When Difference is Not Dagerous

17
Katan 1 In (2001) “When Difference is not Dangerous: Modelling Intercultural Competence for Business”, Giuseppina Cortese and Dell Hymes (eds), Textus XIV, 2, pp. 287-306. David Katan When Difference is not Dangerous: Modelling Intercultural Competence for Business ____________________________________________________ 1. Introduction Interculturality, globalisation, „glocalisation‟ are some of the terms to describe the global village atmosphere of business today, along with the assertion that intercultural competence is necessary. A number of authors, as Brian Spitzberg (1997: 380) mentions, have provided “a list of skills, abilities, and attitudes … for competent interaction and adaptation”. Yet, “there is no sense of integration or coherence across lists”. This paper will begin with a discussion of „difference‟ and the competencies required to communicate effectively in a different culture. I will also discuss the beginnings of an integrated model of intercultural competence and will discuss some of the problems involved in achieving them. The main ideas for the model come from Neuro-Linguistic programming (NLP) and from intercultural studies, while the ideas regarding competencies come from business training programmes. 1.1 Response to ‘Difference’ It will be assumed here that individuals have the competencies necessary to communicate within their own culture, and also that they do not naturally or automatically possess the critical competencies necessary for dealing with different cultures. As the title suggests, approach to “difference” will be a central theme. As Bennett (1998: 3, emphasis in the original) says “the intercultural communication approach is difference-based(see also Barna 1998). According to Bennett (1993) and many others (e.g., Michael Page & Judith Martin 1996: 46; Janet Bennett et al: forthcoming), the normal response to what is different is ethnocentric. The type of response will vary according to the degree that the surrounding environment is perceived to be “‟different‟, and the level at which the difference is perceived. In the world of travel and tourism, „difference‟ is the attracting factor. Erik Cohen distinguishes response to another culture according to whether the person is an organized or individual mass-tourist, explorer or drifter: “the fundamental variable that forms the basis for the fourfold typology of tourist roles proposed here is strangeness versus familiarity” (1972: 177). The difference between the two “mass tourists” is the degree that they are „institutionalized‟ (i.e. the extent to which their travel experiences are organized by intermediaries within the tourism industry). Basically, though, both types of mass tourist “like to experience the novelty of the macroenvironment of a strange place from the security of a familiar microenvironment” (ibid: 166). This need for familiarity and comfort Cohen terms the “environmental bubble”. International business, of course, can be

description

Uploaded from Google Docs

Transcript of When Difference is Not Dagerous

Page 1: When Difference is Not Dagerous

Katan 1

In (2001) “When Difference is not Dangerous: Modelling Intercultural Competence for Business”,

Giuseppina Cortese and Dell Hymes (eds), Textus XIV, 2, pp. 287-306.

David Katan

When Difference is not Dangerous:

Modelling Intercultural Competence for Business

____________________________________________________

1. Introduction

Interculturality, globalisation, „glocalisation‟ are some of the terms to

describe the global village atmosphere of business today, along with the

assertion that intercultural competence is necessary. A number of authors, as

Brian Spitzberg (1997: 380) mentions, have provided “a list of skills,

abilities, and attitudes … for competent interaction and adaptation”. Yet,

“there is no sense of integration or coherence across lists”. This paper will

begin with a discussion of „difference‟ and the competencies required to

communicate effectively in a different culture. I will also discuss the

beginnings of an integrated model of intercultural competence and will

discuss some of the problems involved in achieving them. The main ideas

for the model come from Neuro-Linguistic programming (NLP) and from

intercultural studies, while the ideas regarding competencies come from

business training programmes.

1.1 Response to ‘Difference’

It will be assumed here that individuals have the competencies necessary to

communicate within their own culture, and also that they do not naturally or

automatically possess the critical competencies necessary for dealing with

different cultures. As the title suggests, approach to “difference” will be a

central theme. As Bennett (1998: 3, emphasis in the original) says “the

intercultural communication approach is difference-based” (see also Barna

1998). According to Bennett (1993) and many others (e.g., Michael Page &

Judith Martin 1996: 46; Janet Bennett et al: forthcoming), the normal

response to what is different is ethnocentric. The type of response will vary

according to the degree that the surrounding environment is perceived to be

“‟different‟, and the level at which the difference is perceived.

In the world of travel and tourism, „difference‟ is the attracting factor. Erik

Cohen distinguishes response to another culture according to whether the

person is an organized or individual mass-tourist, explorer or drifter: “the

fundamental variable that forms the basis for the fourfold typology of tourist

roles proposed here is strangeness versus familiarity” (1972: 177). The

difference between the two “mass tourists” is the degree that they are

„institutionalized‟ (i.e. the extent to which their travel experiences are

organized by intermediaries within the tourism industry). Basically, though,

both types of mass tourist “like to experience the novelty of the

macroenvironment of a strange place from the security of a familiar

microenvironment” (ibid: 166). This need for familiarity and comfort Cohen

terms the “environmental bubble”. International business, of course, can be

Page 2: When Difference is Not Dagerous

Katan 2

performed successfully from within one‟s own environmental bubble;

though, equally clearly, this will depend on the type and the degree of

foreign contact. Here, we will assume the more extreme situation of a

professional who will have been chosen for an assignment abroad, or will be

in regular face-to-face professional contact with multicultural teams.

If there is a period of holiday in another culture, for example, before an

assignment abroad, then the response to unfamiliarity may well be positive.

This is known as “the honeymoon period” (Levine and Adelman 1993: 41),

where difference attracts and arouses curiosity. However, just like any

honeymoon, this response to „the other‟ has little to do with immersion in

the day-to-day routine of intercultural life. It is, instead, the response of the

tourist from well inside his or her environmental bubble. Dell Hymes

(personal communication) is sceptical about the validity of this initial stage,

as are a number of other researchers, whose empirical research demonstrates

that predicted “the initial phase of psychological euphoria” (cf Colleen

Ward 1992: 131) simply does not occur during the first few weeks for Peace

Corps volunteers. The “honeymoon phase”, is, I suggest, the response of

Cohen‟s “mass-tourist”, from within the protection of their environmental

bubble.

