1-s2.0-S0010440X15300717-main

download 1-s2.0-S0010440X15300717-main

of 27

Transcript of 1-s2.0-S0010440X15300717-main

  • 7/25/2019 1-s2.0-S0010440X15300717-main

    1/27

    Perceptions of social dominance through facial emotion expressions in

    euthymic patients with bipolar I disorder

    Sung Hwa Kim, Vin Ryu, Ra Yeon Ha, Su Jin Lee, Hyun-Sang Cho

    PII: S0010-440X(15)30071-7

    DOI: doi:10.1016/j.comppsych.2016.01.012

    Reference: YCOMP 51624

    To appear in: Comprehensive Psychiatry

    Please cite this article as: Kim Sung Hwa, Ryu Vin, Ha Ra Yeon, Lee Su Jin,Cho Hyun-Sang, Perceptions of social dominance through facial emotion expressionsin euthymic patients with bipolar I disorder, Comprehensive Psychiatry (2016), doi:10.1016/j.comppsych.2016.01.012

    This is a PDF file of an unedited manuscript that has been accepted for publication.As a service to our customers we are providing this early version of the manuscript.The manuscript will undergo copyediting, typesetting, and review of the resulting proofbefore it is published in its final form. Please note that during the production processerrors may be discovered which could affect the content, and all legal disclaimers thatapply to the journal pertain.

    http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.comppsych.2016.01.012http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.comppsych.2016.01.012http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.comppsych.2016.01.012http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.comppsych.2016.01.012
  • 7/25/2019 1-s2.0-S0010440X15300717-main

    2/27

    ACCEP

    TED

    MANUSCRIPT

    ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT

    Title: Perceptions of social dominance through facial emotion expressions in euthymic

    patients with bipolar I disorder

    Sung Hwa Kima,b; Vin Ryuc; Ra Yeon Had; Su Jin Leeb; Hyun-Sang Choa,b*

    aDepartment of Psychiatry, Yonsei University College of Medicine, Seoul, Republic of Korea

    bInstitute of Behavioral Science in Medicine, Yonsei University College of Medicine, Seoul,

    Republic of Korea

    cDepartment of Psychiatry, Seoul National Hospital, Seoul, Republic of Korea

    dDepartment of Psychiatry, Seoul Bukbu Hospital, Seoul, South Korea

    *Corresponding author

    Hyun-Sang Cho, MD, PhD

    Department of Psychiatry, College of Medicine, Yonsei University

    50 Yonsei-ro, Seodaemun-gu, Seoul 120-752, Republic of Korea

    Tel: +82.2.2228.1587, Fax: +82.2.313.0891

    Email: [email protected]

  • 7/25/2019 1-s2.0-S0010440X15300717-main

    3/27

    ACCEP

    TED

    MANUSCRIPT

    ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT

    Abstract

    The ability to accurately perceive dominance in the social hierarchy is important for

    successful social interactions. However, little is known about dominance perception of

    emotional stimuli in bipolar disorder. The aim of this study was to investigate the perception

    of social dominance in patients with bipolar I disorder in response to six facial emotional

    expressions. Participants included 35 euthymic patients and 45 healthy controls. Bipolar

    patients showed a lower perception of social dominance based on anger, disgust, fear, and

    neutral facial emotional expressions compared to healthy controls. A negative correlation was

    observed between motivation to pursue goals or residual manic symptoms and perceived

    dominance of negative facial emotions such as anger, disgust, and fear in bipolar patients.

    These results suggest that bipolar patients have an altered perception of social dominance that

    might result in poor interpersonal functioning. Training of appropriate dominance perception

    using various emotional stimuli may be helpful in improving social relationships for

    individuals with bipolar disorder.

    Keywords:Social dominance; Facial emotion; Bipolar disorder; Euthymia

  • 7/25/2019 1-s2.0-S0010440X15300717-main

    4/27

    ACCEP

    TED

    MANUSCRIPT

    ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT

    1. Introduction

    Interpersonal relationships may be divided into two systems: the social dominance system

    and the social inhibition system [1]. The function of the social dominance system is to

    recognize social hierarchy in order to successfully compete with people in a more dominant

    position [2]. The social hierarchy is important in interpersonal relationships in that it

    promotes knowledge of ones own place in the hierarchy and eventually it could bring

    appropriate social behavior through behavioral inhibition [3,4]. Recently, comprehensive

    studies of social hierarchy across species revealed that recognizing social hierarchy is

    associated with a neural network system that is distinct from other social cognition [5].

    In interpersonal relationships, social status can be inferred from multiple visual cues, such

    as facial features, posture, gender, age, and facial expressions [6-8]. Facial emotional stimuli

    are important because they provide information about the social hierarchy and regulate both

    the affective state and emotional behavior in response to stimuli [9]. For example, an

    individual who expresses approach-related emotions (e.g., anger) may increase perceived

    power, whereas an individual who shows inhibition-related emotions (e.g., sadness) may

    decrease perceived power [10]. In addition, facial expressions of happy and angry are seen as

    assertive, dominant, and controlling, whereas expressions of fear and sadness are perceived

    as submissive, incompetent, and in need of help [8]. In healthy humans, neutral expressions

    are considered as high social dominance because neutral faces provide the impression ofhaving the ability to handle the situation and reacting non-emotionally to the an event [8,10].

