Article.6.Tim

download Article.6.Tim

of 24

Transcript of Article.6.Tim

  • 7/28/2019 Article.6.Tim

    1/24

    Tim Dalby 5

    Schema Theory: Getting into the Minds

    of your Learners

    Tim Dalby

    Jeonju University

    Abstract

    Trying to define schema is like trying to catch a wisp of smoke.It defies iron-cast definition, yet remains central to the process of reading.This short paper attempts to trace the origins of schema theory from

    its earliest beginnings, through the dark days of behaviorism to anenlightened view of reading as an interactive process. In addition, I

    will exemplify the major elements of schema theory and show howthey have been put into practical use for the benefit of second languagelearners in terms of useful pre- while- and post-reading activities.

    Keywords: Schema theory, Reading, Course books, Extensive reading.I . An Old Idea?

    Schema theory is a not new idea, having origins with Plato and

    the Greek philosophy of ideal types. Kant named and developed the

    concept which was later taken up by Bartlett in his 1932 study to

    observe the effect of culture on the recall of text. Unfortunately at

    this time, behaviourism was in its ascendancy and schema theory took

    a back seat for over forty years (Ajideh, 2003, p. 3). As the influence

    of behaviourism waned, various researchers began to look at reading

    as an interactive process which led to an examination of the role of

    individual background knowledge on the readers comprehension ofa text. Swales defines background knowledge as made up of our assimi-

    lated direct experiences of life and its manifold activities, and our assimi-

  • 7/28/2019 Article.6.Tim

    2/24

    6 Schema Theory: Getting into the Minds of your Learners

    lated verbal experiences and encounters (1990, p. 83).Aspects of this background knowledge have been variously described

    as scripts (Abelson 1976, cited in Brown & Yule, 1983 p. 241), frames

    (Minsky 1975, cited in Yule, 1983, p. 238), scenarios (Sanford & Garrod

    1981, cited in Yule, 1983, p. 245) or simply as background knowledge

    (Grabe & Stoller, 2002, p. 27), each having a slightly different focus,

    function or purpose. For this discussion, I will use the term schema

    to encompass all others. Any definition is unlikely to capture the full

    extent and implication of schema theory. Instead, we must rely onexemplification, judgement and experience to help us understand how

    what we know interacts with what is written to create what is

    understood.When describing types of schemata, Carrell & Eisterhold build upon

    previous arguments to differentiate formal schemata from content sche-

    mata (1983, p. 560). The former cover linguistic elements such as

    language, vocabulary and genre, the latter deal with knowledge of a

    subject, culture and the world. In the next section, these two aspects

    will be discussed in terms of their strengths and weaknesses by examining

    selected research studies and other evidence currently available. Then,

    I will attempt to highlight and provide examples of the contribution

    schema theory has made to the practice of teaching reading in a foreign

    language.

    II. Components of Schema Theory

    2.1 Content Schemata

    When describing the concept of a content schema, Alderson further

    distinguishes between three components: subject knowledge; knowledge

    of the world; and cultural knowledge (2000, p. 43-46). It is to these

    three aspects that I now turn.2.2 Subject Knowledge

    Schema theory suggests that we will comprehend a text better if

    we have prior knowledge of the subject being discussed. This is exempli-

  • 7/28/2019 Article.6.Tim

    3/24

    Tim Dalby 7

    fied by using highly technical texts such as the one below (Zurfluhet al., 2008):

    Phenylketonuria (PKU) is an autosomal recessive inborn error of metabo-

    lism resulting from a deficiency of phenylalanine hydroxylase (PAH;

    612349), an enzyme that catalyzes the hydroxylation of phenylalanine

    to tyrosine, the rate-limiting step in phenylalanine catabolism. If un-

    diagnosed and untreated, phenylketonuria can result in impaired postnatal

    cognitive development resulting from a neurotoxic effect of

    hyperphenylalaninemia.

    Someone with no knowledge of phenylketonuria would be unlikely

    to have pragmatic competence of the passage and would have difficulty

    pronouncing some of the more technical terms correctly.

    Aside from subject knowledge, studies by Rumelhart & Bransford

    have shown that when titles or other cues are removed from a text,

    it becomes difficult to comprehend (Alderson, 2000, p. 43). An example

    from R.C. Anderson et al. (1977, cited in Yule, 1996:148) is reproducedbelow:

    Rocky slowly got up from the mat, planning his escape. He hesitated

    a moment and thought. Things were not going well. What bothered

    him most was being held, especially since the charge against him had

    been weak. He considered his present situation. The lock that held

    him was strong, but he thought he could break it.

