomes. Second, when parents or other family members are confronted withquestions from their children, rather that admitting to not knowing the answer, it is common for

    them to give an incorrect one. Other sources of misconceptions include resource materials, the

    media and teachers. The main issue is that all of the above sources are considered to be

    trustworthy, leading to ready acceptance by students of what they are being taught

    why some science concepts are difficult to change?

    In fact, there is overall consensus among researchers that alternative conceptions about science

    are highly resistant to change (MacBeth 2000). Simply telling somebody something does not

    easily change his or her deep ideas (Redish 1994). One researcher went so far as to say that

    we cannot affect scientific understanding without grasping the depth and tenacity of the

    students preexistingknowledge.

    Chi (2005) mentions why some science concepts are difficult to change? He explains that

    students own ontological categories and the actual categories do not correspond. Member of one

    ontological category is misrepresented as a member of another ontological category.

  • 7/27/2019 Learning Material for Concept and Conceptual Change

    15/28

    15

    Misconceptions in Science

    Brown and Hammer (2008) described a typical example of students misconceptions about

    scientific concepts (in physics). A student may be able to apply F = ma accurately to find a if

    given F and m, but if asked to explain what the equation means might say something like: It

    means that the force of an object depends on how heavy it is and how fast its moving. Thisinvolves alternative ways of thinking about all three variablesforce as a property of an object,

    mass as weight, and acceleration as speed (p. 128). Brown and Hammer (2008) argued that even

    after physics instruction, college graduates continue to have serious misconceptions about

    various concepts like force, energy, and temperature. Even the best and brightest students are not

    learning what educators may think they are learning from their science education.

    There is a wide range of literature about misconceptions in science, which often discusses the

    understandings of physical phenomena by students. Incorrect (unscientific) perceptions are

    called misconceptions. There are a few stages that the majority of literature follows:

    - Describe a phenomenon (like force, temperature, light).

    - Ask students what they think about this phenomenon, what will happen and why.

    - Analyze the answers (correct and incorrect perceptions of the phenomenon).

    - Make an attempt to understand how the incorrect meaning of the phenomenon occurs. Refer to

    cognitive psychology and conceptual change theories.

    - Discuss robustness of student misconceptions.

    - Discuss how to improve the curriculum and teaching methods.

    Streveler et al. (2008) argued that one of the main issues in psychology literature about

    conceptual knowledge is whether the students' knowledge is organized in a coherent structure or

    whether it is fragmented (p.280). Therefore, the literature often considers s tudent

    misconceptions from two perspectives, alternative ideas that are organized as theory or ideas that

    are elements or fragments. Brown and Hammer (2008) argued that these two perspectives on

    misconceptions shifted educators understanding of student errors:

    Whereas previously students were seen as just making mistakes, now they were seen as scientists

    applying alternative theories to interpretations of phenomena. This helped to make sense of why

    students seemed resistant to new ideas and it drew attention to the need to understand their

    existing theories (p. 130).

    Methods to help the students to overcome misconceptions

    The teacher can try the following methods to help the students to overcome misconceptions.

    1. Anticipate the most common misconceptions related to the instructional material.

  • 7/27/2019 Learning Material for Concept and Conceptual Change

    16/28

    16

    2. Encourage students to test their conceptual frameworks after discussing with the other

    students.

    3. The teacher should think about the demonstrations for addressing the common

    misconceptions.

    4. Assess whether the students have been able to grasp the concept according to the scientistbelief or not?

    Biemans mentions that prior knowledge activation and conceptual change can be fostered by a 5-

    step strategy:

    1) searching for ones own preconceptions;

    2) comparing and contrasting these preconceptions with new information;

    3) formulating new conceptions based on previous steps;

    4) applying new conceptions; and

    5) evaluating the new conceptions based on previous study.

    The precursor mentioned in these strategies are used to bring about conceptual change are the

    knowledge of misconceptions.

