the Psychology of Social Conflict and Aggression Sydney Symposium in Social Psychology

343

Transcript of the Psychology of Social Conflict and Aggression Sydney Symposium in Social Psychology

Page 1: the Psychology of Social Conflict and Aggression Sydney Symposium in Social Psychology
Page 2: the Psychology of Social Conflict and Aggression Sydney Symposium in Social Psychology

THE PSYCHOLOGY OFSOCIAL CONFLICT

AND AGGRESSION

Page 3: the Psychology of Social Conflict and Aggression Sydney Symposium in Social Psychology

The Sydney Symposium of Social Psychology seriesThis book is Volume 13 in the Sydney Symposium of Social Psychology series. The aim of the Sydney Symposia of Social Psychology is to provide new, integrative insights into key areas of contemporary research. Held every year at the University of New South Wales, Sydney, each symposium deals with an important integrative theme in social psychology, and the invited participants are leading researchers in the field from around the world. Each contribution is extensively discussed during the symposium and is subsequently thoroughly revised into book chapters that are published in the volumes in this series. For further details see the website at www.sydneysymposium.unsw.edu.au

Previous Sydney Symposium of Social Psychology volumes:

SSSP 1. Feeling and Thinking: The Role oF aFFecT in Social cogniTion** ISBN 0-521-64223-X (Edited by J.P. Forgas). Contributors: Robert Zajonc, Jim Blascovich, Wendy Berry Mendes, Craig Smith, Leslie Kirby, Eric Eich, Dawn Macauley, Len Berkowitz, Sara Jaffee, EunKyung Jo, Bartholomeu Troccoli, Leonard Martin, Daniel Gilbert, Timothy Wilson, Herbert Bless, Klaus Fiedler, Joseph Forgas, Carolin Showers, Anthony Greenwald, Mahzarin Banaji, Laurie Rudman, Shelly Farnham, Brian Nosek, Marshall Rosier, Mark Leary, Paula Niedenthal, Jamin Halberstadt.

SSSP 2. The Social Mind: cogniTive and MoTivaTional aSPecTS oF inTeRPeRSonal BehavioR** ISBN 0-521-77092-0 (Edited by J.P. Forgas, K.D. Williams, & L. Wheeler). Contributors: William & Claire McGuire, Susan Andersen, Roy Baumeister, Joel Cooper, Bill Crano, Garth Fletcher, Joseph Forgas, Pascal Huguet, Mike Hogg, Martin Kaplan, Norb Kerr, John Nezlek, Fred Rhodewalt, Astrid Schuetz, Constantine Sedikides, Jeffry Simpson, Richard Sorrentino, Dianne Tice, Kip Williams, Ladd Wheeler.

SSSP 3. Social inFluence: diRecT and indiRecT PRoceSSeS* ISBN 1-84169-038-4 (Edited by J.P. Forgas & K.D. Williams). Contributors: Robert Cialdini, Eric Knowles, Shannon Butler, Jay Linn, Bibb Latane, Martin Bourgeois, Mark Schaller, Ap Dijksterhuis, James Tedeschi, Richard Petty, Joseph Forgas, Herbert Bless, Fritz Strack, Eva Walther, Sik Hung Ng, Thomas Mussweiler, Kipling Williams, Lara Dolnik, Charles Stangor, Gretchen Sechrist, John Jost, Deborah Terry, Michael Hogg, Stephen Harkins, Barbara David, John Turner, Robin Martin, Miles Hewstone, Russell Spears, Tom Postmes, Martin Lea, Susan Watt.

SSSP 4. The Social SelF: cogniTive, inTeRPeRSonal, and inTeRgRouP PeRSPecTiveS** ISBN 1-84169-062-7 (Edited by J.P. Forgas & K.D. Williams). Contributors: Eliot R. Smith, Thomas Gilovich, Monica Biernat,

Page 4: the Psychology of Social Conflict and Aggression Sydney Symposium in Social Psychology

Joseph P. Forgas, Stephanie J. Moylan, Edward R. Hirt, Sean M. McCrea, Frederick Rhodewalt, Michael Tragakis, Mark Leary, Roy F. Baumeister, Jean M. Twenge, Natalie Ciarocco, Dianne M. Tice, Jean M. Twenge, Brandon J. Schmeichel, Bertram F. Malle, William Ickes, Marianne LaFrance, Yoshihisa Kashima, Emiko Kashima, Anna Clark, Marilynn B. Brewer, Cynthia L. Pickett, Sabine Otten, Christian S. Crandall, Diane M. Mackie, Joel Cooper, Michael Hogg, Stephen C. Wright, Art Aron, Linda R. Tropp, Constantine Sedikides.

SSSP 5. Social JudgMenTS: iMPliciT and exPliciT PRoceSSeS** ISBN 0-521-82248-3. (Edited by J.P. Forgas, K.D. Williams, & W. Von Hippel). Contributors: Herbert Bless, Marilynn Brewer, David Buss, Tanya Chartrand, Klaus Fiedler, Joseph Forgas, David Funder, Adam Galinsky, Martie Haselton, Denis Hilton, Lucy Johnston, Arie Kruglanski, Matthew Lieberman, John McClure, Mario Mikulincer, Norbert Schwarz, Philip Shaver, Diederik Stapel, Jerry Suls, William von Hippel, Michaela Waenke, Ladd Wheeler, Kipling Williams, Michael Zarate.

SSSP 6. Social MoTivaTion: conSciouS and unconSciouS PRoceSSeS** ISBN 0-521-83254-3 (Edited by J.P. Forgas, K.D. Williams, & S.M. Laham). Contributors: Henk Aarts, Ran Hassin,Trish Devine, Joseph Forgas, Jens Forster, Nira Liberman, Judy Harackiewicz, Leanne Hing, Mark Zanna, Michael Kernis, Paul Lewicki, Steve Neuberg, Doug Kenrick, Mark Schaller, Tom Pyszczynski, Fred Rhodewalt, Jonathan Schooler, Steve Spencer, Fritz Strack, Roland Deutsch, Howard Weiss, Neal Ashkanasy, Kip Williams, Trevor Case, Wayne Warburton, Wendy Wood, Jeffrey Quinn, Rex Wright, Guido Gendolla.

SSSP 7. The Social ouTcaST: oSTRaciSM, Social excluSion, ReJecTion, and Bullying* ISBN 1-84169-424-X (Edited by K.D. Williams, J.P Forgas, & W. Von Hippel). Contributors: Kipling D. Williams, Joseph P. Forgas, William von Hippel, Lisa Zadro, Mark R. Leary, Roy F. Baumeister, and C. Nathan DeWall, Geoff MacDonald, Rachell Kingsbury, Stephanie Shaw, John T. Cacioppo, Louise C. Hawkley, Naomi I. Eisenberger Matthew D. Lieberman, Rainer Romero-Canyas, Geraldine Downey, Jaana Juvonen, Elisheva F. Gross, Kristin L. Sommer, Yonata Rubin, Susan T. Fiske, Mariko Yamamoto, Jean M. Twenge, Cynthia L. Pickett, Wendi L. Gardner, Megan Knowles, Michael A. Hogg, Julie Fitness, Jessica L. Lakin, Tanya L. Chartrand, Kathleen R. Catanese and Dianne M. Tice, Lowell Gaertner, Jonathan Iuzzini, Jaap W. Ouwerkerk, Norbert L. Kerr, Marcello Gallucci, Paul A. M. Van Lange, Marilynn B. Brewer.

SSSP 8. aFFecT in Social Thinking and BehavioR* ISBN 1-84169-454-2 (Edited by J.P. Forgas). Contributors: Joseph P. Forgas, Carrie Wyland, Simon M. Laham, Martie G. Haselton Timothy Ketelaar, Piotr Winkielman, John T. Cacioppo, Herbert Bless, Klaus Fiedler, Craig A. Smith, Bieke David, Leslie D. Kirby, Eric Eich, Dawn Macaulay, Gerald L. Clore, Justin Storbeck, Roy F. Baumeister, Kathleen D. Vohs, Dianne M. Tice, Dacher Keltner, E.J. Horberg, Christopher Oveis, Elizabeth W. Dunn, Simon M. Laham, Constantine Sedikides, Tim Wildschut, Jamie Arndt, Clay Routledge, Yaacov Trope, Eric R. Igou, Chris Burke, Felicia A. Huppert, Ralph Erber, Susan Markunas, Joseph P. Forgas, Joseph Ciarrochi, John T. Blackledge, Janice R. Kelly, Jennifer R.Spoor, John G. Holmes, Danu B. Anthony.

Page 5: the Psychology of Social Conflict and Aggression Sydney Symposium in Social Psychology

SSSP 9. evoluTion and The Social Mind* ISBN 1-84169-458-0 (Edited by J.P. Forgas, M.G. Haselton, & W. Von Hippel). Contributors: William von Hippel, Martie Haselton, Joseph P. Forgas, R.I.M. Dunbar, Steven W. Gangestad, Randy Thornhill, Douglas T. Kenrick, Andrew W. Delton, Theresa E. Robertson, D. Vaughn Becker, Steven L. Neuberg, Phoebe C. Ellsworth, Ross Buck, Joseph P. Forgas, Paul B.T. Badcock, Nicholas B. Allen, Peter M. Todd, Jeffry A. Simpson, Jonathon LaPaglia, Debra Lieberman, Garth J. O. Fletcher, Nickola C. Overall, Abraham P. Buunk, Karlijn Massar, Pieternel Dijkstra, Mark Van Vugt, Rob Kurzban, Jamin Halberstadt, Oscar Ybarra, Matthew C. Keller, Emily Chan, Andrew S. Baron, Jeffrey Hutsler, Stephen Garcia, Jeffrey Sanchez-Burks, Kimberly Rios Morrison, Jennifer R. Spoor, Kipling D. Williams, Mark Schaller, Lesley A. Duncan.

SSSP 10. Social RelaTionShiPS: cogniTive, aFFecTive, and MoTivaTional PRoceSSeS* ISBN 978-1-84169-715-4 (Edited by J.P. Forgas & J. Fitness). Contributors: Joseph P. Forgas, Julie Fitness, Elaine Hatfield, Richard L. Rapson, Gian C. Gonzaga, Martie G. Haselton, Phillip R. Shaver, Mario Mikulincer, David P. Schmitt, Garth J.O. Fletcher, Alice D. Boyes, Linda K. Acitelli, Margaret S. Clark, Steven M. Graham, Erin Williams, Edward P. Lemay, Christopher R. Agnew, Ximena B. Arriaga, Juan E. Wilson, Marilynn B. Brewer, Jeffry A. Simpson, W. Andrew Collins, SiSi Tran, Katherine C. Haydon, Shelly L. Gable, Patricia Noller, Susan Conway, Anita Blakeley-Smith, Julie Peterson, Eli J. Finkel, Sandra L. Murray, Lisa Zadro, Kipling D. Williams, Rowland S. Miller.

SSSP 11. PSychology oF SelF-RegulaTion: cogniTive, aFFecTive, and MoTivaTional PRoceSSeS* ISBN 978-1-84872-842-4 (Edited by J.P. Forgas, R. Baumeister, & D.M. Tice). Contributors: Joseph P. Forgas, Roy F. Baumeister, Dianne M. Tice, Jessica L. Alquist, Carol Sansone, Malte Friese, Michaela Wänke, Wilhelm Hofmann, Constantine Sedikides, Christian Unkelbach, Henning Plessner, Daniel Memmert, Charles S. Carver, Michael F. Scheier, Gabriele Oettingen, Peter M. Gollwitzer, Jens Förster, Nira Liberman, Ayelet Fishbach, Gráinne M. Fitzsimons, Justin Friesen, Edward Orehek, Arie W. Kruglanski, Sander L. Koole, Thomas F. Denson, Klaus Fiedler, Matthias Bluemke, Christian Unkelbach, Hart Blanton, Deborah L. Hall, Kathleen D. Vohs, Jannine D. Lasaleta, Bob Fennis, William von Hippel, Richard Ronay, Eli J. Finkel, Daniel C. Molden, Sarah E. Johnson, Paul W. Eastwick.

SSSP 12. PSychology oF aTTiTudeS and aTTiTude change* ISBN 978-1-84872-908-7 (Edited by J.P. Forgas, J. Cooper, & W.D. Crano). Contributors: William D. Crano, Joel Cooper, Joseph P. Forgas, Blair T. Johnson, Marcella H. Boynton, Alison Ledgerwood, Yaacov Trope, Eva Walther, Tina Langer, Klaus Fiedler, Steven J. Spencer, Jennifer Peach, Emiko Yoshida, Mark P. Zanna, Allyson L. Holbrook, Jon A. Krosnick, Eddie Harmon-Jones, David M. Amodio, Cindy Harmon-Jones, Michaela Wänke, Leonie Reutner, Kipling D. Williams, Zhansheng Chen, Duane Wegener, Radmila Prislin, Brenda Major, Sarah S. M. Townsend, Frederick Rhodewalt, Benjamin Peterson, Jim Blascovich, Cade McCall.

* Published by Psychology Press** Published by Cambridge University Press

Page 6: the Psychology of Social Conflict and Aggression Sydney Symposium in Social Psychology

Psychology Press

New York London

Edited by

Joseph P. ForgasUniversity of New South Wales

Arie W. KruglanskiUniversity of Maryland

Kipling D. Williams Purdue University

THE PSYCHOLOGY OFSOCIAL CONFLICT

AND AGGRESSION

Page 7: the Psychology of Social Conflict and Aggression Sydney Symposium in Social Psychology

Psychology PressTaylor & Francis Group711 Third AvenueNew York, NY 10017

Psychology PressTaylor & Francis Group27 Church RoadHove, East Sussex BN3 2FA

© 2011 by Taylor and Francis Group, LLC

Psychology Press is an imprint of Taylor & Francis Group, an Informa business

International Standard Book Number: 978-1-84872-932-2 (Hardback)

For permission to photocopy or use material electronically from this work, please access www.copyright.com (http://www.copyright.com/) or contact the Copyright Clearance Center, Inc. (CCC), 222 Rosewood Drive, Danvers, MA 01923, 978-750-8400. CCC is a not-for-profit organiza-tion that provides licenses and registration for a variety of users. For organizations that have been granted a photocopy license by the CCC, a separate system of payment has been arranged.

Trademark Notice: Product or corporate names may be trademarks or registered trademarks, and are used only for identification and explanation without intent to infringe.

Visit the Taylor & Francis Web site athttp://www.taylorandfrancis.com

and the Psychology Press Web site athttp://www.psypress.com

This edition published in the Taylor & Francis e-Library, 2011.

To purchase your own copy of this or any of Taylor & Francis or Routledge’scollection of thousands of eBooks please go to www.eBookstore.tandf.co.uk.

ISBN 0-203-80381-7 Master e-book ISBN

Page 8: the Psychology of Social Conflict and Aggression Sydney Symposium in Social Psychology

vii

Contents

Preface xiListofContributors xv

Section i introduction and BaSic iSSueS

1 ThePsychologyofSocialConflictandAggression:Homo AggressivusRevisited 3

Joseph P. Forgas, Arie W. Kruglanski, and Kipling D. Williams

2 AnAttachmentPerspectiveonInterpersonalandIntergroupConflict 19

Mario Mikulincer and Phillip R. Shaver

3 TheLinkBetweenOstracismandAggression 37

Kipling D. Williams and Eric D. Wesselmann

4 IsItAggression?:PerceptionsofandMotivationsforPassiveandPsychologicalAggression 53

Deborah South Richardson and Georgina S. Hammock

5 PushingUptoaPoint:ThePsychologyofInterpersonalAssertiveness 65

Daniel Ames

Section ii cognitive and affective influenceS on conflict and aggreSSion

6 NonconsciousBattlesofWill:ImplicitReactionsAgainsttheGoalsandMotivesofOthers 83

N. Pontus Leander and Tanya L. Chartrand

Page 9: the Psychology of Social Conflict and Aggression Sydney Symposium in Social Psychology

contentSviii

7 UsingBothYourHeadandYourHeart:TheRoleofPerspectiveTakingandEmpathyinResolvingSocialConflict 103

Adam D. Galinsky, Debra Gilin, and William W. Maddux

8 AffectiveInfluencesonthePerception,Management,andResolutionofSocialConflicts 119

Joseph P. Forgas and Hui Bing Tan

9 TheEffectsofAngerandAngerRegulationonNegotiation 139

Thomas F. Denson and Emma C. Fabiansson

10 TheRoleoftheQuestforPersonalSignificanceinMotivatingTerrorism 153

Arie W. Kruglanski and Edward Orehek

Section iii conflict and aggreSSion in relationShipS

11 IntimatePartnerViolence:Cognitive,Affective,andRelationalFactors 167

Chris Eckhardt

12 InterdependentGoalsandRelationshipConflict 185

Gráinne M. Fitzsimons and Joanna E. Anderson

13 SilentRage:WhenBeingOstracizedLeadstoAggression 201

Lisa Zadro

14 TheDoormatEffect:OntheDangersofResolvingConflictviaUnilateralForgiveness 217

Laura B. Luchies and Eli J. Finkel

Section iv Social, cultural, and evolutionary factorS in Social conflict

and aggreSSion

15 TheMaleWarriorHypothesis 233

Mark Van Vugt

Page 10: the Psychology of Social Conflict and Aggression Sydney Symposium in Social Psychology

contentS ix

16 ImplicationsofGlobalClimateChangeforViolenceinDevelopedandDevelopingCountries 249

Craig A. Anderson and Matt DeLisi

17 TheMediaandAggression:FromTVtotheInternet 267

Ed Donnerstein

18 AreSupernaturalBeliefsCommitmentDevicesforIntergroupConflict? 285

Robert Kurzban and John Christner

19 TheEffectofReligiousParticipationonAggressionOverOne’sLifetimeandAcrossGenerations 301

L. Rowell Huesmann, Eric F. Dubow, and Paul Boxer

Index 323

Page 11: the Psychology of Social Conflict and Aggression Sydney Symposium in Social Psychology
Page 12: the Psychology of Social Conflict and Aggression Sydney Symposium in Social Psychology

xi

Preface

Thecollectionofchaptersinthisbookreviewssomeofthemostrecentadvancesinthestudyofsocialconflictandaggression,oneofthemostperennialandpuzzlingtopicsinallofpsychology.Thechaptersrepresentavarietyoftheoreticalorienta­tions,rangingfromevolutionaryapproachesthroughcognitive,affective,neuro­psychological,andclinicaltheoriesallthewaytosocialandculturalanalysesofthenatureandcharacteristicsofconflictandaggression.FewtopicsareasimportantyetaspoorlyunderstoodabouthumannatureasthequestionofwhyHomo sapienshappenstobesuchauniquelyconflict­proneandaggressivespecies.

Thebookaimstoprovideanup­to­dateintegrationofsomeofthemostrecentdevelopmentsinsocialpsychologicalresearchonthisissue,offeringaninforma­tive,scholarly,yetreadableoverviewofrecentadvancesinresearchonthenature,antecedents,management,andconsequencesofinterpersonalandintergroupcon­flictandaggression.Thechaptersincludedhereshareabroadintegrativeorienta­tion andwill argue thathumanconflict isbest understood through the carefulanalysisof thecognitive,affective,andmotivationalprocessesof those involvedinconflictsituations,supplementedbyabroadlybasedunderstandingoftheevo­lutionary, biological, as well as social and cultural contexts within which socialconflictoccurs.

Thebookisdividedintofourparts.SectionIdealswithbasicquestionssuchasthefollowing:Whatroledoearlyattachmentexperiencesplayindetermininghowpeoplemanageanddealwithinterpersonalandintergroupconflict in laterlife?Why is social exclusionandostracism—being ignored and rejectedbyothers—suchanimportantsourceofconflictandaggression,andwhatdetermineswhetherthosewhoareostracizedrespondinprosocialratherthanantisocialwaystotheirpredicament? What are the psychological characteristics of those very commoneverydaybehaviors(e.g.,spitefulness,condescension,derogation)thatfallshortofseriousandintentionalharm­doingyetnecessarilyproduceaversiveconsequences?Whatdetermineshowhardandhowfarpeoplewillpushingettingtheirwaywithothers—inotherwords,whatdeterminesassertiveness?

Thesecondsectionaddressesthecognitive,affective,andmotivationalinflu­ences on how people perceive and manage social conflicts, seeking answers toquestionssuchasthefollowing:Whydopeoplesometimesreactinanadversarialwaytotheinferredgoalsandmotivesofothers?Whatroledoaffectivestatesandmoodsplayinthewaypeopleperceive,manage,andresolvesocialconflicts?Howcanonebestmanageangertoperformoptimallyinnegotiatingsituations?How

Page 13: the Psychology of Social Conflict and Aggression Sydney Symposium in Social Psychology

prefacexii

canweexplaintheapparently irrationalandself­sacrificialviolenceof terroristsandsuicidebombers—isthequestforpositivesocialidentityandpersonalsignifi­canceapossibleexplanation?

SectionIII looksat thewayconflictandaggressionoccur insocial relation­ships,perhapsthemostcommoneverydaysettingforreal­lifeconflictexperiences.Chaptersinthissectioninvestigateanumberofintriguingquestions.Forexample,whyisviolencebetweencouplessooftenexplainedandtreatedintermsoffeministexplanationsthatfocusonmaledominanceratherthanbeingbasedonthebestavailable psychological evidence thatdoesnot support feminist ideology? Whatistheroleoftheinterdependentversusconflictinggoalsofpartnersinproducingrelationshipconflict?Howdopeopleinrelationshipscopewiththeconsequencesofbeingignoredandostracized,oneofthemostcommonreal­lifeconflictstrate­giesinrelationships?Whatroledoesforgivenessplayinconflictmanagementandresolution—coulditbethatforgivenessissometimescounterproductiveandmayresultinsuboptimaloutcomesforvictims?

SectionIVofthebookanalyzesconflictandaggressionintermsoflarge­scaleevolutionary,social,andculturalmechanismsandseeksanswerstoquestionssuchas the following:Howcanwebest explain thealmostuniversal tendency in allhuman societies for tribalism and intergroup violence? Are there evolutionarypressuresforadistinctive“malewarrior”culturetoemerge?Iftheglobalwarminghypothesisisindeedcorrect,whatarethelikelyimplicationsofthepredictedcli­matechangeforinterpersonal,intergroup,andinterculturalconflictinthedecadestocome?Whataretheconsequencesofviolencepresentedinthemediaandespe­ciallyontheInternetfortheprevalenceofconflictandaggressioninoursocieties?Whatistheroleofapparentlyirrational,supernaturalbeliefsinfosteringin­groupcohesionandintergroupconflict?Andfinally,howdoesareligiousupbringingandpracticehelptopreventaggressionandviolenceinlaterlife?

Oneneedstorecognize,ofcourse,thatnosinglebookcouldpossiblyincludeeverything that is interesting and exciting in current research on conflict andaggression.Inselectingandinvitingourcontributors,weaimedtoachieveabroadandvariedcoveragethatisneverthelessrepresentativeofthemajornewdevelop­ments in social psychological research on conflict andaggression. The chaptersincludedhererepresentsomeofthebestexamplesofcleartheorizingandcarefulresearchinthiscriticallyimportantarea.

the originS of thiS Book: the Sydney SympoSium of Social pSychology SerieS

ThisbookisthethirteenthvolumeintheSydneySymposiumofSocialPsychologyseries,heldeveryyear at theUniversityofNewSouthWales (UNSW),Sydney.PerhapsafewwordsareinorderabouttheoriginsofthisvolumeandtheSydneySymposium of Social Psychology series in general. First, we should emphasizethat this is not simply an edited book in the usual sense. The objective of theSydneySymposiaistoprovidenew,integrativeunderstandinginimportantareasofsocialpsychologybyinvitingleadingresearchersinaparticularfieldtoa3­day

Page 14: the Psychology of Social Conflict and Aggression Sydney Symposium in Social Psychology

preface xiii

residentialsymposiuminSydney.Thissymposiumhasreceivedfinancialsupportfrom theUniversityofNewSouthWales and theAustralianResearchCouncil,allowingthecarefulselectionandfundingofasmallgroupofleadingresearchersascontributors.Draftpapersbyallcontributorsarepreparedandcirculatedwellinadvanceofthesymposiumandareplacedonourdedicatedwebsite.Thus,par­ticipantshadanopportunitytoreviewandrevisetheirpapersinlightofeverybodyelse’sdraftcontributionevenbeforetheyarrivedinSydney.

Avitalpartofthepreparationofthisbookhasbeentheintensive3­dayface­to­facemeetingamongall invitedcontributors.SydneySymposiaarecharacter­ized by open, free­ranging, and critical discussion among all participants, withtheobjectiveofexploringpointsofintegrationandcontrastamongtheproposedpapers.Afurtherrevisionofeachchapterispreparedsoonaftereachsymposium,incorporatingmanyofthesharedpointsthatemergedinourdiscussions.Thankstothesecollaborativeprocedures,thebookdoesnotsimplyconsistofasetofchapterspreparedbyresearchersinisolation.Rather,thisSydneySymposiumvolumerep­resentsacollaborativeeffortbyaleadinggroupofinternationalresearchersintenton producing a wide­ranging and up­to­date review of research on the nature,antecedents,andconsequencesofsocialconflictandaggression.

Wehopethatthepublishedpaperswillsucceedinconveyingsomeofthesenseoffunandexcitementweallsharedduringthesymposium.Formoreinformationon theSydneySymposiumseriesanddetailsofourpastand futureprojects (aswellasphotosthatshowourcontributorsinmoreorlessflatteringsituations,andother background information) please see our website (www.sydneysymposium.unsw.edu.au).TwelvepreviousvolumesoftheSydneySymposiumserieshavebeenpublished.AllSydneySymposiumbooksfeatureoriginalcontributionsfromlead­inginternationalresearchersonkeyissuesinsocialpsychology.Detailedinforma­tionaboutourearliervolumescanbefoundontheseriespageinthisbookandalsoonourwebsite.

Givenitsbreadthofcoverage,thepresentbookshouldbeusefulbothasabasicreference book and as an informative textbook tobe used in advanced coursesdealingwithsocialconflictandaggression.Themaintargetaudienceforthisbookcomprises researchers, students, and professionals in all areas of the social andbehavioralsciences,suchassocial,cognitive,clinical,counseling,personality,orga­nizational,forensicandappliedpsychology,andsociology,communicationstudies,andsocialwork.Thebookiswritteninareadableyetscholarlystyle,andstudentsattheundergraduateandatthegraduatelevelshouldfinditanengagingoverviewof thefieldandthususefulasa textbook incoursesdealingwithsocial conflictand aggression. The book should also be of particular interest to people work­inginappliedareaswheredealingwithandunderstandingtheprocessesinvolvedinpreventing,managing,andresolvingsocialconflictandaggressionareimpor­tant,suchasorganizational,forensic,clinical,counseling,educational,sports,andhealthpsychology.

Wewant toexpressour thanks to thepeople andorganizations thathelpedtomaketheSydneySymposiumofSocialPsychologyseries,andthis thirteenthvolumeinparticular,areality.Producingacomplex,multiauthoredbooksuchasthisisalengthyandsometimeschallengingtask.Wehavebeenveryfortunateto

Page 15: the Psychology of Social Conflict and Aggression Sydney Symposium in Social Psychology

prefacexiv

workwithsuchanexcellentandcooperativegroupofcontributors.Ourfirstthanksmustgotothem.Becauseoftheirhelpandprofessionalism,wewereabletofin­ishthisproject inrecordtimeandaheadofschedule.Past friendshipshavenotbeenfrayed,andweareallstillonspeakingterms;indeed,wehopethatworkingtogetheronthisbookhasbeenaspositiveanexperienceforthemasithasbeenforus.

The ideaoforganizing theSydneySymposiaowesmuchtodiscussionswithandencouragementbyKevinMcConkey,PeterLovibond,andnumerousothersattheUNSW.OurpastandpresentcolleaguesattheSchoolofPsychologyatUNSWsuchasMarilynnBrewer,KipWilliams,BillvonHippel,andTomDensonandfriendsandcolleaguesfromfurtherafieldhavehelpedwithadvice,support,andsheerhardworktosharetheburdenofpreparingandorganizingthesymposiumandtheensuingbook.WeareespeciallygratefultoSuellenandBillCrano,whohelpedinmorewaysthanwecouldlisthere.Wealsowishtoacknowledgefinan­cialsupportfromtheAustralianResearchCouncilandUNSW,supportthatwasofcourseessentialtogetthisprojectofftheground.Mostofall,wearegratefulfortheloveandsupportofourfamilieswhohaveputupwithusduringthemanymonthsofworkthatwentintoproducingthisbook.

JosephP.Forgas,ArieW.Kruglanski,andKiplingD.WilliamsSydney, Australia

Page 16: the Psychology of Social Conflict and Aggression Sydney Symposium in Social Psychology

xv

ListofContributorsDanielAmesColumbiaUniversityNewYork,NewYork,USA

CraigA.AndersonIowaStateUniversityAmes,Iowa,USA

JoannaE.AndersonUniversityofWaterlooWaterloo,Ontario,Canada

PaulBoxerRutgersUniversityNewark,NewJersey,USAandUniversityofMichiganAnnArbor,Michigan,USA

TanyaL.ChartrandDukeUniversityDurham,NorthCarolina,USA

JohnChristnerUniversityofPennsylvaniaPhiladelphia,Pennsylvania,USA

MattDeLisiIowaStateUniversityAmes,Iowa,USA

ThomasF.DensonUniversityofNewSouthWalesSydney,Australia

EdDonnersteinUniversityofArizonaTucson,Arizona,USA

EricF.DubowBowlingGreenStateUniversityBowlingGreen,Ohio,USAandUniversityofMichiganAnnArbor,Michigan,USA

ChrisEckhardtPurdueUniversityWestLafayette,Indiana,USA

EmmaC.FabianssonUniversityofNewSouthWalesSydney,Australia

EliJ.FinkelNorthwesternUniversityEvanston,Illinois,USA

GráinneM.FitzsimonsUniversityofWaterlooWaterloo,Ontario,Canada

JosephP.ForgasUniversityofNewSouthWalesSydney,Australia

AdamD.GalinskyNorthwesternUniversityEvanston,Illinois,USA

Page 17: the Psychology of Social Conflict and Aggression Sydney Symposium in Social Psychology

liSt of contriButorSxvi

DebraGilinSaintMary’sUniversityHalifax,NovaScotia,Canada

GeorginaS.HammockAugustaStateUniversityAugusta,Georgia,USA

L.RowellHuesmannUniversityofMichiganAnnArbor,Michigan,USA

ArieW.KruglanskiUniversityofMarylandCollegePark,Maryland,USA

RobertKurzbanUniversityofPennsylvaniaPhiladelphia,Pennsylvania,USA

N.PontusLeanderDukeUniversityDurham,NorthCarolina,USA

LauraB.LuchiesNorthwesternUniversityEvanston,Illinois,USA

WilliamW.MadduxINSEADFontainebleau,France

MarioMikulincerInterdisciplinaryCenter(IDC),

HerzlyiaHerzliya,Israel

EdwardOrehekUniversityofGroningenGroningen,theNetherlands

DeborahSouthRichardsonAugustaStateUniversityAugusta,Georgia,USA

PhillipR.ShaverUniversityofCalifornia,DavisDavis,California,USA

HuiBingTanUniversityofNewSouthWalesSydney,Australia

MarkVanVugtUniversityofAmsterdamAmsterdam,theNetherlands

EricD.WesselmannPurdueUniversityWestLafayette,Indiana,USA

KiplingD.WilliamsPurdueUniversityWestLafayette,Indiana,USA

LisaZadroUniversityofSydneySydney,Australia

Page 18: the Psychology of Social Conflict and Aggression Sydney Symposium in Social Psychology

Section IIntroduction and Basic Issues

Page 19: the Psychology of Social Conflict and Aggression Sydney Symposium in Social Psychology
Page 20: the Psychology of Social Conflict and Aggression Sydney Symposium in Social Psychology

3

1ThePsychologyofSocialConflictandAggression

Homo Aggressivus RevisitedJOSEPHP.FORGAS

University of New South Wales

ARIEW.KRUGLANSKIUniversity of Maryland

KIPLINGD.WILLIAMSPurdue University

C onflict andaggressionappear tobe oneof thedefining features ofourspecies.Humansfight,argue,andengageinintraspeciesviolenceataratethatseemstobeuniqueamongspecies.Humanhistoryseemssoreplete

withamazing featsof intraspeciesconflictandviolence that some theoreticianssuchasArthurKoestler(1972)evenbelievedthatkillingourownhasalwaysbeenand continues to be a defining feature of all human societies, from the distanthistoricalpast to thepresent.TheancientMaya thoughtnothingof rippingoutthelivingheartsoftensofthousandsofcaptivesinasingleday;thousandsofcivil­ianswerekilleda fewyearsago inBosniasimplybecause theybelonged to thewrongethnicgroup;Islamicterroristsseemtorejoiceinthemurderofthousandsofinnocentciviliansinthenameofideologiesthatmostofusbroughtupintheliberal,rational,Westerntraditionwouldconsiderbizarreandincomprehensibleatbest(seealsoKruglanski&Orehek;Kurzban&Christner;VanVugt,thisvol­ume).Explainingtheroots,features,andconsequencesofthewayhumanbeingsengageinconflictandaggressionhasthusbeenadefiningconcernforwritersand

Page 21: the Psychology of Social Conflict and Aggression Sydney Symposium in Social Psychology

JoSeph p. forgaS, arie kruglanSki, and kipling d. WilliamS4

philosopherssinceantiquity.Thisvolumeseekstopresentsomeofthemostrecentleading­edgepsychologicalresearchandthinkingonthisperennialtopicfromagroupofdistinguishedinternationalresearchers.

We should also note at the outset, however, that the apparently unlimitedhumancapacityforconflictandviolenceneedstobebalancedagainstourequallyimpressive ability for cooperation and altruism. Notwithstanding the ubiquityof conflict and aggression,humans are also capableof amazing featsof coordi­nation, empathy, and even self­sacrifice, and our species is unique in its abilitytoorganize and integrateextremely large social groups andunits in a way thatlargelyprecludesthedangersofbeingcaughtupindailyconflictandaggression(Dunbar,2008).Mostofuswholiveinmodernindustrializedsocietiesliveuncom­monlypeacefulandsafeliveswherephysicalconflictandaggressionrarelyifevertouchesus(seealsoRichardson&Hammock,thisvolume).Inacuriousway,theverysame impressivehumancapacity forsymbolic thoughtandabstractionthatdrivessomuchinterpersonalandsocialconflictandviolence(seealsoLeanderandChartrand;Fitzsimons&Anderson;Kruglanski&Orehek;VanVugt;Kurzban&Christner,thisvolume)alsoliesattheheartoftheimmensehumancapacityforcooperationandaltruism(seealsoHuesmannetal.,thisvolume).Themainobjec­tiveofthisbookistoreviewandintegratesomeofthemostrecentdevelopmentsinresearchonsocialconflictandaggression,presentingtheworkofaselectgroupofeminentinternationalscholarsinthisfield.

Despitecenturiesofdebate,thereremainfundamentalquestionsaboutthenatureandoriginsofhumanconflictandaggression.Howisconflictgenerated,howdopeoplemanage toresolveanddealwith their interpersonaland inter­groupconflicts,andwhatisthemostappropriatepsychologicalandsocialstrat­egy for managing and limiting the destructive consequencesof social conflictandaggression?Whatroledoevolutionary,cultural,andsocialvariablesplayinthegenerationandresolutionofconflict?Whatare themost importantcogni­tive,affective,andmotivationalmechanismsthatinfluencethewayanindivid­ualexperiencesandrespondstoconflict?Whatcontributioncanpsychologicalresearchonconflictandaggressionmaketounderstandinginterpersonal,rela­tionship,andintergroupconflicts?Thesearejustsomeoftheissuesweintendtoexploreinthisvolume.

Toanswerquestionssuchasthese,thisvolumeissubdividedintofourbasicsections. The first part of the book, after this introductory chapter, addressessomeof thegeneral issuesandtheoriesrelevant toourunderstandingofsocialconflictandaggression(Chapters2–5).Inthesecondsectionofthebook,anum­berofcontributorsconsiderthecognitiveandaffectiveprocessesinvolvedinthewaysocialconflictandaggressionisexperiencedandresolved(Chapters6–10).Thethirdpartofthebookpresentsresearchthatexploresthenatureandconse­quencesofinterpersonalandrelationalconflictandaggression(Chapters11–14).Finally,inthefourthandfinalsectionofthebook,wefocusonthelargerevo­lutionary,social,andculturalvariablesthatinfluencethenatureandoccurrenceofsocialconflictandaggression(Chapters15–19).Wewillbegin,however,withabrieftheoreticalandhistoricalreviewofresearchonconflictandaggressioninsocialpsychology.

Page 22: the Psychology of Social Conflict and Aggression Sydney Symposium in Social Psychology

the pSychology of Social conflict and aggreSSion 5

Studying conflict and aggreSSionThestudyofhumanaggression,theviolenceofpeopleagainsttheirownkind,isalsooneofthemosttime­honoredandfundamentaltopicsofpsychologicalresearchand,beforethat,ofsocialphilosophy.Manyofthegreatthinkersoverthecenturieshaveaddressedthetopic,includingPlato,NiccoloMachiavelli,ThomasHobbes,GeorgSpinoza,andJonathanSwift.Inpsychology,alltheveneratedgrandtheo­ristsofourdisciplinecommentedonhumanaggressionincludingWilliamJames,SigmundFreud,WilliamMcDougall,andKonradLorenz.EvenAlbertEinsteinwasmovedtocommentonhumandestructivenessthatheexplainedintermsoftheinborn“lustforhatredanddestruction.”

Thecentralityofconflictandaggressionasakeytopicforunderstandinghumannatureisevidencedbythefactthatithasbeenstudiedfromanextremelybroadvarietyofpsychologicalperspectives, includingmotivational theoriesof instinct,behavioral theories of learning, cognitive approaches to information processingandattribution,evolutionarytheories,modelsofself­regulationandautomaticity,andbiologicalandneuroscientificvantagepoints.

Aggression and conflict are now also foundational topics in social psychol­ogy,astheywereinsociologyandanthropologybefore.Probablynosingletopicenjoysasmuchcross­disciplinaryinterestasaggression.Majortheoriesandmuchempiricalresearchonthesetopicsemergedfrommanyfields,includingsociology,behavioral genetics, anthropology, ethology, philosophy, literature, and biology.Withinpsychology,everysubdisciplineisrepresented:developmental,clinicalandcounseling,cognitive,neuroscience,humanandanimallearning,motivation,andindustrialandorganizationalpsychology.Themajorgrandtheoriesinpsychologyallweighinonaggression:Freud’spsychoanalytictheory,B.F.Skinner’sbehavior­ism,andCarlRogers’sandAbrahamMaslow’shumanismhaveallproposedexpla­nations for andmechanisms toguardagainst aggression.This all­encompassinginterestisundoubtedlybecauseeventhoughaggressionandconflictareubiquitousamongnearlyallanimalstheyareespeciallysoamonghumans.Asthechaptersinthisbooksuggest,conflictandaggressioncanalternativelybeviewedasfunctionalordysfunctional,canbeanalyzedattheindividual,relational,andsocietallevel,andareoftendiscussedasaconstantsourceofconcernasalegal,political,andsocialproblem.

definitionS of conflict and aggreSSionGiventheubiquityofconflictandaggression,onemaythinkthatattheveryleastwedoknowwhatitisthatwearetalkingabout.Alas,thisisnotthecase.Arecentinternational symposiumonconflict (Kruk,2009) representingnearlyallof therelevantdisciplinesresultedinspiriteddiscussionaboutwhethertherewasevencommonagreement about thedefinitionof aggression.Whereas intent toharmwasacoredefinitionalpropertyusedinmanyofthedisciplines,othersrequiredadditional features such as overt actions, while some denied that intention toharmisevenrelevanttoadefinition(seealsoChapter4).Somefocusedsolelyondirectaggression,whereasothersconsideredindirectandsubtlerforms.So,toask

Page 23: the Psychology of Social Conflict and Aggression Sydney Symposium in Social Psychology

JoSeph p. forgaS, arie kruglanSki, and kipling d. WilliamS6

whetherconflictandaggressionarerelatedpresupposesthateveryonehasamutu­allyagreed­ondefinitionforaggressionandforconflict.Ratherthanimposingcon­sensus,itisprobablybesttoacknowledgetheexistingdiversityofdefinitionsandfoci,aswedohere,so that furtherresearchcandetermine theextent towhichtheseconceptsarerelatedoroperatebydifferentprocesses.