    Additionally, neutral emotions expressed by males are rated as more dominant than those

    expressed by females [8]. Facial features such as a square jaw, low brow position, and male

    gender may be perceived as a high social dominance [8,11,12]. When people are faced with

    dominant cues, they flexibly adopt either dominant or submissive behavior through

    comparing their self-status against other dominance cues [13]. People with a low sense of

  • 7/25/2019 1-s2.0-S0010440X15300717-main

    5/27

    ACCEP

    TED

    MANUSCRIPT

    ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT

    power are more attentive to dominant people and social threats, whereas individuals with a

    high sense of power are more attentive to rewarding aspects of social interactions [1,14].

    According to reports that examined the perception of social dominance in individuals with a

    social anxiety disorder, facial emotions like anger, contempt, disgust, and fear were

    associated with a negative view of dominance (e.g., harsh, threatening) and emotions like

    happiness were associated with a positive view of dominance (e.g., accepting) [15,16].

    Socially anxious individuals perceived themselves as low in the social hierarchy, engaged in

    negative social comparisons [17], and were particularly hypersensitive to dominance stimuli

    such as angry facial expressions [18]. Depressed people also felt defeated and tended to show

    submissive behaviors in response to dominant stimuli [19,20]. Patients with schizophrenia

    have impairments in various social cognitive processes including social perception with

    materials generating social cues [21]. Subjects with ventromedial prefrontal cortical lesions

    showed subtle, abnormal judgments of social dominance using static facial stimuli [22]. So

    abnormal dominance perception or recognition can be observed in specific psychiatric

    disorders and these impairments of dominance perception may lead to problems in

    interpersonal relationships or social adaptation.

    Bipolar disorder is characterized by alternating cycles of manic and depressive episodes,

    interspersed with euthymic periods. During a manic episode, patients show approach

    behaviors such as excessive goal pursuit with overly confidence and less regard toconsequences. Manic symptoms and impairments of social functioning may be caused, in part,

    by underlying deficits in social cognitive functions, such as the perception of potential danger

    cues [23]. During the euthymic period, bipolar patients still tend to think highly of themselves

    in comparison to others and set ambitious goals [24,25]. As for cognitive vulnerability in

    euthymic states, bipolar patients also showed higher levels of dysfunctional attitudes,

    particularly related to need for social perfectionism and approval than healthy controls [26].

  • 7/25/2019 1-s2.0-S0010440X15300717-main

    6/27

    ACCEP

    TED

    MANUSCRIPT

    ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT

    Studies on facial emotional processing revealed mixed results, but some studies among them

    reported altered recognition of facial emotions [27]. Moreover, altered reactivity in the

    prefrontal cortices, including the ventrolateral and ventromedial regions, appears to be

    involved in deficits of emotional processing and regulation in bipolar disorder [28]. Therefore,

    these abnormalities in cognitive and emotional processing may lead to the potential alteration

    in the dominance perception in patients with bipolar disorder.

    In this study, we investigated the perception of social dominance in response to emotional

    stimuli in patients with bipolar I disorder. Using facial expressions of six different emotions,

    including neutral stimuli, we compared the levels of dominance perception in euthymic

    bipolar patients to healthy control subjects.

    2. Methods

    2.1. Participants

    Thirty-five euthymic bipolar I patients were recruited from psychiatric clinics at Severance

    Mental Health Hospital of the Yonsei University Health System. Bipolar disorder was

    diagnosed by two psychiatrists based on clinical interviews and using the criteria for bipolar

    disorder in the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders 4th edition (DSM-IV)

    [29]. Patients diagnoses were briefly confirmed using the Mini-International

    Neuropsychiatric Interview (MINI) [30] by two psychiatrists (S.H.K and H.S.C). Patientswith schizoaffective disorder, severe personality disorder, recent substance abuse, history of

    head trauma, or any other Axis I disorder were excluded. Forty-five healthy control subjects

    were selected from the local community via advertisement and screened by using the MINI to

    exclude neurological disease and other major psychiatric disease. This research was approved

    by the Institutional Review Board of Severance Mental Health Hospital, and written informed

    consent was obtained from all participants.

  • 7/25/2019 1-s2.0-S0010440X15300717-main

    7/27

  • 7/25/2019 1-s2.0-S0010440X15300717-main

    8/27

    ACCEP

    TED

    MANUSCRIPT

    ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT

    2.2. Materials

    We presented 24 Japanese facial emotional expressions from the JACFEE (Japanese and

    Caucasian Facial Expression of Emotion) [39]. Except for surprised facial expressions, the

    facial stimuli consisted of five basic emotions (happy, anger, disgust, fear, and sad) and

    neutral faces. Each emotional expression was composed of two male and two female actors.

