Download - Bimanual Hand Writing in Right-handed and Left-handed Stutterers and Nonstutterers

Transcript
  • Neuropsychologin, Vol. 24, No. 3, pp. 44-447. 1986 Pnnted in Great Britain.

    0028-3932/86 S3.CH3f0.00 Per!gmon Journals Ltd.

    NOTE

    BIMANUAL HAND WRITING IN RIGHT-HANDED AND LEFT-HANDED STUTTERERS AND NONSTUTTERERS

    JAY R. GREINER,* HIRAM E. FITzcERALD*t and PAUL A. COOKE~

    *Department of Psychology and 1 Department of Audiology and Speech Sciences, Michigan State University, MI 48824. U.S.A.

    (Accepted 10 August 1985)

    Abstract-Right-handed stutterers performed more poorly with their nondominant hand when writing digits and letters bimanually, and also made more nondominant hand mirror reversals than did nonstutterers. Left-handed stutterers and nonstutterers differed only in the incidence of mirror reversals with the nondominant hand. The results support the hypothesis that disorganization in the integration of left- and right-hemisphere inhibitory and excitatory processes may be an integral component of stuttering.

    INTRODUCTION THE HYPOTHESIS that stuttering is due to incomplete cerebra1 lateralization of language originated in the early part of this century [21, 22, 301 and has persisted to the present 1311. Attempts to test this hvoothesis used handedness as the index of language lateralization [2, 6, 271. Early theor& relating cerebral lateral organization to stuttering assumed that handedness was a reliable index of the degree and direction of the lateralization of speech. However, efforts to find a causal link between handedness and stuttering have been elusive [l. 2,4, 8,271. Historical efforts to link stuttering to left-handedness may have failed because assessment of handedness was based on self-report or teacher report of the hand preferred for writing [I, 4, S,].

    There is evidence to suggest that the preferred hand for writing is more strongly lateralized than any other measure of hand skill [24], and that differences in skilled performance between the hands are greatest for handwriting [23]. Moreover, the degree to which hand preference varies across tasks led PROVINS et al. [23] to suggest that differences in hand skill performance may be a more sensitive index of the relationship between manual laterality and stuttering than is provided by self-report or questionnaire determination of hand preference.

    Recently, FITZGERALD et al. [9] compared the performance of right-handed adult stutterers and nonstutterers on a bimanual handwriting task. Subjects were required to write the digits l-12 as quickly as they could while using both hands simultaneously. Compared with nonstutterers, stutterers showed significantly poorer handwriting organization of the left hand, greater differences in handwriting between the hands, and more mirror reversals in the left hand. These results suggest that interhemispheric processes interfered with stutterers performance. In contrast, studies with right-handed normal speakers, required to perform a variety of concurrent speech and manual tasks, typically refer to intrahemispheric interference to account for deficits in manual performance [13, 14, 16, 331.

    A number of studies with stutterers using a variety of research techniques have been cited as supporting greater right hemispheric than left hemispheric dysfunction [3, 7, 15, 20, 29, 32, 34, 353. Drawing upon the theories of LASHLEY [ 171 and REIBER ef al. [26], FITZGERALD et al. [9] concluded that what might appear to be incomplete cerebral dominance, may be inefficient interhemisphericcoordination or dysfunctional interhemispheric integration of information. Thus, for stutterers, an underlying problem may be an inability of the left hemisphere to achieve control over the right hemisphere due to a general deficiency in interhemispheric integration of motor, spatial, and temporal components of speech [S, IO, 343.

    The current study was designed as a replication of FITZGERALD et al. [9] by comparing simultaneous handwriting in right-handed stutterers and nonstutterers with performance of left-handed stutterers and nonstutterers. It also was extended by using stimuli designed to preferentially involve both left-hemisphere processing (digits and letters) as well as right-hemisphere processing (geometric stimuli).

    t Address requests for reprints to: Hiram E. Fitzgerald, Department of Psychology, Psychology Research Building, Michigan State University, East Lansing, MI 48824, U.S.A.