Todeterminetheextentthatconflictandaggressionarerelated,onemustask,“Do factors that increase (or decrease) aggression—however we define it—alsosimilarlyincreaseconflict?”Ifthesamefactorshavesimilareffectsonourmea­suresofbothconstructs, thenwecanclaimadegreeof functionaloverlap.Ifafactorincreasesaggressionbutdecreasesconflict,thenwehaveevidencethatwearetalkingabouttwoverydifferentthings.Theresearchrepresentedinthisbookcertainlysuggeststhatconflictandaggressionareatleastrelated—conflictoften,butnotalways,leadstoaggression,andaggressionisaslikelyasnottoperpetuateandexacerbateconflict.Weshouldalsonote,however,thatwhereasconflictoftenhasfunctional,real­worldoriginsandisthereforeoftenresolvablebynonaggres­siveandrationalmeans,aggressionincontrastisoftenbasedondeeplyseated,uni­versal,andsubconscioushumancharacteristicsthatoftendefyrationalexplanationandresolution(seeespeciallyChapters10,15,and18).

pSychological approacheS to conflict and aggreSSion: a hiStorical overvieW

Ingeneral,psychologicalresearchonconflictandaggressionaddressedthreefun­damentalquestions:(1)Wheredoesconflictandaggressioncomefrom?(2)Whatelicitsit?and(3)Whatmodifiesit?

Where Do Conflict and Aggression Come From?

Severalearlytheoristsstressedtheuniversalandinstinctualnatureofconflictandaggression.Inthisvein,Jamesassumedthathuman“bellicosity”wasbiologicallyrootedandthatpeoplewerethemostformidableofallthebeastsofprey.Freud(1922), in reacting to thehugeatrocitiesofWorldWarI, assumed thathumanshaveaninnateaggressivedrive,thedeathinstinctorthanatos.Inanimaginative,ifnotpoetic(butalsoratherfar­fetched)theoreticalmove,heassumedthat thethanatos stems frompeople’sbasicdrive toescapestimulationandto return tothepeaceandquietoftheinorganicworld.Why,then,dopeoplenotjustgoaheadandkillthemselves?Becauseofthecontraryforceembodiedinthelifeinstinct,oreros.Asaconsequence,thethanatosisdisplaced,andinsteadofkillingthemselvespeoplefindconflictwithandaggressagainstothers;inthiswaytheyfindanoutlettoinstinctualpressuresthatwouldhaveotherwiseledtotheirowndemise.

Inasomewhatsimilarvein,McDougall(1921)postulatedapugnacityinstinctinhisfamousIntroduction to Social Psychology.ForMcDougall,aninstinctwasageneralpropensitytopayattentiontoagivenclassofobjects,toexperienceagivenemotiontothoseobjects,andtoacttowardtheminaparticularmanner.McDougallalsobelievedthattheinstinctualdispositioncouldbemodifiedbylearning,sothat

Page 24: the Psychology of Social Conflict and Aggression Sydney Symposium in Social Psychology

the pSychology of Social conflict and aggreSSion 7

initiallyneutralstimulirepeatedlyassociatedwithoriginalinstigatorscouldcometoexcitetheinstinctiveprocess.Ofcourse,suchinstinct­basedexplanationsreallyamountonlytoasemanticsleightofhand—bycallingaggressionaninstinctwearenoclosertounderstandingitsfundamentalnatureandorigins.

Lateron,KonradLorenz(1966),knownasthe“fatherofethology,”alsoembracedtheinstinctdoctrineintheexplanationofaggression.Ethologicallymindedresearch­ers,basingtheirtheoriesonthecarefulobservationofotherspecies,thussawaggres­sionasaspecies­specificadaptationsystem,aninnatebehavioraltendencythat isultimatelyfunctionalbypromotingthesurvivalofthegroup(asdoalsosomerecentevolutionarymodelsofaggression;seeChapters15and18).Aggressionisreleasedinresponsetospecificstimuli.Lorenz’smodelusesahydraulicsystemmetaphor,wherepent­uppressureisreleasedbytherightkindsofelicitingstimuli.

Lorenz,anavowedpessimistabouttheaggressivetendenciesofourspecies,suggestedthattoreducethelikelihoodofmajor“explosions”ofaggressionitisben­eficialtoengageinslowandcontrolledreleasesofaggressiveenergy,forexample,intheformofcompetitivegamesorsport.Notsurprisingly,suchgrandinsightsbasedongeneralizationsofobservationsofloweranimals,includingtheGreylaggoose,resultedinLorenzbeingmuchvilifiedandridiculed.HisnotionofinstinctwasmuchnarrowerthanthoseofMcDougallandFreud;whereastheytalkedofageneralbutflexibletendencywhoseexpressioncantakedifferentforms,Lorenzassumedthatinstinctsinvolverigidfixedactionpatternsthathavetheirownener­giesandthatarereleasedbyspecificstimuli.

Are instinctnotionsofaggressiondead?Notexactly.McDougall’s approach,thoughheavilycriticizedinhisownday,anticipatedLenBerkowitz’scontempo­raryapproachinwhichthetendencytoaggressisseenasinnate,canbeelicitedsubconsciously,forexamplethroughexposuretoaweapon,butisalsoflexibleandmodifiablebylearning.Inthismodel,allmembersofthespeciesareassumedtopossesstheinnatecapacitytoaggress,justliketheyhavetheinnatecapacityforlanguage—what Steven Pinker (1994) labeled “the language instinct.” The waythisinnateproclivityforconflictandaggressionisexpressedmayvarydependingonavarietyoffactors.JustlikeyoumayexpressthelanguageinstinctbyspeakingFrench,Hungarian,orEnglish,onemayexpressone’saggressiveinstinctbyplay­inghockeyorrugby,spreadingmaliciousrumors,writingnastyreviewsabouttheworkofothers,orengaginginterroristactivities.

What Elicits Conflict and Aggressive Behavior?

Thesituationalcuesandcircumstancesthatelicitconflictandaggressionrepresentthesecondfundamentalareaofinvestigationaddressedbyresearchers.Freud,inhisearlytheorizing,beforeintroducingtheconceptofthethanatos,believedthataggressionarosewhenpleasure­seekingorpain­avoidanceimpulseswerethwarted.McDougallargued(contrarytoLorenz)thatnospecificcategoryofstimulisetsofftheaggressiveprocess.Accordingtohim,theinstigationofaggressionhastodowiththeexperienceoffrustrationorinterferencewithactivitiesdictatedbyotherinstincts.Inthissense,McDougall’sviewisrathersimilartothatofferedbytheearlyworkofFreud.

Page 25: the Psychology of Social Conflict and Aggression Sydney Symposium in Social Psychology

JoSeph p. forgaS, arie kruglanSki, and kipling d. WilliamS8

Intriguingly, the notion that thwarting is at the root of aggression found apowerfulandinfluentialreformulationinthewell­knownfrustration–aggressionhypothesisbyDollard,Doob,Miller,Mowrer,andSears(1939).Thefrustration–aggression link is also central to Berkowitz’s (1993) neoassociationist model ofaggressioninwhichaggressionisassumedtofollowfromanger(seealsoChapter9),andanger,inturn,isassumedtofollowfromsomeunpleasantexperience,suchashavingone’sfootsteppedon,havingsomeonerearendyourcar,orfindingoutthat theclassywineyou justordered in a restaurant tastes likevinegar. In thissense, Berkowitz’s view is related to the notion of thwarting: thwarting of yourwishes to have your foot free of pain, or your taste buds pleasantly stimulatedratherthanshockedandtraumatized.

What Modifies Conflict and Aggression?

IncontrasttotherigidmodelofconflictandaggressionpredictinginflexiblefixedactionpatternsasdescribedbyLorenz’sethologicalapproach,mostcontemporarytheoriesrecognizethatconflictandaggressioninhumansoccurinhighlyflexibleand contextually determined ways and can be modified by a variety of mentalandsituationalfactors.Muchresearchattentionhasbeenpaidtothequestionofwhatmodifiesaggression—indeed,mostofthechaptersfeaturedheredealwiththecognitive(Chapter7),affective(Chapters8and9),motivational(Chapters6and10), andculturalaswell as ideologicalmediatorsofconflictandaggression(Chapters 18 and 19). More specifically, a variety of psychological mechanismshavebeenshowntoinfluenceconflictandaggression.

modeling Albert Bandura’s theoretical work on modeling of aggression hasbeenextensivelyappliedtothequestionofwhetheraggressioninthemediamayormaynotincreasetheviewers’tendencytoaggress.Thescientificconsensusonthispointseemstobethatdepictionofviolenceintheentertainmentmedialegiti­mizesaggressionandincreasesthetendencytoaggress(seealsoChapter17).TheU.S.surgeongeneralcametothisconclusion,andsodidsixprofessionalsocietiesofphysiciansandpsychologists.Despite the impressive scientificconsensus andthestrengthoftheevidenceonwhichitisbased,theentertainmentindustryandthenewsmediaremainlargelyskepticalaboutthesuggestionthatviolenceinthemediahasanyadversesocialeffects.Depictionsofevermoreingeniousformsofconflictandviolenceinthemediacontinueunabated.

catharsis Thequestionofwhetheraggressionhasacatharticeffecthasbeenexaminedbyagreatdealofresearch,andthegeneralanswerseemstobeinthenegative.However,amorenuancedviewsuggeststhataggressionagainstaper­petratorofsomeoffense(whetheraloneorwithothers)canbesatisfyingandmayreduce one’s tendency to aggress against that person, and in this limited senseaggressioniscathartic.Ontheotherhand,suchsatisfactionmayonoccasionalsoactasareinforcerandmayincreasethetendencytoemployaggressivemeansinthefuture.

Page 26: the Psychology of Social Conflict and Aggression Sydney Symposium in Social Psychology

the pSychology of Social conflict and aggreSSion 9

norms In thesameway thatmodelingandmediadepictionsmay legitimizeaggression, other social norms and regulations may have the opposite effects,reducinganddelegitimizingconflictandaggression.Forinstance,genderdiffer­encesinphysicalaggressionhavebeenpartiallyexplainedintermsofsocialnorms,aswerecross­culturaldifferencesinthedisplayofaggressivebehaviors,inparticu­larthenotionthatindividualisticculturesaremoreaggressivethancollectivisticcultures(seealsoChapters11and15).

hormones Thehormonalbasisofaggressionhasbeentiedtotestosterone,andthetentativeconclusionseemstobethataconnectionexistsbetweenthemale hormone and assaultive behavior. Intriguingly, the connection seems tobe bidirectional—high degrees of testosterone appear to augment aggres­sivebehavior,andsituations thatelicitaggression in turn increase the leveloftestosterone.

evolution Theevolutionaryapproachtoaggressionhasledtoseveralintrigu­ingrecentlinesofresearchandtheorizing(seealsoChapters15and18).Amongothers,ithasbeenappliedtothefindingthatbloodrelativeskilleachotherrarelyaswellastogenderdifferencesinaggressionandthefindingthatspousebatteringandabusearemorelikelytooccuramonglower(vs.higher)socioeconomicclasses(seealsoChapter11).

Inanimpressivelyaudaciousanalysis,Koestler(1972)evenarguedthatthehumanspeciessuffersfromaseriousevolutionaryflaw,inthatourbrainevolvedinawaythatischaracterizedbythepoorneuralintegrationbetweenthelower,emotional and thehigher, symbolic and rational areas.Koestler, reviewinganimpressiverangeofpsychological,neurological,anatomical,aswellashistoricalandsociologicalevidence,wentsofarastosuggestthatourspeciesisdoomedtoextinctionbyourownuncheckedaggressivetendencies,unlesswefirstfindawaytocorrecttheflawedstructuralpropertiesofourcentralnervoussystem.Koestlerwroteatatimewhenthenuclearannihilationofallhumanswasadis­tinctpossibility,andhistheoriesreceivedtheirfairshareofcriticism;however,the idea that the unparalleled human capacity for intraspecies violence mayreflectaseriousevolutionarilyflawremainsandintriguingpossibility(seealsoChapter15).

Self-regulation Theself­regulationperspectiveonaggressiondeparts fromthe notion that the impulse to aggression is automatic or innate. Instead, self­regulationmodelssuggestthatconflictandaggressionareamenableself­regula­toryefforts.However,thesuccessfulapplicationofself­regulatorycontrolagainstaggressionrequiresscarcepsychologicalresources.Theself­regulatoryframeworksuggests thatdepletingone’s resourceswouldnecessarily reduceone’sability tocontrolaggression.

Thechaptersinthisbookofferabroadrangeofnewinsightsontheseissuesandfocusonhowconflictandaggressioncanbemodified,forexample,byintrain­dividual(seeChapters6,8,and12)aswellasbyinterpersonal(seeChapter14)andsocialandculturalvariables(seeChapters10,17,and19).

Page 27: the Psychology of Social Conflict and Aggression Sydney Symposium in Social Psychology

JoSeph p. forgaS, arie kruglanSki, and kipling d. WilliamS10

meaSuring and operationalizing conflict and aggreSSion

Perhaps more than in other areas of social psychology, conflict and aggressionresearchershaveemployedaningeniousvarietyofmeasuresinanefforttorendertheiroperationaldefinitionsisomorphicwiththeconceptofintendingharm.Fromtheearliestresearchonaggression,clevermethodshaveaboundedthatattempttobridgethegapbetweenmeasuringaggressionononehandandbeingethicalontheother.Thisisatoughrequirementthatmakesaggressionmeasuresdelightfullycleveryetalwaysabitshortofthemark.Thus,inadditiontothewell­knownmea­suresofpunchingBobodolls(Bandura,Ross,&Ross,1961)andpressingleverstoshockanotherindividual(Buss,1961;Milgram,1974),researchershaveplacedparticipantsintoasituationinwhichtheyareaskedtosoundpainfulorunpleasantblastsofnoise,todeliverhotsaucetoindividualswhoareonrecordasdislikinghotsauce(Lieberman,Solomon,Greenberg,&McGregor,1999),tochooseaweaponwithwhichtoshootanotherparticipant(Russell,Arms,Loof,&Dwyer,1996),todrawgraffitionclassicworksofart(Norlander&Gustafson,1997),oreventokillpillbugsinagrinder(Martens,Kosloff,Greenberg,Landau,&Schmader,2007).Ofcourse,therearealsoself­reportmeasuresofwhatparticipantswouldliketodotoanotherindividual(i.e.,aggressivetemptationsanddesires)thatlistanynumberofmeanactsfrominsulttosevereinjury(seeRitter&Eslea,2005,forareviewofthesemethods;seealsoChapter9).

Yetoneissuethatremainsaconcerninallareasthatexamineaggressionandconflict is the external validity of the measures and their relevance to under­standingreal­worldaggressionandconflict.Inmuchlaboratorywork,concernforethicaltreatmentofparticipantsoutweighsdesiresforexternallyvalidmeasures.Thus,aggressionismeasuredsymbolically,indirectly,andalmostalways,withtheimplicitorexplicitapprovaloftheexperimenters.Presentingparticipantswiththeopportunitytoshockanotherperson,todeliverloudnoiseblasts,ortoservemassquantitiesofhotsaucethatanotherindividualmustconsumeallhaveincommonanintenttoharmanother,butwithinthecontextofexperimentalpermissionandtothatextentthesemeasureslackakeycharacteristicofreal­lifeaggression:thatitissociallyundesirableandoftensanctioned.

To claim, therefore, that these studies necessarily predict aggression in therealworldassumesthattheimpactofexternalpermissionisnegligible.Butthisassumptionisquestionable.Itwouldbelikesayingtohighschoolstudentsastheyenterthebuilding,“Herearesomeguns;youmayusethemifyouwish.”Clearly,intherealworld,peopleareawarethatbeingaggressiveisundesirable,unwanted,and often unlawful. Experimental paradigms should attempt to capture andmanipulate“nonpermissible,inappropriate”aggressionsothatwecanaccumulateevidenceastowhetherpermissionmattersforthebothpatternsandmagnitudesofaggression.

Similarly,theuseofgamesinconflictresearchhashadalonghistoryofcontro­versy,yetsuchsimulatedandcontrolledconflictsituationsareclearlyusefulinsomecontexts(seealsoChapter8).However,manyconflictsintherealworldarenotsostructuredwithclearrulesandoutcomesandarenotconductedundertheeyeof

Page 28: the Psychology of Social Conflict and Aggression Sydney Symposium in Social Psychology

the pSychology of Social conflict and aggreSSion 11

experimenters who have created and supervise the rules. Neighbors argue overpropertyandfencesandarefreetouseavarietyoftacticsbutarealsomindfulthatthetacticstheychoosemightbeusedagainsttheminacourtoflaw.Thesetypesofunstructured,freewheeling,yetnonpermissibleformsofconflictarealsoimportanttostudy,andexperimentalsocialpsychologistsshouldstrivetocreateparadigmsthatcreatesuchanatmospherewithinacontrolledcontext(seealsoChapters3,4,and13).Thesecontextsarenoteasytocreate;otherwise,wewouldhaveseenthembynow.Butsocialpsychologistsareacleverbunch,andundoubtedlysomeonewilldevisesuchaparadigminthefuture.Wehopethefutureissoon.

overvieW of the volume

Section I: Basic Issues and Theories

Thebookisorganizedintofourparts.Thefirstpart,afterthisintroductorychapter,isdevotedtodiscussingsomeofthebasicissuesandrecenttheoriesthatinformcontemporaryresearchonconflictandaggression.

Chapter2,byMarioMikulincerandPhillipShaver,offersanovel,attachmenttheoretical perspective on interpersonal and intergroup conflict. In particular,they suggest that attachment theory can help us to understand how (1) peopleexperienceandcopewithinterpersonalconflicts,(2)maladaptiveformsofresolv­ingrelationshipconflictsarise,and(3)intergrouphostilityandaggressioncanbeunderstoodwithinanattachmenttheoreticalframework.Attachmenttheoryoffersimportantnewinsightstohelpexplainindividualdifferencesinadaptiveandmal­adaptive forms of conflicts and conflict resolution in relationships. The chapteralsosuggeststhatattachmenttheorycanbeausefulframeworktounderstandaperson’sattitudesandbehaviortowardout­groupsandtheirpropensityforinter­groupconflict.

InChapter3,KiplingWilliamsandEricWesselmannoutlineacomprehensivetheoryofostracism—beingignoredandexcluded.Ostracismisapainfulyetcom­monexperience,andhumansseemtobeequippedwithanevolvedmechanismfor detecting and responding to cues of exclusion. Such an ostracism detectionsystemcanbetriggeredbyeventhemostminimalcues,andresponsestoostra­cismserve to fortify theneedsatisfaction threatenedbyostracism.Thechapterreviewsresearchonreactionstoostracism,particularlyonwhenandwhyindividu­alschooseaggressive responses rather thanprosocialoptions,andsuggests thatanimportantfactorinostracizedindividuals’responsesisthelikelihoodofbeingreincludeddependingontheirbehavioralresponses.

InChapter4,DeborahRichardsonandGeorginaHammocklookatavarietyof“everyday”formsofconflictandaggressionthatareoftenignoredinaggressionresearch:passiveandpsychologicalaggressionthatisnotmotivatedbytheinten­tiontocauseharm(e.g.,inducingguilt),althoughtheeffectisoftentoharmthetarget.Whereasdirectorphysicalaggressionisrelativelyrare inday­to­daylife,indirect or psychological aggression such as snide remarks or hostile attitudesareverycommon.Indirectaggression is likely toaffect individuals’ relationshipexperienceandsuccessaswellastheirsenseofself.RichardsonandHammock’s

Page 29: the Psychology of Social Conflict and Aggression Sydney Symposium in Social Psychology

JoSeph p. forgaS, arie kruglanSki, and kipling d. WilliamS12

researchprogrambringstotheforefrontformsofeverydayharm­doingandmovesthefocusofaggressionfromtheintentoftheaggressortotheeffectonthevictim.Suchavictim­centereddefinitionisconsistentwithasimpledefinitionofaggres­sionasanybehaviorthatcausesharm.

InChapter5,DanielAmesdiscussesanothercommonanduniversalaspectofsocialconflictandaggression:whatdeterminestheextenttowhichpeoplewillseektobeassertiveandpushhardfortheirinterests?Indailyinteractions,wemustfrequentlychoosebetweengiving inor assertingourwishesoverothers.Whenshouldwepush?Canwepushtoohard?Amesexplorespeoples’informaltheoriesofassertivenessandtheirexpectanciesforsuccess.Whataretheshort­termgainsbutlong­termlossesinbeingassertive?Thechapterarguesthatweallhaveagen­eralbeliefastohowassertiveweshouldbeandhowlikelyitwillleadtosuccess.Further,peoplewilladjusttheseexpectationsdependingontheotherindividualswithwhomaconflictarises.

Section II: Cognitive and Affective Influences

Thesecondpartofthebookdealswithcognitiveandaffectiveinfluencesontheway conflict and aggression occurs. In Chapter 6, Pontus Leander and TanyaChartrand explore the cognitive and motivational mechanisms involved whenanindividual’sowngoalsconflictwiththegoalsandpreferencesheldbyothers.Suchgoal conflicts oftenemerge andescalate automatically in social situations.Insomecircumstancespeoplewillautomaticallyaccommodatetothegoalsandpreferences of the people around them. However, more recent studies indicatethatthemereknowledgeaboutothers’goalsisoftensufficienttoelicitadversarialresponses, especially when the individual is nonconsciously pursuing goals thatareorientedtowardsocialdivergenceorcompetition(e.g.,achievement,autonomy,self­enhancement).Thus, interpersonalconflictsmayoftenunfoldautomatically,asindividualsarenotalwaysconsciousoftheoriginsoftheirinterpersonalcon­flictsorknowthatsuchconflictshaveevenoccurred.

Chapter7explorestheroleofperspectivetakingandempathyinconflictstrat­egies.AdamGalinsky,DebraGilin,andWilliamMadduxsuggestthatthecogni­tiveskillandabilitytobecomeawareofothers’thoughtsandtheaffectivecapacityforempathy—feelingwhattheothersarefeeling—playavery importantrole inhow individuals dealwith and resolve conflicts. Their experiments point to thebenefits in termsofpayoffsofperspective takingoverempathy.Theyalsofind,however, that in certain circumstances in which shared feelings are importantempathymightresultinbetteroutcomesandmorepositivesocialbenefits.

Chapter 8 looks at affective influences on conflict behaviors. Joseph ForgasandHuiBingTanarguethatmoodstateshaveastrongandreliableeffectontheway people perceive, interpret, and respond to conflict. They report numerousexperiments showing thatpositiveaffectproducesamoreconfident,optimistic,andassertive response toconflicts, ashappypeoplenegotiatemoreconfidently,make interpersonaldemandsmoreassertively,and interpret theirownandoth­ers’socialbehaviorsmoreoptimistically.Otherstudiesfindthatnegativemoodscan alsoproducedistinct benefits in conflict situations,whenclose attention to

Page 30: the Psychology of Social Conflict and Aggression Sydney Symposium in Social Psychology

the pSychology of Social conflict and aggreSSion 13

externalinformationisrequired.Thus,thoseinnegativemoodarebetteratdetect­ingdeception,arelesslikelytocommitjudgmentalerrors,havebettereyewitnessmemories,andproducemoreeffectivepersuasivemessages.

AngerregulationandnegotiationisthetopicofChapter9byThomasDensonandEmmaFabiansson.Theyexaminetheeffectivenessofdifferentemotionreg­ulation strategies suchas reappraisal, rumination,anddistraction incontrollinganger during negotiation. Denson and Fabiansson present the results from twoexperimentsexploringwhat impactdifferentangerregulationstrategieshaveonbehaviorinnegotiations.Theirresultssuggestthattheapplicationofreappraisalinnegotiationsettingsisusefulinreducinganger,aggressivebehavior,andconflictcomparedwithruminationordistraction.Thus,traininginreappraisalskillsmaybeparticularlybeneficialforindividualswhowouldotherwiseuseotheremotionregulationstrategies.

InChapter10,ArieKruglanskiandEdwardOreheklookattheroleofthequestforpersonalsignificanceandidentityinextremeformsofaggressionsuchasterror­ismandsuicidebombers.Whatisthepsychologicalexplanationforthemotivationtobecomeasuicidebomber?Theysuggestthataquestforpersonalsignificancemaybeanunderlyingfactor.Ironically,theactofcarryingoutasuicidalmissiononbehalfofone’sgrouporreligioncanelevateone’ssenseofimportanceandmeaning.Inotherwords,deathensuresasenseofimmortality.Inaseriesofingeniousexperi­ments,KruglanskiandOrehekprovideinitialsupportfortheirprovocativetheory.

Section III: Conflict and Aggression in Relationships

Thissectionofthebookturnstoresearchonconflictandaggressionthatoccurwithintheframeworkofestablishedpersonalrelationships.

InathoughtfulChapter11,ChrisEckhardtdiscussesthedamagingrolethatentrenched feminist ideology has played in the way intimate couple violence isdefined, understood, and dealt with within the U.S. social and judicial system.Eckhardtreviewstheknownriskfactorsthatinfluenceinterpartnerviolencesuchascognitiveprocessing,emotionregulation,andrelationaldynamicsthateffectivelydiscriminatebetweenabusiveandnonabusiveindividuals.Hearguesthat,despitestrongandconsistentempiricalfindingsthatcouldinformeffectiveetiologicandinterventionmodelsof interpartnerviolence, there remainsa strong,dominant,andideologicallybasedreactionaryfeministviewthatseesinterpartnerviolenceasprimarilycausedbymaledominanceandmale­centeredsocialnormsandhier­archies.Thereislittleconvincingevidencesupportingthisideologicalposition,yetinterpartnerviolencecontinuestobedefinedandtreated,atleastintheUnitedStates,byinterventionsandmethodsthatareinformedbyfeministideologyratherthantheobjectiveevidence.Thischapterbringsveryvaluableinsightstotheissueofhowsometimesbiasedandprejudgedideologicalpositionsmaythwartthemosteffectivetreatmentofviolence.

InChapter12,GrainneFitzsimonsandJoannaAndersonlookattheroleofincompatiblegoals in thegenerationandmanagementof conflictbetweencou­ples.Coupleshavetomanagecoordinationandconflictonadailybasis,yetmostresearch on couples’ conflict focuses on negotiations and trust games. In their

Page 31: the Psychology of Social Conflict and Aggression Sydney Symposium in Social Psychology

JoSeph p. forgaS, arie kruglanSki, and kipling d. WilliamS14

chapter,FitzsimonsandAndersonsummarize their innovativeprogramofworkexamininghowcouplesthatsharedifferentgoalsarealsomorelikelytoexperienceconflict. Unlike traditional work on couple conflict, Fitzsimons and Anderson’sproject examines similar versus dissimilar goal pursuits and argues that goalsunderliemuchofhowpartnersvieweachotherandunderstandeachotherandtheirmotivationstocooperatewitheachother.Theyfindthatdissimilarpersonalgoalscanleadtoincreasedratesoffightingandincreasednegativityinresponsetocommondisagreements.

InChapter13LisaZadroexaminesthepossiblepsychological,contextual,andemotionalfactorsthatmaymotivatetargetsofrelationalostracismtoenactpuni­tiveandvengefulbehaviors.Factorsthatpotentiallymoderatetheconsequencesofexclusionarediscussedintermsofwhethertheyameliorateorexacerbateaggres­sive reactions. The chapter also introduces new experimental research using anovelostracismparadigm,O-Cam,asimulatedWebconferencethatspecificallyinvestigatestheformsofvengeancethattargetsofostracismarewillingtoimposeonsources.

Chapter14dealswiththepotentialdangersofunilateralforgivenessinresolv­ingconflicts in relationships.LauraLuchiesandEliFinkel lookatquestionsofforgivenessinconflictmanagementandsuggestthatunilateralforgiveness,whenithelpsvictimspreserveavaluablerelationship,isbeneficial,butwhenitpreservesarelationshipthatisunlikelytobevaluableitleadstonegativeoutcomes.Giventhatvictimsandperpetrators share jointcontrolovervictims’postconflictoutcomes,thedatasuggestthatconflictresolutionstrategiespromotingvictims’forgivenessshouldalsoheightenvictims’sensitivitytowhetherforgivenessisoffuturebenefittothem.Further,forgivenessshouldbesupplementedwithstrategiesdesignedtopromoteperpetrators’amendmaking.

Section IV: Social, Cultural, and Evolutionary Factors in Social Conflict and Aggression

Thefinalandfourthpartofthebookdiscussessomeofthelargerevolutionary,cultural,andsocialinfluencesthatinfluencethewaysocialconflictandaggressionoccurs.

InChapter15,MarkVanVugtoutlinesanevolutionary“malewarriorhypoth­esis” toexplainthemanyintriguingformsofevidenceforhumantribalism:thetendencytocategorizeindividualsonthebasisoftheirgroupmembershipandtotreat in­group members benevolently and out­group members malevolently. Hearguesthatthistribalinclinationisanevolvedresponsetothethreatofintergroupaggressionandviolencethatwasendemicinancestralhumanenvironments(andisstillcommontoday).VanVugtsuggeststhatintergroupconflicthasprofoundlyaffectedthepsychologyofmeninparticular—themalewarriorhypothesis—anddiscussestheimplicationsofthishypothesisformanagingintergrouprelationsinoursociety.

InChapter16CraigAndersonandMattDeLisipresentresearchandtheorythatspeculatesabouttheroleofenvironmentalvariablesassociatedwithglobalwarming on aggression. If experts are correct in their predictions of global

Page 32: the Psychology of Social Conflict and Aggression Sydney Symposium in Social Psychology

the pSychology of Social conflict and aggreSSion 15

warming(andtherearemanyunansweredquestionsaboutthisissue),AndersonandDeLisicontendthataggressionwillincreasefortworeasons:(1)increasedtemperaturehasadirectimpactonincreasedaggression;and(2)increasedtem­peraturehasanindirectimpactonsocietalfactorsthatarerelatedtoincreasedaggression, like displacement, poverty, and physically uncomfortable livingconditions.TheyrelyonAnderson’sGeneralAggressionModeltoderivetheseintriguingpredictions.

In Chapter 17, Ed Donnerstein reviews the literature on various forms ofmediaviolence(film,TV,games)aswellasviolenceontheInternet.Asnearlyallaggressivemoviesandgamescanbereadilyaccessedonline,exposuretoviolenceisbecomingmoreaccessiblethanever,includingtominors.Further,entirelynewandrealisticformsofaggressionareavailableonYouTubeandotherInternetsites,withtheattendantimplicitsuggestionthatthereexistsasocialconsensusabouttheappropriatenessandfrequencyofviolentacts.Itisawholenewworldoutthere,andmuchofitisviolent.Whateffectsshouldweexpecttosee?Donnersteinsug­geststhatInternetviolencehasseriousimplicationsforthewayhumanbeingswillcometoconceiveanddefineacceptableandunacceptableformsofaggression.

Chapter 18 by Robert Kurzban and John Christner applies an evolutionaryapproachtoanalyzingwhatrolesupernaturalbeliefsplayingeneratingandmain­taining intergroupconflict.Theyproposethat sharedsupernaturalbeliefs serveanadaptivepurpose,inthattheysignaltobothin­groupandout­groupmembersalike thatan individualcannoteasilychangegroups.Thus, supernaturalbeliefsfunction as commitmentdevices in the sameway thatbodilymarks, scars, andtattoosworkaspermanentsignalsofgroupidentification,precludinggroupswitch­ing. Shared supernatural beliefs are thus “mental markers,” and the surprisingprevalenceofotherwiseclearlyirrationalsupernaturalbeliefsmaybeunderstoodinthoseterms.

In the final chapter, Chapter 19,Rowell Huesmann,Eric Dubow, and PaulBoxer suggest an intriguing and complementary view: that adherence to someforms of supernatural beliefs, especially traditional religious beliefs, may act tolimitandchannelsocialconflictandaggression.Inparticular,regularexposuretoreligiousactivitiesinchildhoodhasameliorativeeffectsonantisocialandaggres­sivebehavior.Itcouldbethatregularchurchattendanceisamarkerofgoodpar­enting,thatreligiousorganizationsprovidesocialsupportwhenproblemsoccur,orthatreligiousexposurehelpsbuildstrongself­regulatinginternalstandards.A40­yearprospectivelongitudinalstudyfindsthatparentalreligiositymayactasalong­termprotectivefactoragainstadultaggression.Remarkably,highreligiosityseemstoexacerbatethetendenciesoflow­aggressiveyouthtogrowuptobelow­aggressive adults but also exacerbates the tendency of high­aggressive youth togrowuptobemoreaggressive.Theseresultsarediscussedintermsofthepoten­tialsocialandpsychologicalprocessesthatcouldexplaintheeffects.

concluSionSUnderstandingthenatureandcausesofsocialconflictandaggressionisoneofthecorequestionsforpsychology.Asthisintroductoryreviewshows,despiteliterally

Page 33: the Psychology of Social Conflict and Aggression Sydney Symposium in Social Psychology

JoSeph p. forgaS, arie kruglanSki, and kipling d. WilliamS16

hundredsofyearsofphilosophicalandempiricalinterestinthistopic,acompleteunderstandingofthenature,characteristics,andconsequencesofhumanconflictandviolence remainas elusive as ever.Theories range frompessimisticpredic­tionsthatseeHomo sapiensasafundamentallyflawedandviolentevolutionaryfreakdoomedtoextinction(Koestler,1972)tooptimisticviewsthatseeconflictandaggressionasnecessaryandadaptiveresponsesystemsthatcanbeeffectivelymanagedusingsocialandculturalengineering(seealsoChapters15and19).Thechapterspresentedhererepresentsomeofthebestcontemporaryworkonsocialconflictandaggressionbysocialpsychologists.Wehavelearnedagreatdealaboutthecognitive,affective,andmotivationalmechanismsthatinfluencethegenera­tion,experience,andmanagementofsocialconflict.Thechaptersincludedhere,intheirvariousways,allconfirmthatthestudyofsocialconflictandaggressionisathrivingandproductivefieldtoday.Wehopethatreaderswillfindthisbookaninformativeandinterestingoverviewofthecurrentstatusofthisfascinatingareaofinquiry.

referenceS

Bandura,A.,Ross,D.,&Ross,S.A.(1961).Transmissionofaggressionthroughimitationofaggressivemodels.Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 63,575–582.

Berkowitz, L. (1980). The frustration–aggression hypothesis. In R. A. Falk & S. S. Kim,(Eds.),The war system: An interdisciplinary approach. (pp.116–??)Boulder,CO:WestviewPress.

Berkowitz,L.(1993).Aggression: Its causes, consequences, and control.NewYork:McGraw­Hill.

Buss,A.H.(1961).The psychology of aggression.NewYork:Wiley.Dollard,J.,Doob,L.W.,Miller,N.E.,Mowrer,O.H.,&Sears,R.R.(1939).Frustration

and aggression.NewHaven,CT:YaleUniversityPress.Dunbar,R.I.M.(2008).Thesocialbrainhypothesisanditsrelevancetosocialpsychology.

InForgas,J.P.,Haselton,M.G.,&vonHippel,W.(Eds.)Evolution and the social mind.(pp.21–33).Vol.9intheSydneySymposiumofSocialPsychologySeries.NewYork:PsychologyPress.

Freud,S.(1922).Beyond the pleasure principle.London:InternationalPsychoanalyticPress.Freud, S. (1930/1989). Civilization and its discontents. New York: W. W. Norton &

Company;Reissueedition.Fromm,E.(1973).The anatomy of human destructiveness.Greenwich,CT:FawcettCrest.Koestler,A.(1972).Janus: A summing up.London:McMillan.Kruk, M. R. (2009). Context, causes, and consequences of conflict. Lorentz Center

InternationalCenterforWorkshops intheSciences.http://www.lorentzcenter.nl/lc/web/2009/343/extra2.php3?wsid=343

Lieberman,J.D.,Solomon,S.,Greenberg,J.,&McGregor,H.A.(1999).Ahotnewwaytomeasureaggression:Hotsauceallocation.Aggressive Behavior, 25,331–348.

Lorenz, K. (1966). On aggression. (Marjorie Kerr Wilson, Trans.) New York: Harcourt,Brace&World,Inc.

Martens,A.,Kosloff,S.,Greenberg,J.,Landau,M.J.,&Schmader,T.(2007).Killingbegetskilling:Evidencefromabug­killingparadigmthatinitialkillingfuelssubsequentkill­ing.Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 33,1251–1264.

McDougall,W.(1921).An introduction to social psychology, 14thed.Boston:Luce.

Page 34: the Psychology of Social Conflict and Aggression Sydney Symposium in Social Psychology

the pSychology of Social conflict and aggreSSion 17

Milgram,S.(1974).Obedience to authority: An experimental view.London:Tavistock.Norlander,T.,&Gustafson,R.(1997).Effectsofalcoholonpicturedrawingduringtheveri­

ficationphaseofthecreativeprocess.Creativity Research Journal, 10,355–362.Pinker,S.(1994).The language instinct. NewYork:Harper.Ritter,D.,&Eslea,M.(2005).Hotsauce,toyguns,andgraffiti:Acriticalaccountofcurrent

laboratoryaggressionparadigms.Aggressive Behavior, 31,407–419.Russell,G.W.,Arms,R.L.,Loof,S.D.,&Dwyer,R.S.(1996).Men’saggressiontoward

womeninabungledprocedureparadigm. Journal of Social Behavior and Personality, 11, 729–738.

Page 35: the Psychology of Social Conflict and Aggression Sydney Symposium in Social Psychology
Page 36: the Psychology of Social Conflict and Aggression Sydney Symposium in Social Psychology

19

2AnAttachmentPerspectiveon

InterpersonalandIntergroupConflictMARIOMIKULINCER

Interdisciplinary Center (IDC) Herzliya

PHILLIPR.SHAVERUniversity of California, Davis

I nrecentyears,attachmenttheory(Bowlby,1973,1980,1982,1988),whichwas originally formulated to describe and explain infant–parent emotionalbonding,hasbeenappliedfirsttothestudyofadolescentandadultromantic

relationships and then to the study of group dynamics and intergroup relation­ships.Inthepresentchapterweexpandthetheoryasitappliestoadultsbydiscuss­ingattachment­relatedprocessesinvolvedin(1)thewayspeoplethink,experience,and cope with interpersonal conflicts; (2) maladaptive forms of conflict resolu­tion within romantic and marital relationships; and (3) intergroup hostility andaggression.Wewillbeginbypresentinganoverviewofattachmenttheoryandourtheoreticalmodeloftheactivationandpsychodynamicsoftheadultattachmentbehavioralsystem(Mikulincer&Shaver,2007a),alongwithanoverviewofsomeoftheintrapsychicandinterpersonalmanifestationsofthesensesofattachmentsecurityandinsecurity(attachmentanxietyandavoidance).Wewillthenfocusonattachmenttheory’scharacterizationofindividualdifferencesinadaptiveandmal­adaptiveformsofexperiencinginterpersonalconflictsandcopingwiththem.Next,wewill reviewfindingsconcerningthewaysattachment securityandthemajorformsofinsecurityaffectvariousformsofconflictresolutionincloserelationships.Finally,wewillreviewrecentfindingsconcerningwaysthesensesofattachmentsecurity and insecurity (anxiety and avoidance) shape a person’s attitudes andbehaviortowardout­groupsandreduceorintensifyintergroupconflict.

Page 37: the Psychology of Social Conflict and Aggression Sydney Symposium in Social Psychology

mario mikulincer and phillip r. Shaver20

overvieW of adult attachment theoryAccordingtoBowlby(1982),humanbeingsarebornwithaninnatepsychobiologi­calsystem(theattachment behavioral system)thatmotivatesthemtoseekproxim­itytosupportiveothers(attachment figures)intimesofneed.Thissystem,whichemerged over the course of evolution, accomplishes basic regulatory functions(protectionfromthreatsandalleviationofdistress)andincreasesthelikelihoodofsurvivalofhumaninfants,whoarebornwithimmaturecapacitiesforlocomotion,feeding,anddefense.Althoughtheattachmentsystemismostcriticalduringtheearlyyearsoflife,Bowlby(1988)assumedthatitisactiveovertheentirelifespanandismanifestedinthoughtsandbehaviorsrelatedtosupportseeking.

