University of Groningen Formation and adaptation of memory ...

27
University of Groningen Formation and adaptation of memory Havekes, Robbert IMPORTANT NOTE: You are advised to consult the publisher's version (publisher's PDF) if you wish to cite from it. Please check the document version below. Document Version Publisher's PDF, also known as Version of record Publication date: 2008 Link to publication in University of Groningen/UMCG research database Citation for published version (APA): Havekes, R. (2008). Formation and adaptation of memory: Neurobiological mechanisms underlying learning and reversal learning. s.n. Copyright Other than for strictly personal use, it is not permitted to download or to forward/distribute the text or part of it without the consent of the author(s) and/or copyright holder(s), unless the work is under an open content license (like Creative Commons). The publication may also be distributed here under the terms of Article 25fa of the Dutch Copyright Act, indicated by the “Taverne” license. More information can be found on the University of Groningen website: https://www.rug.nl/library/open-access/self-archiving-pure/taverne- amendment. Take-down policy If you believe that this document breaches copyright please contact us providing details, and we will remove access to the work immediately and investigate your claim. Downloaded from the University of Groningen/UMCG research database (Pure): http://www.rug.nl/research/portal. For technical reasons the number of authors shown on this cover page is limited to 10 maximum. Download date: 13-12-2021

Transcript of University of Groningen Formation and adaptation of memory ...

University of Groningen

Formation and adaptation of memoryHavekes, Robbert

IMPORTANT NOTE: You are advised to consult the publisher's version (publisher's PDF) if you wish to cite fromit. Please check the document version below.

Document VersionPublisher's PDF, also known as Version of record

Publication date:2008

Link to publication in University of Groningen/UMCG research database

Citation for published version (APA):Havekes, R. (2008). Formation and adaptation of memory: Neurobiological mechanisms underlyinglearning and reversal learning. s.n.

CopyrightOther than for strictly personal use, it is not permitted to download or to forward/distribute the text or part of it without the consent of theauthor(s) and/or copyright holder(s), unless the work is under an open content license (like Creative Commons).

The publication may also be distributed here under the terms of Article 25fa of the Dutch Copyright Act, indicated by the “Taverne” license.More information can be found on the University of Groningen website: https://www.rug.nl/library/open-access/self-archiving-pure/taverne-amendment.

Take-down policyIf you believe that this document breaches copyright please contact us providing details, and we will remove access to the work immediatelyand investigate your claim.

Downloaded from the University of Groningen/UMCG research database (Pure): http://www.rug.nl/research/portal. For technical reasons thenumber of authors shown on this cover page is limited to 10 maximum.

Download date: 13-12-2021

Introduction and scope of this thesis

Chapter 1

10

Chapter 1

Contents

1. The hippocampus 2. Memory formation and the ‘Hebbian rule’: a brief history 3. NMDA and AMPA receptors 4. Protein kinases 5. Protein phosphatases 6. AKAP150 regulates the cellular distribution of PKA and CaN 7. Sleep and hippocampal synaptic plasticity 8. Aim and outline thesis 9. References

11

Introduction and scope of this thesis

1. The hippocampus

1.1. The hippocampal circuitry and its role in memory formationOne of the key structures in the brain that has a wide range of memory functions is the hippocampus (Squire, 1992). The hippocampus can be divided in several major areas: the dentate gyrus (DG), Cornu Ammonis 3 (CA3), Cornu Ammonis 1 (CA1) and subiculum, see figure 1 (Amaral and Witter, 1989). The DG granule cells receive input from the stel-late cells in layer 2 of the entorhinal cortex via the perforant path (Nyakas et al., 1988; Witter, 1993). The granular cells of the DG project to the CA3 pyramidal cells via the mossy fibers. In addition, CA3 pyramidal neurons receive input from two other sources, namely the stellate cells in layer 2 of the entorhinal cortex and from the recurrent col-laterals of the CA3 pyramidal cells themselves. The recurrent collaterals are the most nu-merous type of input to CA3 pyramidal cells (Ishizuka et al., 1990). Besides projections of the CA3 pyramidal cells via the Schäffer collaterals, the CA1 area receives input from layer 3 of the entorhinal cortex through the perforant pathway. The CA1 pyramidal cells project either directly or via the subiculum to layer 5 of the entorhinal cortex (Johnston and Amaral, 1997), as well as to a variety of other areas including the prefrontal cor-tex, lateral septum, anterior thalamus and mamillary bodies (Amaral and Witter, 1995). Based on lesion studies, it has become apparent that the hippocampus plays a cru-cial role in the formation of memories in various learning paradigms including spatial tasks like the Morris water maze (Schenk and Morris, 1985), radial maze (Olton and Papas, 1979), Y maze (Jarrard, 1986; Etkin et al., 2006), as well as in various associative tasks including fear conditioning (Kimble, 1963; Phillips and Ledoux, 1992). The hippocampus can rapidly form spatial representations of the environment (also referred to as spatial maps). In addition, the hippocampus can develop an association of a specific context within which an impor-tant event occurred, for instance where an aversive stimulus was received (e.g. electrical shock was given) or the location of a food reward or a submerged and invisible platform. 1.2. The hippocampus as a comparator of previously stored and novel informationIn addition to the formation of new memories, the hippocampus has been regarded to be the location where previously stored memories can be compared with actual sensory in-formation from the environment (Gothard et al., 1996; Knight, 1996). Based on anatomical connectivity and computational modeling, both area CA3 and area CA1 of the hippocampus have been implicated in the detection of changes in a familiar environment, also known as match-mismatch processing (for review Rolls and Kesner, 2006; Lisman and Otmakhova, 2001). In case of a comparison between recently stored information (e.g. minutes old) and novel sensory information, area CA3 is of paramount importance in this novelty detection (Lee et al., 2004, 2005; Nakazawa et al., 2003). In case of longer time intervals (ranging from hours to days), area CA1 seems to react as a novelty detector, rather than area CA3 (Wan et al., 1999; Fyhn et al., 2002; Jenkins et al., 2004). In contrast to area CA1 and CA3,the DG is generally not seen as the location where novelty detection takes place. The DG plays an essential role in pattern separation (Gilbert et al., 1998; Goodrich-Hunsaker, 2005; Leut-geb et al., 2007; McHugh et al., 2007), a process important to distinguish relatively similar

12

Chapter 1

environments or situations. Input coming from the entorhinal cortex is dispersed onto the broad layer of sparsely firing granule cells. Each granule cell therefore carries only a small and distinct fraction of information of the total input (McNaughton and Nadel, 1989). As sug-gested by Leutgeb and colleagues (2007), the segregation of inputs from the cortex might be retained due to the sparse firing levels of the granular neurons in combination with the relatively meager connections between granule cells and CA3-pyramidal cells.

Figure 1. The hippocampus is a brain structure crucial for learn-ing and memory processes. (A) A representative picture of a coronal section of a mouse brain stained for the catalytic subunit of CaN. (B) Enlargement of the dorsal hip-pocampus stained for the cata-lytic subunit of CaN (See insert A). (C) A schematic representa-tion of the most important subre-gions within the dorsal hippocam-pus: (DG) dentate gyrus; (CA3) Cornu Ammonis 3; (CA1) Cornu Ammonis 1; (SUB) Subiculum.

13

Introduction and scope of this thesis

2. Memory formation and the ‘Hebbian rule’: a brief history

Only half a century ago, initial hypotheses about the neurobiological correlates underlying memory formation were postulated. In the 1950s, Donald Hebb hypothesized that synaptic efficacy increases when: 1) there is presynaptic activity at the synapse, and 2) a critical level of postsynaptic depolarization occurs (Hebb, 1949). This ‘Hebbian rule’ was confirmed by Bliss and Lomo (1973), who showed that brief and intense synaptic activation in the hip-pocampus indeed results in a long-term enhancement of synaptic efficacy of the stimulated synapses (a process referred to as long-term potentiation, LTP). From a theoretical point of view, there was a necessity for an ‘anti-Hebbian rule’ that explained a mechanism controlling the weakening of synaptic connections when presynaptic and postsynaptic activity did not occur together (Stent, 1973). In the early 1990s several investigators revealed that a weak but prolonged stimulation resulted in a weakening of synaptic strength, a process known as long-term depression (LTD) (Dudek and Bear, 1992; Mulkey and Malenka, 1992). The discovery of bi-directional plasticity induced by LTP and LTD opened the possibility to elucidate basic cellular and molecular mechanisms of synaptic plasticity giving insight in the processes underlying the formation of memories. Nowadays, LTP is gene-rally seen as a key mechanism underlying memory formation since it has been shown that learning induces LTP in vivo. In contrast to LTP, the function of LTD in (hippocampal) mem-ory formation remains debated and is less well understood.

