02 Chapters 1-5 Nias · 2019. 3. 5. · Trail 1980, Nias 1984, Zack ald Ligon 1985b). (iii) The...

108
CHAPTER 1 GENERAL INTRODUCTION 1.1 Co-operative breedilg in birds. Co-operative breeding in birds is usually defined as the regular involvement of individuals, other than the parents, in the care and feeding of offspring (Brown 1978). The presence of 'helpers' (Skutch 1935) has been of considerable interest to behavioural ecologists and a number of reviews and theoretical discussions have been published in recent decades (eg. Brown 1975, Wilson 1975, Brown 1978, Koenig and Pitelka 1981, Emlen and Vehrencamp 1983 among others). Much of the interest in co-operative breeding has been due to the apparently altruistic behaviour exhibited by helpers toward the offspring of others and, in order to explain the evolution of suer behaviour, there has been much use made of the concept of ir.clusive fitness (Hamilton 1964) and the theory of kin-selection (Maynard Smith 1964). Co-operative breeding has been documented in many species of bird, with at least thirty-two families and sub- families represented in the Passeriformes alone (Brown 1978, Fry 1977, Emlen 1978, 1984). In Australia, co-operative breeding is widespread with one survey (Dow 1980) listing sixty-five species in twenty families. Among the better studied species in Australia are the babblers Pomatostomus sp. (Brown and Baida 1977, 3rown et al. 1978, 1982, 1983), Noisy Miner Manorina melana:7ephala (Dow 1970,

Transcript of 02 Chapters 1-5 Nias · 2019. 3. 5. · Trail 1980, Nias 1984, Zack ald Ligon 1985b). (iii) The...

  • CHAPTER 1

    GENERAL INTRODUCTION

    1.1 Co-operative breedilg in birds.

    Co-operative breeding in birds is usually defined

    as the regular involvement of individuals, other than the

    parents, in the care and feeding of offspring (Brown 1978).

    The presence of 'helpers' (Skutch 1935) has been of

    considerable interest to behavioural ecologists and a number

    of reviews and theoretical discussions have been published

    in recent decades (eg. Brown 1975, Wilson 1975, Brown 1978,

    Koenig and Pitelka 1981, Emlen and Vehrencamp 1983 among

    others). Much of the interest in co-operative breeding has

    been due to the apparently altruistic behaviour exhibited

    by helpers toward the offspring of others and, in order to

    explain the evolution of suer behaviour, there has been much

    use made of the concept of ir.clusive fitness (Hamilton 1964)

    and the theory of kin-selection (Maynard Smith 1964).

    Co-operative breeding has been documented in many

    species of bird, with at least thirty-two families and sub-

    families represented in the Passeriformes alone (Brown 1978,

    Fry 1977, Emlen 1978, 1984). In Australia, co-operative

    breeding is widespread with one survey (Dow 1980) listing

    sixty-five species in twenty families. Among the better

    studied species in Australia are the babblers Pomatostomus

    sp. (Brown and Baida 1977, 3rown et al. 1978, 1982, 1983),

    Noisy Miner Manorina melana:7ephala (Dow 1970,

  • 1977, 1978a, 1978b, 1979a, 1979b), fairy-wrens Malurus

    cyaneus and M. splendens (Rowley 1965, 1981) and the

    White-winged Chough Corcorax melanorhamphus (Rowley 1978).

    Many species however, have not been studied in detail and

    the occurrence and type of co-operative breeding in these

    species is almost unknown.

    Most co-operative breeding species live in small

    (2 - 20 birds), relatively stable social groups in permanent,

    all-purpose territories, are monogamous, and insectivorous in

    their diet (Ricklefs 1975, Grimes 1976, Brown 1978). In a

    recent survey of Australian species, Ford et al. (unpubl.

    ms.) found that most inhabiter eucalypt woodlands and forests

    rather than rainforests or deserts, were insectivorous, and

    were generally ground-based foragers. Such species included

    the babblers, White-winged Chough, and the fairy-wrens.

    A variety of terms have been used in descriptions

    of co-operative social systems and in the present study I

    have followed the terminology of Emlen (1984). Co-operative

    breeding is defined as any situation in which more than one

    adult of either sex (ie. the minimum requirement for sexual

    reproduction) participate in the raising of offspring.

    Within this general definitic , n I further separate 'helper-at-

    the-nest' systems and 'commural l systems.

    Helper-at-the-nest systems comprise a single,

    monogamous breeding pair together with one or more non--

    breeding helpers. Helpers are often the grown offspring

    of the pair that have failed to disperse from their natal

    group. Although immatures from previous nests within the

    2

  • same breeding season may also help at later nests, this is

    not considered to be co-operative breeding. Some well-

    studied species which are typEcal helpers-at-the-nest species

    include the Florida Scrub Jay Aphelocomacoerulescens

    (Woolfenden 1975, 1978, Woolfnden and Fitzpatrick 1984),

    Stripe-backed Wren Campylorhynchus nuchalis (Rabenold

    and Christensen 1979, Rabenold 1984, Austad and Rabenold

    1985), and the fairy-wrens (Rowley 1965, 1981).

    In communal breeding species however, there may

    be more than one breeding pair in each breeding g roup and

    helpers may also be breeders. Communal breeding species

    include the Acorn Woodpecker Melanerpes formicivorous

    (MacRoberts and MacRoberts 1976, Stacey 1979, Koenig 1981)

    and the Groove-billed Ani Crotophagasulcirostris

    (Vehrencamp 1978). Communal breeding is often treated

    separately in discussions of co-operative breeding and will

    not be treated in detail in this study. The evolution of

    communal breeding is addressed specifically by Emlen (eg.

    Emlen 1982b, 1984, Emlen and Vehrencamp 1983, 1985), and

    Vehrencamp (eg. Vehrencamp 1983).

    1.2 The adaptive significance of co-operative breeding.

    In order to understand better the adaptive

    significance of co-operative breeding it is necessary to ask

    three general questions about the individuals involved;

    (1) Why delay dispersal? (2) Why help?, and (3) Why do

    breeders tolerate helpers?

  • Q.1 : Why do some birds delay their dispersal from

    the natal group?

    In species such as the Superb Fairy-wren, delayed

    dispersal is a necessary precursor to co-operative breeding.

    Since Superb Fairy-wrens, as with many other co-operative

    breeding species (Woolfenden and Fitzpatrick 1978, 1984),

    disperse directly into breeding positions, delayed dispersal

    also implies delayed breeding. Groups of non-breeding

    birds or 'floaters', for example, which live in marginal

    areas or wander throughout the habitat, are not generally

    found in co-operative breeding species (Koenig and Pitelka

    1981). The question of delayed dispersal therefore raises

    two further points; (i) what are the advantages of residing

    in the natal group as opposed to other types of social group,

    or alone, and (ii) why do some birds delay breeding?

    However, it is difficult to test the first of these points

    in a species such as the SupErb Fairy-wren because single

    birds are rarely recorded, particularly during the breeding

    season. Instead, I have con s idered two potential advantages

    of association with the natal group; (i) the opportunity

    to help raise additional siblings, and (ii) the opportunity

    for greater access to breeding positions (eg. through

    inheritance of the breeding position). Both of these

    points are important to the social organisation of fairy-

    wrens and are considered in later chapters.

  • There must also be a consideration of the likely

    success of any dispersal movement, ie. what is the probability

    that dispersal will lead to breeding? Emlen (1982a)

    identified four factors which should influence the success

    of dispersing individuals; (i) the risks associated with

    dispersal itself, (ii) the probability of establishment

    on a suitable territory or patch of habitat, (iii) the

    probability of finding a mate, and (iv) the probability

    of successful reproduction onc.?. established.

    (i) Dispersal should always involve some

    significant risk, most importantly from predation or

    starvation. It is also possible that this risk is propor-

    tional to the time taken and the distance moved during

    dispersal, and may be greater for immature or inexperienced

    birds. In addition, many co-operative breeding species are

    restricted to specific habitat types or have specific

    requirements (Selander 1964, Brown 1974, 1978, Brown and

    Saida 1977, Craig 1979, 1980a, 1980b, Stacey 1979, Trail

    1980, Koenig 1981, Koenig and Pitelka 1981, Zack and Ligon

    1985a). These specific requirements may require extensive

    searching by dispersing birds, further increasing the risks

    associated with dispersal.

    (ii) For species with specific habitat require-

    ments, there may not be sufficient suitable habitat to

    accommodate all of the potential breeders (Selander 1964,

    Brown 1969, 1974, Koenig and Pitelka 1981). In addition

    to the abundance of suitable habitat, spatial and temporal

    variability in habitat quality may also influence the

    5

  • probability of successful establishment (Brown and Balda 1977,

    Trail 1980, Nias 1984, Zack ald Ligon 1985b).

    (iii) The success of a dispersing bird in becoming

    established as a breeder will also depend on the probability

    of locating a sexual partner. Skewed sex-ratios are a

    feature of many co-operative breeding species and a lack of

    mates may be an important constraint on reproductive options

    (Dow 1978a, 1978b, Fry 1972, Emlen 1978, 1984, Reyer 1980,

    Rowley 1981). The male-based sex-ratio found in Superb

    Fairy-wrens at Gungahlin (Rowley 1965) for example, has

    been widely discussed as a constraint on the reproductive

    options of potential breeding males and a factor promoting

    delayed dispersal (Brown 197E, Emlen 1978, 1984, Emlen and

    Vehrencamp 1983, 1985, Wittenberger 1984).

    (iv) Even if dispersing birds are able to obtain

    suitable habitat and a mate, poor environmental conditions

    may reduce the probable success of any reproductive efforts.

