Visual Neglect

21
7/23/2019 Visual Neglect http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/visual-neglect 1/21 MAJOR REVIEW  Visual Neglect Following Stroke: Current Concepts and Future Focus Darren S.J. Ting, MBChB, 1  Alex Pollock, PhD, 2 Gordon N. Dutton, MD, FRCOphth, 3,4,5 Fergus N. Doubal, MRCP, 6 Daniel S.W. Ting, MBBS, 7 Michelle Thompson, BSc (Hons), 6 and Baljean Dhillon, FRCPS, FRCS, FRCOphth 8 1 Victoria Infirmary, Glasgow, UK;  2 Nursing, Midwifery, and Allied Health Professionals (NMAHP) Research Unit, Glasgow Caledonian University, Glasgow, UK;  3 Tennent Institute of Ophthalmology, Gartnavel General Hospital, Glasgow, UK;  4 Royal Hospital for Sick Children, Glasgow, UK;  5  Department of Vision Sciences, Glasgow Caledonian University, Glasgow, UK;  6 Western General Hospital, Edinburgh, UK;  7 Royal Perth Hospital, Western Australia, Australia; and  8 Princess Alexandra Eye Pavilion, Edinburgh, UK  Abstract.  Visual neglect is a common, yet frequently overlooked, neurological disorder following stroke characterized by a deficit in attention and appreciation of stimuli on the contralesional side of the body. It has a profound functional impact on affected individuals. A assessment and management of this condition are hindered, however, by the lack of professional awareness and clinical guidelines. Recent evidence suggests that the underlying deficit in visual attention is due to a disrupted internalized representation of the outer world rather than a disorder of sensory inputs. Dysfunction of the cortical domains and white-matter tracts, as well as inter-hemispheric imbalance, have been implicated in the various manifestations of visual neglect. Optimal diagnosis requires careful history- taking from the patient, family, and friends, in addition to clinical assessment with the line bisection test, the star cancellation test, and the Catherine Bergego Scale. Early recognition and prompt rehabilitation employing a multidisciplinary approach is desirable. Although no treatment has been definitively shown to be of benefit, those with promise include prism adaptation, visual scanning therapy, and virtual reality--based techniques. Further high quality research to seek optimum short- and long-term rehabilitative strategies for visual neglect is required. (Surv Ophthalmol 56:114--134, 2011.   2011 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved.) Key words.  assessment    dorsal stream    neglect    rehabilitation    spatial representation stroke    unilateral neglect    ventral stream    visual neglect I. Introduction  Visual neglect (VN) is a neurological disorder characterized by a deficit in attention to stimuli on one side of the body, almost invariably contra- lateral to the side of the cerebral lesion. 95 The term has been used loosely and interchangeably  with hemispatial neglect, unilateral neglect, and  visual inattention. Its adverse impact on day-to-day functioning and stroke rehabilitation outcome is  well established. Stroke, whether ischemic or hemorrhagic, is a de- bilitating and disabling condition that can impair cognition, visuospatial, language, and motor func- tion. 36 Motor rehabilitation following stroke is well recognized, but rehabilitation for visual neglect, a common and dramatic consequence of stroke, 105 114  2011 by Elsevier Inc.  All rights reserved. 0039-6257/$ - see front matter doi:10.1016/j.survophthal.2010.08.001 SURVEY OF OPHTHALMOLOGY  VOLUME 56   NUMBER 2   MARCH–APRIL 2011

Transcript of Visual Neglect

Page 1: Visual Neglect

7/23/2019 Visual Neglect

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/visual-neglect 1/21

MAJOR REVIEW

 Visual Neglect Following Stroke: Current Conceptsand Future Focus

Darren S.J. Ting, MBChB,1  Alex Pollock, PhD,2 Gordon N. Dutton, MD, FRCOphth,3,4,5

Fergus N. Doubal, MRCP,6 Daniel S.W. Ting, MBBS,7 Michelle Thompson, BSc (Hons),6

and Baljean Dhillon, FRCPS, FRCS, FRCOphth8

1Victoria Infirmary, Glasgow, UK;   2 Nursing, Midwifery, and Allied Health Professionals (NMAHP) Research Unit,Glasgow Caledonian University, Glasgow, UK;   3 Tennent Institute of Ophthalmology, Gartnavel General Hospital,Glasgow, UK;   4 Royal Hospital for Sick Children, Glasgow, UK;   5  Department of Vision Sciences, Glasgow Caledonian University, Glasgow, UK;   6 Western General Hospital, Edinburgh, UK;   7 Royal Perth Hospital, Western Australia, Australia; and   8 Princess Alexandra Eye Pavilion, Edinburgh, UK 

 Abstract.   Visual neglect is a common, yet frequently overlooked, neurological disorder followingstroke characterized by a deficit in attention and appreciation of stimuli on the contralesional side of the body. It has a profound functional impact on affected individuals. A assessment and management of this condition are hindered, however, by the lack of professional awareness and clinical guidelines.

Recent evidence suggests that the underlying deficit in visual attention is due to a disruptedinternalized representation of the outer world rather than a disorder of sensory inputs. Dysfunction of the cortical domains and white-matter tracts, as well as inter-hemispheric imbalance, have beenimplicated in the various manifestations of visual neglect. Optimal diagnosis requires careful history-taking from the patient, family, and friends, in addition to clinical assessment with the line bisectiontest, the star cancellation test, and the Catherine Bergego Scale. Early recognition and prompt rehabilitation employing a multidisciplinary approach is desirable. Although no treatment has beendefinitively shown to be of benefit, those with promise include prism adaptation, visual scanningtherapy, and virtual reality--based techniques. Further high quality research to seek optimum short-and long-term rehabilitative strategies for visual neglect is required. (Surv Ophthalmol 56:114--134,2011.    2011 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved.)

Key words.   assessment      dorsal stream     neglect      rehabilitation     spatial representationstroke     unilateral neglect      ventral stream     visual neglect 

I. Introduction

 Visual neglect (VN) is a neurological disordercharacterized by a deficit in attention to stimulion one side of the body, almost invariably   contra-lateral to the side of the cerebral lesion.95 Theterm has been used loosely and interchangeably 

 with hemispatial neglect, unilateral neglect, and visual inattention. Its adverse impact on day-to-day 

functioning and stroke rehabilitation outcome is well established.

Stroke, whether ischemic or hemorrhagic, is a de-bilitating and disabling condition that can impaircognition, visuospatial, language, and motor func-tion.36 Motor rehabilitation following stroke is wellrecognized, but rehabilitation for visual neglect,a common and dramatic consequence of stroke,105

114 2011 by Elsevier Inc.

 All rights reserved.0039-6257/$ - see front matter

doi:10.1016/j.survophthal.2010.08.001

SURVEY OF OPHTHALMOLOGY  VOLUME 56   NUMBER 2   MARCH–APRIL 2011

Page 2: Visual Neglect

7/23/2019 Visual Neglect

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/visual-neglect 2/21

has received limited attention, as it can be difficult toidentify, and there is currently no consensus con-cerning optimal assessment and treatment of thiscondition.

II. Epidemiology  A US study 141 of 1,281 stroke patients reported

a prevalence rate of VN of 43% and 20% followingright and left hemispheric stroke, respectively. Thisasymmetry in prevalence of VN is consistent withother literature4,13,21 and probably occurs becausethe right hemisphere tends to allocate attention toboth hemispaces, but the left hemisphere accordsattention more selectively to right hemispace.66,116

 Also, VN patients with right hemispheric strokegenerally have a worse prognosis than those with left hemispheric stroke.13,46,131

Despite the high prevalence rate, 61% of patients with VN w ere overlooked during hospital admissionin the US,40 and only 13% of stroke patients wereassessed with standardized neglect assessments ina Canadian study.114 This low detection rate couldbe explained by insufficient awareness of thiscondition or may reflect inherent difficulties indiagnosing neglect in the presence of other moreacute and readily recognizable features of stroke likedysphagia, dysphasia, hemianopia, or hemiplegia.In the UK, the documented prev alence rates   of 

 VN post-stroke vary between 8%164 and 82%162 asa result of heterogeneous patient cohorts, inconsis-tent definition, non-standardized assessment methods, and different timings of the assessment.

Increased age has also been shown to beassociated with a higher incidence and severity of 

 VN.57,141 This could be attributed to prior cerebralatrophy or a physiologically dampened reactiontime in the elderly population during visual search-ing activities, especiall y when intensive visual atten-tion is   required.57,102 Other   demographic factorslike sex111,141 and handedness141 have no significant influence on the occurrence of VN.

III. Varieties of Neglect 

 A wide range of neglect variants have beendescribed as a consequence of the discrepant classifications adopted. Conceptually, it   can beconsidered under the following headings.169

 A. MODALITY (INPUT/OUTPUT)

Neglect can be divided into sensory (input) andpremotor (output) neglect. Sensory neglect ischaracterized by unawareness of sensory stimuli of 

different modalities—including tactile/somatosen-

sory, auditory, and visual or visuospatial in contrale-sional hemispace. Premotor neglect is described asfailure to orientate the limbs towards contralesionalhemispace despite awareness of the stimulus. VNhas been primarily considered part of the spectrumof sensory neglect; recent studies, however, suggest that the underlying deficit primarily is a disruptedinternal computation of the spatial representationof the external world rather than a true sensory deficit (see section V. Pathophysiology in thisreview) This has important implications in for-mulating optimal assessment and rehabilitationstrategies.

B. SPATIAL REPRESENTATION

Spatial representation, which depends on theframe of reference, has also been used to distinguishbetween different neglect subtypes, namely, egocen-

tric (viewer-centered) VN and allocentric (stimulusor object-centered) VN. Egocentric VN  is described asfailure to orientate or attend to the stimuli on thecontralesional hemispace with respect to the mid-sagittal plane of the body, head and eye. In contrast,allocentric VN  refers to the inability to attend to theone side of the stimulus irrespective of the relativeposition of the stimulus with respect to the viewer.

Egocentric VN has a higher reported incidencethan allocentric VN as a sequel to a cerebrovascularevent. As a result, the terms ‘‘visual neglect’’ or‘‘unilateral neglect’’ primarily refer clinically toegocentric VN; however, these neglect subty pes exist as separate entities with distinct neural bases58,109,113

(see section V.E Clinico-anatomical correlations of  visual neglect, herein). VN is a heterogeneousdisorder that involves various neurocognitive re-gions, and broader consideration needs to be takenfor each neglect subtype.

C. RANGE OF SPACE

 According to the classification by ‘‘range of space’’, VN can be divided into personal, periperso-nal, and extrapersonal neglect. This classification

has been widely adopted as part of VN assessment of the functional performance in clinical practice.

IV. Functional Impact 

 VN results in a range of functional impairments, with severe neglect being associated with a poorprognosis.79,163 Those with left VN fail to attend totheir left side, with features ranging from thoseaffecting personal, through peripersonal, to extrap-ersonal space (Table 1). Each subtype of neglect may either manifest as a separate entity or be

associated with other subtypes. For instance, a

 VISUAL NEGLECT FOLLOWING STROKE   115

Page 3: Visual Neglect

7/23/2019 Visual Neglect

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/visual-neglect 3/21

patient affected by peripersonal neglect does not necessarily also manifest extrapersonal neglect.59,173

It is important, however, to recognize that thisclassification concerns the external observers’ in-terpretation rather than the nature of the distur-bance of the internalized, virtual representation of 

the external world that characterizes human vision/ visuospatial perception. Also, VN may be associated with other forms of neglect, including tactile,auditory, and motor neglect.

 V. Pathophysiology 

 VN is a multifaceted disorder. The anatomical,physiological, and conceptual models of cortical

 visual processing have always been highly conten-tious scientific issues. As mentioned, VN can beconceptually divided into several categories accord-

ing to the modality (input/output), the spatialrepresentation, and the range of space. Although various mechanisms and models have been pro-posed and examined, none has fully and successfully captured the entire spectrum of this heterogeneousdisorder. We attempt a systematic overview of pathophysiology and clinico-anatomical correlationsof VN from different perspectives.