3. NMDA and AMPA receptors

One of the two glutamate receptor families involved in LTP and LTD is the ionotrophic receptor family that is ligand dependent. The ionotrophic receptor family can be subdivided into three classes: the α-amino-3-hydroxy-5-methyl-4-isoxazole proprionate (AMPA) re-ceptor, the N-Methyl-D-Aspartate (NMDA) receptor, and kainate receptors (Hollmann and Heinemann, 1994). AMPA receptors are the main transducers of rapid excitatory transmission in the central nervous system (Derkach et al., 2007). They are tetramers comprised of four recep-tor subunits (GluR1-GluR4) and consist of a large extracellular N-terminal domain, three transmembrane domains and an intracellular C-domain. The subunit composition varies between brain regions and affects the functional properties of AMPA receptors and the trafficking of these receptors (Derkach et al., 2007). AMPA receptor functioning is further mediated by several phosphorylation sites on the intracellular C-domain of all four subunits (Song and Huganir, 2002). The GluR1 subunit has one currently known threonine site and various serine phosphorylation sites (Lee, 2006). Of these various phosphorylation sites, the serine-831 (S831) and serine-845 (S845) sites are of particular interest. Changes in the phosphorylation state of both sites have been implicated in the expression of LTP and LTD. Phosphorylation of S831 increases the single channel conductance (Derkach et al., 1999), is enhanced following LTP (Barria et al., 1997; Lee et al., 2000), and is reduced after de-potentiation (Lee et al., 2000). S845 phosphorylation increases the mean open probability

14

Chapter 1

of the channel (Banke et al., 2000), while dephosphorylation of the site occurs after LTD (Kameyama et al., 1998; Lee et al., 1998; Lee et al., 2000; Brown et al., 2005). In addition, enhanced phosphorylation levels of this site are found after de-depression (Lee et al., 2000). Phosphorylation of S845 is also required for the insertion of GluR1 containing AMPA recep-tors into synapses with LTP induction (Esteban et al., 2003), and regulation of AMPA recep-tor recycling (Ehlers, 2000). A recent study by Whitlock et al (2006) assessed whether train-ing in an inhibitory avoidance paradigm evoked changes in the phosphorylation state of both GluR1 serine sites. They revealed that this training induced a rapid enhancement of S831 phoshorylation which returned to baseline levels within an hour. In contrast, no changes in phosphorylation levels were found for the S845 site. Recently, two additional phosphoryla-tion sites of the GluR1 subunit were discovered. Boehm and colleagues (2006) revealed that phosphorylation of the serine-818 site is necessary and sufficient for GluR1 synaptic insertion by LTP. The threonine-840 site is known to be strongly phosphorylated under basal conditions in the hippocampus (Lee et al., 2002), however its function with regard to AMPA receptor functioning remains to be elucidated. Figure 2 shows a schematic survey of the currently known relations between changes in the phosphorylation state of the GluR1 serine sites and AMPA receptor function. An overview of the phosphorylation sites of the GluR1 subunit and the known functions are shown in table 1. For a detailed description of the phosphorylation sites of the GluR2 and GluR4 subunits see, Lee (2006). As mentioned above, it is widely accepted that AMPA receptor functioning is con-trolled by changes in the phosphorylation state of the receptor subunits. However, studies investigating alterations in receptor phosphorylation in relation to memory formation in vivo remain scarce. Whitlock co-workers (2006) recently showed that one trial inhibitory avoid-ance learning produced the same changes in hippocampal AMPA receptor phosphorylation as induction of LTP. In addition, they showed that this learning paradigm indeed induces LTP in the CA1 region of the hippocampus. NMDA receptors stay silent at resting membrane potential, but play a crucial role in the induction of LTP (Nicoll and Malenka, 1999) and LTD (Bear et al., 1999). The receptors consist of two obligatory NR1 subunits and two NR2 subunits (Nakanishi, 1992; Hollmann and Heinemann, 1994). Several isoforms of the NR2 subunit exist (NR2A- NR2D), that control the properties of NMDA receptor channel, like the threshold for the removal of the Mg2+ block (Cull-Candy et al., 2001). When the postsynaptic membrane is sufficiently depolarized, the Mg2+ block is removed from the channel (Mayer et al., 1984). After the removal of the Mg2+ block, the channel can be activated by glutamate resulting in a postsy-naptic influx of Ca2+. This Ca2+ influx triggers various signaling cascades that can lead to either LTP or LTD depending on the duration and level of Ca2+ influx in the postsynaptic cell. For a detailed review of known phosphorylation sites on NMDA receptor subunits, see Lee (2006).

15

Introduction and scope of this thesis

Figure 2. A model for the bidirectional changes in AMPA-receptor phosphorylation and NMDA-receptor-dependent plasticity in a post-synaptic element. High frequency stimulation (HFS) can result in a strong increase in Ca2+ influx leading to the activation of PKA and CaMKII. As a consequence, HFS can lead to a PKA-dependent phosphorylation of the S845 site as well as an enhanced open channel time (P0) when delivered to previously depressed synapses (LTD). Furthermore, HFS can in addition lead to an increase in channel conductancy of GluR1-containing AMPA receptors through the phosphorylation of the S831 site when delivered at a naive synapse resulting in LTP. Recently it was shown that phosphorylation of the S818 (by PKC) is sufficient for the incorporation of AMPA receptors into the membrane with LTP. It is currently unknown whether CaN or PP1 dephosphorylates this serine site. Low frequency stimulation results in a low Ca2+ influx resulting in the activation of the phosphatases CaN and as a consequence also PP1 activation. LFS applied to naive synapses results in exocytosis of GluR1 containing receptors through a CaN-dependent dephosphorylation of the S845 site and reduced open channel time. When applied to a previously potenti-ated synapse, LFS induces a PP1 dependent dephosphorylation of the S831 site resulting in reduced chan-nel conductivity. Grey arrows indicate protein phosphatase-dependent mechanisms, black arrows indicate protein kinase dependent mechanisms. For abbreviations, see text.

16

Chapter 1Ta

ble

1a: P

hosp

hory

latio

n of

AM

PA G

luR1

subu

nits

pho

psho

site

s si

te

Iden

tifie

d fu

nctio

n Pr

otei

n ki

nase

S8

18

Phos

phor

ylat

ion

nece

ssar

yfo

rsyn

aptic

inco

rpor

atio

nan

dLT

P(B

oehm

et

al.,

2006

).PK

C(B

oehm

eta

l.,2

006)

S8

31

Incr

ease

dsi

ngle

cha

nnel

con

duct

ance

ofh

omom

eric

rece

ptor

s(D

erka

che

tal

.,19

99;O

h&

Der

kach

,200

5).

Enha

nced

pho

spho

ryla

tion

follo

win

gLT

P(B

arria

,Mul

lere

tal.,

199

7;L

ee

eta

l.,2

000)

.C

ansu

bstit

ute

forS

845

inm

edia

ting

LTP

(Lee

eta

l.,2

003,

Lee

eta

l.,2

004)

.

CaM

KII

(Bar

ria,e

tal.,

199

7;

Mam

men

eta

l.,19

97)

PKC

(Roc

hee

tal.,

200

2)

T840

H

ighl

yph

osph

oryl

ated

inh

ippo

cam

pusu

nder

bas

alc

ondi

tions

.PK

C(L

eee

tal.,

200

2)

S845

In

crea

sed

mea

nop

enp

roba

bilit

yof

cha

nnel

(Ban

kee

tal.,

200

0).

Incr

ease

dph

osph

oryl

atio

naf

terd

e-de

pres

sion

(Lee

eta

l.,2

000)

.N

eces

sary

fori

nser

tion

into

syna

pses

with

LTP

indu

ctio

n(E

steb

ane

tal.,

20

03).

Can

subs

titut

eS8

31fo

rmed

iatin

gLT

P(L

eee

tal.,

200

3;L

eee

tal.,

200

4).