    When shown to a group of English teachers in Korea, this passagewas interpreted to be about either wrestling or being held in a police

    cell, but neither explanation was completely satisfactory to the teachers.

    Although horoscopes are a notable example of ambiguously written

    texts, as Sadoski et al. point out, rarely is a real text this ambiguous

    (1991:470).

    According to schema theory, when faced with a text, we activate

    the appropriate schema and then add information to it (Carrell &

    Eisterhold, 1983, p. 557). However, as Alderson suggests, schema theorydoes not adequately explain how new information is acquired when

    no schema already exists (2000, p. 44).

  • 7/28/2019 Article.6.Tim

    4/24

    8 Schema Theory: Getting into the Minds of your Learners

    Studies which support the idea of a subject schemas importanceto reading include that of Alderson & Urquhart who reported in 1985

    that students with specialist subject knowledge achieved higher scores

    in reading tests than students without specialist knowledge. They also

    found that higher linguistic proficiency could, to a point, make up

    for a lack of subject knowledge and vice versa (Alderson, 2000, p.

    44). Subject knowledge is also transferrable from L1 to L2 reading

    (Hayashi, 1999, p. 122).

    However, a text needs to be sufficiently specialised as shownin a study which suggests caution for the predictive power of subject

    knowledge (Pateraki, 1997). Of course, where the line exists between

    sufficiently and insufficiently specialised requires more study. Carver

    points out that the weakness of schema theory is that it is only relevant

    for specialised texts which require specialised study - not texts used

    in normal, everyday reading (1992, p. 173).2.3 Knowledge of the World

    Hoey describes schema as a way of organising knowledge and experi-

    ence in the brain. He suggests that when one aspect of a schema is

    activated, the whole schema becomes available to the reader and, in

    turn, affects the interpretation of the text (Hoey, 2001, p. 121). Alderson

    (2000, p. 45) cites this example from Rumelhart, (1985, p. 267): The

    policeman held up his hand and the car stopped.

    Using our traffic officer schema, we usually dont attribute any

    superhuman powers to this policeman. Similarly, the sentence, I wentto the restaurant last night. as exemplified by Cook (1997, p. 86)

    would usually be unremarkable as we would understand that a person

    went to a restaurant, sat down, ordered food, ate, paid and then left.

    Of course, what we imagine happened and what actually happened

    may be entirely different. One representation of the restaurant might

    be a cozy, family-run Italian, another might be a fast-food chain.

    Psychologists call this phenomenon filling in perception and it has

    to do with the way our brains store experiences - not as a whole but

    instead as main ideas which can later be reweaved, rather than recalled

    (Gilbert, 2006, p. 79).

  • 7/28/2019 Article.6.Tim

    5/24

    Tim Dalby 9

    However, a schema remains active until textual or linguistic cuesshow that the schema needs to be changed. In taking an illustrative

    example by Sanford & Garrod (1981) cited in Yule (1996, p. 146-7)

    and reproduced below, we can see the processes that go on as we

    fill in the gaps of the text and interact with it. As you read each sentence,

    try to imagine the situation and then see how it changes as you read

    each subsequent sentence.

    John was on his way to school last Friday.He was really worried about the math lesson.

    Last week he had been unable to control the class.

    It was unfair of the math teacher to leave him in charge.

    After all, it is not a normal part of a janitors duties.

    This is an artificial construction of reading, but does provide an

    insight into how we build information into a sentence based on our

    expectations of what normal is (1996, p. 147).

    However, Ghadessy suggests that vocabulary knowledge is the im-

    portant factor in understanding texts such as these and that schema

    theory is nothing more than a theory of text redundancy (1983, p.

    377). Indeed, as we read through the story of John, the introduction

    of certain words changes our mental image. The role of vocabulary

    is explored in more detail below.2.4 Cultural Knowledge

    Schema theory holds strongest in the area of background cultural

    knowledge. Bartlett was one of the first to write about the effect of

    cultural norms on the recall of information. In his 1932 study, British

    subjects were asked to recall details of The War of the Ghosts, a

    North American Indian folktale, up to a year after first reading. In

    each instance, the recall added or omitted information so that the story

    conformed to the tellers cultural norms (Alderson, 2000, p. 45). Similarly,

    Steffenson & Joag-Devs 1984 study identified the effects of cultural

    schema. They used an American and Indian wedding text to be readby a group of Americans and a group of Indians. As expected, the

  • 7/28/2019 Article.6.Tim

    6/24

    10 Schema Theory: Getting into the Minds of your Learners

    Indian group recalled more ideas from the Indian text than the Americanreaders and vice versa. Also, as in Bartletts study, each group added

    to or modified the foreign culture text so that it conformed more

    closely to their own notions of a wedding. Additionally each group

    added new details to their own culturally-familiar texts (Steffenson

    & Joag-Dev, 1984).