    This type of 'ecological constraint' has been discussed by

    Emlen (eg. Emlen 1981, 1982a, Emlen and Vehrencamp 1983, 1985)

    with particular reference to co-operative species (eg. the

    White-fronted Bee-eater Mera:Ds bullockoides ) living in

    variable environments.

    In summary, the probability of successful dispersal

    and establishment by individual birds may be reduced by a

    number of factors or 'ecological constraints' (Emlen 1982a).

    If this probability is sufficiently low, then individuals

    may benefit from delayed dispersal and association with

  • close relatives. The factors which reduce the dispersal

    and reproductive opportunities for potential breeders have

    been discussed in much of the literature concerning co-

    operative breeding (eg. Selander 1964, Brown 1974, 1978,

    Emlen 1982a, Emlen and Vehrencamp 1983, 1985 among others).

    Q.2 : Why do non-dispersing birds help to raise

    the offspring of others?

    Given that some birds delay their dispersal from

    the natal group, there may be a number of potential benefits

    LO be gained from helping behaviour. Emlen and Vehrencamp

    (1983, 1985) for example, suggest that helpers; (i) may gain

    valuable experience in parental duties, (ii) may be able

    to disperse along with the young that they raise, (iii) may

    be able to raise their own, future helpers, and (iv) may be

    able to enhance their inclusive fitness by the production

    of additional relatives. Sone of the benefits of remaining

    within the natal group and helping may be immediate, such

    as increased survival, or they may be delayed, such as being

    in a position to inherit the natal territory (Emlen 1984,

    Wiley and Rabenold 1984). In addition, benefits may be in-

    direct (ie. affecting the fitness of genealogical relatives,

    Brown 1980). There has been much debate on the relative

    importance of direct versus indirect benefits but the

    available data suggest that both types of benefits may be

    gained from helping behaviour (Vehrencamp 1979). Helpers

    may, for example, gain experience at parental duties at the

    same time as raising additional relatives.

    7

  • However, in order for helpers to gain indirect

    benefits from helping behaviour, it must first be established

    that helpers increase the reproductive output of breeders.

    Although a correlation between the presence, or number, of

    helpers and reproductive output may indicate a positive

    effect of helpers, it does not establish a causal link.

    Lack (1968) for example, recognised that at least two other

    factors, parental age and territory quality, may influence

    both the probability that helpers are present, and the

    reproductive success of the breeders. A number of studies

    for example, have demonstrated a relationship between group

    size (and hence the presence or number of helpers in the

    group), territory quality, and reproductive output by

    breeders (Zahavi 1974, Brown 1978, Gaston 1978b, Koenig

    and Pitelka 1981, Lewis 1981, Zack and Ligon 1985b). The

    only experimental evidence of the effect of helpers on group

    reproductive success (independently of habitat effects) has

    come from a study of the Gre y -crowned Babbler Pomatostomus--_-_-_-_-__

    temporalis. By manipulating the number of helpers present

    in groups of babblers, Brown et al.(1982) were able to show

    that groups with fewer helpers produced fewer fledglings than

    did groups with more helpers, although the mechanism by

    which helpers increased group reproductive success was not

    clear. It had earlier been shown, for example, that helpers

    did not increase the rate at which food was delivered to

    nestlings (Brown et al. 1978) and it was later shown that

    nestling growth was not influenced by the presence of helpers

    (Dow and Gill 1984). It was possible, however, that

    8

  • breeding females expended less energy on parental care when

    helpers were present and were therefore able to make more

    nesting attempts (Brown and Brown 1981).

    An alternative view is that helping behaviour is

    simply mis-directed parental care and not, in itself, an

    adaptive trait (Price et al. 1983, Jamieson 1986). Helpers

    may perform parental duties simply because they are

    stimulated by begging nestlings. Selection against helping

    behaviour might therefore work against the later performance

    of normal parental behaviour and be counter to the fitness

    of the individual (Brown and Brown 1984). Such a hypothesis

    might be refuted however, if it could be shown that helpers

    vary the amount of aid given in accordance with the degree

    of potential benefit to be obtained. Helpers may, for example,

    selectively aid closer relatives (Clarke 1984) or adjust the

    amount of aid given according to their dominance position

    within the group (Carlisle and Zahavi 1986).

    Q.3 : Why should breeders tolerate the presence

    of grown offspring in the natal group?

    Although there are a number of potential costs

    and benefits to group living (Alexander 1974, Wilson 1975,

    Hoogland and Sherman 1976, BErtram 1978), a number of these

    relate more specifically to co-operative breeding. In

    particular, there are severa] important benefits which

    might be gained by allowing g rown offspring to remain

    within the natal group if they participate in parental

    duties. Helpers might increase the survival of offspring

  • by feeding, or reduce the losses associated with brood

    parasitism or predation by mobbing. Even if no immediate

    enhancement of reproductive output results from the presence

    of helpers, assistance given During the nesting period may

    enhance the survival or future reproductive output of

    breeders (eg. Woolfenden and Fitzpatrick 1984). Gaston

    (1978a) suggested that assistance given by helpers may be

    a form of 'payment' to the breeders. Therefore, any cost

    to the breeders, such as increased competition for food,

    might be compensated for by helpers during the raising of

    nestlings.

    In a number of co-operative breeding species,

    helpers may eventually inherit the breeding position within

    the natal group (Brown 1974, Woolfenden and Fitzpatrick 1978,

    Brown and Brown 1981, Rowley 1965, 1981). Co-operative

    breeding might therefore be considered as a strategy for

    retaining good quality territories in the possession of

    genetic relatives, and increasing the probability that

    offspring gain a breeding position (Brown 1974).

    1.3 The Superb Fairy-wren.

    The Superb Fairy-wren is a small (ca. 10g)

    insectivorous passerine common in much of south-eastern

    Australia (Fig. 1.1). During the 1950s and 1960s Ian Rowley

    made a detailed and long-term study of the species at

    Gungahlin, near Canberra A.C.T. Rowley's work was one of

    the first to use individuall y recognizable (colour-banded)

    subjects and his work on the Superb (Rowley 1965) and later

    - 10 -

  • the Splendid (Rowley 1981) Fairy-wrens have become landmarks

    in Australian ornithology.

    Rowley (1965) found that breeding groups of

    Fairy-wrens comprised a single monogamous breeding pair

    together with any adult, non-breeding helpers that were

    present. Immature birds from previous nests during the

    breeding season also accompanied the breeding pair. Adult

    helpers were almost always males, and the offspring of at

    least one of the breeding pair. Helpers were usually one-

    year old birds that had not dispersed from their natal

    group although some older helpers were also present.

    Helpers and immatures participated in a number of

    the parental duties, including the feeding of nestlings and

    dependent fledglings. Helpers also assumed much of the

    responsibility for the tendir:g of dependent fledglings while

    the breeding female began re-nesting. Breeding males also

    fed nestlings and tended fledglings but only the breeding

    female built the nest, laid and incubated the eggs, and

    brooded the nestlings.

    During the breeding season (approximately September

    to January) each breeding group occupied a territory

    containing dense understory vegetation interspersed with

    clear, open ground. In the non-breeding season groups often

    wandered outside their breeding territories into nearby

    'neutral areas', sometimes joining other fairy-wrens in

    temporary feeding aggregations, more permanent 'super-groups',

    or in mixed-species feeding-flocks.

  • Eggs were laid from September to January with a

    peak of egg-laying in October. Up to three successful

    clutches (of 3 or 4 eggs each: were laid within the one

    breeding season and replacement clutches were laid for

    unsuccessful nesting attempts. The eggs were laid on

    successive days, sometimes with a one-day gap between the

    third and fourth egg. Incubation lasted about 14 days and

    nestlings fledged 12 or 13 days after hatching. Fledglings

    remained partially dependent on other birds for food up to

    30 days after leaving the nest.

    Rowley found that pairs with helpers produced more

    fledglings on average (1.06 independent fledglings per

    pair) than did pairs without adult helpers (0.75 independent

    fledglings per pair) on each nesting attempt. Rowley

    therefore concluded that helpers made a significant

    contribution to the reproductive output of the breeders

    that they helped and it was suggested that helpers might

    enhance their fitness to a greater extent by helping, than

    they could by breeding. However, there was little evidence

    of a direct causal link between helping behaviour and

    increased reproductive output for breeders. Rowley did

    suggest however, that the brEeding female, freed from some

    of the responsibilities of parental care, was able to direct

    her efforts into producing extra clutches.

    As with many of his contemporaries in the 1960s,

    Rowley viewed social organisation as an adaptation for the

    benefit of the species, rather than the individual. With the

  • synthesis of social behaviour and population genetics

    however (eg. Hamilton 1964, Maynard Smith 1964,

    Brown 1975, Wilson 1975), social organisations were seen

    more as systems brought about by selection acting on

    individuals, or groups of closely related individuals,

    rather than the species. Several authors (eg. Brown 1975,

    Emlen 1978) have since made a re-assessment of Rowley's

    data in light of modern theories. In particular, Brown

    (1975) used Hamilton's inequality (Hamilton 1964) in order

    to determine which strategy, breeding or helping, would

    maximise the fitness of an individual. Brown (1975) concluded

    that the benefits derived by helpers in aiding close relatives

    (ie. through the operation of kin-selection) were insufficient

    to explain why some birds chose to help, rather than to breed

    and that helping behaviour would have been favoured only if

    birds were in some way prevented from reproducing. The most

    obvious cause of this reproductive constraint on individuals

    was that helpers (mostly males) were unable to find a sexual

    partner. Rowley found that females tended to disperse more

    frequently, and further, than did the more sedentary males.

