The Role of Serotonin in Cognitive Function

download The Role of Serotonin in Cognitive Function

of 10

Transcript of The Role of Serotonin in Cognitive Function

  • 7/28/2019 The Role of Serotonin in Cognitive Function

    1/10

    http://jop.sagepub.com/Journal of Psychopharmacology

    http://jop.sagepub.com/content/27/7/575The online version of this article can be found at:

    DOI: 10.1177/02698811134825312013 27: 575 originally published online 27 March 2013J PsychopharmacolPhilip Cowen and Ann C Sherwood

    understanding depressionThe role of serotonin in cognitive function: evidence from recent studies and implications for

    Published by:

    http://www.sagepublications.com

    On behalf of:

    British Association for Psychopharmacology

    can be found at:Journal of PsychopharmacologyAdditional services and information for

    http://jop.sagepub.com/cgi/alertsEmail Alerts:

    http://jop.sagepub.com/subscriptionsSubscriptions:

    http://www.sagepub.com/journalsReprints.navReprints:

    http://www.sagepub.com/journalsPermissions.navPermissions:

    What is This?

    - Mar 27, 2013OnlineFirst Version of Record

    - Jun 20, 2013Version of Record>>

    at HINARI on July 14, 2013jop.sagepub.comDownloaded from

    http://jop.sagepub.com/http://jop.sagepub.com/http://jop.sagepub.com/content/27/7/575http://jop.sagepub.com/content/27/7/575http://www.sagepublications.com/http://www.sagepublications.com/http://www.bap.org.uk/http://jop.sagepub.com/cgi/alertshttp://jop.sagepub.com/cgi/alertshttp://jop.sagepub.com/cgi/alertshttp://jop.sagepub.com/subscriptionshttp://jop.sagepub.com/subscriptionshttp://www.sagepub.com/journalsReprints.navhttp://www.sagepub.com/journalsReprints.navhttp://www.sagepub.com/journalsPermissions.navhttp://www.sagepub.com/journalsPermissions.navhttp://www.sagepub.com/journalsPermissions.navhttp://online.sagepub.com/site/sphelp/vorhelp.xhtmlhttp://online.sagepub.com/site/sphelp/vorhelp.xhtmlhttp://jop.sagepub.com/content/early/2013/03/22/0269881113482531.full.pdfhttp://jop.sagepub.com/content/early/2013/03/22/0269881113482531.full.pdfhttp://jop.sagepub.com/content/27/7/575.full.pdfhttp://jop.sagepub.com/content/27/7/575.full.pdfhttp://jop.sagepub.com/http://jop.sagepub.com/http://jop.sagepub.com/http://online.sagepub.com/site/sphelp/vorhelp.xhtmlhttp://jop.sagepub.com/content/early/2013/03/22/0269881113482531.full.pdfhttp://jop.sagepub.com/content/27/7/575.full.pdfhttp://www.sagepub.com/journalsPermissions.navhttp://www.sagepub.com/journalsReprints.navhttp://jop.sagepub.com/subscriptionshttp://jop.sagepub.com/cgi/alertshttp://www.bap.org.uk/http://www.sagepublications.com/http://jop.sagepub.com/content/27/7/575http://jop.sagepub.com/
  • 7/28/2019 The Role of Serotonin in Cognitive Function

    2/10

    Journal of Psychopharmacology

    27(7) 575583 The Author(s) 2013Reprints and permissions:

    sagepub.co.uk/journalsPermissions.nav

    DOI: 10.1177/0269881113482531

    jop.sagepub.com

    Introduction

    Cognitive function comprises various neurobiological processesinvolved in attention, learning, memory, planning and decision

    making (Garcia-Carbonero and Paz-Ares 2002; Millan et al.,

    2012). Impairment of cognitive function is a prominent feature of

    many psychiatric and neurodegenerative illnesses and imposes

    substantial disability. There is increasing interest in understanding

    the neurobiology of cognition in healthy individuals in order to

    identify therapeutic targets for improving symptomatic and func-

    tional outcomes in impaired individuals with psychiatric disorders.

    Symptoms of cognitive impairment such as poor concentration,

    memory loss and difficulty with decision making are prevalent in

    patients with depression but are not specific treatment targets, the

    general assumption having been that any deficits will remit as the

    patient recovers from depression (Garcia-Carbonero and Paz-Ares2002; Jaeger et al., 2006; McCall and Dunn 2003; Naismith et al.,

    2007). However, the fact that patients can continue to demonstrate

    cognitive impairments even when apparently clinically recovered

    suggests that this view may be too sanguine (Hasselbalch et al.,

    2011). Indeed, structural imaging studies have revealed consistent

    evidence of hippocampal grey matter reductions in patients with

    recurrent depression, which might be associated with persistent

    problems in episodic memory (McKinnon et al., 2009). Similarly,

    functional imaging studies of working memory also suggest

    abnormal neural activation patterns in remitted depressed patients

    (Kerestes et al., 2012; Schoning et al., 2009).

    Drugs that potentiate serotonin (5-HT) function are the main-stay of depression treatment and are often prescribed for longer-

    term maintenance therapy. However, our understanding of the

    effects of serotonin on cognitive function in healthy humans and

    depressed patients is incomplete. There are useful data from animal

    studies investigating the modulatory effects of specific 5-HT

    receptor subtypes on cognition in animal models but much less

    data on effects in humans. Nevertheless, it is possible that future

    antidepressant compounds with more selective actions at 5-HT

    receptor subtypes may produce specific cognitive benefits in

    depressed patients. The literature relevant to these topics is consid-

    erable, and the objective of this article is to provide a concise over-

    view for clinicians, focusing on how 5-HT impacts cognitive

    function relevant to depressed patients. We will also briefly sum-marize animal studies linking specific 5-HT receptor subtypes with

    cognitive function where relevant to psychotropic drug action.

    The role of serotonin in cognitive function:evidence from recent studies and implicationsfor understanding depression

    Philip Cowen1 and Ann C Sherwood2

    AbstractBackground: Symptoms of cognitive impairment such as poor concentration, memory loss and difficulty with decision making are prevalent in patients

    with depression, but currently are not specific targets for treatment. However, patients can continue to demonstrate cognitive impairments even

    when apparently clinically recovered. Drugs that potentiate serotonin (5-HT) function, such as selective serotonin reuptake inhibitors (SSRIs), are

    the mainstay of treatment for depression. Nevertheless, our understanding of the effects of SSRIs and other conventional antidepressant therapy on

    cognitive function in healthy humans and depressed patients remains limited.

    Objective: The purpose of this article is to provide a concise overview for clinicians on the impact of pharmacological manipulation of 5-HT on

    cognitive function in healthy humans with additional reference to animal models where human data are lacking, particularly regarding specific 5-HTreceptor subtype modulation.

    Findings: The most consistent observation following manipulation of serotonin levels in humans is that low extracellular 5-HT levels are associated

    with impaired memory consolidation. Preclinical data show that agonism and antagonism at specific 5-HT receptors can exert effects in animal models

    of cognition.

    Conclusions: Larger, consistently designed studies are needed to understand the roles of 5-HT in cognition in healthy and depressed individuals.

    Efforts to target specific 5-HT receptors to improve cognitive outcomes are warranted.

    Keywords

    Cognition, serotonin, tryptophan depletion, 5-HT receptor, antidepressants

    1Warneford Hospital, Oxford, UK2The Medicine Group, New Hope, PA, USA

    Corresponding author:

    Philip Cowen, Neurosciences Building, Warneford Hospital, Oxford

    OX3 7JX.