Bowlby(1973)alsodescribedimportantindividualdifferencesinthefunction­ingoftheattachmentsystem.Interactionswithattachmentfigureswhoareavail­ableintimesofneed,aresensitivetoone’sattachmentneeds,andareresponsivetoone’sbidsforproximityfacilitatetheoptimalfunctioningofthesystem.AccordingtoBowlby (1988), thesekindsofpositive interactionspromote the formation ofa senseof attachment security—a sense that theworld is safe, that attachmentfiguresarehelpfulwhencalledupon,andthatitispossibletoexploretheenviron­mentcuriouslyandengageeffectivelyandenjoyablywithotherpeople.Moreover,positiveexpectationsaboutothers’availabilityandpositiveviewsoftheselfascom­petentandvalued(whichBowlbycalledinternal working models)areformed,andaffect­regulationstrategiesareorganizedaroundthesepositivebeliefs.However,whenattachmentfiguresarenotreliablyavailableandsupportive,asenseofsecu­rity is not attained, negative internal working models are formed (e.g., worriesaboutothers’intentionsanddoubtsaboutself­worth),andstrategiesofaffectregu­lationotherthanappropriateproximityseeking(secondary attachment strategies,conceptualizedintermsoftwodimensions, avoidance and anxiety)areadopted.

Instudiesofadolescentsandadults,testsofthesetheoreticalideashavegener­allyfocusedonaperson’sattachment orientation—thesystematicpatternofrela­tionalexpectations,emotions,andbehaviorthatresultsfromaparticularhistoryofattachmentexperiences(Fraley&Shaver,2000;Shaver&Mikulincer,2002).Initially,researchwasbasedonAinsworth,Blehar,Waters,andWall’s(1978)typol­ogyofattachmentpatternsininfancy—secure,anxious,andavoidant—andHazanandShaver’s(1987)conceptualizationofparalleladultstylesinromanticrelation­ships.However,subsequentstudies(e.g.,Brennan,Clark,&Shaver,1998;Fraley& Waller, 1998) revealed that attachment orientations are best conceptualizedasregions ina two­dimensionalspace.Thefirstdimension,attachmentanxiety,reflectsthedegreetowhichapersonworriesthatrelationshippartnerswillnotbeavailable in timesofneedandisafraidofbeingrejectedorabandoned.Thesecond dimension, attachment­related avoidance, reflects the extent to which apersondistrustsrelationshippartners’goodwillandstrivestomaintainbehavioralindependence and emotional distance from partners. People who score low onbothdimensionsaresaidtobesecure,ortohaveasecureattachmentstyle.Thetwodimensions canbemeasuredwithreliableandvalidself­reportscalesandareassociatedintheoreticallypredictablewayswithvariousaspectsofpersonaladjust­mentandrelationshipquality(seeMikulincer&Shaver,2007a,forareview).

Page 38: the Psychology of Social Conflict and Aggression Sydney Symposium in Social Psychology

an attachment perSpective on interperSonal and intergroup conflict 21

Attachment orientations are initially formed in interactions with primarycaregivers during early childhood, as a large body of research has shown(Cassidy & Shaver, 2008), but Bowlby (1988) claimed that memorable inter­actions with others throughout life can alter a person’s working models andcanmovethepersonfromoneregionofthetwo­dimensionalspacetoanother.Moreover,althoughaperson’sattachmentorientation isoftenconceptualizedasasingleglobalorientationtowardcloserelationships,itisactuallyrootedinacomplexnetworkofcognitiveandaffectiveprocessesandmentalrepresenta­tions,whichincludesmanyepisodic,context­related,andrelationship­specificaswellasgeneralattachmentrepresentations(Mikulincer&Shaver,2003).Infact,manystudies indicate thataperson’sattachmentorientationcanchangedepending on context and recent experiences (Mikulincer & Shaver, 2007b).Thismakes itpossible to study theeffectsofexperimentallyprimed securityandinsecurity.

a model of attachment-SyStem functioning in adulthood

Insummarizingthehundredsofempiricalstudiesofadultattachmentprocesses,we(Mikulincer&Shaver,2003,2007a)createdaflowchartmodeloftheactiva­tion and dynamics of the attachment system. The model includes three majorcomponents:(1)monitoringandappraisingthreateningevents;(2)monitoringandappraisingtheavailabilityofexternalorinternalizedattachmentfigures;and(3)monitoringandappraisingtheviabilityofseekingproximitytoan“attachmentfig­ure”asameansofcopingwithattachmentinsecurityanddistress.Italsoincludesexcitatory and inhibitory pathways that result from recurrent use of secondaryattachment strategies, and these feedback pathways affect the monitoring ofthreateningeventsandattachmentfigures’availability.

MikulincerandShaver(2007a)assumedthatthemonitoringofunfoldingeventsresultsinactivationoftheattachmentsystemwhenapotentialoractualthreatissensed(unconsciously)orperceived(consciously).Thatis,duringencounterswithphysicalorpsychologicalthreats—eitherintheenvironmentorintheflowofinter­nalfreeassociations—theattachmentsystemisactivated,andtheprimaryattach­mentstrategyis set inmotion.Thisstrategyleadsadultstoturnto internalizedrepresentationsofattachmentfiguresortoactualsupportiveothersandtomain­tainsymbolicoractualproximitytothesefigures.Recentstudieshaveshownthatthoughtsrelatedtoproximityseekingaswellasmentalrepresentationsofinternal­izedattachmentfigurestendtobeactivatedeveninminimallythreateningsitu­ations (Mikulincer,Birnbaum,Woddis,&Nachmias,2000;Mikulincer,Gillath,&Shaver,2002).However,althoughageanddevelopmentresultinanincreasedabilitytogaincomfortfromsymbolicrepresentationsofattachmentfigures,nooneofanyageiscompletelyfreeofrelianceonothers(Bowlby,1982,1988).

Activationoftheattachmentsystemforcesadecisionabouttheavailabilityofattachmentfigures(thesecondmoduleofourmodel).Anaffirmativeanswertotheimplicitorexplicitquestion“Isanattachmentfigureavailableandlikely

Page 39: the Psychology of Social Conflict and Aggression Sydney Symposium in Social Psychology

mario mikulincer and phillip r. Shaver22

toberesponsivetomyneeds?”heightensthesenseofattachmentsecurityandfacilitatestheuseofconstructiveemotion­regulationstrategies.Thesestrategiesareaimedatalleviatingdistress,maintainingsupportiveintimaterelationships,andbolsteringaperson’ssenseof loveworthinessandself­efficacy.Moreover,theysustainwhatShaverandMikulincer(2002),followingFredrickson(2001),calla“broaden­and­build”cycleofattachmentsecurity,whichexpandsaper­son’sresourcesformaintainingcopingflexibilityandemotionalstabilityintimesofstress,broadenstheperson’sperspectivesandcapacities,andfacilitatestheincorporationofmental representationsof security­enhancingattachmentfig­ures into the self. This broaden­and­build process allows relatively secureindividualstomaintainanauthenticsenseofpersonalefficacy,resilience,andoptimism even when social support is temporarily unavailable (Mikulincer &Shaver,2007a).

Perceived unavailability of an attachment figure results in attachment inse­curity,whichcompounds thedistress arousedby the appraisal of a situationasthreatening.Thisstateofinsecurityforcesadecisionabouttheviabilityoffurther(moreactive)proximityseekingasaprotectivestrategy(thethirdmoduleofthemodel).Theappraisalofproximityasfeasibleoressential—becauseofattachmenthistory,temperamentalfactors,orcontextualcues—resultsinenergetic,insistentattemptstoattainproximity,support,andlove.Theseattemptsarecalledhyper-activating strategies(Cassidy&Kobak,1988)becausetheyinvolveup­regulationoftheattachmentsystem,includingconstantvigilanceandintenseconcernuntilanattachmentfigureisperceivedtobeavailableandsupportive.Hyperactivatingstrategies include attempts to elicit a partner’s involvement, care, and supportthrough clinging and controlling responses (Shaver & Mikulincer, 2002), over­dependenceonrelationshippartnersasasourceofprotection(Shaver&Hazan,1993),andperceptionofoneselfasrelativelyhelplesswithrespecttoemotionregu­lation(Mikulincer&Shaver,2003).Hyperactivatingstrategiesarecharacteristicofpeoplewhoscorerelativelyhighontheattachmentanxietydimension(Mikulincer&Shaver,2007a).

Theappraisalofproximityseekingasnonviablecanresultininhibitionofthequestforsupportandactiveattemptstohandledistressalone(whichBowlby,1988,labeledcompulsive self-reliance).Thesesecondarystrategiesofaffectregulationarecalledavoidant deactivating strategies(Cassidy&Kobak,1988),becausetheirprimary goal is to keep the attachment system deactivated to avoid frustrationand further distress caused by attachment­figure unavailability. This goal leadstothedenialofattachmentneeds;avoidanceofcloseness, intimacy,anddepen­dence inclose relationships;maximizationofcognitive, emotional, andphysicaldistancefromothers;andstrivings forautonomyand independence.Withprac­ticeandexperience,thesedeactivatingstrategiesoftenbroadentoincludeliteralandsymbolicdistancingofoneselffromdistresswhetheritisdirectlyattachmentrelated.Deactivatingstrategiesarecharacteristicofpeoplescoringrelativelyhighonavoidantattachment(Mikulincer&Shaver,2007a).

In short, each attachment strategy has a major regulatory goal (insisting onproximitytoanattachmentfigureoronself­reliance),whichgoesalongwithpar­ticular cognitive and affective processes that facilitate goal attainment. These

Page 40: the Psychology of Social Conflict and Aggression Sydney Symposium in Social Psychology

an attachment perSpective on interperSonal and intergroup conflict 23

strategiesaffecttheformationandmaintenanceofcloserelationshipsaswellastheexperience,regulation,andexpressionofnegativeemotions,suchasanxiety,anger,orsadness(Mikulincer&Shaver,2007a).Moreover,thestrategiesaffectthewaysapersonexperiencesandhandlesconflictualsituationswithotherindividuals(includingaromanticpartnerorspouse)orgroups.Thisisthemainfocusofthefollowingsectionsofthischapter.

attachment orientationS and interperSonal conflictS

Whenanalyzingthepossiblelinksbetweenthefunctioningoftheattachmentsys­temandthewaysapersonexperiencesandregulatesconflictswithotherpeople,itisimportanttorememberthattheattachmentsystemwas“designed,”duringevolu­tion,asaninterpersonalregulatorydevice.AccordingtoBowlby(1982),perceivedthreatsanddangersmakesalientthegoalofgainingproximitytoandsupportfromanattachmentfigure,andthisencouragespeopletolearn,organize,andimplementbehavioralplansaimedatattainingsafetyandsecurity.Importantly,Bowlbyalsoassumedthattheattachmentsystemoperatesina“goal­corrected”manner.Thatis,apersonevaluatestheprogressheorsheismakingtowardachievingsupportandcomfort fromapartnerandcorrects intendedactions ifnecessary toattainthesegoals.Therefore,effectivefunctioningoftheattachmentsystemincludestheuseofpartner­tailoredproximity­seekingstrategiesthattakeintoaccountapart­ner’sneedsandpreferences(creatingwhatBowlby,1973,calleda“goal­correctedpartnership”).Thisfacilitatessatisfying,harmoniousinteractionsthatmightoth­erwisedevolveintointrusive,coercive,orconflictualexchangesrootedincoordi­nation failuresandmismatchedneedsandgoals.Moreover, smooth functioningoftheattachmentsystemhelpspeoplerapidlyandeffectivelyrestorerelationshipharmonywhenevertheyandtheirpartnerhaveincompatibleneedsandgoalsthatcanresultinpainfulinterpersonalconflicts.

AccordingtoMikulincerandShaver(2007a),competentmanagementofinter­personal conflicts is originally learned during interactions between infants andtheirprimarycaregivers,mainlywheninfantssearchforacaregiver’sprotectionor support. During such episodes, children must not only express their needsforproximityandsupporttogainasenseofsecuritybutalsomustlearntoman­age occasional goal conflictsbetween themand their caregivers, because thesemay interferewithcontinuedsupport.Althoughthefoundationof thisability isassumedtobeaninnateaspectoftheattachmentsystem(giventhegoal­correctednatureofthesystem’soperation),interactionswithsensitiveandresponsivecare­giverswhocanflexiblyadapt theirgoalsandresponsestochildren’sattempts todealwithgoalconflictsallowchildrentolearneffectiveconflictmanagementskillsandpracticeandrefinethem.Incontrast,interactionswitharejectingfigurewhorigidlymaintainshisorherowngoalsregardlessofchildren’sattempts to tailortheirbids forproximity to thisfigure’spreferences cast apalloverearly effortstoregulateinterpersonalconflicts.Unresponsiveattachmentfiguresforceachildtoacquirealternativeconflictmanagementskillsthatmayseemadaptiveintheir

Page 41: the Psychology of Social Conflict and Aggression Sydney Symposium in Social Psychology

mario mikulincer and phillip r. Shaver24

originalcontext(e.g.,inhibitingexpressionofone’sneedswhenaparentrespondsbadlytoneedexpression)butcancausetroublelateron,whenapersonencountersnewrelationshippartnerswithdifferentsalientneedsandpreferences.

MikulincerandShaver(2007a)hypothesizedthatrelativelysecureadolescentsand adults are likely to emphasize the challenging rather than the threateningaspectsofinterpersonalconflictsandbelievetheycandealeffectivelywiththem.Thesepositivebeliefsaboutconflictandconflictmanagementarerootedinsecureindividuals’viewsofothersas“wellintentionedandkindhearted”(Hazan&Shaver,1987,pp.518–519)andtheirviewsthattheyarecapableofhandlinglife’sproblems(e.g.,Mikulincer&Florian,1998).Moreover,theirconstructiveapproachtoemo­tionregulation(Shaver&Mikulincer,2007)mayhelpthemcommunicateopenlybutnotthreateninglyduringconflict,negotiatewithothersinacollaborativeman­ner,andapplyeffectiveconflict­resolution strategies, suchascompromisingandintegratingtheirownandtheirpartner’sneedsandbehaviors.Insodoing,secureindividualsarelikelytomovetheirrelationshipsbackfrominevitableconflictstostatesofharmony.

Insecurepeoplearelikelytoappraiseinterpersonalconflictsinmorethreaten­ingtermsandapplylesseffectiveconflict­resolutionstrategies.Foranxiouspeople,conflictsthreatentheirwishtogainapproval,support,andsecurity;theyarousefearofrejectionandtriggerhyperactivatingaffect­regulationstrategies.Thepeo­pleare likely toappraiseconflict incatastrophic terms,display intensenegativeemotions,ruminateobsessively,andhencefailtoattendtoandunderstandwhattheirrelationshippartneristryingtotelltothem.Thisegocentric,fearfulstanceislikelytointerferewithcalm,opencommunication,negotiation,andtheuseofcompromisingandintegratingstrategiesthatdependonkeepingapartner’sneedsandperspectiveinmind.Anxiousindividualsarelikelyeithertotrytodominatetheinteraction(inanefforttogettheirownneedsmet)oraccedesubmissivelytoapartner’sdemandstoavoidrejection.

Avoidantindividualsarelikelytoviewconflictsasaversiveprimarilybecauseconflictsinterferewithautonomyandcallforexpressionsofloveandcareorneedandvulnerability.Avoidantpeoplearelikelytodownplaythesignificanceofcon­flictwhileminimizing the importanceof theirpartner’scomplaints, todistancethemselvescognitivelyoremotionallyfromtheconflict,ortotrytoavoidinteract­ing with their partner. When circumstances do not allow escape from conflict,avoidantindividualsare likelytoattempttodominatetheirpartner,inlinewiththeirneedforcontrol,negativemodelsofothers,andconfidenceintheirownviews.Thisdefensivestanceislikelytointerferewithnegotiationandcompromise.

The hypothesized links between attachment orientations and responses tointerpersonalconflicthavebeenexaminedinseveralcorrelationalstudies.Inthesestudies, participants completed self­report scales measuring attachment orienta­tions as well as scales assessing subjective appraisals of conflicts (e.g., Pistole &Arricale, 2003), conflict­management skills (e.g., Taubman­Ben­Ari, Findler,& Mikulincer, 2002), the use of constructive conflict­management tactics (e.g.,Carnelley, Pietromonaco, & Jaffe, 1994), or the use of aggression and conflict­escalationtactics(e.g.,Creasey&Hesson­McInnis,2001).OtherstudieshaveusedRahim’sOrganizationConflictInventory(ROCI;Rahim,1983)toassessreliance

Page 42: the Psychology of Social Conflict and Aggression Sydney Symposium in Social Psychology

an attachment perSpective on interperSonal and intergroup conflict 25

onintegrating,compromising,dominating,obliging,andavoidingstrategiesduringinterpersonalconflicts(e.g.,Corcoran&Mallinckrodt,2000;Levy&Davis,1988).

These studies indicate that people who score relatively high on attachmentanxietyoravoidanceappraiseconflictsinmorethreateningtermsandbelievetheyarelesscapableofdealingwithconflicts.Moreover,theyreporthavingrelativelypoorconflict­managementskills(e.g.,understandingtheirpartner’sperspective),beingunlikelytorelyoncompromisingandintegrativestrategies,andbeingrela­tively likely toescalateconflicts (usingcoercionoroutrightfighting)or to leavea conflictunresolved.Researchalso indicates thatattachment anxiety isassoci­atedwithconcernsaboutclosenessduringconflicts(Pistole&Arricale,2003)andstrongconflict­relateddistress (e.g.,Creasey&Hesson­McInnis,2001). Inaddi­tion,anxiouslyattachedindividualsreacttotheprimingofrejectionconcernswithlessflexibilityinconflict­managementstrategies(BeinsteinMiller,1996),suggest­ing that their fearofrejection,whenheightenedexperimentally, interfereswithconstructiveapproachestoconflictresolution.

There are also many studies documenting the links between self­reports ofattachment insecurities and conflict­management problems within dating andmarital relationships (e.g., Feeney, 1994; Heene, Buysse, & Van Oost, 2005;Roberts&Noller, 1998). Specifically, attachment insecurities havebeen associ­atedwithreportsoflessexpressionofaffectionandempathyduringconflicts,lessfrequent relianceoncompromising strategies,more frequentuseof coerciveorwithdrawalstrategies,morefrequentengagement inverbalandphysicalaggres­sion,andhigher levelsofpostconflictdistress.At thecouple level,SenchakandLeonard(1992)foundthatcouplesinwhichoneorbothpartnerswereinsecurelyattachedreportedmorewithdrawalandaggressionduringconflictsthancouplesinwhichbothpartnersweresecure.

Thereisalsoevidencethatinsecurepeople’sconflict­managementdifficultiesareevident toobserversofcouplemembers’behaviorduring laboratorydiscus­sionsofunresolvedconflicts.Forexample,KobakandHazan(1991)usedaQ­sortmeasureofmaritalattachmentandfoundthathusbandsandwiveswhowerelesssecureintheirmarriageweremorelikelytodisplayfacialexpressionsofrejectionwhilediscussingadisagreement.Inaddition, insecurehusbandswereless likelytoprovidesupportduringthediscussion.Similarly,Simpson,Rholes,andPhillips(1996),Feeney(1998),andCampbell,Simpson,Boldry,andKashy(2005)foundthatself­reportsofattachmentinsecuritieswereassociatedwithexpressionsofdis­tressduringaconflictdiscussionwithadatingpartner.Feeneyalsofoundthatself­reportsofattachmentinsecuritieswereassociatedwithfewerdisplaysofwarmthandaffectionduringconflictdiscussions.

RelyingontheAdultAttachmentInterview(AAI;George,Kaplan,&Main,1985)toassessadultattachmentorientations,severalstudieshaveprovidedevi­dence for the expectedassociation between insecurities anddestructivebehav­iorsduringconflictswitharomanticpartner(e.g.,Babcock,Jacobson,Gottman,&Yerington,2000;Creasey&Ladd,2005;Crowelletal.,2002).Specifically,indi­vidualscategorizedasinsecurebasedontheAAIhavebeencodedasdisplayinglesspositiveaffectthantheirsecurecounterpartsduringconflictdiscussionsandmorefrequentexpressionsofcontempt,withdrawal,andstonewalling.

Page 43: the Psychology of Social Conflict and Aggression Sydney Symposium in Social Psychology

mario mikulincer and phillip r. Shaver26

There is also evidence linking self­reports of attachment insecurities withheightenedphysiologicalreactivitytorelationshipconflicts.Powers,Pietromonaco,Gunlicks,andSayer(2006)askedcouplestospend15minutesdiscussinganunre­solvedconflict.Salivarycortisol levels (anindexofphysiologicalreactivity)wereassessedbefore,during,andafterthediscussion.Resultsindicatedthatattachmentinsecuritieswereassociatedwithgreaterphysiologicalreactivitytothediscussionandthatgendermoderatedtheeffectsofthespecifickindofattachmentinsecurity(anxietyoravoidance).Whereasavoidantbutnotanxiouswomenshowedheight­enedcortisolreactivity,anxiousbutnotavoidantmenevincedthiskindofresponseinreactiontothediscussion.AccordingtoPowersetal.,thesegenderdifferencescanbeexplainedintermsofgender­relatednormsconcerningconflicts.Previousstudieshaveindicatedthatwhereaswomenareexpectedtotakeanactive,leadingroleduringconflicts(e.g.,toarticulaterelationshipconcerns)menareassignedalessactiverole(e.g.,Christensen&Heavey,1990).Asaresult,thediscussionmaybeparticularly stressful for avoidantwomen,whoprefer todistance themselvesfromrelationshipproblems,andforanxiousmen,whotendtoexpressdistressandtakeacontrollingpositioninthediscussion.

Studieshavealsofoundthatself­reportsofattachmentanxietyareassociatedwithintensificationofthenegativeconsequencesofconflictdiscussions.Forexam­ple,Simpsonetal.(1996)foundthatanxiouslyattachedpeoplereportedastron­gerdeclinethansecurepeopleinloveandcommitmentafterdiscussingamajorrelationshipproblemwithadatingpartner.GalloandSmith(2001)alsofoundthatanxiouswives,comparedwithsecurewives,reactedtoadiscussionaboutarelation­shipdisagreementwithmorenegativeappraisalsoftheirhusbands.InCampbelletal.’s(2005)diarystudyofdailyconflictsbetweendatingpartners,moreanxiousparticipantsreportedmoreconflictualinteractionsacross14consecutivedaysandreactedtodaysofintenseconflictwithasharperdeclineinrelationshipsatisfactionandamorepessimisticviewoftherelationship’sfuture.

Insecurepeople’sdeficienciesinhandlinginterpersonalconflictsarealsoevi­dentinstudiesassessingattachment­relatedvariationsindomesticviolence.Thiskindofviolenceoftenresultsfromrepeatedfailurestosolveinterpersonalconflictsandtopreventconflictescalation—deficienciesweexpect tobeassociatedwithattachmentinsecurity.However,despitebothanxiousandavoidantpeople’sprob­lems in handling interpersonal conflicts, studies have revealed that attachmentanxietyismorestronglyassociatedwithdomesticviolencethanisavoidantattach­ment (e.g., Dutton, Saunders, Starzomski, & Bartholomew, 1994; Henderson,Bartholomew,Trinke,&Kwong,2005).Forexample,Duttonet al. studied160court­mandated men convicted of wife assault and found that self­reports ofattachmentanxietywereassociatedwithmorefrequentandsevereactsofcoer­cionandpartnerabuseduringcoupleconflicts.Secureattachmentwasnegativelyassociatedwithmostfeaturesofdomesticviolenceeveninthisself­selected,court­mandatedpopulation.

Thelinkbetweenattachmentanxietyanddomesticviolenceisevidentintwootherkindsofstudies.First,studiescomparingattachmentorientationsofviolentandnonviolentsampleshavefoundthatpartnerswhoengageindomesticviolencearemoreanxiouslyattached,onaverage,thanpartnerswhodonotresorttoviolence

Page 44: the Psychology of Social Conflict and Aggression Sydney Symposium in Social Psychology

an attachment perSpective on interperSonal and intergroup conflict 27

(e.g.,Bookwala&Zdaniuk,1998).Second,studiesinunrestrictedsamplesofado­lescentsandyoungadultshaveconsistentlyfoundthatyoungmenandwomenwhoscorehigheronattachmentanxietyarelikelytoreportmoreengagementincoupleviolence(e.g.,Roberts&Noller,1998).Importantly,theseassociationscannotbeexplainedbyotherrelationshiporpersonalityvariablesandseemtobemediatedbyrelianceonineffectiveconflict­managementstrategies.

With regard to avoidant attachment, some researchers have suggested thatavoidantindividualswithdrawfromconflictratherthanbecomesoemotionalthattheyattackarelationshippartner(e.g.,Mikulincer&Shaver,2007a).BartholomewandAllison(2006)found,however,thatavoidantpeoplesometimesbecamevio­lentwheninvolvedinanescalatingseriesofconflicts,especiallywithananxiouslyattached partner who demanded involvement. Similarly, Holtzworth­Munroe,Stuart,andHutchinson(1997)foundthatavoidancewasassociatedwithwifebat­tering,andRankin,Saunders,andWilliams (2000) foundthat itwasassociatedwithmorefrequentandsevereactsofdomesticviolenceonthepartofasampleofAfricanAmericanmenarrestedforpartnerabuse.Inaddition,Collins,Cooper,Albino,andAllard(2002)reportedthatavoidancemeasuredduringadolescencepredictedrelationshipviolence6yearslater.

attachment orientationS and intergroup conflict

Thelinkbetweenattachmentinsecuritiesanddestructiveresponsestoconflictisalsoevidentinthefieldofintergrouprelations.Inthiscontext,tensions,frictions,andconflictsbetweengroupsareaconstantandpervasivesourceof intergrouphostility,whichisdirectlymanifestedinout­groupderogation(i.e.,thetendencytoperceivemembersofotherculturalorethnicgroupsinlessfavorabletermsthanmembersofone’sowngroup;seeBrewer&Brown,1998,forareview),prejudice,anddiscriminationagainstout­groupmembers.Whenintergrouprelationsbecometenseandconflictive,thesehostilitiescanresult inviolence,rape,andkillingofout­group members—even genocide (Staub & Bar­Tal, 2003). Although severaleconomicandpoliticalfactorsareinvolvedintheescalationofintergroupviolence(e.g., economic instability, totalitarian regimes), theways individuals experienceandhandleintergrouptensionsandconflictscanexplainindividualdifferencesinintergrouphostilityandaggression.Withthisinmind,we(Mikulincer&Shaver,2001)hypothesizedthatattachmentinsecurities,whicharecharacterizedbycon­flict­managementdeficiencies,wouldbeassociatedwithdestructiveresponsestointergroup conflict and thereby with more hostility and aggression toward out­groupmembers.

Socialidentitytheory(Tajfel&Turner,1986)assumesthatout­groupderoga­tionservesaself­protectivefunction:maintenanceofself­esteem(“We,”includingI,arebetterthan“them”).Thisdefensivetendencyseemslikelytobeespeciallycharacteristic of insecurely attached people. Securely attached individuals canmaintainastableandauthenticsenseofself­worthbyvirtueoffeelinglovedandacceptedbyothersandpossessing special andvaluablequalities (Mikulincer&

Page 45: the Psychology of Social Conflict and Aggression Sydney Symposium in Social Psychology

mario mikulincer and phillip r. Shaver28

Shaver,2005).Theyshouldhavelessneedtofearanddisparageout­groupmem­bers.Inhisaccountofhumanbehavioralsystems,Bowlby(1982)statedthatactiva­tionoftheattachmentsystemiscloselyrelatedtoinnatefearofstrangersandthatsecureattachmentsmitigatethisinnatereactionandfosteramoretolerantattitudetowardunfamiliarityandnovelty.

Inaseriesoffivestudies,we(Mikulincer&Shaver,2001)foundstrongevi­denceforthesetheoreticalideas.First,higherscoresonaself­reportmeasureofattachmentanxietywereassociatedwithmorehostileresponsestoavarietyofout­groups(asdefinedbysecularIsraeliJewishstudents):IsraeliArabs,Ultra­orthodoxJews, Russian immigrants, and homosexuals. Second, experimental heighteningof the sense of attachment security (subliminal presentation of security­relatedwords suchas love andproximity, evocationvia guided imageryof the compo­nents of security­enhancing interpersonal interactions, and visualization of thefacesofsecurity­enhancingattachmentfigures)eliminatednegativeresponsestoout­groups.Theseeffectsweremediatedbythreatappraisalsandwerefoundevenwhenparticipants’senseofpersonalvaluewasthreatenedortheirin­grouphadbeeninsultedbyanout­groupmember.Thatis,experimentallyaugmentedattach­mentsecurityreducedthesenseofthreatcreatedbyencounterswithout­groupmembersandseemedtoeliminatehostileresponsestoout­groupmembers.

Buildingonthesestudies,MikulincerandShaver(2007b)foundthatincreas­ing people’s sense of attachment security reduced actual aggression betweencontendingorwarring social groups.Specifically, Israeli Jewish undergraduatesparticipatedinastudytogetherwithanotherIsraeliJeworanIsraeliArab(ineachcase,thesameconfederateoftheexperimenter)andweresubliminallyandrepeat­edlyexposed(for20millisecondsoneachtrial)tothenameoftheirownsecurity­enhancingattachmentfigure,thenameofafamiliarpersonwhowasnotviewedasanattachmentfigure,orthenameofanacquaintance.Followingtheprimingprocedure, participants were informed that they would evaluate a food sampleand that theyhadbeenrandomly selected togive the confederatehot sauce toevaluate.Theyalsolearnedindirectlythattheconfederatestronglydislikedspicyfoods.(Thisprocedurehasbeenusedinotherstudiesofinterpersonalaggression;e.g.,McGregoretal.,1998).Thedependentvariablewastheamountofhotsauceallocatedtotheconfederate.

Whenparticipantshadbeensubliminallyprimedwiththenameofsomeonewhowasnotanattachmentfigure,theydeliveredalargeramountofhotsaucetotheArabconfederatethantotheJewishconfederate,asignofintergroupaggres­sion.Butsecurityprimingeliminatedthisdifference:participantswhosesenseofsecurityhadbeenenhanceddeliveredequal(relativelylow)amountsofhotsaucetoboththeArabandtheJewishconfederate.Inaddition,participantsscoringhigheronattachmentanxietygavemorehotsaucetotheout­groupmember(IsraeliArab)than to the in­group member (Israeli Jew). Thus, it seems that people who areeitherdispositionallysecureorinducedtofeelmoresecureinaparticularsettingarebetterablethantheirinsecurecounterpartstotolerateintergroupdifferencesandtorefrainfromintergroupaggression.

Althoughthesestudiesindicatethatattachmentinsecuritiesareassociatedwithstrongerintergroupderogationandaggression,theydidnotincludeassessmentsof

Page 46: the Psychology of Social Conflict and Aggression Sydney Symposium in Social Psychology

an attachment perSpective on interperSonal and intergroup conflict 29

thecognitiveprocessesthatunderliesuchconflict­escalationresponses.AccordingtoBar­Tal,Kruglanski,andKlar(1989),thesedestructiveresponsesaredrivenbywhattheycalledconflictschemasormentalsets.Intheirview,peoplewhoholda cooperation set anticipate constructive interactions with out­group membersand cooperative and satisfactory conflict­resolution discussions, which, in turn,movesthemawayfromhostileandaggressiveresponsestoout­groups.Incontrast,peoplewhoholdaconflictsetanticipatehostileandcompetitiveinteractionswithout­groupmembers andunpleasantandantagonisticconflict resolutions,which,in turn,promote intergrouphostility andaggression.Thesemental setsmaybebrought about by either person factors (e.g., prosocial orientation, Carnevale &Probst,1998)orsituationalfactors(e.g.,DeDreu&Nijstad,2008).Basedonfind­ingsreviewedearlier,attachmentorientationsmaybeoneof these factors,withattachmentinsecurities,eitherdispositionalorcontextuallyenhanced,increasingthelikelihoodofadoptingaconflictmentalsetandsecureattachmentincreasingendorsementofacooperationset.

Toexaminethisissue,wefolloweduparecentseriesofstudiesbyDeDreuandNijstad(2008)onmentalsetsandcreativethought.Inoneofthesestudies,partici­pantswereaskedtoindicatetheextenttowhichaparticularobjectvaryinginitsdegreeofprototypicality(e.g.,car,elevator)isanexampleofaparticularcategory(e.g.,vehicle).Inthistask,inclusionratherthanexclusionoftheweakprototypicalobjects(e.g.,elevator)reflectsbroadcognitivecategoriesandflexiblecognitivepro­cessing(Rosch,1975),whichareassumedtofostercreativethought(e.g.,Amabile,1983).DeDreuandNijstadhypothesizedthataconflictsetleads individualstofocus their attentiononconflict­related issues and todismissor ignoreconflict­irrelevantissues.Asaresult,aconflictsetwillinvolvebroaderandmoreinclusivethinkingaboutconflictbutwillresultinnarrow­minded,black­and­whitethink­ingaboutconflict­irrelevantissues.Indeed,thefindingsindicatedthataconflictmentalsetwasassociatedwithmoreinclusionofweakprototypicalexemplarsofconflict­relatedcategoriesbutlessinclusionofweakprototypicalexemplarsofneu­tralcategories.

Based on this finding, we conducted an exploratory two­session laboratorystudy,reportedhere for thefirsttime,that involved80Israeli Jewishuniversitystudents(53womenand27men).Wehypothesizedthatthepatternofcategoryinclusionresponsesreflectingaconflictmentalsetwouldbemorecharacteristicofinsecurelythanofsecurelyattachedpeopleandwouldbemitigatedbysecuritypriming(subliminallypresentingthenameofasecurity­enhancingattachmentfig­ure).Thefirstsessionwasdesignedtoassessparticipants’attachmentorientationsandacquirespecificnamesofsecurity­enhancingfiguresandotherclosepersonstobeusedlaterasprimesinthesecondsession.Inthatfirstsession,participantscompleted the ECR inventory (Brennan et al., 1998), a measure of attachmentanxietyandavoidance,plustwocomputerizedmeasuresofthenamesofattach­mentfiguresandotherclosepersonswhowerenotattachmentfigures.ThefirstofthesetwocomputerizedmeasureswasaHebrewversionoftheWHOTOscale(Fraley&Davis,1997), inwhichparticipantswereaskedtotypeinaMicrosoftExcelworksheet thenamesof theirsecurity­enhancingattachmentfigures.Thescaleincludedsixitems(e.g.,Whoisthepersonyouwouldcountonforadvice?

Page 47: the Psychology of Social Conflict and Aggression Sydney Symposium in Social Psychology

mario mikulincer and phillip r. Shaver30

Whoisthepersonyoucanalwayscounton?),and,foreachitem,participantswrotethenameofthepersonwhobestservedthetargetedattachment­relatedfunction.Inthesecondmeasure,participantswereaskedtowritethenamesoftheirfather,mother,brothers,sisters,bestfriend,currentromanticpartner,grandfathers,andgrandmotherswithoutmakinganyreferencetotheattachmentfunctionstheydidordidnot serve.Weassumed thatbecause someof thesepeople’snameswerenotprovidedasprimaryattachmentfigurestheyprobablydidnotmeetthestrictrequirementsforthatrole.

Inthesecondsession,conducted2weekslaterbyadifferentexperimenter,par­ticipants(allofthemIsraeliJews)wereinvitedtohaveaconversationwithanIsraeliArabstudentabout theMiddleEastconflict.However,before theconversation,participantswereaskedtoperformtwocognitivetasks.Inthefirsttask—a30­trialcomputerizedword­relationtask—participantswererandomlyassignedtooneoftwoconditions:securityorneutralpriming.Inthesecurityprimingcondition(n=40),participantsweresubliminallyexposed(for20milliseconds)tothenameoftheirmostsecurity­enhancingattachmentfigure(basedonthefirstsessionofthestudy).Intheneutralprimingcondition(n=40),theyweresubliminallyexposedtothenameofafamiliarpersonwhowasnotselectedasanattachmentfigure.

Followingtheprimingprocedure,participantsinbothprimingconditionsper­formedthesecondcognitivetask:acategoryinclusiontask.ThistaskwasidenticaltotheoneusedbyDeDreuandNijstad(2008).Specifically,participantsreceivedfourneutralcategoriesandthreeconflict­relatedcategories (randomlyordered),and for each category they rated three objects in terms of their prototypicalityusinga10­pointscalerangingfrom1(not at all)to10(very prototypical).Thefourneutralcategories (withstrong, intermediate,andweakexemplars)werevehicle(bus,airplane,camel),vegetable(carrot,potato,garlic),clothes(skirt,shoes,hand­bag),andfurniture(couch,lamp,telephone).Thethreeconflict­relatedcategories(with strong, intermediate, and weak exemplars) were weapon (gun, jet fighter,screwdriver),army(Cavalry,AlQaida,hooligans),andammunition(bullet,dyna­mite, paving stones). Statistical analyses were performed on the prototypicalityratingsofweakexemplars.Nosignificanteffectswerefoundforratingsofstrongandintermediateexemplars.Foreachparticipant,wecomputedtwototalscores:(1) inclusivenessofneutralcategories(averageofratingsfortheweakexemplarsof the four neutral categories, Cronbach’s alpha = .71); and (2) inclusiveness ofconflictcategories(averageofratingsfortheweakexemplarsofthethreeconflictcategories,alpha=.74).

Totestourpredictions,weconductedtwo­stephierarchicalregressionanaly­seswithparticipants’scoresontheECRattachmentanxietyandavoidancescalesand security priming (a contrast variable contrasting security priming, 1, withneutralpriming,–1)astheindependentvariables.Inthefirststepoftheseanaly­ses,weenteredattachmentanxietyandavoidance(Z­scores)andsecurityprimingasablocktoexaminetheuniquemaineffectsofthesepredictors.Inthesecondstep, the two­way interactionsbetweensecurityprimingandeachof theECRscoreswereenteredasadditionalpredictors.Theseregressionswereperformedseparatelyforinclusivenessofconflict­relatedcategoriesandinclusivenessofneu­tralcategories.

Page 48: the Psychology of Social Conflict and Aggression Sydney Symposium in Social Psychology

an attachment perSpective on interperSonal and intergroup conflict 31

Forinclusivenessofconflict­relatedcategories,theregressionanalysisrevealedsignificantmaineffectsofattachmentanxiety(β=.35,p <.01),avoidantattach­ment(β=.24,p <.05),andsecuritypriming(β=–.27,p <.05).Asexpected,thehighertheattachmentanxietyandavoidance,thehighertheprototypicalityratingsofweakexemplarsofconflict­relatedcategories.Moreover,comparedwithneutralpriming,securityprimingreducedtheprototypicalityratingsofweakexemplarsofconflict­relatedcategories.Theinteractioneffectswerenotsignificant.Thatis,whereas attachment insecurities seemed to involve broader and more inclusivethinking about conflict, securitypriming seemed to reduce the inclusivenessofconflict­relatedcategories.

Forinclusivenessofneutralcategories,theregressionanalysisrevealedsignifi­cantmaineffectsofattachmentanxiety(β=–.28,p <.01)andsecuritypriming(β=.36,p <.01).Asexpected,thehighertheattachmentanxiety,thelowerthepro­totypicalityratingsofweakexemplarsofneutralcategories.Moreover,comparedwith neutral priming, security priming increased the prototypicality ratings ofweakexemplarsofneutralcategories.Also,theinteractionbetweensecurityprim­ingandattachmentanxietywassignificant(β=.31,p <.01).Examinationofthesignificantinteractions(usingtheprocedurefromAiken&West,1991)revealedthatattachmentanxietywasassociatedwithlowerinclusivenessofneutralcatego­riesintheneutralprimingcondition(–1)(β=–.59,p<.01)butnotinthesecurityprimingcondition(+1)(β=.03).Theseslopesindicatethatsecurityprimingwasabletomitigateanxiouslyattachedparticipants’tendencytothinkaboutneutralcategories in lessbroadand inclusive terms—atendencyMikulincer andSheffi(2000)observedpreviouslyusingotherneutralcategoriesandothertasksassessingcreativethoughts.