 A. PHYSIOLOGY OF VISUOSPATIAL PERCEPTION

It is becoming increasingly apparent that ourbrain operates and processes visual information via

two distinct, but complementary, higher neuralprocessing pathways, namely, the dorsal streamand ventral stream (as proposed by Milner andGoodale).56,119,120 They suggested that the dorsalstream, which connects the visual cortex with thesuperior parietal lobe and the intraparietal sulcus,subconsciously assimilates incoming visual informa-tion in order to bring about immediate ‘‘on-line’’,

 visually guided movements through the virtualimages in the mind, which constitute an internaland subconscious egocentric spatial representationof the multidimensional external world. By contrast,

the ventral stream, which bridges the visual cortex

and the inferior temporal lobe, is accountable forour conscious, object-based visual perception andrecognition. Dysfunction of dorsal stream results inoptic ataxia (impaired visual guidance of move-ment), and dysfunction of ventral stream causes

 visual agnosia (impaired recognition of visual

stimuli), respectively. Although this model providesa comprehensive explanation for various compo-nents of visuospatial perception and visuomotorfunction, the manifestation of VN, which normally follows the damage of inferior parietal lobe, has yet to be fully considered in the context of this dorsal--

 ventral dichotomy.156  Also, the double dissociationbetween VN and optic ataxia or visual agnosia hassuggested separate underlying disorders of theneural substrates.73,133

Instead of the occipito-parietal and occipito-temporal pathways, the dorsal--ventral dichotom y 

 variant proposed by Corbetta and Shulman32

focused on the attentional basis of the frontopar-ietal networks, namely, the dorsal and ventralfrontoparietal pathways. The bilateral ‘‘dorsal’’pathways, which connect the superior parietal lobesand the intraparietal sulci with the dorsal frontallobes, including the frontal eye fields, are involvedin goal-directed, top--down attentional selection.32

This top--down attention facilitates visuospatialexploration in the context of prior   knowledge of 

 what to search for or what the task is.33 In contrast,the right lateralized ‘‘ventral’’ pathway, which links

the temporal parietal junction (TPJ), inferiorparietal lobe (IPL), and ventral frontal lobe, isassociated with   stimulus-driven, bottom--up atten-tional selection.32 This bottom--up attention simply refers to the ability to capture an event occurring inthe surrounding world—salience detection.33 In

 view of the overlap between the neural basis of VNand their proposed ‘‘ventral’’ network, Corbetta andShulman infer that V N is associated with a bottom--up attentional deficit.32  Also, their proposed right lateralized ventral pathway helps explain the higherincidence of VN following right posterior parietal

damage.

TABLE 1

 Definitions and Examples of Personal, Peripersonal, and Extrapersonal Neglect 

Personal space—the space occupied by one’s body  combing, grooming, shaving, recognizing the right half of the body only 17,90 and anosagnosia (a feature in which

the patient is unable to recognize his or her own deficit)14,18

Peripersonal space—the space surrounding one’s body, within arm’s reach  eating food from therighthalfoftheplateandneglectingthefoodontheleft,readingtherighthalfofthetwopagesof 

an open book28

Extrapersonal space—the space surrounding one’s body, beyond arm’s reach  failing to identify  meaningful stimuli and people on the left, colliding into objects on the left while mobilizing or

using a wheelchair138

116 Surv Ophthalmol 56 (2) March--April 2011   TING ET AL

Page 4: Visual Neglect

7/23/2019 Visual Neglect

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/visual-neglect 4/21

In addition, consistent activation of both frontaland parietal regions, as evident from imagingstudies,52,134,157 during tasks requiring spatial atten-tion and visuomotor function have supported theneural basis of this fronto-parietal model. Nonethe-less, there is emerging evidence showing that patients with VN may exhibit deficits of bothattentional components,  including sustaining atten-tion (goal-direct ed)24,74 and salience detection(stimulus-driven),23,33  which suggests that a combi-nation of both of the described conceptual frame-

 works is required to explain this complex disorder.

B. SPATIALLY LATERALIZED DYSFUNCTION OF

 VISUAL NEGLECT

 According to the original definition, VN isa disorder characterized by the failure of orientatingor attending towards the stimuli on one side of the

hemispace; it is a form of spatially lateralizeddysfunction. For instance, patients with ‘‘egocentric’’

 VN can exhibit complete inattention (perceptual/input neglect) or impaired visuospatial exploration(premotor/output neglect) confined to contrale-sional hemispace, with normal visuospatial percep-tion within ipsilesional hemispace. This spatially lateralized dysfunction of VN has been well demon-strated by both clinical observation and the VNassessment tools (see subsequent discussion).

Heilman et al66 hypothesized that the right posterior parietal cortex, which is highly specialized

for spatial attention, allocates attention to bothhemispaces, whereas the left posterior parietal cortexallocates attention, more selectively, to the right hemispace. This explains why VN is more commonly reported following right rather than left hemisphericdamage.13,21,141 This is also why the affected in-dividual exhibits deficits on the contralesional side as

 well as the ipsilesional side, to a milder extent,12,149

 when the right hemisphere is affected. Moreover, thecommon paradoxical observation of contralesionallimb apraxia (inability to activate limb) following left IPL damage, but the development of VN following

right IPL damage, underlines the organizational andfunctional asymmetry between both IPLs, at least inthe context of spatial attention.54

On the other hand, Kinsbourne96 suggested that allocation of spatial attention towards both hemi-spaces is balanced by the reciprocal inhibition fromeach hemisphere, with left hemisphere allocatingattention towards the right hemispace and vice

 versa. Therefore, when there is a right hemisphericdamage, the unopposed left hemisphere becomesrelatively hyper-attentive towards the right hemi-space, subsequently leading to left  VN. This model is

further reinforced by Koch et al

100

that describes

relative electroencephalographic hyperexcitability in the left posterior parietal cortex (PPC) of theneglect patients with right hemispheric stroke,resulting in rightward attention bias (or left visualneglect). Improvement of VN was observed in 7 of 10 patients following the application of repetitivetranscranial magnetic stimulation (r-TMS) over t heunaffected PPC. In a similar vein, Oliver et al128

reported beneficial effect in patients with VNfollowing the application of r-TMS on the un-affected PPC. An explanatory mechanism is that r-TMS disrupts the integrated neural activity of theintact side, dampening the attention to the ipsile-sional side, and thereby restoring the orientationbalance between both hemispheres.

C. NON-SPATIALLY LATERALIZED DYSFUNCTION

OF VISUAL NEGLECT

Until recently, VN was thought strictly to beattributable to lateralized disruption of the spatialattention on one side of the visual space; however,there is emerging evidence proposing that VN is not necessarily confined to one hemispace. It cancomprise both spatially lat eralized and non--spatially lateralized dysfunctions.74 In other words, patients

 with VN may completely neglect the contralesionalhemispace (spatially lateralized) while demonstrat-ing global inattention to stimuli regardless of thelocation of the target (non-spatially lateralized).

Both the IPL and the ventral frontal region—

 which, when damaged, are frequently associated with VN—have been shown to exhibit functionsrelated to non-spatial attention including sustainedattention and salience detection.74 Similarly, VNpatients, with damage to the IPL, may demonstratethese non-spatial attentional deficits. This can beassessed b y techniques including the oddball para-digm,31,38 which requires the patient to detect target stimuli interspersed with non-target stimuli   at thesame area, and attentional blink paradigm,75 a mea-sure that assesses the ability of salience detectionand perception of each stimulus among a series of 

rapidly changing stimuli at one spatial location. A delayed ‘‘attentional-blink’’ response is suggestive of an attentional deficit. Although non-spatial inatten-tion may be doubly dissociable from VN,150 thepresence of a non-spatial  attentional deficit aggra-

 vates the severity of VN.75

These intriguing findings enhance understandingof the pathophysiology of VN and, more impor-tantly, facilitate the development of potentially moreeffective therapies including sustained attentionmeasures and pharmacological intervention di-rected t ow ards improving general non-spatial

attention.

107

 VISUAL NEGLECT FOLLOWING STROKE   117

Page 5: Visual Neglect

7/23/2019 Visual Neglect

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/visual-neglect 5/21

D. ROLE OF NON-VISUAL AFFERENT INPUTS

In addition to visual afferent input, egocentricmultidimensional internalized spatial mapping re-quires multiple afferent inputs from non-visualreceptors, including neck muscle19 and extra-ocular muscle proprioceptors,75 and the tactile

and the vestibular systems.125,127

This afferent pro-prioceptive feedback maintains the required stabil-ity and perception of the location of the visualimages by maintaining correct egocentric localiza-tion of the body despite head and eye movement.There is evidence that patients w ho   have alteredextraocular  muscle proprioception175—either fromsurger y,25,161 forced ductions with a suction contact lens,48 or dysfunction of the trigeminal nerve26,171—demonstrate errors in spatial localization and

 visually guided movement.Karnath87 demonstrated the role of neck muscle

proprioception in maintaining egocentric spatiallocalization. He observed that the ipsilesional de- viation of neglect patients when looking straight ahead can be ameliorated by the   applicationof vibration to the contralesional neck,87,152 modi-fying the afferent input from the neck muscleproprioceptors.

In addition, the role of the vestibular system inbody orientation and egocentric spatial localizationhas long been recognized. The vestibular input contributes to spatial orientation mainly via the

 vestibulo-ocular reflex, which involves the trans-mission of afferent inputs from the vestibular nucleito the ocular motor nuclei and higher corticalcenters via the medial longitudinal fasciculus.125,127

Following integration of the afferent inputs in thehigher cortical centers, efferent outputs are trans-mitted along vestibulospinal projections for head,eye, and body orientation—the internal ‘‘plumbline,’’ which accords balance and our knowledge of the vertical, and hence, spatial localization.125,127

This concept has been further reinforced by the useof vestibular caloric stimulation (irrigation of theear canal with either warm water ipsilaterally or cold

 water contralaterally), which transiently reduces the

orientation bias in VN and can  rest ore the egocen-tric spatial reference frame.88,147 Longer terminterventions, designed to ‘‘deceive’’ the sensory input of the visuospatial perception rather thanaddressing the underlying lack of awareness, re-mains elusive.

E. CLINICO-ANATOMICAL CORRELATIONS OF

 VISUAL NEGLECT

Several factors challenge clinicians and researchersinvestigating the neural basis of VN. First, patients

 with VN often manifest extensive brain damage, which

hinders precise anatomical correlation. Second, pureassessment of VN is problematic in the stroke patient 

 who has co-existing cognitive impairment, causingdifficulty in recruiting ‘‘ideal’’ VN patients for furtherinvestigation.Also, the lackof detailed classificationof the different neglect subtypes can lead to over-generalization of the neural basis of VN. For example,thedistinction betweenegocentricVN andallocentric

 VN, which can occur independently,58,109 has not been made in many studies.

 Various cortical regions serving visuospatial atten-tion have been identified by high quality functionalbrain imaging like functional magnetic resonanceimaging and positron emission tomography. Theseregions comprise, in particular, the inferior parietallobe and the TPJ,64,70,123,170 the superior temporalgyrus,85 and the frontal lobe,  including the medialand inferior frontal gyri.75,76 In contrast, the pre-

 viously controversial role of the subcortical regions

implicated in VN has been refuted by recent studies,suggesting that, instead of the subcortical infarctionitself, VN is associated with hypoperfusion of theoverlying cortex and that   subcortical infarctionalone does not result in VN.70

The neuroanatomical findings can be influencedby   the timing of assessment. For instance, Mort et al,123 who investigated the neural basis of VN duringthe chronic phase (63 days post-stroke), havereported that VN is most strongly associated withangular gyrus (part of the inferior parietal lobe)infarction. In contrast, Karnath et al84 examined

their patients at 8 days post-stroke and had observedthe superior temporal gyrus is the most commonimplicated region in VN.

Moreover, the development of diffusion tensorimaging, which provides the facility to track white-matter tracts, has led to a quantum leap in theunderstanding of the neuro-anatomical correlationof VN. Various white-matter tracts have beenident ified,   particularly in the fronto-parietal net-

 work,11,15,37 and their dysfunction can lead to   the‘‘disconnection syndrome’’ underlying VN.11,37 Thisobservation is in accordance with the anatomical

and theoretical model proposed by Corbetta andShulman,32  who focused on the attentional basis of the fronto-parietal network. This suggests that spatial attention operates in a multi-level networkthat requires intact neurocognitive cortical regionsand the underlying connections between them.Interestingly, VN results from both anterior andposterior circulation strokes, depending   on   theaffected corresponding white-matter tracts.15,37

In addition, VN has been classified according tothe site of neuroanatomical damage, which chiefly comprises three areas: the parietal lobes, the frontal

eye fields, and the cingulate regions.

