WHEN ONLINE AFFECTS THE OFFLINE: THE EFFECT OF MISINTERPRETATION IN ONLINE CHATS ON THE EMOTIONAL...

download WHEN ONLINE AFFECTS THE OFFLINE: THE EFFECT OF MISINTERPRETATION IN ONLINE CHATS ON THE EMOTIONAL WELL-BEING OF STUDENTS

of 11

description

Online chats have grown to become more popular over the past few years and, needless to say, the way they distance themselves from time and place are great, but are there any negative consequences connected to computer-mediated communication (CMC)? To be more specific, can misinterpretations in CMC affect the emotional well-being of a person in some way? Previously done research indicated that social skills and interpretations are important aspects in communication and should be taken into account to uncover the answer to this question. Through the means of an online questionnaire, information was gathered from 108 Information Sciences students. The analyses of the answers show that there was a severe lack of females (only seven) in the sample and that other results did not prove to be significant enough to accept the alternative hypotheses. Only negligible correlations between the variables have been found (at most) and there were no significant differences present. The research provides no evidence that misinterpretations in online chats affect the emotional well-being of an individual.

Transcript of WHEN ONLINE AFFECTS THE OFFLINE: THE EFFECT OF MISINTERPRETATION IN ONLINE CHATS ON THE EMOTIONAL...

  • WHEN ONLINE AFFECTS THE OFFLINE: THE EFFECT OF

    MISINTERPRETATION IN ONLINE CHATS ON THE

    EMOTIONAL WELL-BEING OF STUDENTS

    Van den Bos, Lars, University of Utrecht, Princetonplein 5, Buys Ballot Gebouw, 3584 CC,

    Utrecht, The Netherlands, [email protected]

    Van den Heuvel, Joey, University of Utrecht, Princetonplein 5, Buys Ballot Gebouw, 3584

    CC, Utrecht, The Netherlands, [email protected]

    Molenaar, Sabine, University of Utrecht, Princetonplein 5, Buys Ballot Gebouw, 3584 CC,

    Utrecht, The Netherlands, [email protected]

    Robeer, Marcel, University of Utrecht, Princetonplein 5, Buys Ballot Gebouw, 3584 CC,

    Utrecht, The Netherlands, [email protected]

    Abstract

    Online chats have grown to become more popular over the past few years and, needless to say, the way

    they distance themselves from time and place are great, but are there any negative consequences

    connected to computer-mediated communication (CMC)? To be more specific, can misinterpretations

    in CMC affect the emotional well-being of a person in some way? Previously done research indicated

    that social skills and interpretations are important aspects in communication and should be taken into

    account to uncover the answer to this question. Through the means of an online questionnaire,

    information was gathered from 108 Information Sciences students. The analyses of the answers show

    that there was a severe lack of females (only seven) in the sample and that other results did not prove

    to be significant enough to accept the alternative hypotheses. Only negligible correlations between the

    variables have been found (at most) and there were no significant differences present. The research

    provides no evidence that misinterpretations in online chats affect the emotional well-being of an

    individual.

    Keywords: Computer-mediated communication (CMC), Emotional well-being, Misinterpretation,

    Social skills, Gender, Students.

  • 1 Introduction

    Since the upswing of modern Internet facilities around the world, people have the ability to chat to each

    other via a digital medium. The textual form of this chatter is commonly referred to as computer-

    mediated communication (CMC) (Thurlow, Lengel & Tomic, 2004). The use of CMC has changed the

    way people interact in various ways. Chatting to one another leads to large conversations that might be

    interpreted elsewise, since there are no nonverbal cues involved in CMC (Walther, 1992).

    Misinterpretation and misperception are terms that clarify the situation, but will not explain why this

    occurs.