    Email: [email protected]

    JOP27710.1177/0269881113482531Journal of PsychopharmacologyCowena nd Sherwood

    Review

    at HINARI on July 14, 2013jop.sagepub.comDownloaded from

    http://jop.sagepub.com/http://jop.sagepub.com/http://jop.sagepub.com/http://jop.sagepub.com/
  • 7/28/2019 The Role of Serotonin in Cognitive Function

    3/10

    576 Journal of Psychopharmacology 27(7)

    Serotonin and cognition

    Tryptophan depletion studies

    Tryptophan (TRP) is the amino acid precursor of 5-HT, and

    depleting systemic levels of TRP effectively lowers 5-HT in the

    central nervous system. Depletion is accomplished by oral admin-

    istration of an amino acid mixture lacking TRP and enriched forother large, neutral amino acids (LNAA). As a result, circulating

    TRP levels decrease while at the same time competition increases

    for the LNAA transporter needed by TRP to cross the bloodbrain

    barrier. In rats, TRP depletion results in an approximately 50%

    reduction in 5-HT levels in the cortex, striatum, and hippocampus

    (Mendelsohn et al., 2009).

    A number of TRP depletion studies have assessed the relation

    between lowered 5-HT levels and cognition in healthy adults

    (Table 1). Most studies were small and used heterogeneous cog-

    nitive testing methods, which revealed heterogeneous outcomes.

    To increase the power to detect effects in these trials, Mendelsohn

    et al. (2009) conducted a comprehensive review of 66 TRP deple-

    tion studies published starting in 1966 through September 2008

    (Mendelsohn et al., 2009). Fifty-nine trials included healthy indi-viduals and used a placebo drink or TRP loading as a comparator.

    Circulating TRP levels were reduced by 4697%. There was no

    evidence that TRP depletion affected mood in healthy volunteers

    without a family history of depression. However, acute TRP

    depletion impaired episodic memory, which the authors define as

    acquisition and retention of memories for events and experiences

    requiring conscious learning. The most consistent effects of TRP

    depletion across the trials included in the analysis were impaired

    consolidation of episodic memory, primarily that requiring

    verbal learning. Some trials found small effects on encoding of

    verbal learning. In addition, one study showed significant nega-

    tive impact of TRP depletion on contextual episodic memory.

    However, TRP depletion did not appear to affect spatial episodicmemory, or semantic or working memory. Indeed, semantic

    memory tended to improve in the depleted state (Mendelsohn

    et al., 2009).

    The authors also noted results in the majority of studies did not

    support specific effects of TRP depletion on executive function

    (planning, decision making, and response inhibition) (Mendelsohn

    et al., 2009) and showed minimal effects on sustained attention

    (vigilance), selective attention, divided attention, or attentional

    set-shifting (Lash et al., 2000; Mendelsohn et al., 2009).

    Results using BOLD signals in fMRI to assess the function of

    5-HT manipulation on the neural activity involved in cognitive

    processes also are heterogeneous among themselves and variable

    with respect to consistency with other approaches. Van der Veen

    et al. (2006) used fMRI to compare the BOLD response afterdrinking a balanced amino acid mixture and after TRP depletion

    in healthy male volunteers during visual verbal episodic memory

    tasks. TRP depletion increased the number of positively rated

    words and worsened word retrieval. The BOLD response was

    diminished in brain regions normally activated during encoding

    (right hippocampus) but was not affected the response in the

    neurocircuitry activated during the retrieval phase (distributed in

    frontal, parietal, temporal, cingulate, striatal and cerebellar

    regions) (van der Veen et al., 2006). These findings are consistent

    with evidence implicating 5-HT in consolidation of episodic

    memory (Mendelsohn et al., 2009), and suggest that the key role

    is during the encoding phase (van der Veen et al., 2006).

    The same group investigated the impact of TRP depletion on

    performance monitoring and response inhibition (Go/NoGo test),

    two processes necessary for cognitive flexibility (Evers et al.,

    2006). They compared the BOLD response in the dorsolateral pre-

    frontal cortex with and without TRP depletion in 13 healthy male

    volunteers. Although there was no difference in the number of

    correct responses on the Go/NoGo tasks with and without TRP

    depletion, after TRP depletion the BOLD response decreased dur-ing performance monitoring, but not during response inhibition

    (Evers et al., 2006).

    Although there is no evidence that serotonin depletion is suf-

    ficient to induce depression in healthy individuals, approximately

    50% of patients with major depressive disorder (MDD) who are in

    remission after selective serotonin reuptake inhibitor (SSRI) ther-

    apy relapse under conditions of TRP depletion (>80% decrease in

    plasma TRP levels) (Delgado et al., 1999). These findings suggest

    that serotonin is essential for the maintenance of antidepressant

    effects of SSRIs at least in some patients. However, use of a low-

    dose TRP depletion protocol in which competition for the LNAA

    transporter is reduced was able to induce immediate recall deficits

    in recovered patients without affecting mood in two studies(Haddad et al., 2009; Hayward et al., 2005). In both of these stud-

    ies, low-dose TRP depletion (between 64% and 70% reduction in

    plasma TRP) was also associated with negative cognitive bias and

    difficulties with autobiographical memory. Impaired specificity of

    autobiographical memory was reported recently in a preliminary

    study of healthy individuals with a family history of depression

    following a 63% reduction in plasma TRP (Alhaj et al., 2012).

    Conclusions drawn from TRP depletion studies are limited

    because serotonin manipulation is not specific to a particular sero-

    tonin pathway or brain region. However, taken together, the

    results in healthy subjects and in patients with depression support

    the hypothesis that mood and cognitive symptoms have different

    thresholds for serotonin availability and may be mediated by dif-

    ferent serotonin functions.

    Serotonin reuptake inhibition

    5-HT is removed from the synaptic cleft via the serotonin trans-

    porter (SERT). It is generally accepted that SERT inhibition by

    SSRIs increases serotonin availability in the synapse, thus increas-

    ing serotonergic tone. In the short term, however, the increase in

    5-HT activates 5-HT1A autoreceptors in the raphe nuclei, which

    may initiate a transient compensatory inhibition of 5-HT release.

    Although the activation of 5-HT1A autoreceptors inhibits seroto-

    nin release, rat studies show that extracellular 5-HT levels in the

    prefrontal cortex increase during acute and subchronic SSRI

    administration (Ceglia et al., 2004). Thus, even in the short term,SSRI treatment is presumed to result in increased 5-HT levels in

    the central nervous system in some brain regions. With chronic

    exposure to SSRIs, overall 5-HT neurotransmission is increased.

    In three small studies in healthy volunteers, acute (24 h) and

    subchronic treatment (1422 days) with fluoxetine, paroxetine

    and venlafaxine reduced vigilance (sustained attention) (OHanlon

    et al., 1998; Ramaekers et al., 1995; Schmitt et al., 2002). To

    determine if this effect is specific to SERT inhibition, the effects

    of the highly selective SSRI citalopram were compared with those

    of sertraline, an SSRI with some potential activity at the dopamine

    transporter. Acute (24 h) and subchronic (Day 15) treatment with

    citalopram (20 mg and 40 mg, respectively) impaired sustained

    at HINARI on July 14, 2013jop.sagepub.comDownloaded from

    http://jop.sagepub.com/http://jop.sagepub.com/http://jop.sagepub.com/
  • 7/28/2019 The Role of Serotonin in Cognitive Function

    4/10

    Cowen and Sherwood 577

    Table 1. Serotonin manipulation and cognitive effects in healthy individuals.