    As a consequence, it was suggested that females suffered

    a higher mortality rate than males. Some males would there-

    fore be unable to obtain a mate and, unable to reproduce

    independently, would benefit most from remaining in the natal

    group. Males that remained as helpers might improve their

    fitness through better survival, helping to raise additional

    siblings, and by gaining experience. Helpers were therefore

    seen as birds which had been prevented from breeding by a

    - 13 -

  • lack of sexual partners, and helping behaviour as a means

    of enhancing their inclusive fitness while remaining in the

    natal group.

    1.4 Objectives of this study.

    In general, empirical studies and long-term

    observations of co-operative breeding species still lag

    behind the development of theory. Further developments

    may come most easily with comparative studies of related

    species (eg. Zack and Ligon 1985a), studies of different

    populations of the one species (eg. Reyer 1980, 1984), and

    through experimental manipulation (eg. Brown et al. 1982).

    In this study I have attempted to answer the

    three questions posed in Section 1.2 which address the

    adaptive significance of co-operative breeding in Superb

    Fairy-wrens. A colour-banded population was monitored over

    a four year period and particular attention was paid to the

    demography of the population, the behaviour of helpers at

    the nest, and factors which influenced the reproductive

    success of breeders. The following hypotheses were

    considered throughout the study; (i) that delayed dispersal

    and breeding is only favoured when the probability of

    successful dispersal and establishment is sufficiently

    low, and (ii) that helping behaviour is an adaptive trait

    which benefits both the helper and the breeder.

  • Fig. 1.1 The location of Armidale in relation to the

    distribution of the Superb Fairy-wren Malurus cyaneus

    (Blakers et al. 1984).

  • tf' A rmidale

  • CHAPTER 2

    GENERAL METHODS AND STUDY SITE

    The study was conducted from September 1982 to

    February 1986 at Eastwood State Forest (Fig. 2.1), 10km

    south-east of Armidale, New South Wales (30° 35' S, 151P

    44' E). Situated on the New England Tablelands, the

    altitude of the study-site varied from about 975m to 1036m

    above sea-level.

    Table 2.1 shows the monthly rain

    Armidale during the study period. Figures are taken from a

    local newspaper 'The ArmidalE Express' 2nd January, 1986.

    Total annual rainfall averaged about 800mm with most rainfall

    being recorded in late sprin g and summer (October to

    February). However, the firE;t year of the study coincided

    with an extreme drought in the area and only 462.8mm were

    recorded from January to December in that year. Approximately

    average rainfall was recorded for the remainder of the study.

    Summers in Armidale are warm with January temperatures

    ranging from 140 C (mean daily minimum) to 280 C (mean daily

    maximum). Winters are cold by Australian standards with

    July temperatures ranging from 0 - 14° C, light snow was

    recorded (mostly in July) each year.

    Eastwood S.F. is a remnant patch (about 200ha) of

    open eucalypt woodland dominated by stringybarks (mostly

    Eucalyptus caliginosa), with some gums (E. blakelyi and

    E. viminalis) and boxes (E. :nellidora) also present.

    fall recorded at

    - 16 -

  • There was little understorey vegetation present with only

    5% cover at the 1 - 5m level being recorded by Ford et al.

    (1985). Grasses, woodpiles, fallen trees, patches of eucalypt

    regeneration, acacias (mostly Acacia filicifolia) and black-

    berry brambles (Rubus vulgaris comprised the bulk of the

    understorey vegetation or thicket present. The study-site

    was surrounded by partially cleared grazing lands.

    A description of the bird community at Eastwood

    is given by Ford and Bell (1982) and Ford et al. (1985).

    Fairy-wrens were captured with mist-nets, or taken

    from the nest, and banded with numbered aluminium bands

    supplied by C.S.I.P.O. and, later in the study period, by

    the Australian N.P.W.S. Bird and Bat Banding Scheme.

    Throughout the thesis individuals are referred to by the

    number on their bands (eg. 92733, 30824 etc.) Individual

    combinations of plastic colour-bands were also given to

    birds which could be accurately sexed on the basis of

    plumage (see Rowley 1965) enabling visual identification

    of individuals.

    Censuses were made of each group of fairy-wrens

    once or twice monthly in the non-breeding season (March

    to August) and weekly in the breeding season (September to

    February). During the censu3 of each group its position

    was recorded relative to a 50m by 50m grid and then mapped.

    Individual group members were identified and the number of

    new birds or immatures in the group counted.

    Further details of specific methods are given in the

    relevant chapters.

    - 1 7 -

  • m

    Table

    2.1

    Mon

    thly

    rain

    fall (in

    mm

    ) re

    cord

    ed at

    Arm

    idale

    , N

    .S.W

    . (fr

    om

    "Th

    e A

    rmid

    ale

    Expre

    ss"

    2 Jan., 1986 .--

    Yea

    r Ja

    n

    Feb M

    ar

    April M

    ay J

    un

    e

    Ju

    ly

    Au

    gS

    ept

    Oct

    Nov

    Dec

    An

    nu

    al Tota

    l

    1982

    82.4

    55.2

    71.2

    16.6

    7.6

    14.8

    11.8

    0.6

    40.2

    92.2

    9.8

    60.4

    462.S

    1983

    66.0

    55.8

    35.8

    137.0

    137.4

    32.4

    37.8

    24.4

    79.6 111.6

    55.4 138.0

    914.2

    1984

    198.6 117.2

    45.0

    63.4

    15.4

    28.4

    108.0

    37.6

    46.4

    75.2 117.4

    40.8

    893.4

    1985

    41.4 100.4

    41.0

    83.6

    23.2

    42.6

    57.4

    70.4

    56.4

    95.8

    26.6 118.0

    756.8

    1986

    21.0 39.4

    --

    --

    --

    --

    --

    -

    Ave

    rage

    81.9

    73.6

    48.3

    75.2

    45.9

    29.6

    53.8

    33.3

    55.7

    93.7

    52.3

    89.3

    747.8

  • Fig. 2.1 Eastwood State Forest, near Armidale N.S.W.

    Contour intervals at 15m, broken lines indicate drainage

    channels, open circles indicate dams.

  • CHAPTER 3

    POPULATION BIOLOGY AND SOCIAL ORGANISATION

    3.1 Introduction

    Early studies of animal societies relied on

    correlations between aspects of the environment and the

    type of social organisation adopted by different species

    (eg. Crook 1965, Lack 1968, Emlen and Oring 1977).

    Although these correlations provided many insights into

    the ecological factors which shaped social systems, they

    were insufficient in themselves to explain how certain

    social systems had evolved. In the study of co-operative

    breeding, for example, simple models based on environmental

    influences have proved inadequate. Although some authors

    (eg. Selander 1964, Brown 1974, Woolfenden 1975, Ricklefs

    1975) stressed the importance of stable and predictable

    environments, others (eg. Rowley 1965, 1968, Fry 1972) saw

    co-operative breeding as an adaptation to harsh and

    unpredictable environments.

    Recent studies have concentrated on the demography

    of the species concerned in attempts to explain the adaptive

    significance of co-operative breeding (Brown 1978, Emlen

    1982a). In the field, long-term studies have attempted to

    quantify the factors which influence the reproductive

    options of individuals (eg. Rowley 1981, Woolfenden and

    Fitzpatrick 1984). Some experimental testing of hypo-

    theses has begun (eg. Brown et al. 1982) and comparisons are

    - 20 -

  • being made between co-operative and non-co-operative

    breeders (eg. Zack and Ligon 1985a, 1985b).

    In this chapter, the major aim has been to

    quantitatively describe the demography of fairy-wrens at

    Eastwood. The basic social organisation of the fairy-wrens

    is also described and much of the data collected and

    analysed form a basis for later chapters. This chapter

    also relates directly to the first of the three questions

    posed in Section 1.9, ie. Why do some birds delay their

    dispersal from the natal group? An attempt is therefore

    made to describe how and when individuals disperse,

    obtain breeding positions within the population, and which

    factors may influence the probable success of dispersing

    birds.

    3.2 Methods

    The study-site, banding and identification of

    individuals was described in Chapter 2.

    At least once each month between January 1983

    and December 1985 all groups of fairy-wrens were located

    and individuals identified. The presence of immatures

    and unbanded birds was recorced and missing group members

    noted. Immature birds were deemed to be adult in the

    September following their flEdging. Group composition

    was then determined from the census date nearest to the

    beginning of each month. Some data on group composition

    were also collected in 1982.

    - 21 -

  • 3.3 Results

    Although the study area was searched regularly,

    fairy-wrens were rarely recordedoutside of a few areas

    during the breeding season. These areas typically

    contained patches of brambles, sapling re-growth, woodpiles

    and fallen trees (see Chapter 4.)

    The total number of fairy-wrens recorded in

    monthly samples ranged from 13 to 56 birds and a mean

    monthly estimate of 36.2 birds (±10.8) was calculated.

    There were generally fewer birds recorded during the winter

    months (Table 3.1) but this was probably due to the wider

    ranging movements of groups at this Lime (see Chapter 4).

    The total number of fairy-wrens present in each breeding

    season was tabulated according to age, sex, and social

    status (Table 3.2). On occasions, censuses recorded groups

    without breeding males or females and so the number of

    breeding adults present did not always match the number of

    groups present (eg. in November 1982). The age structure

    of the population (Fig. 3.1) changed as immatures were added

    to the population, and as the number of immatures declined

    due to mortality and dispersal. In September, all immatures

    present were re-classified as adult birds.

    Group 0 showed a nuTiber of features typical of

    the changes that occurred in group composition (Fig. 3.2).