Theseresultsprovideencouragingpreliminaryevidencethatattachmentinse­curitiesareassociatedwithaconflictmentalsetandthatevenatemporarysenseofattachmentsecurityreducesthelikelihoodofadoptingsuchmentalsetsduringencounterswithout­groupmembers.Furtherresearchisneededtodetermine(1)whetherinsecurelyattachedindividuals’conflictmentalsetsareactivatedduringencounterswithout­groupmembersortendtobechronicallyactivatedduringevenneutralinterpersonalinteractionsand(2)whetherthesementalsetsunderlieinse­curelyattachedindividuals’hostileandaggressivereactionstoout­groupmembers.

concluding remarkSAttachment theory, which was originally developed to explain infant–caregiverattachment and different attachment patterns in infant–caregiver relationshipsthatseemtoresultfromdifferentkindsofcaregiving,hasbeenextendedfirsttotherealmofadultcouplerelationshipsandnowtorelationshipsinorganizations(e.g.,Shaver&Mikulincer,2008)andtointergrouprelations.Bothcorrelationalandexperimentalstudiesindicatethatinterpersonalconflictsarehandledworsebypeoplewithaninsecureattachmentstyle,whetheranxiousoravoidant,andarehandledbetterbypeoplewithasecurestyle.Weconsiderithighlysignificantthatintergroupconflictsmightbereducedbyhelpingconflictingpartiestofeelmoresecure,notjustintheintergrouprelationshipswheretheconflictsareoccurring

Page 49: the Psychology of Social Conflict and Aggression Sydney Symposium in Social Psychology

mario mikulincer and phillip r. Shaver32

butalsointheircloserelationships,whichattachmenttheoryviewsasthesourceofsecurityandinsecurity.Ourexperiments,whilefairlysimple,suggestthatthisisacausalprocess—thatis,thatsecurityenhancementprecedesamovementtowardmoreprosocialattitudesandbehaviors(Mikulincer&Shaver,2007b).Theeffectsofsuchsecuritymostlikelystemfromfairlydeep,insomecasesnotverballyacces­sible,feelings.Theythusaddtoalloftheworkinsocialpsychologythatfocusesmoreintentlyonformsofverbalnegotiation,rationallyinducedchangesincogni­tions,andvariousformsofexposuretomembersofout­groups.Ourstudiessuggestthattheremaybemanycontributionstoconstructiveconflictresolution,includingonesthatdependonevolvedbehavioralsystemsthatmayatfirstseemtohavelittletodowithinterpersonalconflicts.

referenceS

Aiken,L.S.,&West,S.G.(1991).Multiple regression: Testing and interpreting interac-tions.NewburyPark,CA:Sage.

Ainsworth,M.D.S.,Blehar,M.C.,Waters,E.,&Wall,S.(1978).Patterns of attachment: A psychological study of the strange situation.Hillsdale,NJ:Erlbaum.

Amabile,T.M.(1983).The social psychology of creativity. NewYork:Springer­Verlag.Babcock,J.C.,Jacobson,N.S.,Gottman,J.M.,&Yerington,T.P.(2000).Attachment,emo­

tionalregulation,andthefunctionofmaritalviolence:Differencesbetweensecure,preoccupied, and dismissing violent and nonviolent husbands. Journal of Family Violence, 15,391–409.

Bar­Tal,D.,Kruglanski,A.W.,&Klar,Y.(1989).Conflicttermination:Anepistemologicalanalysisofinternationalcases.Political Psychology, 10, 233–255.

Bartholomew,K.,&Allison,C.J.(2006).Anattachmentperspectiveonabusivedynamicsin intimaterelationships.InM.Mikulincer&G.S.Goodman(Eds.),Dynamics of romantic love (pp.102–127). NewYork:GuilfordPress.

BeinsteinMiller,J.(1996).Socialflexibilityandanxiousattachment. Personal Relationships, 3,241–256.

Bookwala,J.,&Zdaniuk,B.(1998).Adultattachmentstylesandaggressivebehaviorwithindatingrelationships.Journal of Social and Personal Relationships, 15,175–190.

Bowlby,J.(1973).Attachment and loss: Vol. 2. Separation: Anxiety and anger.NewYork:BasicBooks.

Bowlby,J.(1980).Attachment and loss: Vol. 3. Sadness and depression.NewYork:BasicBooks.Bowlby, J. (1982). Attachment and loss: Vol. 1. Attachment (2nd ed.). New York: Basic

Books.(Originaled.1969)Bowlby, J. (1988). A secure base: Clinical applications of attachment theory. London:

Routledge.Brennan,K.A.,Clark,C.L.,&Shaver,P.R.(1998).Self­reportmeasurementofadultattach­

ment:Anintegrativeoverview.InJ.A.Simpson&W.S.Rholes(Eds.),Attachment theory and close relationships(pp.46–76).NewYork:GuilfordPress.

Brewer,M.B.,&Brown,R.J.(1998).Intergrouprelations.InD.T.Gilbert,S.T.Fiske,&G.Lindzey(Eds.),The handbook of social psychology(4thed.,Vol.2,pp.554–594).NewYork:McGraw­Hill.

Campbell, L., Simpson, J. A., Boldry, J., & Kashy, D. A. (2005). Perceptions of conflictand support in romantic relationships: The role of attachment anxiety. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 88,510–531.

Page 50: the Psychology of Social Conflict and Aggression Sydney Symposium in Social Psychology

an attachment perSpective on interperSonal and intergroup conflict 33

Carnelley,K.B.,Pietromonaco,P.R.,&Jaffe,K.(1994).Depression,workingmodelsofothers, and relationship functioning. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 66,127–140.

Carnevale,P.J.,&Probst,T.M.(1998).Socialvaluesandsocialconflictincreativeprob­lem solving and categorization. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74, 1300–1309.

Cassidy,J.,&Kobak,R.R.(1988).Avoidanceanditsrelationtootherdefensiveprocesses.InJ.Belsky&T.Nezworski(Eds.),Clinical implications of attachment(pp.300–323).Hillsdale,NJ:Erlbaum.

Cassidy,J.,&Shaver,P.R.(Eds.).(2008).Handbook of attachment: Theory, research, and clinical applications(2nded.).NewYork:GuilfordPress.

Christensen,A.,&Heavey,C.L.(1990).Genderandsocialstructureinthedemand/with­draw pattern of marital conflict. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 59,73–81.

Collins, N. L., Cooper, M., Albino, A., & Allard, L. (2002). Psychosocial vulnerabilityfrom adolescence to adulthood: A prospective study of attachment style differ­ences in relationship functioning and partner choice. Journal of Personality, 70,965–1008.

Corcoran,K.O.,&Mallinckrodt,B.(2000).Adultattachment,self­efficacy,perspectivetak­ing,andconflictresolution.Journal of Counseling and Development, 78,473–483.

Creasey,G.,&Hesson­McInnis,M.(2001).Affectiveresponses,cognitiveappraisals,andconflicttacticsinlateadolescentromanticrelationships:Associationswithattachmentorientations.Journal of Counseling Psychology, 48,85–96.

Creasey, G., & Ladd, A. (2005). Generalized and specific attachment representations:Uniqueand interactive roles inpredictingconflictbehaviors inclose relationships.Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 31,1026–1038.

Crowell, J. A., Treboux, D., Gao, Y., Fyffe, C., Pan, H., & Waters, E. (2002). Assessingsecurebasebehaviorinadulthood:Developmentofameasure,linkstoadultattach­mentrepresentations,andrelationstocouples’communicationandreportsofrela­tionships.Developmental Psychology, 38,679–693.

DeDreu,C.K.W.,&Nijstad,B.A.(2008).Mentalsetandcreativethoughtinsocialconflict:Threatrigidityversusmotivatedfocus.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 95,648–661.

Dutton,D.G.,Saunders,K.,Starzomski,A.,&Bartholomew,K. (1994). Intimacy­angerandinsecureattachmentasprecursorsofabuseinintimaterelationships.Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 24,1367–1386.

Feeney,J.A.(1994).Attachmentstyle,communicationpatterns,andsatisfactionacrossthelifecycleofmarriage.Personal Relationships, 1,333–348.

Feeney, J. A. (1998). Adult attachment and relationship­centered anxiety: Responses tophysicalandemotionaldistancing.InJ.A.Simpson&W.S.Rholes(Eds.),Attachment theory and close relationships(pp.189–219).NewYork:GuilfordPress.

Fraley,R.C.,&Davis,K.E.(1997).Attachmentformationandtransferinyoungadults’closefriendshipsandromanticrelationships.Personal Relationships, 4,131–144.

Fraley,R.C.,&Shaver,P.R.(2000).Adultromanticattachment:Theoreticaldevelopments,emergingcontroversies,andunansweredquestions.Review of General Psychology, 4,132–154.

Fraley,R.C.,&Waller,N.G.(1998).Adultattachmentpatterns:Atestofthetypologicalmodel.InJ.A.Simpson&W.S.Rholes(Eds.),Attachment theory and close relation-ships(pp.77–114).NewYork:GuilfordPress.

Fredrickson,B.L.(2001).Theroleofpositiveemotionsinpositivepsychology:Thebroaden­and­buildtheoryofpositiveemotions.American Psychologist, 56,218–226.

Page 51: the Psychology of Social Conflict and Aggression Sydney Symposium in Social Psychology

mario mikulincer and phillip r. Shaver34

Gallo,L.C.,&Smith,T.W.(2001).Attachmentstyleinmarriage:Adjustmentandresponsestointeraction.Journal of Social and Personal Relationships, 18,263–289.

George,C.,Kaplan,N.,&Main,M.(1985).The Adult Attachment Interview.Unpublishedprotocol,DepartmentofPsychology,UniversityofCalifornia,Berkeley.

Hazan,C.,&Shaver,P.R.(1987).Romanticloveconceptualizedasanattachmentprocess.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 52,511–524.

Heene,E.L.D.,Buysse,A.,&VanOost,P.(2005).Indirectpathwaysbetweendepressivesymptomsandmaritaldistress:Theroleofconflictcommunication,attributions,andattachmentstyle.Family Process, 44, 413–440.

Henderson, A. J. Z., Bartholomew, K., Trinke, S., & Kwong, M. J. (2005). When lovingmeans hurting: An exploration of attachment and intimate abuse in a communitysample.Journal of Family Violence, 20,219–230.

Holtzworth­Munroe,A.,Stuart,G.L.,&Hutchinson,G.(1997).Violentversusnonviolenthusbands:Differencesinattachmentpatterns,dependency,andjealousy.Journal of Family Psychology, 11,314–331.

Kobak,R.,&Hazan,C.(1991).Attachmentinmarriage:Effectsofsecurityandaccuracyofworkingmodels.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 60,861–869.

Levy,M.B.,&Davis,K.E.(1988).Lovestylesandattachmentstylescompared:Theirrela­tionstoeachotherandtovariousrelationshipcharacteristics.Journal of Social and Personal Relationships, 5,439–471.

McGregor,H.,Leiberman,J.,Greenberg,J.,Solomon,S.,Arndt,J.,Simon,L.etal.(1998).Terror management and aggression: Evidence that mortality salience promotesaggression against worldview­threatening individuals. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74, 590–605.

Mikulincer,M.,Birnbaum,G.,Woddis,D.,&Nachmias,O.(2000).Stressandaccessibilityofproximity­relatedthoughts:Exploringthenormativeandintraindividualcomponentsofattachmenttheory.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 78,509–523.

Mikulincer,M.,&Florian,V.(1998).Therelationshipbetweenadultattachmentstylesandemo­tionalandcognitivereactionstostressfulevents.InJ.A.Simpson&W.S.Rholes(Eds.),Attachment theory and close relationships (pp.143–165).NewYork:GuilfordPress.

Mikulincer,M.,Gillath,O.,&Shaver,P.R.(2002).Activationoftheattachmentsysteminadulthood:Threat­relatedprimesincreasetheaccessibilityofmentalrepresentationsofattachmentfigures.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 83,881–895.

Mikulincer,M.,&Shaver,P.R.(2001).Attachmenttheoryandintergroupbias:Evidencethat priming the secure base schema attenuates negative reactions to out­groups.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 81,97–115.

Mikulincer,M.,&Shaver,P.R. (2003).Theattachmentbehavioral system inadulthood:Activation, psychodynamics, and interpersonal processes. In M. P. Zanna (Ed.),Advances in experimental social psychology (Vol. 35, pp. 53–152). New York:AcademicPress.

Mikulincer, M., & Shaver, P. R. (2005). Mental representations of attachment security:Theoretical foundation for a positive social psychology. In M. W. Baldwin (Ed.),Interpersonal cognition(pp.233–266).NewYork:GuilfordPress.

Mikulincer,M.,&Shaver,P.R.(2007a).Attachment in adulthood: Structure, dynamics, and change.NewYork:GuilfordPress.

Mikulincer,M.,&Shaver,P.R. (2007b).Boostingattachment security topromotemen­tal health, prosocial values, and inter­group tolerance. Psychological Inquiry, 18,139–156.

Mikulincer,M.,&Sheffi,E.(2000).Adultattachmentstyleandcognitivereactionstoposi­tiveaffect:Atestofmentalcategorizationandcreativeproblemsolving.Motivation and Emotion, 24,149–174.

Page 52: the Psychology of Social Conflict and Aggression Sydney Symposium in Social Psychology

an attachment perSpective on interperSonal and intergroup conflict 35

Pistole,M.,&Arricale,F.(2003).Understandingattachment:Beliefsaboutconflict.Journal of Counseling and Development, 81,318–328.

Powers,S.I.,Pietromonaco,P.R.,Gunlicks,M.,&Sayer,A.(2006).Datingcouples’attach­mentstylesandpatternsofcortisolreactivityandrecoveryinresponsetoarelation­shipconflict.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 90,613–628.

Rahim,M.A.(1983).Measurementoforganizationalconflict.Journal of General Psychology,109,189–199.

Rankin,L.B.,Saunders,D.G.,&Williams,R.A.(2000).Mediatorsofattachmentstyle,socialsupport,andsenseofbelonginginpredictingwomanabusebyAfricanAmericanmen.Journal of Interpersonal Violence, 15,1060–1080.

Roberts,N.,&Noller,P. (1998).Theassociationsbetweenadult attachmentandcoupleviolence:Theroleofcommunicationpatternsandrelationshipsatisfaction.InJ.A.Simpson&W.S.Rholes(Eds.),Attachment theory and close relationships(pp.317–350).NewYork:GuilfordPress.

Rosch,E.(1975).Cognitiverepresentationsofsemanticcategories.Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 104, 192–233.

Senchak,M.,&Leonard,K.E.(1992).Attachmentstylesandmaritaladjustmentamongnewlywedcouples.Journal of Social and Personal Relationships, 9,51–64.

Shaver,P.R.,&Hazan,C.(1993).Adultromanticattachment:Theoryandevidence.InD.Perlman&W.Jones(Eds.),Advances in personal relationships(Vol.4,pp.29–70).London:JessicaKingsley.

Shaver,P.R.,&Mikulincer,M.(2002).Attachment­relatedpsychodynamics.Attachment and Human Development, 4,133–161.

Shaver,P.R.,&Mikulincer,M.(2007).Adultattachmentstrategiesandtheregulationofemotion.InJ.J.Gross(Ed.),Handbook of emotion regulation (pp.446–465). NewYork:GuilfordPress.

Shaver,P.R.,&Mikulincer,M.(2008).Augmentingthesenseofsecurityinromantic,leader­follower, therapeutic, and group relationships: A relational model of psychologicalchange.InJ.P.Forgas&J.Fitness(Eds.),Social relationships: Cognitive, affective, and motivational processes(pp.55–74).NewYork:PsychologyPress.

Simpson,J.A.,Rholes,W.S.,&Phillips,D.(1996).Conflictincloserelationships:Anattach­mentperspective.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 71,899–914.

Staub,E.,&Bar­Tal,D. (2003).Genocide,masskillingand intractableconflict:Roots,evolution, prevention and reconciliation. In D. O. Sears, L. Huddy, & R. Jervis(Eds.)Oxford handbook of political psychology (pp.710–751).NewYork:OxfordUniversityPress.

Tajfel,H.,&Turner,J.C.(1986).Thesocialidentitytheoryofintergroupbehavior.InS.Worchel&W.Austin(Eds.),Psychology of intergroup relations(pp.7–24).Chicago:Nelson.

Taubman­Ben­Ari,O.,Findler,L.,&Mikulincer,M.(2002).Theeffectsofmortalitysalienceonrelationshipstrivingsandbeliefs:Themoderatingroleofattachmentstyle.British Journal of Social Psychology, 41,419–441.

Page 53: the Psychology of Social Conflict and Aggression Sydney Symposium in Social Psychology
Page 54: the Psychology of Social Conflict and Aggression Sydney Symposium in Social Psychology

37

3TheLinkBetweenOstracism

andAggressionKIPLINGD.WILLIAMSandERICD.WESSELMANN1

Purdue University

“Socially,Mackandtheboyswerebeyondthepale.SamMalloydidn’tspeaktothemastheywentbytheboiler.Theydrewintothemselvesandnoonecouldforeseehowtheywouldcomeoutofthecloud.Fortherearetwopos­siblereactionstosocialostracism—eitheramanemergesdeterminedtobebetter, purer, andkindlierorhegoes bad, challenges theworld and doesevenworsethings.Thislastisbyfarthecommonestreaction…”

JohnSteinbeck,CanneryRow(1987,pp.250–251)

O stracism2—being ignoredandexcluded—isapainful situation that themajorityofindividualshaveexperiencedatleastonceintheirlivesandsometimesisadailyoccurrence(Nezlek,Wheeler,Williams,&Govan,

2004;Williams,2009).Theseexperiencescanbepsychologicallyandemotionallydamagingtothetarget:theycanleadtoself­defeatingbehavior(Twenge,Catanese,& Baumeister, 2002), impaired self­regulation (Baumeister, DeWall, Ciarocco,& Twenge, 2005; Oaten, Williams, Jones, & Zadro, 2008), and self­perceptionsofdehumanization(Bastian&Haslam,2010).Furthermore,ostracismhasbeenshown to activate the same regions of the brain associated with physical pain

1 Bothauthorscontributedequallytothepreparationofthismanuscript.ThismaterialisbasedonworksupportedbytheNationalScienceFoundationunderGrantNo.0519209.

2 Research isunclearon the specificdifferencesamongostracism, rejection, and social exclusion;oftentimesthesethreetermsareusedinterchangeably.Weacknowledgetherearedebatesabouttherelationsamongtheseterms(seeLearyetal.,2006;Williams,2009),butforthesakeofsimplicitywewillusethetermostracismthroughoutthischapter.

Page 55: the Psychology of Social Conflict and Aggression Sydney Symposium in Social Psychology

kipling d. WilliamS and eric d. WeSSelmann38

(Eisenberger,Lieberman,&Williams,2003)andtolowersignificantlythetargets’perceptionsoffourbasichumanneeds:belonging,control,meaningfulexistence,andself­esteem(Williams,2001,2009;Williams,Cheung,&Choi,2000;Zadro,Williams,&Richardson,2004;seealsoChapter13inthisvolume).

Why oStraciSm hurtS the individualSocial psychologists have theorized that humans are equipped with an evolvedmechanism for detecting and responding to cues of ostracism (Kerr & Levine,2008;Leary,Tambor,Terdal,&Downs,1995;Spoor&Williams,2007;seeotherevolutionarylinkstoconflictinChapters15and18inthisvolume).Thesesystemsareadaptivebecauseatonetimeinourevolutionaryhistorybeingostracizedfromasocialgroupcouldharmanindividual’schancesatsurvival—aformof“socialdeath” (see Williams 2007; also Baumeister & Leary, 1995). Williams (2009)arguesthatthesesystemsshouldbequickandcrude,reactingattheslightestcueofostracism,sothattheindividualcanpreemptivelyforestalloravoidpermanentexpulsion.Williamsposits these cues setoff thedetection system,whichelicitsthe experience ofpain in the target individual (seeChen, Williams, Fitness,&Newton, 2008, for a discussion of social and physical pain; also MacDonald &Leary,2005),andperceptionsofthreatenedbasicneedssatisfaction.

Recent Evidence for the Ostracism Detection System

Severalrecentstudieshaveexaminedthesensitivityoftheostracismdetectionsystembyexaminingtheminimalcuesandboundaryconditionsthatexistforindividualstofeelthestingofostracism.Previousresearchhadfocusedonstudyingostracisminvarioustypesofface­to­face(Williams&Sommer,1997)orelectronicsocialinterac­tions(Smith&Williams,2004;Williamsetal.,2000;Williamsetal.,2002).Wirthandcolleagues(Wirth,Sacco,Hugenberg,&Williams,2010)decidedtofocusonhowsimplenonverbalcuesofostracism(e.g.,lackofeyecontact)influencedthedetectionsystem.Theseresearchersfoundparticipantswhoreceivedlesseyecontactfromavir­tualconfederateweremorelikelytofeelignoredandexcluded,exhibitingthetypicalostracismeffectsofthreatenedneedsatisfactionandworsenedmoods.

Kassner,Wirth,Law,andWilliams(2010)arguethateventhemostminimalcuescantriggerthedetectionsystemaslongassocialinformationisinherentinthesecues.Theyusedavirtualreality­basedparadigmcalledMinimalWorldtoostracizeparticipantsinasituationwheretherewasnosocialinformationpresent.MinimalWorldplacedparticipantsinavirtualenvironmentwheretheysawtwosquaresandasphereinfrontofthem(nonsocialversionsoftheplayeravatarsandtheballintheCyberballparadigm;Williamsetal.,2000).Thespheremovedbackand forthbetween the two squares andoccasionallymoved toward thepartici­pant’spointofviewanddisappeared.Participantswereinstructedtopressoneoftwobuttonswhenthespheredisappeared—onebuttonsentthespherebacktotheleftsquare,andtheotherbuttonsentthespheretotherightsquare.Participantswereassignedrandomlytooneoffourconditionsina2(ostracism–inclusion)×2(socialinformation–noinformation)design.Intheinclusionconditions,theywere

Page 56: the Psychology of Social Conflict and Aggression Sydney Symposium in Social Psychology

the link BetWeen oStraciSm and aggreSSion 39

giventheopportunitytocontrolthesphere33%ofthetime(similartoinclusionmanipulationsinotherostracismparadigms).Participantsintheostracismcondi­tionshadcontroloverthesphereonlytwiceatthebeginningandthenneveragainforthedurationofthestudy.Theresearchersmanipulatedthesocial informationbyinstructinghalfoftheparticipantstomentallyvisualizea“coherentstory”aboutthemovementoftheshapes;theotherparticipantswerenotgiventheseinstruc­tions.Resultsdemonstratedthatparticipantswhowereostracizedandgiventhesocial information experienced distress akin to ostracism in other paradigms,whereasparticipantswhowerenotgiventhesocialinformationdidnothavedif­ferentexperiencesfromtheinclusionconditions.Theseresearchersconcludedthataslongasthereissocialinformationpresent,cuesofostracismshouldactivatethedetectionsystemandthwartindividuals’needsatisfaction.

Otherresearchsuggestscuesofostracismdonothavetobedirectedspecificallyat the individual toactivate theirdetectionsystem.Wesselmannandcolleagues(Wesselmann,Bagg,&Williams,2009)investigatedhowindividualsrespondedtowitnessingastrangerbeingostracized(i.e.,notthrowntoduringavirtualball­tossgame).Notonlydidparticipants recognize theostracized individualwould feeltheeffectsofostracism(i.e.,thwartedneedsatisfactionandworsenedmood),buttheseparticipantsdemonstrateddistresssimilartowhattheywouldfeelasiftheywerepersonallyexperiencingtheostracism.These results, takenwith theotherresearchonboundaryconditionsforostracism,lendcredencetotheargumentthattheostracismdetectionsystemshouldcrudelyandquicklyreacttoeventhemostminimalcuesofostracism(Williams,2009).

Why the oStracized individual hurtS otherSWilliams(2009)arguedthatbehavioralresponsestoostracismserveafortificationfunctionfortheneedsatisfactionthreatenedbyostracism(seealsoLeary,Twenge,&Quinlivan,2006;Warburton,Williams,&Cairns,2006;Williams&Govan,2005).Asubstantialamountofresearchhasbeendedicatedtoexaminingtheeffectsofostra­cismonindividuals’subsequentbehavior,specificallyaggressivebehavior.Individualsappeartobemorelikelytobehaveaggressivelytowardanotherpersonafterbeingostracized,regardlessofwhetherthepersonwasinvolvedoruninvolvedinthetargets’ostracism(Buckley,Winkel,&Leary,2004;Carter­Sowell,VanBeest,vanDijk,&Williams,2010;Chow,Tiedens,&Govan,2008;Twenge,Baumeister,Tice,&Stucke,2001;Twenge&Campbell,2003;Warburtonetal.,2006;Williams,2001).Archivalresearchevensuggests long­termostracismwasapotentialimpetusfortheviolentbehaviorofmanyoftheschoolshootersoverthelastdecade(Leary,Kowalski,Smith,&Phillips,2003).Theostracism→aggressionlinkisnotlimitedtocurrentostracismexperiences—evenrecallingapreviousexperienceofsocialpainisenoughtoincreaseindividuals’temptationsforaggressivebehavior(Riva,Wirth,&Williams,2010).

Restored Control Reduces Aggression After Ostracism

Someresearchershavebeguntoexplore thepotential thataggressiveresponsesto ostracism may serve to fortify control. Preliminary evidence suggests that

Page 57: the Psychology of Social Conflict and Aggression Sydney Symposium in Social Psychology

kipling d. WilliamS and eric d. WeSSelmann40

individuals strive to exert control after being ostracized (Lawson Williams &Williams,1998).Intwostudies,theyfoundthatostracizedmalestoldaconfed­erate to turn his head (supposedly to better “read” his nonverbal facial expres­sions)moreoftenthanincludedmalesandthatostracizedfemalesreportedhigherdesireforcontrolthanincludedfemales.Furthermore,aggressionallowstheindi­vidualtoassertcontrol(Tedeschi,2001).Asanexample,researchhasfoundthatindividualswho feltnocontrolover their elevation tonewmajority status (theywere previously in the minority) were more likely to exert abusive control overthenewminoritymemberscomparedwithindividualswhoserisetonewmajoritystatusappearedtobetheresultoftheirownefforts(Prislin,Williams,&Sawiki,2010).Thus,wereasonedthatwhencontrolwassufficientlythwartedostracizedindividualsmight resort toaggression.Warburton,Williams,andCairns (2006)arguedthatifaggressiveresponsestoostracismservedtofortifythreatenedneeds(e.g.,need forcontrol), aggressiveresponsesshoulddecrease if individualsweregivenanonaggressiveoptiontofortifythemselvesafterostracism.Theresearch­ersmanipulatedthisnonaggressiveoptionbyhavingparticipantslistentoaseriesofaversivenoiseblasts.Halfoftheparticipantsweregivencontrolovertheonsetoftheblasts;theotherhalfhadnocontroloverblastonset.Warburtonandcol­leaguesfoundthatostracizedparticipantswhowerenotgiventhechancetofortifythemselvesbyhavingcontroloverthenoisetaskrespondedmostaggressivelytoostracism.Ostracizedparticipantsgivencontroloverthenoisetaskwerenomorelikelytoaggressthannonostracizedpeople.Theresearchersconcludedthatgivingostracizedparticipantscontroloveranaspectoftheirenvironmentfortifiedtheirthreatenedneedsandreducedtheirrelianceonaggressionasameansoffortifyingtheseneeds.

Predictive Control, Ostracism, and Increased Aggression

Wesselmann and colleagues (Wesselmann, Butler, Williams, & Pickett, 2010)extended the argument that control needs have an important function in theostracism→aggression relation. These researchers argued that unpredictableostracism(typicallythetypeexperiencedinlaboratorystudies;seeTwengeetal.,2001) provides adouble threat for targets: not only does this typeof ostracismthreatenneedsatisfaction,butitalsoshakestheirconfidenceintheirsociometer.Sociometertheory(Learyetal.,1995)isoneofthesocialpsychologicaltheoriesthatproposetheexistenceofapsychologicalmechanism(i.e.,asociometer)thatenablesindividualstodetectcuesofpotentialostracismduringsocialinteractions;a properly working sociometer affords an individual predictive control over aninteraction.

Wesselmannand colleagues (2010) hypothesized the lack ofpredictive con­trol inherent inunpredictableostracismshould increaseparticipants’ aggressiveresponses;participantswhocanpredictostracismshouldstillperceivesomepre­dictive control and be less inclined to respond aggressively. They manipulatedpredictivecontrolbyvaryingconfederatebehaviortowardparticipantsbeforeanostracismmanipulation.Confederatesweretrainedtotreateachparticipanteitherinafriendlyorunfriendlymannerduringagroupdiscussion.Afterthediscussion,

Page 58: the Psychology of Social Conflict and Aggression Sydney Symposium in Social Psychology

the link BetWeen oStraciSm and aggreSSion 41

participantswereinformedthateithereveryone(inclusion)ornoone(ostracism)wantedtoworkwiththeminanupcomingactivity.Ineitheroftheseconditions,participantswereinformedthatthetaskwouldnotaccommodatethatoutcomesotheywouldbeworkingwithanewparticipantwhoarrivedlateforadifferentstudy(thusnotpartoftheparticipants’groupdiscussion).Participantswereinstructedtoprepareasampleofhotsaucefortheirpartnertoconsume(theaggressionmea­sure).Participantsweretoldthattheirpartnerdidnotlikespicyfoodsandthattheirpartnerwouldhavetoconsumehowevermuchtheparticipantallocated.Resultsindicatedthatparticipantswhoweretreatedfriendlybutsubsequentlyostracized(unpredictable ostracism)perceivedthattheywerelesscapableofpredictingoth­ers’behavior(i.e.,hadabrokensociometer)andsubsequentlyallocatedmoregramsofhotsaucethanparticipantswhoweretreatedunfriendlybeforebeingostracized(predictable ostracism).Wesselmannandcolleaguesinterpretedthesefindingsasfurtherevidencefortheimportanceofcontrolneedsinhowaggressivelyindividu­alsmayrespondtoostracism.

are oStracized individual alWayS antiSocial?Antisocialoraggressivereactionsarenottheonlywayindividualsrespondtoostra­cism.Severalstudieshavefoundthatostracizedindividualsmayrespondtotheirtreatment in prosocialways, perhaps striving tobecome reincluded. Ostracizedindividualshavebeenfoundtoworkharderonacollectivegrouptask(Williams&Sommer,1997),toconform(Williamsetal.,2000),tocomply(Carter­Sowell,Chen,&Williams,2008),toobey(Torstrick,2010),tolikeorshowinterestinnewgroups (Maner, DeWall, Baumeister, & Schaller, 2007; Predmore & Williams,1983),andtoattempttogainsocialreassurancebyremainingamemberofagroup(Snoek, 1962) than included individuals. Research also finds these individualsmorelikelytoemulateacooperativegroupmember(Ouwerkerk,Kerr,Gallucci,&VanLange,2005)andtoengageinnonconsciousmimicry(Lakin&Chartrand,2005;Lakin,Chartrand,&Arkin,2008).Finally,ostracizedindividualsaremoresocially attentive (Bernstein, Young, Brown, Sacco, & Claypool, 2008; DeWall,Maner,&Rouby,2009;Gardner,Pickett,&Brewer,2000;Pickett,Gardner,&Knowles,2004).

Howdowemakesenseoftheseseeminglycontradictorybehavioralresponsepatterns?RecallthatWilliams(2009)arguedthatbehavioralresponsestoostracismarefocusedonfortifyingtheirbasicneedsthathavebeenthreatened.Williamsarguedfurtherthatspecificbehavioralresponsestoostracismshoulddependonthetypesofneedsindividualsaremotivatedtofortify.Prosocialresponseslikelyfocusonfortifyingneedsforbelongingandself­esteem—werefertotheseastheinclu-sionary needscluster—andaggressiveresponseslikelyfocusonfortifyingneedsformeaningfulexistenceandcontrol(thepower–provocation needscluster).

This premise has yet to be tested directly in an experimental setting, butthereareseveralstudiesthatcouldbereinterpretedwithinthisframework.First,Warburton and colleagues (2006) found that ostracized participants who hadtheircontrolneedsrestoredbeforetheaggressionmeasurewerenomorelikelytoaggressthanincludedparticipants;ostracizedparticipantswhowerenotafforded

Page 59: the Psychology of Social Conflict and Aggression Sydney Symposium in Social Psychology

kipling d. WilliamS and eric d. WeSSelmann42

this restoration replicated the typical ostracism→aggression relation.AccordingtoWilliams(2009),ostracizedparticipantswhowerefortifiedsubsequentlywouldnotneedtorespondaggressivelybecausetheyhadalreadyrecoveredtheirneedsatisfaction.TheresearchbyWesselmannandcolleagues(2010)alsosupportsthisidea—becausepredictedostracismis lessofa threattoneedsthanunpredictedostracism,lessaggressionwouldbenecessarytorecover.Additionally,theLawson,Williams,andWilliamsstudydemonstratedincreasedordesiredcontrolfollow­ingostracismwasmost likely tooccurwhen individualswereostracizedby twootherswhowerefriendswitheachother(butnotwiththeparticipant).Follow­upresearch demonstrated that people feel the most control threat when they arestrangersamongotherswhoarefriends,soweonceagainseeincreasedcontrolexertionascontrolthreatincreases(Lawson,Williams,&Williams,1998).

OtherresearchofferssupportforWilliams’s(2009)need­fortificationargu­mentfromadifferentperspective.Twengeandcolleagues(Twengeetal.,2007)found that ostracized participants who were either reminded of a positivesocial relationshiporhadapleasant interactionwithanexperimenterbeforean aggression measure were subsequently less likely to respond aggressively.Finally,Bernsteinandcolleagues(Bernstein,Sacco,Brown,Young,&Claypool,2010)demonstratedthatparticipants’needsforbelongingandself­esteemhadanimportantimpactontheirprosocialresponsestoostracism.Theseresearch­ersfoundthathigherneedthreatstobelongingandself­esteemmediatedtherelationbetweenostracismandparticipants’preferences for interactingwithpotentialsourcesofaffiliation.

What factorS determine Which needS are Salient?

Williams (2009) argues that attributions based on situational context and indi­vidualdifferences are likely to to be an importantpredictor in how individualschoosetofortifytheirthreatenedneedsbehaviorally.Theseattributionswilldic­tatewhichclusterofneeds(inclusionaryorpower–provocation)ismostsalientandthus theprimary focus for fortification.Forexample, if the inclusionary clusterismostsalient,ostracizedindividualsshouldchoosetobehaveinprosocialways,whichelevatetheirchanceforsatisfyingbelongingandself­esteem.Alternatively,whenthepower–provocationclusterismostsalient,ostracizedindividualsshouldchooseantisocial(e.g.,aggressive)behaviorstoelevatetheirchanceforsatisfyingcontrolandmeaningfulexistence.

Severaldifferent situational and individualdifference factors can influcenceattributionsforostracismandultimatelythebehavioralreponsesfromindividuals(seeWilliams,2009,fordiscussion).Weproposethatanimportantfactorinostra­cizedindividuals’attributionprocessesistheirlikelihoodofbeingreincludedbythetargetoftheirsubsequentbehavioralresponses(seealsoTwenge,2005).Thepotentialforreinclusionbyanotherindividualorgroupshouldmaketheinclusion­aryclustermostsalient; if there is littlechance forreinclusion, then theclusterthatismostlikelytobefocusedonispower–provocation.Wewillnowrevieweach

Page 60: the Psychology of Social Conflict and Aggression Sydney Symposium in Social Psychology

the link BetWeen oStraciSm and aggreSSion 43

of the paradigms used in studying the ostracism→aggression relation and thendiscusshowthepotentialforreinclusion(orlackthereof)mayhavefacilitatedanti­socialresponsestoostracismduetomakingthepower–provocationclustermostsalientineachoftheseparadigms.

General Descriptions of the Typical Ostracism Paradigms

Severalparadigmsareusedtoinvestigatetheostracism→aggressionrelationinexperimental settings. We will now describe the general elements of each oftheseparadigms.Oneisthelife-aloneparadigm(Twengeetal.,2001),inwhichparticipantsfilloutapersonalityinventoryandarefirstgivenaccuratefeedbackabouttheirintroversion–extraversion.Followingthis,theyarerandomlyassignedtoreceiveaprognosisabouttheirfuturelives:theyaretoldtheywillhavealifecharacterized by strong close relationships or that they will live a life alone,devoidofstrongcontinuousrelationships.Forparticipantsinthelife­alonecon­dition,thefeedbackinformsthemthattheyarepowerlesstodoanythingabouttheirlackofinclusion.

Anotherparadigmistheget-acquaintedparadigm(Buckleyetal.,2004;Chowetal.,2008;Twengeetal.,2001;Twenge&Campbell,2003;Wesselmannetal.,2010). Researchers who use this type of paradigm typically ask participants toengageinagroupactivitydesignedtoallowmembersofthegrouptogettoknoweachother.Following this interaction,participantsare told that theyeitherhadbeenrejectedbymembersofthisgrouporhadbeenacceptedbythem.

Thethirdcommonparadigminostracismresearchistheball-tossing paradigm(Carter­Sowell et al., 2010;Chow et al., 2008; Warburtonet al., 2006). Studiesusing this paradigm engage participants in a ball­tossing game with other con­federates,eitherinaface­to­faceformat(originallyusedinWilliams&Sommer,1997)or via anelectronic­basedcomputerprogramcalledCyberball (originallyusedinWilliamsetal.,2000;seealsothe“O­Cam”inChapter13ofthisvolume).Regardlessoftheformatforballtossing,participantsareeitherincludedbytheconfederates(i.e.,tossedtheball33%ofthetime)orostracizedbyconfederates(i.e.,tossedtheballtwiceatthebeginningofthegameandthenneveragain).Atypicalgamelastsbetween3and5minutes.

Each of these paradigms has been adapted in various studies, manipulat­ing different situational factors to elucidate the processes and nuances of theostracism→aggressionrelation.Wewillnowdiscussthesestudiesindetail,focus­ing on how these different situational factors may have influenced participants’perceptionsofnopotentialforreinclusion,makingthepower–provocationclustermoresalientthantheinclusionarycluster.

Aggression Toward the Source of Ostracism

Themajorityofresearchontheostracism→aggressionlinkhasexaminedaggres­sive behavior toward the sources of ostracism. These studies have used varia­tions on both the get­acquainted paradigm (Buckley et al., 2004; Chow et al.,2009,Study2;Twenge&Campbell,2003,Study3)andtheCyberballparadigm

Page 61: the Psychology of Social Conflict and Aggression Sydney Symposium in Social Psychology

kipling d. WilliamS and eric d. WeSSelmann44

(Carter­Sowelletal.,2010;Chowetal.,2008,Study1).Inallof thesestudies,participantsweregiven theopportunity to aggressagainst the individualswhowereresponsiblefortheirpreviousostracism.Participantsalsowerenotgivenareasonorexplanationfortheirostracism.Inthesesituations,thepower–provo­cationcluster shouldbemost salientbecauseparticipants likelydidnotexpecttheopportunityforreinclusionbythesourcesofostracism.Chowandcolleagues(2009,Study2)manipulated informationabout the sources’perceptionsof theparticipantandfoundthatparticipantswhothoughttheywereostracizedduetothesources’misinformationwerelesslikelytoaggressbecausetheparticipantscould make an external attribution for the ostracism. These participants knewthattheywouldnothavetheabilitytobereincludedbythesourcesofostracism,buttheyalsothoughtthattheostracismwasduetomisinformationratherthansomethinginherentaboutthemselves.

Anecdotally,therearealsoindicationsthattargetsofthedyadicandinterper­sonalformofostracism,thesilenttreatment,oftenresorttoaggressiontowardthesourceofthesilenttreatmentasawaytoprovokearesponse(Zadro,Arriaga,&Williams,2008;forotherexamplesofinterpersonalassertivenessandviolenceinthisvolume,seeChapters2,5,11,12,and13).

Aggression Toward a Stranger

Therehavealsobeenprogramsofresearchexaminingparticipants’aggressiveresponsestowardstrangers.Thesestudieshavealsousedvariationsontheget­acquainted(Twengeetal.,2007,Studies4and5;Twenge&Campbell,2003,Study4)andball­tossparadigms(Warburtonetal.,2006),aswellasthelife­aloneparadigm(Twengeetal.,2001,Studies1–3).Wearguethatthepower–provo­cationclusterwasmostsalient ineachof thesestudies, similar to thestudieswhere the targets of aggression were responsible for participants’ ostracism.Thestudiesthatusedthelife­aloneparadigm(alsoTwengeetal.,2001,Study4) gave ostracized participants the opportunity to aggress against a strangerwhoprovokedthem.Participantslikelydidnotexpecttobeincludedbythesestrangers, even though they were not the source of participants’ ostracism—provocationisnotatypicalresponsefromasourceofinclusionoracceptance(seeLearyetal.,1995).Thestudiesusingtheball­tossingandget­acquaintedparadigmsareabitmoreperplexingwhenconsideringhowthepower–provo­cationclusterwasmostsalient.Noneofthesestudiesofferedparticipantstheopportunitytointeractwiththestrangerotherthanviatheaggressionmeasure,effectivelyremovingthisstrangerasapotentialsourceofreinclusionforpar­ticipants.Wesselmannandcolleagues(2010)offerusanadditionalexplanationforwhytheget­acquaintedparadigmmaymakethepower–provocationclustermoresalientthantheinclusionarycluster.Thetraditionalget­acquaintedpara­digmconfrontsparticipantswithunexpectedostracism,thusthreateningtheirperceptionsoftheirabilitytopredictandforestallostracism.Theseindividualswouldbelesslikelytofocusonstrivingforreinclusion(i.e.inclusionarycluster)and instead should focus moreon fortifyingpower–provocationcluster (typi­callyviaaggression).