    Different actors were used for each emotion and did not overlap. Because facial features such

    as hairstyle, jaw form, and facial rounding may influence the perception of social dominance

    [11], facial hair and blemishes were removed and all faces were unified into an oval annulus

    using Adobe Photoshop. Skin tone was expressed in black and white on a black background

    to put the most focus on the emotional expression of the face. Faces were presented centrally

    on a 13-inch laptop computer screen at a size of 275 420 pixels, and at a viewing distance

    of approximately 60 cm. Facial emotional expressions were presented in a randomized order.

    Participants were instructed to look carefully at each facial emotional expression and rate

    their perceived dominance for each emotion until they pressed key for next facial stimuli.

    To estimate dominance, the translated version of 7-point dominance scale developed by

    Hess [8] (Insecure-Assertive, Placid-Forceful, Non-controlling-Controlling, Submissive-

    Dominant) was used. The item Placid-Forceful was excluded because the translated

    meaning of the Korean words was unclear when the dominance scale was preliminarily

    conducted on seven normal persons. The remaining items were combined into an overalldominance scale (N = 80, Cronbachs = 0.78). Participants rated each face on three 7 point-

    Likert scales ranging from 3 to 3 with opposite descriptions (i.e., 3 = very insecure; 3 =

    very assertive). Thus, higher positive scores were considered to reflect higher levels of

    perceived social dominance, and lower negative scores were regarded as greater levels of

    perceived social submissiveness.

  • 7/25/2019 1-s2.0-S0010440X15300717-main

    9/27

    ACCEP

    TED

    MANUSCRIPT

    ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT

    2.3. Statistical analysis

    The sociodemographic and clinical characteristics of the bipolar patient and healthy control

    groups were compared using a Chi-square test and an independent Students ttest. Perceived

    dominance scores were analyzed by three-way analysis of variance (ANOVA) to assess

    effects of group, gender of the participant, and facial emotional expressions. Post-hoc

    analyses were conducted in 2 ways by using Bonferroni correction method, due to non-

    significant gender of the participant effect: (1) assessing group effects within each facial

    emotional expressions and (2) comparing perceived dominance scores for the five emotional

    expressions with the neutral expressions (happy vs. neutral, anger vs. neutral, disgust vs.

    neutral, fear vs. neutral, and sad vs. neutral) for each group separately. .

    Lastly, Pearsons correlation was used to estimate the correlation between the clinical

    characteristics and perceived dominance scores in bipolar patients. All of the above statistical

    analyses were conducted with SPSS Statistics, version 19.0.

    3. Results

    For perceived dominance, there were no significant main effect of gender of the participant

    (F[1,456] = 0.31, p = 0.575) or significant interactions (group emotion gender of the

    participant,F[5,456] = 0.36,p= 0.877; group gender of the participant, F[1,456] = 0.97,p

    = 0.326; emotion gender of the participant, F[5, 456]=0.13, p=0.986) when including thegender of the participants. As a result, this factor (gender of the participant) dropped from

    further analysis. A two-way group by emotion ANOVA revealed significant effects of group

    (F[1,948] = 20.34, p

  • 7/25/2019 1-s2.0-S0010440X15300717-main

    10/27

    ACCEP

    TED

    MANUSCRIPT

    ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT

    disgust (p neutral, p neutral,p neutral,p

    = 0.010; sad < neutral,p

  • 7/25/2019 1-s2.0-S0010440X15300717-main

    11/27

    ACCEP

    TED

    MANUSCRIPT

    ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT

    of fear. There was a negative correlation (r = 0.34, p = 0.047) between YMRS and

    dominance ratings of fear. However, these data did not reveal any significant correlations

    when we employed a critical p-value of 0.006 to correct for multiple testing using

    Bonferronis method.

    4. Discussion

    In the present study, bipolar patients showed noticeably lower perceptions of social

    dominance based on anger, disgust, fear, and neutral facial emotional expressions compared

    to healthy controls. However, there were no significant differences in the dominance

    perception of happy and sad emotions between bipolar patients and controls. To our

    knowledge, this is the first study to investigate the social dominance perception using facial

    emotional expressions in bipolar I patients.

    This lower dominance perception of negative emotions may be related to impairment of

    emotional processing and regulation that is observed in individuals with bipolar disorder.

    Although it was long believed that deficits of facial emotion recognition remit during the

    euthymic status of bipolar disorder, growing evidence suggests that these impairments persist

    during the euthymic state [40,41]. A meta-analytic study reported small, but significant effect

    sizes for facial emotion recognition in euthymic bipolar disorder [42], in which euthymic

    bipolar patients were found to have enhanced or impaired recognition of negative emotions,especially fear and disgust [43-45]. In another study, several negative facial emotions were

    misrecognized as other emotions in remitted bipolar patients in comparison to healthy

    controls [46]. Patients with bipolar disorder also exhibited selective impaired with negative,

    but not positive, emotional maintenance when compared to healthy controls [47]. Individuals

    with bipolar disorder were also more likely to ruminate about a positive affect and engage in

    risk-taking behaviors when faced with a negative affect [48]. These difficulties with

  • 7/25/2019 1-s2.0-S0010440X15300717-main

    12/27

    ACCEP

    TED

    MANUSCRIPT

    ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT

    experiencing negative emotions may result in overlooking dominance in negative facial

    expressions. Euthymic bipolar patients showed a greater use of suppression and reappraisal,

    indicating difficulty with regulating emotions [49]. Another possible explanation is that

    impaired attention in bipolar disorder [50,51] might influence lower dominance perceptions

    in response to negative emotional facial expressions. Schizophrenia patients showed a

    significant association between attention processes and facial emotion recognition [52,53].