    Pedagogies for Addressing Alternative Conceptions

    A wide range of pedagogies has been developed to address alternative conceptions such as

    learning cycles (Karplus, 1981), Conceptual change theory of Posner et al. (1982), bridging

    analogies (Clement, 1988; Perschard & Bitbol, 2008), microcomputer- based laboratoryexperiences (Thornton & Sokolof, 1990; Thornton, 1987), disequilibration techniques (Minstrell,

    1989; Dykstra, Boyle, & Monarch, 1992), an inquiry approach coupled with concept substitution

    strategies (Harrison et al., 1999), metaconceptual teaching on inducing a particularly problematic

    aspect of the conceptual changes (Wiser & Amin, 2001), and a teaching model (Thomaz et al.,

    1995).

    These approaches tend to have in common the requirement that students encounter phenomena

    that run counter to their existing beliefs. Doing so, they are put in a state of intellectual

    disequilibrium or cognitive conflict. Becoming aware of the conflict between what they believe

    to be correct based on prior experiences and know to be correct based on more recent experience

    helps them to confront and resolve their conflicting perspectives in favor of a proper

    understanding. Such pedagogical approaches that emphasize conflict and resolution appear to

    derive from a Piagetian perspective on learning (Scott, Asoko, & Driver, 1998). In such a

    viewpoint, the learners role in reorganizing their knowledge is central to overcoming the

    alternative conception.

  • 7/27/2019 Learning Material for Concept and Conceptual Change

    17/28

    17

    These and other approaches dealing with alternative concepts typically include three

    fundamental stepsthose identified by the University of Washington Physics Education Group:

    elicit/ confront/resolve (McDermott, 1991). In this model a teacher first elicits a response

    (prediction about what will happen or an indication of agreement or disagreement with a given

    statement) from students, forcing them to commit to an answer in relation to a specific situation.

    Next, the students confront a situation that challenges their beliefs and answers, typically in an

    experiment that the students perform. During this second phase, if the students were incorrect in

    their prediction, they experience cognitive dissonance when confronting the conflict between

    prediction and experience.

    Students quickly come to realize the need for a new understanding about the concept under

    consideration, and are motivated to resolve the conflict with teacher assistance in phase three.

    Another such strategy is that developed for the C3P Project. According to Olenick (2008)

    overcoming alternative conceptions requires the following distinct steps:

    (1) Teachers must recognize that alternative conceptions exist.

    (2) Teachers probe for students alternative conceptions through demonstrations and questions.

    (3) Teachers ask students to clarify their understanding and beliefs.

    (4) Teachers provide contradictions to students alternative conceptions through questions,

    implications, and demonstrations.

    (5) Teachers encourage discussion, urging students to apply physical concepts in their reasoning.

    (6) Teachers foster the replacement of the misconception with new concepts through (i)

    questions, (ii) thought experiments, (iii) hypothetical situations with and without the underlying

    physical law, and (iv) experiments or demonstrations designed to test hypotheses.

    (7) Teachers reevaluate students understanding by posing conceptual questions.

    The Focus of Conceptual Change Research

    In a previous section, I demonstrated why one who simply learns that it is raining outside (or that

    hot air rises, or that anything else happens for that matter!) has not necessarily undergone what

    researchers within the conceptual change domain consider to actually be conceptual change. It

    has been shown that ideas held by learners are rooted within a lifetime of experiences,

    perceptions, cultural influences, and language use, and cannot be easily overthrown. As such, itseems inadequate to attempt to change, idea by idea, the vast inventory of alternative

    conceptions. It is important to understand that conceptual change research is performed by

    people who are heavily involved in the science education system, and who are searching for

    solutions for its crucial problems and inadequacies (Anderson 1987). As such, the futile

    endeavor of altering the plethora of individual ideas is rejected. Instead, conceptual change

    researchers focus their attention on those concepts that are at the core of a system of concepts.

  • 7/27/2019 Learning Material for Concept and Conceptual Change

    18/28

    18

    It is more analogous to what Piaget calls an accommodation, or to what Kuhn calls a paradigm

    shift (Strike 1992). The next two paragraphs present a brief overview of both of these ideas.

