THE STRUCTURE MAGIC I

243

Click here to load reader

Transcript of THE STRUCTURE MAGIC I

Page 1: THE STRUCTURE MAGIC I

THE STRUCTURE /

OF MAGIC

I

A Book about Language

and Therapy

BY R I C H A R D P N DLER

a i d JOHN GRINDER

Page 2: THE STRUCTURE MAGIC I

Science and Behavior Books, Inc. Palo Alto, California 94306

O Copyright 1975 by Science and Behavior Books, Inc. Printed in the United States of America. All rights resewed. This book or parts thereof may not be reproduced in any form without written permission of the Publisher.

Library of Congress Card Number 75-1 2452 ISBN 0831 4-0044-7

Cover design from an original oil painting expressly done for Magic 1 by josh Doolan.

@C y b , ppography by Penguin I Santa Clara, California

Page 3: THE STRUCTURE MAGIC I

DEDICATION

We dedicate this book

to

Virginia Satir

in appreciation for giving us

her intuitions about

people.

These intuitions are

the basis of what

follows

in this book.

Thank you, Virginia.

We

love you.

Page 4: THE STRUCTURE MAGIC I
Page 5: THE STRUCTURE MAGIC I

TABLE OF CONTENTS

Preface

Warning to the Reader .............................. 1

............ . Chapter 1 THE STRUCTURE OF CHOICE 5 Experience and Perception as an

Active Process ........................... 8 Models and Therapy .......................... 13 So What? .................................. 18

Chapter 2 -- THE STRUCTURE OF LANGUAGE ......... 21 The Meta-Model for Language .................. 24 Some Universals o f the Human

Linguistic Process ................ ..... 25 The Transformational Model ................... 27 An Overview ................................ 35

Chapter 3 . THE STRUCTURE OF MAGIC ............. 39 The Meta-Model ............................. 40

.................... Deep Structure and Beyond 45 Challenging Deep Structure .................... 46 An Overview ................................ 53

Page 6: THE STRUCTURE MAGIC I

Table of Contents / iv

Chapter 4 . INCANTATIONS FOR GROWTH .............................. AND POTENTIAL 57

Deletion ................................... 59 ................... Distortion . Nominalizations 74

.............................. Generalization 80 ....................... Well-Formed in Therapy 107

..................... Chapter5 . INTOTHE VORTEX 111 ................................ Transcript 1 112 ................................ Transcript 2 134

Chapter 6 . ON BECOMING A ..................... SORCERER'S APPRENTICE 155

The Second Ingredient: ...................... Reference Structures 157

Enactment: The Instant Replay ............................ of Experience 164

Guided Fantasy . A Journey ........................ into the Unknown 166

..................... Therapeutic Double Binds 169 .............. Other Maps for the Same Territory 172

.................................. Congruity 174 .............................. Family Therapy 176

Conclusion . STRUCTURE OF THE FINAL . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . INCANTATION OF BOOK I 179

TABLE OF CONTENTS FOR .................. THE STRUCTURE OF MAGIC I1 181 A

Appendix A: A BRIEF OUTLINE OF ............... TRANSFORMATIONAL GRAMMAR 183

Appendix B: SYNTACTIC ENVIRONMENTS FOR IDENTIFYING NATURAL LANGUAGE

................. PRESUPPOSITIONS IN ENGLISH 211

......................................... Glossary 215

...................................... Bibliography 219

Page 7: THE STRUCTURE MAGIC I

ACKNOWLEDGMENTS

We would like to thank all o f those who have been helpful in the completion of this book: Jim Anderson and Kristofer Bakke, without whom this book would have taken twice as long, and the makers o f Folgers Coffee, without whose fine product we would not have made it through the long nights.

Page 8: THE STRUCTURE MAGIC I
Page 9: THE STRUCTURE MAGIC I

FOR WORD

WOW! What could anyone say about having their work looked at by four fine eyes in the heads o f two very capable human researchers? This book is the outcome o f the efforts o f two intriguing, smart, young men who are interested in finding out how change takes place and in documenting the process. They seem to have come up with a description o f the predictable elements that make change happen in a transaction between two people. Knowing what these elements are makes it possible to use them consciously and, thus, to have useful methods for inducing change.

I often say to people that I have a right to be a slow learner but educable. What this means to me as a therapist is that I have only one thought - to help the people who come to me in pain to make changes in their lives. How I use my body, my voice, my eyes, my hands, in addition to the words and the way I use words, is my only tool. Since my goal is to make change possible for everyone, every someone offers a new challenge.

Looking back, I see that, although I was aware that change was happening, I was unaware of the specific elements that went into the transaction which made change possible. For years, I wondered what it would be like to be on the other end of me, to view myself working, to view the process o f change from the other side. The authors spent hours looking at video tapes and listening to audio material, and they found patterns emerging which they could document. I do something, I feel it, I see it, my gut responds to it - that is a subjective experience. When I do it with someone

Page 10: THE STRUCTURE MAGIC I

vi i i I Fore word

else, their eyes, ears, body sense these things. What Richard Bandler and John Grinder have done is to watch the process o f change over a time and to distill from it the patterns o f the how process. What they learned relates particularly, in a sophisticated way, to mathematics, physics, neurology and linguistics.

It would be hard for me to write this Foreword without my own feeling of excitement, amazement and thrill coming through. I have been a teacher of family therapy for a long time, as well as a clinician and a theoretician. This means that I have seen change taking place in many families, and I have been involved in training many family therapists. I have a theory about how I make change occur. The knowledge o f the process is now considerably advanced by Richard Bandler and John Grinder, who can talk in a way that can be concretized and measured about the ingredients o f the what that goes into making the how possible.

Virginia M. Satir

Page 11: THE STRUCTURE MAGIC I

INTRODUCTION

It i s a strange pleasure to write an introduction for this book because John Grinder and Richard Bandler have done something similar to what my colleagues and I attempted fifteen years ago.

The task was easy to define: to create the beginnings of an appropriate theoretical base for the describing of human interaction.

The difficulty lay in the word "appropriate" and in the fact that what was to be described included not only the event se- 'quences of successful communication but also the patterns of misunderstanding and the pathogenic.

The behavioral sciences, and especially psychiatry, have always avoided theory, and it is easy to make a l is t of the various maneuvers whereby theory could be avoided: the historians (and some anthropologists) chose the impossible task of making not theory but more data out of what was known - a task for detectives and courts of law. The sociologists trimmed the com- plex variations of known fact to such an ultimate simplicity that the clipped nuggets could be counted. Economists believed in transitive preference. Psychologists accepted all sorts of internal explanatory entities (ego, anxiety, aggression, instinct, conflict, etc.) in a way reminiscent of medieval psycho-theology.

Psychiatrists dabbled in all these methods of explanation; they searched for narratives of childhood to explain current behavior, making new data out of what was known. They attempted to create statistical samples of morbidity. They wallowed in internal and mythical entities, ids and archetypes. Above all, they

Page 12: THE STRUCTURE MAGIC I

Introduction / x

borrowed the concepts o f physics and mechanics - energy, ten- sion, and the like - to create a scientism.

But there were a few beginnings from which to work: the "logical types" o f Russell and Whitehead, the "Games Theory" of Von Neumann, the notions o f comparable form (called "homol- ogy" by biologists), the concepts o f "levels" in linguistics, Von Domarus' analysis of "schizophrenic" syllogisms, the notion of discontinuity in genetics and the related notion of binary informa- tion. Pattern and redundancy were beginning to be defined. And, above all, there was the idea of homeostasis and self-correction in cybernetics.

Out of these scattered pieces came a hierarchic classification of orders of message and (therefore) of orders o f learning, the begin- nings of a theory o f "schizophrenia" and with it an attempt, very premature, to classify the ways in which people and animals code their messages (digital, analogic, iconic, kinesic, verbal, etc.).

Perhaps our greatest handicap at that time was the difficulty which the professionals seemed to experience when they tried to understand what we were doing. Some even tried to count "double binds" in recorded conversations. I treasure somewhere in my files a letter from a funding agency telling me that my work should be more clinical, more experimental, and, above all, more quantitative.

Grinder and Bandler have confronted the problems which we confronted then and this series is the result. They have tools which we did not have - or did not see how to use. They have succeeded in making linguistics into a base for theory and simultaneously into a tool for therapy. This gives them a double control over the psychiatric phenomena, and they have done something which, as I see it today, we were foolish to miss.

We already knew that most o f the premises of individual psychology were useless, and we knew that we ought to classify modes of communicating. But it never occurred to us to ask about the effects o f the modes upon interpersonal relations. I n this first volume, Grinder and Bandler have succeeded in making explicit the syntax o f how people avoid change and, therefore, how to assist them in changing. Here they focus on verbal communication. In the second volume, they develop a general model of communi- cation and change involving the other modes of communication which human beings use to represent and communicate their experience. What happens when messages in digital mode are flung at an analog thinker? Or when visual presentations are offered to an auditory client?

We did not see that these various ways o f coding - visual,

Page 13: THE STRUCTURE MAGIC I

Introduction / xi

auditory, etc. - are so far apart, so mutually different even in neurophysiological representation, that no material in one mode can ever be of the same logical type as any material in any other mode.

This discovery seems obvious when the argument starts from linguistics, as in the first volume o f the present series, instead o f starting from culture contrast and psychosis, as we did.

But, indeed, much that was so difficult to say in 1955 is strikingly easier to say in 1975.

May it be heard!

Gregory Bateson Kresge College University of California, Santa Cruz

Page 14: THE STRUCTURE MAGIC I
Page 15: THE STRUCTURE MAGIC I

Preface

Down through the ages the power and wonder o f practitioners of magic have been recorded in song and story. The presence o f wizards, witches, sorcerers, shamen, and gurus has always been intriguing and awe inspiring to the average person. These people o f power, wrapped in a cloak o f secrecy, presented a striking contra- diction to the common ways o f dealing with the world. The spells and incantations they wove were feared beyond belief and, at the same time, sought constantly for the help they could provide. Whenever these people o f power publicly performed their won- ders, they would both shatter the concepts o f reality of that time and place and present themselves as having something that was beyond learning. In modern time, the mantle o f the wizard is most often placed upon those dynamic practitioners o f psychotherapy who exceed the skill o f other therapists by leaps and bounds, and whose work is so amazing to watch that it moves us with powerful emotions, disbelief, and utter confusion. Just as with all wizards o f the ages o f the earth whose knowledge was treasured and passed down from sage to sage - losing and adding pieces but retaining a basic structure - so, too, does the magic o f these therapeutic wizards also have structure.

The Prince and the Magician Once upon a time there was a young prince who believed in all

things but three. He did not believe in princesses, he did not believe in islands, he did not believe in God. H i s father, the king, told h im that such things did not exist. As there were no prin-

Page 16: THE STRUCTURE MAGIC I

xiv / Preface

cesses or islands in his father's domains, and no sign of God, the prince believed his father.

But then, one day, the prince ran away from his palace and came to the next land. There, to his astonishment, from every coast he saw islands, and on these islands, strange and troubling, creatures whom he dared not name. As he was searching for a boat, a man in full evening dress approached him along the shore.

"Are those real islands?" asked the young prince. "Of course they are real islands," said the man in evening

dress. "And those strange and troubling creatures?" "They are all genuine and authentic princesses." "Then God must also exist!" cried the prince. "I am God," replied the man in evening dress, with a bow. The young prince returned home as quickly as he could. "So, you are back," said his father, the king. "I have seen islands, I have seen princesses, I have seen God,"

said the prince reproachfully. The king was unmoved. "Neither real islands, nor real princesses, nor a real God exist." "I saw them!" "Tell me how God was dressed."

.

"God was in full evening dress." "Were the sleeves of his coat rolled back?" The prince remembered that they had been. The king smiled. "That is the uniform of a magician. You have been deceived." At this, the pkince returned to the next land and went to the

same shore, where once again he came upon the man in full evening dress.

"My father, the king, has told me who you are," said the prince indignantly. "You deceived me last time, but not again. Now I know that those are not real islands and real princesses, because you are a magician."

The man on the shore smiled. "It is you who are deceived, my boy. In your father's king-

dom, there are many islands and many princesses. But you are under your father's spell, so you cannot see them."

The prince pensively returned home. When he saw his father, he looked him in the eye.

"Father, is it true that you are not a real king, but only a magician?"

The king smiled and rolled back his sleeves. "Yes, my son, I'm only a magician." "Then the man on the other shore was God."

Page 17: THE STRUCTURE MAGIC I

Preface / xv

"The man on the other shore was another magician." "I must know the truth, the truth beyond magic." "There is no truth beyond magic," said the king. The prince was full of sadness. He said, "I will kill myself." The king by magic caused death to appear. Death stood in the

door and beckoned to the prince. The prince shuddered. He remembered the beautiful but unreal islands and the unreal but beautiful princesses.

"Very well," he said, "I can bear it." "You see, my son," said the king, "you, too, now begin to be

a magician." Reprinted from The Magus, by John Fowles, Dell Publishing Co., Inc.; pp. 499-500.

Page 18: THE STRUCTURE MAGIC I
Page 19: THE STRUCTURE MAGIC I

Warning to the Reader

The central task o f psychology, whether experimental or applied, is the understanding o f human behavior. Human behavior is extremely complex. To say, however, that our behavior is complex is not to deny that it has structure. In general, modern psychology has attempted to understand human behavior by breaking it down into relatively separate areas o f study - for example, the areas o f perception, of learning, o f language be- havior, o f motor skills. As our understanding of each o f these areas grows, we continue to uncover the structure o f the human be- havior being described - to find that human behavior is rule governed.

To say that human behavior is rule governed is not to say that we can understand it in simple stimulus-response terms. In the study o f human languages, for example, the kind o f rules required to describe this behavior is beyond the capabilities o f S-R theories (Chomsky, 1957). It is useful for an adequate understanding o f this book that you distinguish between rule-governed behavior and determined behavior.

Continuing with the example o f human languages, the number of possible sentences in each human language (e.g., English, Spanish, etc.) is infinite. In other words, the number o f verbal descriptions o f human experiences is limitless. A t the same time, the number o f forms (syntax) in which this infinite set o f meanings is represented is highly restricted - has structure - and, therefore, may be described by a set of rules. This sequence o f words is an English sentence. It has structure, as can be demon-

Page 20: THE STRUCTURE MAGIC I

2 / Warning to the Reader

strated by considering the result of reversing the order o f words: Sentence English an is words of sequence this.

Similarly, in the case of other types o f complex human be- havior, there is an infinite number o f distinct acts. The form o f these acts will have structure - and, therefore, will be describable by some set of rules. To say that human behavior i s describable by some set o f rules is not to warrant that our behavior is determined or predictable.

The most sophisticated study o f human, rule-governed be- havior is the study of human language systems. Specifically, a group o f linguists known as transformational grammarians has developed a set of rules describing the forms which we use to represent and communicate our experience with language. Al- though transformational grammar is a young discipline (initiated in 1955), it has already had a profound effect on experimental psychology, especially modern learning theory. It has yet to have an impact on applied psychology. This book i s designed to make the insights of transformational grammar available and usable to those people who work with complex human behavior.

There are three important pieces o f information in addition to the above background which we want you to have as you begin this book:

1. What's in the book; 2. How to use the book; 3. What you can expect to gain from using the book.

1. What's in the Book This book is designed to give you an explicit set of tools which

will help you to become a more effective therapist. Chapter 1 shows that we do not operate directly on the world in which we live, but rather that we create models or maps o f the world and use these maps to guide our behavior. Further, it states that effective therapy implies some change in the way that a client represents his experience.

Chapter 2 shows you the structure o f one specific way human beings represent their experiences - human language systems. Chap- ter 3 presents a way of using the structure o f language systems as a set o f tools for operating in therapy. These tools are compatible with every form o f psychotherapy o f which we are aware. Chapter 4 presents a step-by-step procedure for learning and using these tools. Chapter 5 is composed of two transcripts with commentary showing the use o f these tools in therapy. Chapter 6 integrates these tools with a number o f well-known, non-verbal techniques from already established forms of psychotherapy.

Page 21: THE STRUCTURE MAGIC I

Warning to the Reader 1 3

2 How to Use this Book This book is not a novel, and we recommend that you not

attempt to read it as you would a novel. This book i s a manual to teach you a set o f tools which will increase your effectiveness as a therapist. As with any manual, it should be read and reread.

To begin this learning process for yourself, a general overall understanding o f Chapters 1, 2, and 3 is adequate. Naturally, the more thoroughly you understand these chapters, the more effec- tively you will be able to apply the specific techniques presented in Chapter 4.

When you reach Chapter 4, slow down. This chapter consists of a set o f step-by-step instructions to give you practice in the use of the techniques. Since this book, the first o f a series, is primarily concerned with verbal techniques, most of the techniques are questions based on the form of the client's communication in therapy. Each o f the techniques presented in Chapter 4 should be studied by i t se l f in order to give you the optimum skill to increase your effectiveness as a therapist. Each of these techniques has at least one step-by-step exercise. To acquire these skills, you must practice them - USE THE EXERCISES.

Chapter 5 is not an example of what we regard as powerful therapy. Chapter 5 is designed to show you how the various techniques work in conjunction with one another. Read through the transcript with i t s commentary, paying attention to the choices that the therapist has and the flow of the verbal exchange between the therapist and the client. You may also wish to cover the commentary and to consider each o f the client's sentences in turn, to determine whether you can identify all of the choices each o f these sentences presents to you as a therapist.

Read through Chapter 6 carefully - i t s purpose i s to teach you to use Chapter 4 techniques to identify the appropriateness o f some of the better known, non-verbal techniques. If any o f the non-verbal techniques presented in this chapter are techniques in which you are already trained, use them as a reference point to integrate other techniques which you find useful in your therapy. If none of your specific techniques is presented, pay particular attention to which o f the Chapter 4 techniques you are using in therapy when you become aware o f an appropriate place for you to employ one of your own specific techniques. This will begin the process of integration o f the tools presented in this manual with your own style of therapy.

3. What You Can Expect to Gain from Using this Book Using this book in the way we suggest will make you a more

Page 22: THE STRUCTURE MAGIC I

4 / Warning to the Reader

effective therapist. This will happen specifically by: 1. Learning a specific set o f questioning techniques based

on the client's verbal communications; 2. Learning how the use o f particular non-verbal tech-

niques may be indicated by verbal cues. The overall effect of this knowledge will be to give you a clear,

explicit strategy for your work in therapy.

Page 23: THE STRUCTURE MAGIC I

Chapter 1

THE STRUCTURE OF CHOICE

. . . operations of an almost mysterious character, which run counter to ordinary procedure in a more or less para- doxical way. They are methods which give an onlooker the impression of magic if he be not himself initiated or equally skilled in the mechanism.

H. Vaihinger, The Philosophy of As If , p. 11

Out of the ranks of modern psychotherapy have emerged a number of charismatic superstars. These people seemingly perform the task of clinical psychology with the ease and wonder of a therapeutic magician. They reach into suffering, pain, and dead- ness of others, transforming their hopelessness into joy, life and renewed hope. Though the approaches they bring to this task seem varied and as different as day and night, they all seem to share a unique wonder and potency. Sheldon Kopp described his experi- ence of one such person in his book Guru (p. 146):

Perls had enormously powerful personal presence, inde- pendence of spirit, willingness to risk going wherever his intuitive feelings took him, and a profound capacity to be intimately in touch with anyone who was open to working with him. . . . It is not unusual to find yourself in tears, or exhausted, or joyful, after watching another being guided through such an experience. So brilliant was his intuition and so powerful were his techniques that sometimes it took Perk only minutes to reach the person on the hot seat. You might be some stuck, rigid, long-dead character,

Page 24: THE STRUCTURE MAGIC I

6 / The Structure of Choice

seeking help and yet fearing that it would come and change things. He would put you on the ho t seat, then do h i s magic. I f you were willing to work, it was almost as though he could reach over, take hold of the zipper on your facade, and pull it down so quickly that your tor- tured soul would fall out onto the floor between the two of you.

Perk was not, and most certainly is not, the only therapist to present himself or herself with such magical potency. Virginia Satir and others we know seem to have this magical quality. To deny this capacity or to simply label it talent, intuition, orgenius is to l imit one's own potential as a people-helper. By doing this, one misses the opportunity to learn to offer those people who come to us an experience which they may use to change their lives to enjoy the fullness of living. Our desire in this book i s not to question the magical quality of our experience of these thera- peutic wizards, but rather to show that this magic which they perform - like other complex human activities such as painting, composing music, or placing a man on the moon - has structure and is, therefore, learnable, given the appropriate resources. Neither is it our intention to claim that reading a book can insure that you will have these dynamic qualities. We especially do not wish to make the claim that we have discovered the "right" or most powerful approach to psychotherapy.' We desire only to present you with a specific set of tools that seem to us to be implicit in the actions o f these therapists, so that you may begin or continue the never-ending process to improve, enrich, and enlarge the skills you offer as a people-helper.

Since this set of tools is not based upon some pre-existing psychological theory or therapeutic approach, we would like to present the simple overview of the human processes out o f which we have created these tools. We call this process modeling.

Through a Glass Darkly Where the logical function actively intervenes, it alters what i s given and causes it to depart from reality. We cannot even describe the elementary processes o f the psyche without at every step meeting this disturbing -o r shall we say helpful? - factor. As soon as sensation has entered the sphere o f the psyche, it i s drawn into the whirlpool o f the logical processes. The psyche quite o f i t s own accord alters both what i s given and presented. Two things are to be distinguished in this process: First, the

Page 25: THE STRUCTURE MAGIC I

The Structure of Choice / 7

actual forms in which this change takes place; and sec- ondly, the products obtained from the original material by this change.

The organized activity of the logical function draws into itself all the sensations and constructs an inner world of its own, which progressively departs from reality but yet at certain points still retains so intimate a connection with it that transitions from one to the other continually take place and we hardly notice that we are acting on a double stage - our own inner world (which, of course, we objectify as the world of sense-perception) and also an entirely different and external world.

H. Vaihinger, The Philosophy of As If, pp. 159-160

A number of people in the history of civilization have made this point - that there is an irreducible difference between the world and our experience of it. We as human beings do not operate directly on the world. Each of us creates a representation of the world in which we live - that is, we create a map or model which we use to generate our behavior. Our representation of the world determines to a large degree what our experience of the world will be, how we will perceive the world, what choices we will see available to us as we live in the world.

. I t must be remembered that the object of the world of ideas as a whole [the map or model - RWB/JTG] is not the portrayal of reality - this would be an utterly impos- sible task - but rather to provide us with an instrument for finding our way about more easily in the world.

H. Vaihinger, The Philosophy of As If, p. 15.

No two human beings have exactly the same experiences. The model that we create to guide us in the world iS based in part upon our experiences. Each of us may, then, create a different model of the world we share and thus come to live in a somewhat different reality.

. . . important characteristics of maps should be noted. A map is not the territory it represents, but, if correct, it has a similar structure to the territory, which accounts for its usefulness. . .

A. Korzybski, Science & Sanity, 4th Ed., 1958, pp. 58-60.

We want to make two points here. First, there is a necessary difference between the world and any particular model or repre-

Page 26: THE STRUCTURE MAGIC I

8 / The Structure of Choice

sentation o f the world. Second, the models o f the world that each of us creates will themselves be different. There are a number o f ways in which this can be demonstrated. For our purposes, we have divided them into three areas2 neurological constraints, social constraints, and individual constraints.

EXPERIENCE AND PERCEPTION AS AN ACTIVE PROCESS

Neurological Constraints Consider the human receptor systems: sight, hearing, touch,

taste, and smell. There are physical phenomena which l ie outside the limits o f these five accepted sensory channels. For example, sound waves either below 20 cycles per second or above 20,000 cycles per second cannot be detected by human beings. Yet these physical phenomena are structurally the same as the physical waves which fall between these limiting figures: the physical waves which we call sound. In the human visual system, we are able to detect wave forms only between 380 and 680 milli-microns. Wave forms above or below these figures are not detectable by the human eye. Again, we perceive only a portion of a continuous physical phenomenon as determined by our genetically given neurological limitations.

The human body is sensitive to touch - to contact on the surface o f the skin. The sense o f touch provides an excellent example o f the profound influence our own neurological system can have on our experience. In a series o f experiments (Boring, 1957, pp. 11 0-1 11) over a century ago, Weber established the fact that precisely the same real world situation is perceived by a human being as two totally distinct tactile experiences. I n his experiments, Weber found that our ability to perceive being touched at two points on the surface o f our skin varied dramat- ically depending upon where on the human body the two points were located. The smallest distance between two points which are experienced as two separate points on the l i t t l e finger must be expanded thirty times before the two points can be distinguished when applied to the upper arm. Thus, a whole range o f identical, real-world stimulus situations are perceived as two totally different experiences solely as a function o f our nervous system. When touched on the little finger, we experience it as being touched in two places, and on the upper arm as being touched in one place. The physical world remains constant and our experience o f it shifts dramatically as a function o f our nervous system.

Similar differences between the world and our experience o f it

Page 27: THE STRUCTURE MAGIC I

The Structure of Choice / 9

can be demonstrated for the other senses (Boring, 1957). The limitations of our perception are clearly recognized by scientists conducting experiments with the physical world as they develop machines which extend these limits. These instruments sense phe- nomena which lie outside the range of our senses, or outside of our ability to discriminate, and present them as signals which fall within our sensory range - signals such as photographs, pressure gauges, thermometers, oscilloscopes, Geiger counters, and alpha

I wave detectors. Thus, one way in which our models of the world '

I will necessarily differ from the world itself is that- our ne.wous system systematically distorts and deletes whole portions of the real world. This has the effect of reducing the range of possible human experience as well as introducing differences between what is actually going on in the world and our experience of it. Our nervous system, then, initially determined genetically, constitutes the first set of filters which distinguish the world - the territory - from our representations of the world - the map.

THROUGH A GLASS DARKLY WITH GLASSES WITH SOCIAL PRESCRIPTIONS

Social Constraints . . . The suggestion is that the function of the brain and nervous system and sense organs is in the main eliminative and not productive. Each person is at each moment ca- pable of remembering all that has ever happened to him and of perceiving everything that is happening everywhere in the universe. The function of the brain and the nervous system is to protect us from being overwhelmed and con- fused by this mass of largely useless and irrelevant knowl- edge, by shutting out most of what we should otherwise perceive or remember at any moment, and leaving only that very small and special selection which is likely to be practically useful. According to such a theory, each one of us is potentially Mind at Large. . . . To make biological survival possible, Mind at Large has to be funneled through the reducing valve of the brain and nervous system. What comes out the other end is a measly trickle of the kind of consciousness which will help us to stay alive on the surface of this particular planet. To formulate and express the contents of this reduced awareness, man has invented and endlessly elaborated upon those symbol-systems and implicit philosophies which we call languages. Every indi-

Page 28: THE STRUCTURE MAGIC I

10 / The Structure of Choice . vidual i s at once the beneficiary and the victim of the linguistic tradition into which he has been born - the beneficiary inasmuch as language gives access t o the ac- cumulated record of other people's experience, the victim insofar as it confirms in him the belief that reduced aware- ness i s the only awareness, and as it bedevils his sense of reality, so that he i s all too apt t o take his concepts for I

data, his words for actual things. Aldous Huxley, The Doors of Perception, New York: Harper & Row, 1954, pp. 22-23.

A second way in which our experience o f the world differs from the world itself is through the set o f social constraints or filters (prescription glasses) - we refer t o these as social genetic factor^.^ By social genetics, we refer to all the categories or filters to which we are subject as members o f a social system: our language, our accepted ways o f perceiving, and all the socially agreed upon fictions.

Perhaps the most commonly recognized social genetic filter is our language system. Within any particular language system, for example, part o f the richness o f our experience is associated with the number o f distinctions made in some area of our ~ensation.~ In Maidu, an American Indian language o f Northern California, only three wordsS are available to describe the color spectrum. They divide the spectrum as follows (the English words given are the closest approximations):

lak tit tulak

I (red)

I (green- blue)

I (yellow-orange-brown)

While human beings are capable of making 7,500,000 different color distinctions in the visible color spectrum (Boring, 1957), the people who are native speakers o f Maidu habitually group their experience into the three categories supplied by their language. These three Maidu color terms cover the same range o f real-world sensation which the eight (specific) color terms o f English do. Here the point is that a person who speaks Maidu is characteris- tically conscious o f only three categories o f color experience while the English speaker has more categories and, therefore, more habitual perceptual distinctions. This means that, while English speakers will describe their experience o f two objects as different (jay, a yellow book and an orange book), speakers o f Maidu will typically describe their experience o f the identical real-world

Page 29: THE STRUCTURE MAGIC I

The Structure o f Choice / 1 1

situation as being the same (two tulak books). Unlike our neurological genetic limitations, those introduced

by the social genetic filters are easily overcome. This is most clearly demonstrated by the fact that we are able to speak more than one language - that is, we are able to use more than one set of social linguistic categories or filters to organize our experience, to serve as our representation of the world.6 For example, take the ordinary sentence: The book is blue. Blue is the name that we, as native speakers of English, have learned to use to describe our experience of a certain portion of the continuum of visible light. Misled by the structure of our language, we come to assume that blue is a property of the object that we refer to as book rather than being the name which we have given our sensation.

In perception, the sensation complex sweet-white is con- stantly occurring in the substance sugar. The psyche then applies to this combination the category of a thing and its attributes: The sugar is sweet. Here, however, the white appears also as an object. Sweet is an attribute. The psyche is acquainted with the sensation white in other cases, where it appears as an attribute, so that, in this case too, white is treated as an attribute. But the category thing- attribute is inapplicable if sweet and white are attributes and no other sensation is given. Here language comes to our help, and by applying the name sugar to the whole perception, enables us to treat the single sensation as attributes. . . . Who authorized thought to assume that white was a thing, that sweet was an attribute? What right had it to go on to assume that both were attributes and then mentally add an object as their carrier? The justifica- tion can be found neither in the sensations themselves nor in what we now regard as reality. . . . All that is given to consciousness is sensation. By adding a Thing to which sensations are supposed to adhere as attributes, thought commits a very serious error. It hypostasizes sensation, which in the last analysis is only a process, as a subsistent attribute, and ascribes this attribute to a thing that either exists only in the complex of sensations itself, or has been simply added by thought to what has been sensed. . . . Where is the sweet that is ascribed to the sugar? It exists only in the act of sensation. . . . Thought not only changes immediate sensation thereby, but withdraws further and further from reality and becomes increasingly entangled in its own forms. By means of the creative faculty - to use this scientific term - it has invented a Thing which is

Page 30: THE STRUCTURE MAGIC I

1 2 1 The Structure of Choice

supposed t o possess an Attribute. This Thing i s a fiction, the Attribute as such i s a fiction, and the whole relation- ship is a fiction.

H. Vaihinger, The Philosophy of As If , p. 167.

The categories o f experience which we share with other mem- bers of the social situation in which we live - for example, the common languge which we share - are a second way in which our models of the world differ from the world itself.