Enha

nced

AM

PAre

cept

orin

serti

on(E

hler

seta

l.,2

000)

PKA

(Roc

hee

tal.,

199

6)

Tabl

e 1b

: Dep

hosp

hory

latio

n of

AM

PA G

luR1

subu

nits

pho

spho

site

s si

te

Iden

tifie

d fu

nctio

n Pr

otei

n ph

osph

atas

e S8

31

Red

uced

cha

nnel

con

duct

ance

,dep

oten

tiatio

nof

pre

viou

sly

pote

ntia

ted

syna

pse

(Lee

eta

l.,2

000)

,(H

uang

eta

l.,2

001)

PP

1(L

eee

tal.,

200

0)

S845

En

docy

tosi

sofG

luR

1co

ntai

ning

AM

PAre

cept

ors(

Kam

eyam

aet

al.,

199

8;

Lee

eta

l.,2

000;

Bea

ttie

eta

l.,2

000;

Sm

ithe

tal.,

200

6).

Nec

essa

ryfo

rLTD

exp

ress

ion

(Lee

eta

l.,2

003;

Lee

eta

l.,2

004)

.D

own

regu

latio

nof

Glu

R1

rece

ptor

cur

rent

(Tav

alin

eta

l.,2

002)

.N

eces

sary

forL

TDe

xpre

ssio

n(L

eee

tal.,

200

3;L

eee

tal.,

200

4).

PP1*

(Lee

eta

l.,2

000)

Cal

cine

urin

*

(Bea

ttie

eta

l.,2

000)

*B

eatti

ean

dco

lleag

ues(

2000

)sug

gest

edth

at,b

ased

on

the

stre

ngth

ofs

ynap

tica

ctiv

atio

nof

NM

DA

rece

ptor

s,ei

ther

CaN

alo

neo

rbot

hC

aNa

ndP

P1a

ren

eede

dfo

rAM

PAre

ndoc

ytos

is.

Ada

pted

and

mod

ified

from

Lee

,200

6.

17

Introduction and scope of this thesis

4. Protein kinases and synaptic plasticity Protein kinases are enzymes that phosphorylate numerous substrates including other pro-tein kinases, receptor subunits, and phosphoproteins. They have different functions within the same cell depending on the subcellular location. The protein kinase families that have been studied most intensively in relation to LTP and memory formation are the calcium/calmodulin dependent kinase family (CaMK family), cyclic adenosine 3’5’ monophosphate-dependent protein kinase (PKA) family, the mitogen-activated protein kinase (MAPK) family and the Ca2+/ phospholipid-dependent protein kinase (PKC). The role of the PKA family and MAPK family will be discussed in paragraph 4.1- 4.2.

4.1. PKA structure, cellular function and memory formation PKA is one of the most intensively studied protein kinases in regard to synaptic plasti-city and memory processing. The mammalian PKA family consists of four regulatory subu-nits (RIα, RIβ, RIIα, and RIIβ), that each have two cAMP binding sites with different af-finities for cAMP, and three catalytic subunits (Cα, Cβ, Cγ). All these subunits show dis-tinct expression patterns across mammalian brain regions (Cadd and McKnight., 1989), and are strongly expressed especially in areas that are essential for learning and memory (e.g. neocortex and hippocampus). In the absence of cAMP, PKA is a tetrameric com-plex consisting of two regulatory subunits and two catalytic subunits. PKA consisting of RI dimers is referred to type I PKA, while PKA consisting of RII dimers is reffered to as type II PKA (Tasken et al., 1993; Francis and Corbin, 1999; Skalhegg and Tasken, 2000). Activation of PKA is initiated via two mechanisms. The first is by influx of Ca2+ through NMDA receptors or voltage-gated Ca2+ channels; Ca2+ (together with calmodu-lin) can activate adenylyl cyclases (AC) resulting in cAMP production (Eliot, 1989) (See Fig.3). Alternatively, the synthesis of cAMP can be initiated through the binding of transmitters and hormones to guanine nucleotide-binding regulatory protein (G-protein) coupled receptors (Tang and Gilman, 1991). G-proteins bind to adenylyl cyclases resulting in its activation, which starts the synthesis of cAMP. As a consequence of binding cAMP to the regulatory subunits, the monomeric catalytic subunits are released and can then phosphorylate various substrates including the GluR1 S845 site (see Fig.2). Besides the S845 of the GluR1 subunit, PKA targets many other substrates. Among them the NMDA receptor subunits NR1 (Tingley et al., 1997) and NR2A (Krupp et al., 2002). Activated PKA can phosphorylate inhibitor 1 (I1), a phosphoprotein which, when phosphorylated, inhibits protein phosphatase 1 (PP1) activity (Huang and Glinsmann, 1976). The released catalytic subunit can also translocate to the nucleus to phosphorylate serine-133 on the cyclic AMP-response element-binding protein (CREB). As a result, transcription of CREB-associated genes (for instance Zif268 and c-Fos) is initiated (Yamamoto et al., 1988; Gonzalez and Montminy, 1989; Sheng et al., 1991). In case of CA1 neurons, MAPK activation is required to couple PKA activation to CREB phospho-rylation (Roberson et al., 1999). In addition to CREB, PKA can also target the transcription factor Elk1 via the MAPK pathway (Roberson et al., 1999; Grewal et al., 2000; Morozov et al., 2003).

18

Chapter 1

Various electrophysiological studies have recognized the role of PKA in L-LTP, a late form of LTP that can last for several hours (Frey et al., 1993; Huang and Kandel, 1994; Nguyen and Kandel, 1996; Abel et al., 1997; Matthies and Reymann, 1993; Woo et al., 2000, 2002, 2003). L-LTP requires NMDA receptor activation (Collingridge et al., 1983), protein synthesis (Frey et al., 1988) and gene transcription (Nguyen et al., 1994). In addition to its role in L-LTP, the role of the protein kinase PKA in the formation of long-term memories has been studied in-tensively using transgenic and/or pharmacological approaches. Mice lacking the RIβ subunit of PKA did not show any abnormalities in the learning paradigms assessing spatial memory formation, due to compensatory mechanisms for the loss of a given subunit (e.g. enhanced expression of the RIα subunit) (Huang et al., 1995). Similarly to the RIβ subunit knock out mice, Cβ1 knock-out mice performed normally in spatial learning tasks (Huang et al.,1995; Qi et al., 1996). Despite the fact that both knock-out mouse lines did not show any reduc-tion in PKA activity and normal performance in various learning tasks, marked defects were found in certain forms of synaptic plasticity including certain forms of LTP and LTD. Using a transgenic approach, Abel and colleagues (1997) generated a mutant mouse overexpress-ing a dominant negative form of the regulatory subunit of PKA R(AB) under control of the CaMKIIα gene (thereby restricting the expression to the postnatal forebrain). In contrast to the conventional knock-out mice, these transgenic mice showed reduced PKA activity, decreased L-LTP and memory deficits in learning paradigms requiring the hippocampus like the Morris water maze and contextual fear conditioning, suggesting that PKA is crucial for the consolidation of long-term memories. In line with these findings, several other studies have shown that pharmacological intervention of PKA activity in various brain regions inhib-its the formation of long-term memories, but not short-term memories in other learning paradigms including tone cued and contextual fear conditioning (Schafe et al., 1999; Schafe & LeDoux, 2000), conditioned taste aversion (Koh et al., 2002) and inhibitory avoidance (Quevedo et al., 2004). While PKA is beneficial for the formation of long-term memories, it was recently shown that PKA functions as a molecular constraint in case of the extinction of previously formed contextual fear memories (Isiegas et al., 2006).