    Accommodation: Piagets notion of an accommodation involves the replacement or

    reorganization of central concepts (Posner 1998). The easiest way to explain what is meant by

    this is to give a couple of examples. I will begin with a simple one (Millhoff 2002): Sometimes old ways or existing schemes of dealing with the world simply dont work. Piaget used the

    term accommodation to describe this changing of an existing scheme to fit new objects. An

    example of accommodation would be the action of a young person who has always ridden a

    bicycle with pedal brakes but then gets on one with hand brakes.

    Accommodation of the existing braking scheme must occur for the bicyclist to be able to stop.

    A more complex example from Piagets own writings involves a four month and twentytwo day

    old infant named Laurent (Piaget 1952): Laurent knows how to strike objects intentionally

    with his hand [He] holds a stick; he does not know what to do with it and slowly passes it from

    hand to hand. The stick then happens to strike a toy hanging from the bassinet hood. Laurent,immediately interested by this unexpected result, keeps the stick raised in the same position, then

    brings it noticeably nearer to the toy. He strikes it a second time. Then he draws the stick back

    but moving it as little as possible as though trying to conserve the favorable position, then he

    brings it nearer to the toy, and so on, more and more rapidly [The] child, intentionally and

    systematically, applies himself to rediscovering the conditions which lead him to this unexpected

    result.

    Paradigm shift: Kuhns notion of the paradigm involves concepts that are organizing in nature,

    and that adequately address contemporary research problems. In his bookThe Structure of

    Scientific Revolution (1970), Kuhn describes the history of science as a series of paradigm shifts.If the dominant paradigm of the time cannot adequately address contemporary problems, a new

    paradigm may arise and compete for acceptance. Normal Science therefore involves research

    that is firmly based on one or more scientific achievements that some particular scientific

    community acknowledges, for a time, as supplying the foundations for its future research and

    practices. However, even if the new paradigm proves better at problem solving, it is often met

    with resistance. Many scientists will adhere to the old paradigm until their deaths even if it

    means ignoring a tremendous amount of evidence. Copernicanism, for example, was not widely

    receivedby the scientific community until nearly a generation after Copernicus death.

    5. Examples of Conceptual Change Research

    When a learner makes a conceptual leap that is analogous to an accommodation or paradigm

    shift, then one can say that conceptual change has finally taken place within that learner. It is

    around this notion that theories of conceptual change are designed.

  • 7/27/2019 Learning Material for Concept and Conceptual Change

    19/28

    19

    Metacognition and Conceptual Change

    RICHARD F. GUNSTONE

    Monash University

    IAN I. MITCHELL

    Monash University andEumemmering Secondary College

    Our meanings for conceptual change and metacognition

    Some general research influences underpinning the growth of concern with conceptual change

    have been described in Chapter 3 of this volume (Section 3: "Understanding and

    Conceptual Change in Science: The Current Research Agenda"). These influences have been

    of fundamental importance to the evolution of our own ideas. Our work in this area over the

    past 15 years has been largely in physics (e.g., Gunstone & White, 1981) and chemistry

    (e.g., Mitchell & Gunstone, 1984). Much of this work has been located in, and therefore

    intertwined with, the dailiness of our normal teaching in high school science (e.g., Baird &

    Mitchell, 1986; Baird & Northfield, 1992; Mitchell, 1993; White & Mitchell, 1994) and teacher

    education (e.g., Champagne, Gunstone, & Klopfer, 1985; Gunstone, 1994; Gunstone &

    Northfield, 1994). Our concerns throughout this work have been to develop our

    understanding of conceptual change and metacognition in the context of normal school and

    university classrooms.

    Conceptual Change

    One significant aspect of our current conception of conceptual change is that the content to

    be learned is a major variable in terms of the process of conceptual change (Mitchell &

    Baird, 1986; White, 1994). We return to this point in the concluding section of the chapter and,

    for the moment, consider conceptual change in more general terms.