Milton Bennett‟s (1993) Developmental Model of Intercultural Sensitivity

(DMIS), in fact, begins with the types of reaction individuals will tend to

have once removed from the protection of the bubble.

2. The Bennett Model

The DMIS has six stages, and each stage represents an important change in

beliefs about difference: “Specifically, we are interested in the way people

construe cultural difference” (ibid 1993: 24). Bennett‟s model is „culture

general‟ i.e. “it describes how learners overcome ethnocentrism regarding

their own cultures and how they achieve sensitivity to other cultures in

general” (Janet Bennett et al: forthcoming). It was developed to take into

account the very earliest developmental stage, from total cross-cultural

incompetence through to total competence.

This model, like any other model, is a generic classification of a long

process, and will not fit all circumstances. For further discussion see M.

Bennett (1993: 28) and J. Bennett et al (forthcoming).

2.1 The ethnocentric Stages

During „the honeymoon period‟, as we have already noted, individuals will

tend to be curious about the new culture, and may well learn to say “thank

you” and “hello/goodbye” in the foreign language: difference is stimulating

and expectations are positive. However, initially (stage 1 in the DMIS),

much of what is different will not actually be perceived. As John Gumperz

(1997: 45) puts it, differences in “contextualization strategies” remain

“undetected”. What little we do perceive about „the other‟ (whether it be the

language or the culture) will be distorted to fit our own cognitive

environment. Attention centres on our own interpretations based on

fulfilling our own expectations.

Page 3: When Difference is Not Dagerous

Katan 3

Bennett calls this stage „Denial‟, and may also involve the use of social or

physical barriers so that more fundamental differences in habitus (Bourdieu

1990, Katan and Straniero-Sergio forthcoming) are simply not encountered,

and the environmental bubble can remain intact.

The next stage (2) is „Culture shock‟: “emotional reactions to the

disorientation that occurs when one is immersed in an unfamiliar culture and

is deprived of familiar cues ...” (Paige, 1993: 2). It is at this stage, outside

the protective habitus of the environmental bubble that we realise that there

is a difference; and hence that there is a real gap between our expected

world and the world we are dealing with - and we can no longer deny the

fact. The „undetetected‟ contextualization cues mentioned by Gumperz

(ibid), at this stage, result in “a significant number of breakdowns” The

most natural reaction to difference in others‟ behaviour, discourse patterns

and value systems is to defend our own, particularly because the difference

is felt as a threat to core beliefs regarding what is „right‟, „normal‟ and

„correct‟: “What is Different is Dangerous” (Hofstede 1991: 109). Geert

Hofstede calls one of his four cultural dimensions „Uncertainty Avoidance‟,

which relates to a culture‟s toleration of what is unfamiliar. As William

Gudykunst (1995: 12) notes “Anxiety is the affective (emotional) equivalent

of uncertainty”. To prevent anxiety building up, the first reflex response is

either „fight or flight‟.

„Fight‟ responses to this anxiety may include active „Denigration‟, a feeling

of „Superiority‟, or even „Reversal‟, which is reversed superiority, i.e. a

belief that one‟s own culture is inferior to another. In all cases, at this stage

of intercultural sensitivity, there is an implicit assumption that evolution

following one particular culture path is the best path for all cultures.

Professional Defence will often be in terms of flight. A recent survey by

William M. Mercer consultants showed that “placing individual employees

[and] whole families – often into quite unfamiliar and challenging

circumstances” (FTexpat: Feb 2001: 4) accounts for most failed

assignments. More exactly, the survey of over 100 multinationals revealed

“family difficulties” and “adversity to change” as the two most significant

(and highly interrelated) factors affecting the success or failure of an

international assignment. This confirms previous results cited in Daniel

Kealey (1996: 83), who also points to other research which estimates that

between 15% to 40% of American business personnel return early; less than

50% of those who stay perform adequately, and the estimated cost for US

firms, in direct costs alone, is (1992 figures) $2 billion.

The last attempt (3) “to preserve the centrality of one‟s own worldview”

(Bennett 1993: 41) is „Minimisation‟, which may include physical or

transcendental Universalism. Put simply, at this stage, the belief is that we

are all human beings biologically or we are all equal under the eyes of

God/Allah – a belief which necessitates a single world, united by pan-

cultural norms, values and beliefs.

Fundamental to moving towards intercultural competence is the realisation

that individuals tend to perceive difference through a simplified mental

model of the world which tells us what to expect. We will now discuss how

Page 4: When Difference is Not Dagerous

Katan 4

this model functions before moving on to the ethnorelative stages and

intercultural competence.

2.1.1. Model of the World

Ideas regarding mental models of the world are well developed in, for

example, Johnson-Laird (1983), Sperber & Wilson (1986), Wallace Chafe

(1990) and Gary Palmer (1996). NLP, itself, was developed on the principle

of mental modelling to help therapists improve their success rate with their

clients‟ „limited model of the world‟ (Bandler and Grinder 1975). The

“Logical Levels of Culture” in Katan (1999a, 1999b) uses, and extends,

NLP theory to model Malinowski‟s (1923) “context of culture”. A further

refinement of the model is presented here, which takes specific account of

Bennett‟s model and the competencies required for successful cross-cultural

communication.

An earlier description of the mental model theory was put forward by one of

the forefathers of NLP, Alfred Korzybski ([1933] 1994: 58, emphasis in the

original), in which he used the metaphor of a “map”: “A map is not the

territory it represents, but, if correct, it has a similar structure to the

territory, which accounts for its usefulness” (cf. Saville-Troike 1989: 24).