    With this research project, the impact of misinterpretation in CMC on Information Science students has

    been examined. The research was limited to this sample frame, since misinterpretation is different

    between all kinds of populations and contexts (Jones, 1995). It is interesting to consider the effects on

    relatively young people that use computers relatively often, which leads to interesting data on several

    aspects, such as their social skills, emotional well-being and how adept someone is at interpreting a

    particular chat in a particular context. The person should, for example, be able to determine the real

    meaning of a particular conversation or be sure that the receiver of a joke also conceives this as such. A

    better view on this aspect would lead to information that gives a more contextualized layer to CMC in

    terms of testing fellow students. These students are ought to distinguish different and manage

    information flows (Borko 1968), but they should also be capable of doing the same in CMC-information,

    because they should not be negatively affected by an incorrect interpretation of a chat. Therefore,

    scientific research has been conducted, literature was consulted, and psychological processes that are all

    informative for the scientific process were explained, so that several interesting conclusions can be

    drawn about this sample.

    The research itself is interesting from a societal point of view, because CMC is an integral part of both

    personal and professional communication (Ku, Chu & Tseng, 2012). Additionally, since there is still a

    lot of knowledge to gather about this topic, the study is interesting from a scientific point of view. The

    research project is based on a research question, and is being accompanied by several literature papers

    that support the reasoning that will be conducted throughout the entire paper. This helped to

    contextualize and clarify the gathered, unknown statistics of this sample.

    The main objective of the research is to determine the effects of misinterpretation on online chatting.

    The CMC will be studied using students in a one-on-one chat scenario. With this, the following research

    question was formulated:

    Is there misinterpretation among students in one-on-one instant computer-mediated communication that has a negative effect on the emotional well-being?

    2 Theory

    To form a theoretical foundation for the research, various papers that were relevant were studied. Even

    though these papers have a lot to say about various aspects of CMC, misinterpretation and emotional

    well-being, there are differences that make this research still viable to do. Emotional adjustments during

    chatting have been analyzed by a few researches already, for instance by Zhe & Boucouvalas (2002),

    where the researches tried to extract emotions from texts by computers. What is missing in this paper,

    is that the focus really lies on the emotions in texts themselves, and not on how these are being

    interpreted. This is exactly the gap that this research attempts to fill. The goal of the research, as

    previously stated, is to see what the effects are when these emotions are being misunderstood in textual

    communication, and what the causes are for these misinterpretations. As a matter of fact, no literature

    could be found that is really similar to the topic that was researched.

    There are, in fact, other papers that might be interesting for some aspects of this research, such as the

    types of chat conversations. According to Link & Wagner (2006) three different types of CMC can be

    defined: one-to-one, one-to-many and many-to-many. Moreover, reasons behind the use of emotions in

    chats, and reasons behind using texting or chatting in general, instead of just talking are also aspects that

  • have been previously researched. Fullwood & Martino (2007) state that the use of emotions (through

    the use of emoticons, for example) can help participants of a chat to be able to perceive the personality

    and emotions of their chat partner(s). These are all very interesting papers, and they are useful for this

    research, but they simply do not cover the principle of misinterpretation, which therefore is a unique

    part of the subject to perform research on. The most relative papers might be Vandergriff (2013) and

    Taesler & Janneck (2010), where the expression of emotion and use of emoticons and other typographic

    markers used in chats are analyzed in these papers. Once again, there was no further elaboration of how

    these are being interpreted by the one that is supposed to understand the receiving emotions from the

    other contact.

    In conclusion, the link between misinterpretation, CMC and emotions is a subject that has not been

    researched (enough), which makes this research full of meaning.

    3 Hypotheses

    This research consists of three abstract constructs: emotional well-being, Communication Interpretation

    Quota (CIQ), and social skills. The only variable in the current research is gender. The conceptual model

    (Figure 1) gives a representation of the abstract constructs and the variable, and the relationships

    between them. The relationships are represented by arrows, meaning that the construct or concept which

    is pointing at another construct or concept has an effect on it. In the conceptual model, the dependent

    variable, explained by the independent variable gender and the mediating variable CIQ, is emotional

    well-being. The two independent variables, namely social skills and gender, explain a students CIQ and emotional well-being respectively. The CIQ is explained by social skills and it explains emotional well-

    being. Thus, this is a mediating variable. In the following subsections, the conceptual model and its used

    terms are elaborated.

