Top 100 University | Rijksuniversiteit Groningen - University of...

177
University of Groningen Efficacy of exercise for functional outcomes in older persons with dementia Sanders, Lianne DOI: 10.33612/diss.102146202 IMPORTANT NOTE: You are advised to consult the publisher's version (publisher's PDF) if you wish to cite from it. Please check the document version below. Document Version Publisher's PDF, also known as Version of record Publication date: 2019 Link to publication in University of Groningen/UMCG research database Citation for published version (APA): Sanders, L. (2019). Efficacy of exercise for functional outcomes in older persons with dementia. Rijksuniversiteit Groningen. https://doi.org/10.33612/diss.102146202 Copyright Other than for strictly personal use, it is not permitted to download or to forward/distribute the text or part of it without the consent of the author(s) and/or copyright holder(s), unless the work is under an open content license (like Creative Commons). Take-down policy If you believe that this document breaches copyright please contact us providing details, and we will remove access to the work immediately and investigate your claim. Downloaded from the University of Groningen/UMCG research database (Pure): http://www.rug.nl/research/portal. For technical reasons the number of authors shown on this cover page is limited to 10 maximum. Download date: 09-07-2021

Transcript of Top 100 University | Rijksuniversiteit Groningen - University of...

  • University of Groningen

    Efficacy of exercise for functional outcomes in older persons with dementiaSanders, Lianne

    DOI:10.33612/diss.102146202

    IMPORTANT NOTE: You are advised to consult the publisher's version (publisher's PDF) if you wish to cite fromit. Please check the document version below.

    Document VersionPublisher's PDF, also known as Version of record

    Publication date:2019

    Link to publication in University of Groningen/UMCG research database

    Citation for published version (APA):Sanders, L. (2019). Efficacy of exercise for functional outcomes in older persons with dementia.Rijksuniversiteit Groningen. https://doi.org/10.33612/diss.102146202

    CopyrightOther than for strictly personal use, it is not permitted to download or to forward/distribute the text or part of it without the consent of theauthor(s) and/or copyright holder(s), unless the work is under an open content license (like Creative Commons).

    Take-down policyIf you believe that this document breaches copyright please contact us providing details, and we will remove access to the work immediatelyand investigate your claim.

    Downloaded from the University of Groningen/UMCG research database (Pure): http://www.rug.nl/research/portal. For technical reasons thenumber of authors shown on this cover page is limited to 10 maximum.

    Download date: 09-07-2021

    https://doi.org/10.33612/diss.102146202https://research.rug.nl/en/publications/efficacy-of-exercise-for-functional-outcomes-in-older-persons-with-dementia(68bdc914-594e-4c47-882b-3769f9662a1e).htmlhttps://doi.org/10.33612/diss.102146202

  • Efficacy of exercise for functional outcomes in older persons

    with dementia

  • The randomized controlled trial described in this thesis was conducted at health care centers

    of ZINN, Dignis, Meriant, TSN Thuiszorg and NNCZ.

    Financial support for the research described in this thesis, and printing of this thesis, was

    provided by the Deltaplan Dementia (ZonMW: Memorabel, project number 733050303), the

    University of Groningen, the University Medical Center Groningen, Alzheimer Nederland,

    the Hersenstichting, and the Research Institute School of Health Research (SHARE).

    Cover design: Lianne Sanders, background graphics ©EdNal@Adobe StockLayout: Bastiaan Sanders and Lianne Sanders using LATEXPrint: Gildeprint, EnschedeISBN (printed version): 978-94-034-2067-7ISBN (electronic version): 978-94-034-2066-0

    PhD training was facilitated by the Research Institute School of Health Research (SHARE).

    Paranymphs: Menno Veldman and Laura Cuijpers

    ©Lianne Sanders, 2019.

    All rights reserved. No part of this book may be reproduced or transmitted in any form or by

    any means, electronic or mechanical, including photocopying, recording, or any information

    storage or retrieval systems, without the prior written permission from the author.

  • Efficacy of exercise for functional outcomes in older persons with

    dementia

    Proefschrift

    ter verkrijging van de graad van doctor aan de Rijksuniversiteit Groningen

    op gezag van de rector magnificus prof. dr. C. Wijmenga

    en volgens besluit van het College voor Promoties.

    De openbare verdediging zal plaatsvinden op

    maandag 9 december 2019 om 12:45 uur

    door

    Lianne Maria Jantien Sanders

    geboren op 2 november 1990 te Apeldoorn

  • Promotores Prof. dr. T. Hortobagyi

    Prof. dr. E.J.A. Scherder

    Prof. dr. E.A. van der Zee

    Copromotor Dr. M.J.G. van Heuvelen

    Beoordelingscommissie Prof. dr. G.J.J.M. Kempen

    Prof. dr. W.H. Brouwer

    Prof. dr. B.C. van Munster

  • Table of Contents

    Foreword 7Chapter 1 General introduction 13Chapter 2 Relationship between drug burden and physical and 27

    cognitive function in a sample of nursing home patients

    with dementia

    Chapter 3 Dose-response relationship between exercise and 49cognitive function in older adults with and without

    cognitive impairment: a systematic review and

    meta-analysis

    Chapter 4 Low- and high-intensity physical exercise has 87small effects on physical but not on cognitive function

    in older persons with dementia: a randomized

    controlled trial

    Chapter 5 Psychometric properties of a Flanker task in a sample 125of patients with dementia: a pilot study

    Chapter 6 Summary and general discussion 143Appendices

    Abstract 163

    Nederlandse samenvatting 164

    Dankwoord 168

    About the author 171

    Research Institute SHARE 175

  • ��������

  • 8

  • This research is performed within the Deltaplan Dementia as part of the program Train the

    Sedentary Brain: Move Smart and Reduce the Risk of Dementia (ZonMW:Memorabel, project

    number 733050303) executed by a consortium in which researchers from the University

    of Groningen, Vrije Universiteit Amsterdam and Radboudumc participate. The Deltaplan

    Dementia is a Dutch multidisciplinary collaborative initiative with the aim of improving

    dementia health care in the Netherlands and abroad. The focus of the Deltaplan Dementia lies

    on three core areas. These are 1) scientific research (the Memorabel program), 2) improving

    dementia care practice, and 3) social innovation to stimulate a dementia-friendly society.

    More information on the Deltaplan Dementia can be found on https://deltaplandementie.nl/nl.

    Within the Memorabel research program, the Train the Sedentary Brain (TTSB) program

    aims to investigate how specific exercise protocols can counter the negative effects of physical

    inactivity on the progression of dementia. An overview of the studies within TTSB is given in

    Figure 1. Together, these studies generate insight into the deleterious effects of physical inac-

    tivity on cognitive function, the interaction between physical inactivity and Apolipoprotein ε4

    (ApoE4, themost important biological risk factor for dementia), and the efficacy of physical ac-

    tivity protocols for physical and cognitive function. These insights are obtained using preclin-

    ical mouse models and clinical protocols. More information on the TTSB program including

    study reports can be obtained from https://www.zonmw.nl/nl/over-zonmw/ehealth-en-ict-in-

    de-zorg/programmas/project-detail/memorabel/train-the-sedentary-brain-move-smart-to-reduce-

    the-risk-of-dementia/.

    9

  • Figu

    re1.

    Interrelationships

    betweenthestu

    dies.

    10

  • 11

  • 12

  • �����������������������������

  • 14

  • Aging and dementia

    The world population is growing older. Between 2010 and 2015, the global life expectancy

    has risen by 5.5 years, mainly due to improvements in child survival rates in developing

    regions. Today, the global average life expectancy is ∼74 years for women and ∼70 years formen. In developed regions, the life expectancy for women and men approaches or exceeds 80

    years [1].

    In 2010 it was estimated that there were 524 million old adults (≥65 years). This numberis expected to triple by 2050 [2]. Considering that old age is the most important risk factor

    for dementia, it is expected that by 2050 the number of older persons with dementia (PwD)

    will triple to 150 million worldwide [3]. Dementia is a syndrome that is characterized by

    progressive neurodegeneration that surpasses normal age-related decline. PwD progressively

    lose cognitive and physical abilities [3,4] and become increasingly dependent upon formal

    and informal care. Dementia may have various causes, and it must be noted that at least

    50% of PwD show signs of multiple dementia pathologies [5]. Alzheimer’s Disease (AD)

    accounts for 60-80% of all dementia cases [6]. AD is characterized by deposits of Amyloid β

    (Aβ) plaques and neurofibrillary tangles. Vascular Dementia (VaD), i.e., dementia resulting

    from extensive damage to brain blood vessels appears in ∼40% of PwD [6]. Dementia withLewy Bodies (LBD), characterized by neuronal abnormalities in Alpha-synuclein protein, is

    recognized as the third most common cause for dementia, accounting for another >10% of

    dementia cases [6].

    It is estimated that 1/3 of dementia cases are attributable to a combination of nine modifi-

    able risk factors: low education in early life; hypertension, obesity and hearing loss in midlife;

    and smoking, depression, physical inactivity, social isolation and diabetes in later life [7]. The

    most important biological risk factor for dementia (AD in particular) is the Apolipoprotein ε4

    (ApoE4) allele. ApoE4 is directly linked to AD through impaired lipoprotein metabolism [8],

    which is associated with immunoreactive accumulations of Aβ and tau pathology and sub-

    sequent declines in neurocognitive health [9,10]. Compared with ApoE2 and/or E3 carriers,

    ApoE4 heterozygotes are three times more likely to develop AD and ApoE4 homozygotes are

    eight times more likely to develop AD [9].

    The economic burden of dementia is high. Costs for prevention, diagnosis, symptom

    treatment, informal care and environmental adaptations for PwD increasingly accumulate

    with time. In 2018, the global cost of dementia exceeded $1 trillion [3]. Considering the

    high societal and economic burden of dementia it is of great importance that treatments are

    15

  • developed. Pharmacological treatments are still insufficient in producing clinically relevant

    effects [11] and frequently cause side effects such as gastrointestinal issues and vertigo

    [12]. Non-pharmacological treatments for dementia may pose fewer side-effects and include

    exercise, music therapy, cognitive stimulation, social stimulation and sensory enrichment.

