linguistics.byu.edu  · Web viewGlottal stops in English appear in a variety of contexts. They...

60
A production and perception study of /t/ glottalization and oral releases following glottals in the US David Eddington, Brigham Young University, [email protected] (corresponding author) Earl K. Brown, Brigham Young University, [email protected] 4041 JFSB Provo, Utah 84602 (801) 422-7452 Short title (<50 characters): /t/ glottalization and oral releases Page 1 of 60

Transcript of linguistics.byu.edu  · Web viewGlottal stops in English appear in a variety of contexts. They...

Page 1: linguistics.byu.edu  · Web viewGlottal stops in English appear in a variety of contexts. They often precede vowel-initial words such as ‘into’ [ʔɪnthuʊ] and serve to break

A production and perception study of /t/ glottalization and oral releases following glottals in the US

David Eddington, Brigham Young University, [email protected] (corresponding author)

Earl K. Brown, Brigham Young University, [email protected]

4041 JFSB

Provo, Utah 84602

(801) 422-7452

Short title (<50 characters):

/t/ glottalization and oral releases

Page 1 of 38

Page 2: linguistics.byu.edu  · Web viewGlottal stops in English appear in a variety of contexts. They often precede vowel-initial words such as ‘into’ [ʔɪnthuʊ] and serve to break

A production and perception study of /t/ glottalization and oral releases following glottals in the US

Abstract:

The articulation of /t/ in American English varies according to linguistic and extralinguistic factors. Concerning social factors, word-final /t/ glottalization is seen more among speakers of African American English (Farrington 2018), younger speakers (Partin-Hernandez 2005, Roberts 2006), and women (Byrd 1994, Eddington and Channer 2010).

This paper examines the production and perception of /t/ in five US states: Indiana, Mississippi, New Mexico, Utah, Vermont. For the production study, participants read a letter containing 24 prenasal word-medial /t/s (e.g., kitten) and 28 prevocalic word-final /t/s (e.g., not ever). For the perception study, 22 speakers recorded a unique sentence, each of which was manipulated acoustically in order to yield both oral and nasal releases of prenasal word-medial /t/ (e.g. button [bʌʔən] vs [bʌʔn̩]), as well as tap and glottal stop pronunciations of prevocalic word-final /t/ (e.g. not ever [nɑɾɛvɚ] vs. [nɑʔɛvɚ]). Next, these recordings were presented to participants who rated the speakers in terms of their perceived age, friendliness, pleasantness, rurality, education level, and whether they were from the same state as the participants.

The production results for prenasal word-medial /t/ (e.g. button) indicate that younger speakers produced oral releases more often than their older counterparts. Age also was related to the realization of prevocalic word-final /t/ as a glottal stop (e.g., not ever), such that younger speakers and women produced glottal stops more often than older speakers. In the perception study, speakers who used glottal stops were viewed as less educated and less friendly. Speakers who used oral releases were perceived as more rustic and less educated.

This paper contributes to the literature documenting the production and perception of /t/ in American English and to the literature that demonstrates the usefulness of using both production and perceptual data to study language variation (e.g., Brown 2015).

Keywords:

glottal stop, tap, oral release, English /t/, American English, experiment, matched guise

Acknowledgements:

ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS GO HERE, IF/ONCE ACCEPTED FOR PUBLICATION

1. Introduction

Glottal stops in English appear in a variety of contexts. They often precede vowel-initial words

such as ‘into’ [ʔɪnthuʊ] and serve to break up hiatus between vowels: ‘spa appointment’

[spaʔəpɔɪntmɨnt]. They also appear as an allophone of /t/, occurring most often in the syllable coda as

Page 2 of 38

Page 3: linguistics.byu.edu  · Web viewGlottal stops in English appear in a variety of contexts. They often precede vowel-initial words such as ‘into’ [ʔɪnthuʊ] and serve to break

in ‘catfish’ [khæʔfɪʃ] (Seyfarth and Garallek 2015). The particular realization of /t/ in word-internal

prevocalic position is a salient characteristic that marks speakers of particular varieties. For example,

North American and some Irish varieties favor taps in this position (e.g., ‘better’ [bɛɾɝ]), while many

Scottish and English varieties prefer glottal stops (e.g., [bɛʔɜ]).

The first documented evidence of glottal pronunciations are found in Britain in the late 19th

century (Andrésen 1968, Collins and Mees 1996), and in the 20th century they have been observed in

many different varieties of English: Received Pronunciation (RP) (Fabricius 2000; Wells 1997),

London (Wells 1982), Scotland (Marshall 2003; Reid 1978), Estuary English (Coggle 1993),

Newcastle (Docherty and Foulkes 1999), Ipswich (Straw and Patrick 2007), Cardiff (Mees 1987), U.S.

(Byrd 1994), New York City (Wells 1982; Levon 2006), Vermont (Roberts 2006), Appalachia (Wells

1982), and California (Partin-Hernandez 2005).

Word-final /t/ is of particular interest because it may be followed by either a consonant, where a

glottal realization is more common, or by a vowel, in which case the tap [ɾ] often occurs. Word-

finally, /t/ occurs in an alternating context where a great deal of variation is expected, and where the

variation in pronunciation is often associated with social factors (Straw and Patrick 2007). For

example, word-final t-glottalization is a characteristic of African American English (Farrington 2019).

In the U.K., younger speakers appear to glottalize more (Klánová 2016, Macaulay 1977, Marshall

2003, Tollfree 1999), a trend also observed in New Zealand (Holmes 1995), and the U.S. (Eddington

and Taylor 2009, Eddington and Channer 2010, Partin-Hernandez 2005, Roberts 2006). Gender is also

a factor, and many studies cite women as more prone to glottalization in the U.S. (Byrd 1994,

Eddington and Taylor, 2009, Eddington and Channer 2010), in New Zealand (Holmes 1995), and in the

U.K. (Klánová 2016, Milroy, Milroy, Hartley, and Walshaw 1994).

Nevertheless, there are some exceptions to these trends. In RP, men are more likely to glottalize

(Barrera 2015). In one study of the Reform American Jewish community, male speakers were also

Page 3 of 38

Page 4: linguistics.byu.edu  · Web viewGlottal stops in English appear in a variety of contexts. They often precede vowel-initial words such as ‘into’ [ʔɪnthuʊ] and serve to break

observed to glottalize more than women (Levon 2006). In like manner, men in Vermont used more

glottal stops than women (Roberts 2006). However, in another study (Bellavance 2017) consisting of

Vermont children at the fourth grade through high school levels, age and gender were not significant

predictors of glottalization. In Ohio, Kazmierski (2018) observed an interaction between age and

gender to the effect that younger women used more glottal stops than both older women, and men in

both age groups.

A number of linguistic variables have been considered in addition to the social variables. In

Barrera’s study of RP (2015), lexical frequency, number of syllables, and preceding vowel were

significant factors. The least clear factor is the role of the vowel following /t/. Ostalski (2013) found

that back vowels favor glottal stops, while Eddington and Taylor (2009) reported that front vowels

favored glottal realizations of /t/. Kazmierski (2018), in contrast, observed no significant influence of

vowel backness. More agreement exists in the literature on the role of the stress on the syllable of the

following vowel, where a following stressed vowel appears to favor glottalization (Eddington and

Channer 2010, Kazmierski 2018).