    When considered in terms of an individual learner, the essence of a constructivist view of

    conceptual change is that it is the learner who must recognize his/her conceptions, evaluate

    these conceptions, decide whether to reconstruct the conceptions, and, if they decide to

    reconstruct, to review and restructure other relevant aspects of their understanding in ways

    that lead to consistency. While ultimately these processes of recognize, evaluate, decide

    whether to reconstruct, review other aspects of understanding are individual, each is

    profoundly influenced (positively or negatively) by the ways in which the teacher, and other

    class members, structure classroom practice.

    These processes of recognize, evaluate, reconstruct, and review do not often lead to

    dramatic conceptual change. Conceptual change is rarely a sharp replacement of

    conception X by conception Y. Rather, conceptual change is more often "an accretion of

    information that the learner uses to sort out contexts in which it is profitable to use one form

  • 7/27/2019 Learning Material for Concept and Conceptual Change

    20/28

    20

    of explanation or another" (Fensham, Gunstone, & White, 1994, p. 6).

    Mitchell (1993) studied the learning of all students in four Grade 10 science classes

    during a 6-week unit on mechanics, a unit very similar to the sequence we describe below.

    He began this research looking for, and expecting to find, numerous examples of "ahha"

    experiences/incidentsthat is, experiences/incidents that were crucial to the conceptualchange of individual students. Although almost all students could on completion of the

    unit identify lessons and specific experiences that they had not found personally useful, there

    was only a very small number of "ahha" experiences. Most students found that most lessons

    made some useful contribution to their learning. Their conceptual change was evolutionary

    rather than revolutionary. This can be explained by the view of Hewson and Hewson (1992)

    that conceptual change involves a change in the status of competing ideas. The new ideas

    (Newtonian in this mechanics case) appeared to need a number of successful classroom

    episodes in order to gain the status of unquestioned superiority.

    It is often appropriate to consider the process as "conceptual addition" rather than

    "conceptual change."

    The example of drinking through a straw illustrates the point about shifts as well as

    additions in meaning. When learners come to understand the notion of pressure

    difference, they do not drop the word "suck," though their conceptions of sucking

    change. Knowledge about pressure has been added, but old kno wledge is revised

    rather than abandoned. A conceptual addition has occurred. Central to this formula-

    tion of what is often described as "conceptual change" is that the individual also has

    informed approaches to deciding which of a number of meanings is appropriate in aparticular context.

    (Fensham, Gunstone, & White, 1995, p 7; emphasis added)

    A further significant aspect of conceptual change, a term we continue to use in this

    chapter because it is so widespread as a general description for the development of

    understanding, is also related to contexts. Often learners will accept the scientific concept in

    one context, but then revert to using their prior conception in another context that we as

    science teachers would see as essentially the same as the first context. That is, conceptual

    change can often be seen to first take place in a particular context. Then the student may

    vacillate between scientific and prior conceptions from one context to another; the

    conceptual change is then context dependant and unstable. Long-term and stable

    conceptual change is achieved when the learner recognizes relevant commonalities across

    contexts and the generality of the scientific conception across these contexts (Tao, 1996; Tao &Gunstone, 1997).

    Metacognition

    Our conception of metacognition has been formed through research in classrooms (ours and

    others). It is a multifaceted conception, described in a number of sources (e.g., Baird &

    Mitchell, 1986; Baird & Northfield, 1992; Gunstone & Northfield, 1994; Mitchell, 1993;

  • 7/27/2019 Learning Material for Concept and Conceptual Change

    21/28

    21

    White & Mitchell, 1994). We now give a brief summary (based on Gunstone, 1994, pp. 134-

    136) of the various and complementary aspects of our meaning for metacognition.

    1. "Metacognition refers to the knowledge, awareness and control of one's own

    learning" (Baird, 1990, p. 184). Metacognitive knowledge refers to knowledge of the nature

    and processes of learning, of personal learning characteristics, and of effective learningstrategies and where to use these. Metacognitive awareness includes perceptions of the

    purpose of the current activity and of personal progress through the activity.