    Similarly, for bipolar patients, altered dominance perception may be associated with impaired

    attention to details of facial expressions. Taken together, altered recognition or maintenance

    of negative emotions, difficulties with regulating emotion, and deficits in attention may lead

    to a decreased perception of dominance in response to these emotions.

    To the best of our knowledge, there has been no investigation of dominance perception in

    response to facial expressions among patients with depression or bipolar depression. Only

    one study directly rated the perceived dominance to anger, neutral, and happy facial

    expressions in individuals who had high or low social anxiety [54]. The results revealed no

    differences among groups; however, the participants (i.e., students) only had speech fear, and

    their anxiety was thus not as severe as patients with social anxiety disorder. Studies that

    measured dominance indirectly using faces have shown that patients with social anxiety

    disorder are hypervigilant to negative facial expressions and that they have an attentional bias

    to social threats [55-57]. For example, patients with social anxiety disorder, who viewthemselves as less dominant, perceive angry faces as challenges to dominance contest [58].

    Patients with social anxiety disorder rated angry faces as more arousing and as more

    unpleasant than controls [59]. In addition, participants with anxiety disorder reported elevated

    emotional reactivity for facial expressions of anger or contempt [60].

    In healthy individuals, the dorsolateral and ventrolateral prefrontal cortices, which are

    associated with the regulation of socio-emotional responses and behavioral inhibition,

  • 7/25/2019 1-s2.0-S0010440X15300717-main

    13/27

    ACCEP

    TED

    MANUSCRIPT

    ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT

    respectively, are activated when faced with dominant persons [3,5,61]. Likewise, altered

    functioning or connectivity, including ventrolateral prefrontal cortex and amygdala, have

    been observed during emotion processing and emotion regulation in individuals with bipolar

    disorder [28]. These altered neural activities may contribute to the emotional dysregulation

    and differences in dominance perception in patients with bipolar disorder.

    There were no between-group differences in the dominance perception of happy and sad

    emotions in this study. It has been reported that happy and sad facial emotions have a lower

    arousal than do fear, anger, and disgust in healthy people [62,63]. Low arousal faces are more

    emotionally ambiguous and appear to be related to an active affective system including

    amygdala and prefrontal cortex [63]. So this relatively low arousal might lead to no group

    differences in the dominance perception. Additionally, cultural differences might influence

    dominance perception. For example, Westerners perceived happy expressions as higher in

    dominance than neutral expressions [8]. However, no differences in dominance perception

    were found between happy and neutral faces in either bipolar patients or healthy controls in

    this study.

    Interestingly, bipolar patients demonstrated not only a significantly lower dominance

    perception of neutral facial expressions but also perceived excessive submissiveness of

    neutral expressions compared to controls. The neutral facial expressions may be rated as

    negative in some circumstances by healthy persons [64], but bipolar patients reported morefear and showed greater limbic hyperactivation when viewing neutral faces compared with

    controls [65]. Bipolar patients also perceived neutral faces as negative when experiencing a

    high emotional state [66]. As a result, these altered interpretations of neutral expressions

    might contribute to different dominance perceptions in individuals with bipolar disorder.

    Although female gender expressions signaled lower dominance than expressions in males,

    no significant interactions involving gender were observed in this study. Male expressers

  • 7/25/2019 1-s2.0-S0010440X15300717-main

    14/27

    ACCEP

    TED

    MANUSCRIPT

    ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT

    were perceived as higher dominant than female ones because the morphological cues were

    more typical for men than for women in healthy participants [67]. In our study, facial shape

    was unified into an oval annulus; therefore, the gender effect on perceived dominance may

    have been attenuated.

    Negative correlations between residual manic symptoms (YMRS) or drive subscale and the

    dominance perception of anger, disgust, and fear were found in patients with bipolar disorder.

    Also, low BIS scores were related to lower dominance perception of fear in bipolar patients.

    Drive is a subscale of the BAS that regulates approach motivation and goal-directed behavior,

    whereas BIS measures the tendency to regulate with anxiety in response to fear stimuli [36].

    Individuals with social anxiety disorder view themselves as inferior and incapable of

    adequately competing with others; they also tend to be more hypervigilant or anxious toward

    dominant stimuli [17,68]. On the other hand, bipolar patients, who have hypomanic

    symptoms and high drive scores, might view themselves as capable of adequately competing

    with others, exhibiting and fearless responses to facial cues that suggest danger [23,69].

    Therefore, bipolar patients who have residual manic symptoms, high goal pursuit, and low

    BIS might have a lower dominance perception of emotions such as anger, disgust, and fear.