In American English, glottal stops tend to occur before consonants, while taps generally

precede vowels. If this is the case, why would word-final /t/ be realized as a glottal stop before a word-

initial vowel (e.g. abou[ʔ] any)? Eddington and Channer (2010) searched the spoken section of the

Corpus of Contemporary American English (Davies 2008) for all bigrams in which the first word

ended in /t/ and observed that such words are more than twice as likely to be followed by a consonant

than a vowel. According to exemplar theory, this means that there would be more glottalization, hence

more instances stored in memory with glottal stops than with other pronunciations. This would result in

a frequency effect (Bybee 2017) that gives some advantage to the glottal realization even in cases when

it appears before a vowel. Kazmierski (2018) tested this hypothesis more rigorously using the Buckeye

Corpus (Pitt, Johnson, Hume, Kiesling, and Raymond 2005) by calculating the ratio of consonant-

Page 4 of 38

Page 5: linguistics.byu.edu  · Web viewGlottal stops in English appear in a variety of contexts. They often precede vowel-initial words such as ‘into’ [ʔɪnthuʊ] and serve to break

initial to vowel-initial words following each word ending in /t/. He found this ratio to be a significant

predictor of t-glottalization in the corpus.

While the realization of /t/ as a glottal stop has received attention in the literature, less has been

said about how glottal stops are released before nasals in words such as mitten and beaten. The most

general pronunciation releases the glottal nasally ([mɪʔn̩]), while oral releases (e.g. [mɪʔən]) have been

documented in Utah (Eddington and Savage 2012), occurring mainly among younger females. Oral

releases have also been observed anecdotally in Indiana1 and New Mexico2.

Clearly the geographic, social, and linguistic factors that govern the glottalization of word-

final /t/ in prevocalic position in American English, as well as those that influence how glottal stops are

released before nasals, merit further investigation. However, extant studies of t-glottalization have

focused principally on variables related to pronunciation. More specifically, little is known about the

geographical distribution of these pronunciations in the US. Anecdotal evidence suggests that there is

some social stigma attached to them, but this has not been explored in the extant literature on the

subject. Therefore, we attempt to investigate how glottal stops and their releases are perceived in terms

of positive and negative social traits. The present study is therefore designed to examine both the

production and social perception sides of the topic by examining the speech and perceptions of

speakers from five U.S. states: Indiana, Mississippi, New Mexico, Utah, and Vermont.

1.1. Research questions

The previous studies on this topic motivate the following research questions:

RQ1: Which linguistic and social variables are correlated with the production of an oral release after

glottalized /t/ in prenasal word-medial position (e.g., kitten)?

1 https://www.waywordradio.org/american-glottalization/2 https://www.youtube.com/watch?v=N5Yy0iWVC00

Page 5 of 38

Page 6: linguistics.byu.edu  · Web viewGlottal stops in English appear in a variety of contexts. They often precede vowel-initial words such as ‘into’ [ʔɪnthuʊ] and serve to break

RQ2: Which linguistic and social variables are related to the production of a glottal stop for /t/ in

prevocalic word-final position (e.g., not ever)?

RQ3: What social traits are perceived to be associated with oral releases of prenasal word-medial /t/

(e.g., button)?

RQ4: What social traits are perceived to be associated with glottal realizations of prevocalic word-

final /t/ (e.g., fat or)?

2. Production Study

2.1. Test items

Participants recorded themselves reading the letter in Appendix A twice. The letter includes 24

words containing a /t/ followed by nasal (PreN, e.g. kitten, written), and 28 words ending in /t/ which

are followed by a vowel-initial word (PreV, e.g. about a, fat or).

2.2. Method

The majority of participants were recruited on Amazon Mechanical Turk and were paid USD $2

for providing a recording. Separate announcements were posted to solicit participants from Indiana,

Mississippi, New Mexico, Utah, and Vermont. A smaller number were recruited using social media and

through third person contacts.3 The participants were directed to a survey placed on the Qualtrics

survey platform in which their demographic information was gathered, and they were given

instructions on how to make an audio recording. Separate recording instructions were given for iOS

(iPhones), and Android OS (Android phones), and well as how to install and make a recording on a

computer using the sound editing software Audacity (Audacity Team 2018). The instructions directed

them to read the letter that appeared in the survey twice in a natural voice, in a quiet location. Once

3 Z-scores were used to avoid a warning message about the model failing to converge.

Page 6 of 38

Page 7: linguistics.byu.edu  · Web viewGlottal stops in English appear in a variety of contexts. They often precede vowel-initial words such as ‘into’ [ʔɪnthuʊ] and serve to break

they completed the recording, they uploaded it on wetransfer.com. This step was included so that they

could send the recording without revealing their email address and thus maintain their anonymity.4

2.3. Participants

Recordings from 94 participants were used. Others were eliminated for a number of reasons

including providing a poor quality recording, not following instructions correctly, or not having lived at

least 60%5 of their life in either Indiana, Mississippi, New Mexico, or Utah. Although efforts were

made to obtain recordings of speakers from Vermont, only five people from that state responded and

provided a recording—this was too few to warrant their inclusion in the production study. The final

result was 22 recordings from Indiana, 27 from Mississippi, 23 from New Mexico, and 22 from Utah.

There were 35 male participants and 59 females, and the mean age of the respondents was 32.2 years

(SD = 10.7).

As far as education is concerned, 11 participants had high school diplomas, 40 had attended

college without graduating, 29 had undergraduate degrees, and 14 had graduate degrees. The majority

of the participants (63) self-identified their race as White, while the remainder consisted of 12 African

Americans, 9 Hispanics, 8 people of multiethnic origin, and 2 belonging to other ethnicities.

2.4. Results of the tests for prevocalic word-final /t/

The authors each examined approximately half of the tokens spectrographically and auditorily.

Cases that were difficult to categorize were examined by both authors. Glottal stops were identified by

irregularly spaced striations on the spectrograms (see Figure 1).6 Taps, in contrast, appeared as brief

voiced occlusions (see Figure 2). Alveolar stops were those that had voiceless occlusions with a

following release burst and aspiration (see Figure 3). Instances which were not clear enough to be

4 We thank Joey Stanley for this idea.5 We did not attempt to distinguish glottal reinforcement of the stop from pure glottal stops.6 This 60% cut-off is an arbitrary threshold that we adopted in order to identify people who had lived at least more than half of their life in their state.

Page 7 of 38

Page 8: linguistics.byu.edu  · Web viewGlottal stops in English appear in a variety of contexts. They often precede vowel-initial words such as ‘into’ [ʔɪnthuʊ] and serve to break

unambiguously categorized were not included in the analysis (N=478). The remaining 4,858 responses

yielded 59.1% taps, 39% glottal stops, and 1.9% alveolar stops.

Figure 1: Prevocalic word-final glottal stop (speaker A2Q3, 12s).

Figure 2: Prevocalic word-final tap (speaker A3N7, 52s).

Figure 3: Prevocalic word-final stop (speaker AW3Z, 253s).

Page 8 of 38

Page 9: linguistics.byu.edu  · Web viewGlottal stops in English appear in a variety of contexts. They often precede vowel-initial words such as ‘into’ [ʔɪnthuʊ] and serve to break

The data were analyzed using the R programming language (R Core Team 2018). In it, a mixed

effects logistic regression was carried out in which participants and test words were included as random

intercepts and the BOBYQA optimizer7 (Powell 2009) was used (Table 1). The dependent variable was

whether the pronunciation was glottal versus other pronunciations combined (i.e. taps and alveolar

stops). The independent variables for the analysis were both social and linguistic. The social variables

included participant gender, age, and state of origin, as well as whether the participant was a college

graduate or not. Race consisted of four categories: Black, Hispanic, White, Mixed/Other. Linguistic

variables included the phones preceding and following the word-final /t/, as well as the stress on the

vowel following /t/.

As far as the statistical analyses are concerned, the idea that the alpha level of 0.05 is special or

ideal has been criticized (Amrhein, Greenland and McShane 2019; Amrhein, Trafimow, and Greenland

2019, Cohen 1994). Kline (2004) actually suggests that an alpha level of 0.1 would be more

appropriate in the behavioral sciences. In either case, this suggests that an alpha level should not be

used to blindly dichotomize factors into those that do and those that do not affect the results. Instead, it

should be taken as a point at which the data are most compatible, with less compatible, but still

influential, data falling farther away (i.e., higher alpha levels). Therefore, we choose to discuss results

that approach, but do not reach an alpha of 0.05 as important trends.