    Metacognitive control refers to the nature of learner decisions and actions during the

    activity. Inadequate knowledge restricts the extent to which awareness and control are

    possible.

    2. Metacognitive knowledge, awareness, and control are all learning outcomes(as well as fundamental influences on the nature of more usual learning

    outcomes). Hence metacognitive knowledge, awareness, and control can be

    developed with appropriate learning experiences. (This is well illustrated by

    extensive work in the Project for Enhancing Effective Learning (the PEEL project);

    see Baird & Mitchell, 1986; Baird & Northfield, 1992).

    3. Often the learning which gives rise to a learner's metacognitive ideas andbeliefs has been unconscious learning, and the learner finds it difficult to

    articulate his/her metacognitive views.

    4. All learners have metacognitive views of some form. That is, all learners havesome form of metacognitive knowledge. This can be of a form that is in conflict

    with the goals of conceptual change teaching (e.g., "it is the teacher's job to tell me

    so I understand"; "we have discussions in science when the teacher can't bebothered teaching"). There are many examples of such conflict (see, for example,

    Baird & Mitchell, 1986; Baird & Northfield, 1992).

    5. There can be tensions between metacognitive knowledge, awareness, andcontrol. Most obvious are contexts where the assessment of learning is via rote

    recallhere learners with enhanced metacognitive knowledge and awareness will

    see that they should not invest time and effort in developing their understanding

    and controlling their learning if they wish high grades. Teaching concerned with

    conceptual change and enhanced metacognition must have assessment

    approaches consistent with these learning goals.

    6. One helpful description of an appropriately metacognitive learner is alearner who undertakes the tasks of linking and monitoring their own learning.

    7. There are a number of commonly occurring poor learning tendenciesexhibited by many learners (Baird, 1986). Examples are superficial or impulsive

    attention, premature closure (where "closure" is used here and at other points in

    this chapter to mean bringing together in a conclusion; "premature closure" is then

  • 7/27/2019 Learning Material for Concept and Conceptual Change

    22/28

    22

    concluding or deciding that the task is complete before this is appropriate), lack

    of reflective thinking, and staying stuck (i.e., one problem or error stops all

    progress). These represent inadequate metacognition and are major barriers to

    learning.

    8.On the other hand, there are good learning behaviors which illustrate more

    appropriate metacognition in classrooms (Baird & Northfield, 1992, p. 63);

    these good learning behaviors can be fostered by appropriate teaching.

    These behaviors are many. Examples include telling teacher what they don't understand,

    planning a general strategy before starting a task, seeking links with other activities or

    topics, and justifying opinions.

    The Intertwined Nature

    of Conceptual Change and Metacognition

    The links between conceptual change and metacognition seem to us an obvious

    consequence of our description of conceptual change. The processes of recognizing existing

    conceptions, evaluating these, deciding whether to reconstruct, and reviewing are all

    metacognitive processes; they require appropriate metacognitive knowledge, awareness, and

    control.

    We now give an outline of a teaching sequence that is derived from these conceptual

    change/metacognition perspectives, beginning with some as pects of the broad intent of

    the sequence. After the outline we discuss some more general aspects of classrooms that

    are relevant to the sequence.

    AN EXAMPLE OF TEACHING

    FROM THESE PERSPECTIVES

    The sequence we describe below is about introductory mechanics. The sequence has been

    shaped by research on students' alternative conceptions in mechanics and by the views of

    conceptual change and metacognition outlined above. We have taught the sequence on a

    number of occasions, and reflections on these experiences have contributed to the form

    given here. Our own uses of the approaches in this content area have largely been with

    Grade 10 high school students in the state of Victoria (where science in Grades 7-10

    of the 6-year high school is a General Science taken by all students) and with science

    graduates who are Biology majors undertaking a 1-year postgraduate course to qualify

    as high school science teachers (and who may therefore be required to teachintroductory mechanics in the General Science program). We describe the sequence in

    terms of a Grade 10 class. We do not give fine detail in our descriptionclass, school,

    and curriculum contexts vary; so then will the fine detail of the sequence.