    Group 0 consisted initially of a breeding pair (30840 M and

    30839 F) and two female helpers (308:38 F and 30846 F).

    After the disappearance of the breeding male, the breeding

    - 22 -

  • position was filled by a male (44875 M) who arrived,

    together with a subordinate male (44877 M) from an unknown

    origin. The subordinate male remained as a helper until

    December 1983 after which it was not seen again. An unbanded

    female (later banded as 44844 F) arrived in the group and

    also acted as a helper but the origin of this female was not

    known. The two unrelated helpers (44877 M and 44844 F)

    left the group in October, and made several unsuccessful

    attempts at nesting together before returning to the group

    in January 1984. 44844 F left again but returned in February

    to fill the vacancy created by the disappearance of the

    previous breeding female. In June and July 1984, both

    breeding birds in Group 0 disappeared and were replaced by

    a breeding pair from a nearby group (Group P). Shortly

    after the arrival of this pair (30842 M, 56499 F) a resident

    immature male (44824 M) left to form a new group with an

    immigrant (unbanded) female. It is possible that the

    immature male had been usurped in his position by the older

    immigrant male, and may otherwise have inherited the

    territory.

    Another group history, that of Group M, is also

    illustrated in Fig. 3.2. Group M consisted initially of a

    breeding pair (30884 M, 18874 F) and a single male helper

    (18872 M). The male helper inherited the breeding position

    (and was joined by an immigrant female from Group L) after

    the disappearance of the breeding pair in July 1983. When

    the breeding male (18872 M) also disappeared in April 1984,

    the breeding position was inherited by a young male (44870 M).

    - 23 -

  • The pairing of this male with his mother was the only

    recorded instance of close inbreeding during this study.

    The relationship between breeders and helpers was

    known positively for only 11 helpers (9 males and 2 females).

    Most helpers were the offspring of at least one member

    of the breeding pair - the only exception being a male

    helping at the nest of an older male sibling (Table 3.4).

    Helpers were most frequent in the 1982/83 breeding

    season when from 55.6% to 83.3% (from monthly census records)

    of all groups had helpers with male and female helpers

    being about equally common. Fewer helpers were present in

    the 1983/84 breeding season when from 0.0% to 28.6% of

    groups had helpers, but again there were about equal

    numbers of male and female helpers present. In both the

    1984/85 and 1985/86 breeding seasons helpers were of

    intermediate frequency relative to other years when from

    22.2% to 62.5% of groups had helpers, of which virtually

    all were males. The only female helper present in the

    1984/85 or 1985/86 breeding season left shortly after the

    start of the breeding season.

    Group composition was further tabulated during

    the first nesting attempt of each pair. A total of 31.4%

    of pairs, in all breeding seasons, had helpers present at

    their first nest of the season but there was rarely more

    than one helper present in any group (Table 3.5).

    If it is assumed that the sex-ratio at fledging is

    1.00, then the number of first-year helpers present in each

    breeding season can be expressed as a percentage of the total

    - 24 -

  • number of potential helpers (ie. fledglings from the previous

    breeding season) present in the population. If it is also

    assumed that the mortality rate of immatures, over the non-

    breeding season, was similar in each study-year, then this

    figure represents the proportion of offspring that remained

    as helpers (Table 3.5).

    Fairy-wrens of known origin (ie. those banded as

    nestlings or immatures) could be aged accurately but many

    birds could only be assigned minimum ages based on the

    number of years they were seen in the study-site. In total,

    31 breeding males and 24 breeding females could be aged at

    the beginning of each breeding season (Table 3.3).

    Although females were more likely to breed in their first

    year than males (X 2 = 7.5, p < 0.01), this result is likely

    to be biased since most newly breeding females were of

    unknown origin and therefore could not be aged. Eleven

    male helpers could be aged at the first nest of each

    breeding season. The majority of male helpers (10 out of

    11) were one-year old and the other was two-years old.

    However, of the 7 male helpers that could not be aged

    accurately, 3 were known to he at least two-years old and

    1 was at least three-years old. Therefore, in total, 17 male

    helpers were at least one-year old, 4 were at least two-

    years old, and 1 was at least three-years old.

    Although none of the 9 female helpers could be

    aged accurately, 3 were present as helpers for two years in

    succession, making them at least two-years old.

    - 25 -

  • Dispersal could only be recorded when known

    individuals were again located in another area after an

    absence from their former group. In Table 3.6 dispersal

    events are tabulated separately for both sexes and for

    breeding birds and helpers. Few examples of dispersal by

    breeding males were recorded and in both cases the two

    males (31167 M and 30842 M) moved to fill a breeding

    vacancy in a nearby group just prior to the start of the

    breeding season. In contrast to the relative sedentariness

    of the males, however, 11 departures by breeding females

    were recorded. These movements occurred throughout the

    year but predominantly in July. Five of the females that

    dispersed (18830 F, 56498 F, 56009 F, 18874 F, 30849 F)

    moved after the absence and presumed death of their mate,

    five females (44844 F, 44900 F on three separate occasions,

    and 30867 F) left their mates;, and one (56499 F) moved

    together with her mate, to breed in a nearby unoccupied

    territory.

    Dispersal by helpers was most frequent just prior

    to, or early in, the breeding season (Table 3.6). Of the ten

    movements recorded for male helpers, nine occurred when they

    left their previous group to become breeders - forming new

    groups in unoccupied patches of habitat. Most of the

    females that joined these males were unbanded and had

    presumably come from outside of the study area. Only once

    was a male helper (30837 M) seen to leave one group and

    join another as a helper.

    -

  • Seven dispersal movements were recorded for

    female helpers of which two (78221 F, 56648 F) formed new

    groups within the study-area with males that had also

    previously been helpers, two (30881 F, 30858 F) left the

    study area completely. One female (30858 F) had also

    moved earlier, along with a breeding female, to join a

    nearby unpaired male.

    The distances moved by birds in dispersal

    movements were calculated in terms of territory diameters

    (approximately 100m). A movement to an adjacent territory

    was therefore counted as a movement of 1 territory diameter.

    Male fairy-wrens rarely moved more than 1 territory diameter

    in distance, but many females. moved 3 or more territory

    diameters when dispersing (Table 3.7).

    The rate of dispersal by helpers to areas outside

    the study area could not easily be determined because of the

    likelihood of mortality and the size of the area to be

    surveyed. However, an estimate of the sex ratio of

    dispersers and the timing of dispersal movements can be

    made if it is assumed that bards arriving in the study-

    site, after dispersal from outside areas, are similar in

    these respects to those leaving the study-site. During

    the three years a total of eight male arrivals in the study-

    site were recorded, seven of these just prior to the start

    of the breeding season (Table 3.6). Four of the males

    either replaced absent breeding males, or formed new

    breeding groups of their own. Two of the male arrivals

    disappeared after a short period, one arrived as a helper

    - 27 -

  • with a dominant male, and one immature male arrived, with

    a small group of others in MErch, before forming a new

    group in October.

    In contrast to the number of male arrivals, a

    total of 27 females arrived in the study-site during the

    three-year period. Most femE:les arrived in the population

    just after, or just before each breeding season. In

    contrast, adult male arrivals were only recorded in the

    months just prior to the breeding season (Table 3.6).

    Fourteen of the female arrivals replaced absent

    females in breeding groups, usually within a week after

    their disappearance, or formed new breeding groups with

    unpaired males. Thirteen females arrived as subordinates

    and remained so until the breeding season. Many of the male

    and female arrivals, particularly those arriving during

    the non-breeding season, entered the study-site as pre-

    formed groups. These groups then attached themselves to

    existing groups until the beginning of the breeding season.

    During the formation of breeding groups in July and August

    some of these birds remained to replace a missing breeder

    while others were not observed again.

    A total of 120 nestlings were banded in the 1982/83,

    1983/84, and 1984/85 breeding seasons of which 103 (85.8%)

    fledged successfully. Of these 103 fledglings, only 16

    (15 males and 1 female) were still present in the study-site

    in the following September. Six of the males were in

    breeding positions, three after inheriting the breeding

    position in their natal group and three after dispersal.

    - 28 -

  • Nine males were still in their natal group as helpers as

    was the single female still present. However not all

    nestlings were banded and these figures may be an under-

    estimate.

    For males which were still present as helpers

    in their natal group, breeding positions also arose during

    the breeding season either through the disappearance of

    the breeding male within the group, or through the

    occurrence of vacancies in nearby groups. Of all the

    helpers recorded, half (11 out of 22) of the males

    remained as helpers throughout the breeding season.

    Of those that found breeding positions, seven dispersed

    and one inherited within the group. Three of the male

    helpers disappeared, presumably dying or dispersing to an

    area outside of the study-site. However, few females (4

    out of 15) remained as helpers throughout the breeding

    season and most either left their natal group (6) or

    disappeared (5).

    Annual mortality rates were calculated for known

    birds according to sex and status (Table 3.8). Most of

    the mortality recorded for breeding males, breeding females

    and helper males occurred during the months of May, June

    and July (Fig. 3.3). Female helpers were not included in

    the analysis because of the possibility of dispersal to

    areas not covered by censuses. The annual mortality of

    breeding males (55.6%) and breeding females (56.0%) did not

    differ significantly, nor did the mortality of breeding

    or helping (50.0%) males (al± Chi-square tests n.s.).

  • Although mortality rates for immatures were

    difficult to calculate because some would have dispersed

    a minimum survivorship rate for immatures was calculated for

    the period from March (when the last nestlings of the

    breeding season had fledged) to the following August (after

    which immatures were classified as adults). In August 1983

    only two out of 11 (18.2%) immatures produced in the 1982/83

    breeding season were still present in the study-site. In

    August 1984 a total of 10 out of 22 (45.5%), and in August

    1985 a total of 6 out of 11 154.5%), immatures were still

    present in the study-site. The average survivorship (40.9%)

    was significantly lower than the 65.8% achieved by adults

    over the same period (X 2 = 4.1, p < 0.05).