This metaphor is particularly useful in intercultural communication as

cartographic maps are representations of topographic reality. They tell us

what to expect and orient us through a maze of local, regional, national or

international signs. The local maps cover a small area and are detailed.

Larger areas are either not covered by the map or are drawn with less detail.

The various filters and the Universal Modelling process are discussed in

Katan (1999a). Here, I would like to focus on 3 related processes in the

representation of „difference‟: Perception, Interpretation and Evaluation.

The evaluation of what is perceived externally will necessarily be according

to an internal map of the world. As Robert Rubenstein (1994: 1003) points

out “expectations about what is proper and good are cultural, and they are

encoded in a society‟s symbolic forms”. The founders of NLP, Bandler and

Grindler (1975: 10) call this filter „social genetics‟, which they define as “all

the categories or filters to which we are subject as members of a social

system: our language, our accepted ways of perceiving, and all the socially

agreed upon fictions”. So, what is potentially manifest may well be

discarded if it does not fit the cultural model of the world. If not deleted,

reality will tend to be distorted, or at least simplified so that it can fit the

model.

A further filter relates to individual constraints. A basic NLP tenet is that “If

the model of the client‟s experiences has pieces missing, it is impoverished.

Impoverished models ... imply limited options for behaviour. As the missing

pieces are recovered, the process of change in that person begins” (ibid: 41).

A simple extension that I would put forward is that a person who begins

working interculturally will also, necessarily, have a limited model of the

world, and too few options to communicate competently (cf. Spitzberg

1997: 384). It is, of course, these constraints that make it possible to orient

ourselves in the communication world so efficiently. We do not need to

process information from the external environment as if it were totally new,

Page 5: When Difference is Not Dagerous

Katan 5

but can rely on our cognitive environment to make the causal links between

what is observed (the perception), the meaning (the interpretation) and the

logical response (the evaluation). Hence reality is what our map says it is.

2.2. Ethnorelative Stages

Acceptance (4) is the first ethnorelative stage and represents “a major

conceptual shift” (Bennett: 1993: 45). People at this stage recognise that

there are different maps of the world, first at the level of „Behaviour‟, then

at the level of underlying communication norms or strategies. At the

„Adaptation‟ stage (5) “new skills appropriate to different world views are

acquired as an additive process” (ibid: 52, original emphasis). What is

important to note is that they can only be acquired once the belief system is

open to change, and “learners ... are able to see their own behaviour in a

cultural context” (Janet Bennett et al: forthcoming).

At the final stage (6), „Integration‟, rather than flexibility at the Behavioural

or Strategic level, Identity itself is a much looser and more flexible concept.

People at this level are „citizens of the world‟ rather than regionally or

nationally anchored and are free to make „contextual evaluation‟. As far as

Bennett is concerned, intercultural competence is gained at the level of

Adaptation; and certainly, most professionals have absolutely no need to

reach the final level, which can be an extremely alienating experience.

3. Competencies

We should now turn to the particular competencies necessary for successful

management of tasks and relationships in an intercultural world. Clearly, the

concept refers to skills or more generic abilities, as Dell Hymes notes (1992:

34). There are, broadly speaking, four types of competence (in business

„competency‟ is the recurrent term): language, communicative, work-related

or performance and inter or cross-cultural.

3.1. Language Competence

Language competence is to do with a speaker‟s knowledge of what

constitutes a well-formed sentence in a language regardless of situation.

Pragmatic competence or Dell Hymes‟ (1986) “communicative

competence” widens the concept to include the individual competencies

essential for successful or appropriate performance, which he calls

dimensions, as follows: knowledge of the systemic potential; competence in

appropriateness, occurrence and feasibility (Hymes 1997 13). This type of

competency requires both knowledge and expectation of who may or may

not speak in certain settings, when, how, and through which routines, “in

short, everything involving the use of language and other communicative

dimensions in particular settings” (Saville-Troike 1989: 21).

3.2. Work Related/Performance Competencies

Work-related competencies provide a framework for making people in

organisations more effective and able to perform better, by using top

performers‟ behaviour, strategies and capabilities as the benchmark.

Page 6: When Difference is Not Dagerous

Katan 6

Competence models are generally the result of work carried out by “an

expert panel” (Daniel Bouchard 1996: 131), a steering committee who

define the competencies for a given job or role. Information is collected

through hard facts (e.g. productivity data) and Behavioural Event Interviews

(BEIs) (Dale 1996: 43-49). BEIs are designed to “identify the thought and

behaviour patterns of people who are successful in the jobs being studied”

(Patricia Marshall 1996: 50). Results from these interviews are cross-

matched against „criterion groups‟, average performers, to distil the

behaviours or thinking patterns that are significantly different.

Yet, with regard to any intercultural component little has changed since

1990, when research carried out on the selection of employees to work

overseas (cited in Kealey 1996: 83) highlighted the fact that companies were

still continuing “to base their selection decisions primarily on technical

competence and experience and ignore the non-technical skills and

knowledge required for success in another culture”.

3.3. Work-Related Competencies and Cultures

Some work has been carried out by Hay/McBer (Larrere 1986: 68-73), for

example, in modelling intercultural competence for the business world.

They have designed job competency evaluations to highlight the differences

between „generic competencies‟, those found in, for example, successful

executives in a monoculture, and „critical‟ or „critical success differences‟,

those key competency differences which allow a small number of

professionals to be successful anywhere, whether it be “from one

organisation to another or from one culture to another or from one economic

circumstance to another” (Larrere 1996: 66).

Research, for example, carried out by Hay/McBer has isolated 5 critical

differences which they have added to their performance competencies.