Page 62: the Psychology of Social Conflict and Aggression Sydney Symposium in Social Psychology

the link BetWeen oStraciSm and aggreSSion 45

the influence of attriButionS and expectationS in the reSignation Stage

Williams(2009)arguesthatifostracismpersistsoveranextendedperiodoftimeindividuals will progress to a third stage—resignation. Extended ostracism canbefromthesame individualorgrouporbyanynumberofdifferentsources.Ifindividualsfind that theirbehavioral responses toostracismfail to restore theirneedsatisfaction,theylearnthatanyattempttorecoverfromostracismis likelyfutile. These individuals should then develop feelings of alienation, depression,helplessness,andunworthiness.It is likelythateachofthesenegativeoutcomesfrom chronic ostracism will influence attributions for ostracism and ultimatelyexpectationsforreinclusion.

Theresignationstageofostracismhasbeenlargelyignoredinscientificresearch,particularlyinexperimentalparadigmsduetotheethicalandpracticalconcernsofmanipulatingchronicostracisminalaboratory.Severalstudieslendsupporttoapotentiallinkamongchronicostracism,expectationsforreinclusion,andaggres­sion.Manerandcolleagues(2007)foundthatindividualswhowerehighinFearofNegativeEvaluation(whoexpectgenerallyunpleasantsocialinteractions)per­ceived potential sources for reinclusion as hostile after experiencing ostracism,whereasindividualslowinthistraitrespondedfavorablytothesesources.Zadro(2004;Chapter13inthisvolume;seealsoWilliams,2001)conductedqualitativeinterviewswithover50individualswhoreportedexperiencingchronicostracismfromcoworkers,friends,orfamilymembers.Severaloftheseindividualsreportedengaging inaggressivebehaviors inanattempt tobenoticed,particularlywhenotherattemptsatreinclusionprovedfutile.Finally,researchusingthelife­aloneparadigm(Twengeetal.,2001)mayalsobereinterpretedinthisframework.Recallthatindividualsinthelife­aloneconditionaretoldthattheywillfaceostracismfortherestoftheirlives,regardlessofhowmuchefforttheydedicatetoreinclusion.Participantsinthisconditiontypicallyrespondtopotentialsourcesofreinclusionwithaggression.Thislife­aloneconditionistheclosestexperimentalmanipulationof chronic ostracismcurrently in the literature, even though it is limited by itsprospectivenature.

reSignation Stage, need Salience, and extreme aggreSSion

Perhapsonereasonforthecurrentfascinationwiththeostracism→aggressionrela­tionisthatwearesearchingforexplanationsforarecentsurgeinseeminglyirra­tional and socially intolerablebehaviors thathave appearedworldwide: randomactsofmonstrousviolence.Innewsreportsthatweconsideralmostroutinenow,wearebombardedwithstoriesofcountlessincidencesinwhichindividuals,oftenstudents inhighschoolorcollege,havewieldedweaponsand,withoutapparentconcernfortheirownsurvival,haveshotandkilledmanyoftheirpeersandteach­ers.Wehavewitnessedpeople’swillingnesstoconductterroristactsagainstcount­lessandunknownothers,againwithplausiblecertaintythatincarryingouttheseactstheywillperishwiththevictims.

Page 63: the Psychology of Social Conflict and Aggression Sydney Symposium in Social Psychology

kipling d. WilliamS and eric d. WeSSelmann46

School Violence

Since1994,inU.S.schoolsalonetherehavebeenmorethan220separateshoot­ingincidentsinwhichatleastonepersonwaskilledand18episodesthatinvolvedmultiplekillings(Andersonetal.,2001).Massshootingsatschoolsandotherpublicplacesareoccurringwithincreasingfrequencyandinagrowingnumberofothercountries.Reasonsforthisupsurgeinviolencearestillnotclear,butarecentlineof investigationhas linkedsuchincidentswithgrowingsocial isolation(Twenge,2000), and furtherevidence isbeginning toemerge thatprolongedexperiencesofostracismmayhaveplayedasignificantmotivatingroleintheactionsofmanyperpetrators.Intheircaseanalysisof15post­1995U.S.schoolshootings,Learyetal.(2003)suggestthatchronicostracismwasamajorcontributingfactorin87%ofcases.StudiesofMartinBryant,who,in1996,killed35peopleatapopulartour­istattractionatPortArthurinTasmania,suggestthathefeltlonelyandisolated(Bingham,2000;Crook,1997),andRobertSteinhauser,whokilled16peopleathisformerhighschoolinErfurt,Germany, in2002,thoughnotasocialoutcast(Lemonick,2002),hadbeengreatlyupsetbyasignificantactofostracism—expul­sionfromhisschool.Veryrecently,atValparaisoHighSchoolinIndiana,a15­year­oldboyheldhostageandslashedwithtwosharp­edgedblades—onedescribedasamachete—sevenofhisclassmates.Whenpeerswereaskedaboutthisboy,itwasreported, “He was so invisible atValparaiso HighSchool this fall that studentswhosatnexttohiminSpanishclassdidn’tknowhisname”(“7ValparaisoHighStudentsHurtinStabbingRampage,”Indianapolis Star,November25,2004).Theconsequencesofbeingostracized,eitherintentionallyorunintentionally,seemtobeathreadthatweavesthroughcaseaftercaseofschoolviolence.

Weproposethattheseeventscanbereinterpretedassituationswherethepower–provocationclusterwaslikelymoresalientthaninclusionaryneeds.Individualswhofacechronicostracismshouldnotexpectopportunitiesforreinclusion,havinganypreviousattemptstofortifytheinclusionaryclusterproveineffective.Ifanindivid­ualhasbeencontinuallythwartedinattemptstobereincluded,thenheorshehasnoreasontoexpecttobeincludedinfutureinteractions.Thus,thepower–provoca­tionclustershouldbethemostsalientoptionforneedfortification,andaggressivebehaviorisaneffectivemethodforsatisfyingthisgoal(Williams,2009).

Extremist Groups

Many instancesof school violence involve lone perpetrators, or atmost a smallgroupofperpetrators(e.g.,theperpetratorsoftheColumbineHighSchoolmas­sacre).Thereareotheractsofviolenceperpetratedbylargergroupsofdisaffectedindividualsindifferentsocialsettingsthatmayberelevanttoourdiscussionofhowextendedexposuretoostracismcanfacilitateviolence.WesselmannandWilliams(2010) argued that individualswhoareconsistentlyostracizedby individualsorgroupsmaybecomepotentialcandidatesforrecruitmentbydubiousgroups,suchascults,gangs,andeventerroristorganizations.Ingeneral,ostracizedindividualsaremorelikelytocomplywithsocialinfluencetactics(Carter­Sowelletal.,2008)ortoconformtogroupnormsandexpectations(Ouwerkerketal.,2005;Williams&

Page 64: the Psychology of Social Conflict and Aggression Sydney Symposium in Social Psychology

the link BetWeen oStraciSm and aggreSSion 47

Sommer,1997).Thisstrivingforacceptanceextendsbeyondcontrolledbehaviors;ostracizedindividualsaremorelikelytomimicotherindividualsinanonconsciousmanner (Lakin&Chartrand,2005;Lakin,Chartrand,&Arkin,2008).What ifostracizedindividualsstrivetobereincludedsomuchthattheydonotrationallyappraisethemotivesbehindorconsequencesofbeingwillinglyinfluencedbythepotentialsourcesofreinclusion?Chapter10inthisvolumeshedsfurtherlightonthispossibility.

We propose that long­term ostracism can cause such a strong desire tobelong, to be liked, by someone—perhaps anyone—that individuals’ abilitytodiscriminategood frombadandright fromwrongmaybe impaired to thepointthattheymaybecomeattractedtocultsandextremistgroupsthatcouldultimately influence them to acts of violence. Political scientists Tom NairnandPaulJames(2005)suggest that theprofileofAustraliancitizenswhohadrecentlyjoinedterroristgroupslikeAlQaedaisofindividualswhofeelisolated,marginalized,orostracizedwithin their society andwhoare attracted to theintense face­to­face connectedness that these extremist groups have to offer.Joiningandfollowingthedictatesofextremistgroupsfulfillneedsnotonlyforbelonging and self­esteembut also for control and recognition because thesegroupspromiseretribution,worldwideattention,andpersonalsignificance(seeKruglanski,Chen,Dechesne,Fishman,&Orehek,2009;seealsoChapter10inthisvolume).

concluSionS and future directionS for reSearch

Ostracismisapainfuleventthatmanyindividualsexperience,sometimesonadailybasis(Nezleketal.,2004).Researchhasdemonstratedthataggressionisacommonresponsetoostracismandislikelyfocusedonrecoveringthwartedneedsatisfac­tion(Williams,2009).Thischapterreviewedresearchonindividuals’reactionstoostracismovertime,particularlyonwhenandwhyindividualschooseaggressiveresponsesratherthanprosocialoptions.WeproposedanextensiontoWilliams’sneed–recoveryhypothesisonhowtopredictwhetherindividualswillrespondwithpro­orantisocialbehaviorsinresponsetoostracism.Wearguethatanimportantfactor inostracizedindividuals’attributionprocesses istheir likelihoodofbeingreincludedbythetargetof theirsubsequentbehavioral responses.Wereviewedtheexperimentalresearchdocumentingtheostracism→aggressionlink,highlight­ingtheelementsofthosestudiesthatareconsistentwiththisinterpretation.

Thethirdstageofostracism—theresignationstage—isparticularlyrelevanttoourdiscussionofexpectationsforreinclusion.Individualswhoexperiencechronicostracismhavehad theirneedsatisfactionconsistently thwartedand likelyhaveresignedthemselvestotheirlackofopportunitiesforinclusion.Theseindividualsfindthemselvesinasituationwherepower–provocationneedsaretheirlastbas­tion for fortification. This argument is consistent with the literature document­ingthelinkbetweenchronicostracismandextremeviolence(Learyetal.,2003;Williams,2001).However,muchofthisresearchiscorrelationalandoftenbased

Page 65: the Psychology of Social Conflict and Aggression Sydney Symposium in Social Psychology

kipling d. WilliamS and eric d. WeSSelmann48

onqualitativeinterviewsoranecdotes.Futureresearchneedstofocusoninves­tigatingtheresignationstageinrigorousexperimentalsettings,particularlyhowperceptionsforreinclusionmaymakeaggressiveresponsesmorelikelythanproso­cial responses to ostracism. When we achieve a sophisticated understanding ofchronicostracism,wewillhavebetterinsightintowhenandwhyindividualsmayengageinantisocialanddestructiveresponses.

referenceS

“7 Valparaiso High Students Hurt in Stabbing Rampage,” Indianapolis Star, November25,2004.

Anderson,M.A.,Kaufman,J.,Simon,T.R.,Barrios,L.,Paulozzi,L.,Ryan,G.,etal.(2001).School­associatedviolentdeathsintheUnitedStates.Journal of the American Medical Association, 286,2695–2700.

Bastian,B.,&Haslam,N.(2010).Excludedfromhumanity:Thedehumanizingeffectsofsocialostracism.Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 46,107–113.

Baumeister,R.F.,DeWall,C.N.,Ciarocco,N.J.,&Twenge,J.M.(2005).Socialexclusionimpairsself­regulation.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,88,589–604.

Baumeister,R.F.,&Leary,M.R.(1995).Theneedtobelong:Desirefor inter­personalattachments as a fundamental human motivation. Psychological Bulletin, 117,497–529.

Bernstein, M. J., Sacco, D. F., Brown, C. M., Young, S. G., & Claypool, H. M. (2010).Apreferenceforgenuinesmilesfollowingsocialexclusion.Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 46,196–199.

Bernstein,M.J.,Young,S.G.,Brown,C.M.,Sacco,D.F.,&Claypool,H.(2008).Adaptiveresponses to social exclusion: Social rejection improves detection of real and fakesmiles.Psychological Science,19,981–983.

Bingham,M. (2000).Suddenly one Sunday (2ded.)Pymble,NewSouthWales:HarperCollins.

Buckley,K.E.,Winkel,R.E.,&Leary,M.R.(2004).Reactionstoacceptanceandrejection:Effectsoflevelandsequenceofrelationalevaluation.Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 40,14–28.

Carter­Sowell,A.R.,Chen,Z.,&Williams,K.D.(2008).Ostracismincreasessocialsuscep­tibility.Social Influence, 3.143–153.

Carter­Sowell,A.R.,VanBeestI.,vanDijk,E.,&Williams,K.D.(2010).Groupsbeingostracizedbygroups: Is thepainshared, is recoveryquicker,andaregroupsmorelikelytobeaggressive?Manuscriptinpreparation.

Chen, Z., Williams, K. D., Fitness, J., & Newton, N. C. (2008).When hurt won’t heal:Exploringthecapacitytorelivesocialpain.Psychological Science, 19,789–795.

Chow,R.M.,Tiedens,L.Z.,&Govan,C.L.(2008).Excludedemotions:Theroleofangerinantisocialresponsestoostracism.Journal of Experimental Social Psychology,44,896–903.

Crook, J. B. (1997). Port Arthur: Gun tragedy, gun law miracle. Melbourne: GunControlAustralia.

DeWall,C.N.,Maner,J.K.,&Rouby,D.A.(2009).Socialexclusionandearly­stageinter­personalperception:Selectiveattentiontosignsofacceptance.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 96,729–741.

Eisenberger,N.I.,Lieberman,M.D.,&Williams,K.D.(2003).Doesrejectionhurt?AnfMRIstudyofsocialexclusion.Science,302,290–292.

Page 66: the Psychology of Social Conflict and Aggression Sydney Symposium in Social Psychology

the link BetWeen oStraciSm and aggreSSion 49

Gardner,W.,Pickett,C.L.,&Brewer,M.B.(2000).Socialexclusionandselectivememory:Howtheneedtobelonginfluencesmemoryforsocialevents.Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 26,486–496.

Kassner,M.P.,Wirth,J.H.,Law,A.T.,&Williams,K.D.(2010).Effectsofmentalvisualizationandsocialcuesondetectionandinfluenceofostracism.Manuscriptinpreparation.

Kerr,N.L.,&Levine,J.M.(2008).Thedetectionofsocialexclusion:Evolutionandbeyond.Group Dynamics: Theory, Research, and Practice, 12,39–52.

Kruglanski,A.W.,Chen,X.,Dechesne,M.,Fishman,S.,&Orehek,E.(2009).Fullycom­mitted:Suicidebombers’motivationandthequestforpersonalsignificance.Political Psychology, 30,331–357.

Lakin,J.L.,&Chartrand,T.L.(2005).Exclusionandnonconsciousbehavioralmimicry.InK.D.Williams,J.P.Forgas,&W.vonHippel(Eds.),The social outcast: Ostracism, social exclusion, rejection, and bullying(pp.279–295).NewYork:PsychologyPress.

Lakin,J.L.,Chartrand,T.L.,&Arkin,R.M.(2008).Iamtoojustlikeyou:Nonconsciousmimicryasanautomaticbehavioralresponsetosocialexclusion.Psychological Science, 19,816–822.

LawsonWilliams,H.,&Williams,K.D.(1998,April).Effects of social ostracism on desire for control.PresentedatSocietyforAustralasianSocialPsychology,Christchurch,NZ.

Leary,M.R.,Kowalski,R.M.,Smith,L.,&Phillips,S.(2003).Teasing,rejection,andvio­lence:Casestudiesoftheschoolshootings.Aggressive Behavior, 29,202–214.

Leary,M.R.,Tambor,E.S.,Terdal,S.K.,&Downs,D.L.(1995).Self­esteemasaninter­personal monitor: The sociometer hypothesis. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,68,518–530.

Leary,M.R.,Twenge,J.M.,&Quinlivan,E.(2006).Interpersonalrejectionasadeterminantofangerandaggression.Personality and Social Psychology Review,10,111–132.

Lemonick,M.D.(2002,May6).Germany’sColumbine.Time Magazine(NewYork),159(18),36–39.

MacDonald,G.,&LearyM.R.(2005).Whydoessocialexclusionhurt?Therelationshipbetweensocialandphysicalpain.Psychological Bulletin, 131,202–223.

Maner,J.K.,DeWall,C.N.,Baumeister,R.F.,&Schaller,M.(2007).Doessocialexclusionmotivateinterpersonalreconnection?Resolvingthe“porcupineproblem.”Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 92, 42–55.

Nairn,T.,&James,P.(2005).Global matrix: Nationalism, globalism, and state-terrorism.London:PlutoPress.

Nezlek,J.,Wheeler,L.,Williams,K.,&Govan,C.(2004,January).Ostracismineverydaylife.PresentedattheSocietyforPersonalityandSocialPsychology,Austin,TX.

Oaten, M., Williams, K. D., Jones, A., & Zadro, L. (2008). The effects of ostracism onself­regulationinthesociallyanxious.Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 27,471–504.

Ouwerkerk,J.W.,Kerr,N.L.,Gallucci,M.,&VanLange,P.A.M.(2005).Avoidingthesocialdeathpenalty:Ostracismandcooperationinsocialdilemmas.InK.D.Williams,J.P.Forgas,&W.vonHippel(Eds.),The social outcast: Ostracism, social exclusion, rejection, and bullying(pp.321–332).NewYork:PsychologyPress.

Pickett,C.L.,Gardner,W.L.,&Knowles,M.(2004).Gettingacue:Theneedtobelongandenhancedsensitivity tosocialcues.Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 30,1095–1107.

Predmore,S.J.,&Williams,K.D.(1983,May).The effects of social ostracism on affilia-tion.Paperpresentedat themeetingof theMidwesternPsychologicalAssociation,Chicago,IL.

Prislin,R.,Williams,K.D.,&Sawicki,V.(2010).Newmajorities’abuseofpower:Effectsofperceivedcontrolandsocial support.Unpublishedmanuscript,SanDiegoStateUniversity,CA.

Page 67: the Psychology of Social Conflict and Aggression Sydney Symposium in Social Psychology

kipling d. WilliamS and eric d. WeSSelmann50

Riva, P., Wirth, J. H., & Williams, K. D. (2010, May). The social impact of suffering:Psychicalpainthwartssocialneeds.PaperpresentedatthemeetingoftheMidwesternPsychologicalAssociation,Chicago,IL.

Smith,A.,&Williams,K.D.(2004).RUThere?Effectsofostracismbycellphonemes­sages.Group Dynamics: Theory, Research, and Practice,8,291–301.

Snoek,J.D.(1962).Someeffectsofrejectionuponattractiontoagroup.Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 64,175–182.

Spoor,J.R.,&Williams,K.D.(2007).Theevolutionofanostracismdetectionsystem.InJ.P.Forgas,M.G.Haselton,&W.vonHippel(Eds.),Evolution and the social mind: Evolutionary psychology and social cognition (pp.279–292).NewYork:PsychologyPress.

Steinbeck,J.(1987).Of mice and men/Cannery row.NewYork:PenguinBooks.(Originalworkpublished1945.)

Tedeschi, J. T. (2001). Social power, influence, and aggression. In J. P. Forgas & K. D.Williams(Eds.),Social influence: Direct and indirect processes(pp.109–128).NewYork:PsychologyPress.

Twenge,J.M.(2000).Theageofanxiety?Thebirthcohortchangeinanxietyandneuroti­cism,1952–1993.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 79, 1007–1021.

Twenge, J. M. (2005). When does social rejection lead to aggression? The influences ofsituations,narcissism,emotion,andreplenishingconnections.InK.D.Williams,J.P.Forgas,&W.vonHippel(Eds.),The social outcast: Ostracism, social exclusion, rejec-tion, and bullying(pp.201–212).NewYork:PsychologyPress.

Twenge,J.M.,Baumeister,R.F.,Tice,D.M.,&Stucke,T.S.(2001).Ifyoucan’tjointhem,beatthem:Effectsofsocialexclusiononaggressivebehavior.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 81,1058–1069.

Twenge,J.M.,&Campbell,W.K.(2003).“Isn’titfuntogettherespectthatwe’regoingto deserve?” Narcissism, social rejection, and aggression. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 29,261–272.

Twenge,J.M.,Catanese,K.R.,&Baumeister,R.F.(2002).Socialexclusioncausesself­defeatingbehavior.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 83,606–615.

Twenge, J. M., Zhang, L., Catanese, K. R., Dolan­Pascoe, B., Lyche, L. R., &Baumeister,R.F.(2007).Replenishingconnectedness:Remindersofsocialactiv­ityreduceaggressionaftersocialexclusion.British Journal of Social Psychology, 46,205–224.

Warburton,W.A.,Williams,K.D.,&Cairns,D.R.(2006).Whenostracismleadstoaggres­sion:Themoderatingeffectsofcontroldeprivation.Journal of Experimental Social Psychology,42,213–220.

Wesselmann,E.D.,Bagg,D.,&Williams,K.D.(2009).“Ifeelyourpain”:Theeffectsofobservingostracismontheostracismdetectionsystem.Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 45,1308–1311.

Wesselmann,E.D.,Butler,F.A.,Williams,K.D.,&Pickett,C.L.(2010).Addinginjurytoinsult:Unexpectedrejectionleadstomoreaggressiveresponses.Aggressive Behavior, 35,232–237.

Wesselmann,E.D.,&Williams,K.D.(2010).Thepotentialbalmofreligionandspiritual­ityforrecoveringfromostracism.Journal of Management, Spirituality, and Religion, 7,29–45.

Williams,K.D.(2001).Ostracism: The power of silence.NewYork:GuilfordPress.Williams,K.D.(2007).Ostracism:Thekissofsocialdeath.Social and Personality Psychology

Compass, 1,236–247.Williams,K.D.(2009).Ostracism:Effectsofbeingexcludedandignored.InM.P.Zanna

(Ed.),Advances in experimental social psychology(Vol.41,pp.275–314).NewYork:AcademicPress.

Page 68: the Psychology of Social Conflict and Aggression Sydney Symposium in Social Psychology

the link BetWeen oStraciSm and aggreSSion 51

Williams,K.D.,Cheung,C.K.T.,&Choi,W. (2000).Cyberostracism:Effectsofbeingignored over the Internet. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 79,748–762.

Williams,K.D.,&Govan,C.L.(2005).Reactingtoostracism:Retaliationorreconciliation?InD.Abrams,M.A.Hogg,&J.M.Marques(Eds.),The social psychology of inclu-sion and exclusion(pp.47–62).NewYork:PsychologyPress.

Williams,K.D.,Govan,C.L.,Croker,V.,Tynan,D.,Cruickshank,M.,&Lam,A.(2002).Investigationsintodifferencesbetweensocialandcyberostracism.Group Dynamics: Theory, Research, and Practice,6,65–77.

Williams,K.D.,&Sommer,K.L.(1997).Socialostracismbycoworkers:Doesrejectionlead to social loafingor compensation. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 23,693–706.

Wirth,J.H.,Sacco,D.F.,Hugenberg,K.,&Williams,K.D.(2010).Eyegazeasrelationalevaluation:Avertedeyegazeleadstofeelingsofostracismandrelationaldevaluation.Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin,36,869–882.

Zadro,L.(2004).Ostracism: Empirical studies inspired by real-world experiences of silence and exclusion.Unpublisheddoctoraldissertation.UniversityofNewSouthWales,Sydney.

Zadro,L.,Arriaga,X.B.,&Williams,K.D.(2008).Relationalostracism.InJ.P.Forgas&J.Fitness(Eds.),Social relationships: Cognitive, affective, and motivational processes(pp.305–320).NewYork:PsychologyPress.

Zadro, L., Williams, K. D., & Richardson, R. (2004). How low can you go? Ostracismbyacomputerissufficienttolowerself­reportedlevelsofbelonging,control,self­esteem,andmeaningfulexistence.Journal of Experimental Social Psychology,40,560–567.

Page 69: the Psychology of Social Conflict and Aggression Sydney Symposium in Social Psychology
Page 70: the Psychology of Social Conflict and Aggression Sydney Symposium in Social Psychology

53

4IsItAggression?

Perceptions of and Motivations for Passive and Psychological Aggression

DEBORAHSOUTHRICHARDSONandGEORGINAS.HAMMOCK

Augusta State University

T his chapter reviews programs of research on correlates and perceptionsof “everyday” formsofaggression thatoftenarenotconsidered in tradi­tionalaggressionresearch.Thesestudiesrevealthateverydaypassiveand

psychologicalaggressionareoftenmotivatedbyintentionsotherthantheinten­tion tocauseharm(e.g., inducingguilt), although theeffect is toharmthe tar­get.Similarly,comparisonofperceptionsofpsychologicalandphysicalaggressionrevealthatpsychologicalaggression,whichisdefinedintermsofharminganindi­vidual’sself­regard,maybeperceivedaslessdamagingthanphysicalaggression,althoughthepotentialforlong­termharmisgreater(e.g.,Follingstad,Rutledge,Berg,Hause,&Polek,1990).

introductionAggressionistypicallydefinedasbehaviorintendedtoharmanotherperson(Baron& Richardson, 1994). Although there has been some argument about whetherintentionshouldbecentraltothedefinitionofaggression(i.e.,aggressionisbehav­iorthatharms,regardlessofintentionoftheaggressor),mostcurrentdefinitionsofaggressioninvolvetheconceptofintentiontoharm.Suchdefinitionsthusrequire

Page 71: the Psychology of Social Conflict and Aggression Sydney Symposium in Social Psychology

deBorah South richardSon and georgina S. hammock54

thatweconsidertheobserver’sinferenceaboutanactor’sgoals(Tedeschi&Felson,1994)andthatweconsideravarietyoftypesofharm.

The harm of direct aggression is readily apparent, and intention seems tobeeasilydetermined.Apersonwhodeliversablowora face­to­face insult toanotherpersonisclearlyintendingtocauseharm.Mosttheoreticaltreatmentsand empirical findings regarding human aggressive behavior have focused onsuch obvious aggressive acts. However, some types of harm are more elusive,moreopentoalternativeinterpretations.Forexample,Imightcommentonanacquaintance’sunusualformofattireoutofcuriosityortomakehimfeelself­conscious;mycuriositywouldnotbeharmful,butanattempttomakehimself­consciouswouldbe.Similarly,forgettingtopickupthewineonthewayhomewhenwearehavingadinnerpartymightbeanhonestmistake,oritmightbeanattempttomakemypartnerlookbadtocompany(thatIdidn’twanttohaveanyway).Theselessdirectformsofaggressiondoindeedcauseharm,buttheyareeasiertodenyandmoredifficulttointerpret;inaddition,theaggressorcandeny the intent to harm. These forms of aggressive behavior may have othergoalsinadditiontoorinsteadofharmtothetarget.Forexample,psychologicalaggressionmayharmthetargetbyhumiliatingordemeaninghimorher;pas­siveaggressionmayharmatargetbyobstructingagoal;indirectaggressionmayharmatargetbydisruptingrelationships.

We argue that it is important to consider the various forms of aggressionbecausedirectorphysicalaggressionisnotwhatpeoplearelikelytoexperiencemost frequently in theirday­to­day lives.Theyaremore likely tobe victimsofsnideremarksorhostileattitudesthantheyaretobevictimsofcriminalviolence.Individuals aremore likely togossip about someone than to slaporkick them.Themore subtle, lessdirect kindsof aggressionare likely to affect individuals’relationshipexperienceandsuccessaswellastheirsenseofself.Thus,wehavefocusedontheseformsofaggressioninanattempttocapturetheexperienceofeverydaypeopleexperiencingeverydayaggression.Ofcourse,avarietyofformsof everyday aggression are not addressed in this chapter—experiences such asroadrage,racialorsexualdiscrimination—butinmanycasesthosespecificformsofeverydayaggressionmightalsobeconsideredexpressionsofeitherpassiveorpsychologicalaggression.

paSSive aggreSSionPassive aggression isbehavior that is intended toharm another living being bynotdoingsomething,byobstructingthetarget’sgoals.Thisisincontrasttootherformsofaggressionsuchasdirectorindirectverbalorphysicalaggression.Directaggressioninvolvesdirectconfrontationwiththetarget(e.g.,physicalblows,verbalinsults),whereas indirectaggression isnonconfrontive,deliveringharmthroughanother person or object (e.g., spreading rumors, damaging target’s property).Indirectaggression(sometimescalledrelationalorsocialaggression)hasreceivedconsiderableattentionfromresearchers inrecentyears(Archer&Coyne,2005;Björkqvist,Osterman,&Lagerspetz,1994;Crick&Grotpeter,1995;Richardson&Green,2006).Chapter17inthisvolumenotesthatindirectaggressionmaytake

Page 72: the Psychology of Social Conflict and Aggression Sydney Symposium in Social Psychology

iS it aggreSSion? 55

avarietyofforms,includingcyberaggressionandusingsocialnetworkingtodeliverharmtoanother.Passiveaggression,however,hasreceivedlittleattention.

Passive aggression has been defined, conceptually and operationally, in awidevarietyofways in the research literature. Ina studyofdrivercharacter­isticsassociatedwithaggressivedriving androadrage,passiveaggressionwasdefinedas“impedingtraffic”(Dukes,Clayton,Jenkins,Miller,&Rodgers,2001,p.323).Kingery’s(1998)AdolescentViolenceSurveyincludesapassiveaggres­sionsubscaleconsistingofitemssuchas“Talkedaboutsomeone’sfaultstootherpeoplesootherswouldn’tlikethem”and“Preventedsomeonefromgoingwherehe/shewantedtogobygettingintheway.”Inastudyinwhichparticipantsjudgedavarietyofaggressionactions,passiveaggressionwasdefinedas“withholdingavailableandneededresources”(Berkowitz,Mueller,Schnell,&Padberg,1986,p.887).

Although direct reference to passive aggression is relatively rare in theresearch literature, several lines of research are closely related. For example,Williamsandcolleagues’workonsocialostracismhassomeclearconnectionstoourconceptofpassiveaggression(Sommer,Williams,Ciarocco,&Baumeister,2001;Williams,1997;Williams,Shore,&Grahe,1998;seealsoChapters3and13inthisvolume).Theseresearchersdefinesocialostracismas“thesilenttreat­ment.”Bothperpetratorsandtargetsofsuchsocialostracismreportthatthepri­marymotiveispunitive,“topunishorcorrectthetarget;tohurtorseekrevenge”(Sommeretal.,p.229), and theprimaryemotionreported inboth targetandsourcenarrativesisanger.Thus,suchsocialostracismmightbereasonablycon­sideredaformofpassiveaggression,behaviorthatisintendedtopunishorhurt,thatinvolvesanger,andthatinvolvesnotdoingsomething(i.e.,notattendingtothetarget).

Passiveaggressionislikelytobeanespeciallyattractivestrategyinsomecon­texts.Likeothernondirectformsofaggression(whichmayincludebothindirector social aggression as well as passive aggression), passive aggression may be adesirablealternativewhenanindividualwantstoavoiddetectionorretaliation.Itprovidestheaggressoraneasyopportunityfordenial(i.e.,“Ididn’tdoanything”)as ifbynotdoingheorsheisblameless.Thus,thepassiveaggressordeniestheharmthatmaycomefromsuchbehavior.

Wehaveconductedtwolinesofresearchaimedatprovidingaclearerunder­standingofthenatureofpassiveaggression.Thefirstwasdesignedtodeterminewhetherpassiveaggressioncanbeconsideredaunique formof aggression,dif­ferentiated fromother formsofnondirectaggression (e.g., indirector relationalaggression).Thesecond,whichwasdesignedtodeterminehowthedifferentformsofaggressionareperceivedbyaggressorsandtargets,ismostdirectlyrelevanttothetopicofthischapter;asweconsiderthequestionoftheextenttowhicheachformofaggressionisperceivedtobeharmfulormightbeperceivedasmeetingothergoals,weare,inessence,asking,“Isitaggression?”

Anunderstandingofthedistinctiveness(orlackthereof)ofpassiveaggressionisanimportantstartingpoint.Thus,wewillsummarizetheresultsofthefirstlineofresearchbeforefocusingontheresearchthatexaminesmotivationsandeffectsofthedifferentformsofaggression.

Page 73: the Psychology of Social Conflict and Aggression Sydney Symposium in Social Psychology

deBorah South richardSon and georgina S. hammock56

Distinctiveness of Passive Aggression

Threestudiesexaminedwhetherpassiveaggressioncouldbedistinguishedfromdirectandindirectaggression.TheRichardsonConflictResponseQuestionnaire(RCRQ),whichhasbeenusedinavarietyofinvestigationsofdirectandindi­rect aggression (Richardson & Green, 2003), was modified to include itemsmeasuringpassiveaggression(e.g.,“Didnotdowhattheotherpersonwantedmetodo,”“Gavethepersonthe ‘silent treatment,’”“Failedtoreturncallsorrespondtomessages”).Inthefirststudy,onconflictintheworkplace,partici­pantsreportedhowtheyrespondedwhenangrywithasupervisor,acoworker,orasubordinate.Thesecondstudyexaminedresponses“ingeneral.”Thethirdstudy inquired about responses when angry with romantic partners, siblings,coworkers,orfriends.

Ifpassiveaggressionisadistinctformofaggressionitshouldalsobedifferen­tiallycorrelatedwithothermeasures.Thesecondtwostudiesinthisseriesexam­inedtherelationshipofthethreeformsofaggressionwithothermeasuresofangerand aggression: Spielberger’s (1999) State­Trait Anger Expression Inventory–2(STAXI­2) andtheBuss–PerryAggressionQuestionnaire(AQ;1992).TheSTAXI measurestrait(i.e.,dispositional)angeraswellasangerexpression(anger­in,anger­out,angercontrol).Anger­ininvolvesthefrequencywithwhichanindividualexpe­riencesangerbutholdsitin(e.g.,“ItendtoharborgrudgesthatIdon’ttellanyoneabout”).Anger­outinvolvesthefrequencywithwhichapersonexperiencesangerandopenlyexpressesit(e.g.,“Ilosemytemper”).Angercontrolconsidersthefre­quencywithwhichanindividualexperiencesangerbutcontrolsit(e.g.,“Icontrolmytemper”).

Theresultsofthetwostudieswereconsistent.Theprimarydistinctionamongtheformsofaggressionwasthatpassiveandindirectaggressionweremorehighlycorrelatedwithanger­inthanwasdirectaggressionandthatdirectaggressionwasmorehighlycorrelatedwithtraitanger,anger­out,andangercontrol.CorrelationswithsubscalesoftheAQrevealedsimilarfindings.RCRQdirectaggressionwasmorehighlyassociatedwithangeranddirectverbalandphysicalaggressionsub­scalesoftheAQthanwerepassiveor indirectaggression.Replicatingresultsofpreviousresearch(Richardson&Green,2003),thesefindingssuggestthatpassiveandindirectaggressionarelikelytobeemployedbyindividualswhoexperience,butmayhavedifficultydirectlyexpressing,theiranger.Directaggression,ontheotherhand,maybeusedbyindividualswhoexpresstheirangermore(i.e.,anger­out), who are generally more angry (i.e., trait anger), and find controlling theirangertobeachallenge.

Basedonanecdotalevidence fromeverydayexperiencesofmembersofourresearchteam(whocouldgivemultipleexamplesofpassiveaggressivebehaviorfromtheirromanticpartners)aswellasliteratureondynamicsofdifferenttypesofrelationships,weexpectedthatpassiveaggressionwouldbeafrequentstrategyinromanticrelationships.Forexample,Gottman(1994)reportsthatonecommonresponseofindividualsinunhappyrelationshipsis”stonewalling,”whichinvolveswithdrawingfrominteractionandrefusingtoaddressthepartner’scomplaints,abehaviorthatourmodelwouldclearlyclassifyaspassiveaggression.

Page 74: the Psychology of Social Conflict and Aggression Sydney Symposium in Social Psychology

iS it aggreSSion? 57

Wewerecorrectinthisprediction.Participantsreportedusingmorepassivethanindirectordirectaggressionwhenangrywitharomanticpartner.However,wefoundthatrespondentsreportedmorepassiveaggressionwhenangrywithany-one (i.e., coworkers, friends, and siblings as well as romantic partners).So, is itaggression?

Motivators and Effects of Different Forms of Aggression

Thecommon correlatesofpassive and indirect aggression suggest that the twoformsofaggressionmaynotbeclearlydistinguishable—thattheybothbelongtoa general category of nondirect aggression.On the other hand,wehave distin­guishedthemconceptually(i.e.,circuitousharmversusharmbynotdoing),andthoseconceptualdefinitionssuggestthataggressorsmightbemotivatedbydiffer­entfactorswhentheyareusingpassiveaggressionthanwhentheyareusingindi­rectaggression.Forexample,passiveaggression,whichinvolvesharminganotherpersonbynot doingsomething,maybeintendedtoannoythetargetasmuchastoharmandmaynotbeperceivedbyeitheraggressorortargetasparticularlyaggres­sive.Thus,weconductedtwostudiestodeterminewhetherpeoplemightperceivetheaggressor’smotivesandtheeffectsonthevictimtovaryasafunctionoftypeofaggression.

Weaskedparticipants toevaluatea seriesofbehaviors fromtheperspectiveeitheroftheaggressororofthetarget.Directaggressivebehaviorsincluded“yellorscream,”“threatentohitorthrowsomething,”and“push,grab,shove.”Indirectaggressivebehaviorsincluded“makenegativecommentsaboutappearance,”“callnamesbehindback,”and“gatherotherpeopletomyside.”Passiveaggressivebehav­iorsincluded“givesilenttreatment,”“avoidinteracting,”and“failtoreturncalls.”

Participantsrespondingfromtheperspectiveoftheaggressorwereaskedwhytheywouldengageinthebehaviorandtoimaginehowthetargetwouldfeel.Forexample,“Whywouldyouyellorscreamatsomeone?”and“Imaginethatyouyellor screamat someone.Howdoyou think thatpersonwould feel?”Participantsresponding from the perspective of the target were asked why someone wouldbehavethatwaytowardthemandhowtheywouldfeelinresponse.Forexample,“Whywouldsomeoneyellorscreamatyou?”and”Howwouldyoufeelifsomeoneyelledorscreamedatyou?”Theaggressormotivationsandtargeteffectswereasfollows: toharm–feltharmed, togainpower–feltpowerless, to control–felt con­trolled, to cause distress–felt distressed, to humiliate–felt humiliated, to causeguilt–feltguilty.

Thefirststudyinthisseriesaskedforresponses“ingeneral”;thesecondstudyasked for responses in specific relationships. In both studies, responses for thespecificformsofeachtypeofaggression(e.g.,fordirectaggression,yelling,push­ing,threatening)werehighlyconsistent,sowecreatedsummaryscoresfordirect,indirect,andpassiveaggression.

BecausethedifferentmotivationsandeffectswerehighlycorrelatedinStudy1,wealsocreatedindicesofgeneralmotivation(i.e.,averageresponsetoallmoti­vations)andgeneraleffects(i.e.,averageresponseacrossalleffects).Thus,inStudy1wewere actually assessing theextent towhichaggressorswouldbegenerally

Page 75: the Psychology of Social Conflict and Aggression Sydney Symposium in Social Psychology

deBorah South richardSon and georgina S. hammock58

motivated to perform the aggressive behaviors and the extent to which targetswouldbegenerally affectedbytheaggressivebehaviors.

Participantsperceivedthattheperpetratorofpassiveaggressionwouldbegen­erallylessmotivatedthantheperpetratorofeitherdirectorindirectaggression.Participantsalsoperceivedthatpassiveaggressionwouldhavelesseffectonthetarget thaneitherdirector indirectaggression.Sinceallofthemotivationsandeffects involved a negative experience for the victim, we can infer that passiveaggressionwasperceivedtobeintendedtoproducealessnegativeexperienceforthevictim,and itwasperceived tobe lessmotivatedby thedesire fornegativeoutcomesforthevictim.Soisitaggression?

The second study in this series involved the samebasicprocedures as thefirst,butparticipantswereaskedtorespondwithreferencetoeitherasame­sexfriendoraromanticpartner.Thatis,fromtheperspectiveoftheaggressor,par­ticipantswereaskedtheextenttowhichtheywouldbemotivatedbythevariousfactors(e.g.,control,harm,humiliate)iftheyweretoaimthepassive,indirect,anddirectaggressiveactstowardasame­sexfriendortowardaromanticpartnerandtheextenttowhichthetargetwouldexperiencethedifferenteffects.Whenrespondingfromtheperspectiveofthetarget,participantswereaskedtheextenttowhichtheywouldexperiencethevariouseffectsandtheextenttowhichtheywouldperceivetheaggressortobemotivatedbythevariousfactors.