    Figure 1. Conceptual Model

    3.1 Emotional well-being

    First of all, emotional well-being is the main abstract construct in the conceptual model. It is the

    construct that is affected by other parts in the conceptual model but does not affect other constructs or

    variables by itself. Eventually, this construct is where the key focus in the research lies, and is therefore

    seen as the main construct in this research.

    Emotional well-being can be described in various ways. It can relate to the quality of someone's life,

    having aspects like personal well-being, self-esteem, productivity and happiness. A good emotional

    well-being will also reduce chances of stress, disorder and sleeping problems (Fredrickson & Joiner,

    2002). This concept - since it is an abstract construct - is key in the research for a potential effect of

    misinterpretation in chats on the emotional well-being. This is why all the arrows end up in this abstract

    construct in the conceptual model, assuming that gender and the Communication Interpretation Quota

    directly affect the emotional well-being.

  • To determine a students emotional well-being in the sample, a previously created scale by the World Health Organization: Regional Office for Europe (1998) was used. This scale consists of five statements

    which are used to measure the participants emotional well-being on a scale of 1-100. The questions in this scale were implemented within the survey as a key part to gaining insight in the emotional well-

    being of the student.

    3.2 Communication Interpretation Quota (CIQ)

    To be able to measure the amount of misinterpretations, a self-constructed abstract construct was added,

    the Communication Interpretation Quota, or CIQ for short. CIQ determines the frequency of

    misinterpretation in small chat conversations that have been created for the research. If there is no case

    of misinterpretation, the participants CIQ will increase by one point. The CIQ is then transformed to a scale ranging from zero to one, with one being no misinterpretation at all, and zero meaning that all the

    chats were misinterpreted. The right interpretation of a chat is determined by giving four multiple choice

    answers, of which only one corresponds with an answer that the sender would (a) find appropriate to not

    notice that there has been a case of misinterpretation, or (b) think that there has not been a

    misinterpretation at all.

    The Conflict Research Consortium (n.d.) suggests that ambiguous messages are likely to be

    misinterpreted. People tend to clarify these messages by using their own expectations. This

    misinterpretation can lead to social conflict, which in itself can lead to negative affect (Abbey, Abramis

    & Caplan, 1985). Negative affectivity involves the experience of negative emotions and poor self-

    concept (Watson & Clark, 1984). Even though not all the aspects of this relationships have been

    researched, it shows that there is a possible negative effect of misinterpretation of messages on how

    people perceive their own emotions. It is expected that students who had more cases of misinterpretation

    in the CIQ questions, and therefore a lower CIQ, will have a significantly lower emotional well-being

    after answering the CIQ questions than before answering them. This leads to the following hypothesis:

    H1: A lower CIQ negatively affects the emotional well-being.

    3.3 Gender

    The variable that affects the emotional well-being is gender. This is a variable that is based on the

    characteristics of an individual, and can be measured precisely. Logically, the variable gender can only

    have two possible values: male or female. Previous research by Simon (2014) states that females are

    usually more negative about their own well-being than men. Men report more frequent positive, and less

    frequent negative feelings than women. Men and women may react differently to misinterpretation and

    that is why this aspect needs to be taken into account in this research. This leads to the second hypothesis:

    H2: Men tend to have a higher overall emotional well-being in comparison to females.

    3.4 Social Skills

    The last abstract construct is social skills. The social skills of an individual is the ability to communicate

    with other human beings, both verbally and non-verbally, and understanding them (SkillsYouNeed,

    n.d.). In short, whether an individual is, or is not, sociable. In the conceptual model, it was expected that

    social skills affect the CIQ.

    A vital part of understanding others, and responding to them, is based on the ability of individuals to

    effectively recognize and use nonverbal behavior (Feldman & Rim, 1991). Having developed social

    skills will make people able to understand their conversation partner better. Being sensitive to the

    thoughts and feelings of others is key to successfully establish and maintain social relationships (Pickett,

    Gardner & Knowles, 2004). Socially skilled people will be better in interpreting conversations correctly,

    have a better sense for what direction the chat is going in, and are more skilled in forming appropriate

    reactions to the conversation partner. Therefore, social skills affect the chances of misinterpreting

    conversations and, in doing so, affect the CIQ. This induces the third, and final, hypothesis:

    H3: There is a positive correlation between social skills and CIQ.