    3.4 Discussion

    The Superb Fairy-wren is a typical 'helper-at-

    the-nest' species in which a single monogamous pair may be

    accompanied during the breeding season by non-breeding

    helpers, usually their offspring from previous generations.

    The construction of group histories and genealogies

    enabled the relationships between helpers and breeding birds

    to be determined in a number of cases. All helpers of

    known origin, except one, were the offspring of at least

    one member of the breeding pair, and often the offspring

    of them both. The only exception was the case of a male

    helper (18837 M) attending the nest of his older brother

    (92733 M). All male helpers were therefore related to the

    breeding male at least. However, the two female helpers

    - 30 -

  • for which genealogies could be constructed were both the

    offspring of the breeding female of the group, but unrelated

    to the male. Although the data are few for female helpers,

    there is the suggestion of a correlation between the sex

    of the helper and the sex of the breeding bird to which

    it is related. There are several reasons why this may be

    advantageous for both the breeders and helpers. For male

    breeders, at least, their inheritors as breeding birds are

    almost certain to be closely related (ie. their sons), thus

    ensuring some greater chance of successful breeding for

    their offspring and thereby improving their own fitness.

    However, this argument cannot be applied to female helpers

    as they did not inherit breecing positions within their

    natal group. Similarly, female helpers cannot be seen as

    being potential mates for male breeders because of their

    pre-breeding dispersal (cf. Rowley 1981). In addition, no

    evidence was collected which might suggest that super numerary

    females breed while acting aE: helpers. Only a single nest

    was ever discovered in a territory at any one time and female

    helpers were never observed incubating eggs or brooding

    nestlings. Furthermore, the number of eggs present in any

    nest never exceeded four (except when a fifth egg was found

    to be a cuckoo's egg); making it unlikely that more than one

    female laid in any one nest. Rowley (in Emlen and Vehrencamp

    1983) found that in M. splerLdens, supernumerary females may

    also build nests and lay eggs within the same territory as

    the dominant breeding female. These females may have been

    mated either with the dominant breeding male or one of the

    male helpers if these were present.

    - 31. -

  • The degree of relatedness between Superb Fairy-wren

    helpers and the offspring that they helped to raise could

    only be determined for those birds of known origin. When

    helpers are the offspring of both parents, for example, they

    have a minimum relatedness coefficient of 0.5 and when they

    are the offspring (or sibling) of only one of the parents

    their coefficient of relatedness is 0.25 (eg. Emlen 1978).

    The average relatedness of helpers to offspring at Eastwood

    was therefore calculated to be 0.34. Rowley calculated a

    similar coefficient (0.36) for M. splendens but no figure

    is available for his study of M. cyaneus at Gungahlin.

    Woolfenden and Fitzpatrick (1984) compared the

    average relatedness (r ) of Florida Scrub Jay helpers with a

    figure calculated by the folThwing equation :

    p r . S x T

    Where r between full sibs is 0.5, S is the annual

    survivorship of adults and T is the age of the helpers.

    This equation gives an average relatedness between helpers

    and offspring which would be predicted if helpers remained

    within their natal groups, regardless of the degree of

    relatedness between them and their beneficiaries. Using this

    reasoning, Woolfenden and Fitzpatrick (ibid. pp 86-89) showed

    that the average relatedness between Florida Scrub Jay helpers

    was almost exactly the same as predicted, suggesting that the

    helpers remained regardless of their degree of relatedness

    to the breeders or their offspring.

  • Using the method of Woolfenden and Fitzpatrick

    (1984), the predicted degree of relatedness among superb

    Fairy-wrens (r p = 0.22) was found to be considerably lower

    than the actual figure obtained for helpers in the population

    (r a = 0.34). This may indicate that helpers were unlikely to

    remain in their natal group unless they were closely related

    to the offspring of the breeding pair. As pointed out earlier,

    helpers were always themselves the offspring (or sibling)

    of at least one of the breeding pair (ie. r a was at least

    0.25).

    Immature: and former helpers acquired breeding

    positions either by dispersal_ into unoccupied patches of

    habitat or into established (jroups which had lost one

    member of the breeding pair. In addition, males also

    inherited the breeding position within their natal group

    following the death, or disappearance, of the breeding male.

    Of the males banded as nestlings, and still present on the

    study-site as adults, 60% started their first year of adult-

    hood as helpers while 20% inherited their breeding positions

    and 20% dispersed and bred outside of the group. During the

    breeding season 50% of the male helpers remained as helpers

    while less than 5% inherited and about 32% dispersed.

    In summary, of the males that survived to their first

    breeding season, about 30% remained as helpers throughout

    the breeding season while about 23% inherited a breeding

    position and 39% dispersed to breed outside of the group.

    Given that the remaining percentage of males died before

    breeding, then each male has about a 62% chance of breeding

    - 3:i -

  • in his first year. The data for females are quite different

    with only a single banded female still present on the study-

    site in her first year as a helper in her natal group. Some

    females, however, were already helpers at the beginning of

    the study and, of these, 26% remained as helpers throughout

    the season. The number of female helpers varied between the

    breeding seasons, being notably absent in the 1984/85 and

    1985/86 breeding seasons.

    One of the major d*fferences between male and

    female fairy-wrens was their tendency to disperse. Male

    fairy-wrens, for example, rarely moved after they had

    acquired a breeding position and any movement was almost

    invariably to an adjacent unoccupied territory. If the

    breeding female disappeared, then the male remained on the

    territory and was later joined by a new female. Most of the

    newly breeding females arrived within a week of a breeding

    vacancy occurring and most were unbanded, indicating that

    they probably originated outside the study-site. In contrast,

    many females left their terr:_tories at some point in their

    life as a breeding adult, us ually to occupy a breeding

    position in another group. Cases of 'divorce' in which a

    female left her mate to breed with another were not uncommon,

    in contrast to other co-operative breeding species where it

    is virtually unknown (Rowley 1983).

    An estimate of the sex-ratio of dispersing birds

    was made on the basis of those birds entering the population.

    Females were found to be 3.4 times more likely to enter the

    population from areas outside the study-site and this figure

    - 34 -

  • is similar to the estimate of 3.5 female to male arrivals

    made on the basis of figures reported in Rowley's study at

    Gungahlin. It is likely that dispersal entails some risk.

    Birds moving through unfamiliar areas, mostly on their own

    or with unrelated birds, miglt be expected to experience a

    greater risk of mortality from predation or starvation, than

    non-dispersers or those that move with their natal group.

    Females often arrived alone just prior to the

    breeding season, usually filling a breeding vacancy in one

    of the local groups. The speed with which many of these

    vacancies were filled suggests that females may continually

    monitor breeding vacancies in the population. However, it

    was not clear if females were 'floating' in the population

    or if they made occasional forays from their natal groups.

    In the non-breeding season, dispersive forays may also occur

    as a group wanders over its :_arger non-breeding season

    home range (see Chapter 4). In other species (eg. Florida

    Scrub Jays, Woolfenden and Fitzpatrick 1984; White-browed

    Sparrow Weavers Plocepasser mahali, Lewis 1982) it has

    been argued that group wanderings may provide opportunities

    for birds to monitor breeding vacancies in the population,

    while remaining in contact with their natal group.

    In two of the breeding seasons (1984/85 and 1985/86)

    there was a skew in the sex-ratio of the population with

    males being about 1.7 times more abundant than females

    at the start of the season. Unfortunately, I was not able

    to determine the sex of many immatures prior to their

    dispersal as problems were ercountered in trying to locate

    - 35 -

  • groups with immatures in the winter months, and in

    identification. Many immatures tentatively identified as

    'females' later turned out to be males and it was also

    difficult to separate adult: females from immatures on the

    basis of plumage. At Gungahlin, however, Rowley (1965)

    found similar numbers of male and female immatures. The

    skewed sex-ratio might be better explained as a result of

    greater female dispersal. IL is suggested that females

    suffer higher mortality because of their greater tendency

    to disperse. It is also likely that since the Eastwood

    and Gungahlin populations were surrounded by (mostly)

    unsuitable habitat, then each study-site might be considered

    as an 'island'. Since the number of females arriving on

    the 'island' might not equal the number leaving, as the

    birds disperse outwards from a source, then this may

    further reduce the number of females in the population.

    Although mortality rates at Eastwood were higher than those

    at Gungahlin (data analysed in Rowley 1981), there were

    no significant differences in the mortality rate of male

    and female breeders. This would also suggest that the

    origin of the skewed sex-ratio is in the greater mortality

    of females during dispersal, or the imbalance in the number

    of female emigrants and immigrants due to the 'island'

    effect.

    Some of the demographic attributes of the fairy-

    wren population seem likely Lo severely curtail the

    reproductive opportunities available to potential breeders.

    Included in these attributes are the continuous residence of

    - 36 -

  • adults on breeding territories, the 'surplus' production of

    offspring relative to the occurrence of breeding vacancies,

    and a skewed sex-ratio (Emlen 1984, Emlen and Vehrencamp

    1983, Woolfenden and Fitzpatrick 1984, Wiley and Rabenold

    1984, among others). The frequency with which breeding

    opportunities arise in the population will therefore

    depend on (i) the current level of resource monopolization

    by breeding birds (particularly if territories and/or

    mates are in short supply), (ii) breeder mortality, and

    (iii) competitor natality and survivorship (Brown 1969,

    1974, Ricklefs 1975).

    In the following model (adapted from Woolfenden

    and Fitzpatrick 1984) some of the factors which may

    promote delayed dispersal among potential breeders are

    considered in more detail.