    Amongst these, exercise may be the preferred choice as it is consistently associated with

    better physical and mental health outcomes in older populations [13] conform the philosophy

    ’Exercise is medicine’.

    Physical activity in old adults with and without dementia

    The American College of Sports Medicine (ACSM) defines physical activity (PA) as ‘any

    bodilymovement produced by skeletal muscles that results in energy expenditure above resting

    levels’ [14]. Exercise is defined as ‘PA that is planned, structured, and repetitive and that has

    as a final or intermediate objective the improvement or maintenance of physical fitness’. In

    other words, exercise is PA that is specifically intended to improve fitness.

    The WHO [13] advises all old adults to perform ≥150 minutes of moderate-intensityaerobic PA and/or ≥75 minutes of vigorous-intensity aerobic PA, in bouts of ≥10 minutes,every week. Whole-body muscle strengthening exercises should be done at least two days

    per week. Especially old adults with mobility impairments are advised to perform balance

    exercises on ≥3 days of the week. For old adults with poor physical health, even low amountsof PA are beneficial. However, a review of PA levels in old adults from six continents

    reported that only 20-60% of old adults met the recommended amounts of PA [15]. PwD

    are even less active as compared to their healthy peers. Van Alphen and colleagues [16]

    showed that community-dwelling PwD are ∼22% less active than cognitively healthy peers.Institutionalized PwD even spend ∼72% of their day sitting. Low levels of PA are associatedwith poor health, increased risk ofmortality and hospitalization and increased risk of cognitive

    impairment [17]. Increasing PA may ameliorate poor health in PwD.

    Exercise and functional outcomes in old adults with and without dementia

    In healthy old adults, aerobic and strength exercise are associated with improvements in

    physical function such as endurance, mobility, gait speed, muscle strength and balance [18-

    21]. Multimodal exercise, i.e., a combination of aerobic, strength, and coordination training

    has the highest efficacy for physical function. Likewise, the beneficial effects of exercise on the

    16

  • aforementioned physical functions are apparent in PwD with multimodal exercise having the

    highest efficacy (see [22] for a review). Furthermore, multimodal exercise may be preferable

    over aerobic-only exercise for Activities of Daily Life (ADL) in PwD [23].

    Exercise-induced improvements in physical function may facilitate cognitive function

    through increases in brain plasticity and activation [24-26]. In 1999, Arthur Kramer and

    colleagues were among the first to show a positive effect of exercise on executive processes

    in a sample of 124 healthy but sedentary old adults [27]. Since then there has been a growing

    body of evidence that aerobic, anaerobic and multimodal exercise is related to better cognitive

    function in healthy old adults, with multimodal exercise having the highest efficacy for

    cognitive function (see [28] for a meta-analysis). In PwD, the effects of exercise on cognition

    are inconclusive [29-33]. A combination of alternating aerobic and strength exercise appears

    to be the most beneficial for physical and cognitive function [23,30] perhaps because 1)

    aerobic exercise is facilitated through strength increases especially in the lower limbs and 2)

    the neuromotor stimulus is higher in combined exercise compared with aerobic- only training

    due to compensatory mechanisms. However, it is unknown whether such a combination is

    also effective in PwD in earlier stages of dementia. Furthermore, it is unknown whether this

    combination of alternating aerobic and strength training is effective when performed for a

    longer period of time. Last, it is uncertain whether the beneficial effects of exercise in PwD

    last after detraining.

    Several underlying neurobiological mechanisms may play a role in the neuroprotective

    effects of exercise. In animals and healthy old adults, exercise is related to dose-dependent in-

    creases in growth factors (insulin-like growth factor type I (IGF1), brain derived neurotrophic

    factor (BDNF), vascular endothelial growth factor (VEGF)), increases in neurotransmitters

    (noradrenalin, serotonin, dopamine) and decreases in inflammatory markers (homocysteine

    [34,35]). These neurobiological changes are related to changes in brain structure and con-

    nectivity [36-39] in areas important for healthy cognitive function e.g. frontal and temporal

    areas. Whether this is also true for PwD is yet to be determined by future studies. Figure 1

    models the relationships between exercise and physical and cognitive function in PwD. This

    model is adapted from Bossers [40].

    17

  • Figure 1. Relationships between exercise, physical function and cognition in PwD. Continu-ous lines represent evidence-based relationships whereas dotted lines represent hypothesized

    relationships.

    Moderators and confounders of exercise effects on physical and cognitive function inPwD

    As previously outlined, the evidence for the efficacy of exercise for functional outcomes in

    PwD is inconclusive. Furthermore, there is insufficient evidence to determine the variables

    that may act as moderators and confounders of exercise effects on physical and cognitive

    function in PwD. Such data are needed to optimize exercise programs for PwD and implement

    18

  • exercise efficiently in daily health care. Previous reviews and meta-analyses in healthy old

    adults have identified several potential moderators and confounders. The effects of exercise

    on executive processes may be more pronounced in older women vs. men [41] and in older vs.

    younger seniors [42]. In addition, the effects of exercise on cognitive function in old adults

    may bemoderated by genes and dietary factors [43]. In PwD, the effects of exercise on physical

    and cognitive function appear to be independent of sex, age and dementia subtype, although

    it is uncertain whether the effects of exercise are moderated by disease severity [22,32,33,44].

    There is little or inconclusive evidence on several other potential moderators and confounders

    of exercise effects in PwD. Use of medications with anticholinergic and/or sedative properties

    (‘inappropriate medications’) as measured by the Drug Burden Index (DBI) is associated

    with lower physical and cognitive function in healthy old adults [45-48]. Whether this is true

    also for PwD is unknown. Data on the associations between DBI and physical and cognitive

    function in PwD is needed to determine whether the effects of exercise on physical and

    cognitive function are potentially confounded by anticholinergic and sedative drug burden.

    In addition to drug burden, exercise type and dose-parameters (program duration, session

    duration, frequency and intensity) may moderate the magnitude of exercise effects on physical

    and cognitive function in PwD. However, the dose-response relationships between exercise

    with physical and cognitive function in PwD are poorly understood. Especially exercise

    intensity may be an important moderator of exercise effects because higher exercise intensity

    is associated with better physical fitness (i.e., VO2max) and health outcomes [49-52] that may

    fuel exercise effects on physical and cognitive function. Studies in which exercise intensities

    are compared among randomized PwD are needed to investigate exercise intensity as potential

    moderator in PwD. No such studies have been performed thus far. Last, preliminary evidence

    has identified ApoE4-carriership to be a potential moderator of exercise effects on functional

    outcomes in old adults with and without dementia [43,53]. However, it is uncertain whether

    ApoE4 carriers as compared to non-carriers show greater or fewer benefits from exercise

    on physical and cognitive function [53-56]. Furthermore, there is a scarcity of randomized

    studies that investigate the strength and direction of this hypothesized moderation in PwD

    specifically.

    The effects of exercise on physical and cognitive function in PwD and potential moderators

    and confounders can only be adequately assessed with performance-based tests that are

    feasible, valid and reliable in PwD. The reliability of six motor tests for endurance, gait speed,

    balance, strength and functional mobility was previously found to be good to excellent in PwD,

    19

  • although the reliability was lower for lower-functioning individuals [57]. Unfortunately, apart

    from global cognitive batteries, there is limited data on the psychometric properties of many

    cognitive tests in PwD. Furthermore, cognitive tests that are frequently used in healthy old

    adults may not be feasible, reliable and valid in PwD. This may be especially true for the

    STROOP task. The STROOP task measures selective attention and inhibitory control [58].

    The STROOP taskmay be difficult for PwDbecause PwDmay experience color confusion [59]

    and difficulties with verbal communication [60]. Furthermore, PwD may find the STROOP

    task instructions difficult to comprehend. The Flanker task could be a suitable non-verbal

    alternative for the STROOP task in PwD. The Flanker task requires the ability to inhibit

    nonrelevant competing responses to a nonverbal target stimulus [61]. The Flanker task may

    be a suitable alternative to the STROOP task in PwD because the Flanker task does not rely

    upon verbal responses and the use of colors. However, the psychometric properties of the

    Flanker task in PwD are yet to be evaluated.

    Objectives and outline of this thesis

    Themain objective of this thesis is to examine the efficacy of alternating aerobic and lower-limb

    strength exercise vs. control activities for physical and cognitive function in a sample of older

    persons with mild-to-moderate all-cause dementia. The secondary objective of this thesis is

    to examine potential moderators and confounders of exercise effects in PwD. In this thesis

    we will focus on anticholinergic and sedative drug burden, exercise type, dose-parameters

    (program duration, session duration, frequency and intensity) and ApoE4-carriership. The

    last objective of this thesis is to examine the suitability of a Flanker task as a measure of

    inhibitory control in PwD.

    Chapter 2 describes the results of a cross-sectional analysis into the associations betweenanticholinergic and sedative drug burden with physical and cognition function in 140 nursing

    home PwD. Chapter 3 describes the results of a systematic review and meta-analysis into theeffects of exercise on cognitive function in old adults with and without cognitive impairments.

    We investigated cognitive status (healthy vs. cognitively impaired), exercise type (aerobic vs.

    anaerobic vs. multimodal vs. psychomotor) and dose-parameters (program duration, session

    duration, frequency and intensity) as potential moderators of exercise effects on cognition.

    Chapter 4 describes the feasibility and results of a 24-week randomized controlled trial (RCT)in 69 PwD visiting daycare or residing in residential care facilities. The primary outcomes

    were physical and cognitive functions measured with performance-based tests. We used a

    20

  • two-arm exercise vs. control design, with the exercises being performed at a low (weeks

    1-12) vs. high (weeks 13-24) intensity to investigate exercise intensity as potential moderator

    of exercise effects. ApoE4-status was determined post-intervention as potential moderator

    of exercise effects. Chapter 5 describes the results of a pilot study into the psychometricproperties (feasibility, validity and tests-retest reliability) of a computerized Flanker task

    in 22 PwD. Finally, Chapter 6 summarizes and discusses the main findings of this thesis.Additionally, Chapter 6 offers suggestions for the implementation of exercise in dementia

    health care practice.

    21

  • References1. World Health Organization. Global Health Observatory (GHO) data - Life expectancy. 2019.