1. Personal v Contractual Business Relationships

2. Bias for Action: Planning v Implementation

3. Exercising authority: Authoritarian v Participatory

4. Time horizon: Short v Long

5. Building Business Relationships: Personal v Contractual

These critical differences are posed as binary opposite cognitive

preferences, and are marked in their Global Adaptability Inventory in terms

of spread between the two extremes, and also in terms of their point of

balance between the two orientations. The closer the balance is to the centre,

and the wider the spread, the more globally adaptable the executive is

deemed to be:

Yet, these critical differences do not actually include any cognition of, or

interest in, culture. How individuals respond to another culture and what

resources they use once outside of their environmental bubble is totally

ignored.

3.4. Communicative Competencies and Culture

Page 7: When Difference is Not Dagerous

Katan 7

Communicative competence models have also included references to factors

influencing intercultural competence. In fact, according to Palmer (1996:

23) Hymes‟s central concern was to “treat speech as a system of cultural

behaviour”. This is the level that Alessandro Duranti (1988: 210) calls

“situated discourse” and can be related to Bronislaw Malinowski‟s “context

of situation” (Malinowski 1935: 18; Katan 1999a:72).

A person‟s map of the world, of course, is more than a series of encultured

communicative language norms and routines. In order to communicate

appropriately, as Muriel Saville-Troike (1989: 24) actually makes clear, not

only do we need “Linguistic knowledge” and “Interaction skills”, but also

“Cultural knowledge”, and in particular shared social knowledge; values and

attitudes; cognitive maps/schemata, and enculturation process (transmission

of knowledge and skills).

Yet, there is a crucial difference in level between communicative and

intercultural competence. While communicative competence answers the

question “How these signs work to channel communication” (Gumperz

1997: 39), and focuses on the abilities necessary to communicate or

participate effectively in a given culture, intercultural competence addresses

the motivation, the values and the beliefs necessary to participate in another

community. As Hymes (1992: 47-48) notes, it is entirely possible for

individuals without communicative competence to participate in a

community “through skilful use of such proficiency as they have”. It is,

then, at the level of intercultural competence that individuals will be

motivated (and sustained) in their decision to participate; and the same

competence will be the first guide to potential success or („flight/fight‟)

failure.

3.5. Intercultural Competencies

As mentioned at the beginning of this paper, there are many intercultural

organisations that have produced a list of those critical competencies

necessary to relate effectively in a variety of cultural contexts. Daniel

Kealey‟s (1986: 84) research on a variety of groups in international

companies and the military concludes as follows: “contrary to popular

opinion, there was a subsequent consensus on the non-technical criteria

required for success in another culture”.

There is also a fair degree of consensus with a more recent list compiled by

TCO-Human Diversity Management-WorldWork (Trickey & Ewington

2001a: unpublished report). In all cases there are 10 orientations, which all

include „Interest in local culture‟ „Flexibility‟, „Openness‟, „Initiative‟ or

„Influencing‟, and „Positive Self-Image or Personal autonomy/Emotional

Resilience‟. TCO-Worldwork has a stronger accent on communication

(listening orientation, sensitivity to context, perceptiveness and

transparency) while Kealey‟s list focuses on more holistic factors, such as

„Empathy‟, „Respect‟, „Sociability‟ and „Tolerance‟.

To move away from lists, though, we need to think in terms of modelling

competency. The NLP map of the world, mentioned earlier, makes a good

beginning.

Page 8: When Difference is Not Dagerous

Katan 8

4. Modelling the Map of the World

Dilts termed his model of the modelling process “The Unified Field Model”

(Dilts 1990: 138; O‟Connor & Seymour 1990: 88-92) after Einstein‟s

attempt to “tie together all physical theories into a single model”. It is, like

the rest of NLP, “an operational framework that synthesize[s] the fields of

neurology, linguistics and artificial intelligence” (Dilts 1996:1). My

adaptation was as follows :

SSLM

Past Future Present 1st

2nd

3rd

Environment

Visible behaviour

Norms/Action chains

Values

Beliefs

Cultural Identity

perceptual position

4.1 Meta Programs

A further modification is suggested by Mike Cechanowicz (2001: 1) who

has adapted the Model to take Meta Programs into account: “context free

filter systems that tell us what information is important to pay attention to ...

generalisations about importance which have belief systems to support

them”. Dilts, who coined the term for this type of filter says they “define our

general approach to a particular issue rather than the details” (1990: 217),

and organise what we sort for (e.g., „same‟ or „difference‟), or how we

orient ourselves to goals and problems, to others, with regard to time,

information organisation and chunk size (cf. Katan 1999a:167-176).

The exact number or type of Meta Programs involved in modelling reality

depends on the scholar. Shelle Rose Charvet (1997), following Roger

Bailey‟s business oriented “Language and Behaviour Profile”, itemises 14

Meta Programs, a reduction from the original 60 postulated by Leslie

Cameron Bandler. She defines these as “the specific filters we use to

interact with the world. They edit and shape what we allow to come in from

the outside world. They also mould what comes in from inside ourselves as

we communicate and behave in the world” (ibid: 11). The first six

categories show “how different people trigger their motivation” (ibid: 17),

i.e. what people will be open to perceiving, and in what terms they will

interpret what they have perceived. For example, the first motivation trait is

proactive/reactive. Will a person tend to act on the environment with little or

no consideration or rather wait, consider and analyse the situation before

acting? A successful interculturalist, as we shall see, will need a strong

reactive tendency.

Page 9: When Difference is Not Dagerous

Katan 9

Another Meta Program sorts for „Sameness‟ or „Difference‟ (cf. Katan

1999a: 168). Which orientation is it more useful to be open to receiving?

Both lists of intercultural competencies mentioned above include

„Openness‟, which suggests, as the TCO-WorldWork document states:

“People who are open tend to be receptive to new ideas (‘new thinking’)

and keen to build relationships with people very different from themselves

(‘welcoming strangers’). They also accept the way others do things

(‘acceptance’), even when attitudes and behaviours are very different from

their own” (Trickey and Ewington 2001a).

