by J. - UvA · combinations, except for tilose cases where a nonsense combination would have pe ri...

10
- 49 - Formt Frequencies d Duration in Runng peech Florina J. Koopms - v Beinum If Phonetics is regded aB the science investigating the process of speng and of heing, it will be necessy to investigate speech ite most normal !orm, nely in connected or free runng speech, in which ordiny conversation is of most frequent occurrence. For Phonology, too, normal speech must be the material from which a system is distilled and on which that system, in turn, is tested. In practice, however, Phonetician and Phonologist prefer to occupy them- selves with isolated sounds and isolated words. This is probably a matter of convenience in one respect and in another the result of the !�ot that, regrettably, we e not illiterate. Owing to the !act that some years ago an experiment was carried out which had involved the making of a series of tape-recordings of some ten speers, recordings of vious types o! speech, ranging from isolated vowels to ordiy Qonversations, the Institute of Phonetic Sciences in Amsterdam happened to be in possession of a corpus o! materi, which was ready to hand for comparison of a number of forms o speech. For a first investigation I confined myself to the vowels spoken by two informanta of the group of ten mentioned above, who, according to the opinion of 200 listeners in a previous experiment, had been considered to have the 'best' and the 'worst' pronunciation. Of these two informants fo types of speech per speer were comped, nely: 1) vowels spoken in isolation ( 2 x 12 vowels) 2) vowels spoken in isolated words ( 5 x 12 vowels) 3) vowels in stressed position in a text read oud (10 x 12 vowels) 4) vowela in unstressed position in a text, read aloud. '10 x 12 vowels) The following can be added about the fo types of speech. ad 1) The twelve vowels of Dutch were pronounced twice random order by both speakers. ad 2) Four series of isolated words were read from sepate cds in random order. The words were of the CVO type, viz. b-t, b-r, � d

Transcript of by J. - UvA · combinations, except for tilose cases where a nonsense combination would have pe ri...

Page 1: by J. - UvA · combinations, except for tilose cases where a nonsense combination would have pe ri the. result_, in which case a E was substitut d by a ' a by a Rt and a l by a -The

- 49 -

Formant Frequencies and Duration in Running .Speech

by Florina J. Koopmans - van Beinum

If Phonetics is regarded aB the science investigating the process of

speaking and of hearing, it will be necessary to investigate speech in ite most normal. !orm, namely in connected or free running speech, in

which ordinary conversation is of most frequent occurrence.

For Phonology, too, normal speech must be the material from which a

system is distilled and on which that system, in turn, is tested. In

practice, however, Phonetician and Phonologist prefer to occupy them­

selves with isolated sounds and isolated words. This is probably a

matter of convenience in one respect and in another the result of the

!�ot that, regrettably, we are not illiterate.

Owing to the !act that some years ago an experiment was carried out

which had involved the making of a series of tape-recordings of some

ten speakers, recordings of various types o! speech, ranging from

isolated vowels to ordinary Qonversations, the Institute of Phonetic

Sciences in Amsterdam happened to be in possession of a corpus o!

material., which was ready to hand for comparison of a number of forms

o:f speech.

For a first investigation I confined myself to the vowels spoken by

two informanta of the group of ten mentioned above, who, according to

the opinion of 200 listeners in a previous experiment, had been

considered to have the 'best' and the 'worst' pronunciation.

Of these two informants four types of speech per speaker were

compared, namely:

1) vowels spoken in isolation ( 2 x 12 vowels)

2) vowels spoken in isolated words ( 5 x 12 vowels)

3) vowels in stressed position in a text read aJ.oud (10 x 12 vowels)

4) vowel.a in unstressed position in a text, read aloud. '10 x 12 vowels)

The following can be added about the four types of speech.

ad 1) The twelve vowels of Dutch were pronounced twice in random order

by both speakers.

ad 2) Four series of isolated words were read from separate cards in

random order. The words were of the CVO type, viz. b-t, b-r, � and

Page 2: by J. - UvA · combinations, except for tilose cases where a nonsense combination would have pe ri the. result_, in which case a E was substitut d by a ' a by a Rt and a l by a -The

- .50 -

� combinations, excep t for tilose cases where a nonsense

combination would have pe�ri the. result_, in which case a E was

substitut�d by a �' a � by a Rt and a l by a �-The fact that £ offers no problems in formant measuring has

been gone into in the account of a previous experiment.