117

Each of the

118 Surv Ophthalmol 56 (2) March--April 2011   TING ET AL

Page 6: Visual Neglect

7/23/2019 Visual Neglect

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/visual-neglect 6/21

neuro-anatomical neglect subtypes exhibits somedegree of site-specific variation (Table 2). Althoughthis classification provides useful insights into thecomplex neuroanatomical correlation of VN, it doesnot include the underlying complex disorders of function. Also, the differentiation of these subtypescan be difficult in clinical practice.

 As discussed earlier, spatial representation takesinto account different frames of reference, primarily 

 viewer-centered (egocent ric) and stimulus-centered(allocentric). Hillis et al70 examined the neural basisof VN subtypes and observed distinct physiology andneural correlates underlying both subtypes with theaid of high quality functional brain imaging. They have reported that egocentric VN  primarily followsdamage to the inferior parietal lobe69,70 and, to someextent, the posterior inferior frontal gyrus,70  whichare implicated in the ventral frontoparietal networkproposed by Corbetta and Shulman.32 Similar asso-ciation betweenegocentric VN and dysfunction of thesupramarginal gyrus (part of the inferior   parietal

lobe) has been observed by Medina et al.113

On theother hand, allocentric VN is more commonly observed following injuries to the temporal regions,including the superior temporal gyrus,70 the ventro-medial temporal area,58 and the posterior inferiortemporal gyrus.113 This is consistent with Milnerand Goodale’s proposal that the temporal or

 ventral pathway is highly accountable for object-based perception and recognition; therefore, dys-function of this pathway leads to allocentric VN. Thefunctional differences and the interactions among

 various intra-temporal regions have yet to be further

investigated.

 Although the neural correlates of left VN have been well studied, right VN following left hemisphericst roke has received limited attention. Kleinman et al98 focused on the neural correlates of VN subtypesfollowing left, instead of right, hemispheric strokeand reported a higher proportion of allocentric VNthan egocentric VN, which contrasts with the findingsof Hillis et al.70 These findings, once again, suggest that both hemispheres are organized and function in

distinct ways in the context of spatial attention.In view of the heterogeneity of VN, we emphasize

that all studies should be interpreted with cautionbecause they are influenced by the type of VNincluded in the study, the assessment methods used,and the timing and type of brain imaging instituted.

F. CLINICAL SYNDROMES ASSOCIATED WITH

 VISUAL NEGLECT

 VN has been found to be associated with othercomplex parietal lobe syndromes that affect visuo-

spatial comprehension and attention, such asGerstmann and Balint syndromes. Gerstmann syn-drome results from a focal lesion affecting theangular and supramarginal gyri near the TPJ,usually on the left, and is characterized by inability to perceive an object properly (agnosia), dy sgra-phia, dyscalculia, and left--right disorientation.132

Balint   syndrome, first reported in the early 20thcentury,9 is an uncommon disorder that classically results frombilateralposteriorparietal pathology andcomprises a constellation of symptoms: unilateral

 visuospatial inattention (left hemineglect), deficits in

 visually guided movement despite normal limb

TABLE 2

Summary of Neuroanatomical Classification and the Functional Consequences of VN 

Type Function of Each Area Presentation

Parietal neglect    The posterior parietal lobes integrate afferent stimuli of any modality including visualstimuli, transforming external space into an

internalized representation

Loss of subconscious synthesis of theinternalized representational space,resulting in unawareness of the visual

stimuli in contralesional hemispace, evenone’s own body (a form of anosagnosia)

 Frontal neglect    The frontal eye fields interact with theparietal lobes in selecting salient information and filtering irrelevant information, thus planning meaningful visually guided movement 

Failure of limb orientation towardscontralesional hemispace in the absence of hemiparesis, even when using theunaffected limb on the ipsilesional side, which suggests the abnormality lies inmotor output instead of sensory input;34,65

this is probably secondary to a defect in theinternalized representation of  motor programming towards the contralesionalside

Cingulate neglect    Provides limbic integration, responsible forallocating  directed attention according tomotivation130

Spends a disproportionately long periodattending to the greater motivational event,omitting the others which have less impact 

Data from Mesulam et al.117

 VISUAL NEGLECT FOLLOWING STROKE   119

Page 7: Visual Neglect

7/23/2019 Visual Neglect

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/visual-neglect 7/21

strength (optic ataxia), and the inability to shift gaze voluntarily to objects of interest  despite normal eyemovements (ocular apraxia).9,63 The spatial disorderof attention emphasized by Balint was furtherregarded   as ‘‘disturbance of visual orient ation’’ by Holmes71 and simultanagnosia by Wolpert.177 Basedon his observation of six soldiers with bilateralparietal-occipital lobe injury particularly affectingthe angular gyri, Holmes concluded that damage tothe angular gyri can lead to w hat he called a ‘‘distur-bance of visual orientation’’71 and results in difficul-ties in precise determination of the multidimensionalposition of an object or relative positions of objects(optic ataxia) and in learning the new surroundingsand in transferring attention from one object toanother (ocular apraxia). On the other hand, theterm ‘‘simultanagnosia’’, coined by Wolpert,177 refersto the inability to perceive and integrate all thecomponents of a visual scene. Each element of Balint 

syndrome—left hemineglect, optic ataxia, and ocularapraxia—may manifest independently.34

Refer to Rizzo and Vecera’s142 study for furtherelaboration.

 VI. Assessment of Visual Neglect 

 A. CURRENT CLINICAL GUIDELINES

Recommendations concerning the specific assess-ment methods of VN remain limited. US39 andUK 77,154 guidelines consistently recommend that 

 visuo-perceptual function, including the potentialfor VN, should be assessed in patients in stroke unitsemploying a multidisciplinary approach. Mini-Mental State Examination and Addenbrooke’sCognitive Examination (2005) are the two most 

 well-recognized general cognitive assessment batter-ies in current clinical practice in the UK.47,121 Thelimitation of both tests is that VN is only assessed by drawing and copying objects (Table 3).

No specific assessment tool is recommended  inthe US. On the other hand, the UK guidelines77

recommend the use of   Behavioral Inattention Test 

 whereas the Canadian

27

guidelines recommend theuse of specific assessment tools for VN, comprisingthe Comb and Razor Test, the Line Bisection Test (LBT), and the Behavioral Inattention Test with thealternatives of the Rivermead, Ontario Society of Occupational Therapy Perceptual Evaluation andthe Motor Free Visual Perceptual Test.

B. HOW IS VISUAL NEGLECT ASSESSED?

The results of an extensive literature searchconcerning the assessment of VN and the underlyingrationale, assessment profiles, with strengths and

 weaknesses of the more common tests of VN are

summarized in  Table 3. The assessment methodscomprise four main groups: pencil-and-paper tests,behavioral assessment, clinical observation, and morerecent techniques.

1. Pencil-and-paper Tests

 VN can be sought by the cancellation   test, theLBT, and the copying and drawing test.144

The cancellation task (Fig. 1) requires the patient to delete multiple identical visual target symbols ona paper sheet. Incomplete or disproportionate de-letion on one side indicates VN. There is robust evidence showing that this type of test, particularly interspersed withdistracters,hasthebest sensitivity of the pencil-and-paper tests.60,61,97,110 Common vari-ants include the star cancellation test, the lettercancellation test, and the bell cancellation test.Moreover, structured observation of cancellation test 

performance, taking into account the location of theinitial commencing point 2 (deemed to be the most sensitive measure) along with the scanning pattern,the search time, and the number of re-cancellationsof the same target,55,108,167 provide useful qualitativeinformation in addition to quantitative scores.

The LBT (Fig. 1) requires the patient to estimateand bisect the midline, thereby determining left-

 ward/rightward orient ation bias. Despite limitations,the construct validity,110 reasonable sensitivity andreliability, and ease of administration render   thisa good supplementary pencil-and-paper test.8 The

minimal time required for administration is a majoradvantage for clinicians.On the other hand, copying and drawing tasks

(Fig. 1) are the least satisfactory pencil-and-papertests in terms of sensitivity, reliability, and validity,8

but have a place in gaining an understanding of thenature of the morbidity.

Lindell et al104 and Jehkonen et al80 havehighlighted the benefit of combining several toolsin yielding a higher detection rate and bettercharacterization of VN. On the basis of the availableevidence, the star cancellation test, coupled with theline bisection test, are recommended as screeningtools for all patients with hemispheric stroke.

2. Behavioral Assessment Tools

 Azuovi et al found that behavioral assessment hasa much higher sensitivity than pencil-and-paper tests(96% vs 65%).5 This finding is in accordance withother similar studies.6,61 Therefore, behavioralassessment is essential to enhance VN detectionrate and, more importantly, to determine   andevaluate the nature and level of functioning.4

The BIT and the Catherine Bergego Scale (CBS)

are two of the most frequently used tests. The BIT

176

is

120 Surv Ophthalmol 56 (2) March--April 2011   TING ET AL

Page 8: Visual Neglect

7/23/2019 Visual Neglect

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/visual-neglect 8/21

Page 9: Visual Neglect

7/23/2019 Visual Neglect

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/visual-neglect 9/21

a 15-item checklist, which comprises six conventionaltests (line crossing, letter cancellation, star cancella-tion, figure copying, line bisection, and free drawing)and nine behavioral tests (picture scanning, tele-phone dialing, menu reading, article reading, tellingand setting the time, coin sorting, address andsentence copying, map navigation, and card sorting).There is consistent evidence showing that BIT servesas a good predictive   measure of functional perfor-mance in daily living.62,115 The major limitation of this battery is that it does not formally assess thepersonal space. This is imperative because eachof theneglect subtypes (personal, peripersonal, and extrap-ersonal) can manifest as a separate entity.

On the other hand, the CBS7 is a 10-item checklist that focuses on functional performance of activitiesof daily living. These include grooming and shavingthe left part of the face, wearing the left sleeve orslipper, eating food on the left side of the plate,

cleaning the left side of the mouth after eating,spontaneous leftward gaze, ‘‘knowledge’’ of the left part of the body, auditory attention to stimuli fromthe left, collisions with objects on the left, leftwardnavigation in familiar places, and locating familiaritems on the left. Although this test was originally designed for the assessment of left VN, it should beapplicable to right VN. We prefer CBS over BITbecause CBS considers all three conceptual spaces,includes anosognosia scoring,7 and has fine psycho-metric properties, including validity, sensitivity, andreliability.5,115  Anosognosia scoring serves as an

important indicator of VN severity.51,137 We recommend that all patients who demonstrate

spatial orientation bias on VN screening test shouldundergo some form of behavioral assessment likeBIT or, preferably, CBS.

3. Clinical Observation and History-taking from

the Patient and the Near Contacts

Skilled clinical obser vation should increase thedetection rate of VN,81  which may be missedbecause of the false negative result of the standard-ized test s and VN’s inherently subtle and fluctuatingcourse.2 Family and friends who are familiar witha patient’s normal behavior may also play a signifi-cant role in picking up subtle neglect behavior. VNmay be missed during the acute phase of stroke but may be picked up later by other allied healthprofessionals or at home by the near contacts.