  • 4 Method

    4.1 Participants

    The questionnaire was filled in by 108 students following the course Scientific Research Methods at the University of Utrecht. In general, students form a suitable and adequate sample for this research.

    They are not just required to use the computer quite regularly for study-related activities, but a fair

    amount of young Dutch people also suffer from negative emotions, such as anxiety and depression

    (CBS, 2014). The only downside that could be foreseen was the low quantity of female students

    following the course, which also reflected in the actual number of females filling in the questionnaire

    (N = 7).

    4.2 Materials

    A questionnaire is a suitable method to answer the research question. Currently, there are some scales

    that already exist, which could be used to determine ones mental condition or state. This was expanded with short made-up chats, with some context added to them. Subsequently, the correct interpretation of

    said chat (the feelings of both chatters involved, the meaning of the conversations and the purpose of

    the conversation) was determined. This was used to see if the students in the sample were able to

    interpret the chats correctly, or in other words, if there was a case of misinterpretation. Furthermore, the

    questionnaire was an easy way to reach a lot of students, while making sure they got the exact same

    questions. As a consequence, the differences in the answers were dependent on the individual itself, and

    not due to the way the questions were asked.

    The questions that were devised for the questionnaire have been categorized, and the answers to these

    questions were related to a specific part of the research (the entire questionnaire can be found in the

    Appendix A). All questions belong to a variable or abstract construct present in the conceptual model.

    Putting aside the general questions, the first question that truly mattered to the research was question

    two, determining the gender. Questions three through seven and 21 through 25 have been used to

    ascertain the emotional well-being of the participants in the sample. Subsequently, questions eight

    through 12 gave insight in a participants social skills, and to conclude, the remaining questions (questions 13 through 20) determined the CIQ of a participant. There was a grand total of 25 questions.

    Since the hypotheses were based on the conceptual model, the answers to the questions in the

    questionnaire provided enough information for conclusions and determination of whether the alternative

    hypotheses should be accepted or rejected. A further elaboration on the questions can be found below.

    The questions will be described in order of appearance.

    First of all, to measure the variable gender, a binary scale question was asked; simply What is your sex?. The percentages of these amounts were calculated by dividing the amount of males by the sample size, the same was done for females.

    Secondly, a participants social skills were measured by asking them to determine how well five statements about their social skills fitted to themselves. The social skills scale had to be accurately

    measuring social skills and it should consist of fewer than seven questions. Since none of the social

    skills scales could be found that met these requirements, a self-constructed scale was added. This scale

    consists of one Likert scale for each of five statements that are closely related to social skills. Hall (1979)

    divided social skills into two broad classes of sending and receiving. Riggio (1986) expands this by

    subdividing social skills into basic dimensions, which involve skill in sending, receiving and controlling

    information. Since the participants answering the questionnaire were taking the role of the receiver, the

    five statements also focused on assessing the receiver class of social skills. Furthermore, the National

    Research Council (2010) states that social skills means having skills in interpreting verbal and nonverbal

    information to respond appropriately. According to the National Research Council, social skills are

    equal to complex communication. By adding this information to the scale, the statements were used to

    determine someones social skill by rating the awareness of reasons behind communication, awareness of peoples means and intentions through chat, phone and real life conversations. In other words, if the participants viewed themselves as being successful in the receiver role of both verbal and nonverbal

  • complex communication. The scores were then added up, creating one total score for each individual.

    The total score was used for the hypotheses testing. This scale, consisting of five items, was found to be

    reliable ( = .839).

    Thirdly, to measure the abstract construct emotional well-being, five seven-point Likert scales were used

    for the statements that relate to emotional well-being. Questions that measure the emotional well-being

    were filled in before and after the test with the chat examples in the questionnaire. The scores of the

    individuals were then applied to a previously created scale devised by the World Health Organization:

    Regional Office for Europe (1998). This is a scale that could measure someones emotional well-being with a limited amount of statements. The scores of the five questions were then added up, and

    transformed into values ranging from zero to 100, as was also done by the World Health Organization.