    Available at: https://www.who.int/gho/mortality_burden_disease_life_tables/en/.Accessed June 24, 2019.

    2. World Health Organization. Global health and ageing. 2011; NIH 11-7737.

    3. World Health Organization. Dementia. 2017. Available at:http://www.who.int/mediacentre/factsheets/fs362/en/. Accessed June 18, 2018.

    4. ToleaMI, Morris JC, Galvin JE. Trajectory of mobility decline by type of dementia. Alzheimer Dis Assoc Disord.2016;30: 60-66.

    5. Rabinovici GD, Carrillo MC, Forman M, DeSanti S, Miller DS, Kozauer N, et al. Multiple comorbid neu-ropathologies in the setting of Alzheimer’s disease neuropathology and implications for drug development.Alzheimers Dement (NY). 2016;3: 83-91.

    6. Alzheimer’s Association. 2019 Alzheimer’s disease facts and figures. Alzheimer’s and Dementia. 2019;15:321-387.

    7. Livingston G, Sommerlad A, Orgeta V, Costafreda SG, Huntley J, Ames D, et al. Dementia prevention, interven-tion, and care. Lancet. 2017;390: 2673-2734.

    8. Eichner JE, Dunn ST, Perveen G, Thompson DM, Stewart KE, Stroehla BC. Apolipoprotein E polymorphism andcardiovascular disease: a HuGE review. Am J Epidemiol. 2002;155: 487-495.

    9. Corder EH, Saunders AM, StrittmatterWJ, Schmechel DE, Gaskell PC, Small GW, et al. Gene dose of apolipopro-tein E type 4 allele and the risk of Alzheimer’s disease in late onset families. Science. 1993;261: 921-923.

    10. Liu CC, Liu CC, Kanekiyo T, Xu H, Bu G. Apolipoprotein E and Alzheimer disease: risk, mechanisms andtherapy. Nat Rev Neurol. 2013;9: 106-118.

    11. Cummings J, Aisen PS, DuBois B, Frolich L, Jack CR,Jr, Jones RW, et al. Drug development in Alzheimer’sdisease: the path to 2025. Alzheimers Res Ther. 2016;8: 39-016-0207-9.

    12. Zorginstituut Nederland. Farmacotherapeutisch kompas - Cholinesteraseremmers, centraal werkend. 2019.Available at: https://www.farmacotherapeutischkompas.nl/bladeren/groepsteksten/cholinesteraseremmers_centraal_werkend. Accessed June 24, 2019.

    13. Garber CE, Blissmer B, Deschenes MR, Franklin BA, Lamonte MJ, Lee IM, et al. American College of SportsMedicine position stand. Quantity and quality of exercise for developing and maintaining cardiorespiratory,musculoskeletal, and neuromotor fitness in apparently healthy adults: guidance for prescribing exercise. Med SciSports Exerc. 2011;43: 1334-1359.

    14. World Health Organization. Information sheet: global recommendations on physical activity for health 65 yearsand above. 2011. Available at: https://www.who.int/dietphysicalactivity/publications/recommendations65yearsold/en/. Accessed June 24, 2019.

    15. Sun F, Norman IJ, While AE. Physical activity in older people: a systematic review. BMC Public Health.2013;13: 449-2458-13-449.

    22

  • 16. van Alphen HJ, Volkers KM, Blankevoort CG, Scherder EJ, Hortobagyi T, van Heuvelen MJ. Older adults withdementia are sedentary for most of the day. PLoS One. 2016;11: e0152457.

    17. Hortobágyi T. The positives of physical activity and the negatives of sedentariness in successful aging. In:Rutgers H, editor. The Future of Health and Fitness. A Plan for Getting Europe Active by 2025. Nijmegen, TheNetherlands: Black Box Publishers; 2014. pp. 54-62.

    18. Layne AS, Hsu FC, Blair SN, Chen SH, Dungan J, Fielding RA, et al. Predictors of change in physical functionin older adults in response to long-term, structured physical activity: the LIFE study. Arch Phys Med Rehabil.2017;98: 11-24.e3.

    19. Stenholm S, Koster A, Valkeinen H, Patel KV, Bandinelli S, Guralnik JM, et al. Association of physical activityhistory with physical function and mortality in old age. J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci. 2016;71: 496-501.

    20. Granacher U, Gruber M, Gollhofer A. Resistance training and neuromuscular performance in seniors. Int J SportsMed. 2009;30: 652-657.

    21. Forte R, Boreham CA, Leite JC, De Vito G, Brennan L, Gibney ER, et al. Enhancing cognitive functioning inthe elderly: multicomponent vs resistance training. Clin Interv Aging. 2013;8: 19-27.

    22. Blankevoort CG, van Heuvelen MJ, Boersma F, Luning H, de Jong J, Scherder EJ. Review of effects of physicalactivity on strength, balance, mobility and ADL performance in elderly subjects with dementia. Dement GeriatrCogn Disord. 2010;30: 392-402.

    23. Bossers WJ, van der Woude LH, Boersma F, Hortobagyi T, Scherder EJ, van Heuvelen MJ. Comparison of effectof two exercise programs on Activities of Daily Living in individuals with dementia: a 9-week randomized,controlled trial. J Am Geriatr Soc. 2016;64: 1258-1266.

    24. Prakash RS, Voss MW, Erickson KI, Lewis JM, Chaddock L, Malkowski E, et al. Cardiorespiratory fitness andattentional control in the aging brain. Front Hum Neurosci. 2011;4: 229.

    25. Wong CN, Chaddock-Heyman L, Voss MW, Burzynska AZ, Basak C, Erickson KI, et al. Brain activation duringdual-task processing is associated with cardiorespiratory fitness and performance in older adults. Front AgingNeurosci. 2015;7: 154.

    26. Pensel MC, Daamen M, Scheef L, Knigge HU, Rojas Vega S, Martin JA, et al. Executive control processes areassociated with individual fitness outcomes following regular exercise training: blood lactate profile curves andneuroimaging findings. Sci Rep. 2018;8: 4893-018-23308-3.

    27. Kramer AF, Hahn S, Cohen NJ, Banich MT, McAuley E, Harrison CR, et al. Ageing, fitness and neurocognitivefunction. Nature. 1999;400: 418-419.

    28. Northey JM, Cherbuin N, Pumpa KL, Smee DJ, Rattray B. Exercise interventions for cognitive function in adultsolder than 50: a systematic review with meta-analysis. Br J Sports Med. 2017.

    29. Eggermont LHP, Swaab DF, Hol EM, Scherder EJA. Walking the line: a randomised trial on the effects of a shortterm walking programme on cognition in dementia. J. Neurol. Neurosurg. Psychiatry 2009;80: 802-804.

    30. Bossers WJ, van der Woude LH, Boersma F, Hortobagyi T, Scherder EJ, van Heuvelen MJ. A 9-week aerobicand strength training program improves cognitive and motor function in patients with dementia: a randomized,controlled trial. Am J Geriatr Psychiatry. 2015;23: 1106-1116.

    23

  • 31. Sobol NA, Hoffmann K, Frederiksen KS, Vogel A, Vestergaard K, Braendgaard H, et al. Effect of aerobic exerciseon physical performance in patients with Alzheimer’s disease. Alzheimers Dement. 2016;12: 1207-1215.

    32. Lamb SE, Sheehan B, Atherton N, Nichols V, Collins H, Mistry D, et al. Dementia And Physical Activity (DAPA)trial of moderate to high intensity exercise training for people with dementia: randomised controlled trial. BMJ.2018;361: k1675.

    33. Toots A, Littbrand H, Bostrom G, Hornsten C, Holmberg H, Lundin-Olsson L, et al. Effects of exercise oncognitive function in older people with dementia: a randomized controlled trial. J Alzheimers Dis. 2017;60:323-332.

    34. Cotman CW, Berchtold NC, Christie LA. Exercise builds brain health: key roles of growth factor cascades andinflammation. Trends Neurosci. 2007;30: 464-472.

    35. Liu-Ambrose T, Donaldson MG. Exercise and cognition in older adults: is there a role for resistance trainingprogrammes? Br J Sports Med. 2009;43: 25-27.

    36. Erickson KI, Voss MW, Prakash RS, Basak C, Szabo A, Chaddock L, et al. Exercise training increases size ofhippocampus and improves memory. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2011;108: 3017-3022.

    37. Colcombe SJ, Erickson KI, Scalf PE, Kim JS, Prakash R, McAuley E, et al. Aerobic exercise training increasesbrain volume in aging humans. J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci 2006;61: 1166-1170.

    38. Maass A, Duzel S, Brigadski T, Goerke M, Becke A, Sobieray U, et al. Relationships of peripheral IGF-1, VEGFand BDNF levels to exercise-related changes in memory, hippocampal perfusion and volumes in older adults.Neuroimage. 2016;131: 142-154.

    39. Voss MW, Erickson KI, Prakash RS, Chaddock L, Kim JS, Alves H, et al. Neurobiological markers of exercise-related brain plasticity in older adults. Brain Behav Immun. 2013;28: 90-99.

    40. Bossers W.J.R. Physical exercise and dementia: delaying cognitive and motor decline via exercise. [S.l.]: [S.n.],2014. 169 p.

    41. Barha CK, Davis JC, Falck RS, Nagamatsu LS, Liu-Ambrose T. Sex differences in exercise efficacy to improvecognition: A systematic review and meta-analysis of randomized controlled trials in older humans. Frontiers inNeuroendocrinology. 2017;46: 71-85. doi: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.yfrne.2017.04.002.

    42. Colcombe S, Kramer AF. Fitness effects on the cognitive function of older adults: a meta-analytic study. PsycholSci. 2003;14: 125-130.

    43. Leckie RL, Weinstein AM, Hodzic JC, Erickson KI. Potential moderators of physical activity on brain health. JAging Res. 2012;2012: 948981.

    44. Groot C, Hooghiemstra AM, Raijmakers PG, van Berckel BN, Scheltens P, Scherder EJ, et al. The effect ofphysical activity on cognitive function in patients with dementia: a meta-analysis of randomized control trials.Ageing Res Rev. 2016;25: 13-23.