4. Recent Development of Assessment 

Technique

One of the main shortcomings of the pencil-and-paper tests is the inability to differentiate between

sensory/input and premotor/output neglect, as

    T   a    b    l   e    3    (   c   o   n    t     i   n   u   e     d

    )

    T

   e   s   t   s

    B   r    i   e    f

    D   e   s   c   r    i   p   t    i   o   n    /    R   a   t    i   o   n   a    l   e

    S   t   r   e   n   g   t    h

    L    i   m    i   t   a   t    i   o   n

     O   c   u     l   o   g   r   a    p     h   y       3

       0

    R   e   q   u    i   r   e   s   t    h   e   p   a   t    i   e   n   t   t   o

    f   o   c   u   s   a   t   t    h   e   m    i    d   p   o    i   n   t   o    f   a   g    i   v   e   n   o    b

    j    e   c   t ,

    i   m   a   g   e   o   r   t   a   s    k

    L   o   o    k    i   n   g    f   o   r   o   r    i   e   n   t   a   t    i   o   n    b    i   a   s   t   o   w   a   r    d   s   t    h   e    i   p   s    i    l   e   s    i   o   n   a    l   s    i    d   e   w    h    i   c    h

    i   n    d    i   c   a   t   e   s    V    N

    A    l    l   o   w   s   p   u   r   e   a   s   s   e   s   s   m   e   n   t   o    f    V    N

   a   s   m   a   n   u   a    l   e   x   p    l   o   r   a   t    i   o   n

    i   s   n   o   t   r   e   q   u    i   r   e    d

 ,   w    h    i   c    h   t    h   u   s

   e   x   c    l   u    d   e   s   t    h   e   m   o   t   o   r

   c   o   m   p   o   n   e   n   t    /   p   r   e   m   o   t   o   r   n   e   g    l   e   c   t

    T    h   e   r   e    i   s   n   o

   r   o    b   u   s   t   e   v    i    d   e   n   c   e   o   n

   t    h   e   v   a    l    i    d    i   t

   y ,   s   e   n   s    i   t    i   v    i   t   y ,   a   n    d

   r   e    l    i   a    b    i    l    i   t   y

   o    f   t    h    i   s   t   e   s   t

    D   o   e   s   n   o   t   a   s   s   e   s   s   p   e   r   s   o   n   a    l   s   p   a   c   e

     V     i   r    t   u   a     l   r   e   a     l     i    t   y

    R   e   q   u    i   r   e   s   a   s   e   t   o    f    h   a   r    d   w   a   r   e    (    i .   e . ,

    h   e   a    d  -   m   o   u   n   t   e    d    d    i   s   p    l   a   y    ) ,   e   y   e

   t   r   a   c    k   e   r   s ,   a   n    d    h   e   a    d  -   p   o   s    i   t    i   o   n   s   e   n   s   o   r

    E   n   a    b    l   e   s   t    h   e   u   s   e   r   s   t   o   e   x

   p    l   o   r   e   r   e   a    l  -   t    i   m   e   v    i   r   t   u   a    l    i   m   a   g   e   s   w    h    i    l   e

   r   e    l   e   v   a   n   t    i   n    f   o   r   m   a   t    i   o   n

    i   n   c    l   u    d    i   n   g    h   e   a    d   m   o   t    i   o   n ,

   s   c   a   n   n    i   n   g   p   a   t   t   e   r   n ,

   a   n    d   s   p   a   t    i   a    l   o   r    i   e   n   t   a   t    i   o   n   a   r   e   r   e   c   o   r    d   e    d   a   n    d   a   n   a    l   y   z   e    d

    A    l    l   o   w   s   a   s   s   e   s   s   m   e   n   t    i   n   a   s    i   m   u    l   a   t   e    d   e   n   v    i   r   o   n   m   e   n   t   r   e  -

    l   a   t    i   n   g   t   o    d   a    i    l   y    l    i    f   e

    C   r   e   a   t   e   s   a   m   o   r   e    i   n   t   e   r   a   c   t    i   v   e ,   v   e

   r   s   a   t    i    l   e   a   n    d   s   a    f   e   r

   e   n   v    i   r   o   n   m   e   n   t    f   o   r   a   s   s   e   s   s   m   e   n   t

   o    f    V    N

    P   r   e   s   e   n   c   e   o    f   e   y   e   t   r   a   c    k   e   r   s   a    i    d   t    h

   e   a   s   s   e   s   s   o   r   t   o   r   e   c   o   r    d

   p   a   t    i   e   n   t    ’   s   v    i   s   u   a    l   s   e   a   r   c    h   p   a   t   t   e   r   n   a   n    d    d   e   v   e    l   o   p    b   e   t   t   e   r

   u   n    d   e   r   s   t   a   n    d    i   n   g   o    f   t    h   e   n   a   t   u   r   e   o    f    V    N

    F   o   c   u   s   e   s   o   n   p   e   r    i   p   e   r   s   o   n   a    l   s   p   a   c   e

    V    i   s   u   o   s   p   a   t    i   a    l

   p   e   r   c   e   p   t    i   o   n    i   n   t    h   e

   v    i   r   t   u   a    l   e   n   v    i   r   o   n   m   e   n   t   m   a   y   v   a   r   y

    b   e   t   w   e   e   n    i   n    d    i   v    i    d   u   a    l   s        7

       8

    F   u   r   t    h   e   r   e   v   a    l

   u   a   t    i   o   n   o    f   v   a    l    i    d    i   t   y ,

   s   e   n   s    i   t    i   v    i   t   y ,

   a   n    d   r   e    l    i   a    b    i    l    i   t   y    i   s

   r   e   q   u    i   r   e    d

     a    T    h   e   s   e    i   n   v   e   s   t    i   g   a   t    i   o   n   s   o   n    l   y   e   v   a    l   u   a   t   e   a   t   t   e   n   t    i   o   n    f   o   r   p   e   r    i   p   e   r   s   o   n   a    l   s   p   a   c   e   a   n    d   c   a   n   n   o   t    d    i    f    f   e   r   e   n   t    i   a   t   e    b   e   t   w   e   e   n    i   n   p   u   t   a   n    d   o   u   t   p   u   t   n   e   g    l   e   c   t .

    T

    h    i   s   t   a    b    l   e    i   s   m   o    d    i    fi   e    d    f   r   o   m    P    l   u   m   m   e   r

   e   t   a    l .

       1       3        7

122 Surv Ophthalmol 56 (2) March--April 2011   TING ET AL

Page 10: Visual Neglect

7/23/2019 Visual Neglect

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/visual-neglect 10/21

these tests require both visual attention and manualexploration. Chiba et al30 have proposed the use of oculography for the midpoint-fixation task, whichremoves the manual component.

 Virtual reality (VR) based techniques may poten-tially overcome the shortcomings of traditionalassessment methods.94 The use of VR systems not only helps to establish a better assessment andunderstanding of the nature of VN, but also createsa safe, versatile, and multimodal virtual environ-ment to assess the patient’s functional performance

in a range of daily living tasks. Conventional testsincluding the line bisection test, the cancellationtest, and daily living tasks   ha ve all been incorpo-rated into a VR system.166 However, the VR assessment focuses on use of peripersonal and, toa certain extent, extrapersonal space and furtherevaluation is required to determine its sensitivity,cost-effectiveness, and practicability.

C. DISTINCTION BETWEEN VISUAL NEGLECT

 AND OTHER VISUAL PROBLEMS

 VN is commonly associated with other visual

problems, including visual extinction and homony-

mous hemianopia (HH). Therefore, understandingof the nature of these visual problems and thedistinct features of each visual condition shouldfacilitate assessment and diagnostic process.

 Visual extinction is described as the failure of identifying one of two simultaneous stimuli, whileretaining the perception of a stimulus presentedsingly in either hemispace.169 Unlike VN, theallocation of attention in the cont ext of visualextinction is largely task-dependent.172 For instance,the ability to localize stimuli on bilateral display can be

markedly impaired (similar to neglect), but withretained normal reporting of numerical stimuli onbilateral presentation.172 Extinction may also occur

 when both stimuli are presented simultaneously  within the same hemifield.172 Nonetheless, visualextinction has been described as a   cardinal signindicative of a deficit in attention,16,139 or as part of neglect disorder.67 Clinically, visual extinction shouldbe sought during neurological examination, as visualextinction and neglect are commonly, but not in-

 variably, associated.49 This can be done by asking thepatient to point to the examiner’s moving finger,

initially onesideat a time andthen bilaterally inmirror

 Fig. 1.   Examples of patient with VN neglecting the stimuli on the left hemispace in: (A ) a cancellation task; (B ) a linebisection task; and (C ) a copying and drawing task. (Adapted from   http://ahsmail.uwaterloo.ca/waktse/assessment.html.)

 VISUAL NEGLECT FOLLOWING STROKE   123

Page 11: Visual Neglect

7/23/2019 Visual Neglect

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/visual-neglect 11/21

image quadrants. The ability to identify a singlestimulus, but not simultaneous stimuli, defines extinc-tion. In addition, Hillis et al68 have shown that visual,tactile, and motor extinction involve mainly the visualassociation cortex, the inferior parietal lobe, and thesuperior temporal gyrus, respectively. These anatom-ical findings, which considerably overlap with theneural underpinnings of VN, help explain thefrequent co-occurrence of both VN and extinction.

Involvement of ophthalmologists and orthoptistsis of potential value, as ‘‘egocentric’’ visual neglect may occasionally coexist with, or masquerade as,a visual field defect,101 despite their distinct patho-physiology. Egocentric VN is characterized by in-attention toward the contralesional hemispace,

 which is independent of the direction of gaze, whereas HH is the actual loss of visual field of contralesional hemispace, which can be compen-sated by the head and eye movements. Scanning

behavior, which relies on spatial attention, serves asthe primary distinguishing factor. Patients with VNcan have profound difficulties in surveying andnavigating the visual scene in affected hemispaceand cannot consciously compensate for within theneglected hemispace. In contrast, HH poses a lesssignificant impact on the affected individual’sscanning behavior because spatial attention of oneside of the visuospatial world is retained, thereby allowing compensatory movement of the eye, head,and body towards the affected visual field. There-fore, behavioral assessment can be very useful in

differentiating these two disorders.In addition, VN, but not HH,   is commonly 

associated with visual extinction.49  Also, othermodalities such as somatosensory, auditory, andmotor neglect may coexist with VN, whereas HH isstrictly a visual deficit. Visual evoked potentials(VEPs) are increasingly used to differentiate VNand HH. Patients with VN usually have a nearnormal response on VEPs,160  whereas patients withHH commonly demonstrate marked disparity   be-tween the normal and affected hemifields.90 Inaddition, various alternative methods hav e been

proposed to distinguish these conditions93

(Table4). Such differentiation may be difficult, as they may overlap or be compounded by a condition calledhemianopia anosognosia, which is characterized by unawareness of the visual field loss.18,29

D. SUMMARY AND RECOMMENDATIONS FOR THE

 ASSESSMENT OF VISUAL NEGLECT

Despite a lack of consensus regarding neglect assessment, we recommend that health professionalsroutinely assess for personal, peripersonal, and

extrapersonal neglect, and, more importantly, the

level of independence in activities of daily living. Werecommend that the star cancellation test and theline bisection test should form part of the routinecognitive assessment battery. This approach is best coupled with the Catherine Bergego Scale andcontinuous clinical observation via multidisciplinary teamwork. Application of virtual reality--based assess-ment may be of potential value in future practice.In addition, it is imperative to distinguish VN fromother visual problems, especially homonymoushemianopia.

 VII. Rehabilitation of Visual Neglect 

There is an extensive body of literature concern-ing the effectiveness of the rehabilitation of VN. Inorder to minimize the selection bias and enhancethe level of evidence, we have, therefore, centered

analysis on high-quality literature such as systematicreviews and randomized controlled trials. Relevant evidence3,20,50,82,103,106,136,140 regarding the effec-tiveness of rehabilitation is summarized in Table 6.

 A. CURRENT CLINICAL PRACTICE

Clinical guidelines77,154 recommend a multidisci-plinary approach in managing cognitive impairment including visual neglect but there is no clear andspecific guidance on the choice of interventions. Asa result, heterogeneous interventions are applied by different institutions, depending on the local

guidelines, health professionals’ clinical experience,and clinical resources.

B. TYPES OF REHABILITATION

Interventions include utilizing remaining intact brain function (compensation), adaptation to theimpairment by using prosthetic devices or environ-mental modification (substitution), or retraining of the impaired function (restitution).92  Althoughcertain interventions involve more than one mech-anism, some fall outside this classification. Wesummarize available interventions (Table 5) and

their effectiveness (Table 6). Traditionally, rehabil-itation focused upon the compensatory and sub-stitutive interventions as the brain was thought tohave limited regenerative ability. However, recent evidence suggests that the brain has the ability to reorganize   and reconstruct following damage(neuroplasticity ),22,78,91,112 suggesting the possibility of success of restitutive rehabilitation.