    The emotional well-being of a participant was determined twice in the questionnaire. This was done to

    appropriately form an answer to the first hypothesis. To properly answer the first hypothesis, not the

    effect of the CIQ on the overall emotional well-being is interesting, but whether the emotional well-

    being was negatively affected after there were potential cases of misinterpretation. For hypothesis one,

    it was expected that more misinterpretations correlate with a bigger negative influence on emotional

    well-being, and thus with a bigger negative difference between the emotional well-being after the

    questions concerning misinterpretation and the questions before that. Both of the times that the scale

    was used in the questionnaire, it was found that the five items forming the scale were internally consist

    according to Cronbachs alpha, with alpha scores of .771 and .761 respectively.

    Finally, the CIQ score was measured over eight multiple choice questions with one correct answer each,

    meaning that the other multiple choice answers were false. The participants were informed of the chat

    environment for every single chat, by adding a general context of the chat followed by a part of the

    conversation up to the point the participant had to form an answer. In the context, information such as

    the nature of the chat, a comprehensive description of the conversation partner, and the time of day the

    chat takes place were given to the participant. By providing this context and the chat itself, the participant

    should be sufficiently informed to choose the correct reply to the chat, in order to avoid

    misinterpretation.

    After gathering data from the questionnaire, the CIQ score was calculated by undertaking multiple steps.

    Firstly, the given answers per question were compared to the correct answer as determined beforehand.

    The correct answers to the CIQ questions are marked by the bold-type text in the questions themselves

    (13 20) in Appendix A. Correct answers were given one point, incorrect answers were given zero points. Secondly, the total of the score of an individual was calculated. Finally, this was then divided by

    eight (due to the presence of eight chat examples). This resulted in scores varying from zero to one. The

    scale was eventually used to test hypotheses about where one of the two variables was the CIQ score.

    No internal consistency reliability, tested by Cronbachs alpha, was calculated for the CIQ questions, because it is only an indicator if the both chat partners are interpreting the chat in the same way. As

    aforementioned, the CIQ is merely an indicator for the frequency of misinterpretations by a student in

    the eight chats combined. None of these eight questions are meant to be interrelated, and therefore no

    internal correlations were expected to be present.

    4.3 Procedure

    The questionnaire started by generally informing the students about the research and the questionnaire,

    itself. This was done in form of a general story about and behind this research and the questionnaire. On

    purpose, the word misinterpretation in the title was changed into interpretation. This was done to ensure that participants would fill in the CIQ questions in the questionnaire according to their own

    thought, instead of actively searching for an obscure answer. The actual content started off with a general

    theoretical basis about misperception and misinterpretation in computer-mediated communication, to

    give the participants an idea of what the questionnaire is about. Secondly, the participants were shown

    gave a short introduction about the abstract construct emotional well-being, and related that to the

    research. The participants were then shown the research question for some extra information. Thirdly,

    the participants were given a short overview of how the components of the questionnaire were ordered

    and what kind of questions could be expected. Finally, the introduction ended by giving a time indication

  • to fill in the questionnaire (five to ten minutes) and thanking the participants in advance. The actual

    introduction given to the students can be found in Appendix A.

    The participants who filled in the questionnaire were not treated any differently across certain

    conditions. Male or female, and which team the participant was from, did not make any difference in

    the way participants had to fill in the questionnaire.

    After finishing the questionnaire, the participants were thanked for filling in the survey, thereby

    informing the participants the questionnaire was done.

    5 Results

    Data from one participant were dropped because the data was found to be corrupt.

    The first hypothesis, A higher recognition of CIQs in chats leads to a higher emotional well-being, was tested using a Pearsons correlation test. For the test, the Communication Interpretation Quota of students (M = .36, SD = .14) were compared to the difference in their score of emotional well-being

    before and after the CIQ questions (M = -1.14, SD = 9.60). Analysis showed an insignificant and

    negligible negative correlation, r(107) = -.12, p = .103, one-tailed.