7 Bound Optimization BY Quadratic Approximation

Page 9 of 38

Page 10: linguistics.byu.edu  · Web viewGlottal stops in English appear in a variety of contexts. They often precede vowel-initial words such as ‘into’ [ʔɪnthuʊ] and serve to break

Table 1. Analysis of deviance of the fixed effects of the glottal stop pronunciation of word-final

prevocalic /t/.

χ2 df p

(Intercept) 5.78 1 0.016

Following vowel 117.00 6 0.000

Age 7.12 1 0.008

Gender 7.03 1 0.008

College Graduate 1.85 1 0.174

State 5.54 3 0.136

Preceding phone 7.57 9 0.578

Stress on following vowel 0.99 1 0.319

In order to determine how following vowels influenced the use of glottal stops, a Bonferroni

post hoc analysis was carried out that compared how each vowel differed from the average of all

vowels in terms of their favoring a glottal pronunciation. It revealed that the vowel [o] favored glottal

stops to a significantly greater extent, while the vowel [ə] disfavored them. However, given the limited

variety of words in the present study, these results may be due to the particular words used in the study

rather than to specific vowels. For instance, all cases of [o] occur in the word or, found in only two

bigrams: lit or and fat or. The vowel [ə] is the only vowel that appears in a variety of words, which

may suggest that schwa itself, rather that the particular word it appears in, statistically disfavors a

glottal realization of a preceding /t/. Perhaps prosodic features also play a part in this alternation, and

future studies might investigate this possibility.

Page 10 of 38

Page 11: linguistics.byu.edu  · Web viewGlottal stops in English appear in a variety of contexts. They often precede vowel-initial words such as ‘into’ [ʔɪnthuʊ] and serve to break

Several social variables proved significant as well. Women, for example, used glottal stops in

42.6% of the cases and men in only 33%. As far as age is concerned, the general trend is that younger

people used more glottal stops (see Figure 4: the solid line is a regression line, with 95% confidence

intervals around it, and the dotted curved line is a loess line). Conspicuously absent is an influence of

the state the participant calls home. The fact that there are no differences in glottalization rates across

these four states gives credence to the idea that the use of the glottal stop may be widely dispersed

across the U.S., rather than more common in western states, as Eddington and Channer (2010) suggest.

However, the present study confirms previous observations that glottalization in the U.S. is more

prevalent among younger speakers and females, with the exception of male speakers from Vermont (cf.

Roberts 2006).

Figure 4. Percent glottal stop of prevocalic word-final /t/ by age.

Page 11 of 38

Page 12: linguistics.byu.edu  · Web viewGlottal stops in English appear in a variety of contexts. They often precede vowel-initial words such as ‘into’ [ʔɪnthuʊ] and serve to break

2.5. Results of the tests for prenasal word-medial /t/

Prenasal word-medial /t/ was also analyzed with a mixed effects logistic regression as in the

previous analysis in which participants and test words were included as random intercepts (Table 2).

After removing responses which were not clear enough to be categorized (N=240), there were 4,272

responses of which 82% were glottal stops followed by nasal releases (e.g. button [bʌʔn̩], see Figure 5),

11.3% were glottal stops followed by oral releases (e.g. [bʌʔən], see Figure 6), 3.2% were nasally

released [d] (e.g. [bʌdn̩]), 2.9% were [t], and the remaining 0.5% were taps (e.g. [bʌɾən]). The

regression analysis contrasted the use of glottal stops followed by oral releases from all other

pronunciations. The exact quality of the vowel produced between the glottal stops and nasals varied

Page 12 of 38

Page 13: linguistics.byu.edu  · Web viewGlottal stops in English appear in a variety of contexts. They often precede vowel-initial words such as ‘into’ [ʔɪnthuʊ] and serve to break

between speakers and was not deemed relevant for the purposes of the present study. However,

impressionistically it appeared to be a lax vowel that varied between [ɪ], [ə], and [ɨ].

Figure 5: Prenasal word-medial glottal stop followed by nasal release (speaker A3N7, 4s).

Figure 6: Prenasal word-medial glottal stop followed by oral release (speaker QUEZ, 205s).

Table 2. Analysis of deviance of the fixed effects of the oral release pronunciation of word-medial /t/ followed by nasal.

χ2 df p

(Intercept) 2.70 1 0.101

Age8 5.99 1 0.014

Page 13 of 38

Page 14: linguistics.byu.edu  · Web viewGlottal stops in English appear in a variety of contexts. They often precede vowel-initial words such as ‘into’ [ʔɪnthuʊ] and serve to break

Preceding vowel 38.53 9 0.000

Race 4.84 3 0.184

Gender 2.04 1 0.153

State 2.79 3 0.426

College Graduate 0.40 1 0.526

As is apparent in Figure 7, oral releases, such as in written [ɹɪʔən], are more common among

younger speakers. The preceding vowel is also significant, with /ɪ/ and /ə/ favoring an oral release

and /æ/ and /o/ disfavoring it. As far as the influence of state is concerned, we specifically chose Utah

as a test state because oral releases have been reported there (Eddington and Savage 2012). They have

also been observed anecdotally in New Mexico and Indiana (see footnotes 1 and 2 above). Mississippi,

on the other hand was included as a kind of control state, as we had no reason to believe that oral

releases are commonly produced there. For this reason, it is an interesting finding that there are no

significant differences between the four states that are compared. However, there is a definite trend in

that direction in terms of the percentage of oral releases (NM 24%, UT 12%, IN 9%, MS 2%).

Figure 7. Percent of glottal stop followed by an oral release of prenasal word-medial /t/ according to

age.

8 We thank Felice Coles, Dawn Nordquist, Mark Waltermire, Karol Ibarra Zetter, and Debbie Wager for their help with recruiting participants.

Page 14 of 38

Page 15: linguistics.byu.edu  · Web viewGlottal stops in English appear in a variety of contexts. They often precede vowel-initial words such as ‘into’ [ʔɪnthuʊ] and serve to break

3. Perception Study

The matched-guise method (cf. Lambert, Hodgson, Gardner & Fillenbaum 1960) in

experimental sociolinguistic research involves the same person reading or speaking in two different

parts of the experiment in such a way that the listener does not realize that the same person is speaking.

The speaker modifies his voice or language, or regional accent, to achieve this deceptive effect. The

purpose of the matched-guise experimental method is to elicit attitudinal data from listeners, with the

intent to attach specific language attitudes to specific pronunciations. A variant of the matched-guise

method is to digitally manipulate the pronunciation of a speaker in a recording with acoustic software

in order to control the pronunciation with precision. For example, Campbell-Kibler (2010) manipulated

word-final -ing (e.g., going) in English, while Podesva, Reynolds, Callier, and Baptiste (2015) changed

the release of /t/ in the speech of six prominent U.S. politicians, and Chappell (2016) altered the

voicing of intervocalic /s/ in Costa Rican Spanish. The authors then gathered attitudinal data from

listeners and correlated social characteristics with specific sounds.

Page 15 of 38

Page 16: linguistics.byu.edu  · Web viewGlottal stops in English appear in a variety of contexts. They often precede vowel-initial words such as ‘into’ [ʔɪnthuʊ] and serve to break

3.1. Test items

The present study consisted of six test sentences with words containing a final /t/ followed by a

vowel (PreV, e.g. met on, wait until, get another) and six sentences with words containing a /t/

followed by a nasal (PreN, e.g. curtain, tighten). A complete list of test sentences appears in Appendix

B. Another six sentences that did not include the variables in question were included as distractors,

along with four lie detector sentences, which are discussed more in the next section. Each sentence was

recorded by a different speaker, all of whom were white females in their 20s from the western U.S.,

who were university students.