    Serotonin

    (5-HT) receptor

    Study method Cognitive effects observed in healthy individuals

    TRP depletion (Mendelsohn et al., 2009) (Meta-analysis of 59 trials) impaired consolidation of

    episodic memory (verbal); did not appear to affect the

    spatial episodic memory, semantic or working memory, or

    executive function; minimal effects on sustained attention,

    selective attention, divided attention, or attentional set-

    shifting

    Serotonin transporter

    (SERT)

    Citalopram (20 mg) (Nathan et al., 2000) (N= 9 males) Acute (14 h): Improved response time and

    sustained attention vs. baseline

    Citalopram (30 mg) vs. placebo and

    atomoxetine (Chamberlain et al., 2006)

    (N= 60 males) Acute (1.5 h) Impaired probabilistic learning

    but not response inhibition

    Citalopram (20 mg on days 18, 40 mg

    on days 915) vs. sertraline or placebo

    (Riedel et al., 2005)

    (N= 24) Acute (24 h) and subchronic (14 days) treatment

    with citalopram (20 mg and 40 mg, respectively) impaired

    sustained attention

    Citalopram 10 mg iv vs. placebo (Harmer

    et al., 2002)

    (N= 24 females) Acute

  • 7/28/2019 The Role of Serotonin in Cognitive Function

    5/10

    578 Journal of Psychopharmacology 27(7)

    attention in 21 healthy subjects. However, sertraline (50 mg

    acutely, and 100 mg subchronically) did not affect sustained atten-

    tion (vigilance) in healthy subjects (Riedel et al., 2005).

    It should be pointed out that decrements in sustained attention

    have not always been found in healthy individuals treated with

    citalopram. Harmer and colleagues (2002) conducted a rand-

    omized, placebo-controlled study of citalopram in 24 healthy

    women. Testing was initiated 45 min after infusing either 10 mgcitalopram or placebo. There was no citalopram effect on

    sustained attention or immediate recall. The reasons for the differ-

    ences may reflect the small sample sizes, different study popula-

    tions (both sexes vs. only women), different testing methods, or

    different drug administration. Perhaps, however, the more inter-

    esting finding in the latter study is that acute citalopram treatment

    enhanced memory consolidation compared with the placebo

    group (Harmer et al., 2002). Once again, these results suggest that

    memory consolidation may be inversely correlated with extracel-

    lular 5-HT levels.

    Several other small studies assessed SSRI effects on various

    cognitive functions. Almeida et al. (2010) compared the acute and

    chronic effects of citalopram 20 mg administered once daily onimpulsive responding and contextual information processing in

    healthy controls. Acute administration (24 h) impaired perfor-

    mance on the Delayed Non-Matching to Sample Task (DNST), a

    measure of working memory. The effect was no longer apparent

    after 28 days. In this study, citalopram treatment did not affect

    performance on Degraded Symbol Continuous Performance Test,

    a measure of impulsive responding, at either time point. The

    authors suggested that the transient effects on the DNST are likely

    attributable to activation of 5-HT1A receptors in the entorhinal

    cortex and hippocampus (Almeida et al., 2010).

    Effects of subchronic sertraline administration on cognitive

    function were evaluated in a randomized, double-blind, cross-

    over study in 12 healthy male volunteers (Siepmann et al., 2003).

    Subjects received sertraline 50 mg or placebo once daily for 14days. In this trial, subchronic sertraline did not affect choice reac-

    tion time, psychomotor coordination, visual memory span, or abil-

    ity to detect discrete sensory data. The lack of effect has been

    attributed to sertraline actions on dopamine levels that may coun-

    ter the impact of 5-HT1A activation. On the other hand, it may

    simply be that SSRIs produce relatively small and inconsistent

    effect on cognition in healthy participants. Similarly, daily admin-

    istration of a dual serotonin norepinephrine reuptake inhibitor,

    venlafaxine 75 mg/day for 7 days followed by 150 mg/day for

    another 7 days, did not significantly alter choice reaction time,

    psychomotor function or memory in healthy subjects (Siepmann

    et al., 2008). Overall, the most consistent observation following

    manipulation of serotonin levels by TRP depletion or SERTblockade seems to be that low extracellular 5-HT levels are asso-

    ciated with impaired memory consolidation.

    Serotonin receptors and cognition data from animal and human studies

    5-HT receptors comprise seven subfamilies including 14 sub-

    types, and some subtypes are expressed as multiple isoforms. All

    are G-protein-linked except 5-HT3 receptors, which are iono-

    tropic. Most 5-HT receptors are present in brain regions associ-

    ated with learning and memory. However, within any particular

    region, they may be expressed on different neuronal subtypes and

    in different layers of the region as heteroreceptors (Puig and

    Gulledge 2011). This diversity allows for complex cellular and

    regional mechanisms for regulation of receptor activity. Generally,

    it is has been suggested that effects of 5-HT receptor subtype

    manipulation on learning and memory are exerted through altera-

    tions in the release of neurotransmitters such as acetylcholine and

    glutamate, which have been more directly implicated in cognitivefunction than 5-HT itself (King et al., 2008). Although selective

    agonists and antagonists have been developed for many 5-HT

    receptor subtypes, agents tested in healthy humans tend to be rela-

    tively non-selective. Nonetheless, evidence supports the potential

    therapeutic value of targeting one or more 5-HT receptors to

    enhance learning and memory in humans.

    5-HT1A receptors

    5-HT1A receptors are widely expressed in the prefrontal cortex,

    hippocampus, and septum, areas associated with learning and

    memory, and in the raphe nuclei, the primary location of 5-HT cell

    bodies in the central nervous system (Albert and Francois 2010).

    Generally, 5-HT1A agonists impair learning in animals, although

    data from 5-HT1A knockout mice are not consistent with this.

    Delivery of 5-HT1A receptor agonists to the rat medial raphe

    nuclei enhanced cognitive performance, whereas performance

    was impaired following systemic delivery or infusion into the hip-

    pocampus. This suggests that activation of 5-HT1A autoreceptors

    improves cognitive function by decreasing 5-HT release, and the

    opposite effect is produced by drugs acting directly at post-synaptic

    5-HT1A receptors (Warburton et al., 1997). However, systemic

    delivery of F15599, an agonist that is highly selective for post-

    synaptic 5-HT1A receptors, did not induce cognitive deficits in

    rats and, unlike non-selective agents, partially reversed phencycli-

    dine-induce impairment of working and reference memory(Depoortere et al., 2010).

    In healthy humans, 5-HT1A partial agonists such as buspirone

    and tandospirone have little or no effect on cognition, though the

    data are somewhat inconsistent (Chamberlain et al., 2007a;

    Takahashi et al., 2010). Interpretation of this effect is complex

    because partial 5-HT1A receptor agonism might increase or

    decrease 5-HT neurotransmission. In contrast, in patients with

    schizophrenia, the same 5-HT1A partial agonists may produce

    small improvements in cognition, suggesting (perhaps not surpris-

    ingly) that the effects of partial agonists on cognition might

    depend on underlying state of 5-HT neurotransmission (Sumiyoshi

    et al., 2000, 2001, 2007). It has been suggested that 5-HT1A-

    modifying drugs might also be helpful for the cognitive impair-

    ments in Alzheimers disease. However, development of

    single-receptor agents that showed promise for improving cogni-

    tion in patients with Alzheimers disease was stopped. Lecozotan,

    a selective 5-HT1A antagonist, and xaliproden, a 5-HT1A agonist,

    both failed to complete FDA registration trials for treatment of

    Alzheimers symptoms (Sabbagh 2009).

    5-HT2 receptors

    5-HT2A receptors, along with 5-HT1A receptors, are among the

    most commonly expressed in the prefrontal cortex. Unlike 5-HT1A

    receptors, however, 5-HT2A receptors are located exclusively

    at HINARI on July 14, 2013jop.sagepub.comDownloaded from

    http://jop.sagepub.com/http://jop.sagepub.com/http://jop.sagepub.com/
  • 7/28/2019 The Role of Serotonin in Cognitive Function

    6/10

    Cowen and Sherwood 579

    postsynaptically. In the cortex, 5-HT2A receptors are localized to a

    subset of GABAergic interneurons and projection neurons,

    consistent with a role in cognition (de Almeida and Mengod 2007;

    Santana et al., 2004). In rats, ()-2,5-dimethoxy-4iodoampheta-

    mine (DOI), a 5-HT2A receptor agonist, impairs short-term

    memory at low doses and long-term memory at higher doses,

    whereas 5-HT2A antagonists improved long-term memory without

    affecting short-term memory (Meneses 2007). The 5-HT2A recep-tor antagonist EMD 281014 improved working memory function

    in rhesus monkeys (Terry, Jr. et al., 2005).