    The sequence begins with the probing of students' existing ideas, then seeks to

    promote the reconstruction of ideas about particular content, then explicitly considers the

  • 7/27/2019 Learning Material for Concept and Conceptual Change

    23/28

    23

    exploration of the consistency ideas across a variety of contexts, and finally concludes

    with some ideas for the assessment of learning from the sequence. It uses a range of

    experiences to foster students' intellectual engagement with the ideas to be learned. We

    assume throughout that much of the language that students use will be tentative,

    exploratory, and hypothetical as they grapple with the ideas of mechanicsthis

    assumption is a consequence of our recognition that students need time to consider and

    discuss these ideas if they are to develop understanding. We also assume that there

    will commonly need to be risk taking on the part of both learners and teachers.

    Throughout the sequence there should be an emphasis on the creation of links between

    lessons and with experiences students bring to the classroom and on the central issue of

    the consistency of students' views across situations. It is intended that students will

    learn from the questions and ideas and exploratory language of other students and that

    the rate of progress through the sequence (and some of the detail of the sequence per

    se) will be influenced by students and their ideas.

    The sequence includes three teaching approaches that may be unfamiliar: predict-

    observe-explain (POE); concept maps; relational diagrams (or Venn diagrams). We give a

    very brief description of the nature and intent of each of these here. Space prevents our

    giving a comprehensive account of the range of ways each of these approaches can be

    used, and of the linkages between these approaches and student learning. Such an

    account is in White and Gunstone (1992).

    Predict-Observe-Explain (POE)

    Students are shown a real situation, asked to give their prediction about the consequences

    of a particular change to the situation and the reasons they have for their prediction,then when the change is made they give their observation, and finally predictions and

    observations are reconciled if necessary. POEs can be used to explore students' ideas at

    the beginning of a topic, or to develop ideas during a topic, or to enhance understanding

    at the end of a topic by having them attempt to apply their learning to a real situation.

    In all of these uses, the reasons students have for a prediction are crucial; predictions,

    reasons and observations are usually best written, not verbal.

    An example of a POE for the content a rea of heat capacity of materials: The situation

    is a beaker of water and a beaker of cooking oil (equal volumes of liquids) placed on a

    hotplate and with thermometers (0-200C.). Students are asked to predict how the

    temperatures of the two liquids will compare when the hot plate has been turned on

    long enough for the water to be boiling (it is helpful to give alternatives for students

    to choose"Is the temperature of the cooking oil less than, the same as, greater than the

    boiling water?"), and to write their reasons. The observation is the readings of the

    thermometers. Reconciliation commonly involves addressing their prediction that the

    cooking oil temperature is less than the boiling water be cause the oil is not burning.

  • 7/27/2019 Learning Material for Concept and Conceptual Change

    24/28

    24

    Concept Maps

    Students have a set of words representing concepts, and perhaps other relevant things, and

    put these on a sheet of paper. They draw lines between those words that they see to be

    linked, and write the nature of the links on the lines. (This last point is crucial; it is the

    nature of the links students perceive that is the essence of the concept map.) Usually theteacher provides the students with the terms to be mapped, although there are a number of

    reasons for students themselves to sometimes generate some or all of the terms. Concept

    maps are highly effective for exploring the links students perceive between ideas, and for

    fostering further linking. (See Chapter 3 of this volume for description and examples of

    concept maps.)

    Relational (or Venn) Diagrams

    Students are given a small number of terms and asked to draw a circle or rectangle for each

    term, arranged so that the shapes represent the relationships between the terms. An example is

    "trees, grasses, flowering plants." The appropriate response has the shape for grasses totallywithin flowering plants, and that for trees partially within and partially without flowering

    plants.

    There are two important issues to recognize in using each of these three approaches,

    particularly concept maps and relational diagrams.