    The implication here for teaching is to ensure that cultural context

    is provided to help students overcome problems of comprehension.

    Even a small amount of cultural instruction can help (Gatbonton &Tucker, 1971, cited in Steffenson & Joag-Dev, 1984, p. 52) One interesting

    study in Turkey created a nativized version of a typical American story

    The Girls in their Summer Dresses by replacing character names with

    Turkish names, New York with Canakkale, and by converting conceptual

    cues, so that eating steak was replaced with eating fish (Razi, 2004,

    p. 285). The study reported a significant increase in comprehension

    due to increased familiarity with the culture (Razi, 2004, p. 287).

    However, a similar study by Alptekin suggests that while a nativized

    text increased students ability to make inferences, it did not make

    up for their lack of linguistic skill (2006, p. 502). Indeed, even Bartletts

    original findings have been questioned as the type of recall changes

    depending on the design of the task (Alba & Hasher, 1983, p. 214).2.5 Formal Schemata

    When describing the concept of a formal schema, Alderson dis-

    tinguishes between language knowledge, genre knowledge and meta-linguistic knowledge (2000, p. 34-43). These three aspects will be dis-

    cussed below.

    Language knowledge

    Aebersold & Field describe a linguistic schema as one which involves

    bottom-up processing skills such as decoding words and organising

    grammatical structure (1997, p. 17). Linguistic ability will depend on

    a persons age, experience, vocabulary size and knowledge of textualconventions: the more linguistic knowledge a person has, the quicker

  • 7/28/2019 Article.6.Tim

    7/24

    Tim Dalby 11

    they are able to process a given text (Alderson, 2000, p. 34). In supportof schema theory, Cooper distinguished between practised and un-

    practised readers and found that students who had better knowledge

    of vocabulary and the syntactic relationships between words were better

    able to comprehend a text. He also showed that practised readers could

    make better use of linking words and linguistic cues in texts (1984,

    p. 133).

    Having an adequate vocabulary for reading is essential. For second

    language learners, reading an unsimplified, academic text requires aminimum vocabulary of around 3000 headwords as well as effective

    strategies for dealing with unknown words (Nation, 1990, p. 116).

    Hazenberg & Hulstijn (1996, p. 145) found that students wishing to

    study at a Dutch university required a vocabulary of around 10,000

    words, much higher than previously thought. According to Nation (2001),

    learners need to know around 98% of a text to be able to understand

    it, which seems to be the agreed level for effective comprehension

    (Read, 2000, p. 83).

    Vocabulary size isnt everything though. There are several elements

    that are necessary to adequately know a word (Nation, 1990, p. 31)

    and this becomes very apparent when dealing with polysemes and

    homonyms. As Cook (1997, p. 86) points out:

    Schema theory can...explain how we choose unconsciously between

    homonyms. We are unlikely to assume the sentence The King put

    his seal on the letter. to be about an aquatic mammal, because that

    is not the kind of seal in most king schemas.

    However, whether this is schema theory in action or merely a result

    of good vocabulary teaching is questionable. A 1998 study by Leffa

    suggests that collocation knowledge is more effective than schema activa-

    tion 94% of the time.

    As previously mentioned, knowledge of syntactic structures can help

    to overcome a lack of subject knowledge. Berman, in a study with

    Hebrew-speaking English learners, showed that her students had diffi-

    culties in English when the structure of a sentence differed from that

    encountered in Hebrew (1984, p. 148). Particular difficulties occurred

  • 7/28/2019 Article.6.Tim

    8/24

    12 Schema Theory: Getting into the Minds of your Learners

    when substantial information occurs before the main verb and whenadverbial phrases are placed before the main clause (Berman, 1984,

    p. 151). Shiotsu & Weir even suggest that knowledge of syntax, rather

    than vocabulary, may be more predictive of reading proficiency than

    previously thought (2007, p. 123).

    However, knowledge of some syntactic structures is not enough.