    441

  • 442 NOTE

    METHODS Subjects

    The subjects were 40 males, including 15 right-handed stutterers (mean age = 25.9 yr; range= 16 54) five left- handed stutterers (mean age=22 yr; range= 1634) 15 right-handed nonstutterers (mean age=27.2 yr. range = 1634) and five left-handed nonstutterers (mean age = 26.2 yr, range = 2&42). Handedness was determined by performance on the Harris Test of Lateral Dominance [12] which has reported test&retest reliabilities ranging in the 0.80s for most subtests, and is one of the few measures of laterality for which reliability information is available [28]. Informed consent was obtained from each subject prior to data collection.

    Procedure

    Subjects were required to perform two different handwriting tasks. Task I required simultaneous handwriting of the digits l-1 2, as fast as possible on a sheet of paper (see [ 121). A piece of cardboard held between the subjects eyes and the paper prevented visual feedback of his handwriting. The task began when the subject indicated that he understood the instructions. No practice trials were given.

    After Task 1 was completed, instructions for Task 2 were given. Task 2 required the simultaneous handwriting of 36 stimuli presented one at a time. Half of the stimuli were single symbols (digits, letters, geometric forms), and half were double symbols (i.e. pairs of digits, letters, or geometric forms). The order of stimulus types was randomized. The digit and letter writing was designed to preferentially engage left-hemisphere processing systems; the geometric form-writing, right-hemisphere processing systems. Each of the 36 symbols was written simultaneously as fast as possible on a separate sheet of paper without visual feedback. The task began when the subject indicated that he understood the instructions. No practice trials were given. The entire experiment (Task 1 and 2 combined) took approximately 30 min to complete.

    Scoring

    Data were scored by three raters who were naive about all aspects of the experiment. Raters received a training session during which they practiced scoring using an empirically derived (scaled from 0 to 10) rating system 191. Pearson correlations were used to calculate the interrater reliability for each digit, letter, and geometric form; reliabilities ranged from 0.85 to 1.00 with a mean of 0.95.

    Task 1 scaring. For Task 1, each digit was compared to a standard Artype transfer number (10 mm high) arranged in a column from 1 to 12 and mounted on poster board (see Fig. 1 for an example). The data sheet containing the two columns of numbers was cut in half, and each half (left-hand column and right-hand column) was mounted on a separate poster board. This was done to minimize any obvious cues to the rater that the digits had been written by the left or right hand. Each digit in a column was rated against its Artype standard on each of six criteria, by subtracting points fron an assigned score of 10, depending on the raters judgement of the following dimensions: (1) Digit alignment, degree of scatter, or compactness. One point was deducted if a digit deviated from the general

    alignment of the digits in the column. (2) Digit overlap. One point was deducted if a digit overlapped another digit in the column. (3) Digit completeness. One to three points were deducted depending on the degree of completeness. (4) Digit structure. One or two points were deducted depending on the degree ofexcess writing attached to the digit. (5) Number orientation, slant. One or two points were deducted depending on the degree of deviation from the

    standard. (6) Legibility. One point was deducted if the digit was illegible.

    This scoring system yielded a rating score from 0 to 10 for each digit, with a score of 10 being assigned to a drawing most like the standard. Scores for each digit in a column were summed and averaged to obtain a score for each hand. Mirror-image reversals were scored and summed separately and did not enter into the scoring of digit writing as described above.

    Task 2 scoring. With two exceptions, the scoring for Task 2 data was identical to the scoring for Task 1. Since symbols in Task 2 were written on separate sheets, they could not be scored on the alignment component of dimension 2, although scatter and compactness were scoreable. Moreover, they could not be scored at all on dimension 2 (overlap). Scores therefore could range from 1 to 10, rather than 0 to 10, with 10 again being most like the standard. Figure 2 shows examples of the Artype standards used in scoring data from Task 2.

    RESULTS Analysis of variance of the mean scores for each hand revealed significant between-group and within-group

    differences. However, in all cases there also were significant interactions with subject handedness.