    Individuals are expected to follow a delayed

    dispersal strategy ( h ) when the expected lifetime

    fitness (Wh ) gained from suct. a strategy is greater than

    that expected from an early ddispersal strategy (W 0 ), ie.

    when :

    Wh > W o

    and when :

    ph Rh > L 0 . Do . Ro

    where lifetime fitness ( W ) = Li .Di .R and Li = the

    probability of survival to dispersal age i, Di = the

    probability of successful establishment as a breeder at

    age i, and R = the total number of offspring expected from

    the first year of breeding onwards.

    37 -

  • The equation can bE . expanded by the inclusion of

    the term ( ! ) which is a meEsure of the additional fitness

    gained through helping behaviour; ie. through the

    production of extra relative: (Vehrencamp 1979, Brown

    1980), thus ;

    L . . (R h -I k ) > Lo.D0.R0.

    The ratio between the survivE.1 of late versus early dispersers

    (ie.L.=Lh/Lo) can then be included in the equation thus ;

    L 1 (Dh.Rh + Fz ) Do.R0-

    The ratio between the probabilities of successful late

    versus early dispersal (I) can also be defined (ie. I =

    D h/D o) and the ratio between the lifetime reproductive

    success of early versus late dispersers (ie. R = R0/Rh).

    If both of these terms are included in the equation, then

    it can be expressed in the following form ;

    (1 - L i . I) D 0 < L i . f2/R.

    The cost of delayed dispersal is therefore reduced by the

    inclusion of I in the equation. If the term (1 - Li.I)

    is negative (ie. I is large), then delayed dispersal is

    favoured under all permutations of L i , K, D 0 and R because

    the right-hand side is always greater than, or equal to,

    zero. The equation in this form produces a very interesting

    result; delayed dispersal can be favoured even in the

    absence of any indirect benefits from helping behaviour

    (ie. k = 0). It is even possible for k to be negative,

    if I is sufficiently large, and delayed dispersal to still

    - 38 -

  • be favoured. The model in the form expressed above predicts

    that delayed dispersal should be favoured over earlier

    dispersal and breeding when ;

    (i) I is greater than 1.0 ; ie. when delayed

    dispersal results in a greater probability of success at

    acquiring a breeding position. Among Superb Fairy-wrens

    this condition is most likely to be met when the probability

    of early success is low (due to a lack of breeding vacancies

    or mates in the population) or when success can be enhanced

    by remaining for a period in the natal group (as in

    situations where inheritance can occur).

    (ii) Li > 1.0; ie. when the survivorship of

    late dispersers to the age of first breeding (eg. the first

    year) is greater than the survivorship of earlier dispersers

    to the same age. In other words, delayed dispersal will

    be favoured if the survivorship of individuals in their

    natal group is greater than their survivorship outside of

    the group. This should most obviously be the case for

    immatures that spend their first winter with their natal

    group. For older birds, however, and particularly for

    males, this advantage might diminish in relationship to the

    cost of non-breeding.

    (iii) When (z > 0; ie. when non-dispersers

    are able to enhance their indirect fitness through the

    production of extra relatives. This condition would only

    be met at Eastwood when helpers were able to enhance the

    reproductive success of the breeders and this point is

    considered in Chapter 6.

    - 39 -

  • In summary, there are several reasons why non-

    dispersal by potential breeders may be favoured. In this

    Chapter some aspects of the population biology of fairy-

    wrens at Eastwood have been discussed. In particular I have

    suggested that the probability that individuals can acquire

    a breeding position within the population is an important

    influence on the costs and benefits of delayed dispersal.

    If potential breeders are unable to obtain suitable

    breeding positions, then they may benefit from remaining

    within the natal group until conditions improve. Two of

    the most important benefits to delayed dispersal are

    (i) potentially better survivorship, and (ii) the

    opportunity to inherit a breeding position within the

    natal group itself. In addition there are several factors

    which may lower the probability of successful establishment

    by potential breeders including (iii) a lack of breeding

    vacancies within the population because of a 'surplus'

    production of offspring in previous seasons, and (iv) a

    lack of sexual partners (in this case females) due to

    differential mortality of the sexes at some stage of their

    life-history.

  • Table

    3.1

    Mon

    thly

    cen

    su

    s r

    ecord

    s o

    f fa

    iry-w

    ren

    s a

    t E

    astw

    ood S

    .F.

    Year

    Cate

    gory

    Jan

    Feb

    Mar

    Apri

    lM

    ayJu

    ne

    Ju

    lyA

    ug

    Sep

    tO

    ctN

    ovD

    ec

    1982

    Nu

    mber

    of;

    Adu

    lts

    --

    --

    --

    --

    4442

    5554

    Imm

    atu

    res

    --

    --

    --

    --

    40

    02

    Un

    know

    ns

    --

    --

    --

    --

    10

    20

    Tota

    l Popu

    lati

    on

    --

    --

    --

    --

    4542

    5756

    Nu

    mber

    of

    Gro

    ups

    --

    --

    --

    --

    1214

    1816

    1983

    Nu

    mber

    of;

    Adu

    lts

    4940

    4044

    3916

    2629

    3334

    3230

    Imm

    atu

    res

    02

    138

    101

    50

    00

    1721

    Un

    kn

    own

    s0

    00

    01

    00

    10

    00

    0

    Tota

    l popu

    lati

    on

    4942

    5350

    5020

    3030

    3334

    4951

    Nu

    mber

    of

    Gro

    ups

    1512

    1213

    125

    911

    1415

    1514

  • Table

    3.1

    (Con

    t'd)

    Year

    Cate

    gory

    Jan

    Feb

    Mar

    Apri

    lM

    ayJu

    ne

    Ju

    lyA

    ug

    Sep

    tO

    ctN

    ovD

    ec

    1984

    Nu

    mber

    of;

    Adu

    lts

    2823

    1723

    2023

    1417

    2524

    2229

    Imm

    atu

    res

    2824

    2228

    2326

    1311

    06

    1811

    Un

    know

    ns

    00

    00

    01

    10

    00

    00

    Tota

    l popu

    lati

    on

    5647

    3951

    4351

    2828

    2530

    4030

    Nu

    mber

    of gro

    ups

    1412

    911

    1013

    98

    89

    99

    1985

    Nu

    mber

    of;

    Adu

    lts

    1913

    1315

    177

    2227

    2228

    2525

    Imm

    atu

    res

    1915

    11 IIn

    n .-2n

    0 u..)

    00

    312

    Un

    know

    ns

    20

    22

    22

    20

    00

    00

    Tota

    l popu

    lati

    on

    4028

    2626

    2718

    3330

    2228

    2837

    Nu

    mber

    of gro

    ups

    96

    56

    73

    77

    911

    1111

  • Table

    3.2

    Com

    posit

    ion

    of

    bre

    edin

    g

    gro

    ups in

    each

    mon

    th

    Bre

    edin

    g s

    eason

    Cate

    gory

    Sep

    tO

    ctN

    ovD

    ecJan

    Feb

    1982/8

    3

    Nu

    mber

    of;

    Bre

    edin

    g m

    ale

    s12

    1416

    1615

    12

    Bre

    edin

    g f

    em

    ale

    s11

    1417

    1514

    12

    Help

    er

    male

    s10

    713

    1313

    11

    Help

    er

    fem

    ale

    s11

    79

    107

    5

    Tota

    l adu

    lts

    4442

    4954

    5940

    Tota

    l im

    matu

    res

    uu

    02

    02

    Adu

    lt s

    ex-r

    ati

    ol

    1.0

    1.0

    0.9

    0.9

    0.8

    0.7

    Nu

    mber

    of

    gro

    ups w

    ith

    A CI

    1 1

    1 f

    hP 1

    p P

    ■ r q

    7R

    10

    1111

    ln

    Tota

    l n

    um

    ber

    of

    gro

    ups

    1214

    1816

    1512

    1.

    Nu

    mber

    of

    fem

    ale

    s p

    er

    male

    .

  • Table

    3.2

    (Con

    t'd)

    Bre

    edin

    g S

    eason

    Cate

    gory

    Sep

    tO

    ctN

    ovD

    ecJan

    Feb

    1983/8

    4

    Nu

    mbe

    rof;

    Bre

    edin

    g m

    ale

    s14

    1515

    1414

    11

    Bre

    edin

    g f

    em

    ale

    s14

    1515

    1414

    12H

    r.,1

    17,r

    ,,,,a

    lnc

    -)n .,

    n

    Help

    er

    fem

    ale

    s3

    31

    10

    0

    Tota

    l adu

    lts

    3334

    3230

    2823

    Tota

    l im

    matu

    r es

    n Vn V

    17-)

    14.

    1-)

    0G

    U')A 4..

    .t

    Adu

    lt s

    ex-r

    ati

    ol

    1.1

    1.1

    1.0

    1.0

    1.0

    1.1

    Nu

    mber

    of

    gro

    ups w

    ith

    adu

    lt h

    elp

    ers

    41

    11

    00

    Tota

    l n

    um

    ber

    of

    gro

    ups

    1415

    1514

    1412

    1.

    Nu

    mber

    of

    fem

    ale

    s p

    er

    male

    .

  • Table

    3.2

    (C

    on

    t'd)

    Bre

    edin

    g s

    eason

    Cate

    gory

    Sep

    tO

    ctN

    ovD

    ecJa

    nFeb

    1984/85

    Nu

    mber

    of;

    Bre

    edin

    g m

    ale

    s8

    99

    99

    6

    Bre

    edin

    g f

    em

    ale

    s8

    99

    77

    5

    Help

    er

    male

    s8

    64

    33

    2

    Help

    er

    fem

    ale

    s1

    00

    00

    0

    Tota

    l adu

    lts

    2524

    2219

    1913

    Tc,L

    al im

    atu

    r es

    n L,r

    1 0

    ■,.