    For example, knowledge of the passive construction alone is not a

    good indicator of performance when reading a passive-voice-laden scien-

    tific text (Alderson, 2000, p. 37).A final element of a linguistic schema is the ability one brings

    from reading in a first language to reading in a second. It had been

    assumed that readers needed to be able to read well in their first language

    in order to read well in a second language (Alderson, 2000, p. 38).

    Instead, several studies have shown that second language knowledge,

    rather than first language reading skill, is a far better predictor of second

    language reading skill. Additionally, there exists a linguistic threshold

    beyond which a student must pass before first language reading skills

    start to make an impact on second language reading. The threshold

    is not a fixed line, but instead varies depending on how demanding

    the task of reading is (Alderson, 2000, p. 39).Genre knowledge

    Carrell suggests that a formal schema exists to identify and recognise

    textual organisation and rhetorical structures (1984, p. 341). Alderson

    refers to the same idea as knowledge of genre (2000, p. 39) andTribble provides the example that we can recognise a letter of rejection

    within the first couple of lines of text (1997, p. 35). Our knowledge

    of genre helps us better predict what we are going to read about, how

    it will be organised and even the kind of vocabulary and syntactic

    structures we are likely to encounter (Harmer, 2001, p. 200).

    Alderson, however, is more cautious and points out that there has

    been very little research into this specific area. What has been researched,

    by Mandler (1978) and Carrell (1981), has tended to confuse text with

    schemata (cited in Alderson, 2000, p. 40). In these studies respectively,

    first and second language students comprehension of a text was affected

  • 7/28/2019 Article.6.Tim

    9/24

    Tim Dalby 13

    when stories were presented that did not conform to the normal storyschema.Metalinguistic knowledge and metacognition

    Knowledge of how a language is structured is unlikely to lead to

    an improvement in reading comprehension. A study by Alderson et

    al. found that linguistic knowledge and metalinguistic knowledge are

    separate abilities, implying that comprehension is possible without meta-

    linguistic knowledge. (1997, cited in Alderson, 2000, p. 42).

    Casanave argues that strategy schemata are as important as content

    and formal schemata (cited in Auerbach & Paxton, 1997, p. 241). Studies

    have supported this and shown that knowledge of reading strategies

    and the ability to control them (turning them into skills, using Grabe

    & Stollers definition (2002, p. 15)) have a marked effect on compre-

    hension (Auerbach & Paxton, 1997). Alderson agrees suggesting that

    poor readers have no knowledge of strategy use (2000, p. 41).

    I I I. Why is Schema Theory Useful?

    Increased understanding

    It could be argued that schema theory has contributed to the under-

    standing of second language reading through the vast amount of research

    it has generated, either to support or discredit it (Alba & Hasher, 1983,

    p. 224). This research has led to an increased knowledge about theprocesses involved in reading. Before schema theorys resurgence, read-

    ing was either a top-down or bottom-up process. Top-down processes

    stem from Goodmans psycholinguistic guessing game model, and

    focus on skills such as inference and prediction (Eskey, 1988, p. 93).

    Bottom-up models see reading as a decoding exercise in which words

    and syntax are processed to create a mental image of the message

    of the text (Grabe & Stoller, 2002, p. 32). Instead, it is now generally

    agreed that reading is interactive, both in the sense of an interactionbetween the top-down and bottom-up processes of reading (Grabe, 1991,

    p. 383) and in the sense that meaning is constructed through a readers

  • 7/28/2019 Article.6.Tim

    10/24

    14 Schema Theory: Getting into the Minds of your Learners

    interaction with the text. In earlier years reading a text was seen aslike opening a box: all the reader had to do was extract the meaning

    from the text (Wallace, 2001, p. 22).

    The movements of narrow and extensive reading trace their roots

    to schema theory. Carrell (1984, p. 339) argues that narrow reading

    can help to build up content knowledge by reading in specific subjects.

    Extensive reading works on the same idea, but instead encourages readers

    to become more familiar with linguistic conventions, vocabulary and

    intertextuality. (Stott, 2001, [online]).Schema theory has also contributed to test design. The implication

    that subject knowledge may predict reading success is something that

    test designers need to consider when choosing passages for high stakes

    tests such as IELTS. Wallace argued strongly that removing the read-

    ing-writing link would disadvantage some students from certain cultures

    unfamiliar with particular writing topics (1997, p. 371). Content knowl-

    edge of a test can be internal, or already held by the candidate, or

    external, that is provided by the test. A test designer can only make

    reasonable assumptions about what a candidate is likely to know before

    taking a test (OSullivan, 2005, p. 14) and the choice of current and

    popular topics such as the environment seems to make sense in terms

    of fairness and of avoiding content bias.