    Betwern-group c~omparisons

    Handwritng organization. Table 1 summarizes the results of the analysis of variance for handwriting organization. As indicated in Table 1. right-handed stutterers and nonstutterers demonstrated nearly identical performances in dominant handwriting on Tasks 1 and 2. However. compared to right-handed nonstutterers. right-handed

  • NOTE 443

    lLEFT HAND -l RIGHT HAND

    a 2 3

    4 5 b 7

    IO

    11 12

    FIG. 1. Bimanual simultaneous handwriting of the numbers 1 through 12 in a right-handed stutterer showing mirror-writing and impaired left-handed performance.

    stutterers had significantly poorer nondominant hand performance for digits in Task 1 and Task 2 and for letters in Task 2. There were no differences between right-handed stutterers and nonstutterers involving handwriting organization for geometric forms; nor were there any significant differences in handwriting organization between left-handed stutterers and nonstutterers.

    Reversals. Inspection of individual subject reversal scores indicated that all reversals in the nonstutterers group were made by a single individual. Because one extreme score in a relatively small sample can bias group comparisons, the ANOVA for reversals was performed with this subject excluded along with one stutterer selected at random. Results of the ANOVA are summarized in Table 2. As indicated in Table 2, right-handed stutterers emitted significantly more total reversals, dominant and nondominant hand reversals than nonstutterers. Left-handed stutterers had significantly more total reversals and nondominant hand reversals than did left-handed nonstutterers when writing letters or digits but not when writing geometric forms. Conversely, there were no reversals in the dominant hand for any left-handed subject.

    Within-group comparisons

    Comparisons within groups resulted in similar findings for stutterers and nonstutterers. Thus, the results described below apply to both stutterers and to nonstutterers. Bimanual handwriting was significantly better organized in the dominant hand than in the nondominant hand for digits and letters. However, the copying of

  • 444

    LEFT HAND

    NOTE

    57

    zs

    RIGHT HAND

    i--l

    i __ >

  • Table

    1. B

    imanual

    handw

    riting o

    rganiz

    ation i

    n r

    ight-

    handed a

    nd left

    -handed s

    tutt

    ere

    rs (S

    ) and n

    onst

    utt

    ere

    rs (N

    S)

    Rig

    ht-

    handed

    Left

    -handed

    Measu

    re

    Task

    1

    Rig

    ht-

    hand d

    igit

    s Le

    ft-h

    and d

    igit

    s Task

    2

    Rig

    ht-

    hand d

    igit

    s Le

    ft-h

    and d

    igit

    s R

    ight-

    hand l

    ett

    ers

    Le

    ft-h

    and lett

    ers

    R

    ight-

    hand g

    eom

    etr

    ic fo

    rms

    Left

    -hand g

    eom

    etr

    ic fo

    rms

    S

    NS

    Mean

    Mean

    9.0

    8

    9.0

    7

    5.8

    0

    6.9

    7

    8.5

    8

    8.5

    2

    6.7

    9

    7.5

    0

    8.3

    9

    8.5

    5

    6.4

    8

    7.3

    2

    8.6

    9

    8.8

    0

    8.3

    7

    8.3

    6

    F df

    P

    0.0

    0

    1,2

    8

    0.9

    3

    7.9

    5

    1,2

    8

    0.0

    08

    0.3

    8

    1,2

    8

    0.5

    4

    6.8

    2

    I,28

    0.0

    15

    2.4

    0

    1,2

    8

    0.1

    3

    9.3

    1

    1,2

    8

    0.0

    05

    0.5

    3

    1,2

    8

    0.5

    0

    0.0

    0

    1,2

    8

    0.9

    7

    S N

    S M

    ean

    Mean

    7.5

    1

    7.0

    0

    8.9

    7

    9.0

    2

    7.4

    8

    7.4

    8

    8.5

    1

    8.5

    0

    6.7

    0

    7.2

    0

    8.4

    1

    8.4

    1

    8.8

    7

    8.8

    0

    8.8

    7

    8.8

    0

    F d

    f P

    0.7

    5

    178

    0.4

    1

    0.0

    1

    L8

    0.9

    0

    0.0

    0

    1,8

    0.9

    9

    0.0

    0

    1,8

    0.9

    7

    0.9

    2

    1.8

    0.3

    7

    0.0

    0

    1,8

    0.9

    9

    0.0

    1

    138

    0.9

    0

    0.0

    1

    198

    0.9

    0

    Reve

    rsals

    Table

    2. R

    eve

    rsals

    in h

    andw

    riting f

    or

    right-

    handed s

    tutt

    ere

    rs (S

    ) and n

    onst

    utt

    ere

    rs (N

    S)