    Hypothesis two, which states that men tend to have a higher overall emotional well-being in comparison

    to females, was tested using the independent sample T-test. In this case, the differences between gender

    in their emotional well-being, both before and after, were tested. The low number of female students (N

    = 7), compared to the number of male students (N = 101) that filled in the study, means that no

    conclusions based on gender can be drawn from this data. Analysis showed no significant difference

    between males (M = .58, SD = .14) and females (M = .60, SD = .11) on the emotional well-being

    measured before the CIQ-test, t(106) = -.31, p = .381, one-tailed. Additionally, analysis also did not

    show a significant difference between males (M = .57, SD = .14) and females (M = .56, SD = .11) in

    their emotional well-being (after), t(106) = .22, p = .413, one-tailed.

  • Figure 2. Scatter plot for the correlation of a students Communication Interpretation Quota and their social skills

    For the third hypothesis, the correlation between a students social skills and their CIQ was measured using Pearsons correlation. As can be seen in Figure 2, the spread of the dots on the scatter plot provide no clear linear trend between the two variables. Even though there seems to be a positive association

    with constant scatter, this relationship is too weak to show any clear relationship. The coefficient of

    determination (R2) confirms this, with only 1.6% common variance between the two variables. The lack

    of a correlation as suggested in the scatter plot is also shown in Pearsons correlation test, where no significant correlation was found between social skills (M = 28.22, SD = 4.16) and Communication

    Interpretation Quota (M = .36, SD = .14), r(107) = .13, p = .098, one-tailed.

    6 Conclusion

    In an effort to find a negative effect of misinterpretation on the emotional well-being of students in one-

    on-one computer mediated communications, data gathered from a self-constructed questionnaire, filled

    in by 108 students following the course Scientific Research Methods, were collected. A few conclusions

    can be drawn in relation to the alternative hypotheses, based on the gathered data. Results from the

    questionnaire regarding the first alternative hypothesis, H1 A lower CIQ negatively affects the emotional well-being, produced no statistically significant correlation. Abbey, Ambramis & Caplan (1985) implied that misinterpretation could lead to negative affect. This negative affect involves

    negative emotion and poor self-concept (Watson & Clark, 1984). Hence, a significant negative

    correlation was expected between the CIQ and the difference in emotional well-being. Even though the

    correlation was not significant, it demonstrated a negative correlation.

    The questionnaire displayed no evidence that the second alternative hypothesis, H2 Men tend to have a higher overall emotional well-being in comparison to females, is true. Results were inconclusive, due

  • to the lack of females in the sample. Therefore, this null hypothesis cannot be disproven and the

    alternative hypothesis that has been devised should not be accepted. This conclusion is not in line with

    previous research, because Simon (2014) suggested that men are generally more positive about their

    own emotional well-being than women.

    Likewise, the results revealed no indication that higher social skills lead to a higher CIQ. Consequently,

    the last alternative hypothesis, There is a positive correlation between social skills and CIQ, should also be rejected. The only evidence that was found regarding this aspect of the questionnaire is that there

    might be an extremely weak positive correlation between social skills and CIQ, which should be

    neglected altogether. The absence of a significant correlation is in disagreement with Pickett, Gardner

    and Knowles (2004). They see being sensitive to thoughts and feelings as key to social skills.

    To conclude, misinterpretation among students in one-on-one computer-mediated communication does

    not have a negative effect on the emotional well-being of said students. According to the theory

    described earlier, online chats can have a certain emotional weight to them and can be interpreted

    incorrectly. Apparently though, no evidence has been found in this research to prove that this

    misinterpretation does in fact negatively affect the emotional well-being.

    7 Discussion

    There are several limitations to this research that have a potential effect on either the quality of the

    findings, or the ability to effectively answer the hypotheses. Firstly, there was a lack of show-up females

    in the sample. The sample size contained 108 participants, of which only 6.48% were women. Even

    though the difference between genders may be present in the general population, the difference between

    the two genders in this sample had to be very large to be statistically significant.