    However, these results should be interpreted carefully, as the statistically significance did not

    appear when applying the conservative threshold.

    The ability to accurately perceive and make inferences about the emotions of other people iscritical to interpersonal and social relationships [70]. In individuals with schizophrenia,

    improvements in perception of facial emotion were shown in response to a training program

    [71], and this has led to improvements in social relationships [72]. Therefore, training of

    appropriate dominance perceptions using various emotional stimuli or situation might be

    helpful in improving social functioning in individuals with bipolar disorder.

    The limitations of this research are the following: First, sample size was relatively small and

  • 7/25/2019 1-s2.0-S0010440X15300717-main

    15/27

    ACCEP

    TED

    MANUSCRIPT

    ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT

    a single site was used for recruitment; therefore, this preliminary study should be replicated

    in larger samples. Second, bipolar patients have been taking mood stabilizers and/or

    antipsychotics. While we cannot completely exclude the medication effects, we did not find

    any significant correlations between the level of dominance perception of each emotion and

    medications (mood stabilizers or neuroleptic doses) in the patient group (all p>0.06). Third,

    our study focused on facial emotions for dominance perception. Although we unified the

    faces, it will be necessary to investigate the contributions of specific facial features and body

    postures or gestures to ratings of dominance. Fourth, we did not assess the reasons why the

    participants judged the emotional faces as more dominant or more submissive. As a result,

    further study will be needed to determine a direct cause of altered dominance perception in

    bipolar disorder. Fifth, the difference in perceived dominance between bipolar patients and

    healthy controls was modest. Thus, these effects might have had a subtle influence on real-

    world behavior. Sixth, given that patients with any other Axis I disorder were excluded, our

    bipolar patients constituted a relatively pure sample; Thus, it may not have been

    representative of general patients with bipolar disorder who have high rates of comorbid

    substance use or anxiety disorders [73]. In addition, we did not directly measure the

    participantsself-esteem or their own sense of social dominance. Further study will be needed

    to determine the relationship between self-esteem and perceived dominance. Moreover, even

    though we measured dominance scores by using Likert scales, future research would considerusing a visual analog scale for increasing sensitivity and decreasing anchoring on specific

    values. Lastly, this was a cross-sectional study. Although lower dominance perception may be

    a trait factor observed during the euthymic state in those with bipolar disorder, an

    investigation into its longitudinal changes during the disease course is needed.

    Despite these limitations, this research is the first of our knowledge to study the perception

    of social dominance in euthymic bipolar patients. Bipolar patients showed a significantly

  • 7/25/2019 1-s2.0-S0010440X15300717-main

    16/27

    ACCEP

    TED

    MANUSCRIPT

    ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT

    lower perception of social dominance in negative, threatening stimuli compared to normal

    participants. This research is important because decreased dominance perception may be

    related to impaired social and occupational functioning and potential components of its

    treatment training in patients with bipolar disorder. This study also provides a foundation for

    future research, focusing on neurobiological or brain imaging of dominance perception in

    individuals with bipolar disorder.

    Acknowledgements

    This study was supported by a faculty research grant (No. 6-2013-0169) of Yonsei

    University College of Medicine.

    Conflicts of interest

    All authors declare that they have no conflicts of interest.

  • 7/25/2019 1-s2.0-S0010440X15300717-main

    17/27

    ACCEP

    TED

    MANUSCRIPT

    ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT

    References

    [1] Keltner D, Gruenfeld DH, Anderson C. Power, approach, and inhibition. Psychol Rev

    2003;110:265-84.

    [2] Wheeler L. Motivation as a determinant of upward comparison. Journal of

    Experimental Social Psychology 1966;1, Supplement 1:27-31.

    [3] Zink CF, Tong Y, Chen Q, Bassett DS, Stein JL, Meyer-Lindenberg A. Know your

    place: neural processing of social hierarchy in humans. Neuron 2008;58:273-83.

    [4] Anderson C, Jennifer BL. The experience of power: examining the effects of power

    on approach and inhibition tendencies. J Pers Soc Psychol 2002;83:1362-77.

    [5] Chiao JY. Neural basis of social status hierarchy across species. Curr Opin Neurobiol

    2010;20:803-9.

    [6] Hess U, Adams RB, Kleck RE. Facial appearance, gender, and emotion expression.

    Emotion 2004;4:378-88.

    [7] Senior C, Phillips ML, Barnes J, David AS. An investigation into the perception of

    dominance from schematic faces: a study using the World-Wide Web. Behav Res

    Methods Instrum Comput 1999;31:341-6.

    [8] Hareli S, Shomrat N, Hess U. Emotional versus neutral expressions and perceptions

    of social dominance and submissiveness. Emotion 2009;9:378-84.[9] Phillips ML, Drevets WC, Rauch SL, Lane R. Neurobiology of emotion perception I:

    The neural basis of normal emotion perception. Biol Psychiatry 2003;54:504-14.