The speakers rendered the /t/ in the relevant words in the PreV sentences with a tap

pronunciation (e.g. me[ɾ] on, wai[ɾ] until), while the /t/ plus nasal sequences in the words in the PreN

sentences were pronounced as glottal stops followed by a schwa (e.g. tighten [taɪʔən]). For most of the

volunteer speakers, achieving an orally released glottal stop word-medially required some training and

practice, as this pronunciation was not natural for them. In contrast, articulating a tap for prevocalic

word-final /t/ was more natural for them.

Using Praat we then created another twelve test sentences by manipulating these original

sentences. Specifically, taps in the PreV sentences (e.g., not ever) were converted into synthesized

glottal stops, primarily by lowering the fundamental frequency of the tap segment of the word

(Pierrehumbert and Frisch 1997). However, in order to achieve natural sounding glottal stops, we also

lengthened the tap to varying degrees and added silence between glottal pulses to render their spacing

more irregular. This was done on a case by case basis until a natural sounding glottal stop was

achieved. Additionally, the PreN words were converted from oral releases (e.g. tighten [taɪʔən]) to

nasal releases (e.g. [taɪʔn̩]) by simply removing the vowel following the glottal stop.

Page 16 of 38

Page 17: linguistics.byu.edu  · Web viewGlottal stops in English appear in a variety of contexts. They often precede vowel-initial words such as ‘into’ [ʔɪnthuʊ] and serve to break

3.2. Participants

A total of 229 people participated in the perception study, 99 males and 130 females. Of these,

70 were from Indiana, 46 from Mississippi, 26 from New Mexico, 66 from Utah, and 21 from

Vermont. Only two respondents had less than a high school degree, while 31 had completed high

school, 83 had attended some college, 98 were college graduates, and the remaining 15 had graduate

degrees. The great majority of the participants (185) were White, while 13 were Black, 4 were Asian, 6

were Hispanic, and 1 Native American. The remaining 20 were of mixed ethnicity.

3.3. Method

Participants from the five states of interest were recruited on Mechanical Turk and paid USD $2

for completing the survey. After answering a few sociodemographic questions, the participants were

given these instructions:

You will hear a sentence then be asked to judge how the person sounds. Please listen to the

entire audio BEFORE answering any of the questions. If you don't follow the instructions the

survey will automatically stop. That means that you won't be able to try it a second time, and

you won't be paid for completing it.

In surveys of this nature it is possible for the participants to answer without actually listening to the

recording. In order to weed out inattentive participants four lie detector recordings were included. In

those recordings a test sentence was followed by explicit instructions to leave all of the answers related

to that recording blank. In order to emphasize the importance of following the instructions, the first test

item was a lie detector. When a participant answered any questions about this first sentence they were

given this message:

Page 17 of 38

Page 18: linguistics.byu.edu  · Web viewGlottal stops in English appear in a variety of contexts. They often precede vowel-initial words such as ‘into’ [ʔɪnthuʊ] and serve to break

You didn't listen to the whole clip and follow the instructions!! If you don't follow the

instructions the survey will automatically stop. That means that you can't try it a second time or

be paid for completing it. Please continue to the next audio clip.

This served as a warning. The remaining three lie detector items were randomly interspersed among the

other test items and distractors. When a participant answered any question about the other three lie

detector sentences the survey would cease and the responses of these participants were eliminated from

the study. Each recording was preceded with these instructions:

Listen to this woman's voice then answer the following 6 questions about how she sounds.

Please listen to the entire recording before answering any of the questions.

The participants were then able to start the recording and answer a series of six questions about the

speaker in the recording (cf. Appendix C). The first question asked the listeners to guess the speaker’s

age on a scale between 12 to 40 years old. Subsequent questions were answered on a Likert-type scale

and solicited the degree to which the speaker in the recording sounded like she was from a town or city,

how pleasant, friendly, and educated she sounded, as well as how much she sounded like she was from

the state the participant was from. For example, for pleasantness the scale was: Very pleasant,

Somewhat pleasant, Neither pleasant nor unpleasant, Somewhat unpleasant, Very unpleasant.

Participants were randomly assigned to one of two groups. The members of each group heard

the same four lie detector recordings, and the same six distractor recordings, however, the groups

differed in terms of what pronunciations they heard in the test sentences (Table 3). This division meant

that no listener heard the same sentence or speaker twice, and allowed a comparison of the resulting

evaluations in terms of the phones that varied. Aside from the first item played to the listeners, which

was the same for all listeners, as mentioned above, the items were played in a random order.

Table 3. Assignment of test recordings to the participants.

Page 18 of 38

Page 19: linguistics.byu.edu  · Web viewGlottal stops in English appear in a variety of contexts. They often precede vowel-initial words such as ‘into’ [ʔɪnthuʊ] and serve to break

Test Group 1 Test Group 2

Lie detector sentences 1-4 Lie detector sentences 1-4

Distractor sentences 1-6 Distractor sentences 1-6

PreV sentences 1-3 with taps PreV sentences 1-3 with glottal stops

PreV sentences 4-6 with glottal stops PreV sentences 4-6 with taps

PreN sentences 1-3 with nasal releases PreN sentences 1-3 with oral releases

PreN sentences 4-6 with oral releases PreN sentences 4-6 with nasal releases

3.4. Results

Since the purpose of the present study was to determine how pronunciation differences affected

the participants’ perceptions of the speakers, the only independent variable that was relevant was the

particular pronunciation and the interaction of pronunciation with the other biographical variables, state

and gender. However, in order to save space, we do not discuss the findings that are not related to

pronunciation. For instance, we observed that female participants rated the speakers as more friendly

than did the male participants, but that result is not germane to the research questions in the present

study. In any event, our analyses investigated how the above mentioned variables are related to

participants’ perceptions of the speaker’s age, friendliness, pleasantness, education level, how much

they sounded urban vs. rural (rurality), and how much they sounded like they were from the state the

participant was from. In the analyses, random intercepts were included for participant and for the

speaker who made the recording. These analyses were carried out using the Jamovi statistical package

(Jamovi Project 2018) which is a GUI for R. It simplifies running the analyses since it does not require

line commands, and has a number of dependencies, including the Gamlj R package, the lmerTest R

package (Kuznetsova, Brockhoff and Christensen 2017) and the lme4 R package (Bates, Mächler,

Bolker, Walker 2015).

Page 19 of 38

Page 20: linguistics.byu.edu  · Web viewGlottal stops in English appear in a variety of contexts. They often precede vowel-initial words such as ‘into’ [ʔɪnthuʊ] and serve to break

Perhaps the most unexpected result is that there were no significant interactions between

pronunciation and state. In fact, no significant interactions between any other variable and

pronunciation occurred at or below the p = 0.05 alpha level. However, we examine a number of

predictors that were not significant at an alpha level of 0.05. In such cases it is common to state that

there was a trend toward significance. The first trend (p = 0.058) is that when rating the speaker’s

friendliness with prevocalic word-final /t/ (e.g., not ever), men perceived speakers who used glottal

stops as less friendly than those who used taps, judging by the means of their responses (glottal stop =

0.57, tap = 0.73), while women’s ratings differed little (glottal stop = 0.79, tap = 0.82). Another

noteworthy trend (p = .083) was seen in perceptions of whether the speakers who were heard in the

recording were from the same state as the participants when the speakers were heard using either oral

or nasal releases with prenasal word-medial /t/ (e.g., kitten). Keep in mind that higher numbers in Table

4 indicate greater perceptions that the speaker is from the same state as the participant. The means in

Table 4 suggests that Mississippians perceive oral releases as more ‘foreign’ sounding, while in

Vermont, Utah, and New Mexico oral releases were viewed as more likely spoken by natives of those

three states. In spite of the anecdotal reports of oral releases in Indiana, no trend was apparent in the

data from that state.

Table 4. Means for ratings of ‘in-stateness’ according to pronunciation of prenasal word-medial /t/.