Participantsrespondingfromtheperspectiveofanaggressorperceivedharmtobeassociatedmorewithdirectandindirectaggressionthanwithpassiveaggres­sion.Controlwasassociatedwithdirectaggression;humiliationandlowlevelsof distress with indirect aggression; guilt with passive aggression. Passive aggres­sionwasperceivedasbeinglessmotivatedby thedesiretocauseharmtoortohumiliatethevictimandmoremotivatedbythedesiretoinduceguiltthandirector indirect aggression. Nevertheless, passive aggression was perceived as beingintended to cause asmuchdistress asdirect aggressionand more distress thanindirectaggression.

Theperceivedeffectsonvictimofeachtypeofaggressivebehaviorarenotablyconsistentwithperceptionsofaggressormotivation.Fromtheperspectiveofthetargetoftheaggressiveacts,powerwasassociatedwithdirectaggression;humili­ationanddistresswithindirectaggression;guiltwithpassiveaggression.Passiveaggressionwasperceivedtoproducelesshumiliation,distress,orharmthaneitheroftheotherformsofaggression.Again,harmwasassociatedprimarilywithdirectandindirectaggression.

Passiveaggressionwasthemostfrequentlyendorsedangerresponseinallthreestudies;ingeneral,participantsreportedmorefrequentuseofpassiveaggressionthaneitherdirectorindirectaggression.Thus,itappearsthataggressionresearchersmaybeignoringaformofaggressionthatpeoplereporttheyengageinquitefrequently.

Insum,ourexaminationofaggressorandvictimperspectivesonthemotiva­tionsandeffectsofdirect,indirect,andpassiveaggressionprovidesafurtherunder­standing of the distinctiveness (or lack thereof) ofpassive aggression.Everydayindirect aggression,which is seenascausingharm,humiliation,anddistress, isconsistentwithourdefinitionofaggression.Everydaypassiveaggressionisseenascausingdistressandguiltbutascausingrelativelylittleharm;soisitaggression?

Page 76: the Psychology of Social Conflict and Aggression Sydney Symposium in Social Psychology

iS it aggreSSion? 59

Conclusion: Is Passive Aggression Aggressive?

Ourseriesofstudiesonpassiveaggressionrevealthatitisan“attractive”responsewhenangrywithsomeone;itisthebehaviorthatrespondentsindicatedtheyper­formedmostfrequentlyingeneralandacrossavarietyofrelationshipswhentheywereangry.Thecorrelationsofself­reportsofaggressionwithmeasuresofangersuggestthatnondirectformsofaggression,includingindirectandpassiveaggres­sion,areendorsedespeciallybyindividualswhoreportdifficultyexpressingtheiranger.Thenondirectnatureoftheseformsofaggressionallowstheindividualtorespondwhenangrybuttoavoiddirectconfrontationwiththevictim.

Accordingtocurrentdefinitionsofaggression,abehaviormustbeintendedtocauseharmtobeconsideredasaggressive.Ourresearchsuggeststhatthenatureof that harm may vary with type of aggression. For example, although passiveaggressionwasperceivedtobelessharmfulthanindirectordirectaggression,itwasperceivedasamechanismforinducingguiltandforcausingdistresstothevictim.Aninterestingcorollaryofthisaspectofpassiveaggressionisthattheulti­mateeffectofthebehavioronthevictimmaydependtoaconsiderableextentonthevictim’schosenresponsetotheaggression.Forexample,ifIintendtoinduceguilt by passive aggressively noting how much hard work I have done for you,butyourefusetoexperienceguilt,thenmypassiveaggressivestrategywillhavefailed.Whenoneadministersdirectverbalorphysicalblowstoavictim—orevenindirectverbalorphysicalblows—itisdifficultforthevictimtoavoidordenytheharm;thevictimhaslittlecontrolovertheadministrationoftheharm.However,in the case of passive aggression, the victim can, in effect, choose whether toacknowledgeorexperiencetheharm.Thismightbeaninterestingquestionforfutureresearchthatwouldfocusondifferentialvictimresponsetopassiveaggres­siveattempts.

Insum,theseexaminationsofpassiveaggressionsuggestthatsuchbehaviorisdeservingoftheattentionofaggressionresearchers.Itisabehaviorthatcancauseharm;itisapreferredresponsetoanger;thenatureanddegreeofharmissubjecttointerpretationbythevictim.

pSychological aggreSSionAggressionresearchershavepaidmoreattentiontoeverydayharmthatisreferredtoaspsychologicalaggression,especiallyinthecontextofresearchonintimatepartnerviolence.Althoughtheresearchliteraturerevealsnoconsistentdefinitionofpsychologicalaggression,thedefinitionstypicallyrefertoemotionalharmortheuseoftacticssuchasdegradation,ridicule,andsocialandfinancialisolation.Weconceptualizepsychologicalaggressionasharmfulbehaviorsthatdamagetheself­conceptoftheindividual.The“everyday”natureofthistypeofaggressioncanbeseeninitsprevalenceinintimaterelationships:CapaldiandCrosby(1997)reportedthat80%ofintimatepartnersengagedinatleastoneactofpsychologi­calaggressionwhilebeingobserveddiscussingaproblemwithapartner;O’LearyandWilliams(2006)reportedprevalenceratesofapproximately90%inacom­munitysample.

Page 77: the Psychology of Social Conflict and Aggression Sydney Symposium in Social Psychology

deBorah South richardSon and georgina S. hammock60

Psychologicalaggressionisperceivedbytargetsasmoreharmfulthanphysi­cal aggression (Follingstad, Rutledge,Berg, Hause, &Polek, 1990; Katz, Arias,& Beach, 2000). Studies of the correlates of psychological and physical aggres­sion reveal common predictors (e.g., Hammock, 2003; Hammock & O’Hearn,2002),andthetwoformsofaggressioncommonlyco­occur(e.g.,Follingstadetal.;Hamby&Sugarman,1999;Hammock&O’Hearn;Murphy&O’Leary,1989).Someresearchers(e.g.,Murphy&Cascardi,1999;Stets,1991)havesuggestedthattheprimarymotivationforbothformsofaggressioninthecontextofintimatepart­nerrelationshipsmaybecontrolofthepartnerandtherelationship.Asisthecasewithpassiveaggression,these“other”motivationsdonotdirectlysuggestintenttoharm,butharmmayneverthelessbetheeffectonthevictim.

Weconductedtwostudiestodeterminehowthirdpartiesperceivethemotiva­tionorintentionsbehindpsychologicallyandphysicallyaggressiveactsinacon­flictbetweenahusbandandwife.Thefirststudyexaminedtheeffectsoftypeofaggression(physicalvs.psychological)aswellasgenderofaggressor.Thesecondstudyalsoconsideredtheeffectoftheperceiver’sownexperienceasvictimorper­petratorofaggressioninintimaterelationships.

Participants were asked to imagine that they were observing their marriedneighborshavingafight.Thescenariodepictedacoupleengagedinaheateddis­cussionaboutmoney.Someparticipantsreadascenarioabouttheperpetratorusingphysicallyaggressiveactssuchasthrowingbooksatthevictim,slappingthevictim,andslammingthevictimagainstthewall;othersreadabouttheperpetratorusingpsychologicallyaggressiveactssuchasbelittlingthevictimandthevictim’sfamily,ridiculingthevictim,threateningtofinanciallyisolatethevictim,andinsultingthevictim.Wealsomanipulatedgendercompositionoftheperpetrator–victimdyad,withsomeparticipantsreadingabouthusband­to­wifeaggressionandothersaboutwife­to­husbandaggression.

Participantsrespondedtoquestionsabout their judgmentoftheactionsandactorsinthescenario,suchaswhetherthebehaviordisplayedwouldbeconsid­eredabusive,whethertheperpetratorshouldbepunishedforhisorheractions,andwhetherthevictimsufferedanyharm.Respondentsalsoreportedtheextenttowhichtheaggressorwasmotivatedtomakethevictimfeelbad,tocontroltherelationship,toinjurethevictim,tohurtthevictim,togainpowerintherelation­ship,andtocontrolthevictim.

Physical aggression was generally perceived to have more negative outcomesthan psychological aggression. Participants reported that perpetrators of physicalaggressionshouldbepunishedmoreandhadengagedinmoreabusiveactionsthanperpetratorsofpsychologicalaggression.Theyalsoconsideredthevictimofphysicalaggressiontohavesufferedmoreharmthanthevictimofpsychologicalaggression.Nevertheless,thehighratingsforbothformsofaggression(wellabovemidpointofscale)suggestthatpsychologicallyaggressiveactionswereconsideredtobeharmful,abusive,anddeservingofpunishment—justnotasmuchsoasphysicalaggression.

Psychologicallyaggressiveperpetratorswereperceivedtobemoremotivatedto control the relationship or partner, togainpower in the relationship, and tomakethevictimfeelbadthanwerephysicallyaggressiveperpetrators.However,psychologicalandphysicalaggressionwerenotratedasdifferentiallymotivatedto

Page 78: the Psychology of Social Conflict and Aggression Sydney Symposium in Social Psychology

iS it aggreSSion? 61

causeinjuryorhurtthevictim.Thus,itappearsthatthirdpartiesrecognizethattheintentoftheperpetratorofpsychologicalaggressionmaybenegativeinavari­etyofways,especiallywithregardtocontrolandpower.

In terms of an overall comparison of psychological and physical aggression,participantsconsideredphysicalaggressiontoleadtomorenegativeoutcomes(e.g.,abuse,harm),buttheydidnotseeitasbeingmoremotivatedtohurtorinjurethevictim.And,assuggestedearlier,psychologicalaggressionwasconsideredtobemoremotivatedtocontrolorgainpowerintherelationships.Intermsofdefiningaggression, perceivers certainly recognize that harm derives from attempts tocausepsychologicaldamagetoapartner.

As mentioned earlier, those who have experienced psychological and physi­calaggressionreportthatpsychologicalaggressionismoreharmfulanddamagingthanphysicalaggression(Follingstadetal.,1990;Katzetal.,2000).Therefore,wereasonedthatindividualswhohavebeenvictimsofpsychologicalaggressionmaybemore likely torecognize thepainandharmofsuchactionswhenservingasthird­partyperceivers.InStudy2wehypothesizedthatthosewhohadbeenvic­timsofphysicalorpsychologicalaggressionwouldperceivethoseactionsasmoredamagingthanthosewhohadnotexperiencedsuchvictimization.WemeasuredexperiencewithpsychologicalandphysicalaggressionwiththeAbusiveBehaviorInventory(Shepard&Campbell,1992).

Experiencewithphysicalandpsychologicalaggressiondidnotrelatetopartici­pants’judgmentsoftheoutcomesofpsychologicalandphysicalaggression.Again,participants believed the physically aggressive actions of the perpetrator weremoredeservingofpunishmentandmoreabusivethanpsychologicallyaggressiveactions.Theyalsoperceivedthephysicallyaggressiveactsasmorewrongthanthepsychologicallyaggressiveacts.Nevertheless,participantsconsistentlyjudgedthepsychologically aggressive actions quite negatively—though not as negatively asthoseassociatedwithphysicalaggression.

Participantexperienceasvictimsofphysicalaggressiondidnotrelatetopercep­tionsofaggressormotivations.However,respondentswhohadmoreexperienceasvictimsofpsychologicalaggressionperceivedthepsychologicallyaggressiveper­petratorasbeingmoremotivatedtomakethevictimfeelbadabouthimselforher­selfandmoremotivatedtocontrolthevictimthanthosewhohadlessexperienceasvictimsofpsychologicalaggression.Experiencewithpsychologicalaggressionwasnotrelatedtotheperceptionofphysicalaggression.

Thesestudiestakentogethersuggestthatpeopleperceivepsychologicalaggressionasharmfultothetargetofsuchactions—althoughthereisconsiderablevariabilityinthenatureoftheharmtheyperceivetobeinflicted.Ratherthanharmingthephysi­calperson,theharmmaybeintheformofpsychologicaldistress(makingthevictimfeelbadabouthimselforherself)orintermsofmanipulationofthevictim(controllingthe relationshipor victim,gainingpower in the relationship).These typesofharmmaynotcomeimmediatelytomindwhenconsideringtheimpactofaggressiveacts.Nevertheless,thedistressandpowerlessnessexperiencedbythevictimofpsychologi­calaggressionarelikelytohaveastrongnegativeimpactontherecipientandontherelationshipbetweenthevictimandtheaggressor,andthesebruisestothepsychearemorefrequentoccurrencesthanthemoresalient,morevisiblebruisestothebody.

Page 79: the Psychology of Social Conflict and Aggression Sydney Symposium in Social Psychology

deBorah South richardSon and georgina S. hammock62

concluSionTheprogramsofresearchreviewedinthischapterbringtotheforefrontformsofharm­doingbehaviorthatarefrequentlyignoredbyaggressionresearchers.Thisresearchsuggestsamoreinclusivedefinitionofaggressionthatconsidersaggres­siontohaveoccurredifatargethasbeenharmed,thusmovingthefocusfromtheintentoftheaggressortotheeffectonthevictimandrecognizingthatavarietyofaggressormotivationsmayleadtoavarietyofharmfuloutcomesforthevictim.Suchavictim­centereddefinitionwouldmoveustowardnewacceptanceofthesimpledefinitionofaggressionasabehaviorthatcausesharm.

Orwemaywanttoconsiderarevolutionarysocialpsychologicaldefinitionofaggressionthatwouldincorporatetheintentionoftheaggressortoproducenega­tiveoutcomesforatargetwhoexperiencesharm.Suchanapproach,whichwoulddefineaggressionasaninteraction,wouldbetrulysocialpsychological.

referenceS

Archer,J.,&Coyne,S.M.(2005).Anintegratedreviewofindirect,relational,andsocialaggression.Personality and Social Psychology Review, 9,212–230.

Baron, R. A., & Neuman, J. H. (1996). Workplace violence and workplace aggression:Evidenceontheirrelativefrequencyandpotentialcauses.Aggressive Behavior, 22, 161–173.

Baron,R.A.,&Richardson,D.R.(1994).Human aggression.NewYork:Plenum.Basow,S.A.,Cahill,K.F.,Phelan,J.E.,Longshore,K.,&McGillicuddy­DeLisi,A.(2007).

Perceptionsofrelationalandphysicalaggressionamongcollegestudents:Effectsofgender of perpetrator, target, and perceiver. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 31,85–95.

Berkowitz,M.W.,Mueller,C.W.,Schnell,S.V.,&Padberg,M.T.(1986).Moralreason­ingand judgmentsofaggression.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 51,885–891.

Björkqvist,K.,Osterman,K.,&Lagerspetz,K.(1994).Sexdifferencesincovertaggressionamongadults.Aggressive Behavior, 20,27–33.

Buss,A.H.(1961).The psychology of aggression.NewYork:JohnWileyandSons,Inc.Buss, A., & Perry, M. (1992). The aggression questionnaire. Journal of Personality and

Social Psychology, 63,452–459.Capaldi,D.M.,&Crosby,L. (1997).Observedandreportedpsychological andphysical

aggressioninyoung,at­riskcouples.Social Development,6(2),184–206.Crick,N.,&Grotpeter, J. (1995). Relational aggression,gender and social­psychological

adjustment.Child Development, 66,710–722.Dukes,R.L.,Clayton,S.L.,Jenkins,L.T.,Miller,T.L.,&Rodgers,S.E.(2001).Effects

ofaggressivedrivinganddrivercharacteristicsonroadrage.Social Science Journal, 38,323–331.

Follingstad,D.R.,Rutledge,L.L.,Berg,B.J.,Hause,E.S.,&Polek,D.S.(1990).Theroleofemotionalabuseinphysicallyabusiverelationships.Journal of Family Violence, 5,107–120.

Gottman,J.M.(1994).Why marriages succeed or fail.NewYork:Simon&Schuster.Hamby,S.L.,&Sugarman,D.B.(1999).Actsofpsychologicalaggressionagainstapartner

andtheirrelationtophysicalassaultandgender.Journal of Marriage and the Family, 61,959–970.

Page 80: the Psychology of Social Conflict and Aggression Sydney Symposium in Social Psychology

iS it aggreSSion? 63

Hammock, G. S. (2003). Physical and psychological aggression in dating relationships:Similarordifferentprocesses?International Review of Social Psychology,16,31–51.

Hammock,G.S.,&O’Hearn,R.(2002).Psychologicalaggressionindatingrelationships:Predictivemodelsformalesandfemales.Violence and Victims, 17,525–540.

Katz,J.,Arias,I.,&Beach,S.R.H.(2000).Psychologicalabuse,self­esteem,andwomen’sdatingrelationshipoutcomes.Psychology of Women Quarterly, 24, 349–357.

Kingery,P.M. (1998).TheAdolescentViolenceSurvey:Apsychometricanalysis.School Psychology International, 19,43–59.

Murphy,C.M.&Cascardi,M.(1999).Psychologicalabuseinmarriageanddatingrelation­ships.InR.L.Hampton(Ed.),Family violence: Prevention and treatment (2nded.)(pp.198–220),ThousandOaks,CA:Sage.

Murphy,C.M.,&O’Leary,K.D.(1989).Psychologicalaggressionpredictsphysicalaggres­sioninearlymarriage.Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 57,579–582.

O’Leary,K.D.,&Williams,M.C.(2006).Agreementaboutactsofaggressioninmarriage.Journal of Family Psychology, 20,656–662.

Richardson, D. R., & Green, L. R. (1997). Circuitous harm: Determinants and conse­quences of nondirect aggression. In Kowalski, R. M. (Ed.), Aversive interpersonal behaviors.(pp.171–188).NewYork:PlenumPress.

Richardson, D. S., & Green, L. R. (2003). Defining direct and indirect aggression: TheRichardson Conflict Response Questionnaire. International Review of Social Psychology, 16,11–30.

Richardson,D.S.,&Green,L.R.(2006).Directandindirectaggression:Relationshipsassocialcontext.Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 36,2492–2508.

Richardson, D. S., Warren, P., Ferguson, H., & Daniel, S. (2007, January). Is passive aggression a unique strategy? Paper presented at International CICA AggressionSymposium,Augusta,GA.

Shepard,M.F.,&Campbell,J.A.(1992).TheAbusiveBehaviorInventory:Ameasureofpsychologicalandphysicalabuse.Journal of Interpersonal Violence, 7,291–305.

Sommer, K. L., Williams, K. D., Ciarocco, N. J., & Baumeister, R. F. (2001). Whensilencespeakslouderthanwords:Explorationsintotheintrapsychicandinterper­sonalconsequencesofsocialostracism.Basic and Applied Social Psychology, 23, 225–243.

Spielberger,C.D.(1999).Manual for the State-Trait Anger Expression Inventory-2.Odessa,FL:PsychologicalAssessmentResources.

Stets, J.E. (1991).Psychologicalaggression indatingrelationships:Theroleof interper­sonalcontrol.Journal of Family Violence, 6,97–114.

Tedeschi, J. T., & Felson, R. B. (1994). Violence, aggression, and coercive actions.Washington,DC:AmericanPsychologicalAssociation.

Williams, K. D. (1997). Social ostracism. In R. Kowalski (Ed.), Aversive interpersonal behaviors(pp.133–170).NewYork:Plenum.

Williams,K.D.,Shore,W.J.,&Grahe,J.E.(1998).Thesilenttreatment:Perceptionsofitsbehaviorsandassociatedfeelings.Group Processes and Intergroup Relations, 1, 117–141.

Page 81: the Psychology of Social Conflict and Aggression Sydney Symposium in Social Psychology

deBorah South richardSon and georgina S. hammock64

appendix a. paSSive aggreSSion itemS for reviSed rcrQ

1.Didnotdowhatthepersonwantedmetodo. 2.Mademistakesthatappearedtobeaccidental. 3.Seemeduninterestedinthingsthatwereimportanttotheperson. 4.Gavethepersonthe“silenttreatment.” 5.Ignoredtheperson’scontributions. 6.Excludedthepersonfromimportantactivities. 7.Avoidedinteractingwiththeperson. 8.Failedtodenyfalserumorsabouttheperson. 9.Failedtoreturncallsorrespondtomessages. 10.Showeduplateforplannedactivities. 11.Sloweddownontasks.

Page 82: the Psychology of Social Conflict and Aggression Sydney Symposium in Social Psychology

65

5PushingUptoaPoint

The Psychology of Interpersonal Assertiveness

DANIELAMESColumbia University

O nmostdays,itdoesnottakeagreatdealofsocial interactionforustorememberthatotherpeoples’goalsandinterestsarenotperfectlyalignedwithourown.Wewanttosleepinlate,andourspouseorchildwantsto

getupearly.Wewantacleansidewalk,butourneighborforgetstopickupafterhisdog.Wewantourworkcolleaguestomeetthedeadlinestheyhavegivenus,butapparentlytheyhaveotherplans.Wishingitwereotherwise—thateveryonewouldwantexactlythesamethingswedo—isfolly.Besides,itwouldnotmakeforaveryinterestingworld;varietyisthespiceoflife.Sodealingwiththis“spice”isasignificantpartofthehumancondition.Howdowecopewiththeever­presentfactthatotherssurrounduswhoseinterestsandgoalsdivergefrom,andsometimesoppose,ourown?Dowepresshardforourgoalstobesatisfied—and,ifso,why?Doweyieldtoothers’claims—and,ifso,when?

Inthischapter,Iwanttoarguethatthesequestionsofhowhardwepushpervadeandtosomeextentdefineourlives.Accordingly,thematterofwhenandwhypeoplepushhardorrelentininterpersonalconflictslargeandsmalldeservesconsiderableattentionandcare.Indeed,fordecadesithasbeenatopicofacademicscrutinyintheliteraturesoninterpersonalconflict,negotiation,andsocialdilemmas.There,awell­establishedthemeintheaccountofwhopusheshardandwhyisthatmotivationsplayacentralrole.Somepeoplecaremoreaboutwinning;othersjustwanttogetalong.Thisseemsirrefutable.Onegoalofthepresentchapteristodescribepastandrecentworkthattakesacomplimentaryapproachtomotivation­focusedaccounts,highlightingtheroleofexpectanciesininterpersonalassertiveness.Pushinghardisnotsolelyafunctionofwhatpeoplewantbutalsoofwhattheybelievewillhappen

Page 83: the Psychology of Social Conflict and Aggression Sydney Symposium in Social Psychology

daniel ameS66

whentheymakeforcefuldemandsorcapitulatetoothers’requests.Icontendthatacompleteaccountofinterpersonalassertivenessneedsbothofthesepieces—expec­tanciesandmotivations.Andbecauseourliveshavesomuchspiceinthem,withthequestionof“howhardshouldIpush”shapingourbehaviorfromsunrisetosleep,weneedacompleteaccountofinterpersonalassertiveness.

aSSertiveneSS definedIbeginbyclarifyingwhat Imeanby the termassertiveness,whichcomesnotsomuchfromanapriori scholarlymodelbut frommy interpretationofevery­day perceptions of interpersonal behavior. This could be seen as a folk modelof interpersonal assertiveness: a continuousdimensioncharacterizing howper­sonsbehaveorrespondinasituationinwhichtheirpositionsorinterestsare,orcouldbe,inconflictwithothers’positionsorinterests.Inotherwords,whengoalsdiverge,howharddopeoplepushfortheirowninterests?Inconsideringthesetofanactor’spossibleresponsesinanygivensocialconflict,Ibelievebothactorsandobserverscanandoftendoarraybehaviorsalongaroughdimensionofassertive­nessrangingfrompassivityandcapitulationatoneextremetoaggressionandhos­tilityattheother.LaterinthischapterIdiscusshowthisunidimensionalmodeloffolkperceptionsfitswithpasttheoreticaldistinctions(e.g.,betweenassertionandaggression).

Someconcreteexampleshelptoillustrateassertivenessasitisapproachedinthis chapter. Imagine that members of a newly formedacademic research cen­termeettodiscussaseniorhire.Onememberadvocatesaparticularchoice,butanother believes this would be a disastrous move. Does the skeptical memberunequivocallydisparagetheproposedchoiceandchampionherownideas?Doesshemakeamoremeasuredobservationaboutexpandingthesetofoptions?Ordoessheholdbackentirely,hopingsomeoneelsewillbreakthesilence?

Imagine directors of two nonprofit organizations who share a building areplanningformuch­neededrenovations.Onedirectorbeginsbytellingtheotherheexpectshisorganization’sspacetobeentirelyrefurbishedeventhoughheintendstopayonlyasmallshareofthecost.Doestheotherdirectorforcefullyrejecttheproposalanddemandgreatercost­sharing?Doessheprobeforflexibility,andpro­poserevisionstotheplan?Ordoessheaccepttheofferasgiven?

Last,consideramanagerconcernedwithhersubordinate’stimemanagementskills.Doessheconfronthimdirectly,stressingnegativerepercussionsifhefailstoimprove?Doessheraisequestionsandoffersuggestionsforchange?Ordoessheavoidtheissuealtogether,hopingitwillcorrectitselfintime?

Thesecaseshighlightthekindsofdailychoicesindividualsmakeintheirinter­personal assertiveness toward others. These situations and behaviors may seemdisparate,butIbelievethattheyhavecommonunderlyingpsychologicalprocessesthatshapeactors’choicesaboutbehaviorandobservers’interpretationsofacts.Idefineassertiveness as adimension ineverydayperceptions reflecting individu­als’ interpersonal willingness to stand up and speak out for their own interestsandideas,pursuingtheirobjectivesandresistingothers’impositions.AsshowninFigure 5.1,oneendofthisfolkspectrumentailspassivityandyielding,whereas

Page 84: the Psychology of Social Conflict and Aggression Sydney Symposium in Social Psychology

puShing up to a point 67

theotherendfeaturesaggressionandhostility.Inbetweenaregradationsrangingfromengagementandinitiationtocollaborationandresistance.

Thisunidimensionalapproachmayseemtoconfounddimensionsthatdeservetobeseparated(e.g.,howmuchoneasksforvs.howoneasksforit)andtomakeneighbors out of constructs that are qualitatively foreign to one another (e.g.,aggressionandassertion).AfirstpointtonoteisthatIusethetermassertivenessheretodescribethewidespectrumthatgradespossibleresponsesinsocialconflict(i.e.,someactsareseenasmoreorlessassertivethanothers)ratherthanapar­ticularpointorsubrangeofresponsesonthespectrum(i.e.,“assertivebehaviors”asthosethatfallbetweenpassivityandaggression).Asecondpointtostressisthatthisspectrumreflectseveryday perceptionsofpossibleresponsesinasocialcon­flict.Whenpeoplethinkabouthowhardtheymightpushinasocialconflict,Isug­gesttheyoftenconsidergradationsofresponsesrangingfrom“givingin”to“askingforwhatIwant”to“demandingthatIgetmyway.”Scholarshaveunderstandablytakenpainstodistinguishbetweenconstructssuchasassertiveness,oftendefinedasexpressingone’sowninterests,andaggression,usuallyseenasinvolvingcoer­cionoranintenttoharm(e.g.,DeGiovanni&Epstein,1978;seealsoChapter4inthisvolume).Myargumentdoesnotdenytheimportanceofsuchscholarlydistinc­tionsbutratherreflectsthefactthattheseboundariesmaybeblurredorgradualinfolkjudgments(whichisexactlywhyscholarshaveworkedsohardtobeprecise

Conflict Avoidant,trivializing

Supplicant,appeasing

Weak opening,ready concessions

Silent with opinions,conformist

Equivocal,indecisive

Candid,constructive

Active, forthright,persuasive

Strong opening,integrative solutions

Egalitarian, open,engaged

Proactive,inclusive

Belligerent,demanding

Bullying,cajoling

Extreme opening,aggressive tactics

Confrontational,dominance-seeking

Unilateral,self-serving

AvoidanceWithdrawalPassivity

InitiationEngagement

Accommodation

CollaborationResistanceAssertion

CompetitionAggressionHostility

Domain Illustrative behavior or style

Assertiveness

Negotiation

Teamwork

Influence

Decisionmaking

figure 5.1 Theeverydayperceptionofassertiveness.

Page 85: the Psychology of Social Conflict and Aggression Sydney Symposium in Social Psychology

daniel ameS68

intheirowndiscussions).Thepresentmodelsimplysuggeststhatpeoplemaycon­sider the implications of different, perhaps qualitatively disparate, responses insocialconflict,decidingthatsomegotoofarwhereasothersdon’tgofarenough.Whetherthisapproachhasmeritshouldbejudged,Ithink,byhowwellitfaresinpredictingbehaviorinsocialconflict.

aSSertiveneSS and outcomeSIeventuallywanttopresentanaccountofchoicesofassertivebehaviors(howdopeopledecidehowhardtopush?),butIfirstturntosomeevidenceofhowasser­tiveness relates to actual interpersonal outcomes (what happens when someonepushes hard?). This step lays important groundwork for the nature and role ofassertivenessexpectanciesbecauseitseemsentirelylikelythatpeoples’folktheo­riesofassertivenesswillatleastpartlyreflecthowactualassertivenessplaysout.Putanotherway,peopledecidehowhardtopush inpartbecause theypredict,flawlesslyornot,whathappenswhentheypushhardorrelentinaparticularsitu­ation.Sowhathappenswhenpeoplepushhardorgivein?

MyanswertothisquestioncomesfromresearchI’vedonewithFrankFlynninthedomainoforganizationalleadership(Ames&Flynn,2007;seeAmes,2008a;Ames,2009forreviews).Webeganbyreviewingthousandsofopen­endedanony­mous comments professionals, including working managers and MBA students,gathered from coworkers on their behavioral strengths (e.g., what makes themeffective)andweaknesses(e.g.,whatbehaviorscouldbedevelopedorimproved).Assertivenesswasnotmuchofafactorincommentsaboutstrengths,whichtendedtorevolvearoundintelligenceandconscientiousness.However,referencestoasser­tivenessdominatedweaknesscomments.Importantly,theydidsoinbothdirec­tions,withsomecommentsreferringtotoomuchassertivenessandothersreferringtotoolittle.Whatmanyprofessionalsandleadersstrugglewith,atleastintheeyesofonlookers,isstrikingtherightbalancewithassertiveness,pushinghardenoughtogetthingsdonebutnotsohardthattheyfailtogetalong.

Thisstandsincontrasttoalongtraditionofworkonindividualdifferencesaslinearpredictorsofleadershipeffectiveness—thoughthereareimportantexcep­tions, such as Fleishman (1995) and Simonton (1985). Past work has tended tohypothesizeaboutandtestforqualitiesthatarepositivelyandlinearlyassociatedwith leadership—that is, more of a given attribute (e.g., intelligence, ambition,extraversion)meansmoreeffectiveleadership.However,ourworkonqualitativecommentsfromcoworkerssuggestedacurvilinear,inverted­U­shapedrelationshipbetweenassertivenessandleadershipeffectiveness.Indeed,severalfollow­upstud­ieswithmanagersusingcontinuousratingmeasureshaveshownthatbothcom­parativelylowandcomparativelyhighassertiveleaderswereratedaslesseffectivebycoworkersthanthoseinthemiddlerange(Ames&Flynn,2007).

To unpack why this happens, we decomposed outcomes into two domains:instrumentalandrelationaloutcomes.Inbrief,wefoundthateachdomainseemedtoaccountfortheeffectatoneendofthespectrum.Instrumentaloutcomes(get­ting one’s way, getting things done) seem to improve noticeably as actors movefromlowtomoderateassertiveness,withfewergainsbeyondthatpoint.Relational

Page 86: the Psychology of Social Conflict and Aggression Sydney Symposium in Social Psychology

puShing up to a point 69

outcomes(gettingalongwithothers)seemtoimproveconsiderablyasactorsmovefromhightomoderateassertiveness,withfewgainsbeyondthatpoint.Thus,highassertiveleaderstendedtobeineffectivelargelybecausetheyfailedtogetalong,whereaslowassertiveleaderstendedtobeineffectivelargelybecausetheyfailedtogettheirwayorgetthingsdone.

Ibelievethelessonsfromthisworkonorganizationalleadershipholdmoregen­erally,characterizingtheconsequencesofinterpersonalassertivenessasacurvilin­eareffectwithinstrumentalandrelationalcomponents.Whathappenswhenpeoplepush very hard? They may undermine their relationships without gaining muchinstrumentally.Whathappenswhenpeoplegivein?Theymayloseinstrumentallywithoutgainingmuchrelationally.Whilesituationaldifferencessurelydictatedif­ferentappropriate levelsofassertiveness inagivensituation(seeAmes,2009), itseemsthattheremaybesomemiddlerangeofassertivenessthattendstooptimizeoutcomes.Thisideaisthestartingpointforanexpectancy­basedaccountofasser­tivebehavioralchoices:whatdoesanindividualactorbelieveistheoptimallevelofassertiveness?Doindividualsvaryinwheretheybelievethisoptimalpointlies—anddoessuchvariancepredicttheirbehavioralchoices?Thenotionthatexpectan­cies suchas thesegovernbehavior iscertainlynotnewandsobeforezeroing inspecificallyonassertivenessexpectancies,itisworthrecognizingthiscontext.

expectancieSFrom its earliest days, psychology has portrayed people as having expectationsaboutothersaroundthemandsuggestedthatthesebeliefshaveafunctioninregu­latingbehavior(seeRoese&Sherman,2007forareview).Muchofthescholarshipon interpersonal expectancies has focusedonexpectations about other people’scharacteristicsandbehavior,asinworkonstereotypingandself­fulfillingprophe­cies(e.g.,Miller&Turnbull,1986).Another importantandrelevant traditionofwork,addressedinChapter2inthisvolume,examinesthenatureanddevelop­mentofpeople’sinternalworkingmodelsofothersandtheirinterpersonalattach­mentstyles(seealsoCampbell,Simpson,Boldry,&Kashy,2005;Pietromonaco&Barrett,2000).Numerousresearchershavehighlightedtheroleofcompetitiveexpectationsaboutothers,linkingconflictbehaviorstoapredictionthatone’scon­flictpartnermay beaggressive, hostile, or untrustworthy (e.g., Crick& Dodge,1994; Diekmann,Tenbrunsel, & Galinsky, 2003; Kelley & Stahelski, 1970; VanLange,1992).

Suchbasicexpectationsaboutothers—whetherintheformofastereotype,anattachmentstyle,orsomeotherkindofrepresentation—arecertainlyimportantin shapingbehavior.However, theassertivenessexpectancyaccountpresentedheredepartsfromthistraditionbyemphasizingexpectedreactionsbyanothertoone’sownassertivebehavior.Rather thanbasicornoncontingentexpectan­cies,thisaccountdealswithcontingentones:IfIdoX,thisotherpersonwilldo,think,orfeelY.Buildingonsocialcognitivemodelsofbehavior(e.g.,Bandura,1986;Mischel&Shoda,1995),severalnoteworthytraditionsofworkhaveexam­ined suchcontingentexpectancies.Onebodyof researchdealswith relationalschemasorscriptsandtheirimpactonrelationshipbehaviorandself­construal

Page 87: the Psychology of Social Conflict and Aggression Sydney Symposium in Social Psychology

daniel ameS70

(seeBaldwin&Dandeneau,2005forareview).Researchinthisveinhasshown,forinstance,thattheamountofangerdisplayedinacloserelationshipdependsonanticipatedpartnerresponse(Fehr,Baldwin,Collins,Patterson,&Benditt,1999).Arelatedareaofinquiryhasexaminedrejectionsensitivity,whichrevolvesaround“anxiousexpectations”ofinterpersonalrejectionandtheassociatedacti­vationofdefensiveresponsesthatcanhavenegativeorevenself­fulfillingeffects(e.g., Downey & Feldman, 1996). Elsewhere, researchers examining genderdynamicsinnegotiationhavelinkedwomen’sassertivebehaviorsto“anticipatedbacklash,”namely,women’s expectationsofhow theirbehavior willbe viewedandderogatedbyothers(Amanatullah&Morris,2010;Bowles,Babcock,&Lai,2007).

Inalloftheseprogramsofwork,peopleareportrayedashavingdifferentinter­nalmodelsofhowotherswill reactto themor theirbehavior.Person­to­personvarianceinthesemodelshasbeenlinkedtoavarietyofinterpersonalbehaviorsand outcomes, such as psychological adjustment. Together, this body of worksuggeststhatthereissubstantialpromiseinexploringhowgeneralassertivenessexpectanciesmightshapebehavior—howhardpeoplepush—acrossavarietyofcontexts.Thosewhopessimisticallyexpectthathighlevelsofassertivenesswillbecostlywilltendtoshowlowerlevelsofassertivenessthanthosewhooptimisticallybelievethathighlevelsofassertivenessbringbenefits.However,toharnessasser­tivenessexpectanciesinourconceptualmodelsandtousetheminourresearch,wefirstneedtoestablishtheformtheseexpectanciestypicallytakeandhowtheycanbestbemeasured,amattertowhichIturnnext.

the nature of aSSertiveneSS expectancieSBasedonthepriorworkshowingthatinterpersonalassertivenessoftenhasacurvi­linear,inverted­U­shapedeffectoninterpersonalrelations(Ames&Flynn,2007),Iexpectthatmanypeoplewillhavecurvilinearexpectancies,assumingthattheycanpushuptoapointbutnofurtherwithoutincurringdamagetotheiroutcomesor relationships. For instance, in a negotiation, people may feel that making amoderatelyassertiveopeningofferingcouldbeeffectivebutthatatsomepointofheightenedassertivenessanopeningcouldbackfire,underminingbothresultsandrelationships.Whilepeopleingeneralmayshowthisformofexpectancy,individu­alswillvaryinwhatpointtheythinktheycanpushupto.Somemaybeveryopti­mistic,assumingtheycandisplayveryhigh levelsof interpersonalassertivenessbeforeincurringcosts.Othersmaybemuchmorepessimistic,assumingthatevenmodestlevelsofassertivenesscouldspelltrouble.Ifthischaracterizationiscorrect,itwouldinvitearesearchapproachthatattemptsto identifyanindividual’sper­ceived“optimal”levelofassertivenessorsomekindofproxyforthisexpectancy.

Itestedthisideabyaskingresearchparticipantstoliterallydrawtheirexpec­tancies(Ames,2008,Study1).Participantsreceivedablankchart,withanx­axisindicatingdegreesofassertivenessanday­axisindicatingeithersocialorinstru­mentaloutcomes;theywerethenaskedtodrawalinerepresentingtheoutcomestheywouldgenerallyexpectforeachlevelofassertiveness.Pilotworkshowedthatpeoplefoundthistasktobeanintuitivewayofexpressingtheirexpectationsthat

Page 88: the Psychology of Social Conflict and Aggression Sydney Symposium in Social Psychology

puShing up to a point 71

weresometimeshardtoputintowords.Asexpected,themajorityofparticipants(some60to70%)drewlinesthathadaclearinverted­Ushape,withamidpointanddownturnedends,forbothsocialandinstrumentaloutcomes.Responsesfromundergraduate students and MBA students were nearly identical. This suggeststhatassertivenessexpectanciesoftentaketheformofimplyinganidealoroptimallevelofassertivenessthatvariesfromonepersontothenextandcouldbetakenasameasureofexpectancies.Thedrawingresultsalsoshowedgreatestvarianceattheextremes:mosteveryoneagreedthatsomemiddlelevelofassertivenessledtogoodoutcomes;peoplevariedmoreconsiderablyontheoutcomestheythoughtwouldbeassociatedwithextremelevelsofassertiveness.Thiswasespeciallytruefor expected instrumental outcomesathighassertiveness: somepeople thoughthigh assertiveness would bring instrumental gains, and others thought it wouldbackfire(seeFigure 5.2).Thissuggeststhatexpectedoutcomesforveryhighlevelsofassertivenesswouldbeanotherwayofmeasuringexpectancies.

aSSertiveneSS expectancieS and BehaviorHavingcharacterizedassertivenessexpectanciesasoftentakingacurvilinearformandvaryingfrompersontopersonatextremelevelsofassertiveness,Isoughtevi­dencelinkingtheseexpectanciestobehavior.Initialevidencecomesfromthepre­viouslynotedline­drawingstudy(Ames,2008),wheremeasuresofbothoptimalassertiveness(thelevelofassertivenessforeachparticipantthatyieldedthegreat­estsocialorinstrumentaloutcomes)andextremeassertiveness(theexpectedsocialor instrumental outcomes for the lowestorhighest levelsof assertiveness)wereassociatedwithself­reportedassertiveness.However,non­self­reportmeasuresofassertivenesswouldarguablymakeamorecompellingcase.