    1 1

    10_..

    1

    Adu

    lt s

    ex-r

    ati

    ol

    0.6

    0.6

    0.7

    0.6

    0.6

    0.6

    Nu

    mber

    of

    gro

    ups w

    ith

    adu

    lt h

    elp

    ers

    55

    32

    22

    Tota

    l n

    um

    ber

    of

    gro

    ups

    89

    99

    96

    1.

    Nu

    mber

    of

    fem

    ale

    s p

    er

    male

    .

  • Tab

    le 3

    .2

    (Cont

    Id)

    Bre

    ed

    in

    g s

    easo

    n

    Cate

    gory

    Sep

    tO

    ctN

    ovD

    ecJa

    n

    1985/8

    6N

    um

    ber

    of

    ;

    Bre

    ed

    ing m

    ale

    s9

    13

    1111

    11

    Bre

    edin

    g f

    em

    ale

    s8

    1211

    1111

    Help

    er

    male

    s5

    33

    33

    He

    1p

    er f

    em

    ale

    s0

    00

    00

    i 1.t. cn

    Tota

    l adu

    lts

    rri

    - 1

    J. V L

    . a _L

    a L.

    22

    0

    28

    0

    25

    3

    2525

    Ad

    ult

    sex

    -r a

    t io

    l0

    . 60

    . 80

    .80

    .80.8

    Num

    ber

    of

    gro

    up

    s w

    ith

    help

    ers

    53

    33

    3

    To

    tal

    nu

    mb

    er

    of

    gro

    up

    s9

    13

    1111

    11

    Feb

    1.

    Num

    ber

    of

    fem

    ale

    s p

    er

    male

    .

  • Table 3 .3 Ales of known individuals at the f irst nest

    of all breeding seasons.

    Age in years Breed Lngmales

    N

    Breedingfemales

    N

    Helpermales

    N

    He 1perfemale s 1

    N

    (%) ( % ) (%) ( %)

    1 8 16 10 0

    (25.3) (66.7) (90.9) (00.0)

    2 9 3 1 0

    (29. ) (12.5) (09.1) (00.0)

    3 + 14 5 0 0

    45.2) (20.8) (00.0) (00.0)

    Total ofknown age 31 24 11 0

    Total ofunknown age 20 27 7 9

    1. No female helpers cou ld be accurate ly aged .

    - 47 -

  • Table 3 .4 Summary of the relationships between adult

    helpers and breeders .

    Re lat ions h ip ofhelper to breeder

    Male Female Coefficient

    He 1per Helper ofRelatedness(r )

    Offspring of ;

    both breeders 4 0 0.50

    male breeder only 4 0 0.25

    female breeder only 0 2 0.25

    neither breeder 0 0 0.00

    Sibling Of ;*male breeder 1 0 0. 50

    female breeder 0 0 0.50

    Unknown 11 7

    Total 20 9 0.34

    * half sibling (r = 0.25 )

    - 48 -

  • Table

    3.5

    Th

    e c

    om

    posit

    ion

    of

    bre

    edin

    g

    gro

    ups a

    t th

    e

    firs

    t n

    est

    of

    each

    bre

    edin

    g s

    eason

    .

    Bre

    edin

    g s

    eason

    Nu

    mber

    of

    gro

    ups

    Nu

    mber

    of

    Help

    ers

    01

    23+

    Mea

    n n

    um

    ber

    Sex-r

    ati

    o o

    fper

    gro

    up

    adu

    lt h

    elp

    ers

    Rete

    nti

    on

    of

    offspri

    ng a

    sh

    elp

    ers

    1

    Male

    s F

    emale

    s

    11982/8

    3

    15

    73

    32

    1.1

    3

    9F:1

    0M

    75-1

    00%

    75-1

    00%

    ,... co

    1983/8

    4

    15

    13

    1 1

    00.2

    0

    2F:1

    M

    23%

    11%

    11984/8

    5

    95

    31

    00.5

    6

    OF:

    4M

    15%

    0%

    1985/8

    6

    12

    93

    0 0

    0.2

    5

    OF:

    3M

    18%

    0%

    Tota

    l51

    35

    95

    20.5

    3

    11F:1

    8M

    1.

    Esti

    mate

    d %

    of

    all p

    revio

    us s

    eason

    's o

    ffsprin

    g r

    em

    ain

    ing a

    s h

    elp

    ers a

    t th

    e s

    tart

    of

    each

    bre

    edin

    g s

    eason

    (ie

    . S

    epte

    mber)

    .

  • Table

    3 .

    6

    Th

    e t

    imin

    g o

    f dis

    pers

    al by f a

    iry-w

    ren

    s a

    ccord

    in

    g t

    o s

    ex a

    nd s

    tatu

    s.

    Cate

    gory

    of

    movem

    en

    tJ

    FM

    AM

    JJ

    AS

    0N

    DTota

    l

    Bir

    ds leavin

    g g

    rou

    ps

    Bre

    ed in

    g m

    ale

    s0

    00

    00

    01

    10

    00

    02

    Bre

    edin

    g f

    em

    ale

    s1

    10

    10

    14

    21

    00

    011

    He

    1per

    male

    s0

    00

    00

    10

    34

    11

    010

    Help

    er

    fem

    ale

    s0

    00

    00

    02

    21

    10

    17

    Tota

    l1

    10

    10

    27

    86

    21

    130

    Bir

    ds a

    rriv

    ing :

    Male

    s0

    01

    00

    00

    43

    00

    08

    Fem

    ale

    s3

    04

    50

    14

    41

    50

    027

    Un

    kn

    own

    s1

    02

    01

    12

    00

    00

    07

    Tota

    l4

    07

    51

    27

    84

    50

    043

  • Table 3 . 7 The distance moved by dispersing fairy-wrens

    as measured in territory diameters (1 territorydiameter approximately = 100m) .

    Sex of disperser

    1

    Dispersal Distance(territory diameters)

    2 3 4 5 or more

    Males

    Females

    7

    2

    2

    0

    0

    3

    1

    0

    0

    4

  • Table 3.8 Mortality of known individuals over three years

    'January 1983 to January 1986)

    Category Number of birds present AnnualMortality

    Jan. Jan.next

    Breeding males 3( 16 55.6%

    Breeding females 11 56.0%

    Male helpers 14 7 50.0%

    Total 34 54.7%

  • F ig . 3.1 Change in population age structure from

    September 1984 to January 1986.

  • adults

    > 1

    yr

    old

    1st

    - y

    ear

    adults

    i mm

    atur

    es

    SN

    JM

    M

    JS

    NJ

    MO

    NTH

  • Fig. 3.2 The history of two groups (0 and M) during

    the study period. Each individual is identified by the

    C.S.I.R.O. band number . De at h , or disappearance , of

    individuals indicated by 3ar re d line -I ) .

    Arrows indicate movements between groups or change of

    status .

  • GRO

    UP 0

    Cat

    egor

    y19

    82

    1983

    ON

    D J F M

    A M

    J J

    A

    1984

    1985

    S0 N

    DJ

    F M

    A M

    J J A SO

    ND

    J FM

    A M

    J J A SO

    N

    D

    Bre

    edin

    g30

    840

    mal

    e

    Bre

    ed in

    g 3

    -fe

    male

    0839

    Help

    er

    mal

    e

    Help

    er

    30838

    fem

    ale

    30846

    I m

    mat

    ures

    -4 4

    4875

    -1 1

    4-4

    844--

    ----

    -j56499-

    h

    448

    77 -

    ----

    -

    2 2

    1 1

    44

    844

    3 2

    4 3

    1 14

    30842 - -------------------- --- ---

    56611-----

    4 4

    8 6

    6

    4 4

    6

    ub

    GRO

    UP

    fem

    ale

    -I

    44

    82

    4

    GRO

    UP

    GRO

    UP

    J

    2 ub

    mal

    es

    GRO

    UP

    P

    1.

    see

    text

    for

    det

    ails

  • 4487

    0-1

    1887

    2

    -1 3

    0989

    _I

    I

    4 3

    Bree

    ding

    3088

    4m

    aleBr

    eedin

    gfe

    mal

    e 18

    874

    Helpe

    r18

    872

    male

    Helpe

    rfe

    mal

    eI m

    matur

    esOt

    hers

    3098

    9 F

    GROU

    PL

    4 4

    4 2

    2 3

    3 3

    5649

    3 —

    1

    u bF I

    i

    Ub

    u bfe

    male

    fem

    ale

    GROU

    P M

    1983

    1984

    Cat

    egor

    y J

    FM

    A

    MJ

    JA

    SO

    ND

    J F

    M

    AM

    JJ

    AS

    ON

    D

  • Fig . 3.3 Distr ibut ion of mortality recorded throughout

    the year . Data inc lude s breeding males , breed ing females ,

    and male helpers. Data from January 1983 to December 1986.

  • c_

    MONTHLY MORTALITY (70 MISSING)

    O tv cn Oo 0 Iv -P. 0) oo 0

    1

    O

  • CHAPTER 4

    HABITAT AND SOCIAL ORGANISATION

    4.1 Introduction

    The type of habitat occupied by species is one

    focus of attention in the study of co-operative breeding.

    The habitat saturation model, for example, predicts that

    delayed dispersal is only favoured when suitable breeding

    habitat is fully occupied by breeding birds.