Notice that, in the case o f the neurological constraints, in normal circumstances, the neurological filters are the same for all human beings - this is the common basis o f experience that we share as members o f the species. The social genetic filters are the same for the members o f the same social-linguistic community but there are a large number o f different social-linguistic communities.

I Thus, the second set o f filters begins to distinguish us from each other as human beings. Our experiences begin to differ more radically, giving rise to more dramatically different representations

' of the world. The third set o f constraints - the individual con- straints - are the basis for the most far-reaching differences among us as humans.

THROUGH A GLASS DARKLY WlTH GLASSES WlTH INDIVIDUAL PRESCRIPTIONS

Individual Constraints A third way in which our experience o f the world can differ

from the world itself is through a set o f filters we call individual constraints. By individual constraints we refer to all the represen- tations we create as human beings based upon our unique personal history. Every human being has a set o f experiences which consti- tute his own personal history and are as unique to him as are his fingerprints. Just as every person has a set o f distinct fingerprints, so, too, does each person have novel experiences of growing up and living, and no two life histories will ever be identical. Again, though they may have similarities, at least some aspects are differ- ent and unique to each person. The models or maps that we create in the process o f living are based upon our individual experiences, and, since some aspects o f our experiences will be unique to us as a person, some parts o f our model o f the world will be singular to each of us. These uncommon ways each o f us represents the world will constitute a set o f interests, habits, likes, dislikes, and rules for behavior which are distinctly our own. These differences in our

Page 31: THE STRUCTURE MAGIC I

The Structure of Choice / 13

experiences will guarantee that each of us has a model of the world which in some way will be different from any other person's model of the world.

For example, two identical twins might grow up together in the same home with the same parents, having almost identical experiences, but each, in the process of watching their parents relate to each other and to the rest of the family, might model their experiences differently. One might say: my parents never loved each other very much - they always argued, and my twin sister was the favorite - while the other might say: my parents really cared about each other - they discussed everything exten- sively and they really favored my twin sister. Thus, even in the limiting case of identical twins, their experiences as persons will give rise to differences in the way they create their own models or perceptions of the world. In cases in which our discussion is of unrelated persons, the differences created in personal models will be greater and more pervasive.

This third set of filters, the individual constraints, constitutes the basis for the profound differences among us as humans and the way we create models of the world. These differences in our models can either be ones that alter our prescriptions (socially given) in a way that enriches our experience and offers us more choices, or ones that impoverish our experience in a way that limits our ability to act effectively.

MODELS AND THERAPY

Our experience has been that, when people come to us in therapy, they typically come with pain, feeling themselves para- lyzed, experiencing no choices or freedom of action in their lives. What we have found is not that the world is too limited or that there are no choices, but that these peol?le.black.thernselves from seeing those options and possibilitiesthat are open to them since \ ' I they are not available in their models of their world.

Almost every human being in our culture in his l i fe cycle has a number of periods o f change and transition which he must negoti- ate. Different forms of psychotherapy have developed various categories for these important transition-crisis points. What's pe- culiar is that some people are able to negotiate these periods of change with l i t t l e difficulty, experiencing these periods as times of intense energy and creativity. Other people, faced with the same challenges, experience these periods as times of dread and pain - periods to be endured, when their primary concern is simple

Page 32: THE STRUCTURE MAGIC I

14 / The Structure of Choice

survival. The difference between these two groups appears to us to be primarily that the people who respond creatively to and cope effectively with this stress are people who have a rich representa- tion or model of their situation, one in which they perceive a wide range of options in choosing their actions. The other people experience themselves as having few options, none of which are attractive to them - the "natural loser" game. The question for us is: How is it possible for different human beings faced with the same world to have such different experiences? Our understanding is that this difference follows primarily from differences in the richness of their models. Thus, the question becomes: How is it possible for human beings to maintain an impoverished model which causes them pain in the face of a multi-valued, rich, and complex world?

In coming to understand how it is that some people continue to cause themselves pain and anguish, it has been important for us to realize that they are not bad, crazy, or sick. They are, in fact, making the best choices from those of which they are aware, that is, the best choices available in their own particular model. In other words, human beings' behavior, no matter how .bizarre it may first appear to be, makes sense when it is seen in the context of the choices generated by their model.' The difficulty is not that they are making the wrong choice, but that they do not have enough choices - they don't have a richly focused image of the world. The most pervasive paradox of the human condition which we see i s that the processes which allow us to survive, grow, change, and experience joy are the same processes which allow us to maintain an impoverished model of the world - our ability to manipukge-:symbols, that is, to create-mdels. So the proces;es which allow us to accomplish the most extraordinary and unique human activities are the same processes which block our further growth if we commit the error of mistaking the model for the reality. We can identify three general mechanisms by which we do this:' Generalization, Deletion, and Distortion.

Generalization i s the process by which elements or pieces of a person's model become detached from their original experience and come to represent the entire category of which the experience is an example. bu r ability to generalize i s essential to coping with the world. For example, it is useful for us to be able to generalize from the experience of being burned when we touch a hot stove to a rule that hot stoves are not to be touched. But to generalize this experience to a perception that stoves are dangerous and, there- fore, to refuse to be in the same room with one is to limit unnecessarily our movement in the world.

Page 33: THE STRUCTURE MAGIC I

The Structure of Choice 1 15

Suppose that the first few times a child is around a rocking chair, he leans on the back and falls over. He might come to a rule for himself that rocking chairs are unstable and refuse to ever try them again. If this child's model o f the world lumps rocking chairs with chairs in general, then all chairs fall under the rule: Don't lean on the back! Another child who creates a model which distinguishes rocking chairs from other kinds o f chairs has more choices in her behavior. From her experience, she develops a new rule or generalization for using rocking chairs only - Don't lean on the back! - and, therefore, has a richer model and more choices.

The same process o f generalization may lead a human being to establish a rule such as "Don't express feelings." This rule in the context o f a prisoner-of-war camp may have a high survival value and will allow the person to avoid placing himself in a position o f being punished. However, that person, using this same rule in a marriage, limits his potential for intimacy by excluding expres- sions which are useful in that relationship. This may lead him to have feelings o f loneliness and disconnectedness - here the person feels that he has no choice, since the possibility o f expressing feelings is not available within his model.

The point here is that the same rule will be useful or not, depending upon the context - that is, that there are no right generalizations, that each model must be evaluated in i t s context. Furthermore, this gives us a key to understanding human behavior that seems to us to be bizarre or inappropriate - that is, if we can ,V see the person's behavior in the context in which it originated.

A second mechanism which we can use either to cope effec- tively or to defeat ourselves is Deletion. Deletion is a process by which we selectively pay attention to certain dimensions o f our $' experience and exclude others. Take, for example, the ability that people have to filter out or exclude all other sound in a room full of people talking in order to listen to one particular person's voice. Using the same process, people are able to block themselves from hearing messages o f caring from other people who are important to them. For example, a man who was convinced that he was not worth caring about complained to us that his wife never gave him messages o f caring. When we visited this man's home, we became aware that the man's wife did, indeed, express messages o f caring to him. However, as these messages conflicted with the generaliza- tion that the man had made about his own self-worth, he literally did not hear his wife. This was verified when we called the man's attention to some o f these messages, and the man stated that he had not even heard his wife when she had said those things.

Page 34: THE STRUCTURE MAGIC I

16 / The Structure of Cho~ce

2' Deletion reduces the world to proportions which we feel -2 capable of handling. The reduction may be useful in some con-

texts and yet be the source of pain for us in others. The third modeling process is that of Distortion. Distortion is

the process which allows us to make shifts in our experience of sensory data. Fantasy, for example, allows us to prepare for experiences which we may have before they occur. People will distort present reality when rehearsing a speech which they will later present. It is this process which has made possible all the artistic creations which we as humans have produced. A sky as represented in a painting by Van Gogh is possible only as Van Gogh was able to distort his perception of the time-place in which he was located at the moment of creation. Similarly, all the great novels, all the revolutionary discoveries of the sciences involve the ability to distort and misrepresent present reality. Using the same technique, people can limit the richness of their experience. For example, when our friend mentioned earlier (who had made the generalization that he was not worth caring for) had the caring messages from his wife pointed out to him, he immediately dis- torted them. Specifically, each time that he heard a caring message that he had previously been deleting, he turned to us, smiling, and said, "She j ~ s t says that because she wants something." In this way, the man was able to avoid allowing his experience to contra- dict the model of the world he had created, and, thereby, he prevented himself from having a richer representation, blocking himself from a more intimate and satisfying relationship with his wife.

r A person who has at some time in his l i fe been rejected makes the generalization that he's not worth caring for. As his model has this generalization, he either deletes caring messages or he reinter- prets these messages as insincere. As he is unaware of any caring messages from others, he is able to maintain the generalization that he isn't worth caring about. This description is an example of the classical positive feedback loop: the self-fulfilling prophecy, or forward feedback (Pribram, 1967). A person's generalizations or expectations filter out and distort his experience to make it consistent with those expectations. As he has no experiences which challenge his generalizations, his expectations are confirmed

i and the cycle continues. In this way people maintain their im- , poverished models of the world.

Consider the classical psychological set or expectancy experi- ment by Postman and Bruner:

. . . In a psychological experiment that deserves to be far better known outside the trade, Bruner and Postman asked

Page 35: THE STRUCTURE MAGIC I

The Structure of Choice 1 1 7

experimental subjects to identify on short and controlled exposure a series of playing cards. Many of the cards were normal, but some were made anomalous, e.g., a red six of spades and a black four of hearts. Each experimental run was constituted by the display of a single card to a single subject in a series of gradually increased exposures. After each exposure the subject was asked what he had seen, and the run was terminated by two successive correct identifications.

Even on the shortest exposures many subjects identi- fied most of the cards, and after a small increase all the subjects identified them all. For the normal cards these identifications were usually correct, but the anomalous cards were almost always identifed, without apparent hesi- tation or puzzlement, as normal. The black four of hearts might, for example, be identified as the four of either spades or hearts. Without any awareness of trouble, it was immediately fitted to one of the conceptual categories prepared by prior experience. One would not even like to say that the subjects had seen something different from what they identified. With a further increase of exposure to the anomalous cards, subjects did begin to hesitate and t o display awareness of anomaly. Exposed, for example, to the red six of spades, some would say: That's the six of spades, but there's something wrong with it - the black has a red border. Further increase of exposure resulted in still more hesitation and confusion until finally, and some- times quite suddenly, most subjects would produce the correct identification without hesitation. Moreover, after doing this with two or three of the anomalous cards, they would have little further difficulty with the others. A few subjects, however, were never able to make the requisite adjustment of their categories. Even at forty times the average exposure required to recognize normal cards for what they were, more than 10 per cent of the anomalous cards were not correctly identified. And the subjects who then failed often experienced acute personal distress. One of them exclaimed: "I can't make the suit out, whatever it is. It didn't even look like: a card that time. I don't know what color it is now 'or whether it's a spade or a heart. I'm not even sure now what a spade looks like. My God!" In the next section we shall occasionally see scientists be- having this way, too.

Either as a metaphor or because it reflects the nature

Page 36: THE STRUCTURE MAGIC I

The Structure of Language / 27

(12) Sandy looked the number up. 4. Referential Indices: Native speakers can determine

whether a word or phrase picks out a particular object in their experience such as my car or whether it iden- tifies a class o f objects: cars. Furthermore, they make consistent judgments about whether two (or more) words refer to the same object or class, e.g., the words Jackson and himself in the sentence

(1 3) Jackson changed himself. 5. Presuppositions: Native speakers can determine what

the experience of the speaker is for him to say a sentence. For example, if I say the sentence

(1 4) My cat ran away. you are entitled (have reason) to believe that, in my experience o f the world, it 's true that

(15) lhaveacat. -

These three general categories o f intuitions that human beings have about their language are represented explicitly in the trans- formational model.

.(

THE TRANSFORMATIONAL MODEL

We will describe how the consistent intuitions we identified about our language are represented in the Meta-model - the model of transformational grammar.

Linguists using this model work to represent these intuitions which are available to every native speaker in an explicit way. Native speakers have two kinds o f consistent intuitions about every sentence o f their language. They are able to determine how the smaller units, such as words, go together to make up the sentence (intuitions about constituent structure) and also what a complete representation o f the sentence would be (the complete- ness o f the logical representation). For example, when presented with a sentence:

( 1 6) The woman bought a truck. a native speaker can group the words into constituents or larger level units such as:

/The woman/ and /bought/ and /a truck/ They will, in turn, group these upits into

/The woman/ and /bought a truck/ The linguist represents these intuitions about what goes together inside a sentence by placing words which form a constituent (such

Page 37: THE STRUCTURE MAGIC I

28 / The Structure of Language

as the and woman) in what linguists call a tree structure which looks like:

The rule is that words which we as native speakers group into a single constituent are attached to the same point or node in the tree structure. The tree structure representation for (1 6) is:

This is called the Surface Structure. The second kind of consistent intuitions that native speakers

have about a sentence such as (1 6) is what a complete representa- tion of its meaning or logical semantic relation would be. One way which these intuitions are represented is:

a truck

This is called the Deep Structure. We are demonstrating how, within the transformational model,

each sentence is analyzed at two levels of-structure corresponding -- A -- -- - --- - to two~~consistent kTnassof intu i t i o z which native speakers have:

4 - SurfaceStructure-- in which their,,htuitions about constituent structure are given--a fi-ee sti-ucture representation - and- ~ e < ~ Striictuie - -in whicri their-intuitions, about what ,q-~o"rffplete' representation of the logicar semantic relations is, are given. Since the model gives two representations for each 'sentence (Surface

Page 38: THE STRUCTURE MAGIC I

The Structure of Language / 29

Structure and Deep Structure), linguists have the job of stating explicitly how these two levels are connected. The way in which they represent this connection is a process or derivation which is a series of transformations.

What Transformations Are A transformation is an explicit statement of one kind of f r

pattern which native speakers recognize among the sentences of their language. For example, compare the two sentences:

(1 7) The woman bought the truck. (1 8) The truck was bought by the woman.

Native speakers recognize that, although these Surface Structures are different, the message communicated, or Deep Structures, of these two sentences is the same. The process by which these two sentences are derived from their common Deep Structure is called a derivation. A derivation is a series of transformations which connects the Deep Structure and the Surface Structure. The deri- vation of one of these two Surface Structures includes the trans- formation called the Passive Transformation. If you examine (1 7) and (18), you will notice that the order of the words is different. Specifically, the phrases the woman and the truck have been transposed. Transformational grammarians state this pattern as:

T passive: Noun phrase1 I Verb Noun phrase2 - L the woman bought the truck

1 Noun phrase2 Be + Verb by + Noun phrase1 - - the truck was bought by the woman

where the symbol ---. means "can be transformed into"

Notice that the statement of this pattern is not limited to just the two sentences (1 7) and (18), but is general in English:

(1 9) a. Susan followed Sam. b. Sam was followed by Susan.

(20) a. The tapeworm ate the president. b. The president was eaten by the tapeworm.

(21 ) a. The bee touched the flower. b. The flower was touched by the bee.

Page 39: THE STRUCTURE MAGIC I

30 / The Structure of Language

This is a simple example o f how two Surface Structures whose derivations differ by only one transformation - the Passive Trans- formation applied in the derivation o f the (b) versions, but not the (a) versions - are formed. Derivations can be much more complex; for example:

(22) a. Timothy thought that Rosemary was guiding the spaceship.

b. The spaceship was thought by Timothy to have been guided by Rosemary.

What all these pairs of sentences demonstrate is that Deep Struc- tures may differ from their related Surface Structures by having the elements or words occur in a different order. Notice that in each pair of sentences, although the word order is different, the meaning appears to be constant. For each pair of sentences which have the same meaning, but different word orders, the linguist states a transformation which specifies exactly the pattern - the way the word order may differ.

Thus, the way that the native speaker's intuition o f synonymy is represented is by stating a transformation which relates the two or more Surface Structures which are synonymous or have the same meaning. For each set of two or more Surface Structures which are synonymous, therefore, the transformational linguist states what the formal patterning is - the transformation. The tes t for synonymy intuitionally is to attempt to imagine whether it would be possible in our (or any imaginary) consistent world that one of the Surface Structures you are testing for synonymy would be true (false) and the other Surface Structure not true (not false). If they always have the same value (both true or both false), they are synonymous. This is known as the paraphrase test. There are a number o f word-order-changing transformations which linguists have identified. The following pairs show some o f these patterns:

(23) a. I want Borsch. b. Borsch, / want.

(24) a. It is easy to scare Barry. b. Barry is easy to scare.

(25) a. George gave Martha an apple. b. George gave an apple to Martha.

(26) a. The Watergate 500 stumbled away. b. A way stumbled the Watergate 500.

(27) a. Writing this sentence is easy. b. It is easy to write this sentence.

Each o f these transformations specifies a way in which word orders can differ, and as a group are called Permutation Transformations.

Page 40: THE STRUCTURE MAGIC I

The Structure of Language / 31

Permutation Transformations are one o f the two major classes o f transformations; the other is called Deletion Transformations. For example:

(28) a. llene talked to someone a great deal. b. llene talked a great deal.

In the (b) version o f (28), one o f the Noun Phrases (i.e., to someone) has been deleted or removed. The general transforma- tion which states this pattern i s called Indefinite Noun Phrase Deletion.

Indef. Noun Phrase Dele.: X Verb Noun Phrase Y --

Ilene talked to someone a great deal

t X Verb Y *L

Ilene talked a great deal

where X and Y are cover symbols or variables for any word(s) in those positions

Once again, there are a number o f deletion transformations which linguists have identified:

Fluffo went to the store and Tab went to the store too. Fluffo went to the store and Tab went too. Tripod ate something. Tripod ate. Natural struck the wall with something. Natural struck the wall.

In each of these pairs, the process or derivation o f the second version includes a transformation which has deleted part o f the complete logical semantic representation which is present in Deep Structure. Again, the meaning appears to remain the same even as elements o f the Deep Structure are deleted.

Linguists distinguish two types of deletion transformations - Free Deletion, or deletion of indefinite elements, and Identity Deletion. Notice in the example pairs:

Page 41: THE STRUCTURE MAGIC I

36 / The Structure of Language

The World-The Complete Linguistic Representation

--I Deep Structure Transformations -

(Derivation) Surface Structure

The Representation (communicated) of the Complete Representation

Meta-model. This Meta-model represents our intuitions about our experience. For example, our intuition of synonymy - the case in which t w o 2 r more Surface Structures have the same semantic meaning, i.e., the same Deep Structure - is represented as:

Deep Structure \

Derivations :- ,_ (series o f \ -% -- transformations) ,I '. - -I --

surface surface Surface Structure 1. Structure 2. Structure 3.

In terms o f a specific example, then:

Deep Structure: Joe says Mary hit Sam. s Surface Surface Surface

Structure 1. Structure 2. Structure 3.

Joe says that Joe says that Sam Sam was said by Mary hit Sam. was hit by Mary. Joe to have been

hit by Mary.

Synonymy in the Meta-model means that the same Deep Structure is connected with more than one Surface Structure.

Ambiguity is the opposite case. Ambiguity is the intuition that native speakers use when the same Surface Structure has more than one distinct semantic meaning and is represented as: (See top of page 37)

Ambiguity in the Meta-model is the case wherein more than one Deep Structure is connected by transformations with the same Surface Structure.

The intuition of well-formedness is represented in the Meta- model in that any sequence o f words is well formed just in case

Page 42: THE STRUCTURE MAGIC I

* The Structure of Language / 37

Deep Structure Deep Structure Deep Structure - \ I / /

Derivation 2, (series o f

* . b

transformations)

Surface Structure

As a specific example:

Deep S t r ~ . ~ FBI agents who Deep S t r ~ c . ~ For someone to are conducting investigate FBI investigations can agents can be be dangerous for dangerous for someone. someone.

Surface Structure: Investigating FBI agents can be dangerous.

there is a series o f transformations (a derivation) which carries some Deep Structure into that sequence of words - a Surface Structure. Thus, the Meta-model is an. explicit representation o f our unconscious, rule-governed behavior.

SUMMARY

Human language is a way o f representationing the world. Transformational Grammar is an explicit model o f the process o f representing and o f communicating that representation o f the world. The mechanisms within Transformational Grammar are universal to all human beings and the way in which we represent our experience. The semantic meaning which these processes rep- resent i s existential, infinitely rich and varied. The way in which these existential meanings are represented and communicated is rule governed. Transformationafi Grammar models not the existen- tial meaning, but the way that infinite set is formed - the rules o f representations themselves.,

The nervous system which is responsible for producing the representational system of language is the same nervous system by which human's produce every other model of the world -

Page 43: THE STRUCTURE MAGIC I

C

38 1 The Structure of Language

thinking, visual, kinistic, etc.. . . The same principles of structure I

I are operating in each o f these systems. Thus, the formal principles I which linguists have identified as part o f the representational system called language provide an explicit approach to under- standing any system of human modeling.

FOOTNOTES FOR CHAPTER 2

1. This use o f language to communicate is actually a special case of the use of language to represent.kommunication is, in this way of thinking, the representation to others of our representation to ourselves. In other words, we use language to represent our experience - this i s a private process. We then use language to represent our representation o f our experience - a social process.

2. The symbol * will be used in this book to identify sequences of English words which are not well-formed sentences o f English.

3. We provide an appendix, which presents the transformational model more thoroughly, and a selective, annotated bibliography for those who wish to further examine the transformational model of language.

4. This i s not true of all linguists who may refer to themselves as transformationalists. The present split in the field - Extended Standard Theorists and Generative Semanticists - is not relevant for our purposes in adapting certain portions o f the Transformational model for our Meta-inodel for therapy. The recent work, especially by people in Generative Semantics, will be useful, we believe, in expanding the Meta-model we present here. See the bibliography for sources.

5. Strictly speaking, the deletion of the elements deleted in the text i s not legitimate from a purely linguistic point of view, as they are carrying referential indices - the process, however, i s typical of clients in therapy.

Page 44: THE STRUCTURE MAGIC I

Chapter 3

THE STRUCTURE OF MAGIC

\

One of the mysteries in the field of therapy is that, although the various schools of therapy have very different forms, they all

:eed t o some degree. This puzzle will be solved when the ctive methods shared by the different psychotherapies can be xibed in a single set of terms, thus making the similarities licit and thereby learnable by therapists of any school.'

. . . this list of similarities [among the various forms of psychotherapy - RBIJG] is hardly comprehensive'; there would seem to be sufficient indication that a more thor- ough study of all forms of psychotherapy in terms of their similar formal patterns would be rewarding. A more rig- orous science of psychotherapy will arrive when the proce- dures in the various methods can be synthesized down to the most effective strategy possible t o induce a person ta spontaneously behave in a different matter.

J. Haley, Strategies o f Psychotherapy, 1967, p. 85

The one feature that is present in all forms of therapy when I are successful is that the people in therapy change in some . This change is given different names by different schools of apy, such as: 1) fixing, 2) cure, 3) growth, 4) enlightenment, )ehavior modification, etc. Whatever the name given the phe- ienon, it somehow makes the person's experience richer and :er. This is not surprising as every form of therapy claims to , people operate more successfully in the world. When people Tge, their experience and model of the world is different. No

Page 45: THE STRUCTURE MAGIC I

40 1 The Structure of Magic I

matter what their techniques, the different forms of therapy make it possible for people to change their model of the world and some make part of that model new. I

What we are offering here is not a new school of therapy, but 1 rather a specific set of tools/techniques which are an explicit representation of what is already present to some degree in each I

form of therapy. The unique aspects of the Meta-model we are presenting are: first, that it is based on the intuitions already available to every native speaker, and second, it is an explicit I

model in that it is learnable. /

THE META-MODEL

The Meta-model we are presenting is in large part inspired by the formal model developed in transformational linguistics. Since the transformational model was created to answer questions which are not immediately connected with the way that humans change, not all portions of it are equally useful in creating a Meta-model for therapy. Thus, we have adapted the model, selecting only the portions relevant for our purposes and arranging them in a system appropriate for our objectives in therapy.

In this chapter, we will present our Meta-model for therapy. Here, our intention is to give you an overall picture of what is available in the Meta-model and how it works. In the two suc- ceeding chapters, we become specific, showing you in a step-by- step format how to apply the Meta-model techniques. For this chapter, we urge you to read through the discussion and attempt to get the overall image we present. We will sharpen and detail that image in the following chapters.

Deletions: The Missing Parts of the Model In most forms of therapy (with the possible exclusion of some

physical therapies) one of the things that goes on is a series of verbal transactions between the "client" and the "therapist." One of the common features of the therapeutic encounter is that the therapist tries to find out what the client has come to therapy for; what the client wants to change. In our terms, the therapist is attempting to find out what model of the world the client has. As clients communicate their models of the world, they do it in Surface Structures. These Surface Structures will contain deletions such as those described in the last chapter. The way that the client uses language to communicate his model/representation is subject to the universal processes of human modeling such as deletion.

Page 46: THE STRUCTURE MAGIC I

The Structure of Magic / 41

The Surface Structure i t se l f i s a representation of the full linguistic representation from which it is derived - the Deep Structure. In the case wherein the linguistic process of deletion has occurred, the resulting verbal description - the Surface Structure - is necessarily missing for the therapist. This piece may also be missing from the client's conscious model of the world. If the model of the client's experience has pieces missing, it is impov- erished. Impoverished models, as we stated before, imply limited options for behavior. As the missing pieces are recovered, the process of change in that person begins.

The f i r y step is for the therapist to be able to determine whether the client's Surface Structure is a complete representation of the full linguistic representation from which it is derived - the Deep Structure. A t this point in time, therapists either have a highly developed sense of intuitions based upon their experiences or they may use the explicit Meta-model to recover the missing pieces. In the Meta-model, the intuitions, which every native speaker of the language has, come into play. The client says:

I'm scared. The therapist now checks his (or her) intuitions to determine whether the client's Surface Structure is complete. One way of doing this (we present this process in detail in the following chapters) is to ask yourself whether you can think of another well-formed sentence in English which has the same process word scare and more noun arguments than the client's Surface Structure with that same verb scare. If you can think of such a Surface Structure, then the client's Surface Structure is incomplete.

Therapists are now faced with three broad option^.^ They may accept the impoverished model, they may ask for the missing piece, or they may guess at it. The first option, accepting the impoverished model, presents the difficulty of making the process of therapy slow and tedious, as it places total responsibility for recovering the model's missing pieces on the client, who is there for assistance in this process in the first place. We are not sug- gesting that change i; not possible in this process, but that it requires a longer period of time than is necessary. The-second -- choice i s for the therapist to ask for the piece that has been linguistically deleted:

C: I'm scared. T : Of what?

Either the.client supplies the material in his model that has been linguistically deleted and the therapist's understanding of that model becomes more complete, or the piece missing from the client's verbal expression is also missing from his model. Clients

Page 47: THE STRUCTURE MAGIC I

42 / The Structure of Mag~c

begin the process of self-discovery and change as they begin to work to fill in the missing pieces and become actively involved in this process o f self-discovery - expanding themselves by ex- panding their model of the world.

Therapists have a third choice - they may, from long experi- ence, have an intuition about what the missing piece is. They may choose to interpret or guess at the missing piece. We have no quarrel with this choice. There is, however, the danger that any form o f interpretation or guessing may be inaccurate. We include a safeguard for the client in our Meta-model. The client tries the interpretation or guess by\the therapist by generating a sentence - which includes that material and checks his intuitions to see whether it fits, makes sense, is an accurate representation o f his model of the world. For example, the therapist may have a strong intuition that the client i s scared o f his father. His intuition may be based upon previous therapy or upon his recognition o f a particular body posture or movement he has seen the client use at other times when the subject o f his father has come up. In this case, the exchange may go:

C: I'm scared. T : / want y o u t o t r y saying this and see whether it f i ts f o r

you: "My father scares me. " What he is asking the client to do here is to say the Surface Structure containing his guess or interpretation and see whether it f i t s the client's full representation, the Deep S t r~c tu re .~ If this new Surface Structure containing the therapist's intuition about the identity o f the deleted portion o f the client's original Surface Structure f i t s the client's model, he will typically experience a certain sensation o f congruity or recognition. If not, the Meta- model techniques are available as a guide for recovering the missing material which actually fits the client's model. The safe- guard for the client's integrity is for the therapist to be sensitive to the client's intuitions and experience by having the client judge whether the therapist's guess is accurate for his model by saying the sentence and seeing whether it f i ts.

The need for therapists to be aware o f the integrity o f their clients has been widely recognized. Polster and Polster (1973, p. 68) comment:

There is no precise yardstick to identify the limits o f an individual's power to assimilate or express feelings which have explosive possibilities, but there is a basic safeguard - not forcing or seducing h im into behaviors which he him- self has not largely set up.

Page 48: THE STRUCTURE MAGIC I

The Structure of Magic 1 43

In general, the effectiveness o f a particular form o f therapy is associated with i t s ability to recover "suppressed" or missing pieces o f the client's model. Thus, the first step in acquiring this set o f tools is to learn to identify the pieces missing in the model - specifically, to identify the fact that linguistic deletion has occurred. The pieces that are missing in the Surface Structure are the material which has been removed by the Deletion Transforma- tions. Recovering the missing material involves a movement to- ward a fuller representation - the Deep Structure.

\ Distortion: P r o c e s s ~ E v e n t

One o f the ways people become immobilized is to turn an ongoing process into an event. Events are things which occur at one point in time and are finished. Once they occur, their out- comes are fixed and nothing can be done to change them.4 This way o f representing their experience is impoverishing in the sense that clients lose control o f ongoing processes by representing them as events. Linguists have identified the linguistic mechanism for turning a process into an event. This is called nominalization and is discussed in the last chapter. The therapist's ability to challenge the distorted portions o f the client's model involving the represen- tation o f processes as events requires that the therapist be able to recognize nominalizations in the client's Surface Structures. This can be accomplished by examining the client's Surface Structure - check each o f the non-verbs in the sentence, asking yourself whether you can think o f a verb or adjective which is closely associated with it in appearance/sound and meaning. (Again, a more detailed procedure will be given in Chapter 4.) For example, as the client begins to discuss some ongoing process in his life - the continuing process o f his deciding to avoid confronting some- one about something - he may represent this process in his Surface Structure by the phrase my decision:

/ really regret m y decision. The therapist, checking for distortions, identifies the noun deci- sion as being similar in appearance/sound and meaning to the process word decide - thus, a nominalization.

The task o f the therapist is to help the client see that what he has represented in his model as a closed, finished event is an ongoing process which may be influenced by him. There are a number o f ways o f accomplishing this. For example, the therapist may ask how the client feels about his decision. When the client responds that he is dissatisfied, the therapist asks what it i s that stops him from reconsidering his decision. The client responds, and the therapist continues to apply the techniques o f the Meta-

Page 49: THE STRUCTURE MAGIC I

44 / The Structure of Magic

model. Here, the therapist is working to reconnect the event with the present process.

Another challenge the therapist may use is: You have made your decision and there is nothing which

you can imagine that would change your decision? Again, the client responds with a Surface Structure which the therapist may use, along with the Meta-model, as a guide to his next move in inducing change in the client.