In subsequent studies (Ames, 2008, Studies 3 and 4), I pursued and foundsuch evidence. For the independent measure of expectancies, participants pre­dictedsocialandinstrumentaloutcomesforarangeofspecificbehaviorsspanningfromlowassertivenesstohighassertiveness.Forinstance,participantsreviewedascenarioinvolvingamanager’slow­ballofferinasalarynegotiation.Participantswentontoconsideranumberofresponses,rangingfromacceptingthelow­ball

Social outcomes

Assertiveness Expe

cted

inst

rum

enta

l out

com

es

Expe

cted

soci

al o

utco

mes

Assertiveness

Instrumental outcomes100

80

60

40

20

0

–20100

+1 SD

+1 SD

–1 SD –1 SD

Mean

Mean

9080706050403020100

100

80

60

40

20

0

–201009080706050403020100

figure 5.2 Plotsofexpectancydrawingmeansandvariance.

Page 89: the Psychology of Social Conflict and Aggression Sydney Symposium in Social Psychology

daniel ameS72

offertorespondingwithanaggressivecounteroffer,andthenratedtheoutcomestheyexpectedwould result, suchasfinalnegotiated salary and liking and trustforthenewemployeeonbehalfofthemanager.Inanotherscenario,participantsimaginedtheywereinateammeetingwithafellowmanagerwhorecommendedastrategicinitiativetheyknewwouldnotbesuccessful.Participantsratedoutcomesforresponsesrangingfromsayingnothingtovociferouslyandforcefullyobjecting.Ineffect,acrossthesescenarios,participantsmadeaforecastofwhattheythoughtwouldhappeniftheyyieldedgroundorfoughthard.Towhatextentwouldtheygettheirway?Andtowhatextentwouldtheygetalong?Theseexpectanciesservedasanindependentvariable,tappingintoparticipants’moregeneralviewsofwhathappenswhentheypushhardorgivein.

Asexpected,participants’self­reportedexpectancymeasuresbasedonaseriesofspecificbuthypotheticalsituationspredictedindicesofparticipants’assertivebehaviorbasedonreportsfromnegotiationcounterpartsandreal­lifecoworkers.Thosewhoexpectedrelativelyminimalcostsforhighlevelsofinterpersonalasser­tiveness (e.g., they thought a manager would find an aggressive counteroffer inthesalarynegotiationacceptable)wereseenbypartnersinanunrelateddyadic,fixed­sumnegotiationexerciseasconsiderablymoreassertive.Expectanciesalsopredictedthevalueclaimedinnegotiationsettlements:thosewhoweremoreopti­misticaboutthepayoffsofhighlyassertivebehaviorachievedmorefavorabledealterms.Inanotherstudy,participantswereratedbyworkcolleaguesfortheirtypi­callevelofassertivenessintheactualworkplace(e.g.,standingtheirgroundinaconflict).Aspredicted,workcolleaguessawthosewhoweremoreoptimisticaboutthepayoffsofhighlyassertivebehaviorinthescenariosasconsiderablymoreasser­tiveintheworkplace.

TheevidenceIhavegatheredsuggeststhatindividuals’assertivenessexpectan­cieshaveaplaceinpredictingtheirbehavior.Whilemyworktodatehasgaugedonly certain kinds of assertive behavior, I suspect assertiveness expectanciesshape other behaviors as well, including those addressed elsewhere in this vol­ume.Forinstance,Chapter11addressesintimatepartnerviolence,notingmodels(e.g.,Holtzworth­Munroe,1992)thatdescribehowindividualschoosebehavioralresponses, ranging frompassivityor acquiescence to violence,based inpartontheir expectations of the behavior’s consequences. Chapters 3 and 13 addressaggressioninthewakeofostracism.Expectanciesaboutwhatdifferentbehaviorswillachieve(e.g.,renewedacceptancebytheostracizers,punishingoutcomesfortheostracizers,experiencedremorseonbehalfoftheostracizers)mayplayaroleinresponsestobeingostracized.Chapter9notesworkonthedisplayofangerincon­flictandnegotiations.Somedisplaysarecertainlyspontaneousanduncontrolled,whereasotherdisplaysmaybecalculated.Behindthesecalculateddisplayslikelylieexpectancies,whetherrightorwrong,aboutwhatdisplaysofangerwillachieve(e.g., intimidation).Chapter10presentsacompellingmotivationalmodelofter­roristbehaviorrevolvingaroundthequestforsignificance.Expectanciesmayhelpdelineatetheseprocesses:chartinganindividual’sexpectanciesaboutwhichactswillleadtowhatkindsofsignificance(e.g.,“IfIdieinanattackIwillbemartyred”vs.“Only ifIbothdieandkillotherswillIbemartyred”)couldhelpusbetterunderstandandpossiblycurtailactsofdramaticviolenceandharm.

Page 90: the Psychology of Social Conflict and Aggression Sydney Symposium in Social Psychology

puShing up to a point 73

Insum,peoplevaryinwhattheyexpecthappenswhentheypushhardorgivein,andtheseidiosyncraticexpectanciesarepredictiveofatleastsomeassertivebehav­iors.Butaretheseeffectsdistinctfromtheeffectsofmotivations,suchasadesireto win or a concern for maintaining relationships?Areexpectancies themselvesmerelyreflectionsofmotivations?Thenextsectiontakesupthesequestions.

aSSertiveneSS expectancieS and motiveSOverthelasthalfcentury,scholarsofconflict,negotiation,andsocialdilemmashaverepeatedlylinkedinterpersonalconflictbehaviortounderlyingmotivations—variously identifiedaspreferences,concerns,priorities, orientations, andvalues.While interaction­specific objectives surelymatter (e.g., “Iwantmymanager togivemea10%raise today”),considerableattentionhasbeenpaidtomoregen­eralsocialmotives(e.g.,“Idon’tcarewhathappenstoothersaslongasIgetwhatI want”). One of the most active traditions of such work revolves around dual­concerntheory(e.g.,Carnevale&Pruitt,1992)andmotivationalorientations(e.g.,Messick&McClintock,1968),whichpositthatpeoplevaryintheirattitudesabouttheir own and their conflict partners’ outcomes. Combinations of these dimen­sions yield different orientations that are often labeled proself or competitive(concernedwithmaximizingthepositivedifferencebetweenselfandother),indi­vidualist(concernedsolelywithone’sownoutcome),andprosocialorcooperative(concernedwithmaximizingjointoutcomes).Anabundanceofresearchhaslinkedthesesocialvalueorientationstoassertivebehaviorsinsocialdilemmasandgames(e.g.,McClintock&Liebrand,1988;VanLange,1999)andinconflictandnego­tiation (e.g.,DeDreu&VanLange,1995;DeDreu,Weingart,&Kwon,2000;Olekalns&Smith,2003).

While thesesocialorientationsseemtoaccount for thebulkofmotivationalworkonconflictbehavior,otherinterpersonalmotiveshavebeeninvokedaswell,suchascommunalvalues(e.g.,Amanatullah,Morris,&Curhan,2008),agreeable­ness(e.g.,Barry&Friedman,1998;Graziano,Jensen­Campbell,&Hair,1996),andneedtobelong(e.g.,Baumeister&Leary,1995;DeCremer&Leonardelli,2003).Inaddition,thereisevidencetosuggestthatidentitymotivations,suchastheneedtosavefaceormaintainanimageoftoughness,canaffectconflictbehav­ior(e.g.,White,Tynan,Galinsky,&Thompson,2004).Inshort,ampleevidenceshowsthatwhatpeoplecareaboutaffectstheirassertivenessinconflictandnego­tiation.Putsimply,motivesmatter.

Theresultsaboutexpectanciesreviewedaboveraisethequestionabouthowmotivesandexpectanciesrelate.Willthelinkbetweenexpectanciesandbehaviorremainaftercontrollingformotivations,orwillitbeovershadowed?Areexpectan­ciessimplyderivativesofmotives?Iexpectthatwhereasassertivenessexpectanciesmightberelatedtosocialmotivations,anindependentexpectancy–behaviorlinkwillgenerallyremainaftercontrollingformotivations.Thelogiccanbeillustratedbyworkinthedomainofriskychoicethatdistinguishesbetweenriskpreferencesandriskperceptions(e.g.,Weber&Milliman,1997).Riskpreferences,analogoustomotivations,concernaperson’sappetiteforrisk.Riskperceptions,analogoustoexpectanciesinthecurrentaccount,concernaperson’sassessmentofhowrisky

Page 91: the Psychology of Social Conflict and Aggression Sydney Symposium in Social Psychology

daniel ameS74

a given option is.Empirically, these preference andperceptionconstructs haveproventobedistinctandbothappeartoexhibitindependenteffectsonriskychoice(e.g.,Weber&Hsee,1998).Twopeoplecouldhaveidenticalriskpreferencesbutdifferintheirchoicesbecauseoneperceivestheoptionasriskyandtheotherdoesnot.Inthedomainofconflict,twopeoplecouldhaveidenticalmotives—thesameconcernsformaintainingrelationships,forinstance—butdifferintheirassertive­nesssimplybecauseoneexpectsabehaviorwoulddamagearelationshipandtheotherdoesnot.

Inbrief,therecentworkIhavedoneonassertivenessexpectanciesisconsistentwith this idea.Across the studies (Ames,2008), I foundweakornonsignificantlinksbetweenexpectancies andmeasuresofmotivations, including social valueorientations, conflict styles,unmitigated communion, and basic questions aboutconcernsforwinningandmaintainingrelationships.Inotherwords,expectanciesarenotmerereflectionsofmotivations.Further,acrossthestudies,bothexpectan­ciesandmotivationsappearedtobesimultaneouslyandseparatelypredictiveofbehavior,suggestingthattheyeachhaveadistinctroletoplay.Assertivebehaviorappearstobeaproductofbothwhatpeoplecareaboutandwhattheybelievewillhappenwhentheygiveinorpushhard.

I have not yet found evidence for an interaction between expectancies andmotivations.However,thelogicforsuchinteractionsseemsclear.Imagineateamleader advocating on her team’s behalf to an organizational leader. She mightexpectthatthehardershepushesthemorecostlyitwillbeintermsofherrela­tionshipwithher leaderbutthebettershewilldointermsofresourcesforherteam.Alongwiththesetwoexpectancieswouldbetwomotivations:concernforherrelationshipwiththeleaderandconcernwiththeresourcesforherteam.Itstandstoreasonthatifshecaresvastlymoreabout,say,herteam’sresources,theresourceexpectancywouldbemorepredictiveofherbehaviorthantherelation­shipexpectancy.Alternately,ifshecaresverylittleabouttheteam’sresources,itseemsunlikelythattheresourceexpectancywouldbeapowerfulpredictorofherbehavior.Inshort,theexpectanciesthatmattermostinpredictingourbehaviorarelikelythoseaboutoutcomesthatmeanthemosttous.Afullaccountofinter­personalassertivenessandbehavioralchoicewould likelyneedtohaveroles forbothmotivationsandexpectanciesaswellasaninteractionbetweenthetwo.

SourceS of expectancieSEvidencethatexpectanciesareanimportantpredictorofassertivebehaviornatu­rally raises another question: where do expectancies come from? The fact thatexpectancies seem to carry across domains implies an underlying core, such asbasicworkingmodelsfortheself,others,andrelationshipsthatarebuiltupoverthecourseof a lifetime (Chapter2 in this volume).Self­esteemmaybepartofthiscore.BaldwinandKeelan(1999)arguedthatindividualshigherintraitself­esteem had morepositive interpersonal expectancies about theirownability tosecureaffiliationfromothers.Indeed,therewassomeevidenceofamodestposi­tivelinkbetweenself­esteemandoptimalassertivenessinthelinedrawingstudydiscussed earlier (Ames, 2008, Study1).Those lower in self­esteemwere more

Page 92: the Psychology of Social Conflict and Aggression Sydney Symposium in Social Psychology

puShing up to a point 75

pessimisticabouttheirabilitytopursuetheirinterestswithoutsufferingrelationalcosts.Futureworkmightfurtherexplorethelinksbetweenassertivenessexpec­tanciesandrelevantworkingmodelsorschema,suchasself­esteem,rejectionsen­sitivity,andattachmentstyles.

Whileexpectanciesmaybepartlyrootedinlong­heldmodelsthataccumulateoveralifetime,theymayalsobeshapedandreinforced—validlyornot—bymoreimmediateevidence.Partof theprocessnodoubt reflects the fact thatpeopleonly experience the outcomes of behaviors they choose, not of behaviors theyforego.Suchisthecasewithanxietydisorders,wheresomeoneafraidofdrivingoverbridges for fearofcollapse, for example,neverdoes soand thusdoesnotexperience theoutcomeofdriving safelyoverabridge, left instead to imaginethattheworstmighthavehappenedifshehaddoneso.Someonewhoispessimis­ticaboutassertinghisownopinioninagroupsettingsystematicallyholdsback,neverexperiencingthepositiveeffectsofspeakingupandthusneveroverturninghisoverlypessimisticexpectancy.Confirmationbiasesandselective interpreta­tion no doubt also play a role. Someone who is optimistic about her ability topush hard without damaging relationships may see what she expects to see inthewakeofaconflict.Shemaytakesuperficialsignsofacceptanceasasignalofhercounterpart’scontentmenteventhoughthecounterpart’sampleresentmentislingeringbelowthesurface.

Another typeofevidence forexpectancies is“vicariousexperience” throughvariousmediasources.Peopleofallagesarefrequentlyexposedtomediaportraitsof assertion–outcome contingencies, as when characters in movies or televisionshowaggressionandexperiencepositiveornegativeoutcomes (e.g.,Huesmann,Moise­Titus, Podolski, & Eron, 2003). As Chapter 17 in this volume notes, theInternetisincreasinglyubiquitousasasourceofinformation,givingviewersnewwaystowatchactualactsofaggression(e.g.,videosof“happyslapping”aggression)ortoassertthemselvesorwatchothersassertthemselvesinnovelways(e.g.,flam­inginachatroomorpostingdisparagingremarkstoaFacebookpage).Elsewhere,workonvideogameviolenceexaminestheimpactofgameplayingandexposureonbehavior(e.g.,Andersonetal.,2004).Together,thesetraditionsofworkhighlightthatthedevelopmentofassertivenessexpectanciesislikelynotsimplyaproductof one’s own direct experience with assertion and outcomes but partly a prod­uctofthecontingenciespresentedinthemediaenvironment.Totheextentthatpeoplearechronicallyexposedtooverlyoptimisticassertivenesscontingenciesinthemedia(e.g.,thataggressionreliablybringsdesirableoutcomes),theymaycometoholdexpectanciesthatreflectsuchcontingenciesandbehaveaccordingly.Oneimplicationisthatthelinkbetweenmediaexposureorconsumptionandaggres­sivebehaviormaybepartiallymediatedbyexpectancies.

Situational influences could also affect expectancies in “nonevidentiary”ways (i.e., throughprocesses other than apparentevidencewhathappens whenonepusheshardorgivesin).Forinstance,whileattachmentstylesmayreflectasomewhatstableinterpersonalschema,evidencesuggeststhatattachmentmotiva­tionscanalsobeprimedandmanipulated,suchasthroughfocusingindividualsonvariousattachmentfigures(seeChapter2inthisvolume).Researchonmoodalsosuggeststhatthoseinhappycomparedwithsadmoodsarelesspoliteintheir

Page 93: the Psychology of Social Conflict and Aggression Sydney Symposium in Social Psychology

daniel ameS76

interpersonalrequestsfromothers(Forgas,1999).Itcouldbethathappymoodsengendermoreoptimisticassertivenessexpectancieswhereassadmoodsengendermorepessimisticones.Situations thatpromote or inhibit empathicor cognitiveperspective takingcouldalsoaffectexpectationsabouthowotherswill react toone’sownassertiveoracquiescentbehavior(seeChapter7inthisvolume).

Foravarietyofreasons—suchasdevelopmentalhistory,distortedormisinter­pretedevidence,andsituationalfactors—peoplemayoftenhavemisguidedexpec­tanciesandmaynoteffectivelybringtheminlinewithreality.Theimplicationmayseemdisconcerting:lefttotheirowndevices,peoplewithmisguidedexpectanciesmightpersistinbehavingonthebasisofdistortedforecasts.However,Ibelievethefactsthatexpectanciesshapebehaviorandthatexpectanciescanberevisedin the face of evidence and feedback is a rather hopeful one. Although peoplemaynotnaturallyorspontaneouslyconfronttherightkindsofevidence,individu­als,organizations,andtrainerscanfindwaystohelpthemdoso,potentiallylead­ingtomoreeffectiveassertivenessandconstructiveinterpersonalconflict.Withinorganizations,multiraterfeedbackhasthepotentialtodeliverusefulinformation;inbusinessschools,negotiations trainingwithrole­playexercisesanddebriefingoftenhelpsindividualcalibratetheirsenseofwhathappenswhentheypushhardorgivein.

expectancieS and other experienceSOthercontributionsinthisvolumehaveencouragedmetothinkbeyondthefocalquestionofthischapter(whenandwhydopeoplepushmoreorlessininterper­sonal conflicts?)and toconsiderhowexpectanciesmight relate tootherexperi­ences.Chapter12inthisvolumedescribesaresearchprogramchartinghowgoalsimilaritypredictsconflictinromanticrelationships:partnerswhohavelessgoalsimilarityreportmoreconflictintheirrelationships.Itispossiblethatsimilarityanddivergencebetweeninstrumentalandrelationalexpectanciescouldshedlightonconflictinromanticrelationships.WhileIhavegenerallyfocusedoninstrumen­talandrelationalexpectancieshavingacommoncore(e.g.,peoplewhoareopti­misticaboutinstrumentaloutcomesforpushinghardtendtobemoreoptimisticaboutrelationaloutcomes,too),theycanalsodiverge.Takethecaseofapersonwithveryoptimisticinstrumentalexpectanciesaboutherpersonalgoals(e.g.,“IfIresistmyspouse’sdemandsonmytime,Icandevotemoretomyworkandachievegreaterprofessionalsuccess”)butverypessimisticrelationalexpectancies(e.g.,“IfIresistmyspouse’sdemandsonmytime,hewillresentmeandourrelationshipwillsuffer”).Suchapatterncouldbeastressfulone,regardlessoftheactor’sbehavioralchoices.Contrastthiswithapersonwhohasoptimisticrelationalexpectanciesinadditiontoinstrumentalones(e.g.,“IfIresistmyspouse’sdemandsonmytime,hewillunderstandandourrelationshipwillremainsecure”).Thispersonmaynotfeelabindortrade­off,thoughitispossiblethattheseoptimisticexpectanciescouldleadtobehavioralchoicesthatwouldcreaterelationshipstress.

Chapter14inthisvolumedescribeshowtheimpactofavictim’sforgivenessinthewakeofatransgressiondependsontheextentandqualityoftheharm­doer’samendmaking.Victimswhoshowedforgivenesstowardaharm­doerwhofailedto

Page 94: the Psychology of Social Conflict and Aggression Sydney Symposium in Social Psychology

puShing up to a point 77

makeamendshadlowersubsequentself­respectthanthosewhoforgaveaharm­doerwhomadeamends.Itseemslikelythatsomepeoplearesimplyhabitual“for­givers”whomaysufferwhentheirforgivenessisunrequited.Inothercases,thiseffectmayrepresentafailedpredictionsuchthat theforgiverhadacontingentexpectancyaboutwhatwouldhappen(e.g.,“IfIforgivehim,hewillapologize,makeamends,andchangehisways”)thatwasnotborneout.Totheextentthatsomecasesentailsuchpredictionfailures,itcouldbeusefultoexplorewhatleadstothiskindofmisplacedoptimism.Theoppositeeffectwouldbeinterestingaswell:whenanoverlypessimisticexpectancy(e.g.,“IfIforgivehim,itwon’tmat­terbecausehe’llneverchangehisways”)leadssomeonetoavoidforgivenessthatcould have been beneficial to both parties involved (cf. Kammrath & Dweck,2006).

concluSion

Our lives are, inmanyways, enrichedby interactingwithotherswhoaspire tothingsthatwedonot.Butthefactthatwearesurroundedbypeoplewithdifferentobjectivesandinterestsmeansthatweareinaconstantseriesofconflicts,mostlylow­gradeones,throughoutourdays,confrontingagainandagainthesameques­tions:HowhardshouldIpush?ShouldIresistmyspouseorchild?ShouldIdefymyneighbororboss?ShouldIgivein?Allofuswhointeractwithotherpeopleansweranongoingbarrageofsuchquestions,oftenarrivingatouranswersseam­lessly,perhapsevenunconsciously.Asscholars,wealreadyknowsomeabouthowpeopleanswerthesequestions,butwecan,should,andnodoubtwillknowmore.Ibelieve assertivenessexpectancies have the potential tohelp usbetterunder­standhowpeoplechoosehowhardtopushandthatcompletemodelsofassertivebehaviorshouldaffordaplaceforexpectancies.Yetvarietyisthespiceoflife,andIwouldbedisappointedifotherscholarsdidnotseethematterdifferently.Ilookforwardtothempushingback,butmaybenottoohard.

referenceS

Amanatullah,E.T.,&Morris,M.W.(2010).Negotiatinggenderroles:Genderdifferencesin assertive negotiating are mediated by women’s fear of backlash and attenuatedwhennegotiatingonbehalfofothers.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 98,256–267.

Amanatullah,E.T.,Morris,M.W.,&Curhan,J.R.(2008).Negotiatorswhogivetoomuch:Unmitigatedcommunion,relationalanxieties,andeconomiccostsindistributiveandintegrativebargaining.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 95,723–738.

Ames,D.R.(2008a).Insearchoftherighttouch:Interpersonalassertivenessinorganiza­tionallife.Current Directions in Psychological Science, 17,381–385.

Ames, D. R. (2008b). Assertiveness expectancies: How hard people push depends onthe consequences they predict. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 95,1541–1557.

Page 95: the Psychology of Social Conflict and Aggression Sydney Symposium in Social Psychology

daniel ameS78

Ames,D.R.(2009).Pushinguptoapoint:Assertivenessandeffectivenessinleadershipandinterpersonaldynamics.InA.BriefandB.Staw(Eds.),Research in Organizational Behavior,29,111–133.

Ames,D.R.,&Flynn,F.J.(2007).Whatbreaksaleader:Thecurvilinearrelationbetweenassertiveness and leadership. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 92,307–324.

Anderson,C.A.,Carnagey,N.L.,Flanagan,M.,Benjamin,A.J.,Eubanks,J.,&Valentine,J.C. (2004).Violent video games: Specific effects of violent content onaggressivethoughtsandbehavior.Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, 36,199–249.

Baldwin,M.W.,&Dandeneau,S.D.(2005).Understandingandmodifyingtherelationalschemas underlying insecurity. In M. Baldwin (Ed.), Interpersonal cognition (pp.33–61).NewYork:GuilfordPress.

Baldwin,M.W.,&Keelan,J.P.R.(1999).Interpersonalexpectationsasafunctionofself­esteemandsex.Journal of Social and Personal Relationships, 16,822–833.

Bandura, A. (1986). Social foundations of thought and action: A social cognitive theory.EnglewoodCliffs,NJ:PrenticeHall.

Barry,B.,&Friedman,R.A.(1998).Bargainercharacteristicsindistributiveandintegrativenegotiation.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74,345–359.

Baumeister,R.F.,&Leary,M.R.(1995).Theneedtobelong:Desireforinterpersonalattach­mentsasafundamentalhumanmotivation.Psychological Bulletin, 117,497–529.

Bowles,H.R.,Babcock,L.,&Lai,L.(2007).Social incentivesforgenderdifferences inthepropensitytoinitiatenegotiations:Sometimesitdoeshurttoask.Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 103,84–103.

Campbell, L., Simpson, J. A., Boldry, J. G., & Kashy, D. (2005). Perceptions of conflictand support in romantic relationships: The role of attachment anxiety. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 88,510–531.

Carnevale, P. J., & Pruitt, D. G. (1992). Negotiation and mediation. Annual Review of Psychology, 43,531–582.

Crick,N.R.,& Dodge,K.A. (1994).A reviewand reformulationof social information­processingmechanisms inchildren’s socialadjustment.Psychological Bulletin, 115,74–101.

DeCremer,D.,&Leonardelli,G.J.(2003).Cooperationinsocialdilemmasandtheneedtobelong:Themoderatingeffectofgroupsize.Group Dynamics: Theory, Research, and Practice, 7,168–174.

DeDreu,C.K.,&VanLange,P.A.(1995).Impactofsocialvalueorientationonnegotiatorcognitionandbehavior.Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 21,1178–1188.

DeDreu,C.K.,Weingart,L.R.,&Kwon,S.(2000).Influenceofsocialmotivesonintegra­tivenegotiation:Ameta­analyticreviewandtestoftwotheories.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 78,889–905.

DeGiovanni,I.S.,&Epstein,N.(1978).Unbindingassertionandaggressioninresearchandclinicalpractice.Behavior Modification, 2,173–192.

Diekmann, K. A., Tenbrunsel, A. E., & Galinsky, A. D. (2003). From self­predictionto self­defeat: Behavioral forecasting, self­fulfilling prophecies, and the effectof competitive expectations. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 85,672–683.

Downey,G.,&Feldman,S.I.(1996).Implicationsofrejectionsensitivityforintimaterela­tionships.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 70,1327–1343.

Fehr,B.,Baldwin,M.,Collins,L.,Patterson,S.,&Benditt,R.(1999).Angerincloserela­tionships:Aninterpersonalscriptanalysis.Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 25,299–312.

Page 96: the Psychology of Social Conflict and Aggression Sydney Symposium in Social Psychology

puShing up to a point 79

Fleishman,E.A.(1995).Considerationandstructure:Anotherlookattheirroleinleader­shipresearch.InF.Dansereau&F.J.Yammarino(Eds.),Leadership: The multiple-level approaches(pp.51–60).Stamford,CT:JAIPress.

Forgas,J.P.(1999).Onfeelinggoodandbeingrude:Affectiveinfluencesonlanguageuseandrequestformulations.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 76,928–939.

Graziano,W.G.,Jensen­Campbell,L.A.,&Hair,E.C.(1996).Perceivinginterpersonalconflict and reacting to it: The case for agreeableness. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 70,820–835.

Huesmann,L.R.,Moise­Titus,J.,Podolski,C.,&Eron,L.D.(2003).Longitudinalrelationsbetweenchildren’sexposuretoTVviolenceandtheiraggressiveandviolentbehaviorinyoungadulthood:1977–1992.Developmental Psychology, 39,201–221.

Kammrath,L.K.,&Dweck,C.(2006).Voicingconflict:Preferredconflictstrategiesamongincremental and entity theorists. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 32,1497–1508.

Kelley,H.H.,&Stahelski,A.J.(1970).Socialinteractionbasisofcooperators’andcompeti­tors’beliefsaboutothers.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 16,66–91.

McClintock,C.G.,&Liebrand,W.B.(1988).Roleofinterdependencestructure,individualvalueorientation,andanother’sstrategyinsocialdecisionmaking:Atransformationalanalysis.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 55,396–409.

Messick,D.M.,&McClintock,C.G.(1968).Motivationalbasisofchoiceinexperimentalgames.Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 4,1–25.

Miller,D.T.,&Turnbull,W. (1986).Expectanciesand interpersonalprocesses. InM.R.Rosenzweig&L.W.Porter(Eds.),Annual review of psychology(Vol.37,pp.233–256).PaloAlto,CA:AnnualReviews,Inc.

Mischel, W., & Shoda, Y. (1995). A cognitive­affective system theory of personality:Reconceptualizing situations, dispositions, dynamics, and invariance in personalitystructure.Psychological Review, 102,246–268.

Olekalns,M.,&Smith,P.L.(2003).Testingtherelationshipsamongnegotiators’motiva­tional orientations, strategy choices and outcomes. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 39,101–117.

Pietromonaco,P.R.,&Barrett,L.F.(2000).Internalworkingmodels:Whatdowereallyknowabouttheselfinrelationtoothers?Review of General Psychology, 4,155–175.

Roese,N. J.,&Sherman, J.W. (2007). Expectancy. In A.W. Kruglanski & E.T.Higgins(Eds.), Social psychology: Handbook of basic principles (pp. 91–115). New York:GuilfordPress.

Simonton, D. K. (1985). Intelligence and personal influence in groups: Four nonlinearmodels.Psychological Review, 92,532–547.

VanLange,P.A.M.(1992).Confidenceinexpectations:Atestofthetrianglehypothesis.European Journal of Personality, 6,371–379.

Van Lange, P. A. M. (1999). The pursuit of joint outcomes and equality in outcomes:An integrative model of social value orientation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 77,337–349.

Weber,E.U.,&Hsee,C.K.(1998).Cross­culturaldifferencesinriskperceptionbutcross­cultural similarities in attitudes towards perceived risk. Management Science, 44, 1205–1217.

Weber,E.U.,&Milliman,R.(1997).Perceivedriskattitudes:Relatingriskperceptiontoriskychoice.Management Science, 43,123–144.

White,J.B.,Tynan,R.,Galinsky,A.D.,&Thompson,L.(2004).Facethreatsensitivityinnegotiation: Roadblock to agreement and joint gain. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 94,102–124.

Page 97: the Psychology of Social Conflict and Aggression Sydney Symposium in Social Psychology
Page 98: the Psychology of Social Conflict and Aggression Sydney Symposium in Social Psychology

Section IICognitive and Affective

Influences on Conflict and Aggression

Page 99: the Psychology of Social Conflict and Aggression Sydney Symposium in Social Psychology
Page 100: the Psychology of Social Conflict and Aggression Sydney Symposium in Social Psychology

83

6NonconsciousBattlesofWill

Implicit Reactions Against the Goals and Motives of Others

N.PONTUSLEANDERandTANYAL.CHARTRANDDuke University

S omeofthemostcomplexandconsumingrelationshipsweexperienceinvolvepeoplewithwhomwe donot always agree—relationshippartnerswhosewants,demands,andneedsare incompatiblewithourown.Whenin the

realorimaginedpresenceofthoserelationships,wemaynotbesoreadilyinclinedtoacquiescetotheirinterests,andresearchinrecentyearssuggeststhat—muchincontrasttoourapparentassimilativetendencies(e.g.,Aarts,Gollwitzer,&Hassin,2004;Shah,2003)—wewilloftenautomaticallyreactagainstothersandtheirgoals.Althoughitmaybeimportantforpeopletogetalongwithandbeacceptedbyothers(Baumeister&Leary,1995),so,too,isitimportantforthemtoregulatetheiraffili­ative tendenciesandneedsvis­à­vis theirotherdesires—forpersonalautonomy,achievement, and positive­self regard—needs that may often be well served byignoringorevenopposingthewillsandwantsofothers.Managingsuchconflictingmotivationsisafundamentalissueinself­regulation(Cantor&Blanton,1996),andalthoughpsychologyhasexaminedseveralwayssuchconflictsplayoutwithintheindividuals’ownminds(Shah,Friedman,&Kruglanski,2002;Shah&Kruglanski,2002),itisnotentirelyclearhowthoseconflictsplayoutintheirinteractionswithothers.Nevertheless,researchinrecentyearssuggeststhatreactingagainstothers’goalscanoftenfacilitateself­regulationinsubtlebutimportantways—evenifitendsuppushingpeopleapartandunderminingtheirrelationships.

In thepresent chapterwe consider three basic routes throughwhich activegoalscannonconsciouslyfomentinterpersonalconflictsand,toatleastsomeextent,sociallyaggressivebehavior.First,individuals’nonconsciousandchronicgoalscaninfluencetheirsocialperceptions inwaysthatputanegativeorhostiletingeon

Page 101: the Psychology of Social Conflict and Aggression Sydney Symposium in Social Psychology

n. pontuS leander and tanya l. chartrand84

their evaluations of others and their goals. Second, individuals may often reactagainsttheperceivedgoalsandmotivesofothersbyeithermovingtocounteracttheirinfluenceorbyadoptingcontrastinggoalsinstead.Third,nonconsciousgoalsoftenactasbehavioral juggernauts in that theycanoperateand triggeraggres­siveness towardothersover thenatural courseof theirpursuit.Taken together,weintendtodemonstratethatactivegoalscannonconsciouslyencourageconflictandaggressionbyinfluencinghowtheirpursuersperceive,reactto,andgenerallybehavetowardothers.

nonconSciouS, goal-directed Social BehaviorResearch in the last few decades has increasingly found that much of humanbehavior andgoal pursuit is automatic, in that it occurs spontaneously,uncon­trollably, and with little to no conscious intent or awareness (Bargh, 1994; seeBargh&Chartrand,1999, for review).Thismeans thatmanyof thegoals thatindividualspursuemaynotbeassubjecttothetypesofconscious,deliberativeprocessing that helps individuals behave in socially appropriate or acceptableways(seealsoChapter4inthisvolumeonotherkindsofpassiveandunconsciousformsofaggression).Indeed,whereaspursuersofaconsciouslyheldgoalmightattend to interpersonal conflicts that arise by carefully reappraising their owngoalandadjustingtheirgoal­directedbehavior,pursuersofanonconsciouslyheldgoalmaynot. Ifonewereconsciouslypursuingahighlycompetitivegoal—say,towinamarathon—onemightpursueitonlyasfarasitisn’thurtingothersordisruptingone’srelationshipstothoseothers.Asnegativefeedbackfromothersincreased,particularlyregardingone’sbehavioralpursuitofthegoal,onemightrespondbyscalingbackthatpursuitorbyfindingotherwaystoameliorateanyriftsthatwerecreated(Carver&Scheier,1998).Withnonconsciousgoal­directedbehavior,however,individualsmaynotbeassensitivetosuchfeedbackbecausetheirgoal­directedactionsareoccurring largelyoutside theirconsciousaware­ness or control; therefore, any negative feedback they receive from their envi­ronmentmaynotbeaseasilyattributedtothegoal(Chartrand,Cheng,Dalton,&Tesser,inpress).Indeed,obnoxiouspeopleoftendonotbelievethattheyarebeingobnoxious(Cunningham,Barbee,&Druen,1997;Davis&Schmidt,1977),andthismaybeinpartbecausetheydonotconsciouslyrealizehowtheirbehaviorisinfluencingandaffectingothers.

Theautomaticityof sociallyaggressivebehaviorhasbeenexamined inpastworkbyconsideringtheassociationsthatmayforminmemorybetweenparticu­lar situations and certain behavioral responses (Todorov & Bargh, 2002), suchthat mere exposure to such situations in the future automatically invokes (or“primes”)ahostileoraversivebehavioralreaction(Anderson&Carnagey,2004;Ratelle,Baldwin,&Vallerand,2005).Inthischapterwewillexaminerecentworksuggestingthatgoals, too,maybecomelinkedinmemorytosituationalcuestobetriggeredtoactivationautomatically(Bargh,1990).Insomecasesthiscouldinvolvethedirectactivationofasociallyaggressivegoal(e.g.,competition),whileatothertimesthiscouldinvolvetheactivationofconceptsinmemorythatindi­rectlyincreasetheaggressivenessofone’sbehavioralpursuitofagivengoal.As

Page 102: the Psychology of Social Conflict and Aggression Sydney Symposium in Social Psychology

nonconSciouS BattleS of Will 85

a classic example of this latter form of indirect influence, participants in onestudywhoweresubtlyexposedtoaseriesofrudeness­relatedwordswere latermorelikelytointerrupttheexperimentertomoveontothenextpartofthestudy(Bargh, Chen, & Burrows, 1996). Similarly, study participants have also beenfound to play more greedily in an “ultimatum game” by keeping more moneyforthemselveswhenacorporate­stylebriefcasewassubtlypresent intheroom(Kay,Wheeler,Bargh,&Ross,2004).Evensubliminalexposuretoimagesofgunsandotherweapons—stimulithatrepresentphysicallyviolentmeansofattainingone’sgoals—canincreasetheaggressivenessofindividuals’thoughts(Anderson,Benjamin,&Bartholow,1998).Exposuretocuesrepresentingotherpeoplewhomindividualsregardasthreateningmayautomaticallyinvokecorrespondinggoalsinmemory(Gillathetal.,2006),evenifthosecuesareonlyincidentallyrelatedtotheirinteractionpartners.

Nonconscious, socially aggressive behavior may also have self­reinforcingqualitiestothemthatmightmakeitdifficultfor individualsto justifybehavingin other ways. As classically demonstrated by Chen and Bargh (1997), partici­pantswhohadbeensubliminallyprimedwithBlackfacessubsequentlydemon­strated more hostility toward another White participant than participants whohadbeenprimedwithWhitefaces.Interestingly,theotherparticipantrespondedmoreaggressively inturn,effectivelyconfirmingtheBlack­primedparticipants’initiallyhostileexpectancies.Thissuggeststhatindividualsstorehostilescriptsinmemorythatmaybetriggeredincidentallybysocialcuestoaffectnotjusttheirownbehaviorbutalsothecorrespondingbehaviorsofthosewithwhomtheyinter­act.Itisperhapsnotsurprising,then,thatindividualswhobehaveinaggressiveorconfrontationalwaysoftenregardtheirownactionsasmorejustifiedthanwhentheyseeothersbehavinginthesameway(Baumeister,Stillwell,&Wotman,1990;Gilbert&Malone,1995).

Ineffect,mereexposuretocertainsocialcues—betheyobjectsorpeople,behaviorsorsituations—cansufficetoactivateconceptsinmemorythatsetindi­viduals againstothersandengenderconflictwith them(see alsoChapter8 inthisvolumeonmoodeffectsonspontaneousaggressiontowardMuslims).Inthenextseveralsections,wewillexamineresearchthatconsidershowindividuals’nonconscious goals both instigate and are instigated by interpersonal conflictandaggression.Wewillexaminehownonconsciouslyactivatedgoalsandchronicmotives tinge and distort individuals’ perceptions of others in ways that leadthemto“see”thoseothersinmorehostileways.Wewillthenconsiderhowcuestoothers’goalsseemtoinherentlypressureindividualstorespondinkind,some­timesleadingthemtoemulatetheaggressivepursuitsofthosearoundthemandatother times leading themto reactagainstothersand theirgoals.Ina thirdsectionwewillexaminehowgoalsmay,ontheirown,nonconsciouslyfacilitatebehavioralaggressionoverthenaturalcourseoftheirpursuit.Thelargerbodyofthisworkwillfocusonthewaysnonconsciousgoalsmayengenderconflictsofinterestandinterpersonalaggression,butwewillconcludebyreviewingimpor­tantevidence suggesting thatnonconsciousgoalsalsooften serve toattenuateconflictsaswell.

Page 103: the Psychology of Social Conflict and Aggression Sydney Symposium in Social Psychology

n. pontuS leander and tanya l. chartrand86

part 1: coming into conflict: the polarizing nature of goalS

Research in the last decade has identified at least two broad ways active goalscan nonconsciously influence social perception: via evaluation and inference.Evaluations are the subjective assessments perceivers make of a target’s favor­ablenessorunfavorableness, and inferences are the assumptions thatperceiversmakeregardingthetraits,preferences,andgoalsthatotherspossess.Evaluationsandinferencescanbehighlyautomaticprocesses(Duckworth,Bargh,Garcia,&Chaiken,2002;Hassin,Aarts,&Ferguson,2005),andbothhavebeenfoundtooperate in serviceof (andbe skewedby) theperceivers’ activegoals (Ferguson,2005;Kawada,Oettingen,Gollwitzer,&Bargh,2004).

Weexamineinthissectionresearchsuggestingthatactivegoalsmaynoncon­sciouslyinfluencehowindividualsevaluatetheirrelationshipstoothersandinter­prettheactionsofthosearoundthem.Inparticular,wefocusonthewaysactivegoals can facilitatedevaluationof relationships anddisliking forpeoplewhodonot facilitategoalpursuit.Wewillpresentevidencesuggestingthatactivegoalscannonconsciously lead individuals to regard others inmorenegative andhos­tileways,potentiallysetting those individualsagainstothers inways thatcreatediscordandunderminethesocialrelationship.Indeed,thehostilitythatindividu­alsnonconsciouslybringintotheirsocialinteractionscanbeself­reinforcing,forinteractionpartnerswho feel that theyarebeingdevaluedandrejected tendtorespondwithgreaterangerandhostilitythemselves(Leary,Twenge,&Quinlivan,2006). At the earliest stages of perception, then, active goals may be operatingtononconsciouslypolarize individualsagainstothersand, thus, set thestageforinterpersonalconflict.