    Additionally, this gender problem is part of the second limitation in the research. The sample that filled

    in the questionnaire is difficult to generalize. This difficulty has two main reasons. First and foremost,

    because the actual percentage of females in universities in the Netherlands is 52% (CBS, 2012),

    compared to the 6.48% females in the sample. Secondly, because it is questionable if Information

    Sciences students are a homogeneous representation of the overall student population.

    The third limitation of the research became evident in the two self-constructed abstract constructs.

    Although the first of these two, social skills, exhibited a high internal consistency reliability, the

    statements making up social skills rest on a frequency of time. This frequency of time was distributed

    over seven possible Likert scale answers, ranging from none of the time to all of time, that could be interpreted differently by each individual. This different interpretation of frequencies could mean that

    the answers on this Likert scale are incomparable. Two participants intending to give the same frequency

    of time could choose two different Likert scale answers, and likewise, two participants choosing the

    same answer could have a different frequency of time in mind.

    Besides social skills, there were several problems encountered with the Communication Interpretation

    Quota, the second self-constructed abstract construct. Firstly, students were not given an indicator

    whether the chosen answer lead to misinterpretation or not, which could explain the lack of a significant

    effect on the emotional well-being. As was apparent in the theory, the emotional well-being of a person

    is only affected if there is a known misinterpretation between both conversation partners. Not giving an

    indication of the occurrence of this misinterpretation made a theoretical effect on a persons emotions improbable. Secondly, since the answers to the CIQ questions were limited to multiple choice, students

    could not form their own reaction to the chat, as is done in a real conversation. This imposes a potential

    misinterpretation of the four given answers students could choose from. Even though one of the four

    answers is the correct answer that is expected by the sender, and is therefore the answer that is correct

    to avoid misinterpretation, it does not mean that the receiver could not have formed an answer that would

    keep up the conversation, without the sender even knowing that his sent message was misinterpreted.

    For instance, chats could change topics, or reactions themselves could be ambiguous. This shows that

    for the creation of a new abstract construct, a whole process is needed to ensure that it is both reliable

    and valid in all possible ways. For example, it is currently unknown if the CIQ gives a good indication

  • of misinterpretation. Unfortunately, since the main focus of the research was on providing an answer to

    the main question, there was no room for forming good and solid abstract constructs.

    Finally, the method chosen to gather data for the hypothesis, a questionnaire, posed four main problems

    itself. Firstly, by establishing an individuals emotional well-being within just one survey, the timespan between the two measurements was really short. Chat sessions themselves could even outlast the length

    of the survey. Even though a students emotional well-being might be negatively affected by misinterpretation, no indications were found for when this would occur. This might be directly after the

    chat, or there might be a delay before this takes place. Secondly, the usage of Likert scales in the

    questionnaire introduced a central tendency bias. Students filling in the questionnaire tend to avoid the

    most extreme answers, such as I feel no commitment at all in the emotional well-being questions, because the occurrence of these is very scarce in the minds perception. Thirdly, in a survey people have to review themselves, which adds a social desirability bias. There is a possibility that students will put

    themselves in the best possible light, instead of giving an objective answer. Lastly, the multiple choice

    questions to measure ones CIQ are subject to the probability of guessing a correct answer.

    Future research should ensure that the two newly introduced abstract constructs are highly reliable and

    valid. Especially, it should form a good abstract construct for measuring misinterpretation in chats.

    Furthermore, future research should focus on experimental research to minimize potential effects of

    bias, control extraneous variables, and compensate the shortcomings the questionnaire brings along.

  • References

    Abbey, A., Abramis, D. J., & Caplan, R. D. (1985). Effects of Different Sources of Social Support and

    Social Conflict on Emotional Well-Being. Basic & Applied Social Psychology, 6(2), 111-129.

    Borko, H. (1968). Information science: What is it? American Documentation, 19(1), 3-5.

    CBS. (2012). Jaarboek onderwijs in cijfers. The Hague: CBS.