1. Compensatory 

 Visuospatial perception relies on intact internalsubconscious computation of the outer world

along with feedback from different sensory 

124 Surv Ophthalmol 56 (2) March--April 2011   TING ET AL

Page 12: Visual Neglect

7/23/2019 Visual Neglect

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/visual-neglect 12/21

modalities.19,75,125,127,175 The compensatory, bot-tom--up approach of rehabilitation focuses on themodulation of the afferent inputs towards thecentral internalized process of the inner mind by utilizing the remaining intact brain function and

 various afferent/sensory modalities, including vi-sual, tactile, vestibular, extraocular, and neck muscleproprioception. This type of intervention does not address the underlying lack of self-awareness of thedeficit by the affected individuals, resulting in only short-lived improvement. Some interventions, suchas feedback training and r-TMS, may involve morethan one mechanism (Table 5).

2. Substitutive

This form of rehabilitation involves neither mod-

ulation of afferent inputs nor enhancement of self-awareness, but rather adaptation to the underlying

 visual neglect with the aid of rehabilitative measuressuch as prism adaptation, visual scanning therapy,and environmental modification. These methodspotentially impact VN more than pure compensatory interventions, as the improvement of VN relies onboth adaptation and subconscious activ e learning,taking advantage of neuroplasticity.145  Also, theaffected individuals may sometimes benefit from‘‘substitutive’’ measures rather than ‘‘restitutive’’methods as they are so severely affected that the

underlying deficit cannot be appreciated (Table 5).

3. Restitutive

This form of rehabilitation focuses on retrainingof the impaired function92 and the modulation of the internalized perception of the outer world,

rather than altering the afferent inputs. This may beachieved by increasing self-awareness with mentalimagery, transcranial magnetic stimulation, virtualreality--based space remapping, and, potentially,feedback training (Table 5).

C. CURRENT EVIDENCE ON THE EFFECTIVENESS

OF REHABILITATION

 As a result of our literature search, eight systematic reviews3,20,50,82,103,106,136,140 concerningthe effectiveness of rehabilitation were identified;they are summarized in Table 6.

 Visual scanning therapy (VST) and prism adapta-tion are the two best-studied neglect rehabilitativeinterventions, probably on account of the ease of administration, cost-effectiveness,   and easily repro-ducible positive results. Jutai et al,82 Luaute et al,106

and Pizzamiglio et al136 recommend the use of VST inpatients with VN. Generalization of task-specific effectst oa f  unctional level hasbeen documented by Geusgenet al.50 However, the role of additional passive sensory stimulationin the context of VST is inconclusive.136,153

There is strong evidence demonstrating that prismadaptation helps rehabilitate VN.3,106 Long-term

beneficial effects can be achieved via a short period

TABLE 4

How to Distinguish Visual Neglect from Homonymous Hemianopia 

Criteria Visual neglect Homonymous Hemianopia

 Visual behavior Lack of attention to contralesionalhemispace, independent of thedirection of gaze

Loss of visual field of contralesionalhemispace, with respect to theposition of the head and eyes

 Awareness of deficit Usually unaware of own deficit Usually retains awareness to acertain extent 

Location of lesion Usually within the territory of themiddle cerebral artery 

Usually within the territory of theposterior cerebral artery 

Uni- vs multi-modal deficit May associate with other modalitieslike auditory, tactile and motor

Strictly confined to visual modality 

Extinction Frequently associated49 Less commonly associatedDrawing from memory Commonly leave out the details on

the affected hemispaceNormal and symmetrical

 Visuospatial disorder More commonly affected Usually not affectedLine bisection Ipsilesional deviation Usually contralesional deviation122

 Attentional ‘‘cueing’’ strategies Presence of cueing may ameliorateneglect (usually transiently)

Presence of cueing does not modulate the disorder

Specific perimetric techniques Difficulty in maintaining centralfixation

Good control of central fixation

 VEP and eye movement registration Near normal response to stimulationon both sides but prolongedlatency may be demonstrated onthe affected side160

Marked disparity between thenormal and affected hemifield90

 VEP 5  visual evoked potentials.This table is modified from Kerkhoff et al.93

 VISUAL NEGLECT FOLLOWING STROKE   125

Page 13: Visual Neglect

7/23/2019 Visual Neglect

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/visual-neglect 13/21

of prism adaptation42 and ongoing improvement hasbeen observed e ven 5 weeks after the active phase of rehabilitation.45 Such long-term effects do not occur

 with passive Fresnel prism exposure,118 demonstrat-ing that improvement relies on more than just modulating the afferent inputs, and requires thefeedback loop of rewarded motor activity.

Nonetheless, the vast majority of the prism adapta-

tion studies are case reports or small case series. A 

recent single-blind randomized controlled pilot t rialof 34 neglect patients conducted by Turton et al168 hasshown that prism adaptation does not provide any beneficial effect on the functional performance of the

 VN patients, as measured by CBS and BIT, despite theimprovement of pointing behavior. The inconsistent effect of prism adaptation observed across different studies may be influenced by the inconsistency of 

prism adaptation methods used, the disparity of 

TABLE 5

Summary of Underlying Rationale/Concepts for Various Types of Rehabilitation for Unilateral Neglect 

Types Interventions Brief Descriptions/Proposed Rationale

Compensation    Visual scanning therapy 174,178 Re-develop an organized scanning pattern by learningsystematic right-to-left search

Optokinetic stimulation124 Induce left pursuit movement with the aid of leftward

moving background targetsLimb activation143 Improve attention on the neglected hemifield by moving

the contralesional limb in the neglected hemispaceTraining with ‘‘cueing’’174  Activate attention on the neglected side with ‘‘cueing’’

stimuli (visual, auditory or tactile)Neck muscle vibration86,88,152 Re-center the spatial egocentric frame of reference by 

modifying the afferent neck proprioceptive inputsrelative to the position of the head to the trunk

Trunk rotation44,88 Re-center the spatial egocentric frame of reference by modifying the afferent information relative to theposition of the head to the trunk

Caloric stimulation147 Induce nystagmus towards the affected side with coldcontralesional or warm ipsilesional caloric stimulation

Eye patches44,179 Increase leftward saccades by occluding the unaffectedright hemifield

Fresnel prisms148 Shift the affected visual hemifield towards the unaffectedipsilesional egocentric frame of reference

Sustained attention training144 Improves spatial attention by generating general arousalsystem

Substitution    Prism adaptation3,118,146 Recalibrate the midline of the egocentric reference frameby using prism adaptation and successive visuo-motoractions; an after-effect of leftward shift towards theneglected hemifield following prism removal may improve VN

Diminished backgroundpattern and foreground clutter35

Minimize the required visual attention during visual scenenavigation by reducing the background and foregroundenvironmental distracters

Restitution    Pharmacological treatment 107 Improve general non-spatial attention by using dopamineand norepinephrine agonists

Mental imagery 158

Improve representational (imagery) neglect by using visual or movement imagery  VR space remapping1 Remap the egocentric reference frame towards the

affected hemifield by using VR systemCompensation and 

Restitution Feedback training159 Increase patients’ self awareness (restitution) with

feedback sessions (video, mirror, verbal, visuomotor)following certain tasks, point out their neglect behavior,and teach them the ‘‘compensatory’’ strategies toimprove functional performance

r-TMS/direct polarization99,128,129

Disrupt the integrated neural activity of the intact side,dampen the attention to the ipsilesional side andthereby restore the orientation balance between bothhemispheres (compensation),99,128 or alternativel y,induce the process of neuroplasticity (restitution)129

Unknown    Music therapy 53 Stimulate cognitive function via sensory and emotional

stimulation

 VR 5 virtual reality; r-TMS   5 repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation.

126 Surv Ophthalmol 56 (2) March--April 2011   TING ET AL

Page 14: Visual Neglect

7/23/2019 Visual Neglect

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/visual-neglect 14/21

TABLE 6

Synthesis of Reviews of Evidence of Effect of Interventions for Visual Neglect 3,20,50,82,103,106,136

Reviews Included Studies Aims Key Findings

 Arene 20073 31 studies—mixed trials of different quality 

 Assess mainly the effectivenessof passive sensory stimulation, prism

adaptation, visual scanningtherapy, andpharmacological treatment 

➢ Passive sensory stimula-tion only confers short-lived improvement 

Prism adaptation demon-strated long term func-tional gain

➢  Visual scanning therapy has no significant impacton overall function

Riggs 2007140 27 studies—11 RCTs, 3 non-RCTs, 13 case studies

 Assess the effectiveness of  visuoperceptual therapy,prism adaptation, eyepatches, and visuomotorfeedback

➢ Improvement was ob-served with hemifield eyepatching, prism adapta-tion, and visuomotorfeedback, mainly by means of standardizedneglect test like line bi-section tests and cancel-lation tests

Geusgen 200750

7 studies—1 RCT, 3 singlesubject, 2 pre-test post-test,1 case control

Evaluate the ‘‘transfer’’ of theeffect of visual scanningstrategies from trained tasksto daily tasks or daily living

➢  Almost all confer positiveimpact on daily tasks ordaily living but three of seven were not statisticallytested

Bowen 200720 12 studies—all RCTs Assess overall effectiveness of  cognitive rehabilitation for visual neglect onindependence of activitiesof daily living

➢ Only  spatiomotor cue-ing83 confers statistically significant improvement on performance instandardized neglect assessment 

➢ None showed statisticalsignificance in functionalperformance

Lincoln 2006103 25 studies—12 RCTs of aboveand 13 CCTs

 Assess overall effectiveness of  various interventionsrelating to visual neglect 

➢ Rehabilitation observedslight immediate andpersisting improvement on standardised neglect test 

➢ Effect on functioning wasstatistically insignificant,for either immediate orpersisting effects

Page 15: Visual Neglect

7/23/2019 Visual Neglect

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/visual-neglect 15/21

Table 6 (continued )

Reviews Included Studies Aims Key Findings

Luaute 2006106 54 studies -- RCTs, controlledtrials, crossover, case-control, single-case, andcase reports

 Assess the effectiveness of  various interventionsrelating to visual neglect 

➢ Improvement has beenobserved following VST,mental imagery and videofeedback training

➢ TR and NMV is partially effective when coupled

 with visual scanningtherapy 

➢ Fresnel prism and passivesensory stimulation donot afford positive func-tional impact 

Pizzamiglio2006136

12 studies— group studies andsingle cases

 Assess the effectiveness of  visual scanning therapy withand without additionalpassive sensory stimulation

➢  VST serves as a usefulneglect rehabilitativeintervention

➢ No beneficial effect gained with additionalperipheral sensory stimulation

 Jutai 20038232 studies, including RCTs,

crossover, cohorts, andsingle group designs

Investigate effectiveness of a variety of interventionsrelating to visual neglect 

➢ There is strong evidencethat rehabilitation specif-ically targeting visual ne-glect and visual scanningimproves functionalperformance

➢ There is moderate evi-dence that Fresnel prismsand hemifield patchingimprove performance instandardized neglect tests

➢ There is limited evidencein favor of TENS, limbactivation, dopamine ag-onist therapy, caloricstimulation improving vi-

sual neglect CCT   5 controlled clinical trial; NMV   5 neck muscle vibration; RCT   5 randomized controlled trial; TR   5  trunk rotation; VST

transcutaneous electrical nerve stimulation.

Page 16: Visual Neglect

7/23/2019 Visual Neglect

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/visual-neglect 16/21

number and duration of therapeutic sessions in-stituted, and the difference in outcome measuresapplied. This study did not address the anatomicalregions involved in their neglect patients. This isimportant because some studies have suggested that any potential beneficial effects  of prism adaptationmay be lesion site--specific.151,155

The effectiveness of rehabilitation based onpassive sensory stimulation, such as eye patches,Fresnel prism, trunk rotation, neck muscle vibra-tion, limb activation, vestibular stimulation, andoptokinetic stimulation, remain inconclusive orunsatisfactory.3,106

 Although it seems logical that longer-term benefitsmay be achieved with restitutive rehabilitation, theevidence is limited. Within the context of restitutiverehabilitation, Luauteet al106considermentalimagery and feedback training, as Grade B interventions,135

largely because theevidence of benefitisonlybasedon

a case study of two patients.158 The efficacy of pharmacological treatment (dopamine and norad-renergic agonists), transcranial magnetic   stimula-tion128 or direct current polarization,99  virtualreality--based space remapping,1 and music therapy mustbe deemed inconclusive.158Thereareconflictingresults from single case reports of the use of dopamineand norepinephrine agonists to improve generalattention.3,10,72 Virtual reality--based space remappinghas shown to be of potential value, but has beenrestricted to patients who hav e   normal inferiorparietal/superior temporal lobes.1

The systemat ic reviews of Bowen et al20 andLincoln et al103 have concluded that none of theinterventions lead to a positive impact on functionalperformance. They excluded the majority of theresearch trials because of unsatisfactory levels of evidence, highlighting the pressing need for highquality research.