    [10] Tiedens LZ. Anger and advancement versus sadness and subjugation: the effect of

    negative emotion expressions on social status conferral. J Pers Soc Psychol

    2001;80:86-94.

    [11] Hess U, Adams Jr RB, Kleck RE. Facial appearance, gender, and emotion expression.

  • 7/25/2019 1-s2.0-S0010440X15300717-main

    18/27

    ACCEP

    TED

    MANUSCRIPT

    ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT

    Emotion 2004;4:378.

    [12] Senior C, Barnes J, Jenkins R, Landau S, Phillips M, David A. Attribution of social

    dominance and maleness to schematic faces. Social Behavior and Personality: an

    international journal 1999;27:331-7.

    [13] Johnson SL, Leedom LJ, Muhtadie L. The dominance behavioral system and

    psychopathology: evidence from self-report, observational, and biological studies.

    Psychol Bull 2012;138:692-743.

    [14] Hall JA, Halberstadt AG, O'Brien CE. Subordination and nonverbal sensitivity: A

    study and synthesis of findings based on trait measures. Sex Roles 1997;37:295-317.

    [15] Phan KL, Fitzgerald DA, Nathan PJ, Tancer ME. Association between amygdala

    hyperactivity to harsh faces and severity of social anxiety in generalized social phobia.

    Biological Psychiatry 2006;59:424-9.

    [16] Stein MB, Goldin PR, Sareen J, Zorrilla LT, Brown GG. Increased amygdala

    activation to angry and contemptuous faces in generalized social phobia. Arch Gen

    Psychiatry 2002;59:1027-34.

    [17] Weisman O, Aderka IM, Marom S, Hermesh H, Gilboa-Schechtman E. Social rank

    and affiliation in social anxiety disorder. Behaviour research and therapy

    2011;49:399-405.

    [18] Trower P, Gilbert P. New theoretical conceptions of social anxiety and social phobia.Clinical Psychology Review 1989;9:19-35.

    [19] Gilbert P. The relationship of shame, social anxiety and depression: The role of the

    evaluation of social rank. Clinical Psychology & Psychotherapy 2000;7:174-89.

    [20] Gilbert P, Allan S. The role of defeat and entrapment (arrested flight) in depression: an

    exploration of an evolutionary view. Psychological medicine 1998;28:585-98.

    [21] Green MF, Horan WP. Social cognition in schizophrenia. Current Directions in

  • 7/25/2019 1-s2.0-S0010440X15300717-main

    19/27

    ACCEP

    TED

    MANUSCRIPT

    ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT

    Psychological Science 2010;19:243-8.

    [22] Karafin MS, Tranel D, Adolphs R. Dominance attributions following damage to the

    ventromedial prefrontal cortex. Journal of cognitive neuroscience 2004;16:1796-804.

    [23] Putman P, Saevarsson S, van Honk J. Hypomanic trait is associated with a

    hypovigilant automatic attentional response to social cues of danger. Bipolar Disord

    2007;9:779-83.

    [24] Johnson SL, Carver CS. Extreme Goal Setting and Vulnerability to Mania Among

    Undiagnosed Young Adults. Cognit Ther Res 2006;30:377-95.

    [25] Johnson SL. Mania and dysregulation in goal pursuit: a review. Clin Psychol Rev

    2005;25:241-62.

    [26] Scott J, Stanton B, Garland A, Ferrier I. Cognitive vulnerability in patients with

    bipolar disorder. Psychological Medicine 2000;30:467-72.

    [27] Mercer L, Becerra R. A unique emotional processing profile of euthymic bipolar

    disorder? A critical review. J Affect Disord 2013;146:295-309.

    [28] Phillips ML, Swartz HA. A critical appraisal of neuroimaging studies of bipolar

    disorder: toward a new conceptualization of underlying neural circuitry and a road

    map for future research. Am J Psychiatry 2014;171:829-43.

    [29] American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental

    disorders, fourth edition, text revision. 4th edition ed. Washington, DC: AmericanPsychiatric Association; 2000.

    [30] Sheehan DV, Lecrubier Y, Sheehan KH, Amorim P, Janavs J, Weiller E, et al. The

    Mini-International Neuropsychiatric Interview (M.I.N.I.): the development and

    validation of a structured diagnostic psychiatric interview for DSM-IV and ICD-10. J

    Clin Psychiatry 1998;59 Suppl 20:22-33;quiz 4-57.

    [31] Young RC, Biggs JT, Ziegler VE, Meyer DA. A rating scale for mania: reliability,

  • 7/25/2019 1-s2.0-S0010440X15300717-main

    20/27

    ACCEP

    TED

    MANUSCRIPT

    ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT

    validity and sensitivity. Br J Psychiatry 1978;133:429-35.

    [32] Asberg M, Montgomery SA, Perris C, Schalling D, Sedvall G. A comprehensive

    psychopathological rating scale. Acta Psychiatr Scand Suppl 1978:5-27.

    [33] Tohen M, Frank E, Bowden CL, Colom F, Ghaemi SN, Yatham LN, et al. The

    International Society for Bipolar Disorders (ISBD) Task Force report on the

    nomenclature of course and outcome in bipolar disorders. Bipolar Disord

    2009;11:453-73.