    45. Gnjidic D, Bell JS, Hilmer SN, Lonnroos E, Sulkava R, Hartikainen S. Drug Burden Index associated withfunction in community-dwelling older people in Finland: a cross-sectional study. Ann Med. 2012;44: 458-467.

    46. Hilmer SN, Mager DE, Simonsick EM, Cao Y, Ling SM, Windham BG, et al. A drug burden index to define thefunctional burden of medications in older people. Arch Intern Med. 2007;167: 781-787.

    24

  • 47. Hilmer SN, Mager DE, Simonsick EM, Ling SM, Windham BG, Harris TB, et al. Drug burden index score andfunctional decline in older people. Am J Med. 2009;122: 1142-1149.e1-2.

    48. Kashyap M, Belleville S, Mulsant BH, Hilmer SN, Paquette A, Tu le M, et al. Methodological challenges indetermining longitudinal associations between anticholinergic drug use and incident cognitive decline. J AmGeriatr Soc. 2014;62: 336-341.

    49. Etman A, Pierik FH, Kamphuis CB, Burdorf A, van Lenthe FJ. The role of high-intensity physical exercise in theprevention of disability among community-dwelling older people. BMC Geriatr. 2016;16: 183.

    50. Foulds HJ, Bredin SS, Charlesworth SA, Ivey AC,Warburton DE. Exercise volume and intensity: a dose-responserelationship with health benefits. Eur J Appl Physiol. 2014;114: 1563-1571.

    51. MacInnis MJ, Gibala MJ. Physiological adaptations to interval training and the role of exercise intensity. JPhysiol. 2017;595: 2915-2930.

    52. Schnohr P,Marott JL, Jensen JS, Jensen GB. Intensity versus duration of cycling, impact on all-cause and coronaryheart disease mortality: the Copenhagen City Heart Study. Eur J Prev Cardiol. 2012;19: 73-80.

    53. Lautenschlager NT, Cox KL, Flicker L, Foster JK, van Bockxmeer FM, Xiao J, et al. Effect of physical activity oncognitive function in older adults at risk for Alzheimer disease: a randomized trial. JAMA. 2008;300: 1027-1037.

    54. Fenesi B, Fang H, Kovacevic A, Oremus M, Raina P, Heisz JJ. Physical exercise moderates the relationship ofApolipoprotein E (APOE) genotype and dementia risk: a population-based study. J Alzheimers Dis. 2017;56:297-303.

    55. Podewils LJ, Guallar E, Kuller LH, Fried LP, Lopez OL, Carlson M, et al. Physical activity, APOE genotype, anddementia risk: findings from the Cardiovascular Health Cognition Study. Am J Epidemiol. 2005;161: 639-651.

    56. Smith JC, Lancaster MA, Nielson KA, Woodard JL, Seidenberg M, Durgerian S, et al. Interactive effects ofphysical activity and APOE-epsilon4 on white matter tract diffusivity in healthy elders. Neuroimage. 2016;131:102-112.

    57. Blankevoort CG, van Heuvelen MJ, Scherder EJ. Reliability of six physical performance tests in older peoplewith dementia. Phys Ther. 2013;93: 69-78.

    58. Stroop JR. Studies of interference in serial verbal reactions. J Exp Psychol Gen. 1992;121: 15-23.

    59. Fisher LM, Freed DM, Corkin S. Stroop Color-Word Test performance in patients with Alzheimer’s disease. JClin Exp Neuropsychol. 1990;12: 745-758.

    60. Hubbard G, Cook A, Tester S, Downs M. Beyond words: older people with dementia using and interpret-ing nonverbal behaviour. Journal of Aging Studies. 2002;16: 155-167. doi: https://doi.org/10.1016/S0890-4065(02)00041-5.

    61. Eriksen BA, Eriksen CW. Effects of noise letters upon the identification of a target letter in a nonsearch task.Percept Psychophys. 1974;16: 143-149.

    25

  • 26

  • �����������������������������������������������������

    ����������������������������������������������������������������������������

    ����������������������������������������������������������������������������������������������������������������� ���������������������������������������������

    ��� ������ ����������������������� ����������������

  • Abstract

    Purpose

    The Drug Burden Index (DBI) is a tool to quantify the anticholinergic and sedative load

    of drugs. Establishing functional correlates of the DBI could optimize drug prescribing in

    patients with dementia. In this cross-sectional study, we determined the relationship between

    DBI and cognitive and physical function in a sample of patients with dementia.

    Methods

    Using performance-based tests, we measured physical and cognitive function in 140 nursing

    home patients aged over 70 with all-cause dementia. We also determined anticholinergic

    (AChDBI) and sedative (SDBI) drug burden separately and in combination as total drug

    burden (TDB).

    Results

    Nearly one half of patients (48%) used at least one DBI-contributing drug. In 33% of the

    patients, drug burden was moderate (0

  • Introduction

    With the world population progressively growing older, the number of older adults with

    dementia increases. Globally, 47.5 billion people suffer from dementia, resulting in a health

    care expenditure of $600 billion [1]. Cognitive and physical abilities of patients with dementia

    decline steadily, compromising daily function and independence and requiring approximately

    one half of patients to move to a nursing home (NH) and receive assistance [2]. The prevalence

    of comorbidities among patients with dementia is high [3]. Depending on the study, 43%

    to 92% of dementia patients are exposed to polypharmacy, i.e., the concurrent use of five or

    more medications from different drug categories [4-7], which can result in serious Adverse

    Drug Reactions (ADRs) such as cognitive impairment, functional decline, and an increase

    in risk for falls and fractures [8,9]. To minimize suboptimal drug use by older adults, the

    Beers criteria categorize inappropriate drugs that patients should use with caution or avoid

    [10]. Even though anticholinergic and sedative psychotropic drugs have especially high risks

    to cause adverse effects, it is estimated that 23% to 47% of dementia patients take at least one

    anticholinergic or sedative drug [11-13].

    Several tools exist to quantify anticholinergic drug burden in older adults, but the agree-

    ment between tools is limited [14]. The Drug Burden Index (DBI) has been identified

    as an appropriate tool to quantify anticholinergic and sedative drug burden in older adults

    [15,16,17]. In community-dwelling older adults, a higher DBI correlates with low physical

    function [18-21], high fall rate [22], difficulty in performing activities of daily living (ADL)

    [20,21], mortality, and hospitalization [23]. However, the evidence is mixed regarding the

    relationship between DBI and cognitive function [15,24,25]. In dementia patients, a higher

    DBI correlates with low self-reported health-related quality of life [7] and a high risk of

    hospitalization and mortality [23]. As far as we know, the relationship between DBI and

    physical and cognitive function has not yet been examined in patients with dementia. To

    minimize suboptimal drug prescribing for patients with dementia, it is necessary to establish

    the functional associations of the DBI in this patient group.

    The purpose of the present cross-sectional studywas to determine the relationship between

    DBI and cognitive and physical function in patients with all-cause dementia. We quantified

    the relationship between anticholinergic (AChDBI), sedative (SDBI), and total drug burden

    (TDB=AChDBI+SDBI) and physical and cognitive function. We hypothesized that AChDBI

    and SDBI individually and in combination in the form of TDB, are inversely associated with

    29

  • physical and cognitive function.

    Methods

    Design

    This study combined data fromaDutch cross-sectional (NederlandsTrial Register (NTR)1230,

    74 participants, data collected between 2004-2007) study and baseline data from a related

    Dutch intervention study (NTR2269, 66 participants, collected between 2010-2014). Each

    participant or a legal representative signed an informed consent approved by the University

    Medical Ethical Committee. The studies were conducted in accordance with the principles

    of the Declaration of Helsinki (64th amendment).

    Sample and procedures

    The current sample consisted of 140 NH residents over age 70 with a dementia diagnosis.

    The two studies differed minimally in terms of inclusion and exclusion criteria, resulting in

    the following overall inclusion criteria for the total sample: age >70, a physician-diagnosed

    dementia reported in the medical chart, the ability to walk short distances without a walking

    aid, and an MMSE score ≥10 and ≤24 (indicating mild-to-moderate dementia). Multipledisease-related exclusion criteria were used for safety reasons (see NTR files 1230 and 2269,

    Appendix 1, online supplementary material). Appendix 1 also describes the recruitment

    procedures. Trained research assistants recorded data on socio-demographic factors (age,

    gender, level of education) and cognitive and physical abilities. To minimize test burden, the

    assessor performed the cognitive and physical assessments in two separate sessions within

    seven days. The researchers extracted data on medical conditions and medication use for all

    participants from the nursing homes’ medical files. Medication data comprised a record of

    all medications taken by the participant at the time of assessment and the subsequent dose,

    frequency and the method of administration. We excluded medication ‘as needed’, topical

    ointments, lubricating eye drops and over-the-counter medications from the current analyses.

    Measurements

    Medication assessment

    To code the drugs, we used theAnatomical Therapeutic Chemical (ATC) classification system,

    30

  • as recommended by the World Health Organization [26]. We quantified the total anticholiner-

    gic and sedative load with the DBI and calculated total Drug Burden (TDB) as follows [15]:

    (1) TDB=AChDBI+SDBI

    where AChDBI and SDBI represent, respectively, total anticholinergic and sedative load.

    We determined sedative and/or anticholinergic load for each drug and summed up as follows:

    (2) AChDBI or SDBI = Σ Di / (δi + Di),

    where Di represents the daily dose taken by the participant and δ represents the recom-

    mended minimum daily dose of the drugs with respectively an anticholinergic or sedative

    load. The recommended minimum daily dose was specified for each drug (i) based on the

    lowestminimumoral dose that is prescribed for any commonmedical indication in older adults.

    Classification of anticholinergic and sedative drugs

    To determine if a drug had sedative and/or anticholinergic effects, the Expertise Centre Phar-

    macotherapy in Older persons (Ephor) [27] was consulted. Figures 1 and 2 show the decision

    tree for the classification process. A drug was classified as anticholinergic when the three

    most common anticholinergic side effects obstipation/constipation, xerostomia and urinary

    retention were described in Ephor or in at least two of the remaining sources: the Sum-

    mary of Product Characteristics (SmPC [28], the Pharmacotherapeutic Compass [29] and the

    Medicines Information Centre of the Royal Dutch Pharmacists Association [30]. If all three

    anticholinergic side effects were mentioned in one of the remaining sources and at least one

    side effect was mentioned in the two other remaining sources, the drug was also classified as

    anticholinergic. A drug was classified as sedative when either sedation, drowsiness, somno-

    lence or impaired coordination and reaction time were listed as side effects in Ephor or at least

    two of the remaining sources. If a drug was known to have both anticholinergic and sedative

    effects, it was classified as anticholinergic [15].