The effect of systematically applying these two techniques: (a) Recovery of pieces removed by the deletion trans-

formations from the Deep Structure. (b) Transformation of nominalizations back into process

words they were derived from - the Deep Structure. yields a fuller representation of the client's model - the linguistic Deep Structure from which the client's initial verbal expressions, or Surface Structures, were derived. This process actively involves the client in filling in the missing pieces and in turning things represented as events back into processes, thereby beginning the process of change.

Deep Structures are fullest linguistic representations of the client's experience. They may differ from that experience in a number of ways which are already familiar to you. These are the three features which are common to all human modeling processes: Deletion, Distortion, and Generalization. These are the universal processes of human modeling - the way that people create any representation of their experience.

The intuitions which are represented in the transformation;il model of language are special cases of these three principles; for example, sentences or Surface Structures which have no expressed subject are examples of the process of deletion. To develop an image of the model the client has, this missing piece has to be restored; the expression has to be reconnected with i t s source - its fullest representation. In the case of a Surface Structure, i t s source and fullest representation is the Deep Structure. In the case of the Deep Structure, the client's experiences are the source for the representation. While Deep Structure is the fullest linguistic repre- sentation, it i s derived from a fuller, richer source - the sum total of the client's experiences.' Not surprisingly, the same universal processes of human modeling which give us a systematic way of assisting the client in going from an impoverished Surface Struc- ture to a complete linguistic representation - the Deep Structure - provide a systematic way of connecting the linguistic representa- tion for that person to the set of full experiences from which the full linguistic representation is derived.

Page 50: THE STRUCTURE MAGIC I

The Structure of Magic / 45

Deep Structure and Beyond As we have repeatedly pointed out, individuals who find

themselves in therapy and wish help in changing are typically there because they feel that they do not have enough choices, that they are unable to behave other than they do. Furthermore, however peculiar their behavior may appear to us, it makes sense in their model of the world.

The therapist has succeeded in involving the client in recov- ering the Deep Structure - the full linguistic representation. The next step is to challenge that Deep Structure in such a way as to enrich it. The therapist has a number of choices at this point. The basic principle here is that people end up in pain, not because the world is not rich enough to allow them to satisfy their needs, but because their representation of the world is impoverished. Corre- spondingly, then, the strategy that we as therapists adopt is to connect the client with the world in some way which gives him a richer set of choices. In other words, since the client experiences pain by having created an impoverished representation of the world and forgetting that the representation is not the world, the therapist will assist the client in changing just in case he comes to behave in some way inconsistent with his model and thereby enriches his model. There are a number of ways of accomplishing this, many of which have been described in detail. The importance of clear sensory channels, the uncovering of patterns of coping with stress learned in the family system, the childhood traumas, the imposition of therapeutic double binds - are all examples of the emphases which the various forms of psychotherapy have selected as their way of challenging the client's impoverished model. Whatever the schod of therapy and whatever its typical emphasis and form of treatment, when successful it characteris- tically involves two features:

(1) A large amount of communication in the form of lang~age.~

( 2 ) A change in the client's representation/model of the world.

What we offer in our Meta-model relates directly to both of these features of successful therapy. Language is both a representa- tional system and the means or process of communicating our representation of the world. The processes which we go through to communicate our experience are the same processes which we go through in creating our experience. Seen in this way, the recovery of the full Deep Structure from the Surface Structure corresponds to the uncovering of the client's full linguistic model of the world; the challenge to the client's Deep Structure is directly a challenge

Page 51: THE STRUCTURE MAGIC I

46 / The Structure of Magic

to the client's full linguistic representation. The same tools/ techniques apply to both.

The processes by which people impoverish their representation of the world are the same processes by which they impoverish their expression o f their representation o f the world. The way that people have created pain for themselves involves these processes. Through them they have created an impoverished model. Our Meta-model offers a specific way to challenge these same processes to enrich their model. First, the Meta-model specifies the process of moving from Surface Structure to Deep Structure. The process of the moving from a Surface Structure with a deletion to the full Deep Structure not only provides the therapist with an accurate image o f the client's model, but in the process the client may, in fact, expand the model in attempting to recover the deletion for which the therapist is asking. Second, it supplies a format for challenging the Deep Structure and reconnecting it with the per- son's experience, thus making change possible.

Having recovered the client's linguistic model o f the world, the therapist may now select any one, or more than one, of a number of techniques o f treatment which he feels useful in the context. The therapist may, for example, choose to impose a therapeutic double-bind (Haley, 1973) or t o use an enactment technique (Perk, 1973), to assist in the process o f change, or continue to challenge the client's model by purely verbal work. I n each o f these cases, language is involved. The effectiveness and potency o f a therapist is intimately connected with the richness o f his Meta- model - the number o f choices he has and his skill in combining these options. Our focus in this work will be on the verballdigital, not the non-verbal/analogicaI techniques, for two reasons:

(1) Verbal transactions are a significant form of communi- cation in all styles o f therapy.

(2) We have developed a model for natural language which is explicit.

We will show in detail later that the Meta-model which we have created from the Transformational Grammar model for a therapeutic Meta-model can be generalized to non-verbal systems of communication as well.'

Challenging Deep Structure For the therapist to challenge the Deep Structure is equivalent

to demanding that the client mobilize his resources to reconnect his linguistic model with his world o f experience. I n other words, the therapist here is challenging the client's assumptions that his linguistic model is reality.

Page 52: THE STRUCTURE MAGIC I

The Structure of Magic 1 47

Challenging Generalizations One element that a client's model will possess which typically

impoverishes his experience is that o f generalization. Correspond- ingly, the Deep Structure which represents the impoverished por- tion o f the model will contain words and phrases which have no referential index and verbs which are incompletely specified.

I

1 Clarity Out of Chaos -the Noun/Arguments As the missing pieces o f the client's Deep Structure are re-

\ covered, the model o f the client's experience may become more complete, yet it may st i l l be unclear and unfocused.' The client ,

I says:

I C: I'm scared. T: Of what? C: Ofpeople.

At this point, the therapist either has a well-developed set o f intuitions about what t o do next or he may use our explicit Meta-model as a guide. One explicit way of determining which portions o f the verbal expression (and the model it represents) are unfocused is to check for noun arguments that have no referential index. The therapist again has three basic choices: to accept the unfocused model, to ask a question which demands focusing o f the model, or to guess what the focused model may be. The choice made by the therapist here has the same consequences as did his attempting to recover pieces missing in the model. If the therapist chooses to ask for the missing referential index, he simply says:

Who, specifically (scares you)? If, on the other hand, the therapist has an intuition about the identity o f the noun phrase which has no referential index, he may decide to guess. I n this case, the same way o f safeguarding the client's integrity is available if the therapist chooses to guess.

C: I'm scared. T: Of what? C: Ofpeople.

The therapist decides to guess who it is who specifically scares the client. Employing the safeguard we recommend, the therapist asks the client t o say the Surface Structure which incorporates the therapist's guess.

T: / want you to try saying this and see whether you feel it fits for you: "My father scares me. "

The client now says the Surface Structure incorporating the guess or interpretation and determines whether it f i t s his model. In either case, the therapist is responding - challenging the client's generalization by demanding that the client connect this generali-

Page 53: THE STRUCTURE MAGIC I

48 / The Structure of Magic

zation with his specific experience - by demanding a referential index. This, the next step in the process o f the therapist's under- standing the client's model, is the challenge to the noun arguments

I which have no referential index.

The word "people" does not pick out a specific individual or

I I

group of individuals in the client's model. The client may supply the referential index missing in the verbal expression and available in his model and the therapist's understanding o f his model is thus more focused, or the referential index may be missing in the client's model also. If that portion o f the client's model is also unfocused, the question by the therapist allows the client to work toward clarifying his model and to become more involved in the process.

Notice that the client may produce a number o f responses I

such as "people who hate me," "all the people I always thought were my friends," "everyone I know," "some o f my family," none of which have referential indices - they are intentional, not extensional, descriptions o f the person's experiencee9 They repre- sent generalizations which are s t i l l not connected to the client's experience. The therapist continues to challenge these formula- tions by asking:

Who, specifically? until they get from the client a verbal expression which has a referential index. Finally, the client responds:

My father scares me. The demand by the therapist for ful l Deep Structure representa- tions which include only words and phrases which have referential indices is a demand that the client re-connect his generalizations with the experience from which they came. Next, the therapist asks himself whether the image he has o f the client's model is clear and focused.

Clarity Out o f Chaos - Verb/Process Words Both the nouns in the verbal expression:

My father scares me. have referential indices (my father and me). The process word or verb in the expression, however, gives us no clear image o f pre- cisely how the experience took place. We know that the client is scared and that his father scares him, but how, exactly, his father scares him is incompletely represented - what, specifically, is it that he does which scares him. The therapist asks the client to focus his image by the question:

How does your father scare you? This is again a request by the therapist for the client to connect his

Page 54: THE STRUCTURE MAGIC I

The Structure of Magic / 53

Structure out into the open for examination and challenge. In order to understand the sentence at all, it is necessary for the therapist to accept the presuppositions:

Her husband doesn't love her. There is an explicit test for what, if any, presuppositions a sen- tence has. The therapist takes the Surface Structure and forms a new sentence which is the same as the old one except that it has a negative word in it attached to the first verb - in this case the sentence:

/ don't realize that my husband doesn't love me. Then, the therapist simply asks himself whether the same sentence would have to be true in order for this new sentence to make sense. Any sentence which must be true for both the client's statement and the new statement, which was formed by the old statement plus the negative word, to make sense is a presupposi- tion. Presuppositions are particularly insidious as they are not presented openly for consideration. They identify in the model some of the basic organizing principles which limit the client's experience.

Once the therapist has identified the presuppositions of the client's statements, he may challenge it directly by the techniques we have already identified in the Deletion Section.

SUMMARY

When therapy, whatever i t s form, is successful, it involves a change in the clients' models in some way which allows clients more choice in their behavior. The methods which we have pre- sented in the Meta-model are effective in enriching a client's model of the world - which entails that some aspect of his model is new. It's important that this new portion of his model be solidly connected with his experience. To insure this, clients must actu- ally exercise, practice, become familiar with, and experience their new choices. Most therapies have developed specific techniques for accomplishing this: e.g., psychodrama, homework, tasks, etc. The purpose of these techniques is to integrate the new aspect of his model into the client's experience.

OVERVIEW

Successful therapy involves change. The Meta-model, adapted from the transformational model of language, provides an explicit

Page 55: THE STRUCTURE MAGIC I

54 / The Structure of Magic

method for understanding and changing clients' impoverished models. One way to understand the overall effect of this Meta- model is in terms o f well-formedness. As native speakers, we can consistently distinguish between groups o f words which are well formed - i.e., sentences - and groups o f words which are not well formed. That is, we can intuitively make the distinction between what is well formed in English and what is not. What we are proposing here is that there is a subset o f the well-formed sen- tences of English which we recognize as well formed in therapy. . This set, the set o f sentences which are well formed in therapy and acceptable to us as therapists, are sentences which:

(1) Are well formed in English, and (2) Contain no transformational deletions or unexplored

deletions in the portion o f the model in which the client experiences no choice.

(3) Contain no nominalizations (process-event). (4) Contain no words or phrases lacking referential indices. (5) Contain no verbs incompletely specified. (6) Contain no unexplored presuppositions in the portion

o f the model in which the client experiences no choice. (7) Contain no sentences which violate the semantic condi-

tions of well-formedness. By applying these well-formedness conditions to the client's Sur- face Structures, the therapist has an explicit strategy for inducing change in the client's model." Using these grammatical conditions appropriate for therapy, therapists enrich their model independ- ently of the particular form o f therapy they do. While this set of tools will greatly increase the potency o f any form o f therapy, we are aware that there is a great deal going on in the therapeutic encounter which is not solely digital (verbal). Rather, we are saying that the digital system is important, and we are offering an explicit Meta-model. The nervous system which produces digital communication (e.g., language) is the same nervous system which generates the other forms o f human behavior which occur in the therapeutic encounter - analogical communication systems, dreams, etc. The remainder o f this book is designed to accomplish two things: first, to familiarize you with the use o f the Meta- model we have presented, and secondly, to show you how the general processes o f the Meta-model for the digital can be gener- alized to these other forms of human behavior.

Page 56: THE STRUCTURE MAGIC I

The Structure of Magic 1 55

FOOTNOTES FOR CHAPTER 3

1. We highly recommend the excellent work by Jay Haley, Gregory Bateson and his associates, Paul Watlawick, Janet Beavin, and Don Jackson. Their studies appear to us to be, at present, the closest approximation along with the Meta-model to achieving this gqal.

2. We are aware that the three options discussed here do not exhaust all the logical or, indeed, practical possibilities. The therapist could, for example, ignore completely the Surface Structure the client presents. The three cate- gories of response by the therapist that we discuss seem to us to be the most frequent.

3. In Chapter 6 we will return to this technique under the general heading of Congruity Technique. Here, simply, the client, by uttering the Surface Structure, calls up the Deep Structure. If the Surface Structure corresponds to a Deep Structure which f i t s his model (is congruent with his model), the client will experience some recognition.

4. In Chapter 2, as well as in the remainder of the book, we adopt the standard philosophical linguistic view that only nouns in the Surface Struc- ture which correspond to verbs in Deep Structure are the result of nominali- zations: the change of the representation of a process into an event. A more radical view i s that even Surface Structure nouns which, by the standard linguistic analysis, do not correspond to verbs in Deep Structure are the representation of a process by an event. In this view, the noun chair i s the event representation of what we actually experience in the process of percep- tion, manipulation, . . . one which has space-time coordinates and duration. The difference, then, between parts o f our experience which are represented in Deep Structure as verbs and those which are represented as nouns is essentially the amount of difference or change we experience in what is represented: choirs change slowly and undramatically, while meetings change more quickly and dramatically.

5. We will return to consider this subject systematically in Chapter 6 under the title of Reference Structures - the sum total of the client's experience - the source from which the full linguistic representation i s derived.

6. The limiting case i s the physical therapies (e.g., Rolfing, Bio- energetics, Shiatsu, . . . ) which emphasize working on the physical represen- tational system - that is, human beings represent their experiences in their body posture, movements, typical muscle contractions, tonus. . . . We return to this topic in Chapter 6. Even in this limiting case, the therapist and the client, typically, talk to one another.

7. This is the focus of Chapter 6 and of Structure of Magic 11. 8. In fact, from the discussion of the types of deletion transformations

in Chapter 2, it follows that every case of Free Deletion i s the deletion of a Deep Structure noun argument which had no referential index.

Page 57: THE STRUCTURE MAGIC I

56 / The Structure of Magic

9. The intentional-extensional distinction i s borrowed from logic. An extensional definition of a set is one which specifies what the members of the set are by simply listing (i.e., enumerating) them; an intentional definition o f '

a set i s one which specifies what the members of the set are by giving a rule or procedure which sorts the world into members and non-members of the set. For example, the set of all humans over six feet in height who live in Ozona, Texas, can be given extentially by a l i s t o f the people who, in fact, live in Ozona, Texas, and are taller than six feet, or intentionally by a procedure, say, for example:

(a) Go to the official directory o f residents of Ozona, Texas. (b) Find each person on the l i s t and determine whether he i s taller

than two yardsticks placed end to end. Korzybski (1933, Chap. 1) has an interesting discussion of this distinction. Notice that, in general, l i s t s or a set specified extentionally have referential indices while sets intentionally given have no referential index.

10. We say necessarily as models are, by definition, reduced with respect to what they represent. This reduction i s at the same time their value and their danger, as we discussed in Chapter 1.

11. In listening to and evaluating the Surface Structure answers that clients present to these questions, all the Meta-model techniques apply. In addition, we have found it effective to demand that the clients give how (i.e., ~rocess) answers rather than why (i.e., justification) answers to these questions.

Page 58: THE STRUCTURE MAGIC I

Chapter 4

INCANTATIONS FOR GROWTH AND POTENTIAL

In the last chapter, we presented the Meta-model for therapy. This Meta-model is based on the intuitions which you already have available to you as native speakers o f your language. The termi- nology, however, that we have adapted from linguistics may be new to you. This chapter is designed to present material which allows you to familiarize yourself with how to apply, specifically, the Meta-model. We recognize that, just as with any new set o f tools, making ourselves competent with it requires some initially focused attention. This chapter provides each therapist who wishes to incorporate this Meta-model into his techniques and way o f proceeding in the therapeutic encounter an opportunity to work with the principles and materials o f the Meta-model. By doing this, you will be able to sensitize yourself, to be able to hear the structure o f the verbal communications in the therapeutic en- counter, and, thereby, to sharpen your intuitions.

The various specific linguistic phenomena which we will pre- sent that you will come to recognize and act upon are the specific ways the three universals o f human modeling are realized in human language systems. As we introduce each specific linguistic phenomenon, we will identify which o f these processes - General- ization, Deletion, or Distortion - is involved. The point is for you to come to recognize and obtain from the client communication which consists wholly o f sentences which are well formed in therapy. You, as a native speaker, are able to determine which sentences are well formed in English; the following examples are designed to sharpen your ability to detect what is well formed in

Page 59: THE STRUCTURE MAGIC I

58 1 Incantations for Growth and Potential

therapy - a subset of sentences that are well formed in English. We will present the material in two steps: recognition of what is well formed in therapy and what to do when you have identified in therapy a sentence which is not well formed.

EXERCISE A

One of the most useful skills that you can exercise as a therapist is that of distinguishing what clients represent with their Surface Structures from what you may under- stand their surface to imply. The question of therapists projecting onto their clients is not a new one. Also, even if a therapist may from his experience understand more about what a client is saying than the client himself may realize, the ability to distinguish is vital. If the client fails to represent something the therapist understands to be there, it is just that piece of information the client may have left out of his representation, or it's just that piece of information which may cue the therapist to use some technique of intervention. In any event, the ability to distinguish what is represented from what you, yourself, supply is vital.

The difference between what you, as a therapist, may understand the client's Surface Structure to imply and what that Surface Structure literally represents comes from you. Those elements that you, yourself, supply may or may not fit the client's model. There are a number of ways to determine whether what you supply is fitting for the client. Your skill as a therapist will increase as your skill in making this distinction increases. What we would like you to do next is to read the following sentence, then close your eyes and form a visual image of what the sentence represents.

The client: I'm afraid! Now examine your image. It will include some visual representation of the client and some representation of the client's being afraid. Any detail beyond these two images was supplied by you. For instance, if you supplied any representation of what the client fears, it came from you and may or may not be accurate. Try this once and read this second Surface Structure; close your eyes and make a visual image.

The client: Mary hurt me.

Page 60: THE STRUCTURE MAGIC I

incantations for Growth and Potential 1 59

Now examine your image. It will include some visual representation of some person (Mary) and some visual representation of the client. Now look closely at how you represented the process of hurting. The verb hurting is a very vague and unspecific word. If you represented the process of hurting, study your image carefully. Perhaps you had an image of Mary physically striking the client, or perhaps an image of Mary saying something mean to the client. You may have had an image of Mary walking through the room that the client was sitting in without speaking to the client. All of these are possible representa- tions of the client's Surface Structure. In each of them you have added something to the representation of the verb to form an image for yourself. You have ways of determining which, if any, of these representations fits the client - you may ask the client to more fully specify the verb hurt, ask the client to enact a specific situation in which Mary hurt him, etc. The important piece is your ability to distinguish between what you supply and what the client is repre- senting with his Surface Structure.

DELETION

The purpose of recognizing deletions is to assist the client in restoring a fuller representation of his experiences. Deletion is a process which removes portions of the original experience (the world) or full linguistic representation (Deep Structure). The lin- guistic process of deletion is a transformational process - the result of deletion transformations - and a special case of the general modeling phenomenon of Deletion wherein the model we create is reduced with respect to the thing being modeled. Deep Structure is the full linguistic representation. The representation of this representation is the Surface Structure - the actual sen- tence that the client says to communicate his full linguistic model or Deep Structure. As native speakers of English, therapists have intuitions which allow them to determine whether the Surface Structure represents the full Deep Structure or not. Thus, by comparing the Surface Structure and the Deep Structure, the therapist can determine what is missing. Example:

(1 ) I'm confused. The basic process word is the verb confuse. The verb confuse has the potential of occurring in sentences with two arguments or noun phrases - in sentences such as:

b

Page 61: THE STRUCTURE MAGIC I

60 1 Incantations for Growth and Potential

( 2 ) I'm confused by people. Since the verb confuse occurs in sentence (2) with two argument nouns ( I and people), the therapist can conclude that Surface Structure (1) is not a full representation of the Deep Structure from which it was derived. In a step-by-step format, the procedure can be outlined as follows:

Step 1 : Listen to the Surface Structure the client presents; Step 2: Identify the verbs in that Surface Structure; Step 3: Determine whether the verbs can occur in a sen-

tence which is fuller - that is, has more arguments or noun phrases in it than the original.

If the second sentence has more argument nouns than the original Surface Structure presented by the client, the original Surface Structure is incomplete - a portion of the Deep Structure has been deleted. The first step in learning to recognize deletions is to identify sentences in which deletions have occurred. Thus, for example, sentence (3) is an essentially complete representation of its Deep Structure:

( 3 ) George broke the chair. On the other hand, sentence (4) i s an incomplete representation of its Deep Structure:

(4 ) The chair was broken. The following set of sentences contains some Surface Struc-

tures which are complete - no deletions - and some which are incomplete - deletions have occurred. Your task is to identify which of the following set of Surface Structures are complete and which contain deletions. Remember that you decide whether deletions have occurred - some of the sentences may be ill formed in therapy for reasons other than deletion. Additional exercises will give you practice in correcting the other things about these sentences which make them ill formed in therapy.

( 5 ) / feel happy. incomplete ( 6 ) I'm interested in continuing this. complete (7) My father was angry. incomplete ( 8 ) This exercise is boring. incomplete (9) I'm irritated about that. complete

The set of sentences below consists wholly of Surface Struc- tures which are incomplete. For each one, you are to find another sentence which has the same process word or verb and which is fuller - that is, has more noun phrases or arguments. Next to each of the incomplete sentences, we have provided an example of a fuller version using the same verb. We suggest that you cover the fuller version, which we have provided, with paper and write out a fuller version of your own before looking at the one we present.

Page 62: THE STRUCTURE MAGIC I

Incantations for Growth and Potential 1 61

For example, with the Surface Structure: (1 0) I'm scared.

one fuller version would be: (1 1 ) I'm scared of people.

or another would be the Surface Structure: (1 2) I'm scared of spiders.

The point, o f course, is not to try to guess which fuller version we would happen to present, but to provide yourself with the experi- ence o f finding fuller versions o f incomplete Surface Structures.

( 1 3) l have a problem. I have a problem with people. (14) You're excited. You're excited about being here. (15) l'msad. I'm sad about my mother. (1 6) I'm fed up. I'm fed up with you. (1 7) You're disturbing. You're disturbing me.

The next group o f sentences consists o f Surface Structures which have more than one verb and may have zero, one or two deletions. Your task is t o determine whether deletions have occurred and, if so, how many. Remember to check each verb separately as each may be independently associated with deletions.

For example, the Surface Structure ( 1 8) / don 't know what to say.

has one deletion associated with the verb say (say to whom). The Surface Structure

(1 9) / said that / would try. has two deletions, one associated with the verb said (said to whom) and one with the verb try (try what).

(20) 1 talked to a man who was 2 deletions: 1 bored. with talked, 1

with bored.

(21 ) / hoped to see my parents. no deletion

(22) I want to hear. 1 deletion: with hear.

(23) My husband claimed he was 2 deletions: 1 frightened. with claimed, 1 -

with frightened.

(24) / laughed and then 1 le f t 1 deletion: with home. laughed.

In each o f the follawing Surface Structures, there is at least

Page 63: THE STRUCTURE MAGIC I

62 1 Incantations for Growth and Potential

one deletion. Find a fuller version for each Surface Structure.

(25) You always talk as though you 're mad.

(26) My brother swears that my parents can't cope.

(27) Everybody knows that you can 't win.

(28) Communicating is hard for me.

(29) Running away doesn't help.

You always talk to me as though

you're mad at someone.

My brother swears to me that my parents can't

cope with him.

Everybody knows that you can't win what you

need.

My communi- cating to you my

hopes about changing myself

is hard for me.

My running away from my home

doesn't help me.

One o f the ways in which Deep Structure process words may occur in Surface Structure is in the form o f an adjective which modifies a noun. I n order for this to happen, deletions must occur. For example, the Surface Structure

(30) / don't like unclear people. contains the adjective unclear. Another Surface Structure which is closely associated with this last sentence is1

(3 1 ) / don't like people who are unclear. I n both o f these Surface Structures, there have been deletions associated with the word unclear (unclear to whom about what). Thus, one fuller version is:

(32) / don't like people who are unclear to me about what tqey want.

I n the next group o f Surface Structures, identify the deletions and present a fuller version o f each o f the sentences.

Page 64: THE STRUCTURE MAGIC I

Incantations for Growth and Potential 1 63

(33) / laughed at the irritating man.

(34) You always present stupid examples.

(35) Self-righteous people burn me UP.

(36) The unhappy letter surprised me.

(37) The overwhelming price of food disturbs me.

I laughed at the man who irritated

me.

You always present examples to me which are

stupid to me.

People who are self-righteous

about drugs burn me up.

The letter which made me unhappy

surprised me.

The price o f food which

overwhelms me disturbs me.

The point o f practicing recognition o f deletions in Surface Structures is to make you conscious o f and to sharpen the intui- tions that you already have as a native speaker. The point is to be aware that deletions have occurred. The next section is designed to allow you to practice assisting the client in recovering the deleted material.

WHAT TO DO

Once the therapist has recognized that the Surface Structure the client has presented is incomplete, the next task is to help the client recover the deleted material. The most direct approach we are aware of is to ask specifically for what is missing. For example, the client says:

(38) I'm upset. The therapist recognizes that the Surface Structure is an incom- plete representa'tion of the Deep Structure from which it came. Specifically, it is a reduced version o f a Deep Structure which has a fuller Surface Structure representation o f the form:

Page 65: THE STRUCTURE MAGIC I

64 1 Incantations for Growth and Potential

(39) I'm upset about someone/something. Thus, to recover the missing material, the therapist asks:

(40) Whomlwhat are you upset about? or more simply

(41 ) about whomlwhat? In the following group of Surface Structures, your task is to

formulate the question or questions which most directly ask for the deleted material. We've provided examples of the kinds of questions which will elicit the deleted material. Again, we suggest that you cover the questions which we have provided and work out your own appropriate questions for each o f the incomplete Surface Structures.

(42) 1 feel happy.

(43) My father was angry.

(44) This exercise is boring.

(45) I'm scared.

~ ( 4 6 ) l have a problem.

(47) 1 don't know what to do.

(48) I said that / would try.

(49) / talked to a man who was bored.

(50) 1 want to hear.

' ( 5 1 ) My husband claimed he was frightened.

happy about whomlwhat?

angry at whomlwhat?

boring to whom?

scared of whomlwhat?

a problem with whom/what?

to do about whomlwhat?

said to whom? try what?

talked about what? bored with

whomlwhat?

want to hear whomlwhat?

claimed to whom?

frightened about whom/what?

Page 66: THE STRUCTURE MAGIC I

lncantations for Growth and Potential 1 65

(52) You always talk as though you're mad.

(53) My brother swears that, my parents can't cope.

(54) Communicating is hard for me.

(55) Running away doesn't help.

(56) 1 don't like unclear people.

(57) 1 laughed at the irritating man.

(58) You always present stupid examples.

(59) Self-righteous people burn me UP.

(60) The unhappy letter surprised me.

(61) The overwhelming price of food disturbs me.

talk to whom? mad at

whomlwhat?

swears to whom? can't cope with

whomlwhat?

whose communicating? communicating about what? to

whom?

whose running away? running

away from whomlwhat?

unclear about what? unclear to

whom?

the man who was irritating to

whom?

present examples to whom?

examples who thinks are stupid?

self-righteous about what?

whom did the letter make

unhappy?

who was overwhelmed?

Page 67: THE STRUCTURE MAGIC I

66 1 Incantations for Growth and Potential

SOME SPECIAL CASES OF DELETION

We have identified three special classes o f Deletions. These are special in the sense that we encounter them frequently in therapy, and the Surface Structure forms that they have can be identified directly.

Class I: Real Compared to What? The first special class o f deletions which we wish to identify

involves comparatives and superlatives. Specifically, the portion of the Deep Structure deleted is one of the terms o f a comparative or superlative construction. Comparatives and superlatives have two forms in English.

(A) Adjective, plus the ending er as in: faster

better smarter

and Adjective plus the ending est as in: fastest

best smartest

or (B) more/less plus Adjective

as in: more interesting more important less intelligent

and mostlleast plus Adjective as in: most interesting

most important least intelligent

Comparatives, as the name suggests, involve a comparison o f (minimally) two distinct things. For example, the Surface Structure:

(62) She is better for me than m y mother. includes both o f the things compared (she and m y mother). The class of Surface Structure which we characterize as involving the , deletion o f one term o f the comparative construction includes, for example:

(63) She is better for me. where one term o f the comparison has been deleted. This kind of deletion is also present in Surface Structures such as:

(64) She is a better woman for me. where the comparative adjective appears in front o f the noun to which it applies.

Page 68: THE STRUCTURE MAGIC I

incantations for Growth and Potential / 67

The comparatives formed with more appear in the two examples:

(65) She is more interesting to me. (66) She is a more interesting woman to me.

Again, one of the terms o f the comparative has been deleted. I n the case o f superlatives, one member o f some set i s selected and identified as most characteristic or having the highest value in the set. For example, in the Surface Structure:

(67) She is the best. (68) She is the most interesting.

the set from which she has been selected is not mentioned. The following set o f Surface Structures is composed o f ex-

amples o f deletion o f one term of a comparative or the deletion o f the reference set or a superlative. These examples are presented to allow you to develop your ability to identify deletions o f this class.

(69) She is most difficult. (70) He chose the best. (7 1 ) That is the least difficult. (72) She always leaves the harder job for me. (73) 1 resent happier people. (74) More aggressive men get what they want. (75 ) The best answer is always more difficult to find. (76) I've never seen a funnier man.

In coping with this class o f deletions, the therapist will be able to recover the deleted material using two simple questions:

For comparatives: The comparative adjective, plus compared to what? e.g., more aggressive compared to what? or, funnier than what?

For superlatives: The superlative, plus with respect to what? e.g., the best answer with respect t o what? the most difficult with respect t o what?

In a step-by-step format, the procedure is: Step 1: Listen to the client, examining the client's Surface

Structure for the grammatical markers o f the compara- tive and superlative construction; i.e., Adjective plus er, morelless plus Adjective, Adjective plus est, most/ least plus Adjective.

Step 2: I n the case o f comparatives occurring in the client's Surface Structuring, determine whether both terms that are being compared are present; in the case of superlatives, determine whether the reference set is present.

Page 69: THE STRUCTURE MAGIC I

68 1 Incantations for Growth and Potential

Step 3: For each deleted portion, recover the missing material by using the questions suggested above.