    There is also limited evidence that drugs with 5-HT2A receptor

    antagonist properties, such as mianserin, may produce benefit in

    cognitive function in patients with schizophrenia (Roth et al.,

    2004). However, it has been difficult to show that atypical antip-

    sychotic drugs, many of which potently block 5-HT2A receptors,

    are better at improving cognitive function than conventional

    antipsychotic agents (Selva-Vera et al., 2010; Wittorf et al., 2008).

    5-HT2C receptors appear to be expressed exclusively in the

    central nervous system, and are subject to extensive RNA editing

    leading to 14 different isoforms (Jensen et al., 2010). Receptor

    mRNA has been localized to GABAergic, glutamatergic anddopaminergic neurons in the frontal cortex, hippocampus, hypo-

    thalamic nuclei, substantia nigra, nucleus accumbens, striatum

    and the raphe nuclei (Jensen et al., 2010). There are relatively few

    studies on the effects of 5-HT2C receptor ligands on cognitive

    function in animals. The 5-HT2C receptor antagonist SB 242084

    improved reversal learning in rats. However, somewhat paradoxi-

    cally, the 5-HT2C agonist CP-809101 has been shown to enhance

    novel object recognition learning in animal models (Siuciak et al.,

    2007).

    Mirtazapine antagonizes 5-HT2C receptors in addition to act-

    ing at alpha-2 receptors. In a small study (N= 18) in healthy indi-

    viduals, verbal memory was impaired with acute (2 days and 9

    days) and subchronic (16 days) treatment with mirtazapine 3045

    mg/d. Escitalopram 1020 mg did not affect verbal memory inthis placebo-controlled cross-over study (Wingen et al., 2006).

    However, adjunctive mirtazapine may enhance cognition in

    patients with schizophrenia (Cho et al., 2011; Stenberg et al.,

    2010, 2011). In one study, mirtazapine improved cognitive per-

    formance in patients with recurrent depression. Although depres-

    sive symptoms improved with treatment, cognitive improvement

    did not correlate with change in depressive symptoms (Borkowska

    et al., 2007). However, whether these procognitive effects are

    mediated by 5-HT2C receptors has not been determined.

    5-HT3 receptors

    5-HT3 receptors are the only ionotropic 5-HT receptors. Withinthe brain, 5-HT3 receptors are primarily localized to GABAergic

    interneurons in the prefrontal cortex, where they contribute to the

    regulation of cholinergic, dopaminergic, and glutamatergic activity

    (Meneses, 2007). 5-HT3 receptors are also found in the hippocam-

    pus (Walstab et al., 2010). 5-HT3 receptor antagonists such as

    ondansetron have been shown to improve learning and memory in

    rat (Hodges et al., 1996) and monkey (Terry, Jr. et al., 1996)

    models of impaired cognition. A rat model showed that treatment

    with the 5-HT3 agonist mCPBG impaired short-term memory at

    low doses and long-term memory at high doses (Meneses, 2007).

    Despite encouraging results in animal models, there is no compel-

    ling evidence that 5-HT3 receptor antagonism improves cognitive

    function in age-related memory decline in humans. However,

    there are some hints of cognitive benefit in patients with schizo-

    phrenia (Akhondzadeh et al., 2009; Levkovitz et al., 2005).

    5-HT4 receptors

    To date, few studies have evaluated the role of 5-HT4 receptors inhealthy humans. However, preclinical studies (King et al., 2008;

    Marchetti et al., 2004; Terry, Jr. et al., 1998) demonstrated a role

    for 5-HT4 binding in learning and memory. Findings in rodents

    suggest that antagonism at 5-HT4 receptors impairs passive avoid-

    ance memory, whereas agonism has rather complex effects but

    does seem capable of reversing scopolamine-induced cognitive

    deficits. The latter effects are thought to occur through modulation

    of memory consolidation via regulation of cholinergic neurons

    (Orsetti et al., 2003). In addition, animal studies suggest that

    5-HT4 receptor activation elicits hippocampal synaptic plasticity.

    Thus, 5-HT4 agonists are being considered for treatment of

    Alzheimers disease (Russo et al., 2009).

    5-HT6 receptors

    The densest distribution of 5-HT6 receptors in rat brain is found

    in the frontal and entorhinal cortices, hippocampus, nucleus

    accumbens and striatum (Codony et al., 2011). Preclinical data

    indicate that the 5-HT6 receptor regulates serotonergic modula-

    tion of dopamine release in the prefrontal cortex and cholinergic

    neurotransmission in the brain. Pharmacological studies in rats

    have further implicated this receptor in memory retention (King

    et al., 2008). Paradoxically, both 5-HT6 receptor agonists and

    antagonists reverse memory deficits in animal models of cogni-

    tive impairment (Codony et al., 2011). However, antagonists have

    little or no cognitive-enhancing affect in healthy animals.

    Consistent with these findings, one study conducted in healthyhumans found that the 5-HT6 receptor antagonist SB-742457 did

    not affect motor activation and fluency, adaptive tracking, subjec-

    tive alertness, or delayed word recognition (Liem-Moolenaar

    et al., 2011). Several agents that are selective for 5-HT6 receptors

    are currently in development for treatment of cognitive symptoms

    in Alzheimers disease (Codony et al., 2011).

    5-HT7 receptors

    5-HT7 receptors are found on glutamatergic neurons in the super-

    chiasmic nucleus of the hypothalamus, hippocampus, cortex,

    thalamus and raphe nuclei (Bonaventure et al., 2011). In a recent

    rat study, the selective 5-HT7 antagonist SB269270 prevented thenegative effects of MK801 but not scopolamine on working mem-

    ory. Moreover, in this same study, SB269270 inhibited MK801

    stimulated glutamate release, but not dopamine (Bonaventure

    et al., 2011). These findings suggest that 5-HT7 receptor inhibition

    may improve working memory in patients with glutamatergic

    dysfunction for example, schizophrenia.

    In summary, a relatively small number of studies have investi-

    gated the role of specific serotonin receptors with cognition in

    healthy humans. Similar to results in TRP depletion studies, in

    most cases, little or no impact on cognition has been observed,

    suggesting that serotonin homeostasis is difficult to perturb in

    the healthy state. Nonetheless, some serotonin receptor modulators

    at HINARI on July 14, 2013jop.sagepub.comDownloaded from

    http://jop.sagepub.com/http://jop.sagepub.com/http://jop.sagepub.com/
  • 7/28/2019 The Role of Serotonin in Cognitive Function

    7/10

    580 Journal of Psychopharmacology 27(7)

    do appear to have differential effects on cognitive function in indi-

    viduals with depression that is separable from effects on depres-

    sive symptoms.

    Cognitive symptoms in depression andantidepressant treatment

    Cognitive symptoms are present in a substantial proportion of

    patients with MDD and often persist after response and remission.

    It is difficult to draw consistent conclusions from the numerous

    studies performed to date, as most were small cross-sectional

    studies using a wide variety of neuropsychological tests. Meta-

    analysis of 15 study samples (N= 644) involving patients with a

    first depressive episode revealed impaired psychomotor speed,

    visual memory and learning, attention, and executive functioning

    compared with healthy matched controls (Lee et al., 2011).

    Impaired executive function was confirmed in a meta-analysis of

    375 depressed patients whose mean performance was almost one

    standard deviation below that of controls (Wagner et al., 2012).

    Moreover, another meta-analysis in remitted MDD subjects (11

    studies, 500 subjects) found evidence of residual impairment in

    attention, memory and executive function or estimated global

    cognitive function in patients vs. normal controls in 9 of 11 stud-

    ies (Hasselbalch et al., 2011). Generally, cognitive deficits in

    depression are broad based and extend into social cognition.

