VESTIBULAR SIGNALS Jan Van Gisbergen

76
VESTIBULAR SIGNALS Jan Van Gisbergen PhD COURSE SENSORY SYSTEMS Utrecht, September 29, 2008

description

VESTIBULAR SIGNALS Jan Van Gisbergen. PhD COURSE SENSORY SYSTEMS Utrecht, September 29, 2008. detection of self motion sensing body orientation in space visual perception in earth-centric coordinates. SCOPE. - PowerPoint PPT Presentation

Transcript of VESTIBULAR SIGNALS Jan Van Gisbergen

Page 1: VESTIBULAR SIGNALS Jan Van Gisbergen

VESTIBULAR SIGNALS

Jan Van Gisbergen

PhD COURSE SENSORY SYSTEMSUtrecht, September 29, 2008

Page 2: VESTIBULAR SIGNALS Jan Van Gisbergen

detection of self motion sensing body orientation in space visual perception in earth-centric coordinates

Page 3: VESTIBULAR SIGNALS Jan Van Gisbergen

SCOPE

Functions and limitations of vestibular sensors

Ambiguity problem of the otoliths

Solution to the ambiguity problem

Visual-vestibular fusion

Transformations from head to body reference frame

Visual space perception in static tilt

Bayesian model

Page 4: VESTIBULAR SIGNALS Jan Van Gisbergen

MULTISENSORY INTEGRATION

Page 5: VESTIBULAR SIGNALS Jan Van Gisbergen

VESTIBULAR SENSORS

functions &

limitations

Page 6: VESTIBULAR SIGNALS Jan Van Gisbergen

VESTIBULAR SENSORS

canals

otoliths

Page 7: VESTIBULAR SIGNALS Jan Van Gisbergen

SEMICIRCULAR CANALS

measure head rotation

Page 8: VESTIBULAR SIGNALS Jan Van Gisbergen

CANAL GEOMETRY

Three perpendicular canals measure head rotation in 3 dimensions

Page 9: VESTIBULAR SIGNALS Jan Van Gisbergen

BEST ROTATION AXES

a = anterior canal

p = posterior canal

h = horizontal canal

rotation in direction of arrow excites afferents from a given canal

Page 10: VESTIBULAR SIGNALS Jan Van Gisbergen

BEST ROTATION AXIS OF CANAL AFFERENT

Yakushin (2006) JNP

Page 11: VESTIBULAR SIGNALS Jan Van Gisbergen

OPTIMAL ROTATION AXES OF CANAL AFFERENTS

Page 12: VESTIBULAR SIGNALS Jan Van Gisbergen

DYNAMICS OF CANAL SIGNALS

Insensitive to low-frequency rotations (high pass filter)

Canal afferent fiber in 8th nerve• High resting discharge• Codes cupula deviation

Page 13: VESTIBULAR SIGNALS Jan Van Gisbergen

CONSTANT ROTATION IN DARKNESS

• rotation percept decays

• after stop, percept of rotation in opposite direction

• reflects cupular mechanics

Page 14: VESTIBULAR SIGNALS Jan Van Gisbergen

OTOLITHS

sensitive to linear acceleration during translation and to tilt, due to the pull of gravity

Page 15: VESTIBULAR SIGNALS Jan Van Gisbergen

HAIRCELL ACTIVATION

• each haircell is connected to separate nerve fiber

• deflecting cilia toward kinocilium depolarizes haircell

Page 16: VESTIBULAR SIGNALS Jan Van Gisbergen

POLARISATION VECTOR

Eron et al. (2008) J. Neurophysiol.

each otolith cell has cosine tuning:

indicates head orientation where pull of gravity has maximum effect

tilt stimuli:

nose down left ear down nose up

Page 17: VESTIBULAR SIGNALS Jan Van Gisbergen

OTOLITHS

sensitive to tilt and translation

Page 18: VESTIBULAR SIGNALS Jan Van Gisbergen

POLARISATION VECTORS

Fernandez and Goldberg (1976) JNP

Page 19: VESTIBULAR SIGNALS Jan Van Gisbergen

AMBIGUITY PROBLEMOF THE OTOLITHS

Page 20: VESTIBULAR SIGNALS Jan Van Gisbergen

OTOLITH SIGNAL IS AMBIGUOUS

hair cells cannot distinguish tilt and translation

Page 21: VESTIBULAR SIGNALS Jan Van Gisbergen

SOMATOGRAVIC ILLUSION

pilot is upright, but feels tilted

(only in darkness)