4.2. MAPK structure, cellular function and memory formationOriginally, the MAPK family was found to be essential in various cellular processes including cell division, differentiation and apoptosis (Schaeffer and Weber, 1999). MAPKs can be acti-vated by a number of extracellular signals including growth and neurotrophic factors (Segal and Greenberg, 1996). During recent years, a significant role for MAPKs in hippocampal synaptic plasticity and memory formation has been recognized. The MAPKs consist of a family of a core of three kinases: MAP kinase kinase kinase (MAPKKK, also known as Raf-1 and B-Raf), MAP kinase kinase (MAPKK, also known as MEK) and MAP kinase (P44 MAPK = erk1, P42 MAPK = erk2). MAPKKK activates MAPKK through phosphorylation. Upon activation MAPKK phosphorylates MAPK at both a threonine and tyrosine residue. MAPK signaling can furthermore be regulated via PKA and PKC signaling mechanisms (Sweat, 2001). Activated MAPK can result in the activation of the transcriptions factor CREB and Elk-1 inducing gene transcription (Wang et al., 2007) (Fig.3). CREB-dependent gene tran-scription is shown to be essential for the maintenance of LTP and long-term memory (Silva et al., 1998). Recently, persistent LTD, a form of LTD that is protein-synthesis dependent

19

Introduction and scope of this thesis

and can last for days, was suggested to be a consequence of altered gene expression (Kau-derer and Kandel, 2000). Thiels and colleagues (2002) revealed that the MAPK cascade contributes to persistent LTD, through the promoting of gene transcription via the serum response element (SRE) (Fig.3). Pharmacological analysis revealed that inhibition of hippoc-ampal MAPK activation resulted in impaired spatial memory formation and LTP (Blum et al., 1999; Selcher et al., 1999). Similar impairments were found for fear motivated learning (Atkins et al., 1998; Cammarota et al., 2000).

Figure 3. A schematic representation of a cell body with a nucleus (square) and a postsynaptic cell com-ponent. Activation of protein kinase and protein phosphatase pathways by HFS and LFS in the postsynaptic structure. Black arrows indicate the activation of protein kinase pathways via HFS which can lead to CREB and Elk-1-associated gene transcription resulting in LTP. Grey arrows indicate the activation of protein phos-phatase and protein kinase pathways as a result of LFS resulting in SRE-associated gene transcription and LTD. For abbreviations, see text.

20

Chapter 1

5. Protein phosphatases Protein phosphatases counteract the actions of protein kinases by dephosphorylating the same substrates. The family of serine/threonine specific protein phosphatases is divided in three different groups of phosphatase that are highly homologous (PP1, PP2A, PP2B). The role of PP2B (and PP1) in relation to synaptic plasticity and memory formation is reviewed in detail in paragraph 5.1 Phosphatase 2C, a fourth known serine/threonine phosphatase belongs to an unrelated second phosphatase family. In addition to serine/threonine phos-phatases, various tyrosine phosphatases are also known to be involved in synaptic plasticity (for detailed reviews see: Wagner et al., 1991; Nairn and Shenolikar, 1992).

5.1. Calcineurin structure, cellular function and memory formation Calcineurin, also known as protein phosphatase 2B (CaN, PP2B), is a member of the threo-nine serine phosphatase family. It is the only calcium-dependent phosphatase in the brain and has a very high affinity for calcium (Klee et al., 1979). It is a heterodimer composed of a catalytic A-subunit (CaNA) and a regulatory subunit (CaNB). Two isoforms are known of the CaNA subunit that are present in the brain (CaNAα and CaNAβ), while CaNB subunit has only one known isoform in the brain (CaNBα1; Wera and Hemmings, 1995). CaN is strongly expressed both presynaptically and postsynaptically in principal neurons and largely absent in interneurons and glia (Sik et al., 1998). High concentrations are found in the hip-pocampus, caudate putamen and substantia nigra, while CaN is moderately present in the cerebral and cerebellar cortex (Goto et al., 1986). CaN is one of the most prominently expressed enzymes in the central nervous system, since it constitutes almost 1% of total protein in the brain (Klee et al., 1979). The CaNA subunit can bind a complex of calcium and calmodulin. Both the catalytic and regulatory subunit are needed for full CaN activity. The CaNB subunit remains tightly bound to the catalytic subunit regardless of the binding of calcium. Binding of calcium to the regulatory subunit enhances CaN activity, which is further regulated by endogenous inhibi-tors, anchoring proteins, ions metals and oxidative processes (Rusnak and Mertz, 2000). CaN has numerous functions in various cellular compartments including the cy-toplasm, nucleus as well as in pre- and post-synaptic elements of synapses. For instance, it dephosphorylates the GluR1 S845 site (Banke et al., 2000), and it shortens the channel open time of NMDA receptors (Lieberman and Mody, 1994; Tong et al., 1995) via targeting of the NR2A subunit (Krupp et al., 2002). In addition, CaN can also activate a second protein phosphatase known as protein phosphatase 1 (PP1), through the dephosphorylation of the phosphoprotein I1 (Huang and Glinsmann, 1976) (Fig.3). PP1 can dephosphorylate nume-rous other substrates including the AMPA receptor GluR1 S831 site (Lee et al., 2000) and the transcription factor CREB (Bito et al., 1996). Overall, activity of CaN (and PP1) can result in suppressed protein synthesis and receptor dephosphorylation (and endocytosis), resulting in a depressed synapse. In agreement with this, various electrophysiological studies have emphasized the importance of these protein phosphatases for LTD and depotentia-tion. In vivo analysis suggested that reducing CaN activity in the forebrain via the expression of

21

Introduction and scope of this thesis

a CaN inhibitor facilitated LTP but had no effect on LTD (Malleret et al., 2001). Similar observations were done using a pharmacological approach (Ikegame and Inokuchi, 2000). Re-duced CaN activity levels enhanced both short-term and long-term memory in various spatial and non-spatial learning tasks (Malleret et al., 2001; Ikegame and Inokuchi, 2000). Likewise, overexpression of forebrain-CaN resulted in defective long-term memory, evident in both a spatial task and a visual recognition task, although it did not affect short-term memory (Man-suy et al., 1998). Similarly, reduction of PP1 activity in the forebrain facilitated the formation of long-term memory and was accompanied by elevated phosphorylation levels of CREB (Genoux et al., 2002). Based on these studies, it was suggested that synaptic weakening negatively affects memory formation and promotes forgetting (assuming that LTP encodes memories). A second view is that both synaptic strengthening and weakening play a crucial role in memory formation (Willshaw and Dayan, 1990; Migaud et al., 1998). Using genetically engineered mice lacking CaN in distinct regions of the forebrain, Zeng et al (2001) showed that CaN is active in situations that require adaptation of previously stored information to match with changes in a familiar environment. In line with this, Runyan and colleagues (2005) showed that intervention of CaN àctivity in the prefrontal cortex impaired the acquisition of consecutive platform locations. Lin et al (2003) in addition revealed that blocking CaN activ-ity in the basolateral amygdala impaired the extinction of previously acquired tone cued fear memories. Overall, these studies indicate that depending on the learning paradigm and ap-proach used, phosphatases can function as a constraint or be necessary for memory formation.

6. AKAP150 regulates the cellular distribution of PKA and CaN

As mentioned above, protein kinases and phosphatases have selective targets in different cellular compartments ranging from receptor subunits in the synaptic ending to gene tran-scription factors in the nucleus. Therefore, it is very important to strictly regulate the intrac-ellular distribution of the protein kinases and phosphatases. One mechanism to orchestrate the localization of kinases and phosphatase is via A-kinase anchoring proteins (AKAPs). The AKAP family consists of at least 50 members with different binding properties and subcellular distributions (Wong and Scott, 2004). One common feature of AKAPs is that they all bind PKA and target it to a specific location within neurons. One of the AKAPs that received particular attention is the mouse AKAP150 (a structural orthologue of the hu-man AKAP79 and bovine AKAP75, Carr et al., 1992). In addition to PKA, AKAP150 binds and targets CaN to specific substrates like the GluR1 subunit and is strongly expressed in brain areas trivial for learning and memory (Glantz et al., 1992; Ostroveanu et al., 2007). AKAP150 is not merely a static protein, but plays an important role in synaptic plasticity since it (at least in part) controls the actions of PKA and CaN. In vitro studies have shown that blocking PKA binding to AKAP150 results in a loss of AMPA receptor surface expression as is observed under conditions of LTD (Snyder et al., 2005). Further-more, it has been reported that, in hippocampal slices, brief NMDA receptor activation leads to persistent removal of AKAP150 and PKA, from post-synaptic membranes to the cytoplasm (Gomez et al., 2002; Smith et al., 2006). In contrast, the same treatment

22

Chapter 1

does not evoke a significant translocation of CaN, suggesting that CaN dissociates from the AKAP150-PKA complex after brief NMDA receptor activation (Smith et al., 2006). Fur-thermore Moita and colleagues (2002) reported that injections with HT-31 (a peptide that inhibits the binding of PKA to AKAPs) in the basolateral amygdala reduced formation of long-term fear memories. Likewhise, Nijholt and colleagues (submitted) showed that in-tra-hippocampal injections with HT-31 inhibited the formation of long-term memories for contextual fear. Overall these studies show that indeed AKAP-150 plays a crucial role in is required for the formation of long-term memories.