    In the classroom, schema theory has influenced the way a typical

    reading lesson is approached, giving increased emphasis to the building

    of interest and accessing previous knowledge (Sheridan, 1978, p. 12).

    Instead of simply open your books to page 35 and read, teachers

    need to be more aware of their students interests when selecting readingmaterials (Pearson-Casanave, 1984, p. 335). As discussed in above,

    if there is a large gap between a readers existing schema and knowledge

    required to read, then effective reading cannot take place as in figure

    1, below.

    FIGURE 1

    Too much distance between what a reader knows and what a reader

    needs to know

  • 7/28/2019 Article.6.Tim

    11/24

    Tim Dalby 15

    Ideally, a teacher will choose materials where there is some overlap

    between a readers schema and the knowledge required to comprehend

    the text as in figure 2, below.

    FIGURE 2

    Just right: a teacher can build on what a reader already knows and

    so make reading interesting.

    Al-Issa provides a useful list of questions that teachers should ask

    when selecting texts, which is reproduced below (2006, p. 45):

    1. Will my students be interested in reading such materials?

    2. Will these materials be relevant to my students English proficiency

    levels?3. What content knowledge is to be extracted from these materials?

    4. Will these materials cause cultural conflicts in the classroom?

  • 7/28/2019 Article.6.Tim

    12/24

    16 Schema Theory: Getting into the Minds of your Learners

    5. How can I motivate my students and involve them in reading suchmaterials?

    6. What kinds of pre-reading, reading, and post-reading activities and

    materials can be designed to increase my students understanding

    of these materials?

    7. Do the reading materials provide students with sufficient background

    information about the content of the text?

    8. How much time and freedom am I giving my students to exercise

    their understandings of the materials?

    9. Am I being sensitive to my students hidden comprehension prob-

    lems?

    10. Am I helping my students become more aware of the fact that

    reading is a highly interactive process?

    11. Are my students changing their attitudes about reading?

    12. Am I allowing my students to become independent, self-directed

    readers?

    It is now more typical for teachers to engage their students in sche-

    ma-activating activities before, during and after the reading process

    in order to build a framework for reading. In the following sections,

    I will describe some of the activities that fall into these three areas,

    though it is by no means an exhaustive review.

    Pre-reading activities

    Murtagh argues that inducing appropriate schemata through suitable

    pre-reading activities is likely to be extremely beneficial (1989, p.

    102, cited in N. Anderson, 1999, p. 12). Evidence to support this viewwas collected by Ajideh and shows how students were able to move

    from text-bound interpretations to more holistic views of any given

    reading material (2003, p. 11-12). This stage is important as it helps

    students focus on the task at hand, encourages predictions to be made

    and tested, and provides for gaps in background knowledge to be identified

    and filled in. Thus the idea of pre-teaching vocabulary now extends

    to pre-teaching subject details, cultural aspects of a text, and providing

    context as part of the framework of reading (Ajideh, 2003, p. 6). All

    this provides a richer experience for teacher and student alike.

  • 7/28/2019 Article.6.Tim

    13/24

    Tim Dalby 17

    FIGURE 3

    A typical pre-reading activity (Craven, 2003, p. 36)

  • 7/28/2019 Article.6.Tim

    14/24

    18 Schema Theory: Getting into the Minds of your Learners

    FIGURE 4

    An example of a research activity (Haugnes, & Maher, 2004, p. 22-23)

  • 7/28/2019 Article.6.Tim

    15/24

    Tim Dalby 19

    Pre-reading activities can be as simple as having a discussion abouta certain topic, which Anderson suggests can help students recall knowl-

    edge they did not realise they had (1999, p. 14). A typical discussion

    activity can be seen below in Figure 3. Alternatively, students can

    be given a writing exercise as a way to research and learn about a

    topic, and become familiar with genre and vocabulary as well as with

    the main ideas that may be related to a topic. An example of this

    kind of exercise can be seen below in Figure 4.

    Semantic mapping is another way to activate a schema. Semanticmapping is much like brainstorming (Anderson, 1999, p. 14), where

    concepts and ideas about a topic are collected and organized - possibly

    on the board. An example exercise is reproduced in Figure 5, below.