Page 22: VESTIBULAR SIGNALS Jan Van Gisbergen

AMBIGUITY PROBLEM

otolith signal may have various causes:

• translation (a)• force of gravity due to tilt (g)• combination of a and g

How can the brain resolve this ambiguity ?

inverse problem

Page 23: VESTIBULAR SIGNALS Jan Van Gisbergen

CANAL- OTOLITH INTERACTION MODEL

• canals detect rotation during tilt changes

• their signal helps to decompose otolith signal

Angelaki et al. (1999)

Page 24: VESTIBULAR SIGNALS Jan Van Gisbergen

CANAL–OTOLITH INTERACTION MODEL

basic principle:- tilt stimulates otoliths AND canals- translation stimulates only otoliths

Merfeld and Zupan (2002) J. Neurophysiology

tilt angle

linear acceleration

angular velocity

Page 25: VESTIBULAR SIGNALS Jan Van Gisbergen

percepts during rotation about a tilted axis

(OVAR)

Vingerhoets et al. (2006) J. Neurophysiol.

Vingerhoets et al. (2007) J. Neurophysiol.

TESTING THE MODEL

Page 26: VESTIBULAR SIGNALS Jan Van Gisbergen

THE ACTUAL MOTION

- rotation about tilted axis

- in darkness

- constant velocity

Page 27: VESTIBULAR SIGNALS Jan Van Gisbergen

MODEL PREDICTIONS

rotation signal decays gradually

wrong interpretation otolith signal: illusory translation percept

Page 28: VESTIBULAR SIGNALS Jan Van Gisbergen

SCHEMATIC SUMMARY OF RESULTS

confirms prediction

rotation percept

translation percept

Actual motion:

Percept:

Page 29: VESTIBULAR SIGNALS Jan Van Gisbergen

TRANSLATION AND ROTATION PERCEPT DATA

rotation percept

translation percept

Page 30: VESTIBULAR SIGNALS Jan Van Gisbergen

VISUAL – VESTIBULAR FUSION

Page 31: VESTIBULAR SIGNALS Jan Van Gisbergen

FUSION OF VISUAL AND VESTIBULAR SIGNALS FOR DETECTION OF

EGOMOTION

• brain interprets whole field motion as due to egomotion

• can detect constant velocity motion (low pass)

• complements vestibular motion detection (high pass)

Page 32: VESTIBULAR SIGNALS Jan Van Gisbergen

CANALS ARE HIGH PASS

Page 33: VESTIBULAR SIGNALS Jan Van Gisbergen

ROTATION IN LIGHT: VISUAL CONTRIBUTION

• Rotation percept in the light is veridical

• Visual system detects low frequencies

• Canals detect high frequencies

circular vection

Page 34: VESTIBULAR SIGNALS Jan Van Gisbergen

CONVERGENCE IN VESTIBULAR NUCLEUS

visual system detects low frequencies

canals detect high frequencies

Page 35: VESTIBULAR SIGNALS Jan Van Gisbergen

OPTIC FLOW PROVIDES LOW FREQUENCY INPUT FROM LINEAR MOTION

• otoliths detect linear acceleration

• optic flow can induce linear egomotion (train illusion!!)

Page 36: VESTIBULAR SIGNALS Jan Van Gisbergen

TRANSFORMATIONS FROM HEAD TO BODY

REFERENCE FRAME

Page 37: VESTIBULAR SIGNALS Jan Van Gisbergen

BALANCE

1. Otoliths measure head position in space

2. To maintain balance, we must know body position in space

3. Which mechanisms are involved?

Page 38: VESTIBULAR SIGNALS Jan Van Gisbergen

BALANCE

1. Measure head position in space (HS)

2. Measure position head on trunk (HB)

3. Compute position of body in space (BS):

BS = HS - HB

Page 39: VESTIBULAR SIGNALS Jan Van Gisbergen

EFFECT OF ELECTRICAL OTOLITH STIMULATION

•experiment in darkness

•results in body tilt

Why?