    Rig

    ht-

    handed

    Left

    -handed

    S

    NS

    F df

    P

    S N

    S F

    df

    P

    Mean

    Mean

    Mean

    Mean

    Tota

    l 3.7

    1

    0.0

    0

    16.4

    3

    1.2

    6

    0.0

    004

    1.6

    0

    0.0

    0

    7.1

    1

    198

    0.0

    3

    Rig

    ht-

    hand

    0.4

    3

    0.0

    0

    4.5

    0

    1.2

    6

    0.0

    4

    1.6

    0

    0.0

    0

    7.1

    1

    138

    0.0

    3

    Left

    -hand

    3.2

    9

    0.0

    0

    18.3

    9

    1.2

    6

    0.0

    002

    0

    0.0

    0

    0

    0

    0

  • 446 NOTE

    speech control centers. Moreover, these data support the contention that right-hemisphere function may be a source of interference in stuttering.

    A~knowledyements--The authors express their appreciation to Lauren Julius Harris for his contributions to the design aspects of this research. Research described in this report was supported, in part, by a grant from the Michigan State University Foundation (H.E.F. and P.A.C.) and by NIMH Training Grant MH-14622-05 to JRG. During preparation of this paper, H.E.F. was supported in part by The Spencer Foundation.

    I. 2. 3.

    4.

    5.

    6. I.

    8.

    9.

    10.

    11.

    12. 13.

    14.

    15.

    16.

    17.

    18.

    19.

    20.

    REFERENCES

    BALLARD, P. B. Sinistrality and speech. J. exp. fed. Rec. 1, 298-310, 1912. BLOODSTEIN, 0. Handbook of Stuttering. Easter Seal Foundation, 1981. BRADY. J. P. and BEKSON, J. Stuttering, dichotic listening, and cerebral dominance. Archs yen. P.sychol. 32, 1449-1452, 1975. CLAIBORNE, J. H. Stuttering in a boy relieved by reversal of manual dexterity: with remarks on the subject of symbol amblyopia. N. Y. med. J. 105, 619~621, 1917. COOPER, M. H. and ALLEN, G. D. Timing control accuracy in normal speakers and stutterers. .I. Speech Hem. Dis. 20, 55 71, 1977. Cox, M. D. The stutterer and stuttering: neuropsychological correlates. J. Flu. Dis. 7, 129-140, 1982. CURRY, F. K. W. and GREGORY, H. H. The performance of stutterers on dichotic listening tasks thought to reflect cerebral dominance. J. Speech Hear. Rex 12, 129- 140, 1969. DANIEL& E. M. An analysis of the relation between handedness and stuttering with special reference to the Orton--Travis theory of cerebral dominance. J. Speech Dir. 5, 309%329, 1940. FITZGERALD. H. E.. COOKE, P. A. and GREINER, J. R. Speech and bimanual hand organization in adult stutterers and nonstutterers. J. Flu. Dis. 9, 51-65, 1984. FITZGERALD, H. E. and DJURDJIC, S. D. Psychophysiological correlates ofdysfluent speech. In Anuurio Verilus, Vol. 1. Universidad Regiomontana, Department de Publicaciones, Monterrey, N.L., Mexico, 1982. GRINER, J. R., FITZGERALD. H. E. and COOKE, P. A. Speech fluency and hand performance on a sequential tapping task in left- and right-handed stutterers and nonstutterers. J. Flu. Dis., in press. HARRIS, A. J. Harris Test of Lateral Dominance, 3rd edn. The Psychological Corporation, New York, 1957. HICKS, R. E. Interhemispheric response competition between vocal and unimanual performance in normal adult human males. J. camp. Physiol. Psycho/. 89, 5@40, 1975. HICKS, R. E., BRADSHAW, G, J., KINSBOURNE, M. and FEIGIN, D. S. Vocal manual trade-offs in hemispheric sharing of human performance control. J. Mot. Behat,. 10, I-6, 1978. JONES, R. K. Observations on stammering after localized cerebra1 injury. J. Neural. Neurosurq. Psychiat. 29, 192.-195, 1966. KINSBOURNE, M. and COOKE, J. Generalized and lateralized effects of concurrent verbalization on a unimanual skill. Q. exp. Psychol. 23, 341 345, 1971. LASHLEY, K. S. The problems of serial order in behavior. In Cerebral Mechanisms in Behavior. L. JEFFRESS (Editor). Wiley, New York, 1951. MOORE, W. H. Bilateral tachistoscopic word perceptlon of stutterers and normal subJects. Brain Laj~(l. 3, 434442, 1976 MOORE, W. H. and HAYNES. W. 0. Alpha hemispheric asymmetry and stuttering: Some support for a segmentation dysfunction hypothesis. J, Speech Heur. Res. 23, 229-247, 1980. MOORE, W. H. and LANG. M. K. Alpha asymmetry over the right and left hemisphere of stutterers and controls preceding massed oral reading. Percept. Mot. Skills 4, 223~-230, 1977.