    I . Students need to be taught how to approach each of these if the students have not

    experienced them before. It is rather like having a class who have never seen multiple choice

    questionsbefore you give multiple choice to such a class you would need to help them

    understand the structure and intent of the questions, and how to respond. Failure to do this

    would mean that students would be unable to respond in the ways you intend. This is less of anissue for POEsthese are sufficiently similar to conventional science demonstrations that

    students will not have great difficulty in seeing what they are required to do, but it is a very

    important aspect of introducing concept maps or relational diagrams to a class who have not

    previously experienced these.

    2. It really is important to try any particular examples of the three approaches yourself

    before you use these with a class. With concept maps and relational diagrams in particular, this

    "trialling" is a necessary step in considering what terms should be given to the students. For

    example, we have been using concept maps in our science teaching for over a decade. Yet

    we still find that our first list of terms to give to students as a concept mapping task isfrequently changed when we try the task ourselves. That is, often when we do the concept

    map ourselves we see that one (or more) terms should be replaced by others for the task to

    achieve what we intend.

  • 7/27/2019 Learning Material for Concept and Conceptual Change

    25/28

    25

    A CONCLUDING COMMENT

    ON INTRODUCING THESE APPROACHES

    The approaches we suggest assume major changes for students and teachers, by comparison

    with a traditional, didactic classroom. We offer now some thoughts, derived from research and

    practice, on the nature of student change required. This is a crucial issue for action, for boththose wishing to introduce these approaches to their classrooms and for those who wish to

    research the consequences of these approaches. Note that the points we now briefly make are

    elaborated, with data, in many aspects of the PEEL project (Baird & Mitchell, 1986; Baird &

    Northfield, 1992; Mitchell, 1993).

    Moving from didactic classrooms to the classrooms involved in our mechanics sequence

    involves changes in students' metacognitive knowledge, awareness, and control. Put another way,

    changes are involved in students' ideas about learning (e.g., discussion can be "real work,"

    considering mistakes can contribute to learning), attitudes toward learning (e.g., there is benefit

    to my learning in investing the effort needed to engage with tasks and in taking risks by publicly

    expressing a point of view), and learning behaviors (e.g., asking questions about issues that are

    puzzling). One substantial difficulty in fostering these changes is that it is often quite impossible

    to meaningfully describe to students what the benefits of the changes will be before starting to

    teach in this manner. It is necessary to slowly build towards the changes through successive

    classroom experiences and to reward (via the nature of assessment) the consequences of students

    engaging intellectually in the ways we describe. Regular, short debriefing of successful

    experiences in terms of the nature of the learning behaviors (e.g., pointing out the value to the

    development of ideas by the class of a "wrong" answer that raised an important issue) is one

    important component of strategies for achieving this student change. Another was referred to

    in 6.2.2 of our mechanics sequence.

    It is common for teachers who are first hearing about or reading about these ideas to be

    sceptical. We frequently experience such reactions in our professional development work with

    other teachers, with comments about classroom management and content coverage often

    being made. The issue of content coverage has already been discussed; classroom

    management is not an ongoing issue when students are genuinely engaged. Our approaches

    have been used by many teachers, and the PEEL project (already frequently referred to in this

    chapter; Baird & Mitchell, 1986; Baird & Northfield, 1992) provides many classroom examples

    of these approaches. We reinforce this assertion of the feasibility of our approaches by

    concluding with an extract from a class disscussion in one of our classes. The class is a Grade10 in an average government high school.

    Two views of whether or not a table pushes up on a book placed on it have already been

    advanced: the table does not push up, initially advanced by Katie ("The table does not push

    up on the book, it just stops it falling; a table can't push"); the table does push up with a

    force equal and opposite to gravity, initially advanced by Ward. Other students have

    advanced arguments to support one of these views. The teacher has set up a meter ruler

  • 7/27/2019 Learning Material for Concept and Conceptual Change

    26/28

    26

    supported at either end and with weights placed at the middle of the ruler as an example of

    a much more flexible "table." (This is in 5.1 of the above sequence.) The extract begins with

    Ward commenting on the meter ruler demonstration. Comments in italics were inserted by the

    teacher to provide explanation of his thinking and decisions.