HS changes

HB is not changed

BS changes, subject corrects

BS = HS - HB

Page 40: VESTIBULAR SIGNALS Jan Van Gisbergen

VESTIBULAR - NECK PROPRIOCEPTIVE INTERACTIONS

neuron b codes linear motion in body-centered reference frame (accounts for neck signals)

neuron c codes motion in head-reference frame

Angelaki (2008) Ann Rev Neuroscience

monkey moves on sled in various directions

cell recording in cerebellum (FN)

Page 41: VESTIBULAR SIGNALS Jan Van Gisbergen

VISUAL SPACE PERCEPTION IN

STATIC TILT

Page 42: VESTIBULAR SIGNALS Jan Van Gisbergen

VISUAL VERTICAL

1. How can we determine the orientation of visual objects relative to the direction of gravity, even when we are tilted in darkness?

2. What is the role of the vestibular system?

Page 43: VESTIBULAR SIGNALS Jan Van Gisbergen

SENSING THE DIRECTION OF GRAVITY

Two different tasks:

1. Set line to vertical (SVV)

2. Estimate your body tilt (SBT)

Van Beuzekom & Van Gisbergen (2000) J. Neurophysiol.

Van Beuzekom et al. (2001) Vision Res.

Kaptein & Van Gisbergen (2004, 2005) J. Neurophysiol.

De Vrijer et al. (2008) J. Neurophysiol.

experiments in darkness

Page 44: VESTIBULAR SIGNALS Jan Van Gisbergen

ACCURACY vs PRECISION

Accuracy:

How close is the response to the true value?

Precision:

How reproducible is the response?

darts analogy:

Page 45: VESTIBULAR SIGNALS Jan Van Gisbergen

ACCURACY AND PRECISION IN LINE TASK (SVV)

accuracy

precision

De Vrijer et al. (2008) J. Neurophysiol.

De Vrijer et al. (2008) in progress

Page 46: VESTIBULAR SIGNALS Jan Van Gisbergen

ACCURACY IN LINE TASK

due to underestimation of body tilt?

Page 47: VESTIBULAR SIGNALS Jan Van Gisbergen

NO UNDERESTIMATION OF BODY TILT

SVV SBT

• Subjects know quite well how they are tilted (SBT)

• Yet, their line settings undercompensate for tilt (SVV)

Van Beuzekom et al. (2001) Vision Res.

Kaptein and Van Gisbergen (2004) J. Neurophysiol.

Page 48: VESTIBULAR SIGNALS Jan Van Gisbergen

PRECISION IN LINE TASK

is scatter in SVV simply reflection of noise in body tilt signal?

De Vrijer et al. (2008) J. Neurophysiol.

De Vrijer et al. (2008) in progress

Page 49: VESTIBULAR SIGNALS Jan Van Gisbergen

SVV LESS NOISY THAN SBT

De Vrijer et al. in progress

psychometric experiments at 0o and 90o tilt:

Page 50: VESTIBULAR SIGNALS Jan Van Gisbergen

SVV LESS NOISY THAN SBT

Page 51: VESTIBULAR SIGNALS Jan Van Gisbergen

SUMMARY SBT AND SVV DATA

Two paradoxical findings:

1. subject knows tilt angle, yet makes biased line settings

2. more certain about line setting than about body tilt

estimate body tilt (SBT) adjust line to vertical (SVV)

Page 52: VESTIBULAR SIGNALS Jan Van Gisbergen

SBT DATA SHOW:

• An unbiased head tilt signal is available

• Noise increases with tilt angle

Page 53: VESTIBULAR SIGNALS Jan Van Gisbergen

SIGNALS REQUIRED FOR SPATIAL VISION

retinal signal

to compute line in space (Ls), brain must combine info about line orientation on retina (LR) and head tilt (HS)

head-tilt signal

Page 54: VESTIBULAR SIGNALS Jan Van Gisbergen

SIMPLY USING RAW TILT SIGNAL …

would not explain SVV bias !!spatial vision would be accurate, but noisy

raw tilt signal

Page 55: VESTIBULAR SIGNALS Jan Van Gisbergen

A BAYESIAN PERSPECTIVE

IDEAL OBSERVER MODEL

Page 56: VESTIBULAR SIGNALS Jan Van Gisbergen

IDEAL OBSERVER STRATEGY

1) Use sensory data: noisy tilt signal suggests range of possible tilt angles (likelihood)

2) Use prior knowledge: we know that large tilt angles are very uncommon (prior)

3) Most likely tilt angle (posterior) is product of likelihood and prior

Eggert (1998) PhD Thesis, Munich

MacNeilage et al. (2007) Exp. Brain Res.