The Meta Programme that these contextualised behaviours refer to is the

„Difference pattern‟, which has a less useful side to it: “People with a

Difference pattern love change; they thrive on it and want it to be constant

and major. They will resist static or stable situations” (Charvet 1997: 78).

More useful would be “Sameness with Exception and Difference (the

double pattern): People with this double pattern like change and

revolutionary shifts but are also comfortable where things are evolving.

They are happy with both revolution and evolution” (ibid).

The basic difference between „the lists‟ and Meta Programs is that “Meta

Programs simply describe the form of our door [through which we interact

with the world], what specifically we let in and out in a given situation. It is

this recognition ... that sets this tool apart from the psychometric profiles

that make sweeping generalizations about our personality” (ibid: 11).

4.2 Intercultural Competence Model

The adaptation of the “Logical Levels of Culture” model for intercultural

competence begins with the Meta Program filter, followed then by two sets

of Logical Levels. The first set represents the context of culture (cf. Katan

1999a: 72-74), which will act as a filter, deleting, distorting and generalising

what the Meta Programs have already modified. This map acts as an

environmental bubble; it provides the orientation and the communication

norms for the more individual map of the world to work with. An

individual‟s mission (direction in life), identity (beliefs about role), beliefs

(about „the other‟), cluster and order of values, and of course capabilities

will, in the first instance, be framed by the higher context of culture: „the

world‟. If we take the case of „the fluent fool‟ (Janet Bennett et al,

forthcoming ), “the person who learns language without learning culture”,

s/he will expect to work within this environmental bubble with all

communities, simply because there is no other map to work with:

Fundamentally, Dilts‟s model remains valid; each level still psychologically

encompasses the level below, following Whitehead & Russell (1910),

Korzybski (1933) and Bateson (1972: 177-200). A competent

interculturalist, on the other hand, according to the proposed model, will be

less subject to a context of culture. In fact, the framing process will initially

be more fluid and more interconnected. Michael Hall (2001) suggests there

is, in fact, a set of capabilities, values and beliefs operating simultaneously

at each logical level. As a person passes through to stages 5 and 6 of the

Developmental Model, so the framing is reversed, with the individual map

of the world framing, or determining, a particular context of culture to act

Page 10: When Difference is Not Dagerous

Katan 10

within. Logically, for this to happen, there will need to be a meta-level,

above both sets of maps, able to organise and choose which contexts of

culture to activate. In a similar vein, Meinert Meyer (1991: 143) suggests

that, what he calls, transcultural competence is the ability to “stand above

both his (sic) own and the foreign culture”, whereas, for Meyer, intercultural

competence is more simply bi-cultural competence. There are two maps or

contexts of culture to choose from, but no meta-level or competency from

which to make that choice.

5. Meta-competences

The meta-thinking style related to choosing the most appropriate map was

originally outlined by Korzybski in the 1930‟s, and is the sign of the

breakthrough to the ethnorelative stages. Following Bennett‟s DMIS, at the

„contextual evaluation‟ stage, the interculturalist will have “the skill to shift

cultural context and the concomitant self-awareness necessary to exercise

choice” (1993: 61). There are two main aspects: intension/extension and

perceptual position.

5.1. Intension/Extension

Korzybski (1994: lviii) in his introduction to the second edition of his

treatise on General Semantics describes two orientations which he borrowed

from logic: „intension‟ and „extension‟. He pointed out (xlvi, emphasis in

the original) that “every identification is bound to be in some degrees a mis-

evaluation”. The perception, interpretation and evaluation filters mentioned

earlier are learnt „intensional‟ Aristotelian orientations. They are dangerous

when our map of the world, the “mere nervous constructs inside our skulls”

(ibid: lii), are believed to reflect reality, and not the learnt response: “we

react ‘as if’ our half-truths or false knowledge were „all there is to be

known‟. Thus we are bound to be bewildered, confused, obsessed with

fears, etc., because of mistakes due to our mis-evaluations, when we orient

ourselves by verbal structures which do not fit facts” (ibid: xlviii, emphasis

in original).

On the other hand „extensional‟, non-Aristotelian orientations, “induce an

automatic delay of reactions, which automatically stimulates the cortical

region and regulates and protects the reactions of the usually over-

stimulated thalamic regions” (ibid: lviii, emphasis in original). It is this

extensional orientation which allows people to be open to learning. What

this means is the ability to perceive and interpret (visualise in Korzybski‟s

language) without immediately evaluating, or rather, without responding

through conditioned reflexes. Common sense would probably tell us what

research has also found: that “expats who adjust well are non-judgemental

and non-evaluative when interpreting the behaviour of locals” (Redding

1996: 393). These expats have, what William Gudykunst (1995: 16) (citing

Langer) terms „mindfulness‟ (rather than „mindlessness‟): the ability to

“consciously decide to stop automatically processing information”.

This ability depends on the existence of another particular ability, the ability

to change frame or perceptual position, and to “stand above”.

Page 11: When Difference is Not Dagerous

Katan 11

5.2. Perceptual Position

This is, at its most basic, “a particular perspective or point of view” (Dilts

1990: 218). It is similar to a Meta Program in that it is context free; and is

essential both for the extensional orientation and for shifting the order of

framing.

To make an informed decision an individual must be able to disassociate

from the anchoring context of one culture, and be able to decide which

context of culture to work within at any one time (or “such proficiency as

they have”) to produce the most effective behaviour. Disassociation,

through change in perceptual position, will always lower the emotional

involvement and anxiety with „the other‟, and automatically reduces the

„fight or flight‟ Meta Program response to perceived threat:

The first perceptual position is that of full internal association. The

individual does not perceive „the other‟, except in terms of „self‟. Any

perceived differences in reality are automatically „corrected‟ through the

Universal Modelling process to fit the person‟s internal model of the world.