Pronunciation State Means

Nasal Release IN .21

Oral Release IN .19

Nasal Release MS -.10

Oral Release MS -.31

Nasal Release NM -.04

Page 20 of 38

Page 21: linguistics.byu.edu  · Web viewGlottal stops in English appear in a variety of contexts. They often precede vowel-initial words such as ‘into’ [ʔɪnthuʊ] and serve to break

Oral Release NM .04

Nasal Release UT .66

Oral Release UT .74

Nasal Release VT -.18

Oral Release VT .19

Judgments of the speaker’s rurality were also influenced by pronunciation (p = .073) to the

effect that glottal stops (-.16) were associated with town dwellers more than taps and other

pronunciations (-.04). Friendliness was likewise affected to a small degree by the pronunciation the

participants heard (p = .091); nasal releases (.98) sounded friendlier than oral releases (.90).

The statistically significant findings relating to pronunciation are summarized in Table 5. The

full statistical results appear in Appendix D. This table contains the results of both the prevocalic (e.g.,

not ever) and the prenasal (e.g., kitten) context in which /t/ occurs. It shows that recordings of speakers

containing oral releases with prenasal, word-medial /t/ (e.g., kitten [kɪʔVn]) were perceived to be less

educated and more rural than when a nasal release [kɪʔn̩]) was heard. The use of glottal stops with

prevocalic, word-final /t/ (e.g., not ever [nɑʔɛvɚ]) was associated with a lower degree of friendliness

and education. Glottal stops were also significantly associated with a slight decrease in the speaker’s

perceived age.

Page 21 of 38

Page 22: linguistics.byu.edu  · Web viewGlottal stops in English appear in a variety of contexts. They often precede vowel-initial words such as ‘into’ [ʔɪnthuʊ] and serve to break

Table 5. Significant variables that involve pronunciation according to the influencing independent

variables and means for each pronunciation.

Independent Variables Mean Phonological context Dependent Variable Values

Rurality -0.27 PreN Oral release

-0.04 PreN Nasal release

Education 0.61 PreN Oral release

0.78 PreN Nasal release

Age 22.3 PreV Glottal Stop

22.6 PreV Non-glottal

Friendliness 0.70 PreV Glottal Stop

0.78 PreV Non-glottal

Education 0.49 PreV Glottal Stop

0.58 PreV Non-glottal

4. Discussion and Conclusions

With respect to RQ1, which asked where prenasal word-medial /t/ is realized with an oral

release (e.g., button [bʌʔən]), we can conclude that it occurs in all four states that we chose to include

in the study. As already mentioned earlier, few participants from Vermont provided a recording for the

production study which is why Vermont was not included. Despite the oral release being present in

Indiana, Mississippi, New Mexico, and Utah, the form used the most was a nasal release (e.g., [bʌʔn̩]).

With respect to which state oral releases were used the most in, New Mexico had the highest rate

(24%) followed by Utah (UT 12%), Indiana (IN 9%), and Mississippi (2%). Despite these differences

in rates of usage of oral releases, state as an independent variable was not found to be a significant

Page 22 of 38

Page 23: linguistics.byu.edu  · Web viewGlottal stops in English appear in a variety of contexts. They often precede vowel-initial words such as ‘into’ [ʔɪnthuʊ] and serve to break

predictor of oral releases when the response variable was specified as the binary distinction between an

oral release and all other articulations (e.g., a nasal release [bʌʔn̩], the alveolar stop [t] [bʌtn̩], a tap

[bʌɾVn̩], and a nasally released [d] [bʌdn̩]).

One variable that shows a significant effect in the regression analysis was the preceding vowel,

and another was age, with younger speakers producing more oral releases than older speakers, despite

the fact that even for younger speakers, nasal releases were the most common pronunciation. This

finding concurs with previous studies in the U.S. (Eddington and Taylor 2009, Eddington and Channer

2010). Whether this trend represents a change in progress in apparent time (cf. Bailey, Wikle, Tillery

and Sand 1991; Cukor-Avila and Bailey 2013) or stable variation is an interesting question, but falls

outside the scope of the current paper and warrants further investigation.

Concerning RQ2, which asked where prevocalic, word-final /t/ is realized as a glottal stop, the

results of the production study show that it is produced in the four states examined, with rates of glottal

stop realization ranging from 35.5% in Mississippi to 45.9% in New Mexico, with a mean of 39.1%,

and a standard deviation of 4.8. As with prenasal word-medial /t/, state did not make a significant

contribution to the prediction of prevocalic word-final /t/, despite differences in rates of usage of glottal

stops versus other realizations of /t/. Although differences of production rates between states were

minimal, gender and age proved important; women and younger speakers produced more glottal stops.

Additionally, the following vowels were found to significantly condition glottal stops: /o/ favored a

preceding glottal stop, and /ə/ disfavored a preceding glottal stop. As pointed out above, with only two

unique two-word strings with a following /o/ (i.e., lit or, fat or) this effect from the following vowel

may be due to the specific word rather than being due to the vowel itself. We trust that future studies

will include a wide variety of words so that the effect of following vowel may be more precisely

determined.

With regard to RQ3, which asked which social qualities are perceived to be associated with an

oral release of prenasal word-medial /t/, the results of the perception study revealed several that were

Page 23 of 38

Page 24: linguistics.byu.edu  · Web viewGlottal stops in English appear in a variety of contexts. They often precede vowel-initial words such as ‘into’ [ʔɪnthuʊ] and serve to break

connected with an oral release. The participants perceived speakers who used an oral release in this

context to be less educated and more likely to be from a small town rather than from a city.

Furthermore, they perceived the speakers who used an oral release to be less friendly and slightly

younger. The latter finding concurs with anecdotal observations that younger speakers in Indiana

produce oral releases more than older speakers (see footnote 1).

Finally, with respect to RQ4, which asked about social qualities that are perceived to be

associated with a glottal realization of prevocalic word-final /t/, the question can be answered in the

following manner: the presence of a glottal stop instead of a tap or other realizations influenced the

listener’s perceptions about the speakers. When speakers produced a prevocalic word-final /t/ as a

glottal stop (e.g., not ever [nɑʔɛvɚ]) rather than a tap (e.g., [nɑɾɛvɚ]) or other realization, listeners

perceived them to be less educated, less friendly, and younger.

A connection between production and perception is apparent with prevocalic word-final /t/ and

age of speaker. As reported above, younger speakers produce more glottal stops than older speakers,

and listeners associate a glottal realization of /t/ in this context with slightly younger speakers. This

may indicate that listeners subconsciously associate this pronunciation with younger speakers. In

contrast, there does not seem to be a connection in these data between the production and perception of

prenasal word-medial /t/ and age of speaker. While the younger speakers produced more oral releases

in this context than older speakers, the listeners did not associate a difference in the perceived age of

speakers who produced oral releases. It may be that in the future an oral release of prenasal word-

medial /t/ will eventually become aware of the use of this articulation and associate it with particular

social characteristics.

Concerning the connection between the perceived level of education of speakers and the

pronunciation of /t/ in the two phonological environments analyzed in this study, listeners perceived

speakers to be less educated both when they articulated an oral release of prenasal word-medial /t/ and

a glottal stop for prevocalic word-final /t/. This perception may be due to the effect of the association of

Page 24 of 38

Page 25: linguistics.byu.edu  · Web viewGlottal stops in English appear in a variety of contexts. They often precede vowel-initial words such as ‘into’ [ʔɪnthuʊ] and serve to break

age with these phenomena, in that younger speakers in general have received less education than older

speakers.

The present paper contributes to the field by documenting certain pronunciations of /t/ in

American English as well as contributing to the literature documenting the connection between sub-

phonemic sounds and social evaluations, or sociophonetic variation (cf. Thomas 2011). The

participants in this study, who came from five U.S. states, perceived different social traits about

speakers based on phonetic differences in /t/ as it appeared in two phonological contexts. This finding

is in line with other studies of sub-phonemic variability and social evaluation cited above (e.g.,

Campbell-Kibler 2010; Podesva, et al. 2015, Chappell 2016).