(Koopmans - van Beinum, 1969). The fifth series of isolated

words were produced by the i.nfo;cmaots by way of naming objects

depicted on cards shown in random order.

ad 3 and 4) A text read aloud can perhaps be considered to be an

intermediate stage towa..:cds normaJ. speech. The investigator has

the added advantage tnat the intention of the speaker is· not open to doubt. The text for reading was a simple, rather duli

story in the A.NWB (Automobile Club) '°'uarterly about a cycling

trip in Denmark. This text was considered to be suitable as the contents were thought to preclude any strong emotions or other

reactions of the reader , which might have influenced the

investigation in any way.

The dichotomy between vowels in stres sed and in unstressed

position was actuateQ by an i�vestigation by the late Pierre

Delattre ( 1969), who showed vowel reduction in :four languages if vowels in strei:;:;;ed position were compared to vowels in un.:.

stressed pos:i tion, (e.g. in Ge·:rma.n · Ma!-1.i"er · - manierier·t.

The criterion for 1::;tres.sed1 in the texts read aloud was in the

first place word accent, but a number of syllables marked as

stressed in words were dropped owing to the influence of the

sentence stress l)attern. 'I'he text of the 1 bes t ' Dpeaker was

taken as the star ting point a..�d was judged by two listeners who came practically ·to the srune conclusions. In order to keep the

materiaJ. as identical. as possible no attention was pai<i to the

fact that in some cases the stress pattern of the two speakers

was not quite identical.. Although it had been the intention to

measur e ten examples of stressed and of unstress ed position per

vowei, this proved impossible in the case of four vowels : the

[ p ] absent throughout the text, the [ u J appeared stressed

five times and four times unstressed , the [ � ] apFeared

stressed twice and twice unstressed, and the [ e J appeared

stressed five times and four times in unstressed position.

The outcome for these vowels is slightly less reliable than .for

the rest of the vowels.

Page 3: by J. - UvA · combinations, except for tilose cases where a nonsense combination would have pe ri the. result_, in which case a E was substitut d by a ' a by a Rt and a l by a -The

- 51 -

In order to elucidate matters the phonetic �otation of the vowels

with the key-words as used in this account follows below:

[ u J in boe t (approximately ti1e vowel of English book ) repair

( 0 J :Ln � (approximately tile vowel of J:'rench � ) boat

[ 0 J in bot (approximately the vowel of English � ) blunt

[ Ei ] i.n � j(a:pprox.imately the vowel of French li-.LS ) bath

[ a J in � (approximately the vowel of Ji'rench �rave ) profit

[ y J in � (approximately tL.e vowel of .ii'rench nu.l.r ) goal

[ /; J in beuk (approximately tht! vowel of French peu ) beech

[ a J in buk ( a.p proxirr;a tely the schwa of Englich amber ) bend

[ i J in bi et ( aiiproxirr!o. tely the vowel of Er en eh :::�:L ) beetroot

[ l J in ill ( approxi.rnately the vowel o.i· i:;np;lish li}J ) bit

[ e J in � (approximately the vowal of l''rench ne ) bite ,

[ € ] in bet (approximately the vowel of }�.c.6lid1 �:;et ) 'batt;e 1

j) .Final d in Dutch i.s devo:Lced.

The formant frequeuce3 of a..Ll t.t.c vo·�;.:;ls ;,rnd their du.ration were mea;;;u:r·ed (l.irectly i:rom t:·.1;: 6G.;ir.cicurve wi�h fa t(;.Lpe-

l'igure 1.

(\...., /\ ,.,P-�- I . \ ... / . \- . /') - �·

-..; '. / \...J ---�j v

.Part of t.he coundcu1·ve wi. '.;;.!J. the zero-cro.s.;;ings of li'1 <wd F2 indicated.

sting

dab

:For ev ery vowel oue mt:;.:t_;':�1ring r)oiJJ.t wa�; c nc;;.;en, viz.. in the middle of

Page 4: by J. - UvA · combinations, except for tilose cases where a nonsense combination would have pe ri the. result_, in which case a E was substitut d by a ' a by a Rt and a l by a -The

- .52 -

the vowel. The fact that only one measuring point per vowel was used

is one of the weaknesses of the method used here; formant shifts in

diphtongized vowels are practically disregarded. The simplicity of

data proce::>sing with one measuring point only decided the issue

however.