Class I I: Clearly and Obviously The second class of special deletions can be identified by ly

adverbs occurring in the Surface Structures the client presents. For example, the client says:

(77) Obviously, my parents dislike me. or

(78) My parents obviously dislike me. Notice that these Surface Structures can be paraphrased by the sentence

(79) I t is obvious that my parents dislike me. Once this form is available, the therapist can more easily identify what portion of the Deep Structure has been deleted. Specifically, in the example, the therapist asks

(80) To whom is it obvious? Surface Structure adverbs which end in ly are often the result

of deletions of the arguments of a Deep Structure process word or verb. The paraphrase test can be used by the therapist to develop his intuitions in recognizing these adverbs. The tes t we offer is that, when you encounter an adverb ending with ly, attempt to paraphrase the sentence in which it appears by:

(a) Deleting the ly from the Surface Structure adverb and placing it in the front of the new Surface Structure you are creating.

(b) Add the phrase it-is in front of the former adverb. (c) Ask yourself whether this new Surface Structure means

the same thing as the client's original Surface Structure. If the new sentence is synonymous with the client's original, then the adverb is derived from a Deep Structure verb and deletion is involved. Now, by applying the principles used in recovering missing material to this new Surface Structure, the full Deep Structure representation can be recovered.

In the following set o f Surface Structures, determine which of them includes an adverb which has been derived from the Deep Structure verb.

( 8 1 ) Unfortunately, you for- = It is unfortunate got to call me on my that you forgot birthday. to call me on my

birthday.

Page 70: THE STRUCTURE MAGIC I

Incantations for Growth and Potential 1 69

(82) / quickly left the f It is quick that I argument. left the argument.

(83) Surprisingly, my father = It is surprising for lied about his drinking. my father to lie

about his drinking.

(84) She slowly started to f It is slow that she cry. started to cry.

(85) They painfully avoided = It i s painful that my questions. they avoided my

questions.

Once the therapist has identified the adverbs that have been derived from Deep Structure verbs by paraphrasing the client's original Surface Structure, he may apply the methods for recov- ering deleted material to the Surface Structure paraphrase. In a step-by-step procedure, therapists can handle this particular class of deletion by:

Step 1: Listen to the client's Surface Structure for ly adverbs.

Step 2: Apply the paraphrase test to each ly adverb. Step 3: If the paraphrase test works, examine the new

Surface Structure. Step 4: Apply the normal methods for recovering the

deleted material.

Class Ill: Modal Operators The third class of special deletions is particularly important in

recovering material which has been deleted in going from the client's experience to his full linguistic representation - Deep Structure. These Surface Structures often involve rules or generali- zations that the clients have developed in their models. For example, the client says:

(86) 1 have to take other people's feelings into account. or

(87) One must take other people's feelings into account. or

(88) I t is necessary to take other people's feelings into account.

You will be able to identify a number of deletions in each of these Surface Structures on the basis of the principles and exercises we

Page 71: THE STRUCTURE MAGIC I

70 1 Incantations for Growth and Potential

have already presented (e.g., feelings about whomlwhat?). The deletion we want to draw your attention to here, however, is a larger scale deletion. These Surface Structures make the claim that something must occur - they immediately suggest to us the question, "Or what?" In other words, for us, as therapists, to come to understand the client's model clearly, we must know the consequences to the client o f failing to do what the client's Surface Structure claims is necessary. We understand Surface Structures o f this class to be of the logical form:

It is necessary that ~7 or ~2 where ~1 is what the client's Surface Structure claims is necessary and ~2 is what will happen if S1 isn't done - the consequence or outcome of failing to do ~1 - then ~1 and ~2 are the deleted material. Thus, the therapist may ask:

(89) Or what will happen? or, in a more expanded form

(90) What would happen i f you failed to ? where you substitute the appropriate part o f the client's original Surface Structure in the . Specifically, using the above as an example, the client says

(91 ) One must take other people's feelings into account. The therapist may respond,

(92) Or what will happen? or, more fully,2

(93) What would happen i f you failed to take other people's feelings into account?

These Surface Structures can be identified by the presence o f what logicians call modal operators o f necessity. These have the Surface forms in English of:

have to asin //Youhaveto ... one has t o . . .

necessary as in It is necessary . . . Necessarily, . . .

should as in One/you/l should. . . must as in //youlone must. . .

The therapist may use these as cue words to recognize this special class o f Surface Structures. In the following set, form a question which asks for the consequence or outcome o f failing to do what the Surface Structure claims is necessary. We use the two question forms we suggested above in the following exercise. Note that these are not the only two possible question forms but, in fact, any question which recovers the deleted material is appropriate.

Page 72: THE STRUCTURE MAGIC I

78 / Incantations for Growth and Potential

the client t o re-connect his linguistic model of the world with the ongoing processes which are present there.

Nominalizations are complex psychologically as well as lin- guistically. Our experience is that they rarely occur by themselves; rather, we encounter them more frequently in a form which involves violations o f one or more o f the other well-formed-in- therapy conditions. Since we have already presented the exercises on deletion, we will now give you a set o f Surface Structures which contain both nominalizations and deletions. We ask that you identify both the nominalization and the deletion, and that you formulate a question or series o f questions which both trans- lates the nominalization back into a process form and asks for the material which has been deleted. For example, given the Surface Structure

The decision to return home bothers me. one question which both translates the nominalization back into a process form and simultaneously asks for the deleted material is:

(1 34) Who is deciding to return home? Again, we suggest that you attempt to formulate your own

question(s) before looking at the examples we offer. The example questions we present are dense - we suggest in practice that a series of questions be used, asking for a piece at a time.

(135) My pain is overwhelming.

gets in mj (1 36) It's my fear that way.

(137) 1 have hope.

Your feeling pain about

whomlwhat is overwhelming

whom?

il Your being afraid o f whomlwhat

gets in your way o f what?

What are you hoping for?

(1 38) My son's beliefs worry me. Your son believes what that worries

you?

Page 73: THE STRUCTURE MAGIC I

Incantations for Growth and Potential / 79

( 1 39) Your bigoted suspicion an- Bigoted toward noys me. whomiwhat?

What is it that you are

suspecting?

EXERCISE B

Since in Meta-model training seminars we have found nominal- izations to be the most difficult phenomena for people to learn to recognize, we have devised the following exercise.

Form a visual image from the following sentences. In each case, see if you can imagine placing each of the non-process or non-verb words in a wheelbarrow.

I want to make a chair. I want to make a decision.

Notice that all the non-verb words in the first sentence (I, chair) can be placed in your mental wheelbarrow. This is not the case with the second sentence (I, decision). I can be placed in a wheelbarrow but a decision cannot. In the following sets of sentences, use this same visual test to train yourself in recognizing nominalizations.

I have a lot of frustration. I have a lot of green marbles.

I expect a letter. I expect help.

My fear is just too big. My coat is just too big.

I lost my book. I lost my temper.

I need water. I need love.

Horses frighten me. Failure frightens me.

The tension bothers me. The dragon bothers me.

Page 74: THE STRUCTURE MAGIC I

80 / Incantations for Growth and Potential

A t least one nominalization occurs in each of the preceding pairs. You may check the accuracy o f your visual test by now applying the purely linguistic test, an ongoing in front of the nominalization. The same word which fits into the linguistic frame -an ongoing - will not fit into your mental wheelbarrow.

GENERALIZATION

How To Get a Clear Image of the Client's Model One of the universal processes which occur when humans

create models of their experiences is that of Generalization. Gener- alization may impoverish the client's model by causing loss of the detail and richness of their original experiences. Thus, generaliza- tion prevents them from making distinctions which would give them a fuller set of choices in coping with any particular situation. At the same time, the generalization expands the specific painful experience to the level of being persecuted by the universe (an insurmountable obstacle to coping). For example, the specific painful experience "Lois doesn't like me" generalizes to "Women don't like me." The purpose of challenging the client's generaliza- tions is to:

(1) Re-connect the client's model with his experience. (2) Reduce the insurmountable obstacles which result

from generalizations to something definite with which he can begin to cope.

(3) Insure detail and richness are present in the client's model, thus creating choices based on distinctions which were not previously available.

Linguistically, we are aware of two important ways which we use to identify the generalizations in the client's model. A t the same time, these provide us with a vehicle for challenging these generalizations. These are the processes of:

(1) Checking for referential indices for nouns and event words;

(2) Checking for fully specified verbs and process words.

Referential Indices The ability of the therapist to determine whether the Surface

Structures presented by the client are connected with the client's experience is essential for successful therapy. One explicit way of determining this is for the therapist to identify words and phrases in the client's Surface Structure which do not have a referential

Page 75: THE STRUCTURE MAGIC I

lncantations for Growth and Potential / 81

index. For example, in the Surface Structure ( 1 40) People push me around.

the noun people carries no referential index and, therefore, fails to identify anything specific in the client's experience. On the other hand, the sentence

( 1 4 1 ) My father pushes me around. contains two nouns (my father and me), both bearing a referential index which identifies something specific in the client's model.

Again, a step-by-step procedure is available. Step 1 : Listen to the client's Surface Structure, identifying

each non-process word. Step 2: For each of these, ask yourself whether it picks

out a specific person or thing in the world. If the word or phrase fails to pick out a specific person or thing, then the therapist has identified a generalization in the client's model. In the following set of Surface Structures, decide for each noun or phrase whether it does or does not have a referential index making it well formed in therapy.

(1 42) Nobody pays any attention to what 1 say.

(143) / always avoid situations I feel uncomfortable in.

(1 44) / like dogs that are friendly.

(1 45) I saw my mother-in-law yesterday.

(1 46) One should respect others' feelings.

(1 47) It's painful for us to see her this way, you know.

(1 48) Let's not get bogged down in details.

Nobody and what have no

referential index.

Situations 1 feel uncomfortable in

- no index.

Dogs that are friendly - no

index.

All nouns have indices.

One and others' - no indices.

It, us, you and this way - no

indices.

Us and details - no indices.

Page 76: THE STRUCTURE MAGIC I

82 / Incantations for Growth and Potential

(1 49) There's a certain feeling in A certain feeling this room. - no index.

(1 50) Everybody feels that way Everybody, that sometimes. way, sometimes

- no indices.

Once the therapist has identified the words and phrases with- out referential indices, it is quite easy to ask for these. Only two questions are required:

(1 5 1 ) Who, specifically? (1 52) What, specifically?

In requiring the client to supply referential indices by answering these questions, the client re-connects the generalizations in his model with his experiences. In the next set o f Surface Structures, formulate the question appropriate for getting the missing refer- ential index.

Nobody pays any attention to what 1 say.

1 always avoid situatibns I feel un- comfortable in.

I like dogs that are friendly.

It's painful for us to see her this way, you know.

Everybody feels that way some- times.

Who, specifically? What,

specifically, do you say?

What situations, specifically?

What dog, specifically?

Who, specifically, is full of pain?

Who, specifically, is us? What way,

specifically? Who, specifically, is

you?

Who, specifically? What way,

specifically? What time, specifically?

There is a special case which we like to emphasize o f certain words which have no referential index. This, specifically, is the set

Page 77: THE STRUCTURE MAGIC I

Incantations for Growth and Potential / 83

of words which contains universal quantifiers such as all, each, every, any. The universal quantifier has a different form when combined with other linguistic elements such as the negative element - never, nowhere, none, no one, nothing, nobody. Uni- versal quantifiers, and words and phrases containing them, have no referential index. We use a special form o f challenge for the universal quantifier and words and phrases containing it. For example, the Surface Structure presented before:

Nobody pays any attention to what / say. may be challenged as we suggested before or with the challenge:

(1 53) You mean to tell me that NOBODY EVER pays attention to you A T A L L ?

What we are doing here is emphasizing the generalization described by the client's universal quantifier by exaggerating it both by voice quality and by inserting additional universal quantifiers in the client's original Surface Structure. This challenge identifies and emphasizes a generalization in the client's model. A t the same time, this form o f challenge asks clients if there are any exceptions to their generalizations. A single exception to the generalization starts the client on the process o f assigning referential indices and insures the detail and richness in the client's model necessary to have a variety o f options for coping.

C: Nobody pays any attention to what I say. T: Do you mean to tel l me that NOBODY EVER pays

attention to y o u A T A L L ? C: Well, no t exactly. T: OK, then; who, specifically, doesn't pay attention to

you? Once the therapist has identified a generalization it can be

challenged in several ways. (a) As mentioned in the section on universal quantifiers, gen-

eralizations can be challenged by emphasizing the universal nature of the claim by the Surface Structure by inserting universal quanti- fiers into that Surface Structure. The therapist now asks the client to check the new generalization explicit in this Surface Structure against his experience. For example, the client says:

C: It S impossible to trust anyone. T: It's always impossible for anyone to trust anyone?

The purpose o f the therapist's challenge to the generalization is to re-connect the client's generalization with the client's experience. The therapist has other options in the way that he may challenge the client's generalizations.

(b) Since the purpose o f challenging the client's generaliza- tions is to re-connect the client's representation with his experi-

Page 78: THE STRUCTURE MAGIC I

84 1 Incantations for Growth and Potential

ence, one very direct challenge is, literally, to ask the client whether he has had an experience which contradicts his own generalization. For example, the client says:

C: lt's impossible to trust anyone. T : Have you ever had the experience of trusting someone?

or Have you ever trusted anyone?

Notice that, linguistically, the therapist is doing several things: Relativizing the generalization to the client's experience by shifting the referential index from no index (the missing indirect object of the predicate impossible [i.e., impossible for whom?] and the missing subject of the verb trust) to linguistic forms carrying the client's referential index (i.e., you).

(c) A third way of challenging generalizations of this form is to ask the client whether he can imagine an experience which would contradict the generalization. The client says:

C: lt's impossible to trust anyone. T : Can you imagine any circumstance in which you could

trust someone? or,

Can you fantasize a situation in which you could trust someone?

Once the client has succeeded in imagining or fantasizing a situa- tion which contradicts the generalization, the therapist may assist the client in opening up this part of his model by asking what the difference between the client's experience and the client's fantasy is, or what prevents the client from achieving the fantasy. Notice that one of the most powerful techniques here is to connect the client with the immediate experience that he is having, i.e., relati- vize the generalization to the process of ongoing therapy directly. The therapist may respond:

Do you trust me right now in this situation? If the client responds positively, his generalization has been con- tradicted. If he responds negatively, all the other techniques are available, e.g., asking what, specifically, is preventing the client from trusting the therapist in this situation.

(d) In the event the client is unable to fantasize an experience which contradicts his generalization, the therapist may choose to search his own models to find a case in which he has had an experience which contradicts the client's generalization. If the therapist can find some experience of his own which is common enough that the client also may have had it, he may ask whether that experience contradicts the client's generalization.

C: lt's impossible to trust anyone.

Page 79: THE STRUCTURE MAGIC I

Incantations for Growth and Potential / 85

T: Have you ever gone to the doctor (or to the dentist, ridden in a bus or taxi or airplane, o r . . .)? Did you trust the doctor (or dentist, or bus driver, o r . . .)?

Once the client has admitted that he has had an experience which contradicts his generalization, he has re-connected his representa- tion with his experience and the therapist is able to explore the differences with him.

(e) Another approach to challenging the client's,generalization is for the therapist to determine what makes the generalization possible or impossible. This technique is described in the section on modal operators of necessity (this chapter, p. 69).

C: It's impossible to trust anyone. T: What stops you from trusting anyone?

or, What would happen i f you trusted someone?

(f) Often the client will offer generalizations from his model in the form of generalizations about another person. For example:

C: My husband is always arguing with me. or,

My husband never smiles at me. Notice that the predicates argue with and smile at describe proc- esses which are occurring between two people. The form of the two sentences is: The subject (the active agent), the verb (the name of the process), and the object (the non-active person involved in the process). In both of the above examples, the client represents himself as the passive member of the process - the object of the predicate - thus avoiding any responsibility for the process or relationship. The generalizations which are reported by the client in these two Surface Structures involved a special kind of deletion - the Deep Structure is adequately represented by these Surface Structures but there is a deletion in the process of representing the client's experience by these Deep Structures. In other words, the client has deleted a portion of his experience when he represented it with the Deep Structure from which these Surface Structures are derived. The image of the processes or relationships of arguing with and smiling at are incomplete as only one person in the relationship is being described as having an active role. When faced with Surface Structures of this type, the therapist has the choice of asking for the way the person charac- terized as passive is involved in the process. One very specific and often potent way of asking for this information is to shift the referential indices contained in the client's generalization. In the examples given the shift would be:4 (See page 86)

Making these shifts in referential indices, the therapist creates a

Page 80: THE STRUCTURE MAGIC I

86 1 incantations for Growth and Potential

My husband I me (the client) and 1 husband

new Surface Structure based on the client's original Surface Struc- ture. Specifically:

My husband always argues with me. I always argue with my husband,

and My husband never smiles at me. I never smile at my husband.

Once the referential indices shift, the therapist may then ask the client to verify these new Surface Structures with the question:

Do you always argue with your husband? and

Do you ever smile at your husband? Here an additional linguistic distinction is available which may

be useful to the therapist: predicates which describe processes or relationships between two people are of two different logical types:

(a) Symmetrical predicates: Predicates which, if accurate, nec- essarily imply that their converse is also accurate. The predicate argue with is o f this logical type. If the Surface Structure:

My husband always argues with me. i s accurate, then necessarily the Surface Structure:

1 always argue with my husband. is also accurate. This property o f symmetrical predicates is repre- sented linguistically by the general form:

If a Surface Structure o f the form X Predicate Y is true and Predicate is a symmetrical predicate, then necessarily the Surface Structure of the form Y Predicate X is also true.

If you are arguing with me, then, necessarily, I am arguing with you. This same point is made by the expression, "It takes two to make an argument." In the case o f applying the referential index shift technique to Surface Structures the therapist knows that the result will be a generalization which is necessarily implied by the original. This technique assists the client in re-connecting his representation with his experience.

(b) Non-Symmetrical Predicates: Predicates which describe a relationship whose converse is not necessarily true. The predicate

Page 81: THE STRUCTURE MAGIC I

Incantations for Growth and Potential 1 87

smile at i s o f this logical type. If the Surface Structure: My husband never smiles at me.

is accurate, then it may or may not be true that the converse Surface Structure (with the referential indices shifted) is also accurate:

I never smile at my husband. While there is no logical necessity that the converse of a Surface Structure with a non-symmetrical predicate will be accurate, our experience has been that the converse is frequently psycho- logically accurate. That is, often when the client states a generali- zation about another person (especially if the relationship between the client and the person being characterized is an important one for the client), the converse is true. Traditionally, this phenom- enon has been referred to in some forms o f psychotherapy as projection. Whether the converse o f the client's Surface Structure turns out to be accurate, by asking the client to verify it, the therapist begins to recover the missing material and to help the client re-connect his representation with his experience.

(c) Clients sometimes present generalizations from their model in the form:

X o r Y For example, a client says:

C: 1 have to take care of other people. to which the therapist may reply (as outlined in the section on modal operators) :

T : Or what will happen? C: Or they won't like me.

Thus, the ful l generalization is: I have to take care of other people or they won't like me.

This generalization involves a claim that there is a necessary causal relationship between the client's taking care o f (or not taking care of) other people and other people's liking the client. The same claim is involved in the Surface Structure:

If 1 don't take care of people, they won't like me. In fact, within formal systems, the logical equivalence holds.'

X or Y = not X--Y

Whether the clients present their generalizations in the X or Y form spontaneously or supply the second portion - the outcome or consequence - upon questioning, their generalizations may be restated by the therapist in the equivalent I f . . . then . . . form. Once the therapist has had the client verify the I f . . . then . . . form of his generalization, he may challenge it by introducing

Page 82: THE STRUCTURE MAGIC I

88 1 Incantations for Growth and Potential

negatives into both portions of the generalization and presenting the resulting Surface Structure to the client:

l f you do take care of other people, they will like you? The therapist may use this reversal technique in combination with other techniques; for example, some of those discussed under modal operators or universal quantifiers, yielding the Surface Structure challenge:

If you do take care of other people, will they necessarily

like you? always

Complex Generalization - Equivalence We want to point out one additional, frequently occurring

form of generalization which i s somewhat more complex than the ones which we have so far considered in this section. These complex generalizations involve Surface Structures which are equivalent in the client's model. Typically, the client says one of these Surface Structures, pauses, and then says the second. The two Surface Structures characteristically have the same syntactic form. For example, the client says:

My husband never appreciates me. . . . My husband never smiles at me.

The two Surface Structures are syntactically parallel: Noun1 - Universal Quantifer - Verb - ~ o u n 2

where Noun1 = my husband Noun2 = me (the client)

Notice that one of these Surface Structures (the first) involves a violation of one of the well-formed-in-therapy conditions; specif- ically, the client is claiming knowledge of one of her husband's inner states (appreciate) without stating how she got her knowl- edge - a case of mind-reading. In the second Surface Structure, the process of one person's smiling or failing to smile at another person is described - a verifiable experience which doesn't require knowledge of the inner state of that other person. Both of these sample Surface Structures are generalizations which may be chal- lenged (using the technique described in the section on universal quantifiers). Here, however, we wish to offer a short-cut technique which often yields dramatic results. The therapist first checks to see if the two Surface Structures are, in fact, equivalents in the client's model. This is easily done by directly asking whether the two Surface Structures are equivalents:

Page 83: THE STRUCTURE MAGIC I

Incantations for Growth and Potential 1 89

C: My husband never appreciates me . . . My husband never smiles at me.

T : Does your husband's not smiling at you always mean that he doesn't appreciate you?

Here the c l ient is faced w i t h a choice - the c l ient w i l l deny the equivalence and the therapist may ask h o w the c l ient does, i n fact, k n o w tha t her husband doesn't appreciate her, or the c l ient verifies the equivalence. If t he equivalence o f these t w o Surface Structures is verified, t he therapist applies the referential index shif t technique:

I My husband me (the client) I me (the client)

My husband

This results i n the t ransformat ion o f t he Surface Structure f rom: Does your husband's not smiling at you always mean that he doesn't appreciate you?

t o the Surface Structure: Does your not smiling at your husband always mean that you don't appreciate him?

Let's review wha t has happened: 1. T h e c l ient says t w o Surface Structures which are separated

b y a pause and have the same syntact ic f o r m - one involv ing mind-reading, t he other not.

2. The therapist checks to see if t he t w o Surface Structures are equivalent.

3. The c l ient verifies the i r equivalence. Thus, we have the situation:

(X not smiling at Y) = (X doesn't appreciate Y) where X is the client's husband and Y is the client

4. T h e therapist shi f ts t he referential indices and asks t he client to verify the new generalization. T h e new Surface Structure has t he same logical fo rm:

(X not smiling at Y) = (X doesn't appreciate Y) where X is the client and Y is the client's husband.

5. Typical ly, the c l ient denies t he equivalence when she is t he active agent subject of the process.

(X not smiling at Y) f (X doesn't appreciate Y) where X is the client and Y is the client's husband

If t he c l ient accepts the new generalization, t he thera- pist has a l l the usual op t ions f o r challenging generalization. O u r experience is t ha t t he c l ient w i l l seldom accept t he new generalization.

Page 84: THE STRUCTURE MAGIC I

90 / Incantations for Growth and Potential

6. The therapist may now begin to explore the difference between the two situations: the one in which the equivalence holds and the one in which it does not. The client, again, has re- connected her generalization with her experience. The overall exchange looks like:

C: My husband never appreciates me. . . . My husband never smiles at me.

T: Does your husband's not smiling at you always mean that he doesn't appreciate you?

C: Yes, that's right! T: Does your not smiling at your husband always mean

that you don't appreciate him? C: No, that's not the same thing. T: What's the difference?

Incompletely Specified Verbs The second form of generalization which occurs in natural

language systems is that o f verbs which are not completely speci- fied. For example, in the Surface Structures,

(1 54) My mother hurt me. (1 55) My sister kicked me. (1 5 6 ) My friend touched me on the cheek with her lips.

the image presented is increasingly more specific and clear. So, in the first, the mother referred to may have caused some physical hurt or the hurt may have been "psychological"; she may have done it with a knife or a word or a gesture, . . . all o f this is lef t incompletely specified. In the next sentence, the sister mentioned may have kicked the speaker with her l e f t or her right foot, but it is specified to have been her foot; where the speaker was kicked is l e f t unspecified. I n the third example, the image presented is even more specified - the way the friend mentioned made contact is stated (touched with her lips) and the place on the speaker's body where contact was made is also specified (on the cheek). Notice, however, that the duration of the contact, the roughness or gentleness, are l e f t ~nspecif ied.~

Every verb o f which we are aware is incompletely specified to some degree. How clear the image is that the verb presents is determined by two factors:

(1) The meaning o f the verb itself. For example, the verb kiss is more specific by i t s meaning alone than the verb touch - kiss is equivalent to a specific form o f touching; namely, touching with one's lips.

(2) The amount of information presented by the rest o f the sentence in which the verb occurs. For example,

Page 85: THE STRUCTURE MAGIC I

incantations for Growth and Potential / 91

the phrase hurt by rejecting is more specified than simp13 the verb hurt.

Since every verb is to some degree incompletely specified, we suggest the following procedure:

Step 1 : Listen to the client's Surface Structure, identifying the process words or verbs.

Step 2: Ask yourself whether the image presented by the verb in i t s sentence is clear enough for you to visualize the actual sequence of events being described.

If the therapist finds that the image he has from the verb and the accompanying words and phrases o f the client's Surface Structure is not clear enough to visualize the actual sequence o f events being described, then he should ask for a more completely specified verb. The question available to the therapist to clarify the poorly focused image is:

How, specifically, did X Y? where X = the subject o f the incompletely specified verb and Y = the incompletely specified verb plus the remainder o f the client's original Surface Structure.

For example, given the Surface Structure ( 1 5 7) Susan hurt me.

the therapist asks for a more fully specified image with the question

( 1 5 8) How, specifically, did Susan hurt you? For the next set o f Surface Structures, formulate a question

which, when answered, would clarify your image o f the action being described.

(159) My children force me How, specifically, do to punish them. your children force

you to punish them? Also, how, specifically,

do you punish your children?

( 1 60) Sharon is always de- How, specifically, does manding attention from she demand attention me. from you?

( 1 61 ) / always show Jane that How, specifically, do / love her. you show Jane that

you love her?

Page 86: THE STRUCTURE MAGIC I

92 / Incantations for Growth and Potential

(1 62) My husband always ig- How, specifically, does nores me. your husband ignore

you?

(1 63) My family is trying to How, specifically, is drive me crazy. your family trying to

drive you crazy?

Every Surface Structure which is well formed in English con- tains a process word or verb. No verbs that we have encountered have been completely specified. Therefore, every one o f the client's Surface Structures is the occasion for the therapist to check to see whether the image presented is clear.

PRESUPPOSITIONS

Presuppositions are one linguistic reflex of the process of Distortion. The therapist's purpose in recognizing presuppositions is to assist the client in identifying those basic assumptions which I

impoverish his model and l imit his options in coping. Linguis- tically, these basic assumptions show up as presuppositions of the client's Surface Structures. For example, to make sense out o f the Surface Structure

(1 64) I'm afraid that my son is turning out to be as lazy as my husband. I

the therapist has to accept as true the situation expressed by the sentence presupposed by this sentence. Specifically,

(1 65) My husband is lazy. Notice that this last Surface Structure, the presupposition o f the one before, does not appear directly as any part o f the sentence which presupposes it. Linguists have developed a test for deter- mining what the presuppositions of any given sentence are. !

Adopted for the Meta-model they are Step 1: Listen for the main process word or verb in the

client's Surface Structure - call this Sentence A. I

Step 2: Create a new Surface Structure by introducing the negative word in the client's original Surface Structure on the main verb - call this Sentence B.

Step 3: Ask yourself what must be true for both A and B to make sense.

All of the things (expressed in the form of other sentences) which must be true for both A and B to make sense are the presupposi- tions of the client's original sentence. Specifically, in the case o f

Page 87: THE STRUCTURE MAGIC I

Incantations for Growth and Potential 1 93

the sentence, I'm afraid that my son is turning out to be as lazy as my husband.

by introducing the negative on the main verb (afraid), the thera- pist creates a second sentence,

( 1 66) I 'm not afraid that my son is turning out to be as lazy as my husband.

The point here is that, for the therapist to make sense out o f this new Surface Structure, it must be true that

( 1 65) My husband is lazy. Since both the client's original Surface Structure and the new Surface Structure formed from it by introducing the negative element require that this last sentence (165) be true, this last Surface Structure is the presupposition of the client's original sentence.

I n the succeeding set o f Surface Structures, identify the pre- suppositions o f each of the sentences.

( 1 67) If you are going to be as unreasonable as you were last time we dis- cussed this, then let's skip it.

(168) If judy has to be so possessive, then I'd rather not be involved with her.

( 1 69) If Fred had enjoyed my company, he wouldn't have left so early.

(170) If you knew how much I s u f f e r e d , y o u wouldn't act this way.

( 1 71) Since my problem is trivial, I'd rather not take up valuable group time.

- We discussed something.

- YOU were unreason- able the last time we discussed something.

Judy is possessive.

Fred didn't enjoy my company.

-I suffer. - You act out this way. - You don't know. . . .

My problem is trivial.

Linguists have identified a large number of specific forms or syntactic environments in language in which presuppositions nec-

Page 88: THE STRUCTURE MAGIC I

94 1 Incantations for Growth and Potential

essarily occur. For example, any portion o f a Surface Structure which occurs after the main verbs realize, be aware, ignore, etc., is a presupposition or necessary assumption of that Surface Struc- ture. Notice that these specific forms or syntactic environments are independent o f the content or meaning of the words and phrases used. We have included an appendix (Appendix B) which identifies these syntactic environments to assist those who wish to train themselves more thoroughly in the recognition of the lan- guage forms which carry presuppositions.

Having identified the presuppositions o f the client's Surface Structures, the therapist may now challenge them. Due to the complexity o f the presuppositions, the therapist has a number o f choices.

1. The therapist may present the client with the presupposi- tion implicit in his original Surface Structure directly. I n doing this, the therapist can ask the client to explore this presupposition, using the other well-formed-in-therapy conditions. For example, the client says, (1 72) I'm afraid that m y son is turning out to be as lazy as

my husband. The therapist identifies the presupposition

(1 7 3) M y husband is lazy. and presents it to the client, asking her how, specifically, her husband is lazy. The client responds with another Surface Struc- ture which the therapist evaluates for well-formedness-in-therapy.

2. The therapist may decide to accept the presupposition and apply the well-formed-in-therapy condition to the client's original Surface Structure, asking to specify the verb, re- cover the deleted material, etc.

We will present a set of Surface Structures which have presup- positions and give some possible ways o f challenging them. Remember that the questions we offer are examples and do not exhaust all the possibilities.

(1 74) l f m y wife is going to What, specifically, be as unreasonable as seemed unreasonable she was the last time / to you about your tried to talk to her a- wife? bout this, then / cer- How, specifically, did tainly won't try again. your wife seem to you

to be unreasonable?