    Polypharmacy

    Polypharmacy and excessive polypharmacy were defined as the concurrent use of 5–9 and >9

    drugs, respectively [4].

    31

  • Non-DBI-contributing drugs

    Other than inclusion in polypharmacy measures, we excluded all non-DBI-contributing drugs

    from the current analyses.

    Comorbidities

    We quantified comorbidity based on the Functional Comorbidity Index (FCI-18) [31] (Ap-

    pendix 3, online supplementary material). The FCI comprises 18 medical conditions that

    negatively impact physical function. The presence or absence of each condition is listed. A

    higher score represents a greater number of comorbidities.

    Functional outcomes

    Motor function

    To characterize motor function, we used several performance-based tests that are frequently

    used for patients with dementia [32]. Functional mobility was quantified with the Six Meter

    Walk Test (m/s, [33]), Timed Up&Go (seconds [34] and 30-seconds Sit to Stand (number of

    correct attempts, [35]). Balance was measured with the Frailty and Injuries: Cooperative

    Studies of Intervention Techniques subtest 4 (FICSIT-4 [36]) and Figure of Eight (seconds

    [37]). Grip strength (kg) was assessed using a Jamar © hand dynamometer.

    Cognitive function

    We employed frequently-used neuropsychological tests [32] to quantify cognitive function,

    including: global cognitive function (Mini Mental State Examination [38]), verbal memory

    (Eight Word Task immediate recall and recognition [39]); verbal working memory (Digit

    Span Forward and Backward [40]); visual memory (Visual Memory Span Forward and Back-

    ward [40]; Rivermead Behavioural Memory Test (RBMT) Faces and Pictures [41]), abstract

    reasoning (Groninger Intelligence Test (GIT) incomplete figures [42]) and basic information

    processing speed (STROOP word card [43], adapted 45 second version). We determined the

    number of correct responses for all tests as outcome measure. For all tasks, higher scores

    indicate a better performance.

    32

  • Figure 1. Classification process of drug as anticholinergic.

    Figure 2. Classification process of drug as sedative.

    33

  • Statistical analyses

    We used SPSS Statistics 23.0 (IBM, Armonk, NY) to compute means and standard deviations

    (SDs) for motor and cognitive outcomes and to analyze the data. Scores on the SixMeterWalk

    Test, Timed Up&Go and Figure of Eight were positively skewed and thus, log10-transformed.

    We imputed missing data for cognitive (5.8% missing) and physical variables (6.0% missing)

    using the Maximum Likelihood - Expectation Maximization algorithm [43]. The scores on

    the cognitive and motor tests, as well as socio-demographic factors and comorbidities were

    used as predictors for missing data completions.

    We set DBI as a categorical ordinal variable (DBI=0, 0

  • of drugs did not correlate with TDB, AChDBI or SDBI. Dementia severity as measured by

    MMSE score did not correlate with TDB, AChDBI or SDBI (respectively r=-0.003; r=-0.002;

    r=-0.002). Age inversely correlated with TDB (r = -0.200, p=0.018). Table 1 summarizes

    patient characteristics in the TDB subgroups. Appendix 2 (online supplementary material)

    lists the DBI-contributing drugs used in the sample. The most commonly used anticholinergic

    drug was the antidepressant Citalopram (n=16). Oxazepam, a benzodiazepine, was the most

    commonly used sedative drug (n=12).

    Relationship between DBI and physical function

    The model not controlled for confounders revealed no group differences in measures of

    motor performance in the TDB subgroups ((F(12,264)=1.159, p=0.313, Wilk’s ∧=0.902, par-tial η2=0.050). The same applies to physical performance in AChDBI ((F(12,264)=0.538,

    p=0.889, Wilk’s ∧=0.953, partial η2=0.024) and SDBI (F(12,264)=1.382, p=0.174, Wilk’s∧=0.885, partial η2=0.059) subgroups. After controlling the models for age, gender, edu-cation and use of walking aid, there were no differences in physical outcomes between the

    subgroups TDB (F(12,220)=1.063, p=0.393, Wilk’s ∧=0.893, partial η2=0.055), AChDBI(F(12,220)=0.766, p=0.685, Wilk’s ∧=0.921, partial η2=0.040), or SDBI (F(12,220)=1.121,p=0.344, Wilk’s ∧=0.888, partial η2=0.058). When FCI score is taken into account as addi-tional covariate, there were no group differences for TDB (F(12,206)=1.114, p=0.350, Wilk’s

    ∧=0.882, partial η2=0.061), AChDBI (F(12,206)=0.726, p=0.725, Wilk’s ∧=0.920, partialη2=0.041) and SDBI (F(12,206)=1.303, p=0.219, Wilk’s ∧=0.864, partial η2=0.071).

    Relationship between DBI and cognitive function

    In the multivariate model not controlled for potential confounders, cognitive outcomes

    were not different for the TDB classes (F(22,254)=1.191, p=0.256, Wilk’s ∧=0.822, par-tial η2=0.093). We found equivocal results when the AChDBI (F(22,254)=0.976, p=0.495,

    Wilk’s ∧=0.850, partial η2=0.078) or the SDBI (F(22,254)=1.005, p=0.459, Wilk’s ∧=0.846,partial η2=0.080) were taken into account separately. After controlling for age, gender and

    education, there were no differences in cognitive outcomes between the subgroups TDB

    (F(22,220)=1.303, p=0.171, Wilk’s ∧=0.783, partial η2=0.115), AChDBI (F(22,220)=1.074,p=0.377, Wilk’s ∧=0.815, partial η2=0.097), and SDBI (F(22,220)=1.170, p=0.277, Wilk’s∧=0.801, partial η2=0.105). With FCI as an additional covariate, therewere no differences

    35

  • Table 1. Patient characteristics in the total sample (N = 140).

    Characteristics Valuea TDB = 0 0

  • in cognitive outcomes between the TDB (F(22,206)=1.352, p=0.142, Wilk’s ∧=0.764, partialη2=0.126), AChDBI (F(22,206)=1.052, p=0.403, Wilk’s ∧=0.808, partial η2=0.101) andSDBI subgroups (F(22,206)=1.250, p=0.210, Wilk’s ∧=0.778, partial η2=0.118).

    Discussion

    To our best knowledge, this is the first cross-sectional study to examine functional correlates

    of the DBI in a population of patients with mild-to-moderate all-cause dementia. We used a

    wide range of reliable and valid measurements to assess cognitive and physical function. We

    found no multivariate relationships between DBI and cognitive and physical functions.

    Relationship between drug burden and physical function

    In the present study, TDB did not correlate with physical function. We hypothesized that

    TDB would negatively correlate with physical function because anticholinergic and sedative

    drugs target central nervous system (CNS) functions that affect physical function, such as

    the gastrointestinal system and neuromuscular processes [21]. A lack of association between

    TDB and physical function in our study contrasts with data in cognitively healthy populations

    [18-20], possibly for two dementia-related potential reasons.

    First, drug prescribing might be more optimal for patients with dementia compared with

    healthy older populations. For dementia patients, a larger emphasis may be placed upon

    tolerability and quality of life instead of treatment of symptoms and quantity of life, resulting

    in better-tailored drug prescribing [45]. Indeed, in our sample, less than half of patients

    used DBI drugs, which is a lower rate compared with the rate in a sample of healthier

    older adults [46]. Such a careful approach may become even more pronounced with older

    age, a hypothesis perhaps reflected by a negative relationship between age and TDB in our

    study. Not only the presence, but also the severity of dementia may be related to more

    appropriate prescribing. However, dementia severity (as measured with MMSE) and DBI

    were unrelated in our study, confirming a lack of co-variation between the odds of being

    prescribed inappropriate medication and dementia severity [6]. Thus, presence rather than

    severity of dementia may be a better indicator of lower risk of suboptimal prescribing.

    A second reason why our results showed no relationship between physical function and

    drug burden could be that numerous dementia-related factors that affect physical health might

    confound the relationship between TDB and physical function in patients with dementia. De-

    37

  • mentia progression [47], age, poor health [48], sedentariness [49], adverse life events, and a

    decline in general well-being [50] all unfavorably affect physical function. The presence and

    manifestation of such factors may greatly vary in patients with dementia and thus increase

    variability within the sample. In combination, these factors might minimize the influence of

    DBI drugs on physical and cognitive function, nullifying a potential relation. However, this

    hypothesis of confounding factors is weakened by the results of a previous randomized con-

    trolled trial (RCT) that aimed to reduce anticholinergic exposure through a 12-week reduction

    intervention in institutionalized patients with dementia. The authors found that physical func-

    tion as measured with the Barthel Index did not improve after a decrease in anticholinergic

    exposure after 12 weeks [51]. Considering that the randomized controlled nature of the study

    should minimize the influence of confounding variables, we cannot definitively conclude that

    confounding factors underlie the lack of relationship between drug burden and physical func-

    tion.

    Our findings qualitatively agree with data in hospitalized patients with multimorbidity,

    43.5% of whom suffered from dementia. In these patients, the use of three or more psy-

    chotropic drugs was related to lower hand-grip strength in both hands, but not lower extremity

    muscle strength [52]. However, further comparisons of our sample with other dementia pop-

    ulations are difficult because the relationship between drug burden (DBI or other measures)

    and physical function in patients with dementia is understudied. Future research should im-

    prove our understanding of the relationship between physical function and drug burden in this

    patient group. Within such research, it is important to consider the care setting as being a

    determinant for more appropriate prescribing in patients with dementia. Our sample included

    patients with dementia in nursing homes, who may be at a lower risk for suboptimal prescrib-

    ing compared with community-dwelling populations with or without cognitive impairment

    [23,46]. Indeed, the use of DBI drugs declines by approximately 5% after NH admission

    [53,54], although there is a paucity of data concerning DBI drug usage in specifically Dutch

    NHs. There may be four reasons why DBI drug prescribing may be more optimal after NH

    admission: 1. NH physicians may be less inclined to prescribe DBI drugs because they are

    specialized in the medical aspects of older patients compared with primary care physicians

    [54], 2. NH staff can quickly recognize and address the adverse effects of DBI-contributing

    drugs; 3. Behavioral disturbances are generally less medicalized in a NH vs. home setting

    because behavior is interpreted in a broader, more accepting context and approached as such

    [55], and 4. NH physicians in particular might recognize the diminished benefit of drugs in

    38

  • light of patients’ low functional level [56,57]. However, the present study could not examine

    in more detail the impact of care setting on drug burden, as we studied only a NH population.