    However, it is important to note that in acute depression, cognitive

    performance can be compromised by other core depressive symp-

    toms such as poor motivation (Millan et al., 2012). There is also

    some evidence of correlations between depression severity and

    the likelihood of experiencing cognitive symptoms (McDermott

    and Ebmeier 2009).

    Depression and cognitive impairment may have a bi-directional

    causality. For example, in a meta-review and meta-regression of

    case control and cohort studies, Ownby et al. (2006) found that ahistory of depression produced a twofold increase in risk for

    subsequent development of Alzheimers disease. How far the

    converse relationship holds is unclear. For example, a recent meta-

    analysis found that patients with clinical dementia had a threefold

    risk of experiencing depression. However, those with milder

    cognitive impairment did not seem at greater risk of suffering

    mood disorder (Huang et al., 2011).

    Efforts to determine whether cognitive symptoms are present

    before onset of depressive mood are inconclusive. One study

    comparing healthy subjects aged 1620 with and without a family

    history of MDD showed that overactivity, as measured by BOLD

    response in brain regions associated with working memory, repre-

    sent a marker for vulnerability to MDD (Mannie et al., 2010).

    However, cognitive function assessments in adolescents with a

    history MDD have yielded mixed results (Castaneda et al., 2008;

    Hermens et al., 2011; Maalouf et al., 2011).

    There is some evidence suggesting that cognitive symptoms

    of depression do improve with antidepressant treatment.

    Generally, the literature suggests that antidepressant treatment

    improves cognitive function in depressed patients over time.

    However, even after many months of treatment, cognitive impair-

    ments in a range of domains are still detectable (Alexopoulos

    et al., 2003; Hasselbalch et al., 2011, 2012; Herrera-Guzman

    et al., 2008; McClintock et al., 2011) and are associated with

    impaired psychosocial functioning (Jaeger et al., 2006; McCall

    and Dunn 2003). A recent meta-analysis by Hasselbalch et al.

    (2011) found that residual deficits in processing speed and cogni-

    tive flexibility related to the cognitive domain of attention. The

    STAR*D study found impaired concentration was one of the most

    frequent residual symptoms in patients who showed an overall

    treatment response (McClintock et al., 2011). These clinical find-

    ings should be considered in the context of animal studies show-

    ing that SSRI treatment, for example, facilitates neuroplasticity

    and neurogenesis in the hippocampus (Malberg et al., 2000;Santarelli et al., 2003). Thus, despite the fact that longer-term

    treatment with antidepressants appears to promote the kind of

    neurobiological changes that would enhance cognitive function,

    many depressed patients continue to show evidence of persistent

    cognitive impairment even after long durations of therapy.

    In a longitudinal study, Herrera-Guzman et al. (2010) found

    that patients with MDD who achieved remission with escitalo-

    pram (n = 36) or the serotoninnorepinephrine reuptake inhibitor

    (SNRI) duloxetine (n = 37) showed improved episodic memory,

    working memory, sustained attention and planning at the end of

    24 weeks of therapy. However, patients in the duloxetine group

    showed consistently better cognitive outcomes than those in the

    escitalopram group (Herrera-Guzman et al., 2009). Cognitiveoutcomes were assessed again 24 weeks after treatment with-

    drawal (Herrera-Guzman et al., 2010). At that time, the symptom

    severity continued to improve from baseline throughout the

    post-treatment phase. Moreover, during the post-treatment phase,

    memory function continued to improve in those previously

    treated with duloxetine, whereas the improvement in memory

    remained stable in the group receiving escitalopram prior to

    treatment withdrawal (Herrera-Guzman et al., 2010).

    Quetiapine, an atypical antipsychotic agent, has antagonist

    properties at 5-HT1A and 5-HT2A receptors as well as at dopamine

    (D) 1 and D2, histamine 1, alpha-1- and -2-adrenergic and mus-

    carinic-1 receptors. A small study (N= 18) tested the hypothesis

    that patients who had cognitive symptoms and did not respond to

    an SSRI alone would benefit from augmentation with quetiapine,in part through its dopamine antagonism. However, only small

    benefit in cognitive function was observed (Olver et al., 2008).

    Ongoing trials

    Several available antidepressants alone or in combination are

    currently in clinical trials to evaluate their procognitive effects

    in patients with MDD. The effects of the SNRI desvenlafaxine

    on cognition and work productivity are being investigated in

    adults with depression who are aged 1955 years (clinical trial

    NCT01468610). A second trial is evaluating the effec ts of

    this agent on white matter structure in a similar population

    (NCT01492621). One trial is assessing the effects of augment-

    ing citalopram with methylphenidate, which acts through inhi-

    bition of both dopamine and norepinephrine reuptake in older

    patients (aged 60 years) (NCT00602290). The amphetamine

    prodrug lisdexamfetamine, whose active metabolite also inhibits

    dopamine and norepinephrine reuptake, is being studied in

    another trial involving patients aged 1865 years who only par-

    tially responded to an SNRI or an SSRI (NCT01148979).

    A new multimodal serotonergic compound, vortioxetine (Lu

    AA21004), is in development for treatment of adults with MDD.

    Preclinical data show that vortioxetine functions as a 5-HT3,

    5-HT7, and 5-HT1D receptor antagonist, a 5-HT1A receptor agonist,

    a 5-HT1B receptor partial agonist, and an inhibitor of the 5-HT

    at HINARI on July 14, 2013jop.sagepub.comDownloaded from

    http://jop.sagepub.com/http://jop.sagepub.com/http://jop.sagepub.com/
  • 7/28/2019 The Role of Serotonin in Cognitive Function

    8/10

    Cowen and Sherwood 581

    transporter in vitro (Bang-Andersen et al., 2011; Westrich et al.,

    2012). In a study of 453 patients 65 years of age with recurrent

    MDD, vortioxetine improved baseline episodic memory (stand-

    ardized effect size, 0.27 for acquisition and 0.24 for delayed

    recall,p < 0.05) and psychomotor speed (standardized effect size,

    0.25,p < 0.05) (Katona et al., 2012). Two trials are underway to

    determine whether this cognitive benefit can be replicated in

    adults aged 1865 years.

    Conclusions

    Available evidence indicates that 5-HT plays in a role in modulat-

    ing cognitive function, even though the effects of global 5-HT

    manipulation on learning, memory and executive function (for

    example, TRP depletion or SSRI treatment) in healthy volunteers

    are not particularly robust. This may be due in part to the complex

    and differing roles of various 5-HT receptor subtypes in cognition.

    However, it does appear that lowering 5-HT levels though TRP

    depletion reliably impairs memory consolidation. Although animal

    studies reveal clear actions of certain 5-HT receptor subtype manip-

    ulations on aspects of learning and memory, there are fewer studiesof selective 5-HT ligands in humans, in whom effects appear mod-

    est and often differ between healthy participants and patient groups.

    It seems clearer that depressive disorders are associated with

    significant and broad-based cognitive impairment, which leads to

    the functional disability seen in patients with acute depression.

    This presumably also makes it more difficult for severely depressed

    patients to utilize certain forms of psychotherapy. Although anti-

    depressant treatment and symptomatic improvement lead to some

    resolution of cognitive deficit, current studies suggest that abnor-

    malities in learning and memory and attention may persist even

    after many months of treatment. These impairments are apparently

    associated with occupational and social dysfunction.

    The clinical data suggest that cognitive impairment in depres-sion is an important target for treatment that is not satisfactorily

    managed by current antidepressant medications. It may be that, as

    in patients with schizophrenia, cognitive dysfunction in depression

    is neurobiologically distinct from other major symptom domains

    that is, not purely secondary to low mood (though there does

    appear to be a correlation between acute depression severity and

    cognitive impairment). If this is the case, additional pharmacologi-

    cal approaches may be needed. The ability of certain 5-HT recep-

    tor subtypes to influence cognition makes these receptors plausible

    targets for future pharmacological treatments designed to improve

    both the emotional and cognitive aspects of depression.