Page 89: THE STRUCTURE MAGIC I

Incantations for Growth and Potential / 95

' (175) If Judy has to be so How, specifically, does

possessive, then I'd Judy seem to you to rather not be involved be possessive? with her.

SEMANTIC WELL-FORMEDNESS

The purpose o f recognizing sentences which are semantically ill formed is to assist the client in identifying the portions o f his model which are distorted in some way that impoverishes the experiences which are available to him. Typically, these impov- erishing distortions take the form o f limiting the client's options in some way that reduces the client's ability to act. We have identi- fied some frequently occurring classes o f semantic ill-formedness which we typically encounter in therapy. We present the linguistic characterization for each class below. The choices which the therapist has for dealing with the first two classes o f semantically ill-formed Surface Structures are essentially the same. Therefore, we will present these choices in one section after we have pre- sented both o f these classes.

Cause and Effect This class o f semantically ill-formed Surface Structures in-

volves the belief on the part o f the speaker that one person (or set of circumstances) may perform some action which necessarily causes some other person to experience some emotion or inner state. Typically, the person experiencing this emotion or inner state is portrayed as having no choice in responding the way he does. For example, the client says,

(1 76) My wife makes me feel angry. Notice that this Surface Structure presents a vague image in which one human being (identified as My wife) performs some action (unspecified) which necessarily causes some other person (identi- fied as me) t o experience some emotion (anger). Ill-formed Sur- face Structures which are members o f this class can be identified by one o f two general forms:

(A) X Verb Y Verb Adjective (cause) (feel (some emotion

experience) or some inner state)

where X and Y are nouns which have different referential

Page 90: THE STRUCTURE MAGIC I

96 / Incantations for Growth and Potential

indices, i.e., refer to different people. The Surface Structure presented above is o f this form - namely:

My wife - makes me feel angry

I Verb

I I Y Verb

I Adjective

(cause) (feel (some emotion experience) or

some inner state)

The other general form which we frequently encounter is that o f underlying Surface Structures such as:

(1 77) Your laughing distracts me. The general form is:

(B) X Verb Verb Y (cause)

where X and Y are nouns which have different referential indices, i.e., refer to different people.

Applying the general form to the example we have:

Your laughing distracts me

I Verb

I Verb (cause)

We will now present a set o f Surface Structures, all o f which are semantically ill formed in the way we have been discussing. Thh is to assist you in training your intuitions to recognize examples o f this type o f semantic ill-formedness.

( 1 7 8) She compels me to be jealous. (1 79) You always make me feel happy. (1 80) He forced me to feel bad. (1 81 ) She causes me a lot of pain. (1 82) Your writing on the wall bothers me. (1 83) Their crying irritates me.

In addition to Surface Structures which are o f these two general forms, there are others which have a different form but have the same meaning relationships. For example, the Surface Structure

(1 84) She depresses me. carries the same meaning relationship as the Surface Structure

(1 85) She makes me feel depressed.

Page 91: THE STRUCTURE MAGIC I

Incantations for Growth and Potential J 97

In fact, to assist therapists in training their intuitions to recognize semantically ill-formed Surface Structures of this type, this para- phrase test can be used. Specifically, if the Surface Structure the client presents can be translated from

X Verb Y where X and Y are nouns with different referential indices

into the general form (a)

X Verb Y Verb Adjective (cause) (feel (emotion or

experience) inner state)

where the adjective is a form related to the verb in the client's original Surface Structure

and the new Surface Structure means the same as the client's original Surface Structure, then the Surface Structure is seman- tically ill formed. As an additional example, the client says,

(1 86) You bore me. To apply the paraphrase test, move the verb in this Surface Structure to the end of the new Surface Structure and put the verb cause or make in i t s original position, and insert the verb feel or experience, yielding,

(1 87) You make me feel bored. The question now is whether this new Surface Structure and the client's original mean the same thing. In this case, they do, and the client's original Surface Structure is identified as being seman- tically ill formed. To assist you in training your intuitions in identifying this class of semantically ill-formed Surface Structures, we present the following set of sentences. Determine which of the Surface Structures are ill formed by using the paraphrase test with form (A).

( 1 88) Music pleases me. = Music makes me feel pleased.

(1 89) M y husband likes me. + My husband makes me feel liked.

(1 90) Your ideas annoy me. = Your ideas make me feel annoyed.

(1 9 1 ) His plan insults me. = His plan makes me fee l insulted.

Page 92: THE STRUCTURE MAGIC I

9 8 / Incantations for Growth and Potential

(192) Policemen follow me. # Policemen make me feel followed.

One additional, frequently occurring surface Structure form in th~s class is

(1 93) I'm sad that you forgot our anniversary. or,

( 1 94) I'm sad since you forgot our anniversary. or,

(1 95) I'm sad because you forgot our anniversary. Once again, these three Surface Structures can be paraphrased by the Surface Structure:

(1 96) Your forgetting our anniversary makes me feel sad. Notice that this last Surface Structure is of the general form (B). Thus, a paraphrase tes t is again available here to assist you in training your intuitions. Specifically, if the client's Surface Struc- ture can be paraphrased by a sentence of the general form (B), it is semantically ill formed.

We present an additional set of Surface Structures. Determine which of them are semantically ill formed by using the paraphrase test with form (B).

(1 97) I'm down since you = Your not helping me won't help me. makes me feel down.

(198) I'm lonely because = Your not being here you're not here. makes me feel lonely.

(1 99) I'm happy that I'm = My going to Mexico going to Mexico. makes me feel happy.

(Note: The paraphrase test works but the Surface Structure is not ill formed since both nouns, X and Y in the general form (B), have the same referential index.)

(200) She's hurt that you're = Your not paying any not paying any atten- attention to her tion to her. makes her feel hurt.

But, In addition to the forms of Surface Structures which we have

presented involving ways that the client experiences having no choice, we have found it useful in teaching other therapists in training to hear the cue word but. This conjunction but, which

Page 93: THE STRUCTURE MAGIC I

Incantations for Growth and Potential 1 99

translates in many o f i t s uses logically as and not, functions to identify what the client considers the reasons or conditions which make something he wants impossible or which make something he doesn't want necessary. For example, the client says:

(201) / want to leave home but my father is sick. When we hear Surface Structures o f this form, we understand the client to be identifying a cause-effect relationship in his model o f the world. Thus, we call Surface Structures o f this general form l mplied Causatives.

(202) X but Y In the specific example above, the client is reporting what is a necessary causal connection in his model, namely, that his father's being sick prevents him from leaving home. The portion o f the Surface Structure represented by X identifies something the client wants (i.e., to leave home) and the portion represented by Y identifies the condition or reason (i.e., my father is sick) that the client is blocked from getting X. We have identified one other common form lmplied Causatives typically have in Surface Struc- tures. The client says:

(203) / don't want to leave home, but my father is sick. In this form o f the lmplied Causative the X represents something the client does not want (i.e., to leave home), and the Y represents the condition or reason that is forcing the client to experience the thing he doesn't want (i.e., my father is sick). I n other words, the client's father's being sick is forcing the client to leave home. These are the two lmplied Causatives that we have most fre- quently encountered. Both o f the forms share the characteristic that the client experiences no choice. In the first case, he wants something (the X in the general form X but Y) and some condi- tion is preventing him from getting it (the Y) . In the second case, the client does not want something (the X), but something else (the Y ) is forcing him to experience it. The following set o f Surface Structures is composed o f examples o f lmplied Causatives to assist you in recognizing the semantic relationship.

(204) / would change but a lot of people depend on me. (205) / don't want to get angry but she is always blaming

me. (206) / would like to get to the bottom of this, but I'm

taking up too much of the group's time. (207) / don't enjoy being uptight but my job demands it.

Therapists have at least the following three choices in coping with lmplied Causatives.

(a) Accept the cause-effect relationship and ask if it is always that way. For example, the client says:

Page 94: THE STRUCTURE MAGIC I

100 / Incantations for Growth and Potential

(205) / don't want to get angry but she is always blaming me.

The therapist may respond: (206) Do you always get mad when she blames you?

The client will often recognize times when she has blamed him and he has not gotten angry. This opens up the possibility of deter- mining what the difference is between those times and when her blaming "automatically makes" the client angry.

(b) Accept the cause-effect relationship and ask the client to specify this relationship of l mplied Causative more fully. To the client's Surface Structure above, the therapist may respond:

(207) How, specifically, does her blaming you make you angry?

The therapist continues to ask for specifics until he has a clear image of the process of Implied Causation as represented in the client's model.

(c) Challenge the cause-effect relationship. One direct way of doing this which we have found useful is to feed back a Surface Structure which reverses the relationship. For example, the client says:

(205) / don't want to get angry but she's always blaming me.

The therapist may respond:' (208) Then, if she didn't blame you, you wouldn't be-

come angry, is that true? or, the client says:

(201 ) / want to leave home but my father is sick. The therapist may respond:

(209) Then, if your father weren't sick, you would leave home, right?

This technique amounts to asking the client to reverse the condi- tion in his model which is preventing him from achieving what he wants, or to reverse or remove the conditions in his model which are forcing him to do something he doesn't want to do and then asking whether this reversal gives him what he wants. Let's examine this process more carefully. If someone says to me:

/ want to relax but my back is killing me. I understand him to be saying:

I can't relax I want to relax but because my

I am not relaxed

back is killing me.

Page 95: THE STRUCTURE MAGIC I

Incantations for Growth and Potential 1 101

Thus, Surface Structures of the form: X but Y

involve a deletion. Their full form is: X and not X because o f Y

Using the previous example we have the initial Surface Structure: I want to leave home but my father is sick.

which, using the equivalence we just suggested, has a full representation:

I want to leave home and ( : z'i} leave home because

my father is sick.

Once this fuller version of the original Surface Structure is avail- able, the therapist may apply the reversal technique for Implied Causatives. From a Surface Structure of the form

X and not X because o f Y he forms a new reversed Surface Structure with only the second part of the fuller version:

not X because o f Y. This new Surface Structure consists of an I f . . . then. . . construc- tion with this latter portion of the full representation reversed where negatives have been added for both the X and the Y portions. In a step-by-step presentation:

(1) Place the latter portion of the full representation in an I f . . . then. . . construction in reversed order -

( } leave home). ' If (my father is sick), then (

{ } means one expression or the otherhot both.

(2) Introduce negatives into both the I f part and the then part -

I f (my father weren't sick), then ( leave home).

Page 96: THE STRUCTURE MAGIC I

or, translating the double negatives into grammatical English:

If (my father weren't sick), then ( ( r''; ] leaue

home).

(3) Present the reversed generalization to the client for verification or denial. If your father weren't sick, you would leave home?

This reversal technique has been, in our experience, very effective in challenging the Cause-Effect generalization involved. The client often succeeds in taking responsibility for his continuing decision to do or not to do what he originally claims someone or something else controls. To review, the reversal technique for Implied Causatives o f the form X but Y involves the following steps:

(1) Expand the client's original Surface Structure to i ts fuller version (with the deletion restored), using the equivalence:

( X but y I-( x and not X because y )

my father I want to my father $ but i s i k ) ( Z e a e h e ) and ({ : z':} ) bemuse (is sick ) ( 2 ) Place the second portion of the restored Surface Structure - the portion after the and - in an I f . . .

then. . . construction in the reversed order: (See page 103) (3) Introduce negatives into the new Surface Structure in both the lf and the then portions:

(See page 103)

Page 97: THE STRUCTURE MAGIC I

( 2 ) ( n o t X because

leavehome because ({ Idon't) ) (3) (If y then not

my father (. sick )-If (;ziyr) then ( ' ;::e)

( I f not Y then not not X)

b father) then ({I I don't ] leave)- home (z;:irr) (4) Present the final form o f the new Surface Structure

generalization?

\ 3

then (1' I can.t] don't not leave) home s 3 2. 0

as a challenge to the client's original 2 s' -

Well, then, if your father weren't sick, you would leave home? G)

(d) One additional technique which we have found useful is to strengthen the client's generalizations about a Implied Causative by inserting the modal operator o f necessity into the client's Surface Structure when we feed 4 it back, asking the client to verify or challenge it. For example, the client says: 3

(201 ) / want to leave home, bu t m y father is sick. Q -0

The therapist may respond: % (21 0) Are you saying that your father's being sick necessarily prevents you from leaving home? 2. 3 The client often will balk at this Surface Structure since it blatantly claims that the two events, X and Y, -

1

are necessarily connected. If the client balks here, the way i s opened for the client and the therapist to explore - how it is not necessary. If the client accepts the strengthened version (with necessarily), the way is opened for S

Page 98: THE STRUCTURE MAGIC I

104 / Incantations for Growth and Potential

exploring how that necessary causal connection actually works, asking for more specifics about that connection. This technique works particularly well in conjunction with options (a) and (b) described above.

Mind Reading This class o f semantically ill-formed Surface Structures in-

volves the belief on the part o f the speaker that one person can know what another person is thinking and feeling without a direct communication on the part o f the second person. For example, the client says:

(21 1 ) Everybody in the group thinks that I'm taking up too much time.

Notice that the speaker is claiming to know the contents o f the minds of all o f the people in the group. I n the following set of Surface Structures, identify those which contain the claim that one person knows the thoughts or feelings o f another person.

(21 2) Henry is angry at me. Yes (21 3) Martha touched me on the shoulder. no (2 1 4) I'm sure she liked your present. Yes (21 5) john told me he was angry. no (21 6) 1 know what makes him happy. Yes (21 7) 1 know what's best for you. Yes (21 8) You know what I'm trying to say. Yes (21 9) You can see how 1 feel. Yes

Another less obvious example o f this same class is Surface Structures which presuppose that some person is able to read another's mind. For example,

(220) I f she loved me, she would always do what I would like her to do.

(2 2 1 ) I'm disappointed that you didn't take my feelings into account.

These two cases o f semantic ill-formedness - Cause and Effect and Mind-Reading - can be dealt with by the therapist in essen- tially the same way. Both of these involve Surface Structures which present an image o f some process which is too vague to allow the therapist to form a clear picture o f what the client's model is. I n the first case, a process is described which claims that one person is performing some action which causes another person to experience some emotion. I n the second case, a process is described which claims that one person comes to know what another person is thinking and feeling. In neither case is it given how, specifically, these processes are being accomplished. Thus, the therapist responds by asking, how, specifically, these processes

Page 99: THE STRUCTURE MAGIC I

Incantations for Growth and Potential 1 105

occur. I n our experience, Surface Structures which include Cause and Effect and Mind-Reading identify portions of the client's model in which impoverishing distortions have occurred. In Cause and Effect Surface Structures, the clients feel that they literally have no choice, that their emotions are determined by forces outside o f themselves. In Mind:Reading Surface Structures, the clients have little choice as they have already decided what the other people involved think and feel. Therefore, they respond on the level o f their assumptions about what these others think and feel when, in fact, their assumptions about the others' thoughts and feelings may be invalid. Conversely, in Cause and Effect, the client may come to feel guilty or, at least, responsible for "causing" some emotional response in another. In Mind-Reading clients may systematically fail to express their thoughts and feelings, making the assumption that others are able to know what they are thinking and feeling. We are not suggesting that it is impossible for one human being to come to know what another is thinking and feeling but that we want to know exactly by what process this occurs. Since it is highly improbable that one human being can directly read another's mind, we want details about how this information was transferred. We view this as being very important, as in our experience the client's assumed ability to read another's mind and the client's assumptions that another can read his mind is the source o f vast amounts o f inter-personal diffi- culties, miscommunication and i t s accompanying pain. Even less probable from our experience is the ability o f one person to directly and necessarily cause an emotion in another human being. Therefore, we label all Surface Structures o f these forms seman- tically ill formed until the process by which what they claim is true is made explicit, and the Surface Structures representing this process are themselves well formed in therapy. The therapist asks for an explicit account o f the process implied by Surface Struc- tures o f these two classes essentially by the question how? As before, in the section on incompletely specified verbs, the thera- pist is satisfied only when he has a clearly focused image of the process being described. This process might proceed as follows:

C: Henry makes me angry. T : How, specifically, does Henry make you angry? C: He never considers my feelings.

The therapist has at least the following choices: (a) What feelings, specifically? (b) How do you know that he never considers your

feelings? The therapist chooses to ask (b) and the client responds:

Page 100: THE STRUCTURE MAGIC I

106 / Incantations for Growth and Potential

C: Because he stays out so late every night. The therapist now has at least the following choices:

(a) Does Henry's staying out at night always make you angry?

(b) Does Henry's staying out at night always mean that he never considers your feelings?

The client's subsequent Surface Structures are subjected to the well-formed-in-therapy conditions by the therapist.

The Lost Performative Each o f us has noticed that in the therapeutic encounter

clients characteristically make statements in the form o f a generali- zation about the world itself , which include judgments which we recognize as being true o f their model o f the world. For example, the client says

(222) It's wrong to hurt anyone's feelings. We understand this sentence to be a statement about the client's model o f the world, specifically, a rule for himself. Notice that the form o f the Surface Structure the client uses suggests a generaliza- tion which is true about the world; the Surface Structure is not relativized to the client. There is no indication in the Surface Structure that the client is aware that the statement made is true for his particular model; there is no indication that the client recognizes that there may be other possibilities. We translate this sentence, then, into the Surface Structure

(223) I say to you that it's wrong for me to hurt anyone's feelings.

Within the transformational model, linguists have presented an analysis which shows that every Surface Structure is derived from a Deep Structure which has a sentence o f the form (see Ross, 1970)

(224) I say to you that S , where S is the Surface Structure. This higher sentence is called the I

Performative and is, in most cases, deleted by a transformation I

called Performative Deletion in i t s derivation to Surface Struc- I tures. Notice that, by this analysis, the Deep Structure explicitly I

identifies the speaker as the source o f the generalization about the I world; in other words, the sentence which shows up in Surface Structures as a generalization about the world is represented in I

Deep Structure as a generalization from the speaker's model o f the I

world. The point o f this is not to have the client present each Surface Structure preceded by the Performative, but rather to

I train ourselves as therapists to recognize that the generalizations I

which the client presents about the world are generalizations ! I

Page 101: THE STRUCTURE MAGIC I

Incantations for Growth and Potential / 107

about his model o f the world. Once recognized, the therapist may challenge these generalizations in such a way that the client comes to see these generalizations as true for his belief system at a specific moment in time. Since these are generalizations about his beliefs, rather than generalizations about the world itself, the therapist may work to assist the client in developing other possible options within his model. This is particularly important in cases in which the generalization reduces the choices experienced by the client. This is typically associated with areas of the client's model in which he experiences pain and has limited options which he does not find satisfying. There are a number o f cue words which we have found useful in identifying Surface Structures o f this class. These include:

good, bad, crazy, sick, correct, right, wrong, only (as in: There is only one way. . .) true, false, . . .

These are only someaf the cue words which you may find useful in identifying Surface Structures o f this class. The identifying feature o f this class i s that the Surface Structures have the form o f making generalizations about the world; they are not relativized to the speaker. Linguistically, all trace o f the Performative has been deleted.

WELL FORMED IN THERAPY

We have presented an extended set o f explicit examples which therapists can use to train their intuitions in identifying the phenomenon we called "well formed in therapy." This constitutes the explicit Meta-model for therapy. While we recognize that our Meta-model covers only a portion of the verbal communication which is possible in therapy, we present in the next chapter examples of therapy in which we have restricted the therapist totally to our Meta-model. This is artificial in that the Meta-model is a set of tools designed to be used in conjunction with the different possible approaches to therapy. We want you to imagine the potentially increased effectiveness o f therapy conducted with our Meta-model incorporated into your specific approach to ther- apy. We want to remind you. that, while our Meta-model is designed specifically for verbal communication, it is a special case of the general modeling that we, as humans, do. We will generalize our Meta-model to other forms o f human representational systems in Chapter Six.

Page 102: THE STRUCTURE MAGIC I

108 1 Incantations for Growth and Potential

EXERCISE C

Each of the specific sections presented detail steps for you to go through in order to sharpen your intuitions regarding well formed in therapy. All that is required is that you read carefully and apply the step-by-step procedures out- lined, and that you have access to some set of Surface Structures. The step-by-step procedures are presented here; the set of Surface Structures to which you may apply these techniques is available wherever people.are talking. One specific way of obtaining Surface Structures to use in applying these techniques is to use your own internal voice (inner dialogue) as a source. We suggest that, initially, you use a tape recorder and tape your internal voice by speaking it out loud. Then use the tape as a source for applying the well-formed-in-therapy conditions. After you have had some practice in this, you may simply become aware of the inner dialogue and apply the conditions directly t r these sentences without going through a tape recorder. This technique will provide you with a limitless source of sentences which you can use to train yourself.

We cannot overemphasize the need to practice and familiarize yourself with all of the material in Chapter Four. The step-by-step procedure makes this material learnable; whether or not you specifically learn this mate- rial will depend upon your willingness to practice. While the step-by-step procedure may at first feel somewhat artificial, after some practice it will become unnecessary for you to proceed in this manner. That is, after training yourself using these explicit methods, you will be able to operate in a rule-governed way, applying the well-formed- in-therapy conditions, without any need to be aware of the step-by-step procedures.

FOOTNOTES FOR CHAPTER 4

1. The general set of transformations which distinguish the derivation ! I

of the Surface Structure (30) in the text from the Surface Structure (31) is called Relative Clause Reduction in the linguistic literature. Both (30) and (31) are derived from the same Deep Structure.

2. Notice that the question What would happen i f you failed to take other people's feelings into account?

Page 103: THE STRUCTURE MAGIC I

Incantations for Growth and Potential 1 109

differs in one important way from the client's Surface Structure that it is derived from

One must take other people's feelings into account. In the client's Surface Structure, the word one occurs as the subject noun argument o f the verb must take. . . . The word one has no referential index. In forming the question, the therapist shifts the subject noun argument of the client's Surface Structure to a noun argument which has a referential index - specifically, the client - i.e., the word you. This kind of referential index shift will be treated in more detail in the section Generalization.

3. We present these two classes of modal operators as separate classes. They are, however, closely connected in the logical systems from which we borrow the terminology. For example, the following equivalence holds logi- cally as well as psychologically:

not possible not (X) =necessary (X) In English, the logical equivalence of the two distinct Surface Structures:

l t is not possible to not be afraid = It is necessary to be afraid. We separate the two classes for the purposes of presentation.

4. Readers familiar with elementary logical systems will recognize this as a case of the ubstitution rule in, for example, the propositional calculus. C The only constr Int is that when some term me i s substituted for some other

' term my husband, then all instances o f the term my husband must be replaced by the term me. The same constraint works well in the context of therapy.

5. The reader familiar with the most elementary of the logical systems can verify this formal equivalence using truth tables:

X Y X v Y - X-Y Thus, the logical equivalence of

X V Y and %X*Y.

where 'L = the negation symbol and-= the implication symbol

In our experience they also have a psychological equivalence. 6. Here, in the analysis of verbs which are differentially specified, we

suspect that some of the research currently being conducted in Generative Semantics (see McCawley, Lakoff, Grinder ana Postal in the bibliography) will be particularly useful in expanding the Meta-model further.

7. Readers familiar with logical systems will notice a similarity between parts of the reversal technique for Implied Causatives and the formal rule of

Page 104: THE STRUCTURE MAGIC I

1 1 0 / Incantations for Growth and Potential

derivation called Contraposition. The transformation of the original Surface Structure into the challenge by the therapist can be represented by the following sequence:

Line 1: X but Y Line 2: X and not X because Y Line 3: not X because Y Line 4: not Y and not not X

Specifically, if the natural language connective because were to be interpreted as the logical connective implies, then the transformation between Lines 3 and 4 i s the formal transformation Contraposition.

Page 105: THE STRUCTURE MAGIC I

Chapter 5

INTO THE VORTEX

In his chapter we will present a series o f (example) transcripts /t with a running commentary. Our point here is to provide for you the opportunity to see the Meta-model in operation. In order to present to you the clearest image of how the Meta-model operates, we have restricted the therapist in these sessions to the use o f Meta-model techniques only. This restriction was placed upon the therapist t o provide material for this book that would be a clear representation of the Meta-model and should not be taken as a statement by us that digital communication is all a therapist needs to know about. Neither is it a representation of the work that we do or that we would recommend that the therapist do. Rather, this is an opportunity for you to see the Meta-model in action and to see how each response f i a t our clients provide in the form o f a Surface Structure is an opportunity for the therapist to proceed in a variety o f ways. This means, as you will see, that at any point in therapy you will have a number o f relevant techniques available. We would like you to imagine the Meta-model techniques used in the following transcripts integrated with the form o f therapy you already use, and to imagine how the Meta-model, in conjunction, could provide a rich set o f choices for you as a therapist.

In the running commentary which we provide for the tran- script, it is not our purpose to present the way we see the therapist seeing, hearing, feeling, and thinking about what i s happening in the therapeutic encounter. We provide the commentary to first, show how what the therapist is doing may be explicitly described in terms of the Meta-model. We are making no claim that the

Page 106: THE STRUCTURE MAGIC I

1 12 / Into the Vortex

intermediate processes which are stated in our commentary as occurring in the model actually occur in the human beings whose behavior is being modeled.' For example, when our commentary points out that the therapist can identify a deletion in the client's Surface Structure by first determining whether he can create another well-formed Surface Structure of English wherein the process word or verb from the client's original Surface Structure appears with more arguments than it has associated with it in the original Surface Structure, and then can subsequently ask for the portion missing from the Deep Structure representation, we are not suggesting that this is, in fact, what the therapist is doing. Further, we are not recommending that you go through these steps. Secondly, in addition to offering the commentary as a way of showing you how verbal behavior in therapy may be under- stood in terms of the Meta-model, the running commentary.will allow you to train and sharpen your intuitions further so that what is described in the commentary in a step-by-step process will become immediate for you. Our experience in training therapists in the Meta-model has been that, typically, they experience a

phase in which they become aware that they are going through a step-by-step process. As they perfect this technique, it becomes automatic and drops out of their consciousness. Their behavior, however, is s t i l l systematic in this respect.

TRANSCRIPT 1 Ralph is 34 years old and works as assistant manager of a

division of a large electronics firm. The client &IS asked what he hoped to get out of the interview

and began:

(1) Ralph: Well. . . I'm The client is experiencing difficulty not really saying exactly what it is that he sure . . . wants. Remember, one of the first

tasks of the therapist is to under- stand the client's model (especially I

those portions which are impov- I

erishing). The therapist here notices a deletion in the first Surface Struc- I ture the client presents. Specif- ically, he identifies the process or relationship word sure, and that the 1 client has provided only one argu- ment or noun (1) for the predicate sure. The therapist can determine

Page 107: THE STRUCTURE MAGIC I

Into the Vortex 1 1 1 3

whether this Surface Structure .is a full representation of the client's Deep Structure by asking himself whether he can create another well-formed Surface Structure of English with the predicate sure and which has more than one argument or noun. For example, the Surface Structure

( ) I'm sure of the answer. In this Surface Structure, there are two arguments or nouns associated with sure: someone who is sure of something (in this case, I), and something that the person is sure of (in this case, the answer). Thus, the therapist knows by his intuitions as a native speaker of English that the client's Deep Structure contained a portion which does not appear in his Surface Structure - it has been deleted. The therapist chooses to try to recover the deleted material by asking for it.

( 2 ) Therapist: You're not Therapist asks for missing portion sure of what? of Deep Structure.

( 3 ) R: I'm not sure that The client has produced a new Sur- this will be face Structure containing the infor- helpful. mation which had been deleted from

his first Surface Structure. The therapist listens to the client and examines his new sentence, noticing, (a) an argument or noun (this) asso- ciated with the verb will be helpful which has no referential index; (b) that the Surface Structure represen- tation is incomplete - this predicate helpful occurs in well-formed Sur- face Structures of English with more than one argument or noun (e.g.,

Page 108: THE STRUCTURE MAGIC I

1 14 / Into the Vortex

T : You're not sure what, specifically, will be helpful to whom?

(5) R: Well, I'm not sure that this experiment will be helpful. You see, when l first went to Dr. G., he asked me if I'd be willing to participate in this experiment, . . . and well, I feel that there's something I really

( ) You are being helpful to me.)

Since helpful can occur with more than one argument noun as it did in the client's Surface Structure, the therapist knows that a portion of the client's Deep Structure has been deleted; (c) that the verb helpful is very incompletely specified; the Surface Structure presents the ther- apist with no clear image of the kind of help the client wants.

By recognizing the specific ways in which the client's Surface Struc- ture fails to be well formed in ther- apy, the therapist has made a number of options available to him- self, such as: (1) he may ask for the referential index - You're not sure that what, specifically, will be help- ful?, (2) he may ask for the deleted material - helpful to whom/what?, ( 3 ) he may ask the client what specific kind of help he had hoped for, -Helpful in what way?

The therapist has chosen to go for both (1) and (2).

The client is expressing concern that the experimental conditions - restricting the therapist to the I Meta-model techniques - will not allow him to get the help that he

I wants. The therapist is attempting to understand the client's model and notices the following: (a) the client's first Surface Structure con- I

tains the nominalization experi- ment derived from the verb to I experiment; it has two noun argu- ments associated with it which have

Page 109: THE STRUCTURE MAGIC I

Into the Vortex / 1 15

need help with but this is just an experiment.

(6) T: How will this just being an experiment prevent you from getting the help that you need?

(7) R: Experiments are for research, but there's something

been deleted - the person doing the experiment and the person or thing being experimented upon; (b) in the client's first Surface Struc- ture, one of the arguments of the verb helpful has been deleted (spe- cifically, helpful to whom); (c) also, in the client's first Surface Struc- ture, the verb helpful is very incom- pletely specified - it presents no clear image; (d) in the latter part of the client's second Surface Struc- ture, the noun something occurs - this noun has no referential index; (e) the Surface Structure noun help is a nominalization from the verb help, is very incompletely speci- fied and has two deletions: it pre- sents no clear image of the person or thing helping and the person or thing being helped; (f) again, the nominalization experiment occurs with both of the deletions men- tioned in (a) above; (g) the client's last Surface Structure in this sec- tion is of the general form X but Y - the Implied Causative. Specifi- cally, the implication is that the client wants something (X = there's something that I really want help with) and there is something which is preventing him from getting it, (Y = this is just an experiment).

The therapist chooses to challenge the Implied Causative (g).

The client responds with a re- statement of the Implied Causative, X but Y. Notice that it still con-

Page 110: THE STRUCTURE MAGIC I

1 1 6 / Into the Vortex

/ really need help with.

( 8 ) T : What, specifically, do you really need help with ?

(9 ) R: I don't know how to make a good impression on people.

tains (a) the old nominalization experiment with two deletions; (b) a new nominalization research with two deletions - the person doing the research, and the person or thing being researched; (c) the noun something which is missing a refer- ential index; and (d) the old nomi- nalization help with its two deletions.

The therapist lets the Implied Caus- ative stand unchallenged and chooses to go after the referential index (c).