    21. ORTON, S. T. Studies in stuttering. Arrhs Neural. Psychint. 18, 671 672, 1927. 22. ORTON, S. T. A psychological theory of reading disability and stuttering in children. N. Enq. J. Med. 199,

    1046 1052, 1929. 23. PROVINS, K. A. and CUNLIFFE, P. The reliability of some motor performance tests of handedness.

    Neuropsycholoyiu IO, 199 206. 1972. 24. PKOVINS, K. A., MILNER, A. D. and KERR, P. Asymmetry of manual preference, tests of handedness. Prrcrpt.

    Mot. S/G//s 54, 179-194. 1982. 25. RECORI)S, M. A.. HEIMBUCH, R. C. and KII)D, K. K. Handedness and stuttering: a dead horse? J. Flu. Dis. 2.

    271 281, 1977. 26. RIEBER, R. W.. SMITH. N. and HARRIS. B. Neuropsychological aspects of stuttering and cluttering. In Thr

    Neuropsycholoyy oflunyuaye, R. W. RIEBEK (Editor). Plenum Press, New York, 1976. 27. ROSENFIELU, D. B. Cerebral dominance and stuttering. J. flu. Dis. 5, 171 185, 1980. 28. SAPPINGTON. J. R. Measures of lateral dominance: interrelationshops and temporal stability. Percept. Mar.

    S/G/tills 53, 783-790. 1980. 29. SUSSMAN, H. M. and MATNEILAGE, P. F. Hemispheric specialization for speech production and perception in

    stutterers. Neuropsycholoyiu 13, 19 26. 1975.

  • NOTE 447

    30. TRAVIS, L. E. Speech Pathology. Appleton-Century-Crofts, New York, 1931. 31. TRAVIS, L. E. The cerebral dominance theory ofstuttering: 1931-1978, J. Speech Hear. Dis. 43,275277, 1978. 32. WADA, J. and RASMUSSEN, T. Intracarotid injection of sodium amytal for the lateralization of cerebral speech

    dominance: experimental and clinical observation. J. Neurosurg. 17, 266-282, 1960. 33. WOLFF, P. H. and COHEN, C. Dual task performance during bimanual coordination. Cortex 16,119-133, 1980. 34. YEUDALL, L. T. A neurospychological theory of stuttering. In Seminars in Speech and Language, Vol. III,

    Stuttering: Experimental Programs in Research and Treatment, E. BOBERG (Editor). Therme-Stratton, New York, 1985.

    35. ZIMMERMAN, G. N. and KNOI-T, J. R. Slow potentials of the brain related to speech processing in normal speakers and stutterers. Electroenceph. clin. Neurophysiol. 31, 1449-1452, 1975.