    [34] Zimmerman M, Posternak MA, Chelminski I. Defining remission on the

    Montgomery-Asberg depression rating scale. J Clin Psychiatry 2004;65:163-8.

    [35] Lee C, Chon K, Hahn D, Kim K. The Korean adaptation if the State-Trait Depression

    Inventory: STDI-K. Korean J Health Psychol 1999;4:1-14.

    [36] Carver CS, White TL. Behavioral inhibition, behavioral activation, and affective

    responses to impending reward and punishment: the BIS/BAS scales. Journal of

    personality and social psychology 1994;67:319.

    [37] Davidson RJ, Jackson DC, Kalin NH. Emotion, plasticity, context, and regulation:

    perspectives from affective neuroscience. Psychological bulletin 2000;126:890.

    [38] Johnson SL, Edge MD, Holmes MK, Carver CS. The behavioral activation system

    and mania. Annu Rev Clin Psychol 2012;8:243-67.

    [39] Biehl M, Matsumoto D, Ekman P, Hearn V, Heider K, Kudoh T, et al. Matsumoto andEkman's Japanese and Caucasian Facial Expressions of Emotion (JACFEE):

    Reliability data and cross-national differences. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior

    1997;21:3-21.

    [40] Hofer A, Biedermann F, Yalcin N, Fleischhacker W. [Neurocognition and social

    cognition in patients with schizophrenia or mood disorders]. Neuropsychiatr

    2010;24:161-9.

  • 7/25/2019 1-s2.0-S0010440X15300717-main

    21/27

    ACCEP

    TED

    MANUSCRIPT

    ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT

    [41] Van Rheenen TE, Rossell SL. Is the non-verbal behavioural emotion-processing

    profile of bipolar disorder impaired? A critical review. Acta Psychiatr Scand

    2013;128:163-78.

    [42] Samame C, Martino DJ, Strejilevich SA. Social cognition in euthymic bipolar

    disorder: systematic review and meta-analytic approach. Acta Psychiatr Scand

    2012;125:266-80.

    [43] Lembke A, Ketter TA. Impaired recognition of facial emotion in mania. American

    Journal of Psychiatry 2002;159:302-4.

    [44] Harmer CJ, Grayson L, Goodwin GM. Enhanced recognition of disgust in bipolar

    illness. Biological Psychiatry 2002;51:298-304.

    [45] Martino DJ, Strejilevich SA, Scpola M, Igoa A, Marengo E, Ais ED, et al.

    Heterogeneity in cognitive functioning among patients with bipolar disorder. Journal

    of affective disorders 2008;109:149-56.

    [46] Hoertnagl CM, Muehlbacher M, Biedermann F, Yalcin N, Baumgartner S, Schwitzer

    G, et al. Facial emotion recognition and its relationship to subjective and functional

    outcomes in remitted patients with bipolar I disorder. Bipolar Disord 2011;13:537-44.

    [47] Gruber J, Purcell AL, Perna MJ, Mikels JA. Letting go of the bad: deficit in

    maintaining negative, but not positive, emotion in bipolar disorder. Emotion

    2013;13:168-75.[48] Fletcher K, Parker GB, Manicavasagar V. Coping profiles in bipolar disorder. Compr

    Psychiatry 2013;54:1177-84.

    [49] Gruber J, Harvey AG, Gross JJ. When trying is not enough: emotion regulation and

    the effort-success gap in bipolar disorder. Emotion 2012;12:997-1003.

    [50] Marotta A, Chiaie RD, Spagna A, Bernabei L, Sciarretta M, Roca J, et al. Impaired

    conflict resolution and vigilance in euthymic bipolar disorder. Psychiatry Res

  • 7/25/2019 1-s2.0-S0010440X15300717-main

    22/27

    ACCEP

    TED

    MANUSCRIPT

    ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT

    2015;229:490-6.

    [51] Bora E, Vahip S, Akdeniz F. Sustained attention deficits in manic and euthymic

    patients with bipolar disorder. Prog Neuropsychopharmacol Biol Psychiatry

    2006;30:1097-102.

    [52] Addington J, Addington D. Facial affect recognition and information processing in

    schizophrenia and bipolar disorder. Schizophrenia research 1998;32:171-81.

    [53] Tsotsi S, Bozikas VP, Kosmidis MH. The role of attention processes in facial affect

    recognition in schizophrenia. Cognitive neuropsychiatry 2015:1-16.

    [54] Vrana SR, Gross D. Reactions to facial expressions: effects of social context and

    speech anxiety on responses to neutral, anger, and joy expressions. Biological

    Psychology 2004;66:63-78.

    [55] Schofield CA, Coles ME, Gibb BE. Social anxiety and interpretation biases for facial

    displays of emotion: Emotion detection and ratings of social cost. Behaviour Research

    and Therapy 2007;45:2950-63.

    [56] Staugaard SR. Threatening faces and social anxiety: a literature review. Clinical

    psychology review 2010;30:669-90.