The client presents a Surface Struc- ture which he sees as providing the referential index for the noun something in his last Surface Struc- ture. This new Surface Structure violates the well-formed-in-therapy conditions of (a) the nominaliza- tion impression with one deletion - the person or thing doing the im- pressing; (b) the adjective good in the phrase good impression is de- rived from a Deep Structure predi- cate X is good for Y, the X in this form is the impression, the Y has been deleted - i.e., who is the im- pression good for - who benefits from this action; (c) the noun people has no referential index; (d) the client's Surface Structure is semantically ill formed as he appears to be mind-reading. He states that he doesn't know how to make a good impression on people but fails to state how he knows that I I this is true. The way he knows he doesn't make a good impression is not stated.

Page 111: THE STRUCTURE MAGIC I

Into the Vortex 1 129

when. . . .

(63) R: No, it's not the same.

(64) T: I t? What's not the same as what?

(65) R: My not noticing is not the same as my mother not noticing - see, she NEVER noticed what 1 did for her.

(66) T: Never?

(67) R: Well, not very many times.

(68) T: Ralph, tell me about one specific time when your

the referential indices that he had shifted earlier. This results in the client's original equivalence state- ment: namely, that

X not noticing = X not interested where X = client's mother

The client recognizes the therapist's challenge before he completes it, interrupts him, and denies that the two cases (where X = the client and where X = the client's mother) are the same. The Surface Structure he uses to deny this fails the well- formed-in-therapy conditions: (a) the pronoun it has no referential index, and (b) the second portion of the comparative has been deleted.

The therapist asks for both the ref- erential index and the missing por- tion of the comparative.

The client fills in the information requested by the therapist. He then goes on to describe the difference between the two cases, namely, that his mother never noticed. This universal quantifier identifies a challengeable generalization.

The therapist challenges the univer- sal quantifier.

The client admits that there were exceptions, thereby coming closer to re-connecting his generalization with his experience.

The therapist attempts to get the client to focus the model by asking for a specific exception to the client's initial generalization.

Page 112: THE STRUCTURE MAGIC I

130 / Into the Vortex

mother noticed what you had done for her.

Well, once when . . . yeah (angrily), 1 even had to tell her.

Had to te l l her what?

That I had done this thing for her. I f she had been interested enough she would have noticed i t herself.

lnterested enough for what?

lnterested enough to show me that she loved me.

Ralph, how did you show your mother that you loved her?

One of the argument nouns associ- ated with the verb tell has been deleted (tell what?).

The therapist asks for the missing piece of the Surface Structure.

The first Surface Structure includes a noun argument (this thing) and lacks a referential index. The client's second Surface Structure in- cludes a deletion associated with the word enough (enough for what), and a pronoun it without a referential index.

The therapist asks for the deleted material.

The client supplies the deleted ma- terial that the therapist requested. This new Surface Structure includes (a) a violation of the semantic well- formedness condition of mind- reading - the client claims to know whether his mother loved him with- out specifying how he got that in- formation; (b) the verb love is very incompletely specified.

The therapist is attempting to gain a clear image of the way that the client and his mother communi- cated their feelings of caring for one another. He has been informed by the client that his mother wasn't interested enough to show him that she loved him. The therapist de- cides to employ the referential

Page 113: THE STRUCTURE MAGIC I

Into the Vortex 1 1 31

(75) R: By doing things for her.

(76) T : Ralph, did your mother ever do things for you?

(77) R: Yes, but she never really . . . never let me know for sure.

index shift technique. Specifically, he makes the substitution

1 your mother you (the client) you (the client) 1 your mother

Thus, the portion of the client's last Surface Structure is transformed

your mother show you that she loved you

you show your mother that you loved her

Having made this shift in referential indices, the therapist asks the client to focus the image, asking for a more completely specified verb.

The client presents a further specifi- cation of the verb, setting up the equivalence

X loves Y = X do things fbr Y where X = the client and

Y = the client's mother

The therapist now shifts the refer- ential indices back to the original Surface Structure (73), and pre- sents one half of the equivalence for the client's verification.

The client agrees that his mother did do things for him, but he denies that the equivalence holds - that is,

X loves Y f X do things for Y where X = the client $ mother

Y - the client The client's new Surface Structure presents the therapist with the fol- lowing options: (a) ask for the dif- ference in the two situations which makes the equivalence fail to hold (identified by the cue word but); (b) there are two occurrences of the challengeable universal quantifier

Page 114: THE STRUCTURE MAGIC I

1 32 / Into the Vortex

never; (c) a deletion associated with the verb know (i.e., know what?); (d) a very incompletely specified verb know.

Never let you know what?

She never let me know for sure i f she really loved me (still sobbing softly).

Did you ever let her know for sure that you loved her?

She knew. . .

How do you know she knew?

l . . . l . . . l g u e s s / don't.

What prevents you from telling her?

The therapist chooses option (c) and asks for the deleted noun argu- ment associated with the verb know.

The client supplies the missing noun argument. His Surface Struc- ture includes (a) a challengeable universal quantifier never; (b) two very incompletely specified verbs know and love.

The therapist again chooses to use the referential index shift tech- nique. The substitution that he uses is the same as the one that he em- ployed in (74).

The client's Surface Structure con- tains (a) a deletion associated with the verb know; (b) a violation of the semantic well-formedness con- dition, mind-reading; (c) a very in- completely specified verb know.

The therapist chooses option (c).

The client wavers, and then admits that he is not able to specify the process by which his mother was supposed to have been able to know that he loved her. This is equivalent to stating that the proc- ess in his model is not specified.

The client has been unable to iden- tify the process by which his mother was supposed to have been

Page 115: THE STRUCTURE MAGIC I

Into the Vortex / 1 33

(85) R: ummm . . . ummm, maybe nothing.

(86) T: MA YBE?

(87) R: /guess 1 could.

(88) T: Ralph, do you guess you could also tell Janet how you feel about her?

(89) R: That's a little scary.

(90) T: What is a little scary?

(91 ) R: That 1 could just go up and tell her.

, able to know that he loved her. The therapist immediately moves to the technique of asking what is it that prevents the client from using the most direct way he knew of com- municating his feelings of love to his mother.

The client wavers, considering the obvious. His Surface Structure in- cludes a very qualified maybe and the universal quantifier nothing.

The therapist works to get more of a commitment from the client.

The client admits the possibility.

The therapist now shifts referential indices again

client's mother I Janet

and asks for a commitment from the client to change the communi- cation process in that relationship so that it is more direct and re- quires no mind-reading.

The client hesitates; his Surface Structure contains (a) a noun argu- ment without a referential index that; (b) a deletion of the noun argument associated with the verb scary (i.e., scary to whom?).

The therapist asks for the missing referential index.

The client supplies the missing in- dex and expresses doubt about the communication commitment that the therapist is asking for.

Page 116: THE STRUCTURE MAGIC I

134 / Into the Vortex

(92) T: What stops you?

(93) R: Nothing, that's what's so scary. (laughing)

The therapist uses the technique of asking' for the generalization, the outcome of the client's action which he finds scary.

The client recognizes that he has that choice.

The therapist at this point moved into non-Meta-model tech- niques, setting up a contract with Ralph to insure that the new possibilities which he had discovered would be acted upon.

TRANSCRIPT 2 This transcript session took place with a group of trainees who

were witnessing a demonstration. Beth is a woman of about 28. She has been married once and has two small children. The demonstration begins:

( 1 ) B: What should / do first?

Tell me what you are doing here; you said in the interview you wanted some help with something (referring to a two-minute interview an hour before in which five people were chosen for this demonstration).

(3) B: Let's see, what am / doing here . . . I . . . /want help with . . . well, it's my ro ommates.

The client begins by requesting di- rection from the therapist.

The therapist begins by asking the client to specify what she is doing here and, referring to a previous conversation, asks her to verify and explain her request for help.

The client sounds hesitant, some- l h a t confused; (a) she leaves a Sur- face Structure uncompleted - help with . . ., pauses, then states . . . it's my roommates. The verb help is very incompletely specified; (b) the nouns i t and roommates have no referential indices.

Page 117: THE STRUCTURE MAGIC I

Into the Vortex / 1 35

Roommates?. . .

(In terrup ting) Karen and Sue, they share the house with me. We also have four children between US.

What kind of help would you like with these two people?

They don't seem to understand me.

How do you know they don ' t understand you?

The therapist decides to ask for a referential index on the noun argu- ment roommates.

The client supplies referential in- dic'es as requested by the therapist. She adds more infomation, thus allowing the therapist a somewhat clearer image of her model.

The therapist makes the assumption that the noun argument roommates fits in the noun argument position of the sentence that the client left incomplete in her second comment. Presupposing this, the therapist re- turns to the client's original Surface Structure and asks the client to further specify the process word help.

The client ignores the therapist's specific question and begins to de- scribe her roommates. Notice that (a) the dative argument associated with the verb seem is missinglde- leted; (b) the client is claiming knowledge of the inner experience of others without specifying how she got that information - a well- formed-in-therapy violation called mind-reading; (c) the client's Sur- face Structure includes the very unspecified verb understand.

The therapist challenges the client's Surface Structure for violating the semantic well-formedness condition (mind-reading). He asks the client to describe how she came to know how they don't understand her.

Page 118: THE STRUCTURE MAGIC I

136 1 Into the Vortex

(9) B: I guess, it's that they're too busy. . . .

(1 0) T: Too busy for what?

(11) B: WeN ... toobusy to see that / have needs.

(1 2) T: What needs?

(1 3) B: That I would like for them to do something for me once in a while.

(1 4) T: Such as what?

(1 5) B: They really have a lot of things to do, but sometimes I feel that they are insensitive.

The client's response fails to be well formed in therapy as: (a) the noun argument i t has no referential index and, (b) the predicate too busy has a deletion associated with it (too busy for what?).

The therapist asks for the deleted portion of the client's last Surface Structure.

The client supplies the missing ma- terial in the form of a new Surface Structure. The new Surface Struc- ture includes a noun argument with no referential index (needs). This particular noun argument is a nomi- nalization from the Deep Structure predicate to need.

The therapist asks for the referen- tial index on the client's nominal- ization needs.

The client's new Surface Structure again lacks a referential index on what she wants from her room- mates (something in for them to do something). The verb do is nearly as incompletely specified as possible.

The therapist continues to ask for the missing referential index.

Again, the client fails to respond to the question from the therapisL3 Her new Surface Structure is in vio- lation of the well-formed-in-therapy conditions (a) missing referential index on . . . a lot of things . . . ; (b) missing referential index on some- times; (c) the almost completely unspecified verb do in . . . things to do . . . ; (d) a missing dative noun

Page 119: THE STRUCTURE MAGIC I

Into the Vortex / 137

(1 6) T: Whom are they insensitive to?

(1 7) B: Me. And. . .

(1 8) T: ln what way are they insensitive to you?

(1 9) B: You see, I do a lot o f things for them, but they don't seem to do anything for me.

(20) T: What don 't they do for you? What needs don't they see that you have?

argument associated with the verb insensitive ( e . insensitive to whom?); (e) by using the verb in- sensitive, the client is claiming knowledge of the inner state of another without specifying the process by which she knows - mind-reading.

The therapist asks for the missing noun argument associated with the verb insensitive [in Deep Structure, option (d) in above].

The client supplies the missing argu- ment and begins something else.

The therapist interrupts, choosing to ask the client to specify how she knows the others involved are in- sensitive to her - option (e).

Again the client fails to respond directly to the therapist's question. Her new Surface Structure violates the following well-formed-in- therapy conditions: (a) missing ref- erential index on a lot of things and anything; (b) the nearly completely unspecified verb do occurs twice in the client's Surface Structure; (c) a challengeable universal quantifier in anything; (d) a deleted dative noun argument associated with the verb seem -seem to whom?

The therapist asks for a couple of the missing referential indices on noun arguments that are floating around - the anything, from the client's Surface Structure (19) and the needs from the client's Surface Structure (11).

Page 120: THE STRUCTURE MAGIC I

138 / Into the Vortex

(21 ) B: I 'm a person, too, and they don't seem to recognize that.

(22) T: How don't they recognize that you're a person?

(23) B: They, both o f them, never do anything for me.

(24) T: They NEVER do ANYTHING for you?

The client continues to fail to re- spond to the therapist's question. The new Surface Structure contains (a) a presupposition carried by the word too at the end of the Surface Structure I 'm a person. The impli- cation is that someone else (uniden- tified) is a person - hence, no referential index; (b) a deleted dative noun argument associated with the verb seem - (seem to whom?); (c) the client is claiming knowledge of the inner state of another (. . . they don't seem to recognize . . .) without stating how she got this information; (d) a rela- tively incompletely specified verb recognize.

The therapist is trying to get an image clear to him of the client's model - he keeps returning to the specification of what the room- mates actually do - just as he did with (10)' (14)' (18)' (20)' and this request. The therapist challenges the ill-formedness of the relatively incompletely specified verb recognize.

The client responds to the therapist with a Surface Structure which can be challenged on the grounds of: (a) a universal quantifier - never, identifying a generalization; (b) a noun argument associated with the general verb do, lacking a referen- tial index - anything; (c) the nearly completely unspecified pro-verb5 do.

The therapist chooses to challenge the generalization. He does it by emphasizing (voice quality) the uni-

Page 121: THE STRUCTURE MAGIC I

Into the Vortex / 139

(25) B: No, not never, but 1 always do things for them whether they ask or not.

(26) T: Let me see if / understand at this point. If someone recognizes that you are a person, then they will always do things for you whether you ask or not?

(27) B: Well, maybe not always, . . .

(28) T: I'm a bit confused at this point; could you te l l me what

versa1 quantifiers in the client's original Surface Structure when feeding the sentence back to the client for verification or denial.

The therapist's challenge to the client's last generalization is suc- cessful (i.e., No, not never). She goes on to state a new generaliza- tion identified by: (a) the universal quantifier always; and containing (b) a noun argument without a ref- erential index - things, (c) the nearly completely unspecified verb do, (d) the deletion of two noun arguments associated with the verb ask (ask for/about what? and ask whom). Remember, the therapist is still trying to find out who is doing what specifically for whom - what the client means when she says that her roommates fail to recognize her as a person.

The therapist thinks that he has identified a generalization - specif- ically, an equivalence between

X not recognize Y = X do things for as a person Y whether Y

asks or not

He puts the generalization in the form of an equivalence generaliza- tion and asks the client to confirm or deny it.

The client balks at the generali- zation.

The therapist returns to attempting to find out what, specifically, the client's roommates do that the client represents as not recognizing

Page 122: THE STRUCTURE MAGIC I

146 1 Into the Vortex

(5 1 ) B: (pause) . . . (Puts her hands in her lap and her face in her hands). Mui. . .kannnt (mumbling).

: (Softly, but directly) Beth, do you ask when you want something?

( 5 3 ) B: lcan't

(54) T: What prevents you?

(55) B: ljustcan't,.. JUST CAN'T

(56) T: Beth, what would happen if you asked for something when you want it?

to one another what they want and need. He asks her specifically whether she asks them when she wants something.

The client is experiencing a strong emotion.

The therapist persists in his attempt to get a clear image of the process by which the client expresses her needs and wants. He repeats the question.

The client uses a modal operator of impossibility, leaving off the re- mainder of the sentence.

The therapist has now identified an important portion of the client's model. Here the client experiences no choice (53) and a great deal of pain (51). The therapist begins to challenge the limiting portion of the client's model by asking what, specifically, makes this impossi- bility for her impossible.

The client simply repeats that it is not possible for her to ask - she again indicates that she has strong emotions in this area of her model by her changing voice quality and volume.

The therapist continues to chal- lenge the impoverishing portion of the client's model. He shifts to another of the Meta-model tech- niques described under modal

Page 123: THE STRUCTURE MAGIC I

Into the Vortex / 147

operators, asking for an outcome.

(5 7) B: 1 can't because people will feel pushed around i f I ask for things from them.

(5 8) T: Do people ask for things from you?

(59) B: Yes.

(60) T: Do you always feel pushed

The client is willing to give the out- come. There are several violations of the well-formed-in-therapy con- ditions in her Surface Structure which may be challenged; (a) the modal operator can't; (b) the Cause-Effect relationship X because Y identified by the word because; (c) noun arguments with no refer- ential indices, people and things; (d) a crystal-ball mind-reading viola- tion . . . people will feel pushed; (e) a deletion noun argument associ- ated with the verb pushed around - pushed around by whom?

The therapist is going to challenge the necessity of the Cause-Effect relationship or generalization which the client has in her model. He begins by shifting referential indices

J I (the client) I people people I (the client)

Thus, the part of the generalization that the therapist is focusing on shifts

I ask for things from people. People ask for things from me.

Having made the shift, he presents the client with the result for verifi- cation or denial.

The client verifies that she has had the experience

The referential index shift which the therapist began in (58) con-

around? tinues as he uses the same shift

Page 124: THE STRUCTURE MAGIC I

148 / Into the Vortex

I I (the client) people people I (the client)

Thus, the other portion of the client's original generalization becomes

(61) B: No, notalways, but sometimes / do.

(62) T: Beth, are you aware that thirty minutes ago you came to me and asked i f / would work with you? You asked for something for yourself?

I People feel pushed around. . . I feel pushed around. . .

The therapist now presents this piece of the transformed original Surface Structure, challenging it by emphasizing the universality of the claim with his voice quality empha- sis on the universal quantifier always.

The client denies that the Cause- Effect relationship is necessary [op- tion (b) under (57)l. Her new Surface Structure can be challenged on (a) missing referential index on sometimes; (b) nearly completely specified verb do or under the assumption that the pro-verb do refers back to pushed around, then the missing noun argument pushed around by whom, and a relatively unspecified verb pushed around; (c) the cue word but.

Instead of pursuing any of the vio- lations of the well-formed-in- therapy conditions in the client's last Surface Structure, the therapist continues to challenge the Cause- Effect generalization [option (b) in (57)l. The therapist shifts the refer- ential indices of the original generalization. (See page 149)

The therapist has relativized the client's generalization to the on- going present in therapy. He calls

Page 125: THE STRUCTURE MAGIC I

Into the Vortex / 149

(63) B: (pause) Yesssss

(64) T: Did I feel pushed around?

(65) B: / don't think so.

(66) T: Then, could you imagine asking for something for yourself from one of your roommates and their not feeling pushed around?

1 You (the client) 1 people You (the client) me (the

therapist). The result is:

I You (the client) asked for some- thing from people.

You asked for something from me (the therapist).

her attention to this, an experience which contradicts the client's gener- alization. The therapist asks her to verify or deny this experience.

The client verifies her experience.

The therapist invites the client to check out the remainder of her original Cause-Effect relationship [option (b) in (57)] with an exer- cise in reading the therapist's mind.

The client avoids the mind-reading while checking out the remainder of her generalization.

The therapist has succeeded in getting the client to deny the gen- eralization in her model which is causing her dissatisfaction and pain (a) by shifting referential indices so that she recalls experiences she her- self has had where she didn't feel pushed around when other peopIe asked her for things, and (b) by connecting her generalization with her immediate experience in ther- apy. He now shifts referential in- dices again, this time back to the original difficulty the client has with her roommates. He first asks her if she can fantasize an excep- tion to her original generalization

Page 126: THE STRUCTURE MAGIC I

150 / Into the Vortex

with her roommates specifically.

(67) B: Yes, maybe.

(68) T: Would you like to try ?

(69) B: Yes, l would.

(70) T: And how will you know if your roommates feel pushed around?

(71) B: Both of them would probably tell me.

(72) T: Beth, do you tell people when you feel pushed around?

(73) B: Not exactly, but I let them know.

The client verifies this possibility.

The therapist moves to gain the client's commitment to an ex- ception to her original generaliza- tion in actual experience as well as fantasy.

The client indicates that she is willing to try an actual experiment with her roommates.

The therapist, having received the client's commitment, returns to the central part of his image of the client's model which he has not yet clarified for himself - the process by which the client and her room- mates let one another know what they each want and need - the same process he was trying to clarify in (8), (18), (22)' (30), (34), (36)' (40)' and (42).

The client supplies the information which clarifies the therapist's image of her model of how her room- mates communicate to her how they're feeling.

The therapist now goes after the other half of the communication process: how she lets them know how she is feeling, what she wants.

The client's Surface Structure in- cludes (a) a deletion of a noun argu- ment associated with the verb know; (b) a very poorly specified verb phrase let know; (c) the cue word but.

Page 127: THE STRUCTURE MAGIC I

Into the Vortex 1 1 51

(74) T: How do you let them know?

(75) B: I guess just by the way I act; they should be able to tell.

(76) T: How? Are they supposed to be able to read your mind again?

(77) B: Well, no.

(78) T: What stops you from telling them directly that you don't want to do something or that you feel pushed around?

(79) B: lcouldn'thurt their feelings.

The therapist, who is still trying to get a clear image of how the client communicates her feelings to her roommates, challenges the poorly specified verb phrase.

The new Surface Structure includes violations of the following well- formed-in-therapy conditions: (a) referential index missing the way; (b) a very incompletely specified verb act; (c) a very incompletely specified verb phrase be able to tell; (d) a deletion of one of the noun arguments associated with the verb tell (to tell what?); (e) the cue word should.

The therapist persists in demanding the specifics of the communication from the client to her roommates.

The client denies that her room- mates should be able to read her mind.

The therapist chooses to challenge the impoverished portion of the client's model again [option (b) in (57) 1.

The client responds with a Surface Structure which involves: (a) a modal operator of impossibility; (b) a very unspecified verb hurt; (c) a semantically ill-formed Cause- Effect, I cause them to feel hurt, relationship; (d) missing referential index on feelings.

Page 128: THE STRUCTURE MAGIC I

152 / Into the Vortex

(80) T: Does telling someone no, or that you feel pushed around, always hurt their feelings?

(81) B: Yes,nobodylikes to hear bad things.

(82) T: Beth, can you imagine that you would like to know i f your roommates feel pushed around by you so that you could be more sensitive to them?

(83) B: Yes.

(84) T: Then, could you also imagine your roommates wanting to know when you feel pushed around so that they could become more sensitive to you?

(85) B: ummmmmmm (pause) l guess

The therapist chooses to challenge the semantic ill-formedness of Cause-Effect relationship [option (c) in (79)], emphasizing the uni- versality by inserting the universal quantifier always.

The client verifies that the generali- zation is part of her model. In addi- tion, her Surface Structure has violations: (a) missing referential index on nobody; (b) missing refer- ential index on things; (c) a mind- reading violation, nobody likes; (d) a universal quantifier identifying a challengeable generalization - no- body - all people not; (e) a deletion associated with the Deep Structure predicate bad -bad for whom?

The therapist decides to continue to challenge the impoverishing gen- eralization in the client's model. He asks the client to imagine an experi- ence which contradicts the generali- zation she has in her model, or to verify or deny it.

The client verifies it.

The therapist now uses the same situation which the client has just verified; this time, however, he uses it with the referential index shift.

I roommates I I (the client) I (the client) roommates

The client hesitates, then verifies the fantasized situation. Her Sur-

Page 129: THE STRUCTURE MAGIC I

Into the Vortex / 1 53

you 're right.

(86) T: About what?

(87) B: If l let them know when I feel pushed around or want something, then maybe they would be more sensitive.

face Structure reply includes the deletion of a noun argument associ- ated with right, i.e., you're right about what?

The therapist asks for the deleted noun argument.

The client supplies the missing piece and acknowledges her under- standing of how breaking her own generalization could be a good experience for her and her room- mates.

The therapist at this point moved into some non-Meta-model techniques to give Beth a chance to integrate her new learnings and connect her new representations with her experience. This also allowed the therapist to see if there was anything else that interfered with Beth's communicating her needs to her room mates.

I n this chapter, we have presented two transcripts which show therapists using the Meta-model techniques and only these tech- niques in the therapeutic encounter. Even with these artificial restrictions, the power of the Meta-model techniques is apparent. The Meta-model provides the therapist with a rich set of choices at each point in the therapeutic exchange. The overall effect o f this results in an explicit direction or strategy for therapy - the enrichment and expansion o f the limiting portions o f the client's model. The Meta-model is not designed for use by itself, but rather as a tool to be integrated with the powerful techniques, verbal and non-verbal, available from the various forms of psychotherapy. We turn to this topic now.

FOOTNOTES FOR CHAPTER 5

1. This i s the same point that we have made before. Models, including the Meta-models we present here, are not claims about actually occurring events within the person, people and processes being modeled, but rather are explicit representations of the behavior of those things which allows one to see the rule-governed nature of the person, people, and processes being

Page 130: THE STRUCTURE MAGIC I

154 / Into the Vortex

modeled. Such models represent the portions of the process which are systematic. For example, in the Meta-model, there i s no representation for the distance between the client and the Tower of London at different points in the session - we doubt that the client's behavior is systematic in this way. Some models may have as part of their purpose the representation of the inferred internal events in the person, people and processes being modeled - these are called simulation models.

2. The word they, lacking referential indices in this sentence, may, in fact, refer back to the noun argument women in the previous Surface Structure. The noun argument women i tself, however, also lacks a referential index.

3. Experienced therapists will recognize patterns in the way a client responds or fails to respond to his or her context - in this case, specifically, the therapist. The client has failed consistently to respond to the therapist's questions. We are presently at work on an explicit model of therapeutic techniques for challenging these kinds o f patterns - see The Structure of Magic 11 (forthcoming).

4. The word that in the client's Surface Structure i s missing a referen- tial index - it may refer to the first clause I'm a person, too.

5. Linguists refer to the verb do as a pro-verb. It functions for verbs in a manner parallel to the word it for nouns, and is as devoid of specific meaning as the pronoun it.

6. The use o f the referential index shift has proven in our experience to be particularly appropriate when the client i s engaging in a great deal o f mind-reading - the appropriate use of these more advanced techniques based on the verbal exchange will form part of the subject matter for The Structure of Magic 11.

Page 131: THE STRUCTURE MAGIC I

Chapter 6

ON BECOMING A SORCERER'S APPRENTICE

The different forms of psychotherapy are all effective to some extent, although they look very different to most observers. The fact that these seemingly different approaches to the therapeutic encounter are all to some extent effective was a puzzle for some years. During these years both practitioners and theoreticians spent much energy and creativity arguing the necessary superiority of one form of psychotherapy over the others. In recent years, fortunately, this kind of debate has begun to disappear and psy- chotherapists from different schools have begun to show a lively interest in the methods and techniques of others. As Haley has com men ted, (Advanced Techniques of Hypnosis & Therapy, pp. 530-535)

In the last decade, the idea of exploring new methods has been adopted by many psychiatrists and has led to such innovations as behavior therapy, conditioning treatment, and marital and family therapy. We have seen the passing of an emphasis upon ritual and a move toward judging therapeutic procedures by results instead of conformity to a particular school. It has even become respectable now to work in different ways with different types of patients . . . (Haley quoting Erickson directly) . . . "One of the impor- tant things to remember about technique . . . is your willingness to learn this technique and that technique and then to recognize that you, as an individual personality, are quite different from any of your teachers who taught you a particular technique. You need to extract from the

Page 132: THE STRUCTURE MAGIC I

156 / The Sorcerer's Apprentice

various techniques the particular elements that allow you t o express yourself as a personality. The next most impor- tant thing about a technique i s your awareness o f the fact that every patient who comes in to you represents a different personality, a different attitude, a different back- ground o f experience. Your approach to h im must be in terms o f h im as a person with a particular frame o f reference for that day and the immediate situation."

People who come to us in therapy typically have pain in their lives and experience little or no choice in matters which they consider important. All therapies are confronted with the problem of responding adequately to such people. Responding adequately in this context means to us assisting in changing the client's experience in some way which enriches it. Rarely do therapies accomplish this by changing the world. Their approach, then, is typically to change the client's experience of the world. People do not operate directly on the world, but operate necessarily on the world through their perception or model o f the world. Therapies, then, characteristically operate to change the client's model of the world and consequently the client's behavior and experiences.

Certain therapists, coming from dramatically different- appearing forms of psychotherapy, have come to be recognized as particularly effective in assisting clients in changing their experi- ences. Their behavior in psychotherapy appears to be extremely systematic to us in that they have a set o f powerful techniques for directly challenging and expanding the client's model of the world. These techniques have been widely adopted by other therapists, but, unfortunately, without the dramatic results typical o f this first group. The difference here seems to us to be that the first group o f therapists have very clear intuitions about how to employ these techniques to challenge and expand the client's model. I n other words, these psychotherapists are able to identify when the use o f some particular technique is appropriate. The use o f these same techniques by others often leads to very uneven results; sometimes they will succeed dramatically, other times they appear to miss altogether; at times the use of these techniques appears to be appropriate, at other times not.

We have thus far in this book presented a Meta-model for use by therapists in their verbal exchanges in the therapeutic encounter. The Meta-model is a tool that is available to therapists from any school o f psychotherapy. I t s practicality is two fold: first, it offers explicit direction (i.e., step-by-step and, therefore, learnable) for what to do next at any point in the therapeutic

Page 133: THE STRUCTURE MAGIC I

The Sorcerer's Apprentice / 1 57

encounter, and second, anyone who is a native speaker of English already has the intuitions necessary to use the Meta-model and he only needs to become conscious of these intuitions.

As we have stated repeatedly, our Meta-model does not, by any means, exhaust the choices or possibilities of what a therapist might do in the therapeutic encounter. Rather, it is designed to be integrated with the techniques and methods in already established forms o f psychotherapy. The integration o f the explicit Meta- model with the techniques and methods o f therapy in which you are already skilled will not only expand the choices you have as a therapist, but it will increase the potency of your style of therapy by making the interventions you use directed

Page 134: THE STRUCTURE MAGIC I

162 1 The Sorcerer's Apprentice

How do you feel about his telling you he doesn't care about whether the house is clean or not?

I feel angry - in fact, damn mad . . . that's what we fight about all the time.

How do you feel about feeling angry?

How do I feel about feeling angry ?

Yes, how do you feel about feeling angry at Paul?

Well, I don't feel so good about it.

The therapist, using his knowledge that the client's reference structure must include her feelings about Paul's behavior as a necessary con- dition for being a well-formed-in- therapy reference structure, asks for that component.

The client supplies her feelings about Paul's behavior. Her new Sur- face Structure includes a universal quantifier (all) which identifies a generalization which the therapist may challenge.

The therapist ignores the violation of the well-formed-in-therapy con- dition concerning generalizations, and, instead, chooses to shift levels, asking the client about her feelings about her image of herself in her model of the world (her reference structure).

The client appears to be initially confused by the therapist's ques- tion requiring her to shift levels. This is a common reaction to such level shifts in our experience; clients, however, do have the re- sources to deal with this kind of maneuver.

The therapist repeats the question.

The client supplies her feelings about her feelings - her self- esteem.

The therapist begins to explore the client's model at this new level by asking her to specify her verb more fully. Changes at this level - the level of self-esteem - are extremely important, since a

Page 135: THE STRUCTURE MAGIC I

The Sorcerer's Apprentice 1 1 63

person's self-image affects the way a person organizes his entire experience or reference structure. Therefore, changes at this level of structure permeate the client's entire model of the world.