    FIGURE 5

    A semantic mapping exercise (Anderson, 1999, p. 15)

    This method can help counteract the possible negative effects of

    activating knowledge by challenging preconceived notions such as

    stereotypes. Sometimes our knowledge of a subject stops us from ad-

    equately comprehending a text. As Cook (1997, p. 86) suggests:

    In unfamiliar situations, attention to detail and a willingness to aban-don and change our schemas are still the hallmarks of a flexible and

    open mind.

  • 7/28/2019 Article.6.Tim

    16/24

    20 Schema Theory: Getting into the Minds of your Learners

    FIGURE 6

    A method for previewing (Mikulecky, 1990, p. 37)

  • 7/28/2019 Article.6.Tim

    17/24

    Tim Dalby 21

    FIGURE 7

    Prediction and discussion based on pictures (Craven, 2003, p. 56)

  • 7/28/2019 Article.6.Tim

    18/24

    22 Schema Theory: Getting into the Minds of your Learners

    It is also useful for the teacher to be provocative in asking questionsto get students thinking about and discussing the issues (N. Anderson,

    1999, p. 17). Previewing (see Figure 6, below) encourages students

    to predict the content of a text using visual cues such as titles, photos

    and even text organisation (see Figure 7, below).

    Students can also try to predict the structure of the text - and thus

    build up a mental portfolio of genres used in English. Swaffar emphasises

    the importance of being able to distinguish genres such as academic

    essays, plays, magazine fluff and recipes as an important step on theroad towards giving learners the skills they need to recognise the grammat-

    ical structures, stylistic markers and how ideas are developed in different

    texts (1991, cited in Ajideh, 2003, p. 8).

    While-reading activities

    An appreciation of schema theory has led to a better structure for

    comprehension questions. Pearson & Johnson argued that questions

    should be textually explicit, textually implicit (but still on the page)or schematically implicit (cited in Sheridan, 1978, p. 14-15). As discussed

    earlier, this has had an effect on high-stakes tests such as IELTS. It

    is now less common to see culturally-loaded questions exemplified

    in Steffenson & Joag-Devs study (1984, p. 51).

    Other activities generally take the form of work requiring students

    to read for different purposes. Typically, students first read for vocabulary

    meaning, then again to find main ideas, or skimming, and finally again

    to find details, or scanning. Grabe & Stoller cite, reading to learn,reading to integrate information and reading for general compre-

    hension as additional reasons to read (2002, p. 13-15) all of which

    can be practised.

    Good reading teachers can also use while-reading activities to build

    upon pre-reading activities so that learners check earlier predictions,

    identify key vocabulary and monitor the skills and strategies they are

    using while they read. Auerbach & Paxtons study (1997, p. 238) was

    based on the premise that:

    ...readers...can become aware of their existing strategies, expand their

  • 7/28/2019 Article.6.Tim

    19/24

    Tim Dalby 23

    repertoire of strategies, revise their conceptions of reading, and gaincontrol of strategy choices, enhancing comprehension and recall.

    As fluent reading concerns a combination of many different processing

    resources, it is important to work on them both individually and as

    processes that require careful cooperation (Grabe & Stoller, 2002, p.

    14).

    Post-reading activitiesPost-reading activities can take place as a discussion or debate on

    the text or the issues raised in it. Readers can be encouraged to describe

    what they got from the text or how they interpreted it. Alternatively,

    students could be asked to write comprehension questions for other

    students. This helps develop comprehension skills as well as encouraging

    a readers ownership of the text (Carrell, 1984, p. 337). Writing activities,

    such as producing a summary are also common and seek to encourage

    a deeper processing of the text and its various features (Grabe, 1991,p. 394).

    IV. When is a Theory not a Theory?

    Schema is a nebulous term (Alba & Hasher, 1983, p. 225) that

    defies rigorous definition much like the term culture. Much of the

    criticism of schema theory is levelled in this way. If it cannot be defined,

    then it cannot be tested and so conclusions cannot be drawn from

    it (Sadoski et al., 1991, p. 468). Maybe because of this lack of rigor,

    a vast amount of research has been undertaken to either prove or disprove

    schema theory. This research has led to interesting developments in

    reading research as well as propagating new theories that deal with

    more precisely-defined processes, such as Sadoskis dual coding theory

    (1991). In fact it seems that many of the components of schema theory

    can be explained in other terms or by other processes (Alba & Hasher,

    1983, p. 225). Additional arguments can be levelled at the nature ofthe bizarre texts used to support schema theory, the method of study

  • 7/28/2019 Article.6.Tim

    20/24

    24 Schema Theory: Getting into the Minds of your Learners

    (Sadoski et al., 1991, p. 469-470) and the reliance on recall as thecriterion of successful reading, which is not necessarily the same as

    comprehension.