    In addition to TDB, we hypothesized that higher AChDBI and SDBI separately correlated

    with lower physical function. Separate associations of AChDBI vs. SDBI with physical

    function could arise from pharmacodynamic differences between these two drug classes:

    whereas anticholinergic drugs mainly target the cholinergic system involved in excitatory

    processes, sedative drugs generally influence inhibitory mechanisms by targeting levels of

    gamma-aminobutyric acid (GABA), although several DBI drugs target both systems (e.g.,

    citalopram). In older women anticholinergic compared with sedative drug burden (not mea-

    sured with DBI) more strongly correlated with impaired balance, mobility, gait, chair stands

    and grip strength [58]. Sedative burden was associated with impaired mobility and grip

    strength only. In contrast, Gnjidic et al. [21] reported that SDBI predicted poorer perfor-

    mance on measures of gait, balance and grip strength in older men, whereas AChDBI was

    associated with weaker grip strength only. The difference between these studies might be

    explained by differences in sedative drug usage [58] or gender differences. However, con-

    trasting with these studies, neither AChDBI nor SDBI correlated with physical function in our

    sample. The effects of anticholinergic and sedative drugs may be less discernable in dementia

    patients vs. community-dwelling populations because amyloid β deposition in Alzheimer’s

    Disease (AD) disrupts the excitatory/inhibitory balance system [59]. Consequently, DBI

    drugs that target the anticholinergic system, may disrupt the GABA system as well (and vice

    versa). Thus, functional correlates of AChDBI/SDBI, if any, may be indiscernible in dementia

    patients.

    Relationship between drug burden and cognitive function

    Contrary to our hypothesis, we found no association between TDB and cognitive function.

    Associations between higher drug burden and lower cognitive function in other populations

    can be explained by the detrimental effects of anticholinergic and sedative drugs on CNS

    processes involving vision, attention, sedation and psychomotor speed [21]. The finding

    that higher anticholinergic burden was not related to lower cognitive function is similar to

    the results of a study in 224 community-dwelling patients with AD, where anticholinergic

    load was quantified with the Anticholinergic Burden scale [60], and in line with a study

    in patients with multimorbidity (43.5% dementia) showing that users of anticholinergic or

    sedative drugs did not have lower cognitive function compared with non-users [52]. The

    39

  • aforementioned explanations for a lack of association between TDB and physical function

    may be equally applicable to the lack of association between TDB and cognitive function.

    That is, drug prescribing may be more optimal for patients with dementia compared with

    healthy older populations because of a higher emphasis on tolerability and quality of life

    instead of treatment of symptoms and quantity of life. Alternatively, the effects of drug use

    on cognitive function may be harder to detect in patients with dementia patients due to the

    large variety of dementia-related influencing factors.

    Similar to TDB, we found no evidence in support of our hypothesis that higher AChDBI

    and SDBI are separately associated with lower cognitive function. In older women, higher

    anticholinergic burden (not measured with DBI) correlated more strongly with lower global

    cognitive function than sedative burden [58]. No evidence was reported for different cognitive

    domains. In addition, a higher AChDBI was associated with lower memory performance and

    lower performance on the Trail Making Test B in cognitively healthy older adults [25]. No

    associations between SDBI and cognitive domains were reported. The lack of discernible

    associations of anticholinergic vs. sedative drugs on cognitive function in our study may

    result from the simultaneous dysregulation of the excitatory/inhibitory systems by either

    anticholinergic or sedative drugs in patients with dementia, as described previously in this

    paper.

    Study limitations

    Several limitations warrant caution in interpreting our results. First, the sample size of the

    current study is small compared to other studies [18] and the studied DBI-subgroups were

    of unequal sizes. This might have negatively affected statistical power. Second, our sample

    consisted of patients with mild to moderate dementia, with the mean MMSE score indicating

    moderate dementia. Therefore, the results are not directly generalizable to a more severe

    dementia population. Also, due to the cross-sectional design of our study, we cannot exclude

    the issue of confounding by indication. We are thus unable to make claims about causal

    effects of DBI drugs on functional outcomes in dementia. A complicating factor is that the

    efficacy of DBI drugs in the later stages of dementia is not yet adequately assessed. To gain

    optimal understanding of the positive and negative effects of DBI drugs in dementia patients,

    future experimental studies on the efficacy of DBI drugs with varying degrees of dementia

    are needed. Additionally, differences between how previous studies and we classified DBI

    is one source of inconsistency [18]. The source of this inconsistency is a lack of consensus

    40

  • as to which drugs to classify as anticholinergic, sedative, or both. In particular, the current

    DBI may be an underestimation of the true anticholinergic and sedative burden, as drugs

    with both anticholinergic and sedative effects are classified as anticholinergic-only. Thus,

    our indices, compared with other studies, could yield a different drug burden value compared

    with other methods. To minimize such differences, we used a detailed classification process

    that included several reliable sources and we also strictly adhered to the original method [15]

    in estimating DBI. Also, the DBI does not account for medications-as-needed, which could

    have influenced the study results if participants took such medications before the assessments.

    Furthermore, patients with dementia in NHs may be inherently different from community-

    dwelling patients. Associations of DBI with functional outcomes are therefore not directly

    generalizable to a community-dwelling dementia population. Further research could focus

    on the associations of DBI with functional outcomes in different samples of community-

    dwelling versus institutionalized patients with dementia. Longitudinal research could be done

    by following dementia patients through the process of institutionalization, while tracking

    medication records and functional outcomes. In addition, the mean number of comorbidities

    in our sample (M=2) was lower compared with another sample of NH patients with dementia

    (M=4 on a summary scale (not FCI), [61]). The difference between our study and the study

    by Sloane et al. [61] could result from the exclusion of people with multiple health-related

    conditions in our sample, which could have resulted in a healthier-than-average group of

    participants. Contrarily, the FCI does not comprise all medical conditions, and the lower

    comorbidity scores in our sample may have resulted from the exclusion of several medical

    conditions on the FCI, such as cancer or thyroid disease. Altogether, caution is advised when

    generalizing our results to other NH patients with dementia. Moreover, the DBI does not

    include a weighting factor for the relative anticholinergic activity of each DBI-drug. Not all

    drugs have equally strong anticholinergic effects [62]. The use of specifically high potency

    anticholinergics has been linked to an increased risk of all-cause dementia in older adults

    [63]. Duran et al. [64] provide a differentiation in anticholinergic potency of drugs with

    anticholinergic properties (Appendix 2, online supplementary material). Appendix 2 shows

    that our sample predominantly used drugs with low anticholinergic potency, which is not

    reflected in the DBI. This may at least partly account for the absence of a relationship between

    DBI and functional outcomes, and warrants a careful generalization of our results to other

    NH populations with dementia. Lastly, Dutch NHs might be inherently different in terms of

    drug prescribing practices compared with NHs in other countries [65]. Therefore, we urge

    41

  • caution in generalization of these results to other NHs.

    Clinical implications

    A lack of association between DBI and functional outcomes raises questions about the DBI

    as a clinical assessment tool of drug burden in patients with dementia. DBI is considered

    as a valid and useful tool to evaluate drug burden in many populations. However, it does

    not account for the many drug-drug interactions between DBI drugs. Also, DBI does not

    consider possible adverse effects of other non-DBI contributing drugs. The identification

    of inappropriate prescribing in patients with dementia is particularly challenging because

    evidence-based guidelines are lacking and health care practitioners are unsure about the best

    prescribing choices [66]. As a result, DBI might over- or underestimate true drug burden. To

    prevent underestimation of drug burden, perceived medication effects could be assessed by

    inquiring patients and caregivers.

    Conclusion

    In contrast with previous studies in healthy older adults, DBI did not correlate with cognitive

    and physical function in a sample of institutionalized patients with dementia. The lower

    use of DBI contributing drugs in our sample compared with a community-dwelling healthy

    population, might indicate that drug prescribing is more optimal for patients with dementia

    compared with cognitively healthy older adults. Further experimental research into the

    efficacy of DBI drugs for patients with dementia of different severities and etiologies, and

    in different care settings, is needed to clarify the relationship between DBI and functional

    outcomes in patients with dementia. To achieve or maintain optimal disease management for

    patients with dementia, prudence is urged when prescribing anticholinergic or sedative drugs

    for the treatment of neuropsychiatric complaints.

    Acknowledgements

    The cross-sectional study (NTR1230) was funded by Stichting Fontis Vitaal and the VU

    University Amsterdam. The intervention study (NTR2269) was funded by Fonds NutsOhra

    and the UniversityMedical Centre Groningen. The current study is supported by the Deltaplan

    Dementie (ZonMW: Memorabel) and the University Medical Centre Groningen.

    42

  • Conflict of interest

    The authors declare no conflicts of interest.

    43

  • References1. World Health Organization. Dementia. 2015; Available at:

    http://www.who.int/mediacentre/factsheets/fs362/en/. Accessed June 18, 2015.

    2. CIZ. Aanspraak op AWBZ-zorg. 2013; Available at:http://www.nji.nl/nl/Download-NJi/Aanspraak-op-AWBZ-zorg.pdf. Accessed June 10, 2016.

    3. Bunn F, Burn AM, Goodman C, Rait G, Norton S, Robinson L, et al. Comorbidity and dementia: a scopingreview of the literature. BMC Med. 2014 Oct 31;12:192-014-0192-4.

    4. Nederlands Huisartsen Genootschap. Multidisciplinaire richtlijn Polyfarmacie bij ouderen. 2012; Available at:https://www.nhg.org/sites/default/files/content/nhg_org/uploads/polyfarmacie_bij_ouderen.pdf.Accessed June 10, 2016.