    Acknowledgements

    This review was sponsored by the Takeda Pharmaceutical Company, Ltd,

    as part of a joint clinical development program with H. Lundbeck A/S. Dr.

    Cowen and Ann C. Sherwood, PhD (medical writer) drafted and reviewed

    successive versions of the manuscript. Editorial support, including styling

    and editing for journal submission, was provided by The Medicine Group,

    New Hope, Pennsylvania.

    Conflict of interest

    PJ Cowen is a member of an advisory board for Lundbeck and has pro-

    vided expert advice to legal representatives of GlaxoSmithKline. He

    received no remuneration for the preparation of the paper.

    Ann C Sherwood has no disclosures or conflicts of interest.

    Funding

    This work has been supported by Takeda and H. Lundbeck A/S,

    Deerfield, IL.

    References

    Akhondzadeh S, Mohammadi N, Noroozian M, et al. (2009) Added

    ondansetron for stable schizophrenia: A double blind, placebocontrolled trial. Schizophr Res 107: 206212.

    Albert PR and Francois BL (2010) Modifying 5-HT1A receptor gene

    expression as a new target for antidepressant therapy.Front Neurosci

    4: 35.

    Alexopoulos GS, Raue P and Arean P (2003) Problem-solving therapy

    versus supportive therapy in geriatric major depression with executive

    dysfunction.Am J Geriatr Psychiatry 11: 4652.

    Alhaj HA, Selman M, Jervis V, et al. (2012) Effect of low-dose acute

    tryptophan depletion on the specificity of autobiographical memory

    in healthy subjects with a family history of depression.Psychophar-

    macology (Berl) 222: 285292.

    Almeida S, Glahn DC, Argyropoulos SV, et al. (2010) Acute citalopram

    administration may disrupt contextual information processing in

    healthy males.Eur Psychiatry 25: 8791.

    Bang-Andersen B, Ruhland T, Jorgensen M, et al. (2011) Discovery of1-[2-(2,4-dimethylphenylsulfanyl)phenyl]piperazine (Lu AA21004):

    A novel multimodal compound for the treatment of major depressive

    disorder.J Med Chem 54: 32063221.

    Bonaventure P, Aluisio L, Shoblock J, et al. (2011) Pharmacological

    blockade of serotonin 5-HT(7) receptor reverses working memory

    deficits in rats by normalizing cortical glutamate neurotransmission.

    PLoS One 6: e20210.

    Borkowska A, Drozdz W, Ziolkowska-Kochan M, et al. (2007) Enhancing

    effect of mirtazapine on cognitive functions associated with prefrontal

    cortex in patients with recurrent depression.Neuropsychopharmacol

    Hung9: 131136.

    Castaneda AE, Suvisaari J, Marttunen M, et al. (2008) Cognitive func-

    tioning in a population-based sample of young adults with a history

    of non-psychotic unipolar depressive disorders without psychiatric

    comorbidity.J Affect Disord110: 3645.Ceglia I, Acconcia S, Fracasso C, et al. (2004) Effects of chronic treat-

    ment with escitalopram or citalopram on extracellular 5-HT in the

    prefrontal cortex of rats: Role of 5-HT1A receptors. Br J Pharmacol

    142: 469478.

    Chamberlain SR, Del CN, Dowson J, et al. (2007a) Atomoxetine

    improved response inhibition in adults with attention deficit/

    hyperactivity disorder.Biol Psychiatry 62: 977984.

    Chamberlain SR, Muller U, Blackwell AD, et al. (2006) Neurochemi-

    cal modulation of response inhibition and probabilistic learning in

    humans. Science 311: 861863.

    Chamberlain SR, Muller U, Deakin JB, et al. (2007b) Lack of deleterious

    effects of buspirone on cognition in healthy male volunteers. J Psy-

    chopharmacol21: 210215.

    Cho SJ, Yook K, Kim B, et al. (2011) Mirtazapine augmentation enhances

    cognitive and reduces negative symptoms in schizophrenia patients

    treated with risperidone: A randomized controlled trial.Prog Neuro-

    psychopharmacol Biol Psychiatry 35: 208211.

    Codony X, Vela JM and Ramirez MJ (2011) 5-HT(6) receptor and cogni-

    tion. Curr Opin Pharmacol11: 94100.

    de Almeida AJ and Mengod G (2007) Quantitative analysis of gluta-

    matergic and GABAergic neurons expressing 5-HT(2A) recep-

    tors in human and monkey prefrontal cortex. J Neurochem 103:

    475486.

    Delgado PL, Miller HL, Salomon RM, et al. (1999) Tryptophan-depletion

    challenge in depressed patients treated with desipramine or fluoxetine:

    implications for the role of serotonin in the mechanism of antidepres-

    sant action.Biol Psychiatry 46: 212220.

    at HINARI on July 14, 2013jop.sagepub.comDownloaded from

    http://jop.sagepub.com/http://jop.sagepub.com/http://jop.sagepub.com/http://jop.sagepub.com/
  • 7/28/2019 The Role of Serotonin in Cognitive Function

    9/10

    582 Journal of Psychopharmacology 27(7)

    Depoortere R, Auclair AL, Bardin L, et al. (2010) F15599, a preferential

    post-synaptic 5-HT1A receptor agonist: activity in models of cognition

    in comparison with reference 5-HT1A receptor agonists.Eur Neuro-

    psychopharmacol20: 641654.

    Drueke B, Baetz J, Boecker M, et al. (2009) Differential effects of

    escitalopram on attention: A placebo-controlled, double-blind cross-

    over study.Psychopharmacology (Berl) 207: 213223.

    Drueke B, Boecker M, Schlaegel S, et al. (2010) Serotonergic modula-

    tion of response inhibition and re-engagement? Results of a study in

    healthy human volunteers.Hum Psychopharmacol25: 472480.

    Evers EA, van der Veen FM, van Deursen JA, et al. (2006) The effect

    of acute tryptophan depletion on the BOLD response during perfor-

    mance monitoring and response inhibition in healthy male volunteers.

    Psychopharmacology (Berl) 187: 200208.

    Garcia-Carbonero R and Paz-Ares L (2002) Antibiotics and growth fac-

    tors in the management of fever and neutropenia in cancer patients.

    Curr Opin Hematol9: 215221.

    Haddad AD, Williams JM, McTavish SF, et al. (2009) Low-dose trypto-

    phan depletion in recovered depressed women induces impairments

    in autobiographical memory specificity.Psychopharmacology (Berl)

    207: 499508.

    Harmer CJ, Bhagwagar Z, Cowen PJ, et al. (2002) Acute administration

    of citalopram facilitates memory consolidation in healthy volunteers.Psychopharmacology (Berl) 163: 106110.

    Hasbroucq T, Rihet P, Blin O, et al. (1997) Serotonin and human informa-

    tion processing: fluvoxamine can improve reaction time performance.

    Neurosci Lett229: 204208.

    Hasselbalch BJ, Knorr U, Hasselbalch SG, et al. (2012) Cognitive deficits

    in the remitted state of unipolar depressive disorder.Neuropsychology

    26: 642651.

    Hasselbalch BJ, Knorr U and Kessing LV (2011) Cognitive impairment

    in the remitted state of unipolar depressive disorder: A systematic

    review.J Affect Disord134: 2031.

    Hayward G, Goodwin GM, Cowen PJ, et al. (2005) Low-dose tryptophan

    depletion in recovered depressed patients induces changes in cogni-

    tive processing without depressive symptoms. Biol Psychiatry 57:

    517524.