The duration of each vowel was measured in milliseconds- llhe starting­

poillt of the vowel was considered to be there where the formant pattern

of the vowel. was clearly visible for the first time and the end of the

vowel was taken to be the point where the pertaining formant pattern

disappeared. If the vowel was adjacent to a voiced consonant tM: vowel

proper was isolated with the aid of the gates o! the rotating reproduc­

ing head. The judgement was made by ear, but also by eye with the a.id

of the screen of the oscilloscope. lf the vowel preceded a.r.i. [r ] the

[e)-like part, forming the transition between vowel and [r) was sub­

tracted from the duration of the vowel.

The results of the measurements were proceesed by a IBM 1130 computer

and are rendered in figures 2 to 5.

Formant frequencies�

Figures 2 and 3 tihow the distribution of the vowels in the formant

field in the four types of speech investigated. The ellip.:.>es indicate

the areas embracing 50% of t11e realizations if a normal oi-variate

distribution is assumed. The size of the ellipses is defined by the

size of the standard deviations.

On comparing the four phases of ::>peech the following can be ascertain­

ed:

1) Both speakers make progressively smaller contrasts when comparing

phases one to four;

2) Both speakers show formant reduction in the direction of [a); 3) The amount of reduction di±'fers for the two informants; especially

for the unstressed positions a greater reduction can be seen for

the speaker who articulated 'worse';

4) For both speakers the formant triangle remains quite distinct in

all four phases; the relative system of contrasts is left in tact

in all cases.

Page 5: by J. - UvA · combinations, except for tilose cases where a nonsense combination would have pe ri the. result_, in which case a E was substitut d by a ' a by a Rt and a l by a -The

- 53 -

Duration.

ln figures 4 and 5 the mean values for the duration of the vowels in

the four forms of speech investigated are rendered by graphs;

separately for ei.ther speak.er.

The order in which the vowels have been put was determined by the mean

values of the vowels, as .spoKen by the "best11 speaker, for the isolated

vowels, in such a way that the duration decreases from left to right,

with one exception, the order of [ € J and [ i J were reversed so that

the so-called short vowels remained one group.

When comparing th� four phases of speech we can establish the following:

1) Both speakers make their vowels progressively shorter from phases one

to four;

2) The difference between 11long" and rrshort11 vowels i.n phase one and two

is considerably greater for the better speaker than for the "worse"

articulating speaker;

3) For both speakers the difference in dllration between '1longrr and

tts.hort" vowels disappears comp.ietely wilen used in u.nstres.oed position

(phase 4); I+) l t looks as if the so-called vowels of intE:rmediate ler.i.e-th [ y J

[ u J and [ i J do not differ in d11rati.on, when .'llca'-:lu.red in connected

1;;peech, from Uie 11short11 vuwtJls. Unfortu:1ately the material for [ y ] and [ u J in triis exp�riment wab rather lin�ted.

'l'1w ma.teritl used w� of too limited a ri.i.;i.ture and rn.or·e informants will

have to be used for a similar ex1)e:.ri.rn.•�lit. besi.des, not only ·texts read

aloud will be sufficient mo.tor.ial, but ordina.ry evt:;-...·y-d1�y conversati.ons

will have to forru the co:cpu�-> of an iI1vestit;atj.<.rn.

Page 6: by J. - UvA · combinations, except for tilose cases where a nonsense combination would have pe ri the. result_, in which case a E was substitut d by a ' a by a Rt and a l by a -The

300

�00

:::. ("\ ,,.--. · u· . _, -.J

600

700

BOO

900

N I � lJ_

0 l.fl �

11 :V,, v w�j

'

§ "'""'

0

KJ "'""'

I"' - ....