D. SUMMARY AND RECOMMENDATION FOR 

REHABILITATION OF VISUAL NEGLECT

The overall efficacy of rehabilitation remainsuncertain. Based on the best available evidence, wefeel that visual scanning therapy and prism adapta-tion warrant consideration. In our experience,education and training of family and close friendsto understand the nature of VN and how to assist VNpatients on a daily basis are equally important.

 VIII. Prognosis

 A few studies have observed spontaneous recovery in patients with mild to moderate visual neglect,especially during the acute phase,41 and recov ery 

has been seen up to 90 days post-stroke.

126

Unresolved visual neglect predicts poorer functionaland   rehabilitation outcome in post-stroke pa-tients.79,89,163,165 Longer-term follow-up is required,as there has been no systematic reported evaluationof outcome beyond 1 year.79

IX. Conclusion VN is a common, but frequently overlooked,

condition. Greater awareness and understanding of this condition should improve the quality of life of post-stroke patients. Detection is by combination of pen-and-paper tests, behavioral assessment tools,clinical observation and history taking and, poten-tially, virtual reality assessment. Although there islack of robust evidence of the efficacy of VNrehabilitation, visual scanning therapy and prismadaptation warrant consideration. There is a press-ing need for well designed, high quality research,

particularly randomized controlled trials, todemonstrate the clinical efficacy of current rehabil-itation strategies. Increasingly sophisticated neuro-imaging tools, including magnetic resonancescanning coupled with detailed clinical observationand neuro-ophthalmic examination, will improveunderstanding of the nature of VN, enablinga deeper appreciation of how an individual with

 VN recognizes, organizes, and orients ‘‘self’’ inrelation to the external environment.

 X. Method of Literature Search

Multiple electronic databases (EMBASE, Cochranelibrary, and Medline) were searched, reference lists

 were hand-searched, and experts in the field werecontacted to identify peer-reviewed literature con-cerning visual neglect. Key literature which addressesthe functional impact, neuroanatomical causes,assessment, and treatment of visual neglect wasidentified. Certain keywords were used during theliterature search, including   visual neglect, unilateral neglect, visual inattention, and  hemispatial neglect .

 XI. DisclosureThe authors reported no proprietary or commer-

cial interest in any product mentioned or concept discussed in this article. Alex Pollock is employed by the Nursing Midwifery and Allied Health Profes-sionals (NMAHP) Research Unit, which is funded by the Scottish Government’s Chief Scientist’s Office.

References

1. Ansuini C, Pierno AC, Lusher D, et al. Virtual reality applications for the remapping of space in neglect patients.

Restor Neurol Neurosci. 2006;24(4--6):431--41

 VISUAL NEGLECT FOLLOWING STROKE   129

Page 17: Visual Neglect

7/23/2019 Visual Neglect

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/visual-neglect 17/21

2. Appelros P, Nydevik I, Karlsson GM, et al. Assessingunilateral neglect: shortcomings of standard test methods.Disabil Rehabil. 2003;25(9):473--9

3. Arene NU, Hillis AI. Rehabilitation of unilateral spatialneglect and neuroimaging. Euro Medicophys. 2007;43:255--69

4. Azouvi P, Bartolomeo P, Beis JM, et al. A battery of tests forthe quantitative assessment of unilateral neglect. RestorNeurol Neurosci. 2006;24(4--6):273--85

5. Azouvi P, Olivier S, Montety G, et al. Behavioral assessment of unilateral neglect: study of the psychometric propertiesof the Catherine Bergego Scale. Arch Phys Med Rehabil.2003;84(1):51--7

6. Azouvi P, Samuel C, Louis-Dreyfus, et al. Sensitivity of clinical and behavioural test of spatial neglect after right hemisphere stroke. J Neurology Neurosurgery Psychiatry.2002;73:160--6

7. Azouvi P, Marchel F, Samuel C, et al. Functional conse-quences and awareness of unilateral neglect: study of anevaluation scale. Neuropsychol Rehabil. 1996;6:133--50

8. Bailey MJ, Riddoch MJ, Crome P. Evaluation of a test battery for hemineglect in elderly stroke patients for use by therapists in clinical practice. NeuroRehabilitation. 2000;14:139--50

9. Balint R. Seelenlahmung des "Schauens,"optische Ataxie,

raumliche Storung der Aufmerksamkeit. Monatschrift furPsychiatrie und Neurologie. 1909;25:51--181

10. Barrett AM, Crucian GP, Schwartz RL, et al. Adverseeffect of dopamine agonist therapy in a patient withmotor-intentional neglect. Arch Phys Med Rehabil. 1999;80:600--3

11. Bartolomeo P, Thiebaut SM, Doricchi F. Left unilateralneglect as a disconnection syndrome. Cereb Cortex. 2007;17(11):2479--90

12. Bartolomeo P, Chokron S. Left unilateral neglect or right hyperattention? Neurology. 1999;53:2023--7

13. Beis JM, Keller C, Morin N, et al. Right spatial neglect afterleft hemisphere stroke: qualitative and quantitative study.Neurology. 2004;63:1600--5

14. Berti A, Bottini G, Gandola M, et al. Shared corticalanatomy for motor awareness and motor control. Science.

2005;309:488--9115. Bird CM, Malhotra P, Parton A, et al. Visual neglect afterright posterior cerebral artery infarction. J Neurol Neuro-surg Psychiatry. 2006;77:1008--12

16. Bisiach E. Extinction and neglect: same or different?. InPaillard J (ed) Brain and Space. Oxford, Oxford University Press, 1991, pp 251--7

17. Bisiach E, Perani D, Vallar G, et al. Unilateral neglect:personal and extra-personal. Neuropsychologia. 1986;24:759--67

18. Bisiach E, Vallar G, Perani D, et al. Unawareness of diseasefollowing lesions of the right hemisphere: anosognosia forhemiplegia and anosognosia for hemianopia. Neuropsy-chologia. 1986;24:471--82

19. Blouin J, Vercher JL, Gauthier GM, et al. Perception of passive whole-body rotations in the absence of neckand body proprioception. J Neurophysiol. 1995;74(5):2216--9

20. Bowen A, Lincoln NB. Cognitive rehabilitation for spatialneglect following stroke. Cochrane Database of SystematicReviews;. Issue 2. Art. No.:CD003586. DIU:10.1002/14651858.CD003586.pub2.

21. Bowen A, McKenna K, Tallis RC. Reasons for variability inthe reported rate of occurrence of unilateral spatial neglect after stroke. Stroke. 1999;30:1196--202

22. Buonomano DV, Merzenich MM. Cortical plasticity: fromsynapses to maps. Ann Rev Neurosci. 1998;21:149--86

23. Butler SH, Gilchrist ID, Ludwig JH, et al. Impairments of oculomotor control in a patient with a right temporo-parietal lesion. Cogn Neuropsychol. 2006;23(6):990--9

24. Buxbaum LJ, Ferraro MK, Veramonti T, et al. Hemispatialneglect: subtypes, neuroanatomy and disability. Neurology.2004;62:749--56

25. Campos EC, Bolzani R, Schiavi C, et al. Further evidencefor the role of proprioception in space perception. DocOphthalmol. 1989;72:155--60

26. Campos EC, Chiesi C, Bolanzi R. Abnormal spatiallocalization in patients with herpes zoster ophthalmicus:evidence for the presence of proprioceptive information.

 Arch Ophthalmol. 1986;104:1176--727. Canadian Best Practice Recommendations for Stroke Care

(2008 Update). CMAJ. 2008;179:E1--E9328. Caramazza A, Hillis AE. Spatial representation of words in

the brain implied by studies of a unilateral neglect patient.Nature. 1990;346(6281):267--9

29. Celesia GG, Brigell MG, Vaphiades MS. Hemianopicanosognosia. Neurology. 1997;49:88--97

30. Chiba Y, Nishihara K, Yamaguchi A, et al. Midpoint fixationtask: quantitative assessment of visual neglect. J ClinNeurosci. 2008;15(6):647--9

31. Clark VP. Responses to rare visual target and distractorstimuli using event-related fMRI.J. Neurophysiology. 2000;83:3133--9

32. Corbetta M, Shulman GL. Control of goal-directed andstimulus-driven attention in the brain. Nat Rev Neurosci.2002;3:201--5

33. Corbetta M, Kincade JM, Ollinger JM, et al. Voluntary orienting is dissociated from target detection in human

posterior parietal cortex. Nat Neurosci. 2000;3:292--734. Damasio AR, Tranel D, Rizzo M. Disorders of complex

 visual processing, in Mesulam MM (ed) Principles of Behavioral and Cognitive Neurology. New York, OxfordUniversity Press, 2nd ed 2000, pp 332--72

35. Das M, Bennett DM, Dutton GN. Visual attention as animportant visual function: an outline of manifestations,diagnosis and management of impaired visual attention. Br

 J Ophthalmol. 2007;91:1556--6036. Davidson’s Principles & Practice of Medicine, 20th edition.

London, Churchill Livingstone, 2006.37. Doricchi F, Thiebaut SM, Tomaiuolo F, et al. White matter

(dis)connections and gray matter (dys)functions in visualneglect: gaining insights into the brain networks of spatialawareness. Cortex. 2008;44(8):983--95

38. Downar J. A cortical network sensitive to stimulus salience

in a neutral behavioral context across multiple sensory modalities. J Neurophysiology. 2002;87:615--2039. Duncan PW, Zorowitz R, Bates B, et al. Management 

of adult stroke rehabilitation care: a clinical practiceguideline. Stroke. 2005;36:e100--4310.1161/01.STR.0000180861.54180.FF

40. Edwards DF, Hahn MG, Baum CM, et al. Screening patients with stroke for rehabilitation needs: validation of the post-stroke rehabilitation guidelines. Neurorehabil NeuralRepair. 2006;20:42--8

41. Farne A, Buxbaum L, Ferraro M, et al. Patterns of spontaneous recovery of neglect and associated disordersin acute right brain-damaged patients. J Neurol NeurosurgPsychiatry . 2004;75(10):1401--10

42. Farne A, Rossetti Y, Toniolo S, et al. Ameliorating neglect  with prism adaptation: visuo-manual and visuo-verbalmeasures. Neuropsychologia. 2002;40:718--29

43. Ferber S, Karnath HO: How to assess spatial neglect----linebisection or cancellation tests? J Clin Exp Neuropsychol.2001;23:599--607

44. Fong KN, Chan MK, Ng PP, et al. The effect of voluntary trunk rotation and half-field eye-patching for patients withunilateral neglect in stroke: a randomized controlled trial.Clin Rehabil. 2007;21:729--41

45. Frassinetti F, Angeli V, Meneghello F, et al. Long-lastingamelioration of visuospatial neglect by prism adaptation.Brain. 2002;125(Pt 3):608--23

46. Gaber TAZK. Evaluation of the Addenbrooke’s CognitiveExamination’s validity in a brain injury rehabilitationsetting. Brain Injury. 2008;22(7--8):589--93

47. Gainotti G, Messerli P, Tissot R. Qualitative analysis of unilateral spatial neglect in relation to laterality of cerebrallesions. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry. 1972;35:545--50

130 Surv Ophthalmol 56 (2) March--April 2011   TING ET AL

Page 18: Visual Neglect

7/23/2019 Visual Neglect

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/visual-neglect 18/21

48. Gauthier GM, Nommay D, Vercher JL. Ocular muscleproprioception and visual localization of targets in man.Brain. 1990;113(6):1857--71

49. Geeraerts S, Lafosse C, Vandenbussche E, et al. A psychophysical study of visual extinction: ipsilesionaldistractor interference with contralesional orientationthresholds in visual hemineglect patients. Neuropsycholo-gia. 2005;43(4):530--41

50. Geusgens CAV, Winkens I, van Heugten CM, et al.Occurrence and measurement of transfer in cognitiverehabilitation: a critical review. J Rehabil Med. 2007;39:425--39