These particular categories and techniques o f Satir's offer a beginning to determine the set o f the minimum necessary compo- nents for completeness o f the well-formed-in-therapy reference structures. I n observing extremely effective therapists, such as Satir, we have identified other types o f categories which we offer as part o f the set o f minimum components which must be present for a reference structure to be well formed with respect to com- pleteness, another way of checking for completeness in the client's reference structures. These include:

(a) The way the client is representing his past experiences in the present - these are often in the form o f rules about his behavior;

(b) The way the client is representing his present experience in the present - that is, what the client is aware of now;

(c) The way the client is representing his possible future experiences in the present - that is, his expectations o f what he expects the outcome o f his behavior will be.

Notice that the four initial components presented by Satir (client's feelings, others' feelings, the context, client's feelings about his feelings) will occur as components of each of these three representations - the past, the present, and the future - as the client is representing them now. We have found these categories very useful in organizing our model and behavior in therapy in attempting to assist clients in developing complete reference struc- tures. As you will have noticed in the explicit techniques o f the Meta-model as presented in Chapters 3, 4, and 5 , the Meta-model includes techniques for recovering and challenging the categories of the reference structure outlined here. Rules, based on the client's experience as represented in the present, are another name for generalizations based on the client's experience, as are the client's expectations. I n each case, the client will present the material the therapist requests when challenging and enriching the client's model in the form o f Surface Structures which are subject to the well-formed-in-therapy conditions which the Meta-model specifies. The point o f presenting these categories is to offer some clear suggestions about what the necessary components of a com- plete, well-formed reference structure for the linguistic Deep Structure might be. Additional suggestions as to what the neces- sary components o f a complete reference structure might be have been offered by various philosophers (any of the well-known

Page 136: THE STRUCTURE MAGIC I

164 / The Sorcerer's Apprentice

western philosophers who dealt explicitly with epistomology - for example, i n the empiricist tradition, Locke, Berkeley, Hume, and in the idealist tradition, Kant, Hegel, Vaihinger, etc.) and seman- ticists, logicians, linguists (for example, Korzybski, Humboldt, Carnap, Tarski, Chomsky, Katz, etc.).

For the remainder o f this chapter we will select and discuss a number o f techniques from different forms o f psychotherapy. It is not our intention to teach these techniques here. Rather, in each case, we will show how the technique, as presently used, implicitly challenges the client's representation of the world, and how each o f these techniques may be integrated with the Meta-model. We have selected these particular techniques simply because we are familiar with them and know from our experience that they are powerful therapeutic tools. We would also like to state that we are by no means saying they are any more powerful than other techniques, or that they lend themselves more readily to being integrated with the Meta-model, but rather we wish to provide a cross-section o f techniques and chose from the ones we know.

Enactment: The Instant Re-Play of Experience By enactment we refer to those techniques that involve the

client in dramatizing an actual or fantasized experience. Enact- ment may involve only the client or it may involve other partici- pants as well.

By taking the word as an absolute, never investigating its personal significance, the word acquires a life o f its own. Reifying the word in this way removes it from its practical function as a more or less efficient way of referring to a process which remains alive and has continually changing referents. Enactment is one way o f keeping alive the words a person uses to characterize himself or someone else. Keeping h i s language connected to action permits feelings o f change and growth. . . .

(I. and M. Polster, Gestalt Therapy Integration, p. 00)

The solution (to the question o f what the set o f necessary components o f a complete reference structure is) is complex. Fortunately for psychotherapy, this solution is not required for therapy to proceed. One way o f avoiding this difficulty and at the same time gaining access to something closer to the client's refer- ence structure is to have the client present the experiences from which the full linguistic representation was der i~ed.~ For example, the client has difficulty expressing anger toward her husband. We

Page 137: THE STRUCTURE MAGIC I

The Sorcerer's Apprentice / 1 65

know this as she began by presenting a series of Surface Structures which we then subjected to the well-formed-in-therapy conditions, finally arriving at the ful l linguistic representation. A t this point, in order to determine what the reference structure from which this ful l linguistic representation was derived is, we may ask the client to enact a specific occasion on which she was unable to express her anger at her husband. I n addition to re-connecting the client's Deep Structures with a fuller approximation to their reference structures, the techniques o f enactment typically accomplish two other things:

1. The client, in re-creating his experience, becomes aware o f parts o f the reference structure or experience which had no representation in the Deep Structure;

2. Enactment gives the therapist access to two important things: (a) A close approximation to the reference structure itself

- the client's experience - and, therefore, provides the therapist with a wealth o f accurate material to use in the therapeutic encounter;

(b) The opportunity to see an example of modeling by the client directly. I n other words, through enactment, the therapist has available an approximate reference struc- ture. By comparing it with the client's verbal descrip- tion o f that experience, the therapist has an example o f the generalizations, deletions and distortions typical o f the client.

A number o f things occur when the client enacts his experi- ence. First, the client's present experience itself comes to chal- lenge and expand his model o f the world, as he experiences it in his enactment possibilities which had been previously deleted, and some o f the missing portions of the representation are recovered. Secondly, the portions o f the client's model which were vague and unfocused are clarified, as the enactment is a specific experience - equivalent t o the supplying o f referential indices by the client, in this case experientially rather than linguistically. The enactment is essentially a dramatization o f what the client has represented as an event - the enactment i tse l f denominalizes the representation; that is, it transforms the event back into a process, and, in this process, presents a much more fully specified image o f the process (equivalent to more fully specifying the verb in Meta-model tech- niques). These four aspects o f a typical enactment taken together result in an experience which lies in part outside the boundaries o f the client's initial linguistic representation. Since the enactment technique implicitly challenges the client's model by these four

Page 138: THE STRUCTURE MAGIC I

166 1 The Sorcerer's Apprentice

aspects, if the enactment technique is integrated with the Meta- model techniques the result is that the enactment technique i tse l f becomes more powerful and direct by explicitly challenging the client's linguistic representation.

In any therapeutic situation in which the technique o f enact- ment is fully integrated with the Meta-model, the therapist has an extremely rich set of choices. Common to all o f these is the suggestion that the therapist have the client describe his ongoing experience during the dramatization. This ongoing description, as well as any other verbal communications by the client to other participants in the enactment, will, o f course, be a series of Surface Structures. The therapist subjects these Surface Structures to the well-formed-in-therapy conditions by using Meta-model questioning. This insures that the material which the enactment technique makes available implicitly i s recovered in a completely explicit manner. The enactment technique is designed to make available a close approximation to the reference structure from which the impoverished portion of the client's linguistic represen- tation was derived. The richer approximation to reference struc- ture provided by enactment includes both verbal and analogical forms o f communication. I n addition to subjecting the client's reports o f the ongoing experience, and his communications to other participants, to the well-formed-in-therapy conditions, the therapist has available this fuller representation - the enactment experience itself which the therapist may use as an approximate reference structure to compare directly with the client's verbal description.

The therapist may wish to use some o f the necessary compo- nents o f a complete reference structure suggested previously. The therapist may, for example, insure by questioning that the client is representing his feelings about the enactment experience explicitly by asking directly for those feelings. Or, for example, the therapist may pay particularly close attention to whether the client explic- i t ly represents sensations gained through each o f the five senses - that is, the therapist may check to see whether the client looks at and sees clearly the actions o f the other participants in the dramatization, or the therapist may check to *see whether the client listens and hears clearly the things said by himself and by the other participants in the dramatization.

Guided Fantasy - A Journey into the Unknown By guided fantasy we refer to the process in which clients use

their imagination to create a new experience for themselves. Fantasy i s an expansive force in a person's life - it reaches

Page 139: THE STRUCTURE MAGIC I

The Sorcerer's Apprentice / 167

and stretches beyond the immediate people environment or event which may otherwise contain him. . . . Sometimes these extensions (fantasy) can gather such great force and poignancy that they achieve a presence which is more compelling than some real-life situations. . . . When these fantasies can emerge in the therapy experience, the re- newal o f energy may be vast, sometimes bordering on the unassimilable and often marking a new course in the indi- vidual's sense o f self.

(Polster & Polster, Gestalt Therapy Integrated, 1973, p. 255.)

The purpose o f guided fantasy is to create an experience for the client which, at least in part if not in i t s entirety, has not been previously represented in his model. Thus, guided fantasies are most appropriately used when the client's representation is too impoverished to offer an adequate number o f choices for coping in this area. Most typically, these are cases where the client is either in a situation or feels that he will be in a situation in which he hasn't sufficient representation in his model to respond in a way that he thinks is adequate. Often, the client experiences a great deal o f uncertainty and fear about the resolution o f these situa- tions. For example, a client feels blocked from expressing his feelings o f softness and tenderness toward his son. He has never expressed these feelings and is very apprehensive about what will happen if he does, although he has no clear idea o f what that happening might be. Here, we may choose to use a guided fantasy technique - having the client create by fantasy the experience which he both wants and fears. This experience will serve as a reference structure for the client, assisting him in overcoming his fear and ultimately giving him more choice in this area o f his l i fe. Guided fantasy, then, serves as a tool for the therapist in accom- plishing two things:

1. It provides the client with an experience which is the basis for a representation in his model where previously there had been either no representation or inadequate represen- tation. This provides him with a guide for future behavior and coping in this area;

2. It provides the therapist with an experience which the therapist can use to challenge the client's presently impov- erished model.

In addition to these gains for both the therapist and the client, a guided fantasy is an opportunity for the therapist to observe the client creating not only a new experience but also a representation

Page 140: THE STRUCTURE MAGIC I

168 / The Sorcerer's Apprentice

of that experience. Here, the therapist sees in the creation o f this new fantasy experience the universal modeling processes o f Gener- alization, Deletion and Distortion as they are typically employed by the client. The employment of the guided fantasy experience is parallel to the Meta-model technique of recovery o f large-scale deletions under the category o f modal operators. This technique differs from the process o f enactment in that enactment recovers and brings into the present experience of the client something quite close to a reference structure from the client's past, while guided fantasy creates a reference structure for the client in the present.

Since guided fantasy is the creation o f a reference structure, tne therapist may wish to use the necessary components o f a complete reference structure suggested previously in guiding the client's fantasy. Specifically, for example, the therapist may, by questioning, direct the client to report on his feelings at different points in the fantasy, or direct the client's attention to one or more o f the five senses to insure a complete reference structure emerges in the client's fantasy.

We have found, in our experience, that guided fantasies often take the form o f a metaphor rather than a direct representation o f the "problem" that the client first identifies. For example, a client comes to a therapy session complaining that she is unable to get angry at someone with whom she works. Using the Meta-model techniques, we discover that the client also feels unable to express anger at her father and husband, and, in fact, she is unable to identify anyone at whom she feels she could express anger. There are a number o f techniques available in the Meta-model to chal- lenge this generalization; however, guided fantasy is particularly appropriate for situations in which the client has little or no representations in his model for such experiences. If, through the technique o f guided fantasy, the client succeeds in expressing anger at someone in his fantasy ( i t doesn't matter whom), then he will have created a new reference structure which contradicts the generalization in his model. Often, once the client has successfully generated reference structures which contradict the generalization in his model, the generalization disappears, and the problems that were a result o f the generalization also disappear or are reduced.

For example, once a young woman came into a seminar in which Meta-model techniques were being taught. Before the semi- nar began, she burst out into a frantic episode in which she claimed she was terrified that she was going crazy. Using Meta- model techniques, the teacher was able to determine that she felt she was losing control and did not know what was happening to

Page 141: THE STRUCTURE MAGIC I

The Sorcerer's Apprentice / 1 69

her; her l i fe was in turmoil, her future a frightening and dismal unknown. The teacher of the seminar asked her to close her eyes and te l l him what she saw. After some initial difficulty, she proceeded to describe herself as standing on the edge of a large crevasse which was steep and foreboding. The teacher told her to slowly proceed into the crevasse and explore it, asking her to continually report on what she experienced, giving details of sight, hearing, feeling, smelling, and constantly reassuring her she could proceed through each obstacle. She finally proceeded down and back up, remarking, when she arrived at the top again, that it was still a gloomy day but that somehow she f e l t better. When she opened her eyes, her fear was gone and she fe l t that she could survive all that faced her. This experience offered a new reference structure in which this young woman was able to face an unknown experience; this new reference structure also expanded her model in such a way that it allowed her to believe that somehow she would survive whatever was happening to her in her life.

By the solution or resolution of a "problem" by metaphor in guided fantasy, we refer to a situation in which the client uses guided fantasy to create a new reference structure or experience in which he achieves that which was formerly not possible. Once the new situation - the one created in the fantasy - is successfully resolved, the "problem" which the client originally had either disappears or at least becomes less formidable, and, typically, the client feels able to cope with it. The created "problem" and the original "problemJ' must share a similarity of structure - they must both be "problems" relating to the same impoverishing generalization in the client's model of the world.4

Once a therapist has succeeded in developing a guided fantasy with his client, this fantasy, itself, i s an experience available for the enactment process.

Therapeutic Double Binds By therapeutic double binds we mean situations, imposed

upon the client by the therapist, in which any response by the client will be an experience, or reference structure, which l ies outside the client's model of the world. Thus, therapeutic double binds implicitly challenge the client's model by forcing him into an experience which contradicts the impoverishing limitations of his model. This experience then comes to serve as a reference structure which expands the client's model of the world. In the Meta-model, when the therapist uncovers an impoverishing gener- alization in the client's model, particularly one which involves a Cause-Effect, semantically ill-formed violation and/or a modal oper-

Page 142: THE STRUCTURE MAGIC I

1 70 / The Sorcerer's Apprentice

ator, the therapist may challenge this generalization by asking the client whether this generalization is necessarily or always true (see, Techniques for Challenging Generalizations, Chapter 4), to iden- t i fy and dramatize an experience which contradicts this generaliza- tion (enactment), or, in a case in which the client does not have such an experience available, the therapist may ask the client to create an experience which contradicts his generalization (through the technique o f guided fantasy). If these three techniques fail to produce the contradictory experience, or if the therapist is so inclined, he may choose to create a double-bind situation in which the client's response is an experience which contradicts the client's impoverishing generalization.

During one therapeutic session, in the course o f using Meta- model techniques with a group, the therapist assisted the client in arriving at the generalization which was true in her model; namely, "I can't say NO to anyone because I can't hurt anyone's feelings." I n this particular case, the therapist chose to use the Meta-model technique of asking what, specifically, would happen if the client were to say NO to someone. Her reply was that they would be badly hurt, that they might even die. Noticing the lack o f a referential index o f the noun argument anyone, the therapist decided to ask who, specifically, might be hurt and die. The client, now greatly agitated, recounted a traumatic experience from her childhood when she had said NO to her father's request to stay at home with him. Upon returning home later that same evening, the client discovered her father had died, and she had taken the responsibility for his death, attributing it to her having said NO to him.

The therapist now moved into an enactment technique, asking the client to recreate the situation described with her father. Even after the enactment technique showed that the original experience from which the client had made the generalization was one in which she had had no choice about whether she would stay with her father or not, she adamantly refused to give up her generaliza- tion. Here, although the enactment technique proved useful in recovering the traumatic experience, providing material which challenged certain other generalizations in the client's model, it did not, in itself, contradict the client's generalization about the consequences o f saying NO to someone. I n this case, note that the recovery and enactment o f the original experience from which the client made a generalization did not contradict the generalization; it simply identified the source of the generalization. Thus, after the enactment, the client's model was st i l l impoverished in this area - she still could not imagine saying NO to someone without

Page 143: THE STRUCTURE MAGIC I

The Sorcerer's Apprentice / 1 7 1

there being unacceptable consequences. The therapist in this case next chose to use a therapeutic double-bind technique. What the therapist did was to tell the client to go around the room to each of the people in the group and say NO about something to each. The client reacted strongly, refusing to perform the task, making further statements such as

NO! it's impossible for me to say NO to people! You can't expect me to do it just because you ask me to.

The client continued in this way for several minutes, refusing to carry out the task set for her by the therapist, until the therapist pointed out that she had, in fact, been saying NO to the therapist during this time! The therapist then pointed out that he had not been hurt and certainly had not died, contrary to her generaliza- tion. This experience was so powerful for the client that she was immediately able to move around the room and say NO t o the other members o f the group.

Consider the position in which the therapist placed the client by demanding that she say NO to the members of the group:

1. The client had stated her generalization / can ' t say N 0 to anyone. . . .

2. The therapist structured a therapeutic double bind with the demand that the patient

Say NO to each of the people in this group. 3. Notice the choices available to the client; she may

(a) Say NO to each member o f the group, or

(b) Say NO to the therapist. 4. Whichever choice the client makes, she generates an experi-

ence which contradicts her original generalization. This experience serves the client as a reference structure to guide her in representing her world in richer terms.

The therapist makes the contradictory nature o f the new exper- ience explicit by pointing out (using the Meta-model technique) that the Cause-Effect relationship which the client's generalization claimed was necessarily true failed to be true in this experience.

One o f the ways in which we have found therapeutic double binds particularly useful is in the area referred to by many thera- pies as homework. By homework we mean contracts which we make with the clients in which they agree to perform certain actions between therapeutic sessions. In the area o f therapeutic double binds in homework, a client in a therapy session uncovered the generalization that

/ can't try anything new because / might fail. When the therapist, using Meta-model techniques, asked what

Page 144: THE STRUCTURE MAGIC I

172 / The Sorcerer's Apprentice

would happen if she did try something new and failed, she replied that she wasn't sure, but that it would be very bad. She expressed a great deal of fear of the consequences of failing at something new and again stated that it was impossible, therefore, for her to try something new. At this point, the therapist decided to impose a therapeutic double bind and use the time between sessions for carrying out this bind. He made a contract with her that she would, each day between this session and the next, try something new and fail at it. Again, notice the structure of the situation created by this demand by the therapist of the client:

1. The client has the generalization in her model 1 can't fail at anything new;

2. The therapist structures a double bind with the contract Each day, between this session and the next, you will try something new and fail at it;

3. Notice the choices available to the client: (a) She can try something new each day between this

session and the next and fail at it, thus fulfilling the contract,

or (b) She can fail to fulfill the contract, i tsel f a new

experience; 4. Whichever situation occurs, the client will have an experi-

ence which will contradict her generalization and give her a reference structure which increases the amount of her choices available in the world as represented in her model.

We are not suggesting that double binds constitute the only kind of homework, but rather that homework can consist of a double bind, and, further, that generalizations can be challenged by experiences extending after the interview or session itself. It is necessary only that these experiences create some new reference structure that contradicts the impoverishing portions of the client's model.

We would also like to state at this point that homework assignments also are useful for giving clients a direct chance to try out any new dimensions created in their models in the course of a therapeutic session.

Other Maps for the Same Territory Human beings represent their experiences with systems other

than language. The most basic distinction which has been offered as a way to understand the different maps that we, as humans, develop to guide ourselves in the world is the one between digital and analogical representational systems (see Bateson, 1973;

Page 145: THE STRUCTURE MAGIC I

The Sorcerer's Apprentice 1 1 73

Wilden, 1973, for example). The best known digital representa- tional system is the one which is the focus of our Meta-model - the natural language system. The most commonly referred to example of an analogical representational system is body expres- sion. There are a number of therapies which deal primarily with these body or analogical representational systems. For example, therapies such as Rolfing, Bio-energetics, etc. challenge and expand the client's model by operating directly upon the client's analogical representation of the world of his experience. One point at which these two types of representational systems come to- gether is in the use of voice quality - an analogical system - which is used to carry and express the primary digital system, natural language. One frequently cited example of a mixed system is that of dreams, wherein both digital and analogical representa- tions are present.

For the purpose of therapy, it is essential for the therapist to understand that the full linguistic representation - the set of Deep Structures - is, itself, a derived model or representation of the world. Beyond the full linguistic representation is what we have referred to as the reference structure - that person's most com- plete representational system, the stored experiences that consti- tute that person's life history. This most complete model - the person's l i f e experiences - is the reference structure not only for the set o f Deep Structures which are the basis of the'primary digital representational system, but also for those experiences which serve as the reference structures for the other human representational systems, analogical as well as digital.

One of the most powerful skills which we exercise as commu- nicators and therapists is our ability to represent and communicate our experiences in any of the representational systems which we have available as humans. Further, experienced therapists will recognize the power of assisting clients in shifting their'representa- tional systems. For example, a client states that she has a severe headache. This is equivalent to the client's informing the therapist that she has represented some specific experience kinesthetically in a way which is causing her pain. One very powerful choice which the therapist has is to have her shift representational systems. Specifically, assuming that the therapist has already iden- tified that the client has a highly developed ability to represent her experiences visually, the therapist tel ls the client to close her eyes and describe the specifics of the headache, at the same time forming a clearly focused image of the headache. There are varia- tions of this which the therapist may employ to assist the client in achieving a visual representation. For example, he may have the

Page 146: THE STRUCTURE MAGIC I

174 / The Sorcerer's Apprentice

client breathe deeply and, once a rhythm o f breathing has been established, have the client exhale the headache forcefully onto a chair in front o f her, creating a visual image there. The outcome o f this shift o f representational system is assisting the client in representing her experience in a representational system in which she will not cause herself pain. The power of the technique o f shifting the client's experiences from one representational system to another can hardly be overestimated. In Volume II o f The Structure o f Magic, we present an explicit model for the identifica- tion and utilization o f the client's most frequently employed representational system.

Congruity Different portions of a person's reference structure can be

expressed by different representational systems. These may occur simultaneously. There are two logical possibilities when two dis- tinct representational systems are expressing different portions o f the person's reference structure simultaneously.

First, the portion of the person's reference structure which one representational system is expressing f i t s with the portion of the person's reference structure which the other representational system is expressing. We refer to this situation as a consistent double message, or congruity or congruent communication by the person involved.

Secondly, the portion of the reference structure which one representational system is expressing does not fit with the portion of the reference structure which the other representational system is expressing. We refer to this situation as an inconsistent double message, incongruity or incongruent communication. For example, if, in a therapeutic session, the client is sitting calmly in a chair and speaking with a quiet, controlled voice, and states

/ am really furious - God damn it, I 'm no t going to stand for this.

we have a classic example o f an inconsistent double message or incongruent communication. The digital system (language) and an analogical system (body and voice quality) do not match.

One o f the most impoverishing situations which we have encountered in therapy is the situation wherein a person maintains contradictory portions o f his reference structure. Typically, these contradictory portions have the form o f two contradictory gener- alizations which apply to the same area o f behavior. Most fre- quently, the person whose reference structure includes these inconsistent generalizations has the experience o f being immobi- lized, being profoundly confused, or oscillating between two in-

Page 147: THE STRUCTURE MAGIC I

180 / Final Incantation of Book /

Surface Structures - is derived from their full linguistic represen- tation, the Deep Structures. The transformational Meta-model describes these concepts and mechanisms explicitly; these are specific cases of the general modeling processes of Generalization, Distortion and Deletion.

Adapting the concepts and mechanisms of the transforma- tional model of the human representational system of language for the purposes of therapy, we developed a formal Meta-model for therapy. The Meta-model is formal because:

(a) It is explicit; that is, it describes the process of therapy in a step-by-step manner, guaranteeing that the Meta-model is learnable. This results in an explicit strategy for therapy.

(b) It is independent of content, dealing with the form of the process, and, therefore, has universal applicability.

The Meta-model relies only upon the intuitions which every native speaker has of his language. The overall implication of the Meta- model for therapy is the notion of well formed in therapy. This is a set of conditions which must be met by the Surface Structures which the client uses in therapy in order for these structures to be acceptable. Using this appropriate grammar for therapy, we, as therapists, can assist our clients in expanding the portions of their representations which impoverish and limit them. This results in enriching their lives in such a way that they experience more options in their behavior, more opportunities to experience the joys and richness that l i f e has to offer. When integrated with the people-helper skills which you already have available to you as a therapist, this process of growth and change is profoundly ampli- fied. This language of growth is then truly THE STRUCTURE OF MAGIC.

We are delighted to point out not only that the last incanta- tion for growth and potential is that you yourself can use this language of growth to enrich the skills you have as a people-helper, but also that you can use this language of growth to enrich your own l i fe and your own potential as a human being.

To be continued in The Structure o f Magic /I.

Page 148: THE STRUCTURE MAGIC I

Table of Contents for THE STRUCTURE OF MAGIC I1

By john Grinder and Richard Bandler

PART I. Representational Systems - Other Maps for the Same Territory

1. The Structure of the Choice o f Choice: Possible Analogue Maps.

2. The Meta-Map o f Maps: Identifying the Most Fre- quently Chosen System o f Representation

3. Meta - So What: Change within a Representational System

4. Meta-Tactics I: Changing Representational Systems

PART II. Incongruity

1. Incongruency: Expanding the Double-Bind Theory 2. Contradictions: A Vice and a Tool 3. Meta - So What: Utilization o f Polarity Splits 4. Meta-Tactics I I

PART Ill. Fuzzy Functions: The Neurology of Choice

PART IV. Formal Notation

1. Vocabulary 2. Axioms 3. Transformations/Rules of Derivation 4. Generative Formulae

Page 149: THE STRUCTURE MAGIC I

182 / Magic I1

PART V. Family Therapy - The Delicate Flower

1. What is US: The Family as System 2. What can be US 3. A New US 4. Meta-Tactics

APPENDIX I. Examples of Utilization of Formal Notation

APPENDIX II. Future Plans

Page 150: THE STRUCTURE MAGIC I

Appendix A

A BRIEF OUTLINE OF

TRANSFBRMATIONAL GRAMMAR

What we want to do in this appendix is to present a basic sketch o f the structure of human language systems. This sketch is drawn from a formal theory o f language known as transforma- tional grammar and constitutes only the briefest outline of that theory.'

The theory o f transformational grammar was developed to explicitly describe patterning in human language systems. You and I, as human beings, have consistent intuitions about the structure of our language and about i t s transformational grammar as a formal representation o f those intuitions. For example, native speakers o f English agree that the sequence o f English words in (A) forms a sentence o f their language while the sequence of words in (B) does not:

(A) Hans' mother called Sigmund up. (B) Called mother Sigmund Hans up.

Furthermore, our intuitions are that the words Hans and mother go together in some way that the words mother and called do not. Again, when given sentence (C), a native speaker will recognize it as having a special relationship to (A).

(C) Hans' mother called up Sigmund. which he will describe as saying the same thing or having the same meaning. Finally, a native speaker o f English will identify (D) as a member o f a special se t of sentences

( D ) Murdering peasants can be dangerous. which constitutes the set of ambiguous sentences in English. These different classes of intuitions that you and I have, as native

Page 151: THE STRUCTURE MAGIC I

184 / Appendix A

speakers of a natural language, can be described as: lntuitions which allow me to consistently decide which sequences of words in my language constitute sentences (that is, well-formed sequences) of my language. We will refer to this as well-formedness. lntuitions which allow me consistently to decide which words in a sentence go together to form a higher level unit or constituent. We will refer to this as constituent structure. lntuitions which allow me consistently to decide which sentences have which kind of logical/semantic relations, relations such as, Which sentences of different structure or form have the same meaning? I will refer to this as syn- onymy. Relations such as, Which sentences have more than one meaning? we will refer to as ambiguity.

The grammar of a natural language is intended to represent these three classes of intuitions. The central data that a transfor- mational grammar is designed to present in a systematic way are the intuitions native speakers such as you and I have about the structure of our language. By consistently decide we mean both that when we are presented with the same sentence at any two points in time our intuitions about i t s structure will be constant and also that other native speakers will have the same intuitions about the structure of that sentence. This behavior that we, as native speakers, exhibit is rule-governed behavior. That is to say that, although we may not be conscious of or able to articulate the rules that we use when we make intuitional judgments about the structure of our language, our behavior can be described by some set o f explicit rules. Linguists construct grammars by developing these systems of rules. One of the things which such systems specify is which sequences of words in the language are well formed, that is, are sentences. This characteristic of rule systems addresses the first question, the membership question. In what follows, we distinguish between the components of the system and the mechanics of the components of that system. The major components of the system and the system i tse l f do not involve concepts that are particularly difficult. We want to caution the reader not to become bogged down in the mechanics of the system, and for this reason we have separated them from the system proper.

Page 152: THE STRUCTURE MAGIC I

Appendix A / 185

WELL-FORMEDNESS AND CONSTITUENT STRUCTURE

One way of thinking about how grammars work, with respect to well-formedness, is to imagine the situation in which we have a large basket full of small slips of paper. Each slip of paper has a word of the English language written on it. Our friend, Atiko, is with me. Atiko is a member of i tribe called the Dasenetsch of South East Ethiopia. He does not speak or understand English. He draws out ten slips of paper at a time, arranging them from le f t to right in front of him in the order that he drew them from the basket. Now his task is to decide whether each sequence of ten words constitutes a well-formed sequence of English. We are able to assist him only by supplying him with a grammar or system of rules which he can use to decide whether the sequence is, in fact, well formed. Considered from this point of view, a grammar is a decision procedure which partitions the set of all possible se- quences of English words into a set of well-formed sequences and a set of ill-formed sequences. Since Atiko does not know the English language, the rules must be explicit; the process that he uses cannot rely upon his intuitions to make judgments on any of the sequences. Further, if the system of rules constitutes an adequate grammar (with respect to well-formedness), then each member of the well-formed set will be judged well formed by native speakers of English and no member of the other set will be identified as well formed by native speakers. We will present the kind of rule systems used by transformational linguists shortly. These rule systems will be more intelligible if we first discuss constituent structure. Consider sentence (1 ) below.

(1 ) Dick admitted Spiro had contacted the boys at I TT. Sentence (1) is judged by me, by you, and by all speakers of English to be well formed. Now, ask yourself whether you cag detect any internal structure to the sentence. For example, do you find that the words the and boys go together in some intuitive way that the words boys and at do not? Or, again, do the words had and contacted go together in some way that contacted and the do not? For native speakers of English, the answer is yes for both of the questions. We can continue through the sentence, using our intuitions about the internal structure of the sentence to decide how to group the individual words in the sentence into higher level, multiple-word units. After we complete this first run through the sentence, we can begin again, this time grouping the initial groupings or constituents into higher level constituents. For example, the constituents had contacted and the boys go together in some way that Spiro and had contacted do not. This procedure

Page 153: THE STRUCTURE MAGIC I

186 1 Appendix A

i s iterative. The intuitions o f native speakers o f English, like you and me, about the constituent structure of their language are consistent. To repeat, by consistent we mean that, given the same sentence now and again in ten years, our judgments about i t s internal structure will be constant. Furthermore, our judgments will match those of other native speakers o f the language. Within the theory o f transformational grammar, these kinds of intuitions are represented by what are called tree structures. There i s a simple procedure for going from our intuitions to tree representa- tion: words that go together in my intuitive groupings are domi- nated by (attached to) the same tree node. The and boys go together according to our intuitions about the initial groupings; therefore, the tree representation will include the structure

the boys

In actual tree representations, the nodes (here represented by 0's) carry labels which identify their parts o f speech, such as S for Sentence, NP for Noun Phrase, VP for Verb Phrase, N for Noun, V for Verb, Det for Determiner, PP for Prepositional Phrase, Prep for Preposition, etc. The actual representation for the constituent the boys looks like

h. the h boys

The tree (2) represents our intuitions about the internal structure of sentence (1): (See page 187)

Now, knowing the procedure for mapping onto tree represen- tations from intuitions about grouping or constituent structure, you can read through the tree structures and see whether your intuitions match ours. For example, the words had contacted the boys at ITT form a constituent (VP), but not Spiro and had contacted. This is reflected in the tree structure by the fact that the first sequence is exhaustively dominated (by exhaustively, we mean that the node that dominates these words dominates these words and no others) by a single node, but there is no single node

Page 154: THE STRUCTURE MAGIC I

Appendix A 1 187

A 4 A Dick 7s, I

Dit 6 A /PP /P,

$ Spiro had contacted Det N Prep NP

DA the boy'^ A at D t / \ A $ ITT

which exhaustively dominates the words Spiro and had contacted. We pointed out earlier that grammars are systems o f rules. What, then, does the system of rules which specifies the tree structure (2 ) look like? In order to make the answer to this question more intelligible to you, we want to take a brief excursion into formal or logical systems.