In summary, this paper shows the utility of employing both production and perceptual data

collected online to study regional linguistic variation (cf. Brown 2015) as well as the social

implications and perceptions of that variability. Further, this paper contributes to the field detailing the

articulation of /t/ in American English.

Page 25 of 38

Page 26: linguistics.byu.edu  · Web viewGlottal stops in English appear in a variety of contexts. They often precede vowel-initial words such as ‘into’ [ʔɪnthuʊ] and serve to break

References

Amrhein, Valentin, Sander Greenland, and Black McShane. 2019. Retire statistical significance.

Nature 567. 305-307.

Amrhein, Valentin, David Trafimow, and Sander Greenland. 2019. Inferential statistics as descriptive

statistics: There is no replication crisis if we don’t expect replication. The American Statistician

73. 262-270.

Andrésen, Bjørn Stålhane. 1968. Pre-Glottalisation in English Standard Pronunciation. Olso:

Norwegian Universities Press.

Audacity Team. 2018. Audacity: Free Audio Editor and Recorder [Computer application]. Version

2.3.0. Retrieved from https://audacityteam.org.

Bailey, Guy, Tom Wikle, Jan Tillery and Lori Sand. 1991. The apparent time construct. Language

Variation and Change 3(3). 241–264.

Barrera, Berta Badia. 2015. “A Sociolinguistic Study of T-Glottalling in Young RP: Accent, Class and Education.” University of Essex.

Bates, Douglas, Martin Mächler, Ben Bolker and Steve Walker. 2015. Fitting Linear Mixed-Effects

Models Using lme4. Journal of Statistical Software 67(1). 1–48.

Bellavance, Sarah. 2017. “Co-Occurrence of /t/ Variants in Young Vermont Speakers.” Burlington,

VT: University of Vermont.

Boersma, Paul and David Weenink. 2019. Praat: doing phonetics by computer [Computer program].

www.praat.org

Brown, Earl K. 2015. On the utility of combining production data and perceptual data to investigate

regional linguistic variation: The case of Spanish experiential gustar “to like, to please” on

Twitter and in an online survey. Journal of Linguistic Geography 3(2). 47–59.

Byrd, Dani. 1993. “54,000 American Stops.” UCLA Working Papers in Phonetics 83: 97–116.

Page 26 of 38

Page 27: linguistics.byu.edu  · Web viewGlottal stops in English appear in a variety of contexts. They often precede vowel-initial words such as ‘into’ [ʔɪnthuʊ] and serve to break

Campbell-Kibler, Kathryn. 2010. The sociolinguistic variant as a carrier of social meaning. Language

Variation and Change 22(3). 423–441. doi:10.1017/S0954394510000177.

Chappell, Whitney. 2016. On the social perception of intervocalic /s/ voicing in Costa Rican Spanish.

Language Variation and Change 28(3). 357–378. doi:10.1017/S0954394516000107.

Coggle, Paul. 1993. Do You Speak Estuary? London: Bloomsbury.

Cohen, Jacob. 1994. The earth is round (p < .05). American Psychologist 49, 997-1003.

Collins, Beverley, and Inger M. Mees. 1996. “Spreading Everywhere?: How Recent a Phenomenon Is

Glottalisation in Received Pronunciation?” English World-Wide 17: 175–87.

Cukor-Avila, Patricia and Guy Bailey. 2013. Real Time and Apparent Time. In J.K. Chambers and

Natalie Schilling (eds.), The Handbook of Language Variation and Change, 239–262. 2nd edn.

Malden, MA: Wiley-Blackwell.

Docherty, Gerry, and Paul Foulkes. 1999. “Sociophonetic Variation in ‘Glottals’ in Newscastle

English.” In Proceedings of the 14th International Congress of Phonetic Sciences, 1037-1040.

Berkeley: University of California.

Eddington, David, and Caitlin Channer. 2010. “American English Has Goʔ a Loʔ of Glottal Stops:

Social Diffusion and Linguistic Motivation.” American Speech 85 (3): 338–51.

Eddington, David, and Matthew Savage. 2012. “Where Are the Moun[ɁƏ]Ns in Utah?” American

Speech 87 (3): 336–49.

Eddington, David, and Michael Taylor. 2009. “T-Glottalization in American English.” American

Speech 84 (3): 298–314.

Fabricius, Anne H. 2000. “T-Glottaling Between Stigma and Prestige: A Sociolinguistic Study of

Modern RP.” Ph.D Dissertation, Copenhagen Business School.

Farrington, Charlie. 2019. “Incomplete Neutralization in African American English: The Case of Final

Consonant Voicing.” University of Oregon.

Page 27 of 38

Page 28: linguistics.byu.edu  · Web viewGlottal stops in English appear in a variety of contexts. They often precede vowel-initial words such as ‘into’ [ʔɪnthuʊ] and serve to break

Holmes, Janet. 1995. “Two for /t/: Flapping and Glottal Stops in New Zealand English.” Te Reo 38:

53–72.

Jamovi project. 2018. Jamovi (Version 0.9) [Computer Software]. www.jamovi.org

Kazmierski, Kamil. 2018. “Word Boundary Intervocalic T-Glottaling  in Midland American English.”

Adam Mickiewicz University.

Kim, Chaeyoon, Sravana Reddy, James Stanford, Ezra Wyschogrod, and Jack Grieve. 2018. “Bring on

the Crowd! Using Online Audio Crowdsourcing for Large-Scale New England Dialectology

and Acoustic Sociophonetics.” American Speech, 1–38.

Klánová, Aneta. 2016. “Sociophonetic Study of Substitutional Glottalization in Native English

Speakers.” Univerzita Karlova v Praze.

Kline, R. B. 2004. Beyond significance testing: Reforming data analysis methods in behavioral research. Washington D.C.: American Psychological Association.

Kuznetsova, Alexandra, Per B. Brockhoff and Rune H. B. Christensen. 2017. lmerTest Package: Tests in Linear Mixed Effects Models. Journal of Statistical Software 82(13). 1–26.

Lambert, Wallace E., Richard. C. Hodgson, Richard C. Gardner and Samuel Fillenbaum. 1960. Evaluational reactions to spoken languages. The Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology 60(1). 44–51.

Levon, Erez. 2006. “Mosaic Identity and Style: Phonological Variation among Reform American

Jews.” Journal of Sociolinguistics 10: 181–204.

Macaulay, Ronald K.S. 1977. Language, Social Class, and Education: A Glasgow Study. Edinburgh:

The University Press.

Marshall, Jonathan. 2003. “The Changing Sociolinguistic Status of the Glottal Stop in Northeast

Scottish English.” English World-Wide 24: 89–108.

Mees, Inger. 1987. “Glottal Stop as a Prestigious Feature in Cardiff English.” English World-Wide 8:

25–39.

Milroy, James, and Lesley Milroy. 1978. “Belfast: Change and Variation in an Urban Vernacular.” In

Sociolinguistic Patterns in British, edited by Peter Trudgill, 19–36. Baltimore: University Park

Press.

Page 28 of 38

Page 29: linguistics.byu.edu  · Web viewGlottal stops in English appear in a variety of contexts. They often precede vowel-initial words such as ‘into’ [ʔɪnthuʊ] and serve to break

Milroy, James, Lesley Milroy, Sue Hartley, and David Walshaw. 1994. “Glottal Stops and Tynside

Glottalization: Competing Patterns of Variation and Change in British English.” Language

Variation and Change 6: 327–357.

Ostalski, Przemyslaw. 2013. “Glottal Stops in General American (Intervocalic Environments).” In

Teaching and Researching English Accents in Native and Non-Native Speakers, edited by E.

Waniek-Klimczak and L.R. Shockey, 241–51. Second Language Learning and Teaching.

Berlin: Springer-Verlag.