    However, the intuitive appeal of schema theory and its influence

    on teaching practice has had a generally positive effect on reading

    instruction, leading to a more learner-centred approach which focuses

    on individual needs and celebrates the diversity of background

    experiences. The reading classroom is no doubt a better place thanks

    to the work of Plato, Kant, Bartlett and all the other researchers thathave contributed to establishing reading as an interaction between the

    reader and the text, rather than viewing the reader as a mere passive

    receiver of information.

    References

    Aebersold, J.A., & Field, M.L. (1997). From reader to reading teacher.

    Issues and strategies for second language classrooms. Cambridge:

    Cambridge University Press.

    Ajideh, P. (2003). Schema theory-based pre-reading tasks: A neglected

    essential in the ESL reading class. The Reading Matrix: An

    International Online Journal [online]. Available from: http://www.

    readingmatrix.com/articles/ajideh/article.pdf[accessed25/10/10].

    Alba, J.W., & Hasher, L. (1983). Is memory schematic? Psychological

    Bulletin 93(2), 203-231.

    Alderson, C. (2000). Assessing reading. Cambridge: CambridgeUniversity Press.

    Al-Issa, A. (2006). Schema theory and L2 reading comprehension:

    Implications for teaching. Journal of College Teaching & Learning

    3(7), 41-48. [online]. Available from http://www.cluteinstitute

    -onlinejournals.com/PDFs/2006100.pdf[accessed25/10/10].

    Alptekin, C. (2006). Cultural familiarity in inferential and literal

    comprehension in L2 reading. System, 34, 494-508.

    Anderson, N. (1999). Exploring second language reading: Issues andstrategies. Boston: Heinle & Heinle Publishers.

    Auerbach, E.R., & Paxton, D. (1997). Its Not the English Thing:

  • 7/28/2019 Article.6.Tim

    21/24

    Tim Dalby 25

    Bringing reading research into the ESL classroom. TESOL Quarterly,31(2), 237-261.

    Berman, R.A. (1984). Syntactic components of the foreign language

    reading process. In J.C. Alderson, & A.H. Urquhart, (Eds.) Reading

    in a Foreign Language. (pp.139-159) London: Longman.

    Brown, G. & Yule, G. (1983). Discourse analysis. Cambridge: Cambridge

    University Press.

    Carrell, P. & Eisterhold, J.C. (1983). Schema theory and ESL reading

    pedagogy. TESOL Quarterly 17(4), 553-573.Carrell, P.L. (1984). Schema theory and ESL reading: Classroom

    implications and applications. The Modern Language Journal 68(iv),

    332-343.

    Carver, R.P. (1992). Effect of prediction activities, prior knowledge,

    and text type upon the amount comprehended: using rauding theory

    to critique schema theory research. Reading Research Quarterly

    27(2), 165-174.

    Cook, G. (1997). Key concepts in ELT: Schemas. ELT Journal 51(1),

    86.

    Cooper, M. (1984). Linguistic competence of practised and unpractised

    non-native readers of English. In J.C. Alderson, & A.H. Urquhart,

    (Eds.) Reading in a Foreign Language. (pp.122-138) London :

    Longman.

    Craven, M. (2003). Extending reading keys. Oxford: Macmillan

    Publishers Ltd.

    Craven, M. (2003). Introducing reading keys. Oxford: Macmillan

    Publishers Ltd.Eskey, D.E. (1988). Holding in the bottom: An interactive approach

    to the language problems of second language readers. In Carrell,

    P.L., Devine, J. & Eskey, D.E. (Eds.) Interactive approaches to

    second language reading. (pp. 93-100). Cambridge: Cambridge

    University Press.

    Ghadessy, M. (1985). Comments on recent articles on schema theory.

    A reader reacts: Word knowledge and world knowledge. TESOL

    Quarterly 19(2), 375-382.Gilbert, D. (2006). Stumbling on happiness. New York: Knopf, Random

    House.

  • 7/28/2019 Article.6.Tim

    22/24

    26 Schema Theory: Getting into the Minds of your Learners

    Grabe, W. (1991). Current developments in second language readingresearch. TESOL Quarterly 25(3), 375-406.

    Grabe, W. & F. Stoller (2002). Teaching and researching reading,

    London: Pearson Education, Longman.