51. Gialanella B, Mattioli F. Anosognosia and extrapersonalneglect as predictors of functional recovery followingright hemisphere stroke. Neuropsychol Rehabil. 1992;2:169--78

52. Giesbrecht B, Woldorff MG, Song AW, et al. Neuralmechanisms of top--down control during spatial andfeature attention. Neuroimage. 2003;19(3):496--512

53. Gilbertson S, Ischebeck W. Merging pathways: musictherapy in neurosurgical rehabilitation. Acta Neurochir-urgica (Suppl). 2002;79:41--2

54. Goldenberg G, Karnath HO. The neural basis of imitationis body part specific. J Neuroscience. 2006;26:6282--7

55. Golisz KM. Dynamic assessment and multicontext treat-

ment of unilateral neglect. Top Stroke Rehabil. 1998;5(1):11--28

56. Goodale MA, Milner AD. Separate visual pathways forperception and action. Trends Neurosci. 1992;15(1):20--5

57. Gottesman RF, Kleinman JT, Davis C, et al. Unilateralneglect is more severe and common in older patients

 with right hemispheric stroke. Neurology. 2008;71:1439--44

58. Grimsen C, Hildebrandt H, Fahle M. Dissociation of egocentric and allocentric coding of space in visual searchafter right middle cerebral artery stroke. Neuropsycholo-gia. 2008;46(3):902--14

59. Halligan PW, Marshall JC. Left neglect for near but not farspace in man. Nature. 1991;350:498--500

60. Halligan PW, Wilson B, Cockburn J. A short screening test for visual neglect in stroke patients. Int Disabil Stud. 1990;

12(3):95--961. Halligan PW, Marshall JC, Wade DT. Visuospatial neglect:underlying factors and test sensitivity. Lancet. 1989;2(8668):908--11

62. Hartman-Maeir A, Katz N. Validity of the BehavioralInattention Test (BIT): relationships with functional tasks.

 Am J Occup Ther. 1995;49(6):507--1663. Harvey M. Translation of [Psychic paralysis of gaze, optic

ataxia, and spatial disorder of attention] by RudolphBalint. Cogn Neuropsychol. 1995;12:261--82

64. Heilman KM, Valenstein E, Watson RT. Neglect and relateddisorders. Semin Neurol. 2000;20(4):463--70

65. Heilman KM, Valenstein E, Day A, et al. Frontal lobeneglect in monkeys. Neurology. 1995;45:1205--10

66. Heilman KM, Van Den Abell T. Right hemispheredominance for attention: the mechanism underlyinghemispheric asymmetries of inattention (neglect). Neurol-ogy. 1980;30:327--30

67. Hilgetag CC, Theoret H, Pascual-Leone A. Enhanced visuospatial attention ipsilateral to rTMS-induced, virtuallesions’ of human parietal cortex. Nat Neurosci. 2001;4:953--710.1098/rstb.1999.0482

68. Hillis AE, Chang S, Heidler-Gary J, et al. Neural correlatesof modality-specific spatial extinction. J Cogn Neurosci.2006;18(11):1889--98

69. Hillis AE. Rehabilitation of unilateral spatial neglect: new insights from magnetic resonance perfusion imaging. ArchPhys Med Rehabil. 2006;87(12 Suppl. 2):S43--9

70. Hillis AE, Newhart M, Heidler J, et al. Anatomy of spatialattention: insights from perfusion imaging and hemispatialneglect in acute stroke. J Neurosci. 2005;25(12):3161--7

71. Holmes G. Disturbances of visual orientation. Br JOphthalmol. 1918;2:449--68, 506--16.

72. Hurford P, Stringer AY, Jann B. Neuropharmacologictreatment of hemineglect: a case report comparingbromocriptine and methylphenidate. Arch Phys MedRehabil. 1998;79:346--9

73. Husain M, Nachev P. Space and the parietal cortex. TrendsCogn Sci. 2007;11(1):30--6

74. Husain M, Rorden C. Non-spatially lateralised mechanismsin hemispatial neglect. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2003;4:26--36

75. Husain M, Shapiro K, Martin J, et al. Abnormal temporaldynamics of visual attention in spatial neglect patients.Nature. 1997;385:154--6

76. Husain M, Kennard C. Visual neglect associated withfrontal lobe infarction. J Neurol. 1996;243(9):652--7

77. Intercollegiate Stroke Working Party. National clinicalguidelines for stroke, 3rd edition. London: Royal Collegeof Physicians (2008)

78. Interrante V, Kearney JK, Proffitt D, et al. Elucidatingfactors that can facilitate veridical spatial perception inimmersive virtual environments. Presence: Teleoperatorsand Virtual Environments. 2008;17(2):176--98

79. Jehkonen M, Ahonen JP, Dastidar P, et al. Visual neglect asa predictor of functional outcome one year after stroke.

 Acta Neurol Scand. 2000;101(3):195--20180. Jehkonen M, Ahonen JP, Dastidar P, et al. How to detect 

 visual neglect in acute stroke. Lancet. 1998;351(9104):

727--72881. Jepson R, Despain K, Keller DC. Unilateral neglect:

assessment in nursing practice. J Neurosci Nurs. 2008;40(3):142--9

82. Jutai JW, Bhogal SK, Foley NC, et al. Treatment of visualperceptual disorders post stroke. Top Stroke Rehabil. 2003;10(2):77--106

83. Kalra L, Perez I, Gupta S, et al. The influence of visualneglect on stroke rehabilitation. Stroke. 1997;28(7):1386--91

84. Karnath HO, Berger MF, Kuker W, et al. The anatomy of spatial neglect based on voxelwise statistical analysis: a study of 140 patients. Cereb Cortex. 2004;14:1164--72

85. Karnath HO, Ferber S, Himmelbach M. Spatial awareness isa function of the temporal not the posterior parietal lobe.Nature. 2001;411:950--3

86. Karnath HO. Transcutaneous electrical stimulation and vibration of neck muscles in neglect. Exp Brain Res. 1995;105(2):321--4

87. Karnath HO. Subjective body orientation in neglect andthe interactive contribution of neck muscle proprioceptionand vestibular stimulation. Brain. 1994;117(5):1001--12

88. Karnath HO, Christ K, Hartje W. Decrease of contralateralneglect by neck muscle vibration and spatial orientation of trunk midline. Brain. 1993;116:383--96

89. Katz N, Harman-Maier A, Ring HM, et al. Functionaldisability and rehabilitation outcome in right hemispheredamaged patients with and without unilateral spatialneglect. Arch Phys Med Rehabil. 1999;80:379--84

90. Kelly JP, Weiss AH. Comparison of pattern visual-evokedpotentials to perimetry in the detection of visual loss inchildren with optic pathway gliomas. J AAPOS. 2006;10(4):298--306

91. Kempermann G, Brandon EP, Gage FH. Environmentalstimulation of 129/Svj mice causes increased cell pro-liferation   and neurogenesis in the adult dentate gyrus.Curr Biol. 1998;8:939--42

92. Kerkhoff G. Neurovisual rehabilitation: recent develop-ments and future directions. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychia-try. 2000;68:691--706

93. Kerkhoff G, Schindler I. [Hemi-neglect versus hemianopia.Differential diagnosis]. Fortschr Neurol Psychiatr. 1997;65(6):278--89

94. Kim K, Kim J, Ku J, et al. A virtual reality assessment andtraining system for unilateral neglect. Cyberpsychol Behav.2004;7(6):742--9

95. Kim M, Na DL, Kim GM, et al. Ipsilesional neglect:behavioural and anatomical features. J Neurol NeurosurgPsychiatry. 1999;67(1):35--8

 VISUAL NEGLECT FOLLOWING STROKE   131

Page 19: Visual Neglect

7/23/2019 Visual Neglect

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/visual-neglect 19/21

96. Kinsbourne M. Orientational bias model of unilateralneglect: evidence from attentional gradients within hemi-space. In Robertson IH, Marshall JC (eds) UnilateralNeglect: Clinical and Experimental Studies. London,Lawrence Erlbaum, 1993, pp 63--86

97. Kinsella G, Packer S, Ng K, et al. Continuing issues inthe assessment of neglect. Neuropsychol Rehabil. 1995;5:239--58

98. Kleinman JT, Newhart M, Davis C, et al. Right hemispatialneglect: frequency and characterization following acute left hemisphere stroke. Brain Cogn. 2007;64(1):50--9

99. Ko MH, Han SH, Park SH, et al. Improvement of visualscanningafterDC brain polarizationof parietalcortexin strokepatients with spatial neglect. Neurosci Lett. 2008;448(2):171--4

100. Koch G, Oliveri M, Cheeran B, et al. Hyperexcitability of parietal-motor functional connections in the intact left-hemisphere of patients with neglect. Brain. 2008;131(12):3147--55

101. Kooistra CA, Heilman KM. Hemispatial visual inattentionmasquerading as hemianopia. Neurology. 1989;39:1125

102. Kosnik W, Winslow L, Kline D, et al. Visual changes in daily life throughout adulthood. J Gerontol. 1988;43:63--70

103. Lincoln NB, Bowen A. The need for randomised treatment studies in neglect research. Restor Neurol Neurosci. 2006;24:401--8

104. Lindell AB, Jalas MJ, Tenovuo O, et al. Clinical assessment of hemispatial neglect: evaluation of different measuresand dimensions. Clin Neuropsychol. 2007;21(3):479--97

105. Linden T, Samuelsson H, Skoog I, et al. Visual neglect andcognitive impairment in elderly patients late after stroke.

 Acta Neurol Scand. 2005;111(3):163--8106. Luaute J, Halligan P, Rode G, et al. Visuospatial neglect:

a systematic review of current interventions and theireffectiveness. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2006;30:961--82

107. Malhotra P, Parton A, Greenwood R, et al. Noradrenergicmodulation of space exploration in visual neglect. AnnNeurol. 2006;59:186--90

108. Manly T, Dove A, Blows S, et al. Assessment of unilateralspatial neglect: scoring star cancellation performance from

 video recordings----method, reliability, benefits, and nor-mative data. Neuropsychology. 2009;23(4):519--28

109. Marsh E, Hillis AE. Dissociation between egocentric andallocentric visuospatial and tactile neglect in acute stroke.Cortex. 2008;44(9):1215--20

110. Marsh NV, Kersel DA. Screening tests for visual neglect following stroke. Neuropsychol Rehabil. 1993;3:245--57

111. McGlone J, Losier BJ, Black SE. Are there sex differences inhemispatial visual neglect after unilateral stroke? Neuro-psychiatry Neuropsychol Behav Neurol. 1997;10(2):125--34

112. McMillan TM, Robertson IH, Wilson BA. Neurogenesisafter brain injury: implications for neurorehabilitation.Neuropsychol Rehabil. 1999;9:129--33

113. Medina J, Kannan V, Pawlak MA, et al. Neural substrates of  visuospatial processing in distinct reference frames: evi-dence from unilateral spatial neglect. J Cogn Neurosci.2009;21(11):2073--84

114. Menon A, Korner-Bitensky N, Wood-Dauphinee S, et al. Assessment of unilateral spatial neglect post stroke inCanadian acute care hospitals: are we neglecting neglect?Clin Rehabil. 2006;20(7):623--34

115. Menon A, Korner-Bitensky N. Evaluating unilateral spatialneglect poststroke: working your way through the maze of assessment choices. Top Stroke Rehabil. 2004;11(3):41--66

116. Mesulam MM. Functional anatomy of attention andneglect: from neurons to networks, in Karnath HO, Milner

 AD, Vallar G (eds) The cognitive and neural bases of spatialneglect. New York, Oxford University Press, 2002, pp 33--45

117. Mesulam MM. Spatial attention and neglect: parietal,frontal and cingulate contributions to the mental repre-sentation and attentional targeting of salient extrapersonalevents. Phil Trans R Soc Lond B. 1999;354:1325--46

118. Michel C, Pisella L, Halligan PW, et al. Simulating unilateralneglect in normals using prism adaptation: implications fortheory. Neuropsychologia. 2003;41(1):25--39