Formal Systems Formal systems are composed of three component^:^

a vocabulary a set o f axioms a set o f rules o f formation or derivation.

The more important concepts (for our purposes here) of formal systems can be illustrated by an extremely simple system - call it SIMPLE. (See page 1 88)

Page 155: THE STRUCTURE MAGIC I

188 1 Appendix A

System SIMPLE vocabulary: _L , L, 5 set of axioms: 1 . rules of formation or derivation:

(a) *-I * ( (b) *-G

(The symbol 6 represents the empty sequence.) The symbol - means that the material which appears on the left-hand side of it may be replaced by (be re-written as) the material on the right-hand side o f the symbol. Now, let's turn SIMPLE on and watch the way it operates. The Meta-rule (a rule about rules) for formal systems o f this class specifies that we must justify each statement that we make in the system. There are two possible justifications: either what we write down is an axiom o f the system or it is a substitution specified by the rules o f deriva- tion from the line which we have just written. To begin, since there are no existing lines, the first line must be the axiom o f the system

line justification * axiom o f the system

Now, we examine the line which we have just written and deter- mine whether any o f the symbols written there are on the left- hand side of the rules of derivation. The symbol * is the only candidate, and, in fact, appears on the left hand of both o f the rules of derivation for SIMPLE. We then choose one o f the rules and write the next line

line *

) * (

justification

axiom o f the system by rule o f derivation (a)

We now repeat the procedure, scanning the last line and comparing the symbols there with the symbols which appear on the left hand of the rewrite arrows. Within this system, as long as we continue to choose rule o f derivation (a), the procedure will ~ o n t i n u e . ~

Suppose we choose the rule (a) twice more (See page 189) When we examine the bottom line o f the sequence, we find no symbols which occur on the left hand o f a re-write arrow. The

Page 156: THE STRUCTURE MAGIC I

Appendix A / 189

line justification

axiom of the system by ru l e of derivation (a) by rule of derivation (a) by ru le of derivation (a)

What happens now if we choose rule of derivation (b)?

line justification

axiom of the system by ru l e of derivation (a) by rule of derivation (a) by ru l e of derivation (a) by rule of derivation (b)

procedure now terminates. The results of the procedure, the collection of the lines top to bottom, is called the derivation. The final line o f any such derivation is called a theorem of the system and is said to have been proven in the system. Finally, a sequence in the vocabulary of a system is said to be well formed if it is a theorem of that system. Looking at the system from an overall point of view, we can see that a sequence in the vocabulary of that system is well formed with respect to that system, just in case there is a derivation proceeding from an axiom of the system by means of the rules of derivation to a sequence which contains no symbol which occurs on the left-hand side of one of the rules of derivation for that system, a theorem. If we collect all of the theorems of a system, we have the set of well-formed sequences in the vocabulary of the system.

Now, we want to explicitly draw the parallelism between the system SIMPLE and natural language systems. The first task that we have when functioning as a linguist is to specify the set of well-formed sequences in the vocabulary of the natural language system for which we are attempting to construct a grammar. Using SIMPLE as a model, then, if we were able to specify a system of rules which gave as theorems for all the sequences of words in that language which native speakers judged to be well formed, then we would have succeeded in answering the membership question.

Some Mechanics of the Membership and Constituent Structure Issues

Let's see what a system of rules for natural language might be.

Page 157: THE STRUCTURE MAGIC I

190 1 Appendix A

System DEEP Vocabulary: S (Sentence), NP (Noun Phrase), VP (Verb

Phrase), N (Noun), De t (Determiner), V (Verb), PP (Prepositional Phrase), Prep (Preposition)

Axiom: S Rules of derivation:

(a) S-NP VP

( c ) VP-V (NP) (PP)

( d ) PP-Prep NP

where symbols within parenthesis may be omitted and symbols within brackets represent a disjunctive choice, i.e., choose either one line of symbols or the other but not both.

The Meta-rule for this system is the same as that mentioned for SIMPLE - each line of the derivation must either be an axiom or must be derivable from the previous line by a rule of derivation. Applying the procedure we used for SIMPLE, we have

line

S

NP VP

Det N V P

Det N V NP

Det N V S

Det N V NP VP

Det N V D e t N V P

Det N V Det N V NP PP

Det N V Det N V Det N PP

Det N V Det N V Det N Prep

Det N V Det N V Det N Prep

justification

axiom of the system

by rule of derivation a

by rule of derivation b

by rule of derivation c

by rule of derivation b

by rule of derivation a

by rule of derivation b

by rule of derivation c

by rule of derivation b

NP by rule of derivation d

Det N by rule of derivation b

Page 158: THE STRUCTURE MAGIC I

Appendix A 1 1 91

it is not difficult to map from derivations to tree representa- tion; return to the first line o f the derivation and begin reading down the derivation line by line. In each line, one rule of deriva- tiorr was applied to replace one symbol by some other symbol (a). The rest o f the symbols in the line have simply been carried down or re-copied from the line immediately above. These symbols carry no new information and are, therefore, redundant. We remove the redundancy by erasing or leaving out all of the symbols in each successive line o f the derivation which are not affected by the rule of derivation which was applied, If we perform this operation for this first few lines of derivation, we have the figure

Now, we return to the first line of the derivation, and as we read down, we connect the symbol which was replaced in the upper line o f each adjacent pair of lines with the symbol(s) which replaced it in the lower line of the pair. The results for the first few lines look like this:

Page 159: THE STRUCTURE MAGIC I

192 1 Appendix A

When we carry out these two procedures for the entire deriva- tion, we have the tree representation

(3) /\ /Np\ /vp\

De t N V NP I

De t N V NP- / \ /"\

Det N Prep NP /\

Det N

This tree structure is identical to tree structure ( 2 ) which we discussed earlier except that the words o f English attached to the lowest nodes in tree (2) are missing from this tree. To apply these, we need a lexicon (or enlarged dictionary). This lexicon gives all o f the words o f English with certain additional information. For example, verbs are listed in this lexicon showing in what kind o f tree structure they can be placed. The verb admit may fit into a tree structure under a V node if that V node is followed by an NP node,' as in tree structure (Z), but it cannot be placed in a tree structure under a V node if nothing follows that V node, as in

/"\ /Np\

VP

Det N I v

n 0

L people

L laughed

This kind o f information listed in the lexicon prevents ill-formed sequences such as6

*People admit *Dick laughed Spiro had contacted the boys at /TT

For nouns, the lexicon gives information showing with what kinds of verbs the noun may be used. This information prevents ill-formed sequences such as7

Page 160: THE STRUCTURE MAGIC I

Appendix A / 1 93

* The wall laughed *The wall admitted Spiro had contacted the boys at /TT

In general, then, the lexicon contains sufficient information to capture the dependencies between verbs and their accompanying noun phrases. Given the lexicon, we now need only a rule of substitution which checks the information in the lexicon against the tree structure and places the word involved under the lowest node if there is no conflict between the information in the lexicon and the structure of the tree. I f we carry out this substitution operation for tree (3), one of the resulting trees will be tree (2)) repeated here for convenience.

Dick admit-

ted/ s\

the boys at Det N

! A ITT

What, then, does the system DEEP do for us? First, DEEP represents intuitions about the constituent structure. How? Ex- amine the rules of derivation for DEEP. Take rule (d), for example.

PP -Prep NP In addition to being interpreted simply as a rule of derivation, rule

Page 161: THE STRUCTURE MAGIC I

194 1 Appendix A

(d) can be interpreted as a rule o f constituent structure; it makes the general claim that prepositional phrases (in English) are com- posed o f a preposition followed by a noun phrase. More generally, each of the rules o f derivation specifies that the symbols which appear on the right-hand side o f the re-write arrow are the constit- uents which are exhaustively dominated by, and therefore replace- ments for, the symbol which appears on the left-hand side o f the arrow. Secondly, the system DEEP is a first approximation for a system which represents intuitions about well-formedness; that is, what are the sentences o f English. The answer provided by DEEP is all the theorems o f DEEP. How do we decide whether the answer provided by DEEP is accurate? In principle, we just turn DEEP on, collect all o f the theorems, and compare that set to the set o f sentences identified by native speakers o f the language. Practically, however, we can show that DEEP is not a complete answer simply by finding one well-formed sentence of English which is not a theorem of DEEP. Sentence (4) is one such sentence.

(4) The boys at I T T were admitted b y Dick to have been contacted b y Spiro.

How do we decide whether (4) i s a theorem o f DEEP? First, we go through (4) using our intuitions to determine what the appropriate groupings and, therefore, tree representations for them are. We notice on the initial grouping, for example, that the words at and I T T go together in some way that neither boys and at nor I T T and were do. On the second run through the sentence, we notice that the constituents were admitted and b y Dick go together in a way that neither at I T T and were admitted nor b y Dick and to have been contacted do. After proceeding systematically through the sentence, we can represent our intuitions by tree structure (5). (See page 195)

Our intuitions represented in this tree structure make several interesting claims. They claim that there is a constituent composed of by, followed by a Det, followed by an N, wherein all three o f these constituents are exhaustively dominated by the node NP. This claim is sufficient to demonstrate that DEEP is only a partial answer to the membership question. How? By examining the rule of DEEP which specifies what constituents are exhaustively domi- nated by NP, that is, rule o f derivation (b). Since no rule of derivation expands NP as by + Det + N, we see that in no derivation o f DEEP (and, therefore, in no theorem o f DEEP) can there be a case in which an NP directly dominates the element by. In order for that configuration to arise, there would have to have been a rule for the form. We, therefore, can conclude that there is

Page 162: THE STRUCTURE MAGIC I

Appendix A / 195

n A ITT Ad @I Spiro

at least one well-formed sequence o f English which DEEP fails to enumerate. But before we try to find a way to supplement DEEP with an additional system or some additional rules o f derivation, we want to talk about our intuition about synonymy.

SYNONYMY

Check your intuitions about the relationship between sentence (2) and sentence (4), repeated below.

(2) Dick admitted Spiro had contacted the boys at I TT. (4) The boys at ITT were admitted by Dick to have been

contacted by Spiro. Native speakers of English judge the sentences (2) and (4) to be synonymous. Synonymy is a relationship which holds between two (or more) sentences when they always have the same truth value - they are always both true or always both false. In other words, assume that the words Dick and Spiro and the boys at ITT refer to the same things as they are used in both sentences (2) and (4). Can you imagine a world, logically consistent, in which one of these sentences is true and the other false? If you are unable to, then the pair is said to be synonymo~s.~ So, not only does sentence (4) represent a counter example to the claim that DEEP is an adequate grammar with respect to well-formedness, but it - along with sentence (2) - brings up the issue o f how intuitions o f synonymy are to be represented, how to determine which sen- tences o f different form or structure have the same meaning. In

Page 163: THE STRUCTURE MAGIC I

200 1 Appendix A

be...en by...

the system, I want to mention only one additional transformation - RAISING.

X V [ N P Y ] 2-X V NP [ Y ] Z S S S S

The overall derivation has the same effect as any derivation in a formal system: it carries the axioms of the system by the rules of derivation into the theorems or well-formed sequences (and, in this case, tree structures) of the system. If you compare the theorem for which we have just given the derivation with the tree representation (4), you will discover that, except for a few node differences which are affected by some minor clean-up transforma- tions of English, the two trees are identical. Now, how does this account for the intuitions of well-formedness and synonymy? First, we showed that the system DEEP failed to account for at least one well-formed sentence of English, namely, sentence (4). Notice now that DEEP plus TRANS, in fact, accounts for that sentence. In order for us to explain how the synonymy question is handled, we need to develop some terminology.

The Complete Model Within the theory of transformational grammar, each sentence

receives a double analysis: an analysis of the constituent structure, or what things go together, and an analysis of the meaning, or logical relations. Transformational grammar makes the claim that, in order to capture the consistent intuitions that you and I have as native speakers of English, two distinct levels of structure must be identified. These are called the Deep Structure and the Surface Structure. The Deep Structure is the level of structure in which the meaning or losrcal relations information is stated for the sentence under ana~ysfs the Surface Structure is the level of

Page 164: THE STRUCTURE MAGIC I

Appendix A 1 201

line

4) ' Dick admitted

4) Spiro

justification axiom of the system (theorem of DEEP)

had Det N PP con- tacted

the

4 ITT

by rule of derivation (a)

Page 165: THE STRUCTURE MAGIC I

202 / Appendix A

/ TiN

De t

A 6 A Dick

"\ 7\ P

/A admitted

NP be contacted b y Det N

I\ Det N i$ Spiro

by rule of derivation (a)

Page 166: THE STRUCTURE MAGIC I

Appendix A 1 207

(13)

system DEEP

axiom

S 0

I . 0

heorems 1

theorems of DEEP I AXIOMS OF TRANS - I Deep Structures

system TRANS

t axiom

I f theorems

- ---

these two distinct meanings by the symbols A and B, then how can we account for this property of ambiguity within the system of transformational grammar that we have developed here? The answer i s quite simple: consider the case o f synonymy. Synonymy is the case in which the same Deep Structure maps onto more than one Surface Structure. Ambiguity is the inverse of synonymy, namely, where different Deep Structures map onto the same Surface Structures. In other words, a Surface Structure will be ambiguous if there is more than one derivation leading from distinct Deep Structures. If there are two such derivations, then the Surface Structure which results is ambiguous in two ways, that is, it is connected by derivations with two distinct Deep Struc- tures. If there are n such derivations, then the resulting Surface Structure is n ways ambiguous. Figure (15) may help you to see the relationship of ambiguity in transformational terms (see page 208).

This last characterization o f the relationship o f ambiguity in transformational terms completes the sketch o f the theory o f transformational grammar which we want to present in this work.

Transformational grammar is the name o f the portion o f the field of linguistic research which we have used as a reference point in adapting linguistic models as a Meta-model for therapy. A t this point in time in the development o f the field o f transformational

Page 167: THE STRUCTURE MAGIC I

208 / Appendix A

(15) Deep Deep Deep Structure1 Structure2 . \ I

-

grammar, there are at least two groups o f researchers who consider themselves and members of the other group to have a distinctive and competing model for the dominant paradigm in linguistics. These two groups call their models the Extended Standard Theory and the Generative Semantics models. The concepts and processes which we have selected from transformational grammar are avail- able in both models. In other words, both groups o f people will be able to identify the formally equivalent concepts and processes in their model. Models are useful for much that falls outside formal equivalence. Specifically, the names o f the concepts and processes given to the experiences o f having intuitions about language pre- sent different images. They suggest through mechanisms such as presuppositions, entailments, invited inferences, and the syntax o f their expression different perceptions and attitudes. The majority o f the names we have chosen to use here are drawn from the Extended Standard Theory. For the purposes o f perceiving lan- guage while doing linguistics analysis and for formal elegance, we chose the Generative Semantics model. For the purposes o f de- scribing our experiences in therapy, in talking to people training themselves to be therapists, we have found the terminology o f the Extended Standard Theory more useful; thus, it was our choice in this book. We have attempted in the Glossary to give the nota- tional equivalences in the Generative Semantics model for the terms used here in the cases which seem important to us. We have an intuition that the Generative Semantics model will be most useful in the area o f Logical Semantic relations. Some fine work is being done in that area by linguists George Lakoff, Lauri Kar- tunnen, Georgia Green, Jerry Morgan, Larry Horne, Paul Postal, Haj Ross, Mass-aki Yamanashi, Dave Dowty, etc.; by logicians Hans Herzberger, Bas van Fraasen, Saul Kripke, etc.; and by people in Artificial Intelligence such as Roger Schank, Terry Winograd, etc. These kinds o f images have been useful to both o f us in representing and communicating our experiences in therapy.

Page 168: THE STRUCTURE MAGIC I

Appendix A / 209

FOOTNOTES FOR APPENDIX A

1. For a fuller presentation of the theory o f transformational grammar, see Chomsky (1 957), (1 965); Grinder and Elgin (1 973); Langacker (1 973); etc.

2. For a fuller discussion, see any introductory logic text; for example, Tarsky (1 943), Kripke (1972).

3. Because it is. 4. Since there i s no limit to the number of times that we may choose

rule of derivation (a), there i s no longer sequence of lines, and, therefore, the set of lines generated i s infinite. Actually, if you examine the structure of the set of rules of derivation, you'll find that the axiom expands into itself; that is, the symbol 5 appears on both sides o f the re-write arrow. The symbol, therefore, i s constantly replacing itself. This property of rule system i s called recursion; it guarantees that the set will generate an infinite set of lines of derivation.

5. This i s actually incomplete as the verb admit goes into a tree structure in which the verb i s followed by two NP nodes; we will correct this later.

6. What i s going on in the sentences listed i s that the structural requirements o f the verbs involved are being violated. For example, the verb laugh requires that it not be followed by some noun phrase. In more traditional grammatical terms, the verb laugh i s an intransitive verb; it takes no direct object.

7. What i s going on in the sentences listed is that the meaning require- ments, or the selectional restrictions of the verbs, are being violated. Verbs such as laugh and admit require that their subjects be human (or, at least, animate).

8. If you are able to, phone us and charge it to the publisher. 9. See any introductory treatment of the predicate calculas; for

example, in the sources listed in Footnote 6. 10. Notice that the transformation itself created the constituent struc-

ture which we could not account for by the rules of derivation for DEEP. Specifically, the sub-tree

Page 169: THE STRUCTURE MAGIC I

C

Glossary / 21 7

Presupposition: A basic underlying assumption which is necessary for a representation to make sense. Within language systems, a sentence which must be true for some other sentence to make sense.

Reference Structure: The sum total of experiences in a person's l i fe history. Also, the fullest representation from which other representations within some system are derived; for example, the Deep Structure serves as the Reference Structure for Surface Structure.

Representation: An image of something which is different from the thing itself; a map, a model.

Rule-Governed Behavior: Behavior which is systematic and can be represented explicitly by a set of rules. In the case of human rule-governed behavior, no awareness of the rules is necessary.

Semantics: The study of meaning. Synonymy: The name of the experience which people have with

sentences of distinct form which have the same meaning; e.g., The cat chased the rat and The rat was chased by the cat. In the transformational model o f language, two or more sen- tences are said to be synonymous if they are derived from the same Deep Structure.

Syntax: The study of the order and patterning of elements of a system. Within language, the study of the order and patterning of words and phrases.

Surface Structure: The sentences, derived from Deep Structure, which native speakers of the language speak and write.

Well-Formed: Meeting some set of conditions about form; e.g., well-formed in English, well-formed in therapy.

Page 170: THE STRUCTURE MAGIC I
Page 171: THE STRUCTURE MAGIC I

Bibliography

In this bibliography, our purpose is to provide references which will allow you to pursue any interests of which you have become aware in reading our book. We have divided the references into three sections:

Section I. Transformational Grammar

Section 11. Therapy

Section Ill. Modeling/Formal Systems/Epistemology

In each of these sections, we identify a small number of works which we have found particularly useful in developing our own models. The references given are not exhaustive, nor are they the only places where the ideas they contain can be found. We hope you enjoy your reading. If you know o f other reference works which you have found particularly clear and useful in your experi- ence in these areas, we would each appreciate hearing from you about them. Finally, if you wish to pursue some idea or line o f thought or experience set off by our book and the bibliography is inadequate for your purposes, write to us and we will each try to suggest references for you.

META-MODELS c/o Science and Behavior Books, Inc. P.O. Box 1 1457 Palo Alto, CA 94306

Page 172: THE STRUCTURE MAGIC I

220 / Bibliography

I. Transformational Grammar A. Basic References

Bach, E. Syntactic Theory. New York: Holt, Rinehart and Winston, Inc., 1974. A carefully presented overview of syntax as done by transformationalists.

Chomsky, N. Syntactic Structures. The Hague: Mouton, 1957. The book whicp established the transforma- tional model in linguistics; the style Chomsky uses is difficult for many readers. The portions of the book most connected with the Meta-model are the Preface; Chapters 2, 3,5,6, 8; and the Summary.

Chomsky, N. Aspects of the Theory of Syntax. Cambridge, Mass.: M IT Press, 1965. This is one of the most acces- sible descriptions of the linguistic model from which we have borrowed heavily. Again, some readers find the author's style difficult. We especially recommend Chapters 1 and 2.

Chomsky, N. Language and Mind. New York: Harcourt Brace Jovanovich, Inc., 1968. Four lectures which Chomsky gave as a visiting professor at Berkeley; less technical than his other two works we list.

Grinder, J., and Elgin, S. A Guide to Transformational Grammar. New York: Holt, Rinehart and Winston, 1973. A very comprehensive overview of the entire field of transformational grammar; includes summaries of, and commentaries on, Chomsky's Syntactic Struc- tures and Aspects. See especially Chapters 1, 2,4,5,6, 7, 8, 10, and 13.

Jacobs, R., and Rosenbaum, P. English Transformational Grammar. Waltham, Mass.: Ginn/Blaisdell, 1968. A very readable work as an introduction to the field; not particularly comprehensive.

Langac ke r, R. Language and Its Structure. New Y ork : Harcourt Brace Jovanovich, Inc., 1967. A readable introduction which treats language both by the trans- formational model and more generally.

Lyons, J. Introduction to Theoretical Linguistics. Cam- bridge, England: Cambridge University Press. A schol- arly work which presents an overview of language in general; includes a section on the transformational model.

B. Other Useful Transformational Work Bever, T. G. "The Cognitive Basis of Linguistic Structure."

Page 173: THE STRUCTURE MAGIC I

Bibliography 1 221

In J. Hayes (ed.), Cognition and the Developments of Language. New York: John Wiley and Sons, 1970. An excellent account of how language as a representa- tional system might be connected to general modeling abilities of human beings - especially the way that children develop these abilities.

Fillmore, C. "The Case for Case." In E. Bach and R. Harms (eds.), Universals in Linguistic Theory. New Y ork: Holt, Rinehart and Winston, 1968. A readable account of a somewhat different version of the transforma- tional model - useful suggestions about what a com- plete representation of reference structure might be.

Greene, G. "How to Get People to Do Things With Words." In Papers from the 8th Regional Meeting of the Chicago Linguistic Society. Chicago, I I I.: U n iver- sity of Chicago, 1970. An excellent example of the Generative Semantics approach which we fee l will con- tribute much to an enlarged Meta-model for therapy.

Grinder, J . On Deletion Phenomena in English. The Hague: Mouton, 1974. Very technical; useful for discussion of different types of deletion. See Chapters 1,2, and 3.

Gruber, J. "Studies in Lexical Relations." Unpublished doctoral dissertation, MIT, 1965. Excellent suggestion for a complete representation of reference structures.

Horn, L. "A Presuppositional Analysis of Only and Even." In Papers from the 5th Regional Meeting of the Chicago Linguistic Society. Chicago, Ill.: University of Chicago, 1969. Another fine example of the Genera- tive Semantics type of research which we feel will con- tribute to an enlarged Meta-model for therapy.

Kartunnen, L. "Remarks on Presuppositions." A t the Texas Conference on Performances, Conversational Implicature and Presuppositions, mimeograph, March 1973. Kartunnen has a series of incisive papers on presuppositional phenomena in English. We suggest you write to him directly at the University of Texas for copies.

Katz, J. Semantic Theory. New York: Harper and Row, 1972. A most up-to-date account of the kind of seman- tic theory most compatible with non-Generative Semantics transformational grammar.

Lakoff, G. Linguistics and Natural Logic. Ann Arbor, Mich.: University of Michigan, 1970. A valuable com- pendium of some of the more recent work in Genera-

Page 174: THE STRUCTURE MAGIC I

222 / Bibliography

tive Semantics by i t s most prolific spokesperson. G. Lakoff is presently at the University of California, Berkeley.

McCawley, J . "Lexical Insertion in a Transformational Grammar." I n Papers from the 4th Regional Meeting of the Chicago Linguistic Society. Chicago, Ill.: U niver- sity of Chicago, 1968. One o f the initial articles estab- lishing Generative Semantics; good suggestions about the representation of reference structures.

Postal, P. "On the Derivation o f Pseudo-Adjectives." Paper delivered to the 44th Annual Meeting of the LSA, 1969.

Postal, P. "On the Surface Verb Remind." In Linguistic Inquiry, 7; 1 :37-120. Postal's work is highly theoret- ical; the first reference has excellent examples o f the patterns of derivation as Deep Structure Predicates are mapped into Surface Structure Adjectives. The second reference is very useful in making suggestions about the representation of reference structures.

Ross, J . R. "On Declarative Sentences." I n R. Jacobs and P. Rosen bau m, Readings in English Transformational Grammar. Waltham, Mass.: Ginn/Blaisdell, 1970. This is the linguistic basis for the section in Chapter 4 called The Last Performative and an excellent example of linguistic analysis.

Sapir, E. The Selected Writing of Edward Sapir. D. Mandel- baum (ed.). University of California Press, Berkeley, 1963. One of the classical linguists who had a fine sensitivity for modeling.

Searle, J. Speech Acts. Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press, 1969. A modern work in pragmatics with the transformational model as a basis. Readable.

Whorf, B. "Grammatical Categories." In J . E. Carroll (ed.), Language, Thought and Reality. New York: John Wiley and Sons, 1956. Another classical linguist who addressed the issue of the way language shapes perception.

II. Therapy J ac kson, D. D. Communication, Family and Marriage. Palo

Alto: Science and Behavior Books, 1968. An excellent anthology containing the papers o f the MRIIBateson research group.

J ac kson, D. D. Therapy, Communication and Change. Palo

Page 175: THE STRUCTURE MAGIC I

Bibliography 1 223

Alto: Science and Behavior Books, 1968. An excellent anthology containing the papers o f the MRI/Bateson research group.

Haley, J. Advanced Techniques of Hypnosis and Therapy: Selected Papers of Milton H. Erickson, M.D. New York: Grune and Stratton, 1967. An incredible collec- tion o f papers describing the powerful techniques o f Milton Erickson.

Haley, J. Uncommon Therapy. New York: Grune and Stratton, 1968. A valuable statement o f Erickson's powerful work with an interesting commentary by Jay Haley.

Perk, F. The Gestalt Approach: Eyewitness to Therapy. Palo Alto: Science and Behavior Books, 1973. A clear presentation of Gestalt therapy theoretical foundations.

Polster, I. and M. Gestalt Therapy Integrated. New York: Bruner/Mazel, 1973. A useful presentation o f some o f the techniques of Gestalt therapy.

Satir, V. Conjoint Family Therapy. Palo Alto: Science and Behavior Books, 1964. A basic and most useful text on family therapy.

Satir, V. Peoplemaking. Palo Alto: Science and Behavior Books, 1972. An excellent and highly readable intro-

- - - -

duction to communications and therapy. Watzlawick, P.; Beavin, J.; and Jackson, D. Pragmatics of

Human Communications. New York: W. Norton, 1967. A highly readable presentation o f Bateson's ideas (e.g., meta-communication).

Watzlawick, P.; Weakland, J.; and Fisch, R. Change. New York: W. Norton, 1974. An interesting attempt to integrate mathematical models with patterns o f human change.

II I. Modeling/Formal Systems/Epistemo1ogy Ashby, W. R. An Introduction to Cybernetics. Chapman

and Hall, Ltd., and University Paperbacks, 1956. An excellent introduction to modelings and representa- tional systems; requires some mathematical back- ground; worth working through carefully.

Bateson, G. Steps to an Ecology of Mind. New York: Ballantine Books, 1972. We recommend this book highly; it is a collection of Bateson's work. Very enter- taining; simultaneously irrelevant and profound.

Page 176: THE STRUCTURE MAGIC I

224 / Bibliography

Boyd, D. lntroduction to Systems Analysis, (in press) 1975. A highly readable, clear presentation o f modeling; emphasizes process.

Carnap, R. The Logical Syntax of Language. Totowa, New jersey: Littlefield, Adams and Company, 1959. A for- mal, sophisticated approach to linguistic analysis. A highly technical piece o f work; difficult to read.

Copi, I. lntroduction to Logic. New York: Macmillan, 1961. An excellent introductory text to logical systems.

Herzberger, H. "The Logical Consistency o f Language." Hatvard Educational Review, 35:469-480; 1965. An example o f a clear philosophical analysis o f one o f the formal properties o f the human representational system of language.

Hume, D. Enquiry Concerning Human Understanding. Ox- ford, England: Oxford University Press. A classical essay on epistemology, the process o f human modeling.

Korzybski, A. Science and Sanity. Lakeville, Connecticut: The International Non-Aristotelian Library Publishing Company, 4th Edition, 1933. The basic reference work for general semantics. Korzybski understood and dis- cussed clearly the map/territory, intentionall extensional distinctions,. . . in human modeling. Read the Prefaces, Part I, and Part I I.

Miller, G. A.; Galanter, E.; and Pribram, K. Plans and the Structure o f Behavior. New York: Holt, Rinehart and Winston, Inc., 1960. One o f the clearest presentations of a theoretical basis for human behavior; suggestions for a representational system for reference structures; easy and enjoyable reading.

Newell, A.; and Simon, H. A. Human Problem Solving. Englewood Cliffs, New Jersey: Prentice-Hall, 19.71. An exciting excursion into the neurological basis for human modeling. A clear presentation.

Russel I, B. lntroduction to Mathematical Philosophy. Lon- don, England: George Allen and Unwin, Ltd., 2nd Edition, 1921. A readable, clear presentation of some o f the more important concepts o f modern logic, in- cluding theory o f logical types.

Schank, R.; and Colby, K. Computer Models o f Thought and Language. San Francisco: W. H. Freeman and Company. 1973. A good, representative collection o f

Page 177: THE STRUCTURE MAGIC I

Bibliography / 225

modeling as done in computer simulations. Tarski, A. Introduction to Logic. New York: Oxford Uni-

versity Press, 1941. An excellent introduction to logi- cal systems, a very readable style, no background required.

Vaihinger, H. The Philosophy of "As I f" London, England: Routledge, Kegan and Paul, Ltd., 1924. An excellent source for discussions of human modeling. F. Perk claimed Vaihinger supplied the philosophical foundations for his Gestalt therapy.

Watzlawick, P.; Beavin, J.; and Jackson, D. Pragmatics of Human Communication. New York: W. W. Norton and Company, 1967. A very readable, clear presentation of some of the basic ideas of communication with con- nections to systems analysis.