An example given by Dilts (1990: 8-9) is of a psychiatric patient convinced

that he is dead. A doctor proves to him that this cannot be so by making an

incision in the patient‟s arm. The patient seeing the blood explains: “I‟ll be

damned. Corpses do bleed!”. In terms of interculturality this is equivalent to

the “tourist fantasy [which] rules out the possibility of authentic cultural

experience” (Bryan Turner 1994: 185).

The 2nd

perceptual position is still associated. The focus is external, towards

„the other‟, so difference is perceived as difference. The tourist is no longer

living out a fantasy; s/he is fully aware of the reality. The interpretation and

the evaluation, on the other hand, is still, however, internal, in terms of his

or own personal and culture-bound norms, which will not necessarily be

valued as expected.

The 3rd

perceptual position, on the other hand, is disassociated. In this

position an individual is able to adopt a third frame of reference, or rather a

meta-position, from which s/he is able to perceive, interpret and evaluate

both the model of the world of „self‟ and that of the „other‟. It is this level of

awareness, the 3rd

position, also known as „mind shifting‟ (see Katan 1999b:

Individual Map of the World

Mission Identity

Beliefs

Values

Contexts of Culture 1, 2, 3,....

Metaprograms

e.g. reactive, same with difference

Capabilities

Behaviour

Environment: Reality

part of Context of Culture 2

3rd perceptual

position.

A Meta-State

Identity-

interculturalist

Beliefs - relativity of

personal/cultural

maps

Beliefs

Values

Norms

Beliefs

Values

Norms

Beliefs

Values

Norms

1st position

2nd

position

Associated Perceptual Positions Disassociated Perceptual Position

Intercultural Competence

Communicative Competence

Performance competence

Language competence

Page 12: When Difference is Not Dagerous

Katan 12

419) which is absolutely vital to intercultural competence. Giuseppina

Cortese (1999: 348), writing about the translator, also points to the

importance of “Mobilizing the higher-order cognitive resources, which have

scope at the metalevel ... to question all the meaning making levels of a text

...”.

The ability to disassociate, to move above one particular mindset, logically

entails entering and associating into another mindset which is at a meta-

level to both one‟s own map of the world and that of the context of culture.

This is what Michael Hall (1995: 12-13, emphasis in the original) calls a

“meta-state”: These “…represent highly complex pieces of awareness

involving self-reflexive consciousness ... This unique human ability to think

about oneself, one‟s thinking, feeling, choosing, behaving, etc. creates the

ability to transcend one‟s immediate time, space, being, values,

experiences, etc. and to bring other awareness to bear upon things”.

6 Training for competence

As Korzybski ([1933] 1994: 456, emphasis in the original) makes clear, “it

is practically impossible, without special training” to transcend one‟s

socially conditioned reflexes . There is, in fact, now a clear idea of the need

to concentrate on those areas “associated with success that are perhaps more

amenable to modification through cross-cultural training programs …”

(Kealey 1996: 84). Examples cited by Kealey are “work role clarity” and

“cultural knowledge” compared with the less modifiable “individual‟s

flexibility or capacity for relationship building”. The TCO-WorldWork list

of „untrainables‟ includes: Openness, (aspects of) Flexibility, Personal

autonomy, and Emotional resilience.

Gudykunst et al (1996: 62) actually warn against the reports (that they

themselves cite) which suggest that short-term experiential cultural

sensitivity training is effective in increasing cultural awareness and

potentially changing attitudes. In fact, a number of interculturalists working

in business believe that the training can only be effective during or after a

trainee has lived or worked in another culture (Williams and Bent: 1996:

392). Trickey (personal communication) disagrees and continue to use, for

example, „Culture Shock‟ simulations to raise awareness of the intercultural

dimension (for further discussion see Barna 1983; Furnham 1993: 100; J.

Bennett et al forthcoming, Weaver 1993: 162). Most of the argument can be

reduced to questions regarding the trainee‟s ability to handle the anxiety of

experiencing culture shock, which may be simulated in terms of content, but

is certainly not so in terms of trainee response.

Other aspects can have a more serious affect on training: the competence

of the trainer; the limited time usually available to attempt to induce change;

and the clash between trainee and the overall organizational needs, both real

and imagined. Trickey and Ewington, for example, accept that the following

competencies are not trainable: Openness, Personal autonomy, (aspects of)

Flexibility and Emotional resilience. Unfortunately these also happen to be

the fundamental competencies required for assignments abroad and for

regular face-to-face business in an international context. “Openness” is

Page 13: When Difference is Not Dagerous

Katan 13

related to the “Same/Difference” sorting pattern and Bennett‟s DMIS. Only

when a trainee is at stage 4 will a trainee be motivated to learn about and

respect difference. Clearly, this will be an „intellectual‟ stage 4. In practice,

actual and prolonged contact with „difference‟ outside the protective

environmental bubble will tend to result in some form of culture shock and

negative evaluation, however positive and „open‟ the intentions (cf. Colleen

Ward 1992: 131) .

6.1 Towards a Hierarchy of Competencies

Though there is not the space here to develop or support the argument, I

would suggest that the „untrainables‟ are specifically linked to meta-

programmes and perceptual position, and hence true intercultural

competencies while the other competencies are communicative

competencies, which can be more easily developed. These competencies

underused resources within an individual‟s map of the world, such as

Perceptiveness, Listening Orientation, and Transparency. Below these are

the technical/work related competencies and language competence, which

can only be activated appropriately across cultures once the meta

programmes and perceptual positions are functioning at interculturally

competent level.