    CBS. (2014). Gezondheid, aandoeningen, beperkingen; leeftijd en geslacht. Retrieved from

    http://statline.cbs.nl/StatWeb/publication/?VW=T&DM=SLNL&PA=81174NED&D1=47-52,60-

    65&D2=1-2&D3=1-4&D4=0&D5=l&HD=111101-1022&HDR=G4,G3,G1,G2&STB=T.

    Conflict Research Consortium. (n.d.). Misinterpretation of Communication. Online Training Program

    on Intractable Conflict (OTPIC) Retrieved September, 25, 2014, from

    http://www.colorado.edu/conflict/peace/problem/misinter.htm

    Feldman, R. S., & Rim, B. (1991). Fundamentals of Nonverbal Behavior: Cambridge University

    Press.

    Fredrickson, B. L., & Joiner, T. (2002). Positive emotions trigger upward spirals toward emotional

    well-being. Psychological Science, 13(2), 172-175.

    Fullwood, C., & Martino, I. M. (2007). Emoticons and Impression Formation. Applied semiotics: a

    learned journal of literary research on the World Wide Web, 7(19).

    Hall, J. A. (1979). Gender, gender roles, and nonverbal communication skills. In R. Rosenthal (Ed.),

    Skill in nonverbal communication (pp. 32-67). Cambridge, MA: Oelgeschlager, Gunn and Hein.

    Jones, S. G. (1994). CyberSociety: Computer-Mediated Communication and Community (4th ed.):

    SAGE Publications.

    Ku, Y.-C., Chu, T.-H., & Tseng, C.-H. (2013). Gratifications for using CMC technologies: A

    comparison among SNS, IM, and e-mail. Computers in Human Behavior, 29(1), 226-234.

    Lisa, L., & Daniela, W. (2009). Computer-Mediated Communication in Virtual Learning

    Communities. In A. Chee Siang & Z. Panayiotis (Eds.), Human Computer Interaction: Concepts,

    Methodologies, Tools, and Applications (pp. 316-322). Hershey, PA, USA: IGI Global.

    National Research Council. (2010). Exploring the Intersection of Science Education and 21st Century

    Skills: A Workshop Summary. Washington DC: National Academies Press.

    Pickett, C. L., Gardner, W. L., & Knowles, M. (2004). Getting a cue: The need to belong and

    enhanced sensitivity to social cues. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 30(9), 1095-1107.

    Riggio, R. E. (1986). Assessment of Basic Social Skills. Journal of Personality of Social Psychology,

    51(3), 649-660.

    Simon, R. W. (2014). Sociological Scholarship on Gender Differences in Emotion and Emotional

    Well-Being in the United States: A Snapshot of the Field. Emotion Review, 6(3), 196-201.

    SkillsYouNeed. (n.d.). What are Social Skills? Retrieved September 25, 2014, from

    http://www.skillsyouneed.com/ips/social-skills.html

    Taesler, P., & Janneck, M. (2010). Emoticons and impression formation: the impact of emoticon use

    on the perception of online communication partners. Gruppendynamik Und

    Organisationsberatung, 41(4), 375-384.

    Thurlow, C., Lengel, L., & Tomic, A. (2004). Computer Mediated Communication: SAGE

    Publications.

    Vandergriff, I. (2013). Emotive communication online: A contextual analysis of computer-mediated

    communication (CMC) cues. Journal of Pragmatics, 51(0), 1-12.

    Walther, J. B. (1992). Interpersonal Effects in Computer-Mediated Interaction: A Relational

    Perspective. Communication Research, 19(1), 52-90.

    Watson, D., & Clark, L. A. (1984). Negative affectivity: The disposition to experience negative

    aversive emotional states. Psychological Bulletin, 96(3), 465-490.

    World Health Organization: Regional Office for Europe. (1998). Wellbeing measures in primary

    health care: The DepCare Project. Paper presented at the Consensus meeting, Stockholm, Sweden.

    Zhe, X., & Boucouvalas, A. C. (2002). Text-to-emotion engine for real time internet communication.

    Proceedings of International Symposium on Communication Systems, Networks and DSPs, 164-

    168.