    Harmer, J. (2001). The practice of english language teaching3rd Edition.

    Harlow: Longman.

    Haugnes, N., & Maher, B. (2004). Northstar: Reading and writing,

    basic/low intermediate, second edition. White Plains, N.Y.: Pearson

    Education Inc.Hayashi, K. (1999). Reading strategies and extensive reading in EFL

    classes. RELC Journal 30(2), 114-132.

    Hazenberg, S., & Hulstijn, J.H. (1996). Defining a minimal receptive

    second-language vocabulary for non-native university students: An

    empirical investigation. Applied Linguistics 17(2), 145-163.

    Hoey, M. (2001). Textual interaction. An introduction to written discourse

    analysis. London: Routledge.

    Leffa, V.J. (1998). Textual constraints in L2 lexical disambiguation.

    System 26, 183-194.

    Mikulecky, B.S. (1990). A short course in teaching reading skills.

    Reading, MA: Addison-Wesley.

    Nation, P. (2001) Learning vocabulary in another language. Cambridge:

    Cambridge University Press.

    Nation, P. (1990) Teaching and learning vocabulary. Boston: Heinle

    and Heinle.

    OSullivan, B. (2005). An overview of test validity-linking the language

    testing module notes to current thinking. Reading, UK: TheUniversity of Reading.

    Pateraki, E.M. (1997). The Influence of background knowledge on

    reading comprehension. Unpublished MA TEFL dissertation, Centre

    for Applied Language Studies, Reading University.

    Pearson-Casanave, C.R. (1984). Communicative pre-reading activities:

    Schema theory in action. TESOL Quarterly 18(2), 334-336.

    Razi, S. (2004). The effects of cultural schema and reading activities

    on reading comprehension. In M. Singhal & J. Liontas (Eds.)Proceedings of the First International Online Conference on Second

    and Foreign Language Teaching and Research. September 25-26,

  • 7/28/2019 Article.6.Tim

    23/24

    Tim Dalby 27

    2004-Beyond Borders [online] Available from: http://www. readingmatrix.com/conference/pp/proceedings/razi.pdf[accessed25/10/10].

    Read, J. (2000). Assessing vocabulary. Cambridge: Cambridge University

    Press.

    Sadoski, M., Paivio, A., & Goetz, E.T. (1991). A critique of schema

    theory in reading and a dual coding alternative. Reading Research

    Quarterly 26(4), 463-484.

    Sheridan, E.M. (1978). A review of research on schema theory and

    its implications for reading instruction in secondary reading. [online]Available from:

    http://www.eric.ed.gov/PDFS/ED167947.pdf[accessed 25/10/2010].

    Shiotsu, T., & Weir, C.J. (2007). The relative significance of syntactic

    knowledge and vocabulary breadth in the prediction of reading

    comprehension test performance, Language Testing, 24(1), 99-128.

    Steffenson, M.S., & Joag-Dev, C. (1984). Cultural knowledge in reading.

    In Alderson, J.C. & Urquhart, A.H. (Eds.) Reading in a foreign

    language. (pp. 48-61). London: Longman.

    Stott, N. (2001). Helping ESL students become better readers: Schema

    theory applications and limitations. The Internet TESL Journal,

    VII (November) [online]. Available from: http://iteslj.org/Articles

    /Stott-Schema.html[accessed25/10/2010].

    Swales, J. M. (1990). Genre analysis. Cambridge: Cambridge University

    Press.

    Tribble, C. (1997). Writing. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Wallace, C. (1997). IELTS: Global implications of curriculum and

    materials design. ELT Journal 51(4), 370-373.Wallace, C. (2001). Reading. In R. Carter & D. Nunan (Eds.) The

    Cambridge guide to teaching english to speakers of other languages.

    Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Yule, G. (1996). The study of language, 2nd edition. Cambridge:

    Cambridge University Press.

    Zurfluh, M. R., Zschocke, J., Lindner, M., Feillet, F., Chery, C., Burlina,

    A., Stevens, R. C., Thony, B., & Blau, N. (2008). Molecular genetics

    of tetrahydrobiopterin-responsive phenylalanine hydroxylasedeficiency. Human Mutation. 29, 167-175.

  • 7/28/2019 Article.6.Tim

    24/24

    28 Schema Theory: Getting into the Minds of your Learners

    Tim DalbyJeonju University

    [email protected]

    Received: 2010-10-26

    Peer reviewed: 2010-11-24

    Accepted: 2010-12-14