    5. Montastruc F, Gardette V, Cantet C, Piau A, Lapeyre-Mestre M, Vellas B, et al. Potentially inappropriatemedication use among patients with Alzheimer disease in the REAL.FR cohort: be aware of atropinic andbenzodiazepine drugs! Eur J Clin Pharmacol. 2013 Aug;69(8):1589-1597.

    6. LauDT,MercaldoND,Harris AT, Trittschuh E, Shega J,Weintraub S. Polypharmacy and potentially inappropriatemedication use among community-dwelling elders with dementia. Alzheimer Dis Assoc Disord. 2010 Jan-Mar;24(1):56-63.

    7. Bosboom PR, Alfonso H, Almeida OP, Beer C. Use of Potentially Harmful Medications and Health-RelatedQuality of Life among People with Dementia Living in Residential Aged Care Facilities. Dement Geriatr CognDis Extra. 2012 Jan;2(1):361-371.

    8. Oyarzun-Gonzalez XA, Taylor KC, Myers SR, Muldoon SB, Baumgartner RN. Cognitive decline and polyphar-macy in an elderly population. J Am Geriatr Soc. 2015 Feb;63(2):397-399.

    9. Maher RL, Hanlon J, Hajjar ER. Clinical consequences of polypharmacy in elderly. Expert Opin Drug Saf. 2014Jan;13(1):57-65.

    10. American Geriatrics Society. Updated Beers Criteria for Potentially Inappropriate Medication Use in OlderAdults. J Am Geriatr Soc. 2015 Oct 8.

    11. Eggermont LH, de Vries K, Scherder EJ. Psychotropic medication use and cognition in institutionalized olderadults with mild to moderate dementia. Int Psychogeriatr. 2009 Apr;21(2):286-294.

    12. Koyama A, Steinman M, Ensrud K, Hillier TA, Yaffe K. Ten-year trajectory of potentially inappropriate medica-tions in very old women: importance of cognitive status. J Am Geriatr Soc. 2013 Feb;61(2):258-263.

    13. Parsons C, Johnston S, Mathie E, Baron N, Machen I, Amador S, et al. Potentially inappropriate prescribing inolder people with dementia in care homes: a retrospective analysis. Drugs Aging. 2012 Feb 1;29(2):143-155.

    14. Pont LG, Nielen JT, McLachlan AJ, Gnjidic D, Chan L, Cumming RG, et al. Measuring anticholinergic drugexposure in older community-dwelling Australian men: a comparison of four different measures. Br J ClinPharmacol. 2015 Nov;80(5):1169-1175.

    15. Hilmer SN, Mager DE, Simonsick EM, Cao Y, Ling SM, Windham BG, et al. A drug burden index to define thefunctional burden of medications in older people. Arch Intern Med. 2007 Apr 23;167(8):781-787.

    44

  • 16. Wouters H, van der Meer H, Taxis K. Quantification of anticholinergic and sedative drug load with the DrugBurden Index: a review of outcomes and methodological quality of studies. Eur J Clin Pharmacol. 2017Mar;73(3):257-266.

    17. Cardwell K, Hughes CM, Ryan C. The association between anticholinergic medication burden and health relatedoutcomes in the ’oldest old’: a systematic review of the literature. Drugs Aging. 2015 Oct;32(10):835-848.

    18. Kouladjian L, Gnjidic D, Chen TF, Mangoni AA, Hilmer SN. Drug Burden Index in older adults: theoretical andpractical issues. Clin Interv Aging. 2014 Sep 9;9:1503-1515.

    19. Hilmer SN, Mager DE, Simonsick EM, Ling SM, Windham BG, Harris TB, et al. Drug burden indx score andfunctional decline in older people. Am J Med. 2009 Dec;122(12):1142-1149.e1-2.

    20. Gnjidic D, Bell JS, Hilmer SN, Lonnroos E, Sulkava R,Hartikainen S. DrugBurden Index associatedwith functionin community-dwelling older people in Finland: a cross-sectional study. Ann Med. 2012 Aug;44(5):458-467.

    21. Gnjidic D, Cumming RG, Le Couteur DG, Handelsman DJ, Naganathan V, Abernethy DR, et al. Drug BurdenIndex and physical function in older Australian men. Br J Clin Pharmacol. 2009 Jul;68(1):97-105.

    22. Wilson NM, Hilmer SN, March LM, Cameron ID, Lord SR, Seibel MJ, et al. Associations between drug burdenindex and falls in older people in residential aged care. J Am Geriatr Soc. 2011 May;59(5):875-880.

    23. Gnjidic D, Hilmer SN, Hartikainen S, Tolppanen AM, Taipale H, Koponen M, et al. Impact of high risk druguse on hospitalization and mortality in older people with and without Alzheimer’s disease: a national populationcohort study. PLoS One. 2014 Jan 13;9(1):e83224.

    24. Gnjidic D, Le Couteur DG, Naganathan V, Cumming RG, Creasey H, Waite LM, et al. Effects of drug burdenindex on cognitive function in older men. J Clin Psychopharmacol. 2012 Apr;32(2):273-277.

    25. Kashyap M, Belleville S, Mulsant BH, Hilmer SN, Paquette A, Tu le M, et al. Methodological challenges indetermining longitudinal associations between anticholinergic drug use and incident cognitive decline. J AmGeriatr Soc. 2014 Feb;62(2):336-341.

    26. WHO Collaborating Centre for Drug Statistics Methodology. Guidelines for ATC classification and DDDassignment. 2013. Available at: https://www.whocc.no/filearchive/publications/1_2013guidelines.pdf.Accessed June 18, 2015

    27. Ephor. 2016. Available at: http://www.ephor.nl/. Accessed June 14, 2015.

    28. Electronic Medicines Compendium. Summary of Product Characteristics. 2016; Available at:https://www.medicines.org.uk/emc/. Accessed June 14, 2015.

    29. Zorginstituut Nederland. Farmacotherapeutisch Kompas. 2016; Available at:https://www.farmacotherapeutischkompas.nl/. Accessed June 14, 2015.

    30. Informatorium Medicamentum KNMP kennisbank. 2016; Available at: https://kennisbank.knmp.nl.Accessed June 14, 2015.

    31. Groll DL, To T, Bombardier C, Wright JG. The development of a comorbidity index with physical function as theoutcome. J Clin Epidemiol. 2005 Jun;58(6):595-602.

    32. Bossers WJ, van der Woude LH, Boersma F, Scherder EJ, van Heuvelen MJ. Recommended measures for theassessment of cognitive and physical performance in older patients with dementia: a systematic review. DementGeriatr Cogn Dis Extra. 2012 Jan;2(1):589-609.

    45

  • 33. Hoeymans N, Wouters ER, Feskens EJ, van den Bos GA, Kromhout D. Reproducibility of performance-basedand self-reported measures of functional status. J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci. 1997 Nov;52(6):M363-8.

    34. Podsiadlo D, Richardson S. The timed "Up & Go": a test of basic functional mobility for frail elderly persons. JAm Geriatr Soc. 1991 Feb;39(2):142-148.

    35. Jones CJ, Rikli RE, BeamWC. A 30-s chair-stand test as a measure of lower body strength in community-residingolder adults. Res Q Exerc Sport. 1999 Jun;70(2):113-119.

    36. Rossiter-Fornoff JE, Wolf SL, Wolfson LI, Buchner DM. A cross-sectional validation study of the FICSITcommon data base static balance measures. Frailty and Injuries: Cooperative Studies of Intervention Techniques.J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci. 1995 Nov;50(6):M291-7.

    37. Hess RJ, Brach JS, Piva SR, VanSwearingen JM. Walking Skill Can Be Assessed in Older Adults: Validity of theFigure-of-8 Walk Test. Physical Therapy. 2010;90(1):89-99.

    38. Folstein MF, Folstein SE, McHugh PR. "Mini-mental state". A practical method for grading the cognitive stateof patients for the clinician. J Psychiatr Res. 1975 Nov;12(3):189-198.

    39. Lindeboom J, Jonker C. Amsterdam dementia screening test, manual. Swets and Zeitlinger, Lisse. 1989.

    40. Wechsler D. Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale – Third Edition: Administration and Scoring Manual. ThePsychological Corporation, SanAntonio. 1997.

    41. Wilson BA, Cockburn J, Baddely A. The Rivermead Behavioural Memory Test. Thames Valley Test Company,Titchfield. 1987.

    42. Luteijn F, Vanderploeg FAE. Groninger Intelligence Test Manual. Swets and Zeitlinger, Lisse. 1983.

    43. Hammes J. The STROOP color-word test: manual. Swets and Zeitlinger, Amsterdam 1973.

    44. Do CB, Batzoglou, S.What is the expectationmaximization algorithm? Nature Biotechnology. 2008;26:897-899.

    45. Hersch EC, Falzgraf S. Management of the behavioral and psychological symptoms of dementia. Clin IntervAging. 2007;2(4):611-621.

    46. Castelino RL, Hilmer SN, Bajorek BV, Nishtala P, Chen TF. Drug Burden Index and potentially inappropriatemedications in community-dwelling older people: the impact of Home Medicines Review. Drugs Aging. 2010Feb 1;27(2):135-148.

    47. Tolea MI, Morris JC, Galvin JE. Longitudinal associations between physical and cognitive performance amongcommunity-dwelling older adults. PLoS One. 2015 Apr 13;10(4):e0122878.

    48. van Alphen HJ, Hortobagyi T, van Heuvelen MJ. Barriers, motivators, and facilitators of physical activity indementia patients: a systematic review. Arch Gerontol Geriatr. 2016 May 31;66:109-118.

    49. van Alphen HJ, Volkers KM, Blankevoort CG, Scherder EJ, Hortobagyi T, van Heuvelen MJ. Older adults withdementia are sedentary for most of the day. PLoS One. 2016 Mar 31;11(3):e0152457.

    50. Hortobágyi T. The positives of physical activity and the negatives of sedentariness in successful aging. In:Rutgers H, editor. The Future of Health and Fitness. A plan for getting Europe active by 2025 Nijmegen, TheNetherlands: BlackBox Publishers; 2014. p. 54-62.