119. Milner AD, Goodale MA. The Visual Brain in Action.Oxford, Oxford University Press, 2nd ed. 2006

120. Milner AD, Goodale MA. The Visual Brain in Action.Oxford, Oxford University Press, 1995

121. Mioshi E, Dawson K, Mitchell J, et al. Addenbrooke’sCognitive Examination Revised (ACE--R): a brief cognitivetest battery for dementia screening. Int J Geriatr Psychiatry.2006;21(11):1078--85

122. Mitra AR, Abegg M, Viswanathan J, et al. Line bisection insimulated homonymous hemianopia. Neuropsychologia.2010;48(6):1742--9

123. Mort DJ, Malhotra P, Mannan SK, et al. The anatomy of  visual neglect. Brain. 2003;126(9):1986--97

124. Na DL, Son Y, Kim CH, et al. Effect of background motionon line bisection performance in normal subjects. Cortex.2002;38:787--96

125. Nathan PW, Smith M, Deacon P. Vestibulospinal, reticulo-spinal and descending propriospinal nerve fibres in man.Brain. 1996;119:1809--33

126. Nigel VM, Denyse AK. Screening tests for visual neglect following stroke. Neuropsychol Rehabil. 1993;3(3):245--57

127. Nishiike S, Guldin WO, Baurle J. Corticofugal connectionsbetween the cerebral cortex and vestibular nuclei in rat.

 J Comp Neurol. 2000;420:363--72

128. Oliveri M, Bisiach E, Brighina F, et al. rTMS of theunaffected hemisphere transiently reduces contralesional

 visuospatial hemineglect. Neurology. 2001;57:1338--40129. O’Malley MK, Ro T, Levin H. Assessing and inducing

neuroplasticity with transcranial magnetic stimulation androbotics for motor function. Arch Phys Med Rehabil. 2006;87(12):59--66

130. Pandya DN, Van Hoesen GW, Mesulam MM. Efferent connections of the cingulated gyrus in the rhesus monkey.Exp Brain Res. 42:319--330, 198

131. Park KC, Lee BH, Kim EJ, et al. Deafferentation—disconnection neglect induced by posterior cerebral artery infarction. Neurology. 2006;66:56--61

132. Pearce JM. Gerstmann’s syndrome. J Neurol NeurosurgPsychiatry. 1996;61(1):56

133. Perenin MT, Vighetto A. Optic ataxia: a specific disruption

in visuomotor mechanism. I. Different aspects of the deficit in reaching for objects. Brain. 1988;111:643--74134. Perry RJ, Zeki S. The neurology of saccades and covert 

shifts in spatial attention: an event related fMRI study.Brain. 2000;123:2273--88

135. Philips B, Ball C, Sacket D, et al. Oxford centre forevidence-based medicine levels of evidence (May 2001).

 www.cebm.net/index.aspx?o51047136. Pizzamiglio L, Guariglia C, Antonucci G, et al. Develop-

ment of a rehabilitative program for unilateral neglect.Restor Neurol Nerosci. 2006;24(4--6):337--45

137. Plummer P, Morris ME, Dunai J. Assessment of unilateralneglect. Phys Ther. 2003;83:732--40

138. Qiang W, Sonoda S, Suzuki M, et al. Reliability and validity of   a wheelchair collision test for screening behavioralassessment of unilateral neglect after stroke. Am J PhysMed Rehabil. 2005;84(3):161--6

139. Rafal RD. Neglect [Review]. Curr Opin Neurobiol. 1994;4:231--6

140. Riggs RV, Andrews K, Roberts P, et al. Visual deficit interventions in adult stroke and brain injury. Am J PhysMed Rehabil. 2007;86:853--60

141. Ringman JM, Saver JL, Woolson RF, et al. Frequency, riskfactors, anatomy, and course of unilateral neglect in anacute stroke cohort. Neurology. 2004;63(3):468--74

142. Rizzo M, Vecera SP. Psychoanatomical substrates of Balint’s syndrome. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry. 2002;72:162--78

143. Robertson IH, MacMillan TM, MacLeod E, et al. Re-habilitation by limb activation training reduces left-sidedmotor impairment in unilateral neglect patients: a single-blind randomised control trial. Neuropsychol Rehabil.2002;12:439--54

132 Surv Ophthalmol 56 (2) March--April 2011   TING ET AL

Page 20: Visual Neglect

7/23/2019 Visual Neglect

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/visual-neglect 20/21

144. Robertson IH, Tegner R, Tham K, et al. Sustained attentiontraining forunilateral neglect: theoretical and rehabilitationimplications. J Clin Exper Neuropsychol. 1995;17:416--30

145. Rode G, Pisella L, Rossetti Y, et al. Bottom--up transfer of sensory-motor plasticity to recovery of spatial cognition:

 visuomotor adaptation and spatial neglect. Prog Brain Res.2003;142:273--87

146. Rode G, Rossetti Y, Li L, et al. Improvement of mentalimagery after prism exposure in neglect a case study. Behav Neurol. 1998;11:251--8

147. Rode G, Perenin MT. Temporary remission of representa-tional hemineglect through vestibular stimulation. Neuro-Report. 1994;5:869--72

148. Rossi PW, Kheyfets S, Reding MJ. Fresnel prisms improve visual perception in stroke patients with homonymous hemi-anopia or unilateralvisual neglect. Neurology. 1990;40:1597--9

149. Russell C. Attention modulates the visual field in healthy observersand parietalpatients. NeuroReport.2004;15:2189--93

150. Saphiro K, Hillstrom A, Husain M. Control of visuotempo-ral attention by inferior parietal and superior temporalcortex. Curr Biol. 2002;12:1320--5

151. Sarri M, Greenwood R, Kalra L, et al. Prism adaptationaftereffects in stroke patients with spatial neglect: patho-logical effects on subjective straight ahead but not visualopen-loop pointing. Neuropsychologia. 2007;46:1069--80

152. Schindler I. Neck muscle vibration induces lasting recovery inspatial neglect. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry. 2002;73:412--9

153. Schroder A, Wist ER, Homberg V. TENS and optokineticstimulation in neglect therapy after cerebrovascular acci-dent: a randomized controlled study. Eur J Neurol. 2008;15(9):922--7

154. Scottish Intercollegiate Guidelines Network (SIGN). Man-agement of patients with stroke: rehabilitation, preventionand management of complications, and discharge plan-ning. Edinburgh: SIGN; 2010. (SIGN publication no. 118).

 Available from url: http://www.sign.ac.uk155. Serino A, Bonifazi S, Pierfederici L, et al. Neglect treatment 

by prism adaptation: what recovers and for how long.Neuropsychol Rehabil. 2007;17:756--87

156. Singh-Curry V, Husain M. The functional role of theinferior parietal lobe in the dorsal and ventral stream

dichotomy. Neuropsychologia. 2009;47:1434--48157. Slagter HA, Giesbrecht B, Kok A, et al. fMRI evidence forboth generalized and specialized components of atten-tional control. Brain Res. 2007;1177:90--102

158. Smania N, Bazoli F, Piva D, et al. Visuomotor imagery andrehabilitation of neglect. Arch Phys Med Rehabil. 1997;78:430--6

159. Soderback I, Bengtsson I, Ginsburg E, et al. Video feedbackin occupational therapy: its effects in patients with neglect syndrome. Arch Phys Med Rehabil. 1992;73:1140--6

160. Spinelli D, Burr DC, Morrone MC. Spatial neglect isassociated with increased latencies of visual evokedpotentials. Vis Neurosci. 1994;11(5):909--18

161. Steinbach MJ, Smith DR. Spatial localization after strabis-mus surgery: evidence for inflow. Science. 1981;213:1407--9

162. Stone SP, Halligan PW, Greenwood RJ. The incidence of neglect phenomena and related disorders in patients with

an acute right or left hemisphere stroke. Age Aging. 1993;22:46--52

163. Stone SP, Patel P, Greenwood RJ. Selection of acute strokepatients for treatment of visual neglect. J Neurol NeurosurgPsychiatry. 1993;56(5):463--6

164. Sunderland A, Walker CM, Walker MF. Action errors anddressing disability after stroke: an ecological approach toneuropsychological assessment and intervention. Neuro-psychol Rehabil. 2006;16:666--83

165. Sunderland A, Wade D, Langton-Hewer R. The naturalhistory of visual neglect after stroke: indications from twoassessment methods. Int Disabil Stud. 1987;9:55--61

166. Tanaka T, Sugihara S, Nara H, et al. A preliminary study of clinical assessment of left unilateral spatial neglect usinga head mounted display system (HMD) in rehabilitationengineering technology. J Neuroeng Rehabil. 2005;2:31--40

167. Toglia JP. A dynamic interactional model to cognitiverehabilitation, in Katz N (ed) Cognition and Occupation inRehabilitation: Cognitive Models for Intervention inOccupational Therapy. Bethesda, MD, American Occupa-tional Therapy Association, 1998, pp 5--50

168. Turton AJ, O’Leary K, Gabb J, et al. A single blindedrandomised controlled pilot trial of prism adaptation forimproving self-care in stroke patients with neglect. Neuro-psychol Rehabil. 2010;20(2):180--96

169. Vallar G. Spatial hemineglect in humans. Trends Cogn Sci.1998;2:87--97

170. Vallar G, Perani D. The anatomy of unilateral neglect afterright-hemisphere stroke lesions: a clinical/CT-correlationstudy in man. Neuropsychologia. 1986;24:609--22

171. Ventre-Dominey J, Dominey PF, Sindou M. Extraocularproprioception is required for spatial localization in man.NeuroReport. 1996;7:1531--5

172. Vuilleumier PO, Rafal RD. A systematic study of visualextinction between- and within-field deficits of attention inhemifield neglect. Brain. 2000;123:1263--9

173. VuilleumierPO, ValenzaN, MayerE, etal. Near andfar visualspace in unilateral neglect. Ann Neurol. 1998;43:406--10

174. Weinberg J, Diller L, Gordon WA, et al. Visual scanningtraining effect on reading-related tasks in acquired right brain damage. Arch Phys Med Rehabil. 1977;58:479--86

175. Weir CR. Spatial localisation: does extraocular muscleproprioception play a role? Graefes Arch Clin ExpOphthalmol. 2000;238:868--73

176. WilsonBA, CockbrunJ, Halligan PW. Behavioural InattentionTest. Titchfield, UK, Thames Valley Test Company Ltd, 1987

177. Wolpert I. Die Simultanagnosie Storung der Gesamtauffas-sung. Z ges Neurol Psychiatry. 1924;93:397--415

178. Young GC, Collins D, Hren M. Effect of pairing scanningtraining with block design training with block designtraining in the remediation of perceptual problems in left hemiplegics. J Clin Neuropsychol. 1983;5:201--12

179. Zeloni G, Farne A, Baccini M. Viewing less to see better. JNeurol Neurosurg Psychiatry. 2002;73:195--8

Reprint address: Dr Darren SJ Ting, MBChB, Victoria In-firmary, Langside Road, Glasgow G42 9TY, U.K.

 VISUAL NEGLECT FOLLOWING STROKE   133

Page 21: Visual Neglect

7/23/2019 Visual Neglect

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/visual-neglect 21/21

Outline

I. IntroductionII. Epidemiology 

III. Varieties of neglect 

 A. Modality (input/output)

B. Spatial representationC. Range of space

IV. Functional impact  V. Pathophysiology 

 A. Physiology of visuospatial perceptionB. Spatially lateralized dysfunction of visual

neglect C. Non-spatially lateralized dysfunction of vi-

sual neglect D. Role of non-visual afferent inputsE. Clinico-anatomical correlations of visual

neglect F. Clinical syndromes associated with visualneglect 

 VI. Assessment of visual neglect 

 A. Current clinical guidelinesB. How is visual neglect assessed?

1. Pencil-and-paper tests2. Behavioral assessment tools

3. Clinical observation and history-takingfrom the patient and the near contacts

4. Recent development of assessment technique

C. Distinction between visual neglect andother visual problems

D. Summary and recommendations for theassessment of visual neglect 

 VII. Rehabilitation of visual neglect 

 A. Current clinical practiceB. Types of rehabilitation

1. Compensatory 2. Substitutive3. Restitutive

C. Current evidence on the effectiveness of rehabilitationD. Summary and recommendation for reha-

bilitation of visual neglect 

 VIII. PrognosisIX. ConclusionX. Method of Literature Search

XI. Disclosure

134 Surv Ophthalmol 56 (2) March--April 2011   TING ET AL