�' \ .f .()(. \ t ·to(. � � ? .r+f

0 0 IJ1 �

c •

� 0 0

" 0 � ru

y �

• • .f._. �y "y , ' ... �

0

KJ ru

0 0 lf1 ru --

. . L�l

,.,. -©--:-.. .. � ....... ' l ......

' . l ..... .

-

0 0 U1 0 " 0 ru (TJ

F2 HZ

a 4\

��"":'···) ' .....

,,, -1e ,- �

; ' '

' �

. -

��

c.-:---> 0

I"'"- .... I \

\' I { e �.,-7,1 ,�>''

I \ .L' i E «.t�·,.. ... ,\. \ff)v v E.._._��

i'i.g. 2 Eli.ipses of the formant aJ.'eas of the 11better11 arti.culatinc; S.f:r:�ak.er embracing 5070 of the reaJ.iz.J.tiom; assuming a normal bi-variate dis­tribution, given for every vowel in four types of Bpeech.

isolated vowels ••••••vowels in isolated words I \ l ' I vowels in stressed posi tiou in a text,

:'ead aloud vowels in unstressed pooition in a text, read aloud

\J1 +-

Page 7: by J. - UvA · combinations, except for tilose cases where a nonsense combination would have pe ri the. result_, in which case a E was substitut d by a ' a by a Rt and a l by a -The

300

400

SOO

500

700

800 .

900

f'j I � LL.

� 0 0 0 � 0 0 0 0 0 0 U1 0 0 PJ 0 U1 0 "" 0 ru Ll1 " 0 IJl "" � � tj tj � ru ru ru ru

F2 HZ u�� �� y .. ::�c:>'I _ .... ..,, .� . ._,,

---- ,,-1 3'

#. '

.�-+{� .J .,.,-.7',. .... 1, '

J .f' I '

- , -r �

' � ,, - - - k . , ,,, - ... -�.;..

'

/ ' '

/ \ , I

V € \

�°" _,, 2 I J

... I / • i

a

\ ....

,-

__ ..J' � -4-f;��.

� la_; w....,.:f' � a�

Q Fig. 3 Ellipses of the formant areas of the "worse"

articulating speak.er embracing 500fo of the realizations assuming a normal bi-variate dis­tribution, given for every voweJ. in four types of speech.

isolated words • • • •• voweJ.s in isolated words I' 'I I vowels in streosed position in a text,

i•ead aloud ----- vowels in unstressed position in a text,

read aloud

Page 8: by J. - UvA · combinations, except for tilose cases where a nonsense combination would have pe ri the. result_, in which case a E was substitut d by a ' a by a Rt and a l by a -The

msec

l'to

310

uo

2.••

/to

f'lo

11•

/00

lo

- 56 -

Fig. 4 Mean values of the vowel duration in four types of speech of the "better11 articulating speaker.

• •isolated vowels

• • ••• vowels in isolated words

•t '''•vowels in stressed position in a text, read aloud

e----• vowels in unstressed position in a text , read a.loud

...__ ..... � ... ----- .....

... .... ....,,.

.

a 0 v l I

Page 9: by J. - UvA · combinations, except for tilose cases where a nonsense combination would have pe ri the. result_, in which case a E was substitut d by a ' a by a Rt and a l by a -The

340

°!)()O

/�O

160 /"IO

/l.O

/oo

- 57 ..

Fig. 5 Mean values of the vowel duration in four types of speech of the 11worse" a.r ticu.lating speake:t·.

• •isolated vowels

••••••vowels in isolated words

., 11 'I• vowels in stres::;ed :posit.ion in a text, read aloud

-----•vowels in unstre13sed pooition in a text, read aloud

0 e. y . (. I

Page 10: by J. - UvA · combinations, except for tilose cases where a nonsense combination would have pe ri the. result_, in which case a E was substitut d by a ' a by a Rt and a l by a -The

- 58 -

References:

Delattre, �ierre, (1969). 'An Acoustic and Articulatory �tudy of Vowel

Heduction in Four Languages', in IRAL, Vol VIl/4, November, pp 295-325

Koopmans-van Beinum, F.J. (1969). 'Nog meer fonetische �ekerheden', in

De Nieuwe Taalgids, 62 jrg, pp 245-2)0