7. Sleep and hippocampal synaptic plasticity

Over the years it has become apparent that sleep has many functions including a role in processes underlying memory formation (Stickgold and Walker, 2005; Boonstra et al., 2007). Graves and co-workers (2003) revealed that sleep deprivation given 0-5 hours after training in a contextual fear conditioning paradigm impaired memory consolidation resulting in impaired performance during the retention test given 24 hours after the conditioning. In contrast, sleep deprivation given 6-10 hours after conditioning had no effect on memory consolidation. Furthermore, they showed that sleep deprivation did not interfere with tone-cued learning (a hippocampus-independent version of the task), suggesting that particularly the hippocampus is sensitive to sleep loss. Similar deficits have been found for another learning paradigm that requires the hippocampus (Palchykova et al., 2006a, 2006b). Look-ing at the effect(s) of sleep deprivation on hippocampal functioning at the molecular level, it has been suggested that loss of sleep affects LTP, NMDA receptor composition and surface expression in the hippocampus (Campbell et al., 2002; Davis et al., 2003; McDermott et al., 2003; McDermott et al., 2006; Kopp et al., 2006). In contrast to NMDA receptor function, the effect of sleep loss on AMPA receptor function has received scarce attention.

8. Aim and outline thesis

As described above, the contribution of hippocampal subregions in the detection of changes in a familiar environment has received considerable attention, but still remains unclear. The majority of these studies were based on subregion specific lesions or protein expression pat-terns of immediate early genes that can be activated via several cellular signaling pathways. Therefore, it is unclear which hippocampal signal transduction cascades play a role in the hippocampus in match-mismatch detection. The major aim of this thesis is to investigate whether cellular signaling cascades that are known to be involved in hippocampal memory formation are similarly involved in the detection of and adaptation to changes in a familiar environment (e.g., memory flexibility). Emphasis is given to the activity balance of the pro-tein kinase PKA and the protein phosphatase calcineurin. In the majority of the studies described in this thesis, we used a simple mem-ory paradigm, the Y-maze reference task. The Y maze consists of a start arm and two test arms forming the Y. During training, mice could visit both accessible arms, but

23

Introduction and scope of this thesis

only one of the two arms was baited. With ongoing training, mice progressively learned to locate the baited arm. After the animals learned to discriminate the baited arm, the food reward was relocated to the previously non-baited arm and subjects had to learn that the previously non-baited arm was now baited (reversal training).In chapter 2, we explored the role of hippocampal CaN in the formation of new memories and the adaptation to changes in a familiar environment: Do Y-maze training and reversal training induce changes in hippocampal CaN expression, activity levels and protein levels? In the next set of experiments, we used a transgenic approach that allowed us to temporally suppress CaN activity in forebrain neurons. In chapter 3A, we tested whether reducing neuronal CaN activity in the forebrain altered the rate of acquisition during training and re-versal training in the Y maze. In chapter 3B, the same transgenic mouse model was used as in chapter 3A, but in this study we used an alternative learning task namely contextual fear conditioning paradigm to investigate the role of CaN in learning and memory. Does reducing forebrain CaN activity affect consolidation and extinction of contextual fear memories? Since the phosphorylation state of many substrates is dependent on the balance between PKA and CaN activity, the question remained whether training and reversal training induced changes in hippocampal PKA. Therefore, in chapter 4 we focused on hippocam-pal PKA and explored if training and reversal training in the Y maze changed hippocampal PKA expression and protein levels. Secondly, we investigated if training and reversal training evoked changes in the phosphorylation state of the AMPA receptor GluR1-S845 site that is targeted by both PKA and CaN (Chapter 4). As described in the introduction, the hippocampus is an essential brain structure for the formation of new memories. One of the other brain regions that is also known to be par-ticularly important in reward-motivated learning is the striatum. For that reason, in chapter 5, we studied PKA and CaN protein concentrations as well as AMPA receptor phosphoryla-tion levels at the GluR1-S845 site in the striatum during learning and reversal learning in the Y maze. Although the hippocampus is particularly sensitive to sleep loss, the cellular mecha-nisms underlying sleep deprivation-induced hippocampal dysfunctioning are hardly under-stood. In chapter 6, we investigated the effect of 6 and 12h of sleep deprivation on hip-pocampal AMPA receptor function and the underlying mechanisms. By studying the effect of sleep loss on learning and reversal learning (chapter 7). In this chapter, we applied 5 hours of sleep deprivation directly after each training session and determined if sleep deprivation af-fected the acquisition rate during training and reversal training in the Y maze. In addition, we described changes in the hippocampal expression of the immediate early genes c-Fos, Zif268 and the phosphorylation state of P44/P42 MAPK after Y-maze training with or without sleep deprivation. To further investigate the role of sleep in memory consolidation, in chapter 8 we analyzed the behavioral performance of mice with disturbed sleeping patterns (due to the loss of the Cry1 and Cry2 genes that are a crucial component of the circadian system). In relation to this thesis, the main question of this chapter was: Do disturbed sleeping patterns affect the acquisition rate during learning and reversal learning in the Y-maze? Chapter 9 summarizes and discusses all findings of thesis and provides an overall conclusion and future perspectives.

24

Chapter 1

9. References

Abel T, Nguyen PV, Barad M, Deuel TA, Kandel ER, Bourtchouladze R (1997) Genetic demonstration of a role for PKA in the late phase of LTP and in hippocampus-based long-term memory. Cell 88: 615-626.

Amaral DG, Witter MP (1989) The three-dimensional organization of the hippocampal formation: a review of anatomical data. Neuroscience 31: 571-591.

Amaral DG, Witter MP (1995) The hippocampal formation. In: Paxinos, G. (Ed.), The rat central nervous system. Academic Press, San Diego pp. 443-493.

Atkins CM, Selcher JC, Petraitis JJ, Trzaskos JM, Sweatt JD (1998) The MAPK cascade is required for mammalian associative learning. Nat Neurosci 1: 602-609.

Banke TG, Bowie D, Lee H, Huganir RL, Schousboe A, Traynelis SF (2000) Control of GluR1 AMPA receptor function by cAMP-dependent protein kinase. J Neurosci 20: 89-102.

Barria A, Muller D, Derkach V, Griffith LC, Soderling TR (1997) Regulatory phosphorylation of AMPA-type glutamate receptors by CaM-KII during long-term potentiation. Science 276: 2042-2045.

Bear MF (1999) Homosynaptic long-term depression: a mechanism for memory? Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 96: 9457-9458.

Bito H, Deisseroth K, Tsien RW (1996) CREB phosphorylation and dephosphorylation: a Ca(2+)- and stimulus duration-dependent switch for hippocampal gene expression. Cell 87: 1203-1214.

Bliss TV, Lomo T (1973) Long-lasting potentiation of synaptic transmission in the dentate area of the anaesthetized rabbit following stimulation of the perforant path. J Physiol 232: 331-356.

Blum S, Moore AN, Adams F, Dash PK (1999) A mitogen-activated protein kinase cascade in the CA1/CA2 subfield of the dorsal hippocampus is essential for long-term spatial memory. J Neurosci 19: 3535-3544.

Boehm J, Kang MG, Johnson RC, Esteban J, Huganir RL, Malinow R (2006) Synaptic incorporation of AMPA receptors during LTP is controlled by a PKC phosphorylation site on GluR1. Neuron 51: 213-225.

Boonstra TW, Stins JF, Daffertshofer A, Beek PJ (2007) Effects of sleep deprivation on neural function-ing: an integrative review. Cell Mol Life Sci 64: 934-946.

Brandon EP, Zhuo M, Huang YY, Qi M, Gerhold KA, Burton KA, Kandel ER, McKnight GS, Idzerda RL (1995) Hippocampal long-term depression and depotentiation are defective in mice carrying a targeted disruption of the gene encoding the RI beta subunit of cAMP-dependent protein kinase. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 92: 8851-8855.

25

Introduction and scope of this thesis

Brown TC, Tran IC, Backos DS, Esteban JA (2005) NMDA receptor-dependent activation of the small GTPase Rab5 drives the removal of synaptic AMPA receptors during hippocampal LTD. Neuron 45: 81-94.