    Ward:The ruler is pushing up, it's like a spring, because when you put itthe weight( on it, it's like you've loaded a spring. If you took it off it's

    going to spring back up.

    (This comment could lead to the class working out how a table can push up. I wa nt

    it noted, but I don't want to reveal my views yet.)

    Teacher:So you reckon because it's bent it's pushing back up?

    II am extracting neutrally whatIsee as the key new point Ward is making.]

    Ward: Yes.

    Brad:If it was pushing up, wouldn't it be straight?

    'Brad is reacting to Ward's views. His argument is a common one. Brad cannot

    (yet) picture the ruler pushing up and not moving up. This issue is central and I

    want it thought through. I respond to Brad similarly to how I responded to Ward.

    Teacher:If it's pushing up wouldn't it be straight ... so you're saying it's not

    pushing up at the moment because it's bent?

    Brad and Kay:Yeah.

    Ward:'interrupts' It must be pushing down with the same power as it'spushing up.

    Teacher:So you're saying gravity is pulling down and the ruler's pushing

    up

    Danielle:'interrupts] If it was pushing down the same as it was pushing up

    the ruler would be straight.

    I Danielle now introduces a new possibility. I want everyone to be clear on what itisso I draw a diagram

    of her view beside the summary of the views ofWard andKatie that I drew earlier. Danielle's view, that

    there must be some upwards force, may be very useful in moving toward Newton's Third Law.]

    Teacher:All right. So Danielle's argument is ... they can't be equal and

    opposite because the ruler would not be bent.

    Danielle:It could be a force going up and bigger force going down.

    Teacher:OK. So you're prepared to accept some upward force, but it must

  • 7/27/2019 Learning Material for Concept and Conceptual Change

    27/28

    27

    be less than gravity down, so your drawing of this one, Danielle, would be

    'Teacher draws diagram'. You're prepared to accept that (points to an

    upward force) but because the ruler's bent, this one (the downwards

    force) must be bigger.

    Ward:If there was more force pointing down, then why isn't it going down?Kay and Danielle: 'interrupting) But it hasit has gone down.

    This is the start of a sequence of student-to-student debate where Idon't even need to maintain a

    chairperson role. The students are very involved and interested. I

    Ward:But it's notit's not moving down, what I mean is it's not moving

    now. So if there's still more force pushing down, then it's not still going

    down.

    Danielle:But it can't be equal.

    Brad:But it's like adding more weight on the floor. It can't go down any

    further.

    Ward: Why not?

    lames:Because he hasn't got any more weights. I Not a very useful comment.)

    Ward:If there's still more force pushing down, why can't it bend?

    Brad:Because this is the baseyou knowI mean if you stick the books

    on the table they're not going to push the table under the ground. I Brad

    believes that the rigid floor does not need to exert an upwards force to prevent weights placed on it frommoving downwards.)

    Ward:The table would just push back up again.

    Kay:Anyway if you put those weights on this table 'points to a class-

    room table) they're not going to go down.

    Danielle:No, but if you put heavier things on it, it might.

    I Kay may be getting convinced by Ward. She raises the important point that we have achieved little by

    showing a ruler table bends under a weight unless we can also show that "real" tables bend. I intend to

    show this later by standing students on tables. I don't want to discourage Kay, but I do want to stop the

    discussion becoming too complex by dealing with two sorts of tables at onceDanielle has made a good

    point. I decide to intervene, to promise Kaywe will address her querybut totryand resolve one issue at a

    time.)

    Teacher:OK so we will have to go back and check on this 'classroom) table

    later, won't we. You're saying on this 'ruler] table, I've rigged the situation

  • 7/27/2019 Learning Material for Concept and Conceptual Change

    28/28

    28

    a bit, Kay, because I got a bendy ruler. OK. We'll come back to that ...

    probably today ... but let's just stay with this situation ... 'the ruler

    table) may not be identical to that, but it's a situation in its own right.

    Kay: Yeah.

    II think thatKay trusts me to remember to return to her point.)

    5. Metacognition and Conceptual Change 163