    Hermens DF, Redoblado Hodge MA, Naismith SL, et al. (2011) Neuro-psychological clustering highlights cognitive differences in young

    people presenting with depressive symptoms.J Int Neuropsychol Soc

    17: 267276.

    Herrera-Guzman I, Gudayol-Ferre E, Herrera-Abarca JE, et al. (2010)

    Major Depressive Disorder in recovery and neuropsychological

    functioning: Effects of selective serotonin reuptake inhibitor and

    dual inhibitor depression treatments on residual cognitive deficits in

    patients with Major Depressive Disorder in recovery.J Affect Disord

    123: 341350.

    Herrera-Guzman I, Gudayol-Ferre E, Herrera-Guzman D, et al. (2009)

    Effects of selective serotonin reuptake and dual serotonergic-

    noradrenergic reuptake treatments on memory and mental processing

    speed in patients with major depressive disorder.J Psychiatr Res 43:

    855863.

    Herrera-Guzman I, Gudayol-Ferre E, Lira-Mandujano J, et al. (2008)Cognitive predictors of treatment response to bupropion and cogni-

    tive effects of bupropion in patients with major depressive disorder.

    Psychiatry Res 160: 7282.

    Hodges H, Sowinski P, Turner JJ, et al. (1996) Comparison of the effects

    of the 5-HT3 receptor antagonists WAY-100579 and ondansetron on

    spatial learning in the water maze in rats with excitotoxic lesions of

    the forebrain cholinergic projection system. Psychopharmacology

    (Berl) 125: 146161.

    Huang CQ, Wang ZR, Li YH, et al. (2011) Cognitive function and risk

    for depression in old age: A meta-analysis of published literature.Int

    Psychogeriatr23: 516525.

    Jaeger J, Berns S, Uzelac S, et al. (2006) Neurocognitive deficits and

    disability in major depressive disorder.Psychiatry Res 145: 3948.

    Jensen NH, Cremers TI and Sotty F (2010) Therapeutic potential of

    5-HT2C receptor ligands. ScientificWorldJournal10: 18701885.

    Katona C, Hansen T and Olsen C (2012) A randomized, double-blind,

    placebo-controlled, duloxetine-referenced, fixed-dose study compar-

    ing the efficacy and safety of Lu AA21004 in elderly patients with

    major depressive disorder.Int Clin Psychopharmacol27: 215223.

    Kerestes R, Ladouceur CD, Meda S, et al. (2012) Abnormal prefron-

    tal activity subserving attentional control of emotion in remitted

    depressed patients during a working memory task with emotional dis-

    tracters.Psychol Med42: 2940.

    King MV, Marsden CA and Fone KC (2008) A role for the 5-HT(1A),

    5-HT4 and 5-HT6 receptors in learning and memory. Trends Phar-

    macol Sci 29: 482492.

    Lash TL, Silliman RA, Guadagnoli E, et al. (2000) The effect of less than

    definitive care on breast carcinoma recurrence and mortality. Cancer

    89: 17391747.

    Lee RS, Hermens DF, Porter MA, et al. (2011) A meta-analysis of cog-

    nitive deficits in first-episode Major Depressive Disorder. J Affect

    Disord140: 1131242.

    Levkovitz Y, Arnest G, Mendlovic S, et al. (2005) The effect of ondanse-

    tron on memory in schizophrenic patients.Brain Res Bull65: 291295.

    Liem-Moolenaar M, Rad M, Zamuner S, Cohen AF, et al. (2011) Central

    nervous system effects of the interaction between risperidone (singledose) and the 5-HT6 antagonist SB742457 (repeated doses) in healthy

    men.Br J Clin Pharmacol71: 907916.

    Maalouf FT, Brent D, Clark L, et al. (2011) Neurocognitive impairment in

    adolescent major depressive disorder: State vs. trait illness markers.

    J Affect Disord133: 625632.

    Malberg JE, Eisch AJ, Nestler EJ, et al. (2000) Chronic antidepressant

    treatment increases neurogenesis in adult rat hippocampus.J Neuro-

    sci 20: 91049110.

    Mannie ZN, Harmer CJ, Cowen PJ, et al. (2010) A functional magnetic

    resonance imaging study of verbal working memory in young people

    at increased familial risk of depression.Biol Psychiatry 67: 471477.

    Marchetti E, Chaillan FA, Dumuis A, et al. (2004) Modulation of mem-

    ory processes and cellular excitability in the dentate gyrus of freely

    moving rats by a 5-HT4 receptors partial agonist, and an antagonist.

    Neuropharmacology 47: 10211035.McCall WV and Dunn AG (2003) Cognitive deficits are associated with

    functional impairment in severely depressed patients.Psychiatry Res

    121: 179184.

    McClintock SM, Husain MM, Wisniewski SR, et al. (2011) Residual

    symptoms in depressed outpatients who respond by 50% but do not

    remit to antidepressant medication. J Clin Psychopharmacol 31:

    180186.

    McDermott LM and Ebmeier KP (2009) A meta-analysis of depression

    severity and cognitive function.J Affect Disord119: 18.

    McKinnon MC, Yucel K, Nazarov A, et al. (2009) A meta-analysis exam-

    ining clinical predictors of hippocampal volume in patients with

    major depressive disorder.J Psychiatry Neurosci 34: 4154.

    Mendelsohn D, Riedel WJ and Sambeth A (2009) Effects of acute tryp-

    tophan depletion on memory, attention and executive functions: A

    systematic review.Neurosci Biobehav Rev 33: 926952.Meneses A (2007) Stimulation of 5-HT1A, 5-HT1B, 5-HT2A/2C, 5-HT3

    and 5-HT4 receptors or 5-HT uptake inhibition: Short- and long-term

    memory.Behav Brain Res 184: 8190.

    Millan MJ, Agid Y, Brune M, et al. (2012) Cognitive dysfunction in psy-

    chiatric disorders: Characteristics, causes and the quest for improved

    therapy.Nat Rev Drug Discov 11: 141168.

    Naismith SL, Longley WA, Scott EM, et al. (2007) Disability in major

    depression related to self-rated and objectively-measured cognitive

    deficits: A preliminary study.BMC Psychiatry 7: 32.

    Nathan PJ, Sitaram G, Stough C, et al. (2000) Serotonin, noradrenaline

    and cognitive function: A preliminary investigation of the acute phar-

    macodynamic effects of a serotonin versus a serotonin and noradrena-

    line reuptake inhibitor.Behav Pharmacol11: 639642.

    at HINARI on July 14, 2013jop.sagepub.comDownloaded from

    http://jop.sagepub.com/http://jop.sagepub.com/http://jop.sagepub.com/http://jop.sagepub.com/
  • 7/28/2019 The Role of Serotonin in Cognitive Function

    10/10

    Cowen and Sherwood 583

    OHanlon JF, Robbe HW, Vermeeren A, et al. (1998) Venlafaxines

    effects on healthy volunteers driving, psychomotor, and vigilance

    performance during 15-day fixed and incremental dosing regimens.

    J Clin Psychopharmacol18: 212221.

    Olver JS, Ignatiadis S, Maruff P, et al. (2008) Quetiapine augmentation

    in depressed patients with partial response to antidepressants. Hum

    Psychopharmacol23: 653660.

    Orsetti M, Dellarole A, Ferri S, et al. (2003) Acquisition, retention, and recall

    of memory after injection of RS67333, a 5-HT(4) receptor agonist, into

    the nucleus basalis magnocellularis of the rat.Learn Mem 10: 420426.

    Ownby RL, Crocco E, Acevedo A, et al. (2006) Depression and risk for

    Alzheimer disease: Systematic review, meta-analysis, and metare-

    gression analysis.Arch Gen Psychiatry 63: 530538.

    Poirier MF, Galinowski A, Amado I, et al. (2004) Double-blind compara-

    tive study of the action of repeated administration of milnacipran

    versus placebo on cognitive functions in healthy volunteers. Hum

    Psychopharmacol19: 17.