Cadd GG, Uhler MD, McKnight GS (1990) Holoenzymes of cAMP-dependent protein kinase contain-ing the neural form of type I regulatory subunit have an increased sensitivity to cyclic nucleotides. J Biol Chem 265: 19502-19506.

Cammarota M, Bevilaqua LR, Ardenghi P, Paratcha G, Levi dS, Izquierdo I, Medina JH (2000) Learn-ing-associated activation of nuclear MAPK, CREB and Elk-1, along with Fos production, in the rat hip-pocampus after a one-trial avoidance learning: abolition by NMDA receptor blockade. Brain Res Mol Brain Res 76: 36-46.

Campbell IG, Guinan MJ, Horowitz JM (2002) Sleep deprivation impairs long-term potentiation in rat hippocampal slices. J Neurophysiol 88: 1073-1076.

Collingridge GL, Kehl SJ, McLennan H (1983) Excitatory amino acids in synaptic transmission in the Schaffer collateral-commissural pathway of the rat hippocampus. J Physiol 334: 33-46.

Cull-Candy S, Brickley S, Farrant M (2001) NMDA receptor subunits: diversity, development and disease. Curr Opin Neurobiol 11: 327-335.

Davis CJ, Harding JW, Wright JW (2003) REM sleep deprivation-induced deficits in the latency-to-peak induction and maintenance of long-term potentiation within the CA1 region of the hippocampus. Brain Res 973: 293-297.

Derkach V, Barria A, Soderling TR (1999) Ca2+/calmodulin-kinase II enhances channel conductance of alpha-amino-3-hydroxy-5-methyl-4-isoxazolepropionate type glutamate receptors. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 96: 3269-3274.Derkach VA, Oh MC, Guire ES, Soderling TR (2007) Regulatory mechanisms of AMPA receptors in synaptic plasticity. Nat Rev Neurosci 8: 101-113.

Dudek SM, Bear MF (1992) Homosynaptic long-term depression in area CA1 of hippocampus and ef-fects of N-methyl-D-aspartate receptor blockade. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 89: 4363-4367.

Ehlers MD (2000) Reinsertion or degradation of AMPA receptors determined by activity-dependent endocytic sorting. Neuron 28: 511-525.

Eliot LS, Dudai Y, Kandel ER, Abrams TW (1989) Ca2+/calmodulin sensitivity may be common to all forms of neural adenylate cyclase. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 86: 9564-9568.

Esteban JA, Shi SH, Wilson C, Nuriya M, Huganir RL, Malinow R (2003) PKA phosphorylation of AMPA receptor subunits controls synaptic trafficking underlying plasticity. Nat Neurosci 6: 136-143.

Etkin A, Alarcon JM, Weisberg SP, Touzani K, Huang YY, Nordheim A, Kandel ER (2006) A role in learn-ing for SRF: deletion in the adult forebrain disrupts LTD and the formation of an immediate memory of a novel context. Neuron 50: 127-143.

26

Chapter 1

Francis SH, Corbin JD (1999) Cyclic nucleotide-dependent protein kinases: intracellular receptors for cAMP and cGMP action. Crit Rev Clin Lab Sci 36: 275-328.

Frey U, Krug M, Reymann KG, Matthies H (1988) Anisomycin, an inhibitor of protein synthesis, blocks late phases of LTP phenomena in the hippocampal CA1 region in vitro. Brain Res 452: 57-65.

Frey U, Huang YY, Kandel ER (1993) Effects of cAMP simulate a late stage of LTP in hippocampal CA1 neurons. Science 260: 1661-1664.

Fyhn M, Molden S, Hollup S, Moser MB, Moser E (2002) Hippocampal neurons responding to first-time dislocation of a target object. Neuron 35: 555-566.

Genoux D, Haditsch U, Knobloch M, Michalon A, Storm D, Mansuy IM (2002) Protein phosphatase 1 is a molecular constraint on learning and memory. Nature 418: 970-975.

Gilbert PE, Kesner RP (2006) The role of the dorsal CA3 hippocampal subregion in spatial working memory and pattern separation. Behav Brain Res 169: 142-149.

Glantz SB, Amat JA, Rubin CS (1992) cAMP signaling in neurons: patterns of neuronal expression and intracellular localization for a novel protein, AKAP 150, that anchors the regulatory subunit of cAMP-dependent protein kinase II beta. Mol Biol Cell 3: 1215-1228.

Gomez LL, Alam S, Smith KE, Horne E, Dell’Acqua ML (2002) Regulation of A-kinase anchoring pro-tein 79/150-cAMP-dependent protein kinase postsynaptic targeting by NMDA receptor activation of calcineurin and remodeling of dendritic actin. J Neurosci 22: 7027-7044.

Gonzalez GA, Montminy MR (1989) Cyclic AMP stimulates somatostatin gene transcription by phos-phorylation of CREB at serine 133. Cell 59: 675-680.

Goodrich-Hunsaker NJ, Hunsaker MR, Kesner RP (2005) Dissociating the role of the parietal cortex and dorsal hippocampus for spatial information processing. Behav Neurosci 119: 1307-1315.

Gothard KM, Skaggs WE, McNaughton BL (1996) Dynamics of mismatch correction in the hippocam-pal ensemble code for space: interaction between path integration and environmental cues. J Neurosci 16: 8027-8040.

Goto S, Matsukado Y, Mihara Y, Inoue N, Miyamoto E (1986) The distribution of calcineurin in rat brain by light and electron microscopic immunohistochemistry and enzyme-immunoassay. Brain Res 397: 161-172.

Graves LA, Heller EA, Pack AI, Abel T (2003) Sleep deprivation selectively impairs memory consolida-tion for contextual fear conditioning. Learn Mem 10: 168-176.

Grewal SS, Fass DM, Yao H, Ellig CL, Goodman RH, Stork PJ (2000) Calcium and cAMP signals dif-ferentially regulate cAMP-responsive element-binding protein function via a Rap1-extracellular signal-regulated kinase pathway. J Biol Chem 275: 34433-34441.

Hebb D (1949) Organization of behavior. John Wiley and Sons.

27

Introduction and scope of this thesis

Hollmann M, Heinemann S (1994) Cloned glutamate receptors. Annu Rev Neurosci 17: 31-108.

Huang FL, Glinsmann WH (1976) Separation and characterization of two phosphorylase phosphatase inhibitors from rabbit skeletal muscle. Eur J Biochem 70: 419-426.

Huang YY, Kandel ER (1994) Recruitment of long-lasting and protein kinase A-dependent long-term potentiation in the CA1 region of hippocampus requires repeated tetanization. Learn Mem 1: 74-82.

Huang YY, Kandel ER, Varshavsky L, Brandon EP, Qi M, Idzerda RL, McKnight GS, Bourtchouladze R (1995) A genetic test of the effects of mutations in PKA on mossy fiber LTP and its relation to spatial and contextual learning. Cell 83: 1211-1222.

Ikegami S, Inokuchi K (2000) Antisense DNA against calcineurin facilitates memory in contextual fear conditioning by lowering the threshold for hippocampal long-term potentiation induction. Neuro-science 98: 637-646.

Ishizuka N, Weber J, Amaral DG (1990) Organization of intrahippocampal projections originating from CA3 pyramidal cells in the rat. J Comp Neurol 295: 580-623.

Isiegas, C., Park, A., Kandel, E. R., Abel, T., & Lattal, K. M. (2006). Transgenic inhibition of neuronal protein kinase A activity facilitates fear extinction. Journal of Neuroscience, 26, 12700-12707.

Jenkins TA, Amin E, Pearce JM, Brown MW, Aggleton JP (2004) Novel spatial arrangements of familiar visual stimuli promote activity in the rat hippocampal formation but not the parahippocampal cortices: a c-fos expression study. Neuroscience 124: 43-52.

Johnston D, Amaral DG (1997) The synaptic organization of the brain. In: Stepherd GM, editor Hip-pocampus New York: Oxford university Press. 417-458

Kameyama K, Lee HK, Bear MF, Huganir RL (1998) Involvement of a postsynaptic protein kinase A substrate in the expression of homosynaptic long-term depression. Neuron 21: 1163-1175.

Kauderer BS, Kandel ER (2000) Capture of a protein synthesis-dependent component of long-term depression. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 97: 13342-13347.

Kimble DP (1963) The effects of bilateral hippocampal lesions in rats. J Comp Physiol Psychol 56: 273-283.