    A number of studies have attempted to measure the

    influence of habitat variables, particularly vegetation

    structure, and its influence on social organisation. Group

    size in co-operative breeding species may be influenced

    by vegetation structure (Brown and Balda 1977, Gaston

    1978b, Vehrencamp 1978, Craig 1979, Brown et al. 1983,

    Zack and Ligon 1985a, 1985b) and the quality of breeding

    territories may have an impertant influence on group

    reproductive success and the reproductive options for young

    birds (eg. Lewis 1981, Zack and Ligon 1985a, 1985b) as well

    as adult survival.

    An important consequence of any correlation

    between group size and some measure of territory quality

    (such as food abundance or t:he availability of nest-sites)

    is the additional influence of territory quality on

    reproductive success. It may therefore be difficult to

    separate the effects of territory quality and the presence,

    or number, of helpers on reproductive success (Lack 1968,

    - 56

  • Zahavi 1974, Brown 1978, Brown and Brown 1981, Lewis 1981,

    Woolfenden and Fitzpatrick 1984, Zack and Ligon 1985a).

    In order to understand the evolution of co-operative

    breeding systems more fully, it is therefore necessary to

    determine the proximate factors that influence social

    variables such as group size.

    In the present study an attempt was made to

    determine how certain habitat variables may influence

    the social organisation of fairy-wrens in the Eastwood

    population. A number of habitat variables were measured

    and tested for correlation with group size. Territory

    size was measured and differences in territorial behaviour

    in the breeding and non-breeding season are described.

    The following questions were considered :

    (1) What are the characteristics of territories occupied

    by fairy-wrens at Eastwood?

    (2) How is suitable breeding habitat distributed through

    the study area?

    (3) Does the distribution or abundance of suitable habitat

    influence the dispersion, demography or social

    organisation of the population?

    (4) Does habitat influence the reproductive opportunities

    of potential breeders?

  • 4.2 Methods

    During periodic censuses of group composition,

    and at other times when fairy-wrens were encountered, the

    location of each group was recorded relative to a 50m x 50m

    grid established throughout the study-site. Only one such

    record was made for any one group within the same day.

    During the occasional times when individual fairy-wrens

    were separated from other group members (eg. during

    incubation by the female), the location of the breeding

    male was used as the group record.

    Territorial boundaries and the area covered

    by each territory (TAREA) were determined after twenty or

    more records of group locat_ons had been made. Territorial

    disputes were infrequently observed and no systematic

    attempt was made to use thi3 behaviour to determine

    territorial boundaries.

    The composition and number of fairy-wrens present

    in different groups varied within, and between, seasons and

    years (Chapter 3). Group size (GSIZE) was therefore

    defined as the maximum number of adult fairy-wrens present

    on a territory after breeding groups had been formed.

    In the 1982/83 an3 1983/84 breeding seasons

    a number of habitat variables were measured in each

    territory. Variables were chosen on the basis of being

    potential determinants of the suitability of territories

    for occupation and reproduction. Of particular interest

    were features of the habitat used for nest--sites (brambles,

    - 58 -

  • woodpiles and trees), and foraging sites (mostly small trees,

    acacias, and open ground, see Chapter 5). No attempt was

    made to determine the relative abundance of food (eg.

    arthropods) in different terfitories.

    The number of trees in twenty 20m quadrats within

    each territory was recorded in three separate categories :

    NTREE01 = the number of trees (other than Acacia spp) 0 to 5m

    in height; NTREE02 = the number of trees (other than

    Acacia spp) over 5m in height and; NACACIA = the number of

    acacia trees. The presence of acacias (89% of which were

    recorded as being less than 5m in height) was recorded

    separately since they seemed to be preferred as foraging

    sites over eucalypts (H.A. Ford pers comm., also Chapter 5).

    A line-intercept method (Lucas and Seber 1977) was

    used to estimate the total area covered by woodpiles, fallen

    trees and dead branches within each territory (AWOOD) and

    the proportion of the territory covered (PWOOD). The total

    area of blackberry brambles (BAREA) was measured for each

    territory, and the proportion of the territory covered by

    brambles (PBAREA) was calculated. A percentage canopy

    cover (CCOVER) was estimated visually from forty sample-

    points within each territory.

    In the 1984/85 and 1985/86 breeding season

    measurements were made of group size (GSIZE), territory area

    (TAREA) and bramble area pe. ) f territory (BAREA) only.

    Habitat variables were tested for correlation with

    GSIZE by determining the relevant Pearson correlation

    coefficient. Percentile and proportionate variables were

    transformed when appropriate using the arcsin transformation

    (Sokal and Rolf 1969).

    - 59 -

  • 4.3 Results

    Fairy-wren territories at Eastwood S.F. were found

    in areas of disturbed vegetation usually along forest roads,

    on the edge of the forest ad 4 oining cleared land, and

    along temporary water courses (Fig. 4.1). These areas

    appeared to contain larger areas of dense sapling regrowth,

    fallen trees, woodpiles and brambles than the less disturbed

    areas of the forest. The distribution of brambles in

    particular, coincided with the distribution of fairy-wren

    territories (Figs. 4.2, 4.3, 4.4, 4.5).

    Table 4.1 presents the values obtained for

    measurements of group size, territory area, and the density

    of birds in occupied areas. There were no obvious relation-

    ships between these measures in comparisons between breeding

    seasons.

    A summary of the habitat variables measured in

    each of the breeding seasons is presented in Table 4.2.

    Territory area was inversely related to group size in the

    1982/83 breeding season (r = -0.47, p < 0.05), but no

    significant correlations bet4een group size and territory

    area were found in the other breeding seasons.

    An inverse correlation was found between

    territory area and the area of brambles per territory

    in the 1982/83 breeding season, but not in other seasons.

    There were positive correlations between territory area

    and several of the habitat variables (NACACTA, AWOOD,

    CCOVER) measured in the 1982/83 breeding season (Table 4.3).

    - 60 -

  • A number of changes in the position and size of

    particular territories in different years were noted and

    can be seen in a comparison of figs. 4.2, 4.3, 4.4 and 4.5.

    These changes are summarised as follows :

    (1) The extinction of territories after the death or

    disappearance of the occupants (groups A, L, in Fig.

    4.2; groups Q, C2, Cl, B, V in Fig. 4.3; group J,

    in Fig. 4.4).

    (2) The creation (or re-creation) of new territories

    when breeding pairs occupied a formerly vacant patch

    of habitat (groups Q, X, in Fig. 4.3; groups V, W, B,

    Q in Fig. 4.5).

    (3) The extension of territorial boundaries to encompass

    vacated patches of habitat (groups F, H, V, R, T in

    Fig. 4.3; group 0 in Fig. 4.4).

    (4) The splitting of a single territory into two smaller

    territories (groups H1, H2 in Fig. 4.2; groups Cl, C2

    in Fig. 4.3; groups Ml, M2 in Fig. 4.5). The

    circumstances surrounding the split of territory H

    into two separate territories in the 1982/83 breeding

    season (Fig. 4.2) were unknown. However, in both

    other cases (group C in 1983/84 breeding season,

    group M in 1985/86 breeding season) the split occurred

    when a male helper and a newly arrived female took

    over part of the territory of the group for their

    exclusive use. In both cases the areas used by the

    newly formed pair were on the periphery of the former

    single territory and did not contain substantial areas

    - 61 -

  • of brambles. Neither of the newly formed pairs

    produced independent fledglings and in one case (group

    C2) the male returned to his former group at the

    conclusion of the breeding season, after the

    disappearance of his maze.

    Territorial behaviour differed in the non-breeding

    season (March to August) from the pattern found in the

    breeding season (September to February). Territorial

    boundaries were less well defended in the non-breeding

    season as fairy-wrens foraged over a larger area, often

    leaving their breeding territories for much of the day and

    only returning in the evening to roost. It was common for

    several groups of fairy-wrens to forage together in

    temporary feeding aggregations, or in more permanent

    'super-groups' (Rowley 1965) and mixed species flocks

    were frequently observed.

    Each of the habitat variables measured in the

    1982/83 breeding season was tested for correlation with

    GSIZE (Table 4.4). The significant habitat variables

    (BAREA, and PBAREA) were both measures of the amount of

    blackberry brambles present within a territory.

    In the four breeding seasons, nine groups of

    fairy-wrens were found on territories without significant

    (ie. greater than lm in area) patches of brambles. All of

    these groups were simple pairs without helpers.

    Although several of the habitat variables

    measured features of the habitat that seemed typical of

    fairy-wren territories (eg. dense sapling growth, the

  • presence of acacias and woodpiles) none of these other

    variables was correlated with GSIZE. A backward - stepping,

    multiple-regression analysis (Nie et al. 1975) using all

    habitat variables again showed the importance of brambles

    as a determinant of group size (GSIZE = 2.55 + 1.06 PBAREA,

    F(1,17

    ) = 19.30, P < 0.001).

    In the 1984/85 breeding season a positive, but

    non-significant correlation tetween GSIZE and BAREA (r = 0.60 ,

    0.05 < p < 0.10) was also found but in the 1983/84 and

    1985/86 breeding seasons the number of groups with helpers

    was too few to make compariscns.

    In order to determine why territories with a

    large area of brambles present generally held larger groups,

    a test of the survivorship of fairy-wrens was made.

    Territories were divided intc, groups containing small (0 to

    10m2 ), medium (10 to 100m2 ) End large (over 100m2 ) areas

    of brambles. The percentage of fairy-wrens surviving from

    one breeding season to the next was then calculated for each

    of the three categories (Table 4.5). Although a trend

    towards higher survivorship was seen with increasing

    bramble size, the differences; between categories were not

    significant (X2 = 3.9, P > 0.05). However survivorship

    is only one of the possible determinants of group size and

    others such as reproductive success of the breeders and

    the frequency of dispersal of immatures a