With regard to the essential competencies, according to NLP: “Meta

Programs may appear to be part of our individual nature, and therefore

permanent; in fact [they] can shift in response to changes in ourselves and

our surrounding environment” (Charvet 1997: 11), which is exactly in line

with Bennett‟s Developmental Model. NLP methodology has been

developed around change, and in particular around opening up options in an

individual‟s map of the world, though Meta Program modification is

certainly more problematic (cf Cechanowicz 2001: 1). This is not the area,

though, for intercultural trainers, but for therapists, which leads to the

greatest problem regarding Meta Program change for intercultural

competence. NLP generally operates on the one condition that the client is

at least aware that a change of some sort is required. Very often, however,

the professionals who would most benefit from change are those least likely

to be interested in making any. These people will be firmly anchored to one

of the ethnocentric stages, which they believe reflects reality. These

participants, managers, who are used to taking the initiative and making

decisions based on their own resources, do not perceive any need to change

their highly developed (and highly rewarded) intension orientation.

6.2 Intercultural training today

With this realisation, a number of organisations are now using a

psychometric inventory of cross-cultural sensitivity to assess a participant

before any training (Mitchell Hammer 1988). They are designed to generate

a graphic profile of an individual‟s predominant stage of development. A

more recent test, in the process of being produced (Trickey and Ewington

2001) is called the “International Profiler”. The aim of this questionnaire is

not so much to test how ethnocentric or ethnorelative an individual is, but

rather to generate a more complete profile of orientations, the potential

Page 14: When Difference is Not Dagerous

Katan 14

competencies. The same test may also be used to benchmark the job itself;

so professional intercultural encounters and assignments may soon be

classified according to a specific profile of intercultural competencies to be

matched by suitable professionals. We are still, though, a long way from

overcoming the first barrier and ensuring that what is different be a

challenge rather than a danger.

Page 15: When Difference is Not Dagerous

Katan 15

REFERENCES

Bandler R. and J. Grinder, 1975, The Structure of Magic I, Science and Behavior Books,

Palo Alto, CA.

Bateson G., 1972, Steps to an Ecology of Mind, Ballantine Books, New York.

Bouchard D., 1996, “Getting the Right People for the Job”, in N. Boulter, M. Dalziel and J.

Hill (eds.), People and Competencies: The Route to Competetive Advantage, Kogan

Page, London, pp. 129-140.

Bandura A., 1982, „Self-Efficacy Mechanism in Human Agency‟, American Psychologist, 37, pp. 122-147.

Barna L. M., 1983, “The Stress Factor in Intercultural Relations”, in D. Landis and R.

Brislin (eds.), Handbook of Intercultural Training 2, Pergamon, Elmsford (NY),

pp.19-49

Bassnett, S., R. M, Bollettieri Bosinelli, M, Ulrych (eds.) Translation Studies Revisited,

Textus, XII, 2.

Bennett M. J., 1993, “Towards Ethnorelativism: A Developmental Model of Intercultural

Sensitivity”, in R. M. Paige (ed.), pp. 21-72.

Bennett, J., Bennett M., and W. Allen, forthcoming, “Developing Culture in the Language

Classroom, in R. M. Paige, D. Lange, and Y. A. Yershova, (eds.), Culture as the

Core: Integrating Culture into the Language Curriculum,: Center for Advanced Research in Language Acquisition, University of Minnesota (MI).

Boulter, N., M. Dalziel and J. Hill (eds.), People and Competencies: The Route to

Competitive Advantage, Kogan Page, London

Bourdieu, P., 1990, in Other Words: Essays Towards a Reflexive Sociology, Polity Press,

Cambridge.

Cechanowicz M., 2001, “Replacing the Neurological levels-Model with a More Accurate

Version – The Psychological Structure of Human Systems, Active Choice”, in NLP,

www.acnlp.no/newnl.htm.

Chafe W., 1990, “Some Things That Narratives Tell Us about the Mind”, in B. K. Britton

and A. D. Pellegrini (eds.), Narrative Thought and Narrative Language, Lawrence

Erlbaum Associates, Hillsdale (NJ), pp. 79-98.

Charvet S. R., 1997, Words That Change Minds: Mastering the Language of Influence, Kendell/Hunt, Dubuque (OH).

Cohen E., 1972, “Towards a Sociology of International Tourism”, Social Research, 39, 1,

pp. 164-182.

Cortese G., 1999, “The Imperfect Mediator”, in S. Bassnett, R. M, Bollettieri Bosinelli, M,

Ulrych (eds.), pp. 333-350.

Coupland N. and A. Jaworski (eds.), 1997, Sociolinguistics: a Reader and Coursebook,

Macmillan Press, Basingstoke, Hampshire.

Dale M.,1996, How to be a Better Interviewer, Kogan Page/Industrial Society, London.

Dilts R., 1990 Changing Belief Systems with NLP, Meta Publications, Cupertino, CA.

Dilts R., 1996, Systemic NLP – A Unified Field Theory, http://www.nlpu.com/unified.htm.

Ftexpat, 2001, “Getting the Balance Right: is Sufficient Attention Being Paid to the Cost of Failed Assignments?”, February, p. 4.

Duranti, A., 1988, “Ethnography of Speaking: Towards a Linguistics of Praxis”, in

Linguistics: The Cambridge Survey, vol. 4: Language: The Sociocultural Context,

Cambridge University of Press, Cambridge, pp. 210-228.

Furnham A.,1993, “Communicating in Foreign Lands: the Cause, Consequences and Cures

of Culture Shock”, in Culture and Language Learning in Higher Education, M. Byran

(ed.), Multilingual Matters, Cleveland (OH), pp. 91-109.

Gudykunst W., 1995, “Anxiety/Uncertainty Management (AUM) Theory: Current Status,

in Intercultural Communication Theory, R. L. Wiseman (ed.), Sage, Thousand Oaks

(CA), pp. 8-58.

Gudykunst W. B., R. M. Guzley, M. M. Hammer, 1996, “Designing Intercultural Training”,

in D. Landis and R. S. Bhagat (eds.), Sage, Thousand Oaks (CA), pp. 61-80.