Klee CB, Crouch TH, Krinks MH (1979) Calcineurin: a calcium- and calmodulin- binding protein of the central nervous system. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 76: 6270-6273

Klee CB, Ren H, Wang X (1998) Regulation of the calmodulin-stimulated protein phosphatase, cal-cineurin. J Biol Chem 273: 13367-13370.

Knight R (1996) Contribution of human hippocampal region to novelty detection. Nature 383: 256-259.

Koh MT, Bernstein IL (2003) Inhibition of protein kinase A activity during conditioned taste aversion retrieval: interference with extinction or reconsolidation of a memory? Neuroreport 14: 405-407.

28

Chapter 1

Kopp C, Longordo F, Nicholson JR, Lünthi A (2006) Insufficient sleep reversibly alters bidirectional synaptic plasticity and NMDA receptor function. 26: 12456- 12465.

Krupp JJ, Vissel B, Thomas CG, Heinemann SF, Westbrook GL (2002) Calcineurin acts via the C-termi-nus of NR2A to modulate desensitization of NMDA receptors. Neuropharmacology 42: 593-602.

Lee HK, Kameyama K, Huganir RL, Bear MF (1998) NMDA induces long-term synaptic depression and dephosphorylation of the GluR1 subunit of AMPA receptors in hippocampus. Neuron 21: 1151-1162.

Lee HK, Barbarosie M, Kameyama K, Bear MF, Huganir RL (2000) Regulation of distinct AMPA receptor phosphorylation sites during bidirectional synaptic plasticity. Nature 405: 955-959.

Lee HK, Kameyama K, Yu S, Rosetti L, He C, et al (2002) Identification of a novel phosphorylation site on the GluR1 subunit of AMPA receptors. Soc Neurosci 747.14

Lee HK (2006) Synaptic plasticity and phosphorylation. Pharmacol Ther 112: 810-832.

Lee I, Rao G, Knierim JJ (2004) A double dissociation between hippocampal subfields: differential time course of CA3 and CA1 place cells for processing changed environments. Neuron 42: 803-815.

Lee I, Hunsaker MR, Kesner RP (2005) The role of hippocampal subregions in detecting spatial novelty. Behav Neurosci 119: 145-153.

Leutgeb JK, Leutgeb S, Moser, M Moser EI (2007) Pattern separation in the dentate gyrus and CA3 of the hippocampus. Science 315: 691-695.

Lieberman DN, Mody I (1994) Regulation of NMDA channel function by endogenous Ca(2+)-depend-ent phosphatase. Nature 369: 235-239.

Lin CH, Yeh SH, Leu TH, Chang WC, Wang ST, Gean PW (2003) Identification of calcineurin as a key signal in the extinction of fear memory. J Neurosci 23: 1574-1579.

Lisman JE, Otmakhova NA (2001) Storage, recall, and novelty detection of sequences by the hippoc-ampus: elaborating on the SOCRATIC model to account for normal and aberrant effects of dopamine. Hippocampus 11: 551-568.

Malleret G, Haditsch U, Genoux D, Jones MW, Bliss TV, Vanhoose AM, Weitlauf C, Kandel ER, Winder DG, Mansuy IM (2001) Inducible and reversible enhancement of learning, memory, and long-term potentiation by genetic inhibition of calcineurin. Cell 104: 675-686.

Mansuy IM, Mayford M, Jacob B, Kandel ER, Bach ME (1998) Restricted and regulated overexpression reveals calcineurin as a key component in the transition from short-term to long-term memory. Cell 92: 39-49.

Matthies H, Reymann KG (1993) Protein kinase A inhibitors prevent the maintenance of hippocampal long-term potentiation. Neuroreport 4: 712-714.

Mayer ML, Westbrook GL, Guthrie PB (1984) Voltage-dependent block by Mg2+ of NMDA responses in spinal cord neurones. Nature 309: 261-263.

29

Introduction and scope of this thesis

McDermott CM, LaHoste GJ, Chen C, Musto A, Bazan NG, Magee JC (2003) Sleep deprivation causes behavioral, synaptic, and membrane excitability alterations in hippocampal neurons. J Neurosci 23: 9687-9695.

McDermott CM, Hardy MN, Bazan NG, Magee JC (2006) Sleep deprivation-induced alterations in excitatory synaptic transmission in the CA1 region of the rat hippocampus. J Physiol 570: 553-565.

McHugh TJ, Jones MW, Quinn JJ, Balthasar N, Coppari R, Elmquist JK, Lowell BB, Fanselow MS, Wilson MA, Tonegawa S (2007) Dentate Gyrus NMDA Receptors Mediate Rapid Pattern Separation in the Hippocampal Network. Science.

Migaud M, Charlesworth P, Dempster M, Webster LC, Watabe AM, Makhinson M, He Y, Ramsay MF, Morris RG, Morrison JH, O’Dell TJ, Grant SG (1998) Enhanced long-term potentiation and impaired learning in mice with mutant postsynaptic density-95 protein. Nature 396: 433-439.

Moita MA, Lamprecht R, Nader K, LeDoux JE (2002) A-kinase anchoring proteins in amygdala are involved in auditory fear memory. Nat Neurosci 5: 837-838.

Morozov A, Muzzio IA, Bourtchouladze R, Van Strien N, Lapidus K, Yin D, Winder DG, Adams JP, Sweatt JD, Kandel ER (2003) Rap1 couples cAMP signaling to a distinct pool of p42/44MAPK regulating excitability, synaptic plasticity, learning, and memory. Neuron 39: 309-325.

Mulkey RM, Malenka RC (1992) Mechanisms underlying induction of homosynaptic long-term depres-sion in area CA1 of the hippocampus. Neuron 9: 967-975.

Nairn AC, Shenolikar S (1992) The role of protein phosphatases in synaptic transmission, plasticity and neuronal development. Curr Opin Neurobiol 2: 296-301.

Nakanishi S (1992) Molecular diversity of glutamate receptors and implications for brain function. Sci-ence 258: 597-603.

Nakazawa K, Sun LD, Quirk MC, Rondi-Reig L, Wilson MA, Tonegawa S (2003) Hippocampal CA3 NMDA receptors are crucial for memory acquisition of one-time experience. Neuron 38: 305-315.

Nguyen PV, Abel T, Kandel ER (1994) Requirement of a critical period of transcription for induction of a late phase of LTP. Science 265: 1104-1107.

Nguyen PV, Kandel ER (1996) A macromolecular synthesis-dependent late phase of long-term po-tentiation requiring cAMP in the medial perforant pathway of rat hippocampal slices. J Neurosci 16: 3189-3198.

Nicoll RA, Malenka RC (1999) Expression mechanisms underlying NMDA receptor-dependent long-term potentiation. Ann N Y Acad Sci 868: 515-525.

Nijholt IM, Ostroveanu A, Luiten PGM, Van der Zee EA, Eisel UL. Inhibition of PKA anchoring to A-kinase anchoring proteins impairs the consolidation of contextual fear memories. Submitted.

30

Chapter 1

Nyakas C, Luiten PG, Balkan B, Spencer DG (1988) Changes in septo-hippocampal projections after lateral entorhinal or combined entorhinal-raphé lesions as studied by anterograde tracing methods. Brain Res Bul 21:285-293.

Olton DS, Papas BC (1979) Spatial memory and hippocampal function. Neuropsychologia 17: 669-682.

Ostroveanu A, Van der Zee EA, Dolga AM, Luiten PG, Eisel UL, Nijholt IM (2007) A-kinase anchoring protein 150 in the mouse brain is concentrated in areas involved in learning and memory. Brain Res 1145: 97-107.

Palchykova S, Winsky-Sommerer R, Meerlo P, Durr R, Tobler I (2006) Sleep deprivation impairs object recognition in mice. Neurobiol Learn Mem 85: 263-271.

Palchykova S, Crestani F, Meerlo P, Tobler I (2006) Sleep deprivation and daily torpor impair object recognition in Djungarian hamsters. Physiol Behav 87: 144-153.

Phillips RG, LeDoux JE (1992) Differential contribution of amygdala and hippocampus to cued and contextual fear conditioning. Behav Neurosci 106: 274-285.

Roberson ED, English JD, Adams JP, Selcher JC, Kondratick C, Sweatt JD (1999). The mitogen-activat-ed protein cascade couples PKA and PKC to cAMP response element binding protein phosphorylation in area CA1 of hippocampus. J Neurosci 19: 4337-4348.