Partin-Hernandez, Allyn. 2002. “You So Don’t Talk Like Me: An Exploration of Southern California

Sound Changes.” presented at the American Dialect Society, San Francisco, CA.

Pierrehumbert, Janet B., and Stefan Frisch. 1997. “Synthesizing Allophonic Glottalization.” In

Progress in Speech Synthesis, edited by Jan P. H. van Santen, Richard W. Sproat, Joseph P.

Olive, and Julia Hirshberg, 9–26. New York: Springer.

Pitt, Mark A., Keith Johnson, Elizabeth Hume, Scott Kiesling, and William Raymond. 2005. “The

Buckeye Corpus of Conversational Speech: Labeling Conventions and a Test of Transcriber

Reliability.” Speech Communication 45 (1): 89–95.

Podesva, Robert J, Jeremy Reynolds, Patrick Callier and Jessica Baptiste. 2015. Constraints on the

social meaning of released /t/: A production and perception study of U.S. politicians. Language

Variation and Change 27(1). 59–87.

Powell, M. J. D. 2009. The BOBYQA algorithm for bound constrained optimization without

derivatives. Report, Department of Applied Mathematics and Theoretical Physics, Cambridge

University.

Preston, Dennis R. 1996. Where the worst English is spoken. In E. Schneider (ed.), Focus on the USA

(Varieties of English around the World), 297–360. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.

Preston, Dennis R. (ed.). 1999. A Handbook of Perceptual Dialectology. Vol. 1. Amsterdam: John

Benjamins.

Page 29 of 38

Page 30: linguistics.byu.edu  · Web viewGlottal stops in English appear in a variety of contexts. They often precede vowel-initial words such as ‘into’ [ʔɪnthuʊ] and serve to break

R Core Team. 2013. R: A language and environment for statistical computing. R Foundation for

Statistical Computing, Vienna, Austria

Reid, Euan. 1978. “Social and Stylistic Variation in the Speech of Children: Some Evidence from

Edinburgh.” In Sociolinguistic Patterns in British, edited by Peter Trudgill, 158–171.

Baltimore: University Park Press.

Roberts, Julie. 2006. “As Old Becomes New: Glottalization in Vermont.” American Speech 81: 227–

49.

Seyfarth, Scott, and Marc Garellek. 2015. “Coda Glottalization in American English.” In Proceedings

of the 18th International Congress of Phonetic Sciences, 5. Glasgow: The Scottish Consortium

for ICPhS 2015.

Straw, Michelle, and Peter L. Patrick. 2007. “Dialect acquisition of glottal variation in /t/: Barbadan in

Ipswich.” Language Sciences 29: 385–407.

Thomas, Erik. 2011. Sociophonetics: An Introduction. New York: Palgrave.

Tollfree, Laura. 1999. “South-East London English: Discrete versus Continual Modelling of Consonant

Reduction.” In Urban Voices: Accent Studies in the British Isles, edited by P.Foulkes and P.

Docherty, 163–184. London: Arnold.

Wells, J.C. 1982. Accents of English. Vol. 3. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Page 30 of 38

Page 31: linguistics.byu.edu  · Web viewGlottal stops in English appear in a variety of contexts. They often precede vowel-initial words such as ‘into’ [ʔɪnthuʊ] and serve to break

Appendix A

The version of the letter that the speakers had access to did not have boldface or underlined keywords.

Underlined words are those containing a /t/ followed by nasal sequence (24), while boldface words are

those test items ending in /t/ that are followed by a following vowel-initial word. (28)

Dear Mom,

I know it's been a while since I've written. In the last letter I told you about a kitten I found that

looked like it hadn't eaten for a month. Well, it's living in my apartment now. It was in pretty bad

shape, definitely not fat or clean. It actually looks like it'd been beaten but it wasn’t frightened of

people at all. I've named it Keaton and my goal is to fatten it up a bit and show it people aren't so bad.

There are good things and bad things about having a cat in my apartment. The bad thing is that

it's started to climb up the drapes which made the curtain rods wobbly so I had to tighten them. He has

a certain fascination with buttons, and has bit a few off my sweaters. Now, I make sure to shut the

closet anytime I’m home, and not ever use the expensive satin sheets. He shreds the paper for the toilet

almost every day. I went into the bathroom one day and it looked like there were cotton balls by the

toilet and on the floor. Yesterday when I came home from work he'd done a number on the carpet in

the bedroom. I about flattened him but then realized that I'd forgotten to let him out in the morning, so

it really wasn't his fault.

In spite of the problems it's fun to have him. He seems to know when it's lunch time because

when I go toward the kitchen at about eleven, his face brightens up, he straightens his tail and starts to

flick it around, then he tries to beat me to the kitchen. I usually take him outside and let him sniff

around while I sit on the edge of the fountain and study for my Lit or Latin class. A guy from my

building saw me sitting there with Keaton. He sat on the bench and started playing with him. His name

is Lance Patton. He's an English major and is Colton Smith's roommate. They met on a study abroad

Page 31 of 38

Page 32: linguistics.byu.edu  · Web viewGlottal stops in English appear in a variety of contexts. They often precede vowel-initial words such as ‘into’ [ʔɪnthuʊ] and serve to break

trip to Great Britain. We got into a conversation and I think he may be interested in me. He actually

put a note with my number in his cell phone. What a way to sweeten my day!

I can't wait any longer to be home.

Love your daughter,

Candace

Page 32 of 38

Page 33: linguistics.byu.edu  · Web viewGlottal stops in English appear in a variety of contexts. They often precede vowel-initial words such as ‘into’ [ʔɪnthuʊ] and serve to break

Appendix B

Underlined words are those containing a /t/ followed by nasal sequence, while boldface words are those

test items ending in /t/ that are followed by a following vowel-initial word.

Test items containing /t/ followed by nasal (PreN)

1. I know it's been a while since I've written. In the last letter I told you about the kitten I found

that looked like it hadn't eaten for a month. Well, it’s living at my house now.

2. The cat started to climb up the drapes which made the curtain rods wobbly so I had to tighten

them. I hope my landlord doesn’t notice.

3. So, I learned that my new cat has a certain fascination with buttons, and likes to bite them off

my sweaters. I’m hoping he’ll stop when he’s older.

4. Yesterday when I came home from work he'd done a number on the carpet. I about flattened

him but then realized I'd forgotten to leave the door to the basement open, so it really wasn't his

fault.

5. He seems to know when it's close to lunch time because when I go toward the kitchen, his face

brightens up, he straightens his tail and then he tries to beat me there.

6. I always thought that cotton was a Latin word, but it’s actually an Arabic word that the Romans

borrowed.

Test items containing word-final /t/ followed by a vowel (PreV)

1. They make a mess in the house so now I make sure to shut the closet anytime I’m home, and

not ever let the cat around the laundry either. That’s taken care of the problem.

2. I totally forgot about all the new carpet on the floor, and left my muddy boots on when I came

in. Luckily, they were not even going to mention it.

Page 33 of 38

Page 34: linguistics.byu.edu  · Web viewGlottal stops in English appear in a variety of contexts. They often precede vowel-initial words such as ‘into’ [ʔɪnthuʊ] and serve to break

3. So, we met on Saturday, and I can’t wait until we get another chance to see each other again.

Can you believe we both like hip hop?

4. He’s not fat or anything, maybe a bit overweight if you consider his height. But I just want

people to stop making rude comments about him.

5. They usually just sit around playing video games, but the other day they started to fight over

some stupid thing and Jason put a major hole in the closet door with his fist.

6. The worst thing was that the toilet overflowed the night Irene came over so we spent the whole

time cleaning up the mess.

Appendix C

Test questions

(The numbers in the responses are the values used in the statistical analyses. They were not visible to

the participants.)

1. How old does she sound? Move the slider to the age.

(The slider ranged from 12 to 40.)