    [57] Yoon KL, Zinbarg RE. Threat is in the eye of the beholder: Social anxiety and the

    interpretation of ambiguous facial expressions. Behaviour research and therapy

    2007;45:839-47.[58] hman A. Face the beast and fear the face: Animal and social fears as prototypes for

    evolutionary analyses of emotion. Psychophysiology 1986;23:123-45.

    [59] Straube T, Kolassa I-T, Glauer M, Mentzel H-J, Miltner WH. Effect of task conditions

    on brain responses to threatening faces in social phobics: an event-related functional

    magnetic resonance imaging study. Biological psychiatry 2004;56:921-30.

    [60] Social CRD, Threat P. Neural Bases of Social Anxiety Disorder. Arch Gen Psychiatry

  • 7/25/2019 1-s2.0-S0010440X15300717-main

    23/27

    ACCEP

    TED

    MANUSCRIPT

    ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT

    2009;66:170-80.

    [61] Marsh AA, Blair KS, Jones MM, Soliman N, Blair RJR. Dominance and submission:

    the ventrolateral prefrontal cortex and responses to status cues. Journal of cognitive

    neuroscience 2009;21:713-24.

    [62] Russell JA, Bullock M. Multidimensional scaling of emotional facial expressions:

    similarity from preschoolers to adults. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology

    1985;48:1290.

    [63] Gerber AJ, Posner J, Gorman D, Colibazzi T, Yu S, Wang Z, et al. An affective

    circumplex model of neural systems subserving valence, arousal, and cognitive

    overlay during the appraisal of emotional faces. Neuropsychologia 2008;46:2129-39.

    [64] Lee E, Kang JI, Park IH, Kim JJ, An SK. Is a neutral face really evaluated as being

    emotionally neutral? Psychiatry Research 2008;157:77-85.

    [65] Rich BA, Vinton DT, Roberson-Nay R, Hommer RE, Berghorst LH, McClure EB, et

    al. Limbic hyperactivation during processing of neutral facial expressions in children

    with bipolar disorder. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 2006;103:8900-5.

    [66] Mansell W, Lam D. I Wont Do What You Tell Me!: Elevated mood and the

    assessment of advice-taking in euthymic bipolar I disorder. Behaviour Research and

    Therapy 2006;44:1787-801.

    [67] Hess U, Adams Jr R, Kleck R. Who may frown and who should smile? Dominance,affiliation, and the display of happiness and anger. Cognition & Emotion

    2005;19:515-36.

    [68] Leber S, Heidenreich T, Stangier U, Hofmann SG. Processing of facial affect under

    social threat in socially anxious adults: mood matters. Depress Anxiety 2009;26:196-

    206.

    [69] Alloy LB, Abramson LY, Walshaw PD, Gerstein RK, Keyser JD, Whitehouse WG, et

  • 7/25/2019 1-s2.0-S0010440X15300717-main

    24/27

    ACCEP

    TED

    MANUSCRIPT

    ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT

    al. Behavioral approach system (BAS)-relevant cognitive styles and bipolar spectrum

    disorders: concurrent and prospective associations. J Abnorm Psychol 2009;118:459-

    71.

    [70] Wallace CJ. Community and interpersonal functioning in the course of schizophrenic

    disorders. Schizophr Bull 1984;10:233-57.

    [71] Wolwer W, Frommann N, Halfmann S, Piaszek A, Streit M, Gaebel W. Remediation

    of impairments in facial affect recognition in schizophrenia: efficacy and specificity

    of a new training program. Schizophr Res 2005;80:295-303.

    [72] Sachs G, Winklbaur B, Jagsch R, Lasser I, Kryspin-Exner I, Frommann N, et al.

    Training of affect recognition (TAR) in schizophrenia--impact on functional outcome.

    Schizophr Res 2012;138:262-7.

    [73] Krishnan KRR. Psychiatric and medical comorbidities of bipolar disorder.

    Psychosomatic medicine 2005;67:1-8.

  • 7/25/2019 1-s2.0-S0010440X15300717-main

    25/27

    ACCEP

    TED

    MANUSCRIPT

    ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT

    Figure 1. Perceived dominance scores to each emotion for bipolar patients and healthy

    controls, with standard error bars. * :p

  • 7/25/2019 1-s2.0-S0010440X15300717-main

    26/27

    ACCEP

    TED

    MANUSCRIPT

    ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT

    Figure 1

  • 7/25/2019 1-s2.0-S0010440X15300717-main

    27/27

    ACCEP

    TED

    MANUSCRIPT

    ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT

    Table 1.Demographic and clinical characteristics of subjectsBipolar Control t or p-value

    (n=35) (n=45)

    Age 35.710.3 32.88.5 1.37 0.17Gender (M/F) 20/15 21/24 0.87 0.35

    Education (years) 11.71.7 12.11.4 -1.23 0.22

    Estimated IQ 107.911.6 111.811.6 -1.46 0.15

    No. admission 3.93.7 -

    Duration of illness (years) 9.88.8 -

    YMRS 3.84.8 0.71.0 3.65