    51. Yeh YC, Liu CL, Peng LN, Lin MH, Chen LK. Potential benefits of reducing medication-related anticholinergicburden for demented older adults: a prospective cohort study. Geriatr Gerontol Int. 2013 Jul;13(3):694-700.

    46

  • 52. Kersten H, Hvidsten LT, Gloersen G, Wyller TB, Wang-Hansen MS. Clinical impact of potentially inappropriatemedications during hospitalization of acutely ill older patients with multimorbidity. Scand J Prim Health Care.2015;33(4):243-251.

    53. Zuckerman IH, Josue Hernandez J, L. Gruber-Baldini A, Richard Hebel J, Stuart B, Zimmerman S, et al.Potentially inappropriate prescribing before and after nursing home admission among patients with and withoutdementia. Am J Geriatr Pharmacother. 2005 December 2005;3(4):246-254.

    54. Dhalla IA, Anderson GM, Mamdani MM, Bronskill SE, Sykora K, Rochon PA. Inappropriate prescribing beforeand after nursing home admission. J Am Geriatr Soc. 2002 Jun;50(6):995-1000.

    55. Charmaz K, Belgrave LL. Modern symbolic interaction theory and health. In: Cockerham WC, Annandale E,editors. Medical sociology on the move: new directions in theory. Dordrecht: Springer; 2013. p. 28.

    56. Nygaard HA, Naik M, Ruths S, Straand J. Nursing-home residents and their drug use: a comparison betweenmentally intact and mentally impaired residents. The Bergen district nursing home (BEDNURS) study. Eur JClin Pharmacol. 2003 Sep;59(5-6):463-469.

    57. Elseviers MM, Vander Stichele RR, Van Bortel L. Drug utilization in Belgian nursing homes: impact of residents’and institutional characteristics. Pharmacoepidemiol Drug Saf. 2010 Oct;19(10):1041-1048.

    58. CaoYJ,Mager DE, Simonsick EM,Hilmer SN, Ling SM,WindhamBG, et al. Physical and cognitive performanceand burden of anticholinergics, sedatives, and ACE inhibitors in older women. Clin Pharmacol Ther. 2008Mar;83(3):422-429.

    59. Nava-Mesa MO, Jimenez-Diaz L, Yajeya J, Navarro-Lopez JD. GABAergic neurotransmission and new strategiesof neuromodulation to compensate synaptic dysfunction in early stages of Alzheimer’s disease. Front CellNeurosci. 2014 Jun 25;8:167.

    60. Fox C, Livingston G, Maidment ID, Coulton S, Smithard DG, Boustani M, et al. The impact of anticholinergicburden in Alzheimer’s dementia-the LASER-AD study. Age Ageing. 2011 Nov;40(6):730-735.

    61. Sloane PD, Zimmerman S, Gruber-Baldini AL, Hebel JR, Magaziner J, Konrad TR. Health and functionaloutcomes and health care utilization of persons with dementia in residential care and assisted living facilities:comparison with nursing homes. Gerontologist. 2005 Oct;45 Spec No 1(1):124-132.

    62. Salahudeen MS, Duffull SB, Nishtala PS. Anticholinergic burden quantified by anticholinergic risk scales andadverse outcomes in older people: a systematic review. BMC Geriatr. 2015 Mar 25;15:31-015-0029-9.

    63. Gray SL, Anderson ML, Dublin S, Hanlon JT, Hubbard R, Walker R, et al. Cumulative use of strong anticholin-ergics and incident dementia: a prospective cohort study. JAMA Intern Med. 2015 Mar;175(3):401-407.

    64. Duran CE, Azermai M, Vander Stichele RH. Systematic review of anticholinergic risk scales in older adults. EurJ Clin Pharmacol. 2013 Jul;69(7):1485-1496..

    65. Taxis K, Kochen S, Wouters H, Boersma F, Jan Gerard M, Mulder H, et al. Cross-national comparison ofmedication use in Australian and Dutch nursing homes. Age Ageing. 2017 Mar 1;46(2):320-323.

    66. Disalvo D, Luckett T, Agar M, Bennett A, Davidson PM. Systems to identify potentially inappropriate prescribingin people with advanced dementia: a systematic review. BMC Geriatr. 2016 May 31;16(1):114-016-0289-z.

    47

  • 48

  • ����������������������������������������������������

    ������������������������������������������������������������������������

    ������������������������������������

    ����������������������������������������������������������������������������������������

    ��������������� ��  � ��

  • Abstract

    This systematic review and meta-analysis examined the dose-response relationship between

    exercise and cognitive function in older adults with and without cognitive impairments.

    We included single-modality randomized controlled aerobic, anaerobic, multicomponent or

    psychomotor exercise trials that quantified training frequency, session and program duration

    and specified intensity quantitatively or qualitatively. We defined total exercise duration in

    minutes as the product of program duration, session duration, and frequency. For each study,

    we grouped test-specific Hedges’ d (n=163) and Cohen’s d (n=23) effect sizes in the domains

    Global cognition, Executive function and Memory. We used multilevel mixed-effects models

    to investigate dose-related predictors of exercise effects.In healthy older adults (n=23 studies),

    there was a small positive effect of exercise on executive function (d=0.27) and memory

    (d=0.24), but dose-parameters did not predict the magnitude of effect sizes. In older adults

    with cognitive impairments (n=13 studies), exercise had a moderate positive effect on global

    cognition (d=0.37). For older adults with cognitive impairments, we found evidence for

    exercise programs with a short session duration and high frequency to predict higher effect

    sizes (d=0.43-0.50). In healthy older adults, dose-parameters did not predict the magnitude of

    exercise effects on cognition. For older adults with cognitive impairments, exercise programs

    with shorter session duration and higher frequency may generate the best cognitive results.

    Studies are needed in which different exercise doses are directly compared among randomized

    subjects or conditions.

    50

  • Introduction

    The number of dementia patients may triple to 135M globally by 2050 [1]. Dementia is char-

    acterized by a progressive decline in neurocognitive function. Pharmacological treatments

    may moderate symptoms but can cause adverse effects [2]. Exercise might be an effective and

    safe alternative to drugs to slow cognitive decline. Exercise may improve certain cognitive

    functions in old age by inducing the release of brain-derived neurotrophic factor (BDNF

    [3,4]) and insulin-like growth factor-1 (IGF-1 [5,6]), thereby potentially facilitating structural

    and connectivity changes in the hippocampus, temporal lobe, frontal areas and corpus callo-

    sum [7-11], structures that are activated during tasks requiring executive function, attention,

    processing speed and memory.

    Exercise type and dose-parameters may determine the magnitude of effects on cognition

    and how long these effects persist after an intervention [12,13]. Dose-parameters include

    program duration (number of weeks), session duration (length of each session in minutes

    including warm-up and cool-down), frequency (session rate per week) and intensity. Exercise

    intensity refers to the amount of effort or energy that is required to perform a physical activity

    [14] and is often expressed as percentage of maximal oxygen update (VO2max) required

    during a physical activity [15].

    High compared with low exercise dose-parameters tend to predict better physical fitness

    outcomes in older adults. Meta-analyses revealed that longer program duration [16-18]

    and higher intensity [16,18] were associated with gains in muscle strength and VO2max of

    older adults. Program duration also correlated with gains in endurance, lower extremity

    muscle strength, balance and levels of Activities of Daily Living (ADL) in older subjects

    with dementia [19]. Exercise intensity was related to improvements in fitness- and health-

    related parameters such as VO2max and mortality in healthy middle-aged and older adults

    [20-25]. Exercise-induced improvements in physical fitness may facilitate brain plasticity and

    secondarily improvements in cognitive function through increases in brain activation. Indeed,

    higher cardiorespiratory fitness [26,27] and exercise-induced adaptions in blood lactate [28]

    were previously associated with higher brain activation in anterior and motor areas [26,27],

    fronto-cingulo-parietal networks [28] and better executive performance [26,27]. Considering

    that exercise dose-parameters are related to increases in fitness, and fitness increases may in

    turn be related to cognitive function by facilitating brain plasticity, exercise dose-parameters

    may be related to increases in cognitive functions. Indeed, in healthy young and older adults,

    51

  • high dose-parameters of acute exercise were related to gains in executive function such as

    processing speed and inhibitory control [29-32]. Exercise-induced cognitive benefits of acute

    exercise may accumulate to greater and lasting cognitive improvements with chronic exercise

    in a dose-specific way.

    The relationship between exercise dose-parameters and cognitive functions in chronic

    exercise studies is still not fully understood. A meta-analysis suggested that exercising

    for 45-60 minutes per session, at least at moderate intensity, and at the highest feasible

    frequency can improve global cognition, attention, executive function and (working) memory

    in healthy adults over 50 [33], but the authors did not examine total dose. A meta-analysis of

    18 randomized controlled trials (RCTs) [34] showed that weekly exercise duration (≤150or >150 minutes) was not related to changes in cognitive function in older adults with

    cognitive impairments, specifically Alzheimer’s disease (AD) and non-AD dementia, but

    other dose-parameters were not investigated. There is thus a need to systematically review

    whether improvements in cognitive function scale with exercise dose-parameters separately

    and as total dose and if dosing effects vary with cognitive status. The aim of the present

    review was to examine the relationship between exercise dose-parameters (program and

    session duration, frequency, intensity) and cognitive function (global cognition, executive

    function, memory) in adults with vs. without cognitive impairments. We quantified the dose-

    response relationship separately between the responses to aerobic, anaerobic, multimodal, and

    psychomotor interventions and changes in global cognition, executive function, and memory

    using advanced statistical modeling. We hypothesized that the magnitude of exercise effects

    on global cognition, executive function, and memory is related to exercise dose-parameters

    separately or combined as total dose. The results of the present study can be used to update

    exercise recommendations and implement exercise programs for older adults with and without

    cognitive impairments.

    Methods

    The current protocol is registered with the Open Science Framework

    (url: https://osf.io/qe43p/). PRISMA guidelines were followed [35] (S6 Checklist, online

    supplementary material).

    52

  • Search strategy and selection criteria

    We searched databases PubMed, Embase, Psycinfo, Web of Science and the Cochrane Central

    Reg