    Puig MV and Gulledge AT (2011) Serotonin and prefrontal cortex func-

    tion: Neurons, networks, and circuits.Mol Neurobiol44: 449464.

    Ramaekers JG, Muntjewerff ND and OHanlon JF (1995) A comparative

    study of acute and subchronic effects of dothiepin, fluoxetine and

    placebo on psychomotor and actual driving performance. Br J Clin

    Pharmacol39: 397404.Riedel WJ, Eikmans K, Heldens A, et al. (2005) Specific serotonergic

    reuptake inhibition impairs vigilance performance acutely and after

    subchronic treatment.J Psychopharmacol19: 1220.

    Roth BL, Hanizavareh SM and Blum AE (2004) Serotonin receptors rep-

    resent highly favorable molecular targets for cognitive enhancement

    in schizophrenia and other disorders. Psychopharmacology (Berl)

    174: 1724.

    Russo O, Cachard-Chastel M, Riviere C, et al. (2009) Design, synthesis,

    and biological evaluation of new 5-HT4 receptor agonists: Applica-

    tion as amyloid cascade modulators and potential therapeutic utility in

    Alzheimers disease.J Med Chem 52: 22142225.

    Sabbagh MN (2009) Drug development for Alzheimers disease: Where

    are we now and where are we headed? Am J Geriatr Pharmacother

    7: 167185.

    Santana N, Bortolozzi A, Serrats J, et al. (2004) Expression of seroto-nin1A and serotonin2A receptors in pyramidal and GABAergic neu-

    rons of the rat prefrontal cortex. Cereb Cortex 14: 11001109.

    Santarelli L, Saxe M, Gross C, et al. (2003) Requirement of hippocampal

    neurogenesis for the behavioral effects of antidepressants. Science

    301: 805809.

    Schmitt JA, Ramaekers JG, Kruizinga MJ, et al. (2002) Additional dopa-

    mine reuptake inhibition attenuates vigilance impairment induced by

    serotonin reuptake inhibition in man.J Psychopharmacol16: 207214.

    Schoning S, Zwitserlood P, Engelien A, et al. (2009) Working-memory

    fMRI reveals cingulate hyperactivation in euthymic major depression.

    Hum Brain Mapp 30: 27462756.

    Selva-Vera G, Balanza-Martinez V, Salazar-Fraile J, et al. (2010) The

    switch from conventional to atypical antipsychotic treatment should

    not be based exclusively on the presence of cognitive deficits. A pilot

    study in individuals with schizophrenia.BMC Psychiatry 10: 47.Siepmann M, Grossmann J, Muck-Weymann M, et al. (2003) Effects of

    sertraline on autonomic and cognitive functions in healthy volunteers.

    Psychopharmacology (Berl) 168: 293298.

    Siepmann T, Mueck-Weymann M, Oertel R, et al. (2008) The effects of

    venlafaxine on cognitive functions and quantitative EEG in healthy

    volunteers.Pharmacopsychiatry 41: 146150.

    Siuciak JA, Chapin DS, McCarthy SA, et al. (2007) CP-809,101, a selec-

    tive 5-HT2C agonist, shows activity in animal models of antipsychotic

    activity.Neuropharmacology 52: 279290.

    Stenberg JH, Terevnikov V, Joffe M, et al. (2010) Effects of add-on

    mirtazapine on neurocognition in schizophrenia: A double-blind,

    randomized, placebo-controlled study.Int J Neuropsychopharmacol

    13: 433441.

    Stenberg JH, Terevnikov V, Joffe M, et al. (2011) More evidence on

    proneurocognitive effects of add-on mirtazapine in schizophrenia.

    Prog Neuropsychopharmacol Biol Psychiatry 35: 10801086.

    Sumiyoshi T, Matsui M, Yamashita I, et al. (2000) Effect of adjunctive

    treatment with serotonin-1A agonist tandospirone on memory func-

    tions in schizophrenia.J Clin Psychopharmacol20: 386388.

    Sumiyoshi T, Matsui M, Yamashita I, et al. (2001) The effect of tandospi-

    rone, a serotonin(1A) agonist, on memory function in schizophrenia.

    Biol Psychiatry 49: 861868.

    Sumiyoshi T, Park S, Jayathilake K, et al. (2007) Effect of buspirone, a

    serotonin1A partial agonist, on cognitive function in schizophrenia:

    A randomized, double-blind, placebo-controlled study. Schizophr Res

    95: 158168.

    Takahashi M, Iwamoto K, Kawamura Y, et al. (2010) The effects of

    acute treatment with tandospirone, diazepam, and placebo on

    driving performance and cognitive function in healthy volunteers.

    Hum Psychopharmacol25: 260267.

    Terry AV, Jr., Buccafusco JJ and Bartoszyk GD (2005) Selective

    serotonin 5-HT2A receptor antagonist EMD 281014 improvesdelayed matching performance in young and aged rhesus mon-

    keys.Psychopharmacology (Berl) 179: 725732.

    Terry AV, Jr., Buccafusco JJ, Jackson WJ, et al. (1998) Enhanced delayed

    matching performance in younger and older macaques administered

    the 5-HT4 receptor agonist, RS 17017. Psychopharmacology (Berl)

    135: 407415.

    Terry AV, Jr., Buccafusco JJ, Prendergast MA, et al. (1996) The 5-HT3

    receptor antagonist, RS-56812, enhances delayed matching perfor-

    mance in monkeys.Neuroreport8: 4954.

    van der Veen FM, Evers EA, van Deursen JA, et al. (2006) Acute tryp-

    tophan depletion reduces activation in the right hippocampus during

    encoding in an episodic memory task.Neuroimage 31: 11881196.

    Wagner S, Helmreich I, Lieb K, et al. (2012) A meta-analysis of execu-

    tive dysfunctions in unipolar major depressive disorder (MDD) with-

    out psychotic symptoms and their changes during antidepressanttreatment.Eur Psychiatry 27 Supplement 1:1.

    Walstab J, Rappold G and Niesler B (2010) 5-HT(3) receptors: Role in

    disease and target of drugs.Pharmacol Ther128: 146169.

    Warburton EC, Harrison AA, Robbins TW, et al. (1997) Contrasting

    effects of systemic and intracerebral infusions of the 5-HT1A receptor

    agonist 8-OH-DPAT on spatial short-term working memory in rats.

    Behav Brain Res 84: 247258.

    Westrich L, Pehrson A, Zhong H, et al. (2012) In vitro and in vivo effects

    for the multimodal antidepressant vortioxetine (Lu AA21004) at

    human and rat targets.Int J Psychiatry Clin Pract16: 47.

    Wingen M, Kuypers KP and Ramaekers JG (2007a) The role of 5-HT1a

    and 5-HT2a receptors in attention and motor control: A mechanistic

    study in healthy volunteers.Psychopharmacology (Berl)190: 391400.

    Wingen M, Kuypers KP and Ramaekers JG (2007b) Selective verbal

    and spatial memory impairment after 5-HT1A and 5-HT2A receptorblockade in healthy volunteers pre-treated with an SSRI. J Psycho-

    pharmacol21: 477485.

    Wingen M, Langer S and Ramaekers JG (2006) Verbal memory perfor-

    mance during subchronic challenge with a selective serotonergic and

    a mixed action antidepressant.Hum Psychopharmacol21: 473479.

    Wittorf A, Sickinger S, Wiedemann G, et al. (2008) Neurocognitive

    effects of atypical and conventional antipsychotic drugs in schizo-

    phrenia: A naturalistic 6-month follow-up study. Arch Clin Neuro-

    psychol23: 271282.

    at HINARI on July 14, 2013jop.sagepub.comDownloaded from

    http://jop.sagepub.com/http://jop.sagepub.com/http://jop.sagepub.com/http://jop.sagepub.com/