Qi M, Zhuo M, Skalhegg BS, Brandon EP, Kandel ER, McKnight GS, Idzerda RL (1996) Impaired hip-pocampal plasticity in mice lacking the Cbeta1 catalytic subunit of cAMP-dependent protein kinase. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 93: 1571-1576.

Quevedo J, Vianna MR, Martins MR, Barichello T, Medina JH, Roesler R, Izquierdo I (2004) Protein synthesis, PKA, and MAP kinase are differentially involved in short- and long-term memory in rats. Behav Brain Res 154: 339-343.

Rolls ET, Kesner RP (2006) A computational theory of hippocampal function, and empirical tests of the theory. Prog Neurobiol 79: 1-48.

Runyan JD, Moore AN, Dash PK (2005) A role for prefrontal calcium-sensitive protein phosphatase and kinase activities in working memory. Learn Mem 12: 103-110.

Rusnak F, Mertz P (2000) Calcineurin: form and function. Physiol Rev 80: 1483-1521.

Schaeffer HJ, Weber MJ (1999) Mitogen-activated protein kinases: specific messages from ubiquitous messengers. Mol Cell Biol 19: 2435-2444.

Schafe GE, Nadel NV, Sullivan GM, Harris A, LeDoux JE (1999) Memory consolidation for contextual and auditory fear conditioning is dependent on protein synthesis, PKA, and MAP kinase. Learn Mem 6: 97-110.

31

Introduction and scope of this thesis

Schafe GE, LeDoux JE (2000) Memory consolidation of auditory pavlovian fear conditioning requires protein synthesis and protein kinase A in the amygdala. J Neurosci 20: RC96.

Schenk F, Morris RG (1985) Dissociation between components of spatial memory in rats after recovery from the effects of retrohippocampal lesions. Exp Brain Res 58: 11-28.

Segal RA, Greenberg ME (1996) Intracellular signaling pathways activated by neurotrophic factors. Annu Rev Neurosci 19: 463-489.

Selcher JC, Atkins CM, Trzaskos JM, Paylor R, Sweatt JD (1999) A necessity for MAP kinase activation in mammalian spatial learning. Learn Mem 6: 478-490.

Sheng M, Thompson MA, Greenberg ME (1991) CREB: a Ca(2+)-regulated transcription factor phos-phorylated by calmodulin-dependent kinases. Science 252: 1427-1430.

Sik A, Hajos N, Gulacsi A, Mody I, Freund TF (1998) The absence of a major Ca2+ signaling pathway in GABAergic neurons of the hippocampus. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 95: 3245-3250.

Silva AJ, Kogan JH, Frankland PW, Kida S (1998) CREB and memory. Annu Rev Neurosci 21: 127-148.

Skalhegg BS, Tasken K (2000) Specificity in the cAMP/PKA signaling pathway. Differential expression,regulation, and subcellular localization of subunits of PKA. Front Biosci 5: D678-D693.

Smith KE, Gibson ES, Dell’Acqua ML (2006) cAMP-dependent protein kinase postsynaptic localization regulated by NMDA receptor activation through translocation of an A-kinase anchoring protein scaf-fold protein. J Neurosci 26: 2391-2402.

Snyder EM, Colledge M, Crozier RA, Chen WS, Scott JD, Bear MF (2005) Role for A kinase-anchoring proteins (AKAPS) in glutamate receptor trafficking and long term synaptic depression. J Biol Chem 280: 16962-16968.

Song I, Huganir RL (2002) Regulation of AMPA receptors during synaptic plasticity. Trends Neurosci 25: 578-588.

Squire LR (1992) Memory and the hippocampus: a synthesis from findings with rats, monkeys, and humans. Psychol Rev 99: 195-231.

Stent GS (1973) A physiological mechanism for Hebb’s postulate of learning. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 70: 997-1001.

Stickgold R, Walker MP (2005) Memory consolidation and reconsolidation: what is the role of sleep? Trends Neurosci 28: 408-415.

Sweatt JD (2001) The neuronal MAP kinase cascade: a biochemical signal integration system subserv-ing synaptic plasticity and memory. J Neurochem 76: 1-10.

Tang WJ, Gilman AG (1991) Type-specific regulation of adenylyl cyclase by G protein beta gamma subunits. Science 254: 1500-1503.

32

Chapter 1

Tasken K, Skalhegg BS, Solberg R, Andersson KB, Taylor SS, Lea T, Blomhoff HK, Jahnsen T, Hansson V (1993) Novel isozymes of cAMP-dependent protein kinase exist in human cells due to formation of RI alpha-RI beta heterodimeric complexes. J Biol Chem 268: 21276-21283.

Thiels E, Kanterewicz BI, Norman ED, Trzaskos JM, Klann E (2002) Long-term depression in the adult hippocampus in vivo involves activation of extracellular signal-regulated kinase and phosphorylation of Elk-1. J Neurosci 22: 2054-2062.

Tingley WG, Ehlers MD, Kameyama K, Doherty C, Ptak JB, Riley CT, Huganir RL (1997) Characteriza-tion of protein kinase A and protein kinase C phosphorylation of the N-methyl-D-aspartate receptor NR1 subunit using phosphorylation site-specific antibodies. J Biol Chem 272: 5157-5166.

Tong G, Shepherd D, Jahr CE (1995) Synaptic desensitization of NMDA receptors by calcineurin. Sci-ence 267: 1510-1512.

Wagner KR, Mei L, Huganir RL (1991) Protein tyrosine kinases and phosphatases in the nervous sys-tem. Curr Opin Neurobiol 1: 65-73.

Wan H, Aggleton JP, Brown MW (1999) Different contributions of the hippocampus and perirhinal cortex to recognition memory. J Neurosci 19: 1142-1148.

Wang JQ, Fibuch EE. Mao L (2007) Regulatin of mitogen-activated protein kinases by glutamate recep-tors. J Neurochem 100:1-11.

Wera S, Hemmings BA (1995) Serine/threonine protein phosphatases. Biochem J 311 ( Pt 1): 17-29.Whitlock JR, Heynen AJ, Shuler MG, Bear MF (2006) Learning induces long-term potentiation in the hippocampus. Science 313: 1093-1097.

Willshaw DJ, Dayan P (1990) Optimal plasticity from matrix memories: what goes up must come down. Neural Comput 2: 85-93.

Witter MP (1993) Organization of the entorhinal-hippocampal system: a review of current anatomical data. Hippocampus 3 Spec No: 33-44.

Wong W, Scott JD (2004) AKAP signalling complexes: focal points in space and time. Nat Rev Mol Cell Biol 5: 959-970.

Woo NH, Duffy SN, Abel T, Nguyen PV (2000) Genetic and pharmacological demonstration of differ-ential recruitment of cAMP-dependent protein kinases by synaptic activity. J Neurophysiol 84: 2739-2745.

Woo NH, Abel T, Nguyen PV (2002) Genetic and pharmacological demonstration of a role for cyclic AMP-dependent protein kinase-mediated suppression of protein phosphatases in gating the expres-sion of late LTP. Eur J Neurosci 16: 1871-1876.

Woo NH, Duffy SN, Abel T, Nguyen PV (2003) Temporal spacing of synaptic stimulation critically mod-ulates the dependence of LTP on cyclic AMP-dependent protein kinase. Hippocampus 13: 293-300.

33

Introduction and scope of this thesis

Wu ZL, Thomas SA, Villacres EC, Xia Z, Simmons ML, Chavkin C, Palmiter RD, Storm DR (1995) Altered behavior and long-term potentiation in type I adenylyl cyclase mutant mice. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 92: 220-224.

Xing J, Ginty DD, Greenberg ME (1996) Coupling of the RAS-MAPK pathway to gene activation by RSK2, a growth factor-regulated CREB kinase. Science 273: 959-963.

Yamamoto KK, Gonzalez GA, Biggs WH, III, Montminy MR (1988) Phosphorylation-induced binding and transcriptional efficacy of nuclear factor CREB. Nature 334: 494-498.

Zeng H, Chattarji S, Barbarosie M, Rondi-Reig L, Philpot BD, Miyakawa T, Bear MF, Tonegawa S (2001) Forebrain-specific calcineurin knockout selectively impairs bidirectional synaptic plasticity and work-ing/episodic-like memory. Cell 107: 617-629.

34