2. How much does she sound like she's from a small town or from a city?

-2 Definitely from a small town

-1 Probably from a small town

0 Can’t tell

1 Probably from a city

2 Definitely from a city

3. How pleasant does she sound?

2 Very pleasant

1 Somewhat pleasant

0 Neither pleasant nor unpleasant

Page 34 of 38

Page 35: linguistics.byu.edu  · Web viewGlottal stops in English appear in a variety of contexts. They often precede vowel-initial words such as ‘into’ [ʔɪnthuʊ] and serve to break

-1 Somewhat unpleasant

-2 Very unpleasant

4. How educated does she sound?

2 Very educated

1 Somewhat educated

0 Neither educated nor uneducated

-1 Somewhat uneducated

-2 Very uneducated

5. How much does she sound like she is from [state participant is from]?

2 Definitely from [state participant is from]

1 Probably from [state participant is from]

0 Can’t tell

-1 Probably not from [state participant is from]

-2 Definitely not from [state participant is from]

6. How friendly does she sound?

2 Very friendly

1 Somewhat friendly

0 Neither friendly nor unfriendly

-1 Somewhat unfriendly

-2 Very unfriendly

Page 35 of 38

Page 36: linguistics.byu.edu  · Web viewGlottal stops in English appear in a variety of contexts. They often precede vowel-initial words such as ‘into’ [ʔɪnthuʊ] and serve to break

Appendix D

Statistical results of the matched guise experiment.

PreN dependent variable: Education F Num df Den df pGENDER 2.734 1 219 0.100PRONUNCIATION 13.795 1 1125 < .001STATE 2.530 4 219 0.041GENDER ✻ PRONUNCIATION 0.375 1 1125 0.541GENDER ✻ STATE 0.113 4 219 0.978PRONUNCIATION ✻ STATE 0.923 4 1125 0.450

GENDER ✻ PRONUNCIATION ✻ STATE 0.566 4 1125 0.687

PreN dependent variable: Age (of speaker)STATE 0.4679 4 218 0.759GENDER 0.3035 1 218 0.582AGE (of listener) 1.3567 1 219 0.245PRONUNCIATION 0.5739 1 1126 0.449STATE ✻ GENDER 1.1932 4 218 0.315STATE ✻ PRONUNCIATION 0.7856 4 1125 0.535GENDER ✻ PRONUNCIATION 0.0635 1 1125 0.801STATE ✻ GENDER ✻ PRONUNCIATION 0.3976 4 1126 0.810GENDER ✻ STATE ✻ PRONUNCIATION 1.2764 4 1126 0.277

PreN dependent variable: FriendlinessGENDER 3.6513 1 219 0.057STATE 1.2798 4 219 0.279PRONUNCIATION 2.8534 1 1127 0.091GENDER ✻ STATE 0.9293 4 219 0.448GENDER ✻ PRONUNCIATION 0.0410 1 1126 0.840STATE ✻ PRONUNCIATION 0.5902 4 1126 0.670GENDER ✻ STATE ✻ PRONUNCIATION 1.2764 4 1126 0.277GENDER ✻ PRONUNCIATION 0.4701 1 1126 0.493STATE ✻ GENDER ✻ PRONUNCIATION 0.7745 4 1126 0.542

PreN dependent variable: PleasantnessSTATE 1.8597 4 219 0.119GENDER 3.9449 1 219 0.048PRONUNCIATION 0.9013 1 1126 0.343STATE ✻ GENDER 0.0619 4 219 0.993STATE ✻ PRONUNCIATION 0.4805 4 1126 0.750GENDER ✻ PRONUNCIATION 0.4701 1 1126 0.493STATE ✻ GENDER ✻ PRONUNCIATION 0.7745 4 1126 0.542

PreN dependent variable: StateGENDER 4.144 1 219 0.043

Page 36 of 38

Page 37: linguistics.byu.edu  · Web viewGlottal stops in English appear in a variety of contexts. They often precede vowel-initial words such as ‘into’ [ʔɪnthuʊ] and serve to break

STATE 14.614 4 219 < .001PRONUNCIATION 1.255 1 1126 0.263GENDER ✻ STATE 0.320 4 219 0.864GENDER ✻ PRONUNCIATION 1.414 1 1126 0.235STATE ✻ PRONUNCIATION 2.293 4 1126 0.058GENDER ✻ STATE ✻ PRONUNCIATION 0.389 4 1126 0.817

PreN dependent variable: RuralitySTATE 2.999 4 219 0.019GENDER 0.825 1 219 0.365PRONUNCIATION 17.199 1 1126 < .001STATE ✻ GENDER 0.445 4 219 0.776STATE ✻ PRONUNCIATION 1.534 4 1126 0.190GENDER ✻ PRONUNCIATION 1.841 1 1125 0.175STATE ✻ GENDER ✻ PRONUNCIATION 0.233 4 1126 0.920

PreV dependent variable: EducationSTATE 3.377 4 219 0.010GENDER 9.226 1 219 0.003PRONUNCIATION 3.907 1 1127 0.048STATE ✻ GENDER 0.596 4 219 0.666STATE ✻ PRONUNCIATION 0.566 4 1127 0.687GENDER ✻ PRONUNCIATION 1.708 1 1127 0.191STATE ✻ GENDER ✻ PRONUNCIATION 0.542 4 1127 0.705

PreV dependent variable: Age (of speaker)STATE 0.240 4 218 0.916GENDER 1.495 1 218 0.223AGE (of listener) 6.736 1 219 0.010PRONUNCIATION 1.651 1 1127 0.199STATE ✻ GENDER 1.010 4 218 0.403STATE ✻ PRONUNCIATION 1.188 4 1127 0.314GENDER ✻ PRONUNCIATION 0.625 1 1126 0.429STATE ✻ GENDER ✻ PRONUNCIATION 0.737 4 1127 0.567

PreV dependent variable: FriendlinessSTATE 0.670 4 219 0.614GENDER 4.349 1 219 0.038PRONUNCIATION 7.167 1 1127 0.008STATE ✻ GENDER 0.351 4 219 0.843STATE ✻ PRONUNCIATION 1.350 4 1127 0.249GENDER ✻ PRONUNCIATION 3.014 1 1127 0.083STATE ✻ GENDER ✻ PRONUNCIATION 0.415 4 1127 0.798

Page 37 of 38

Page 38: linguistics.byu.edu  · Web viewGlottal stops in English appear in a variety of contexts. They often precede vowel-initial words such as ‘into’ [ʔɪnthuʊ] and serve to break

PreV dependent variable: StateSTATE 4.7322 4 219 0.001GENDER 0.9830 1 219 0.323PRONUNCIATION 0.0238 1 1128 0.878STATE ✻ GENDER 0.2746 4 219 0.894STATE ✻ PRONUNCIATION 1.3917 4 1128 0.235GENDER ✻ PRONUNCIATION 2.2890 1 1127 0.131STATE ✻ GENDER ✻ PRONUNCIATION 1.2613 4 1128 0.283

PreV dependent variable: PleasantnessSTATE 1.141 4 219 0.338GENDER 8.267 1 219 0.004PRONUNCIATION 1.076 1 1127 0.300STATE ✻ GENDER 0.519 4 219 0.722STATE ✻ PRONUNCIATION 0.219 4 1127 0.928GENDER ✻ PRONUNCIATION 1.244 1 1127 0.265STATE ✻ GENDER ✻ PRONUNCIATION 1.002 4 1127 0.405

 

PreV dependent variable: RuralitySTATE 3.4671 4 218 0.009GENDER 2.4652 1 218 0.118AGE 0.0352 1 221 0.851PRONUNCIATION 3.2141 1 1128 0.073STATE ✻ GENDER 0.3559 4 218 0.840STATE ✻ PRONUNCIATION 1.3898 4 1128 0.235GENDER ✻ PRONUNCIATION 1.3530 1 1127 0.245STATE ✻ GENDER ✻ PRONUNCIATION 1.4871 4 1128 0.204

Page 38 of 38