De Vrijer et al. (2008) J. Neurophysiol.

Page 57: VESTIBULAR SIGNALS Jan Van Gisbergen

IDEAL OBSERVER STRATEGY

Tilt prior has 2 effects on SVV:

• Less noise

• Bias at large tilt

Page 58: VESTIBULAR SIGNALS Jan Van Gisbergen

WHY WOULD THIS MAKE SENSE?

1) Less noise in spatial vision

2) Downside: bias at large tilts

3) Average performance improves (large tilts are rare)

Page 59: VESTIBULAR SIGNALS Jan Van Gisbergen

DEMO

BIAS EFFECT INCREASES WITH TILT

Page 60: VESTIBULAR SIGNALS Jan Van Gisbergen

no bias

De Vrijer et al. (2008) J. Neurophysiology

no bias

Page 61: VESTIBULAR SIGNALS Jan Van Gisbergen
Page 62: VESTIBULAR SIGNALS Jan Van Gisbergen

small bias

small bias

Page 63: VESTIBULAR SIGNALS Jan Van Gisbergen

large bias

Page 64: VESTIBULAR SIGNALS Jan Van Gisbergen

large bias

Page 65: VESTIBULAR SIGNALS Jan Van Gisbergen

MODEL PARAMETERS

1) head tilt noise level in upright

2) increase of head tilt noise with tilt

3) prior width

4) eye torsion amplitude

Page 66: VESTIBULAR SIGNALS Jan Van Gisbergen

MODEL FITS: SVV ACCURACY

Page 67: VESTIBULAR SIGNALS Jan Van Gisbergen

MODEL FITS: SVV ACCURACY

< 0

Page 68: VESTIBULAR SIGNALS Jan Van Gisbergen

QUESTIONS1) The canal-related neuron illustrated in the paper by Yakushin (see slide 10 in lecture) can be

identified by its excitatory and inhibitory responses to sinusoidal rotation about various axis orientations relative to the head.

 

a)      What would happen if the same head-fixed rotations would be applied, but now with the animal in supine position, rather than in the prone position shown in the figure? Explain your answer.

b)      Sketch expected response patterns for a neuron getting its input from the right anterior canal, when using the same rotation axes as shown in the lecture sheet?

c)      Estimate the frequency of rotation and explain why its choice is important

 

 

 

2) In the scheme showing how combining a signal Hs and a signal coding Hb can yield a signal body orientation in space (Bs), the two input signals have opposite signs (see slide 38).

a)      Explain why this makes sense, given the fact that we can tilt the head and still remain upright

b)      Explain why electrical otolith stimulation does cause perturbation of balance

 

 

 

Page 69: VESTIBULAR SIGNALS Jan Van Gisbergen
Page 70: VESTIBULAR SIGNALS Jan Van Gisbergen
Page 71: VESTIBULAR SIGNALS Jan Van Gisbergen
Page 72: VESTIBULAR SIGNALS Jan Van Gisbergen

MODEL EXPLANATION OF NOISE LEVELS:SVV vs SBT PRECISION

• SVV is less noisy than the SBT (remarkable, but explained by model)

• SBT becomes more noisy at larger tilt (supports model assumption)

• SBT noise levels compatible with head-tilt fit results

Page 73: VESTIBULAR SIGNALS Jan Van Gisbergen

CONCLUSION

Accuracy-precision trade-off in spatial vision:

• Bayesian strategy reduces noise at small tilts

• causes systematic errors at large tilts

Page 74: VESTIBULAR SIGNALS Jan Van Gisbergen

CENTRIFUGE EXPERIMENT

proefpersoon wordt in donker langdurig rondgedraaid op centrifuge

• draait nog steeds rond maar voelt geen rotatie meer

• zit in feite rechtop maar voelt zich gekanteld

Page 75: VESTIBULAR SIGNALS Jan Van Gisbergen

OVAR

Page 76: VESTIBULAR SIGNALS Jan Van Gisbergen

TWO TRANSFORMATIONS OF PRIMARY VESTIBULAR SIGNALS

1. Signals from the canals (ω) are used to decompose signals from otolith afferents (α) into gravitational (g, orientation) and translational (f) components.

2. Gravitational estimates are also used to transform head-fixed angular velocity signals from the

semicircular canals (ω) into inertial velocity, i.e., space-referenced angular velocity (ωs)