Page 16: When Difference is Not Dagerous

Katan 16

Gumperz, J. J., 1997, “Communicative Competence”, in N. Coupland and A, Jaworski

(eds.) Sociolinguistics: a Reader and Coursebook, Macmillan Press, Basingstoke,

Hampshire, pp. 39-48.

Hall, M., 1995, Meta-States: A Domain of Logical Levels, Empowerment Technologies,

Grand Junction (CO).

Hall, M., 2001, How Meta-States Enriches Logical Levels in NLP,

http://www.neurosemantics.com/ArticlesMS_In_Logical_Levels.htm

Hammer, M., 1988, “A Measure of Intercultural Sensitivity: The Intercultural Development

Inventory”, in, S. Fowler and M. Fowler (eds,), The Intercultural Sourcebook: Vol 2,

Intercultural Press, Yarmouth (ME).

Hofstede, G., 1991, Cultures and Organisations: Software of the Mind, McGraw-Hill, London.

Hymes, D., 1986, “Models of the Interaction of Language and Social Life” in J. Gumperz

and D. Hymes (eds.), Directions in Sociolinguistics: The Ethnography of

Communication, Blackwell, Oxford, pp. 35-71.

Hymes, D., 1992, “The Concept of Communicative Competence”, in M. Pütz (ed.) Thirty

Years of Linguistic Evolution, John Benjamins, Amsterdam and Philadelphia, pp. 31-

58.

Hymes, D., 1997, “The Scope of Sociolinguistics”, in N. Coupland and A, Jaworski (eds.),

pp. 12-22.

Johnson-Laird, P. N., 1983, Mental Models: Towards a Cognitive Science of Language,

Inference, and Consciousness, Cambridge University Press, Cambridge. Katan, D., 1999a, Translating Cultures: an Introduction for Translators, Interpreters and

Mediators, St. Jerome, Manchester.

Katan, D., 1999b, “‟What is it that‟s going on here?‟: Mediating Cultural Frames in

Translation, in S. Bassnett, R. M, Bollettieri Bosinelli, M, Ulrych (eds.), pp. 409-426.

Katan, D. and F. Straniero-Sergio, forthcoming, “Submerged Ideologies in Media

Interpreting” in …..

Kealey D. J., 1996, “The Challenge of International Personnel Selection”, in D. Landis and

R. S. Bhagat (eds.), pp. 81-105.

Korzybski, A., [1933] 1994, Science and Sanity: An Introduction to Non-Aristotelian

Systems and General Semantics, 5th Edition, Institute of General Semantics, Eglewood

(NJ). Landis D. and R. S. Bhagat (eds.), Handbook of Intercultural Training, 2nd Edition, Sage,

Thousand Oaks (CA).

Larrere J., Worldwide Leadership Differences, in N. Boulter, M. Dalziel and J. Hill (eds.),

pp. 61-74.

Levine, D. R. and M. B. Adelman, 1993, Beyond Language: Cross-Cultural

Communication, Englewood, Prentice-Hall Cliffs (NJ).

Malinowski, B., 1935, Coral Gardens and their Magic, vol. 2, Allen and Unwin, London.

Reprinted as 1967, The Language of Magic and Gardening, Indiana University

Studies in the History and Theory of Linguistics, Indiana University Press,

Bloomington, Indiana.

Marshall, P., 1996, “Why are Some People More Successful Than Others?”, in N. Boulter,

M. Dalziel and J. Hill (eds.), pp. 49-60. Meyer , Meinert, 1991, “Developing Transcultural Competence: Case Studies of advanced

Foreign Language Learners, in D. Buttjes and M. Byram (eds) Mediating Languages

and Cultures:Towards a Theory of Foreign Language Education, Multilingual

Matters, Clevedon, PA, pp. 136-158.

O'Connor, J. and J. Seymour, 1990, An Introduction to Neurolinguistic Programming,

Mandela, London.

Paige. R. M (ed.), 1993, Education for the Intercultural Experience, Intercultural Press,

Yarmouth (MA).

Paige, M. R., 1993, “On the Nature of Intercultural Experiences and Intercultural

Education‟, in Education for the Intercultural Experience, in M. R. Paige (ed.), pp. 1-

21.

Page 17: When Difference is Not Dagerous

Katan 17

Paige, R. M. and J. N. Martin, 1996, “Ethics in Intercultural Training”, in D. Landis and R.

S. Bhagat (eds.), pp. 35-60.

Palmer, G. B., 1996, Towards a Theory of Cultural Linguistics, University of Texas Press,

Austin, Texas.

Williams, G. and R. Bent, 1996, “Expatriate Managers for Southeast Asia”, in D. Landis

and R. S. Bhagat (eds), pp. 383-399

Rubenstein, R. A., 1994. “Collective Violence and Common Security“ in Tim Ingold (ed.),

Companion Encyclopedia of Anthropology, Routledge, London/New York.

Saville-Troike M., 1989, The Ethnography of Communication: An Introduction, 2nd Edition,

Basil Blackwell, New York.

Sperber, D. And D. Wilson, 1986, Relevance: Communication and Cognition, Basil Blackwell, London.

Spitzberg B. H., 1997, “A Model of Intercultural Competence”, in L. A Samovar and R. E.

Porter. (eds.), Intercultural Communication, Wadsworth. Belmont, CA.

Trickey D., and N. Ewington, 2001a, Competencies for Transferring Job Expertise to an

International Market, , TCO-Human Diversity Management/WorldWork,

unpublished.

Trickey D., and N. Ewington, 2001b, The International Profiler Questionnaire, TCO-

Human Diversity Management/WorldWork,. unpublished

Turner B., 1994, Orientalism, Post-Modernism and Globalism, Routledge, London.

Whitehead, A. N. and B. Russell, 1910, Principia Mathematica, 2nd Edition, Cambridge

University Press, Cambridge.