Goal-Tinged Interpersonal Evaluations

Goalshavelongbeenregardedasafilterforperceivingtheworld,leadingindividu­alstoevaluatestimuliaseitherpositiveornegativebasedontherelevanceofthosestimuli to the individuals’ current needs and goals (Lewin, 1935). Importantly,such goal­tinged evaluations occur spontaneously and without much consciousintent,awareness,orcontrol(Ferguson&Bargh,2004).Thismeansthatindividu­alsmightautomaticallydismissordevalue stimuli thatare seenas irrelevantorinterferingwiththeirgoals(Brendl,Markman,&Messner,2003),evenwhensuch“stimuli”areotherpeople. Indeed, recentworkbyFitzsimonsandShah (2008)foundthatparticipantswhowereprimedinadvancewithanonconsciousachieve­mentgoal(asopposedtonotbeingprimedwithanygoalinparticular)reportedlowerrelationshipclosenessandplacedlessimportanceontheirrelationshipstoothers who were not instrumental to their pursuit of achievement. These goal­primedstudyparticipantswerealsomoremotivatedtoavoidnoninstrumentaloth­ers—indicatinganimplicitaversiontothoserelationships—whileinactivepursuitoftheirnonconsciousgoal.

Suchgoaldependencyinrelationshipevaluationoccursnot justwithincloserelationshipswheresuchevaluationsmaybetargetedtowardaspecificotherbut

Page 104: the Psychology of Social Conflict and Aggression Sydney Symposium in Social Psychology

nonconSciouS BattleS of Will 87

mayalso extend to their evaluations ofothers whom theyonly see peripherallyor incidentally. In one recent study by Bargh and colleagues (Bargh, Green, &Fitzsimons,2008),participantsweregiventhegoaltoevaluateavideotapedpersonforwhattheythoughtwaseitherareporterjob(onewhererudenessandassertive­nessisapositiveattribute)orawaiterjob(whererudenessisanegativeattribute).Assuch,thegoalparticipantsweregiveninadvancewouldfavoreitherrudenessorpoliteness—treatingoneaspositiveandoneasnegativedependingonwhichjobthevideotapedinterviewwaspresumablyfor.Partwaythroughthevideotapedinterview,a“colleague”oftheinterviewerenteredtheroomandinterruptedtheinterview,doingsoeitherverypolitely(apologizingprofusely)orveryrudely(act­ingannoyedandaggressive).Importantly,althoughparticipantsinitiallyexpectedtoevaluatetheinterviewee,theywereactuallytaskedwithratingthe“colleague”who interrupted—an incidental other who was not the focal target of the par­ticipant’sgoal.Nevertheless,consistentwiththeperspectivethatactivegoalscanaffectevenone’sevaluationsofincidentalothers,resultsindicatedthatparticipantswhohadthefocalgoalofevaluatingforthewaiterpositiontendedtoshowlesslik­ingfortherudeinterrupterthanthepoliteone;incontrast,participantswhohadthefocalgoalofevaluatingforthereporterpositiontendedtoshowlesslikingforthepolite interrupter thantherudeone.Subsequentdebriefingfoundthatpar­ticipantswerenotconsciouslyawareoftheinfluencethattheirfocalgoalhadontheirsubsequent,unrelatedevaluation,whichsuggeststhatactivegoalsmayoftennonconsciouslysetpeopleagainstnotjustthefocaltargetoftheirevaluationsbutalsoanyonewhoentersthepursuer’sfieldofperceptionalongtheway.

Thus, individualsmayoftennonconsciouslydevaluerelationshipsanddislikeothers who do not meet the criteria for their active goals, even if those othersarenot the focal targetsof their evaluations. Thismightpromote interpersonalconflictsinacoupleways.First,individualsmaywithdrawfromorreactwithaver­siontononinstrumentalothers,whichmayinturnelicitmoreangerandhostilityfromthoseothers (Learyetal.,2006).Second,and intriguingly, italsosuggeststhatindividualswhoarethemselvespursuingmoresociallyaggressivegoalsmayactuallydrawclosertootherswhopossessappropriatelyaggressivetraits—attri­butesthatmaybedesirableinthemomentbutmayquicklylosetheirappealandbecome toxic to their relationshiponce their focal goal is satiatedand they arenowentangledwiththisaggressiveother(Barghetal.,2008;Forster,Liberman,&Higgins,2005).Indeed,pastworkon“fatalattractions”hasshownthattheveryfeaturesthatinitiallydrawindividualstowardotherscanoftenbethesamefea­turesthatendupfomentingrelationshipconflictandnegativitylateron(Felmlee,1995).Goal­dependentevaluations,then,maynonconsciouslypolarizeindividualsandsetthemagainstothersbyeitherpushingthemawayfromthosewhoarenotusefulinthemomentordrawingthemtowardthosewhoareusefulinthemomentbutmaybedifficulttoputupwithlateron.

Goal-Biased Inferences

Thebroad influence of activegoals—suchas their tendency to distort andbiassocialperception—canaffectnotjustindividuals’evaluationsofothersbutalsohow

Page 105: the Psychology of Social Conflict and Aggression Sydney Symposium in Social Psychology

n. pontuS leander and tanya l. chartrand88

thoseindividualsinterprettheactionsofothers.Indeed,perceiverstendtoratherautomaticallyassumethattheactionsofothersoperateinserviceofsomecorre­spondinggoal(Hassinetal.,2005),meaningthattheyinfergoalsinothersauto­maticallyandbasedonwhateverbehavioralcuesarereadilyperceptible.However,individualstypicallyrelyonverylittleinformationtomaketheirautomaticinfer­ences(Winter&Uleman,1984):notonlyaresocialsituationsoftenhighlyambigu­ous,butalsoperceivers’ownactivegoalsandchronicmotives tendto influencewhatbehavioralcuestheyaresensitivetoandwhatgoalstheyaremostlikelyto“see”inothers.AsperhapsbestdescribedbyKelly(1955),perceivers’ownmotiva­tionalorientations(e.g.,aggressivenessvs.gentleness)seemtooperateaspersonalscanningpatternsprojectedontotheenvironmenttodetectblipsofmeaning.Forexample,chronicallyaggressiveindividualstendtoratherautomaticallyinterpretthe actions of others in more aggressive terms, something not observed amongnonaggressiveindividuals(Zelli,Huesmann,&Cervone,1995).Relatedly,activat­ingaself­protectiongoalledonesampleofWhitestudyparticipantstoreport“see­ing”greaterangerinthephotographedfacesofBlackmen—moreangerthantheyreportedseeinginthephotographedfacesofWhitemen(orwomenofanyrace;Maneretal.,2005).Thissuggeststhatindividuals’ownactivegoalsnonconsciouslyinfluencehowmuchaggressionandhostilitytheyinferinothers.

Thetendencyforindividualstoprojecttheirownmotivationsontotheenviron­mentalsoextendstogoalsthatareactivatednonconsciously.InastudybyKawadaandcolleagues(2004),participantswerefirstprimedeitherwithanonconsciousgoaltocompetewithothersorwithnogoalinparticular.Participantsthenreadafictionalscenarioinwhichtwomenwereabouttoengageinaprisoner’sdilemmagameinwhichcooperationbybothpartieswouldyieldmildlypositiveoutcomesforbothmenbutcompetitionbyoneofthemwouldyieldgreatergainsforhimattheexpenseofhispartner.Althoughparticipantswerenotgivenanyconcretecuesregardinghowthetwomenwouldbehave,itwasmadeclearthatifeitherpartnerdecidedtoplaythisgameaggressivelyhewouldhandilybeathispartnerintermsoftotalgains.Toassessthekindofinferenceparticipantsmadeasafunctionoftheirnonconsciousgoalprimingcondition,participantswereinstructedtoguesshow aggressively they thought the men would play. Results indicated that par­ticipantsprimedwithanonconsciousgoaltocompeteguessedthatthetwomenwouldplaymoreaggressivelyagainsteachotherthanparticipantsnotprimedwithagoal, suggesting that theirownnonconscious competitivenessgoalhadbiasedthemtoinfergreatercompetitivenessinothers.

Itmayalsobethecasethatindividuals’goal­tingedevaluationsofothersinter­actwith theirgoal­biased inferences, resulting inperceptionsofothers thatareboth negatively tinged and hostilely interpreted. For instance, individuals’ ownmotivational orientations can interact in important ways to influence the kindsof motivationsbrought to mindbyothers. Inone recent study (Brazy,Shah, &Devine, 2005), White participants initially completed an implicit measure oftheir own chronic prevention and promotion motivational orientations and alsoa measure of their prejudicial attitudes toward Blacks. Participants were thensubliminally primed with concept words relating to African Americans, duringwhich their response latencies tomotivationalwords linked toeitherpromotion

Page 106: the Psychology of Social Conflict and Aggression Sydney Symposium in Social Psychology

nonconSciouS BattleS of Will 89

(e.g., lazy, outgoing)orprevention(e.g.,threatening, considerate)wereassessed.Theresearchers foundthathighlyprejudicedparticipantswhopossessedstrongpromotion­relatedmotivationalorientationsdemonstratedgreatercognitiveacces­sibility of the stereotyped promotion­related words (e.g., lazy), whereas highlyprejudiced participants who possessed strong prevention­related motivationalorientationsdemonstratedgreatercognitiveaccessibilityofthestereotypedpre­ventionwords (e.g., threatening). Ineffect, theseparticipants’ownmotivationalorientationsinteractedwiththeirprejudicestoshowdifferenttypesofnegativelytingedmotivationalinferences.Thisstudyindicatesthatindividuals’ownmotiva­tions may nonconsciously influence both their evaluations and inferences to bemorenegativeandhostile.

Inthissectionwereviewedtworoutesthroughwhichgoalscannonconsciouslysetpeopleagainstothers:bydevaluingthemduetotheirlackofgoalinstrumentalityorbynonconsciouslyprojectingtheirowngoalsontothem.Thus,evenattheearliestmomentofexposuretocertainothers,individualsmayalreadybeevaluatingthemnegativelyorperceivingthemaspotentialthreats,obstacles,orcompetitorstogoalpursuit.Importantly,theseinitialandimmediateimpressionsofothersmightinformtheperceivers’laterbehavior;ifthoseinitialimpressionsareaversiveorhostile,thentheperceiversmayreactbytakingonoppositionalgoalsorbypursuingtheirgoalsmoreaggressivelyinthosesituations.Whereasthissectionwasaboutthewaysactivegoalsmightnonconsciouslyinfluencesocialperception,wenowmoveontothewaysthatindividualsreacttoothersandtheirperceivedgoalsandmotives.

part 2: counteracting and contraSting againSt otherS’ goalS

A growing part of the work on implicit motivational influences has examinedhowindividualsautomaticallyadoptandpursuethegoalstheyperceiveinothers(Aartsetal.,2004);somegoalsareevenlinkedinmemorytocertainrelationshippartners, such that subliminal exposure to cues reminding individuals of thoserelationships(e.g.,primingconceptwordsrelatedtofather)cansufficetotriggeractivationofagoalassociatedwiththatrelationship(e.g.,toachieveacademically;Shah,2003).Indeed,mereexposuretocertaincuescannonconsciouslytriggerthepursuitofgoalsthatothersholdforus,thatwetypicallypursueinthoseothers’company,oreventhatthoseotherspursueforthemselves(Fitzsimons&Bargh,2003;Leander,Shah,&Chartrand,2009;Shah).Moreover,suchinfluencesarefre­quentlyenhancedwhenthetriggeringcuerepresentsacloserelationshippartnerorin­groupmember,suggestingthatimplicitmotivationalinfluencesarefeltmorestronglywhentheycomefromotherswithwhomwemaybeentangledinotherways(Leanderetal.;Loersch,Aarts,Payne,&Jefferis,2008;Shah).However,notall motivational influences are desired, and, as is often the case, the perceivedinfluenceofothersandtheirgoalscanbeexperiencedasaversiveorunwanted,oftentriggeringanimplicitreactionagainstsuchinfluence.

Thismaybeespecially true inone’scloserelationships,wherethe influenceof others may be more frequent, harder to escape, and potentially recurring if

Page 107: the Psychology of Social Conflict and Aggression Sydney Symposium in Social Psychology

n. pontuS leander and tanya l. chartrand90

theindividualallowsittohappen(Brehm,1989).Researchonsocialallergens,forexample, has found that individuals’ relationship partners often unintentionallyexhibitarangeofodiouspersonalhabitsthatgrateontheindividualsovertimeandfomentincreasinglyhostilereactions(Cunninghametal.,1997;Cunningham,Shamblen,Barbee,&Ault,2005).Weproposethatasimilarprocessmayoccurfortheperceivedgoalsofothers:peoplemayautomaticallyreactagainstthegoalsheldorpursuedbyotherswhensuchinfluencesare,insomeway,perceivedasintrusive,aversive,orunwanted.

Thelureexertedbygoal­triggeringenvironmentalcuescanbeverydifficultto ignore;suchcuespullat individuals’attentionalandself­regulatoryresourcesrather automatically (Shah & Kruglanski, 2002). This potentially suggests that,whenthemotivationalinfluenceofanotherpersonisperceivedasinterferingwithone’sownpursuitsorongoingsenseofself,individualswillfeelcompelledtoreactinoppositionalwayswithoutknowingwhy,orevenrealizingthattheyarereact­ingagainstanythingatall.Suchreactionsagainstothers’goalscouldleadpeopletononconsciouslycounteractothersbymovingtoopposeorcompetewiththoseothers’goalsor tosimplycontrastthemselvesagainst thoseothers’goals.Eitherway,despite theinfluenceofothersoftenonlyoccurringwithintheindividuals’ownminds,theymayneverthelessreacttosuchperceivedinfluencebyengaginginproverbialbattlesofwillbeforetheyor their interactionpartnersconsciouslyrealizethataconflictofinterestsexistsbetweenthem.Inthepresentsection,then,weexaminehowgoalcounteractionandcontrastmightoccurnonconsciously ineverydaysocialsituationstofomentinterpersonalconflictandaggressioninsubtlebutimportantways.

Counteraction

Sometimes theperceived influenceof others’ goals and the potential impact oftheirpursuitscanbe regardedas imposing, interfering,orviolating individuals’self­regulatorypriorities.Whenthisoccurs,individualsmightrespondbymovingtocounteract the impactoftheotherperson’smotivational influence.Suchgoalcounteraction—reacting against the implicit motivational influence of others—hasbeenobservedmostreadilyinresearchinwhichothers’goalsareperceivedto interferewith individuals’ fundamentalself­relatedneeds—forautonomyandself­directedness,positiveself­regard,andoptimaldistinctiveness(Brehm,1966;Brewer, 1991; Tesser, 1988).Counteractionmight involve adopting anopposinggoal—thatis,onethatisincompatiblewiththeotherperson’sgoal(amotivational“counterforce”;Brehm)—tosupersedetheimpactoftheotherperson’sinfluence;however,itmayalsoofteninvolveadoptingaverysimilargoaltoeffectivelycom­petewiththeoffendingother.

Perhapsthebestwell­knownformofcounteractionisreactanceagainsttheperceived controlling influenceofothers. Indeed, a longhistoryof psychologi­cal research indicates that when individuals feel like their behavioral freedomisbeing threatenedby someoneorby some social institution those individualswill often reactbydirectlyopposing theperceivedmotivesof the targetother(Brehm,1966). Inrecentyears, studieshavedemonstratedthat suchreactance

Page 108: the Psychology of Social Conflict and Aggression Sydney Symposium in Social Psychology

nonconSciouS BattleS of Will 91

against thegoalsheldbyothersmightoftenplayoutnonconsciously andauto­matically. In one such study (Chartrand, Dalton, & Fitzsimons, 2007), chroni­callyreactantparticipants(andtheirchronicallynonreactantcounterparts)firstprovided the names ofothers who had a goal for them to eitherwork hard orrelax.Participants were then subliminally primedwithoneof thosenames (orwithanonsenseword in the control condition), afterwhich theirperformanceonananagramtaskwasassessed.Resultsindicatedthatnonreactantindividualsprimedwiththe“workhard”significantotherperformedbetterontheanagramtest than thoseprimedwith the “relax” significantother.However, chronicallyreactantindividualsshowedtheoppositepattern—theyperformedworse(better)ontheanagramtaskwhensubliminallyprimedwiththenameofsomeonewhowantedthemtoworkhard(relax).Thissuggestsnonconsciousreactanceagainstthegoalsheldbyothers.

Beyondreactingagainstthegoalsthatotherswantindividualstopursue,thereisalsoevidencetosuggestthatreactantindividualswillimplicitlycounteractthegoals that others are pursuing for themselves. Although past research suggeststhat individuals automatically “catch” the goals they see others pursuing (“goalcontagion”; Aarts et al., 2004), so too might they counteract the goals they seeotherspursuing. Ina studyconductedshortlyafter the2005hurricaneKatrina(Leander,Shah,&Chartrand,2010),participantsimaginedthattheirroommatewas planning a trip to the Southern Coast and were shown one of two sets ofimagesimplyingwhattheirfriendhadpackedforit(and,therefore,theirgoalforthetrip).Inthe“volunteer”goal­inferencecondition,thefriendhadpackedmate­rialsimplyingagoaltoworkforhurricanerelief(imagesshowing,e.g.,workboots,cleaningsupplies),andintheotherconditiontheroommatehadpackedmaterialssuggestingagoalunrelatedtowork.SimilartoresultsobservedbyChartrandetal.(2007),chronicallyreactantparticipantswerelessmotivatedtovolunteerafterinferringthattheirroommatepossessedagoaltovolunteer,suggestingcounterac­tionagainsttheroommate’sgoal.

Otherresearchsuggeststhat,ratherthanadoptinganoppositionalmotivationalstate,individualsmaymovetocounteractothersbynonconsciouslyadoptingandpursuingarelatedgoalthemselves,whicheffectivelysuggestsamovetocompetewith thoseothers.Onerecent seriesofstudiesdemonstrated that seeingothersengaginginblameattributionstoprotecttheirself­imagesoftenledparticipantstononconsciouslyadoptasimilarself­imageprotectiongoalandsubsequentlytoengageinmoreblamebehaviorsthemselves(Fast&Tiedens,2010).Thisindicatesthat individuals will oftennonconsciously adopt the samegoals they seeotherspursuinginordertocounteractthoseothers.

Takentogether,thepreviouslydescribedstudiessuggestthatmereexposuretoothersandtheirgoalscanelicitcounteractiveresponses.Inallofthesestudies,debriefingprocedureswereusedtoensurethatparticipantswerenotawareoftheinfluencethattheirexposuretothegoalsofothershadontheirownsubsequentmotivations,supportingthenotionthatcounteractionmayoccurwithlittletonoconsciousintentorawarenessandthatpeoplemayoftensettheirowngoalsagainstthegoalsofothersaftermerelyassumingthatthoseothersareactingagainsttheirinterestsinsomeway.

Page 109: the Psychology of Social Conflict and Aggression Sydney Symposium in Social Psychology

n. pontuS leander and tanya l. chartrand92

Contrast

Sometimestheimpactofothers’goalsandmotivesdoesnotelicitacounteractivereactionsomuchasadifferentiatingone.Whenindividualsregardthemselvesortheirvaluesasdifferentfromothers,theymightautomaticallyinferfromcuestothoseothers’goalsthattheyshouldbedoingtheopposite.Whereascounteractionimpliesamotivationalcounteroffensive,contrast ismoreaboutdifferentiation—distinguishingtheirownmotivationalstatefromatargetotherthattheyregardas“unlike”themselves.

The implicitnatureof individuals’ tendencies towardcontrasting themselvesawayfrom“different”otherswasfirstdemonstratedinDijksterhuisetal.’s(1998)work on assimilation and contrast to social stereotypes. In their research, par­ticipantswerefirstprimedwitheitheranintellectuallystereotypedgroup(“pro­fessors”vs. “supermodels”)orwithanexemplar fromoneof those intellectuallystereotyped groups (“Albert Einstein” vs. “Claudia Schiffer”). Participants thencompletedanintellectualtask,anddemonstratedopposingeffects:Whereasthestereotyped group prime (e.g., “professors”) facilitated assimilation to the ste­reotype(betterperformanceontheintellectualtask), theexemplarprimes(e.g.,“Einstein”)facilitatedcontrastagainstthestereotype(worseperformanceontheintellectual task). Similar contrast effects have been observed following subtleexposuretomembersofout­groupswhenone’santagonismtowardthemishigh(Spears,Gordijn,Dijksterhuis,&Stapel,2004),againstotherswhenone’smotiva­tiontoaffiliatewiththemislow(Sinclair,Huntsinger,Skorinko,&Hardin,2005),andagainstotherswhenone’scompetitivenessmotivationorcontrolmotivationishigh(Stapel&Koomen,2005;Tiedens&Jimenez,2003).Aswereviewinthissec­tion,suchcontrasteffectsmayalsoapplytothewaysthatindividualsreacttooth­ers’goalsandmotivationalstates.Forinstance,individualswillimplicitlydevaluegoalsthattheyregardasbeingtooordinaryortypicaltopursue—adirectresultoftheirtendenciestocontrastthemselvesmotivationallyfromotherswhenseekingtodifferentiatethemselves(Leander,Shah,&Chartrand,2010).

Aclassicexampleofmotivationalcontrastinvolvesindividualsdistancingthem­selvesfromgoaldomainsinwhichtheyarebeingoutperformed.Althoughthesuc­cessesofcloseotherscanoftenbeinspiring,sotoocantheybedeflatingwhentheyremindindividualsoftheirownshortcomings(Lockwood&Kunda,1997).Suchinfluencescanleadindividualstoadoptcontrastinggoalsandmotivationalstateswheninthecompanyofcloseotherswhoareoutperformingthem(Tesser,1988).Recent work has examined the implicit nature of this contrast effect (Leander,Shah,&Chartrand,2010).Participantsinonestudyfirstimaginedthattheywereeitherbeingoutperformedacademicallybyafriendornot,afterwhichtheywereledtoinferthatthefriendwaseitherstillactivelypursuinganachievementgoalornot.Participantswhohadfirstimaginedbeingoutperformedacademicallyandwere then led to infer that their friend was currently in pursuit of an achieve­mentgoalsubsequentlyshowedreducedsalienceofanacademicachievementgoalthemselvesonawordjudgmenttask,suggestingthattheycontrastedthemselvesagainsttheachievementgoaloftheiroutperformingfriend.Importantly,partici­pantsindicatednoconsciousawarenessofhowtheimaginedscenariomighthave

Page 110: the Psychology of Social Conflict and Aggression Sydney Symposium in Social Psychology

nonconSciouS BattleS of Will 93

affectedtheirsubsequentbehavioronthegoalsaliencetask,suggestingtheimplicitnatureoftheircontrast.

Individualsmayalsocontrastthemselvestothegoalsofotherswhenthosegoalsconflictwiththeirownvaluesorchronictendencies.Forinstance,Aartsandcol­leagues(2004)demonstratedthatexposuretocuessuggestingthatatargetotherwasinpursuitofagoaltohavecasualsexactuallyreducedthedesirabilityofasexgoalinparticipantswhoalsolearnedthatthetargetotherwasinacommittedrelation­shipalready(andwasthuscheating).Inanotherstudyinvolvingsubliminalpriming,weassessedparticipants’historyofmarijuanauseandalsoobtainedthefirstnamesofrelationshippartnerswhomtheyassumedintendedtoeitherusemarijuanaornotintheupcomingmonth.Participantswerethensubliminallyprimedwithoneofthosetwonames(theprodrugtempterorsomeoneelse),afterwhichtheyweregivenadrugpreventionmanualtoreadandtheamountoftimetheyspentreadingitwasrecorded.Interestingly,thoseparticipantswhotendedtoabstainfrommari­juanausewhoweresubliminallyprimedwiththenameofaprodrugtempterspentrelativelymore timereading thedrugpreventionmanual (Leanderetal.,2009),suggestingimplicitcontrastagainstthetempter’sgoaltousedrugs.

Sometimesindividualsmaycontrastthemselvestootherssimplybecausetheyseethoseothersasunmotivatedtowardagoaltowhichtheyarethemselveshighlycommitted. Inonerecent setof experimentsexamining the impact thatothers’indifferencehasonindividuals’ownmotivationandbehavior(Leander&Shah,2010),participantswereeithersubliminallyprimedwithimagesofothersexpress­ingapathyandalackofmotivationtowardacademicachievementorprimedwithotherimagesbeforetheyworkedonananagramtaskassessingtheirownpursuitofacademicachievement.Priortothis,however,halftheparticipantsineachsub­liminalprimingconditionwereprimedinadvancewithanonconsciousachieve­mentgoal,withtheotherhalfnotprimedwithanygoal.Theresultsthatfollowedsupportnonconsciousmotivationalcontrast:participantswhohadbeenprimedinadvancewithanonconsciousachievementgoalsubsequentlydemonstratedheight­ened anagram task performance when primed with the indifference of others.Thatis,individualswithanactivatedachievementgoalcontrastedthemselvestotheabsenceofmotivationtheysawinothersbyworkinghardertowardtheirnon­consciouslyheldacademicachievementgoal.

Individualsmightalsocontrasttothegoalsofinteractionpartnerswhosenon­verbalmannerismssubtlyindicatesocialasynchrony.Recentworkonbehavioralmimicryhasfoundthatindividualstendtoassimilatetothegoalsperceivedtobeheldbythosewhomimicthem,buttheymightignoreorevencontrasttothegoalsofthosewhodonotmimicthem.Intworecentstudies(Leander&Chartrand,2010), participants who indicated high sensitivity to behavioral cues to others’internal states—a form of empathy—showed a significant loss of achievementmotivation themselveswhen interactingwith a confederatewhoexpressedhighachievementmotivationoverthecourseoftheinteractionbutdidnotmimicthem.Importantly, participants indicated no conscious awareness of the confederate’snonverbalbehaviororhowitmighthaveinfluencedthem,suggestingthatindivid­ualswhoarehighlysensitivetobehavioralcuestoothers’internalstatesmightusesuchcuestodeterminewhethertoassimilatetoorcontrastagainstthoseothers’

Page 111: the Psychology of Social Conflict and Aggression Sydney Symposium in Social Psychology

n. pontuS leander and tanya l. chartrand94

goals.Thissuggeststhatthesubtlestbehavioralcuesmaynonconsciouslytriggercontrastagainst thegoalsassumedtobeheldbyan interactionpartner,evenatzeroacquaintancewiththatperson.

Itmayalsobethatindividualsnonconsciouslyassimilatetoothers’goalstocon­trasttothegoalsassumedofbroadersocialinstitutions—assimilatingtoafriend’sgoal to rebelagainstabroadersocietal lawornorm.Asdiscussedearlier,manyindividualscanbeimplicitlytemptedtoindulgeinillegalsubstances(Leanderetal.,2009),butrecentstudiesgoasfarastosuggestthatreactancemotivation—whichisusuallyassociatedwithreactingagainstthegoalsofothers—canactuallyfacilitateassimilationtoothers’pursuitsofsuchthingsasunderagealcoholcon­sumption(Leander,Shah,Chartrand,&Fitzsimons,2010).Thus,evenwhenindi­vidualsarenotcontrastingagainstthegoalsofothers,theymayoftenassimilatetootherstocontrastagainstbroadersocialinfluences,whichmayfomentotherformsofconflictthatextendbeyondtheimmediateinterpersonalsituation.

Whetherby counteracting the impositionsofothers’ goalsorby contrastingtodissimilarordislikedothers,individualsreadilyandnonconsciouslyadoptandpursuegoalsthatgoagainsttheperceivedwillandpreferencesofthosearoundthem.Importantly,suchgoalconflictbetweenindividualsmaybeabasicsourceofrelationshipstrifeanddissatisfaction—historyiscertainlyrifewithexamplesofhowcompetingor incompatiblegoalscanpreclude theopportunity toestablishfunctionalrelationships.What’sinterestingaboutthesestudiesisthatthesecon­flictsofinterestoccurnotjustnonconsciouslybutalsowhollywithinthemindsofstudyparticipantswhoaresimplybeingpresentedwithsocialcuesinalaboratorysetting. This suggests that individuals are quite susceptible to cues that triggertheiroppositionaltendencies,cuesthatleadthemtospontaneouslyreactagainstandopposetheperceivedgoalsofothersbeforetheyoranypotentialinteractionpartnerisconsciouslyawarethatsuchaconflictexists.

part 3: goal-directed aggreSSionIntheprevioussectionsweconsideredwaysinterpersonalconflictandaggressionmay stem from responding to or reacting against others and their goals. Yet tobeexamined,however, ishowactivegoalsmightfomentaggressivebehaviorontheirown,overthenaturalcourseoftheirpursuit.Giventherelativelyreflexivenatureanduncontrollabilityofnonconsciousgoals,theymaynotbeasburdenedbytherulesofpolitesocietyinthesamewaythatconsciouslyheldgoalsare.Inthisthirdandfinalsection, then,weexamineevidencesuggesting thatdispositionalandsituationalfactorsmightoftenleadindividualstononconsciouslypursuetheirgoalswithgreater impunity andheightenedbehavioral aggression.Examplesofthisfrompastworkhaveconsideredhowindividuals’ownchronicpredispositionsmightleadthemtononconsciouslypursuetheirgoalswithgreateraggressionwhenthe situation warrants. Children with more aggressive tendencies, for instance,ratherautomaticallygeneratemorehostilesolutionstosocialproblemscomparedwithchildrenwithlessaggressivetendencies(Bloomquist,August,Cohen,Doyle,&Everhart,1997),suggestinganimplicittendencytowardaggressioninpursuingtheirsocialgoals.

Page 112: the Psychology of Social Conflict and Aggression Sydney Symposium in Social Psychology

nonconSciouS BattleS of Will 95

Researchonnonconsciouslycuedsocialpoweralsosupports thenotionthatindividuals with certain chronic tendencies may automatically respond to suchcuesbypursuingtheirgoalsinmoreaggressiveorself­centeredways.Forinstance,participantsinonestudywhohadarelativelystrongexchangeorientationtotheirsocialrelationships(tit­for­tat,asopposedtoamorecommunalorientation)whowerenonconsciouslyprimedwithsocialpowersubsequentlybehavedmoreselfishlyonatask­sharingexercisebyoverloadingtheirpartnerwiththemoreoneroustasksandassigningtheeasiertaskstothemselves(Chen,Lee­Chai,&Bargh,2001).Theeffectsofprimingsocialpowercanalsononconsciouslyenhancethepursuitofsexgoalsamongmenwithhighpower–sexassociations inmemoryandamongmenwithstrongerpredispositionstowardsexualharassment(Bargh,Raymond,Pryor,& Strack, 1995). Thus, situational cues that implicitly invoke concepts of socialpowercanoftenenhanceindividuals’ownbehaviorallyaggressivetendencies.

Recent work suggests that failing at a nonconscious goal may also instigatesocially aggressivebehavior.Psychologyhas longacknowledged that failing at agoalcansometimestriggermorehostileandaggressiveresponses in individuals(Berkowitz,1989;Dill&Anderson,1995),andonerecentseriesofstudiesdemon­stratesthatsuchaggressioncanoccuramongindividualswhofailatnonconsciousgoals(Jefferis&Chartrand,2010).Inthesestudies,participantswerefirstprimedwithanimpressionformationgoalandthenledtofailatthatgoalpriortocomplet­ingvarioustasksmeanttoassesstheirsubsequentaggressiveness.Inoneofthesestudies,participantswhowereledtofailattheirnonconsciouslyactivatedimpres­sion–formationgoalsubsequentlypouredmorehotsauceintoacontainerthatwasgoingtobeconsumedbysomeonewhomtheyknewhatedspicyfoods.

Thus, individuals’chronictendenciesandgoaloutcomesmaynonconsciouslyinfluence theaggressivenessof theirsocialbehaviors, suggesting thatgoalsmayoften instigateconflictsand interpersonalaggressionontheirownandover thenaturalcourseoftheirpursuit.

concluSionInthisworkweexaminedthreebroadwaysgoalscannonconsciouslyfomentinter­personalconflictandaggression.First,activegoalscanshapesocialperceptionsinways thatpromotedevaluingof relationshipsandset individuals againstoth­erswhomtheyassumearepotentialcompetitorsfortheirgoals.Second,individu­alsmightoftencounteractorcontrastthemselvesagainstothers’goals,either inreactiontotheperceivedimpositionofothers’influenceortosimplydifferentiatethemselvesfromthoseothers.Third,nonconsciousgoalsmayoftenemploysociallyaggressivebehavioralstrategiestofacilitategoalpursuitorcopewithafailedpur­suit. Evidence from these three routes suggests that interpersonal conflict andaggressionmayoftenbeinherentinthepursuitofgoalsandbeacontributortothegoalsthatindividualstakeon,value,andoppose.Giventhatmanyoftheseinflu­encesareoccurringentirelywithinthemindsoftheindividualsthemselves,their“reactions”totheperceivedaffrontsofothersmayactuallybewhatinitiatescon­flictinaninteractionorrelationship.Indeed,intheresearchweexamined,itwasalwaystheparticipants’owngoals,needs,andchronictendenciesthatshapedtheir

Page 113: the Psychology of Social Conflict and Aggression Sydney Symposium in Social Psychology

n. pontuS leander and tanya l. chartrand96

perceptions and reactions to others. This potentially suggests that it is throughtheirownsubsequentbehaviorthattheyelicittheverykindsofhostilitythattheyautomaticallyexpectedfromtheirinteractionpartners,effectivelyreinforcingtheirinitialreactions(Chen&Bargh,1997).

One may generally conclude that individuals are most likely to oppose oraggressagainstothers’goalsandpreferenceswhentheirownneedsarenotbeingmet.Somegoalsthatindividualsbringintoasituationareinherentlyaggressive(seeParts1and3), andsomepeople inherentlyelicitmotivationallyaggressive reac­tions(Part2).However, suchreactionsdonotnecessarily implya failed interac­tion. Whether goals—even competitive or aggressive ones—interfere with one’srelationships likely depends on how those goals interact with the goals of one’sinteractionpartner.Inanysocialsituation—beitacompetitivesportorcommunalget­together—bothinteractionpartnersbringwiththemcertaingoalsandexpec­tationsthat,ifmet,couldresultinanoverallpositiveexperienceforthatinterac­tion.Ifbothinteractionpartnerswantandexpectcompetition,thensomelevelofoppositionandaggressionwillonlyfacilitate the interactionandthusenablethegoal’spursuit(tocompeteyouhavetohavesomeonetocompetewith).Wesuggestthatinteractionpartnerslikelybegintoperceiveaggressionandinterpersonalcon­flictwhenthereisamismatchofgoalsandexpectations,suchaswhenonepersonwishestobecompetitiveandtheotherdoesnot—orevenwhenonepersonwantstopursue(orisoverzealouslypursuing)anaffiliativegoal(e.g.,helping,romance)thattheotherdoesnotwanttobeapartof.Indeed,themotivationalfitbetweentwoindividualsmaydeterminehowoppositionalandaggressivebehaviorsaresub­jectivelyexperienced.

Giventhatthepresentchapterfocusedoninterpersonalconflictandaggression,itmaybeeasytoconcludethatgoalsoperatewithahighdegreeofimpunity,ifnotdisdain,forothers’needs.Thismaycertainlybetrueinmanycases,butawealthofevidencealso suggests thatgoalsoftenoperate tononconsciouslyreduceandminimizesuchconflicts.Themostpowerfulexampleofthisstemsfromresearchonthenonconsciouspursuitofprosocialgoals:whereaspossessingstrongprejudi­cialattitudescanenhancestereotypedmotivationalattributions,sotoodoespos­sessingchronicegalitariangoalshelp automatically inhibit stereotypeactivation(Moskowitz,Gollwitzer,Wasel,&Schaal,1999).Furthermore,whereasexchange­orientedindividualswhoareprimedwithpowermaybehavemoreselfishly,com­munallyorientedindividualsprimedwithpowerbehavemoreresponsibly(Chenetal.,2001).Evenwhen incompetitionwithan interactionpartner, individualswho are concurrently pursuing a prosocial goal will often nonconsciously scalebacktheirowneffortswhenoutperformingtheircompetitor(Bargh&Gollwitzer,1994).Furthermore, researchonnonverbalbehaviorhas foundawealthofevi­dencesuggestingthatsubtlecuesintheformofbehavioralmimicryreadilyelicitassimilation toan interactionpartner’sgoalsandvalues (Leander&Chartrand,2010;Leander,Chartrand,&Wood,2010;Maddux,Mullen,&Galinsky,2008).Therefore,despitethemanywaysgoalsfacilitateconflictandaggressioninrela­tionships, so too might individuals nonconsciously move to maintain a relativesenseofpeaceandharmonytoprotecttheirrelationships.

Page 114: the Psychology of Social Conflict and Aggression Sydney Symposium in Social Psychology

nonconSciouS BattleS of Will 97

It is important that we also note that many socially aggressive goals aredependentonothersandcannotbeeffectivelypursuedinthoseothers’absence(e.g., competition, rebellion, sexuality; Baron & Boudreau, 1987). Despite thepotential problems of goal influences on social inferences and reactions, indi­vidualsmayoftenbecompelledtoperceiveothersascompetitorsorasviabletargets to react against in order to satiate their chronic and recurring needs.Afterall,rebelsneedasocialinstitutiontorebelagainst,andpartisanpoliticiansneedopponents todecry—adoptingopposinggoalsmay represent thepursuitoftheirownunconsciousgoaltorebel.Thus,activegoalsmightoftenneedtononconsciouslymanufactureinterpersonalconflicts(realorimagined)tofacili­tatetheirownattainment.Theveryactofaggressingagainstothersandreactingagainsttheirgoals,then,mayhaveitsownfunctionalqualitiesthathaveyettobebeenfullyconsidered.

referenceS

Aarts,H.,Gollwitzer,P.M.,&Hassin,R.(2004).Goalcontagion:Perceivingisforpursuing.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 87(1),23–37.

Anderson,C.A.,Benjamin,A.J.,&Bartholow,B.D.(1998).Doesthegunpullthetrig­ger?Automaticprimingeffectsofweaponpicturesandweaponnames.Psychological Science,9(4),308–314.

Anderson,C.A.,&Carnagey,N.L.(2004).Violentevilandthegeneralaggressionmodel.InA.Miller(Ed.),The social psychology of good and evil(pp.168–192).NewYork:GuilfordPublications.

Bargh,J.A. (1994).Thefourhorsemenofautomaticity:Awareness, intention,efficiency,andcontrolinsocialcognition.InR.S.Wyer,Jr.&T.K.Srull(Eds.),Handbook of social cognition(2nded.)(pp.1–40).Hillsdale,NJ:Erlbaum.

Bargh,J.A.&Chartrand,T.L.(1999).Theunbearableautomaticityofbeing.American Psychologist, 54(7),462–479.

Bargh,J.A.,Chen,M.,&Burrows,L.(1996).Automaticityofsocialbehavior:Directeffectsoftraitconstructandstereotypeactivationonaction.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,71(2),230–244.

Bargh, J. A., & Gollwitzer, P. M. (1994). Environmental control of goal­directed action:Automatic and strategic contingencies between situations and behavior. In W. D.Spaulding(Ed.),Integrative views of motivation, cognition, and emotion(Vol.41,pp.71–124).Lincoln:UniversityofNebraskaPress.

Bargh,J.A.,Green,M.C.,&Fitzsimons,G.M.(2008).Theselfishgoal:Unintendedcon­sequencesofintendedgoalpursuits.Social Cognition,26(5),534–554.

Bargh,J.A.,Raymond,P.,Pryor,J.B.,&Strack,F.(1995).Attractivenessoftheunderling:Anautomaticpower­sexassociationanditsconsequencesforsexualharassmentandaggression.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,68(5),768–781.

Baron, R. M., & Boudreau, L. A. (1987). An ecological perspective on integrating per­sonalityandsocialpsychology.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,53(6),1222–1228.

Baumeister,R.F.,&Leary,M.R. (1995).Theneed tobelong:Desire for interpersonalattachments as a fundamental human motivation. Psychological Bulletin, 117(3),497–529.

Page 115: the Psychology of Social Conflict and Aggression Sydney Symposium in Social Psychology

n. pontuS leander and tanya l. chartrand98

Baumeister,R.F.,Stillwell,A.,&Wotman,S.R.(1990).Victimandperpetratoraccountsofinterpersonalconflict:Autobiographicalnarrativesaboutanger.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 59(5),994–1005.

Berkowitz,L. (1989).Frustration­aggressionhypothesis:Examinationandreformulation.Psychological Bulletin, 106(1),59–73.

Bloomquist,M.L.,August,G.J.,Cohen,C.,Doyle,A.,&Everhart,K.(1997).Socialprob­lemsolvinginhyperactive­aggressivechildren:Howandwhattheythinkinconditionsofautomaticandcontrolledprocessing.Journal of Clinical Child Psychology, 26(2),172–180.

Brazy,P.C.,Shah,J.Y.,&Devine,P.G.(2005).Considering the regulatory focus of implicit associations.Manuscriptunderrevision,UniversityofWisconsin–Madison.

Brehm,J.W.(1966).Social psychology: A series of monographs, treatises, and texts. NewYork:AcademicPress.

Brehm, J. W. (1989). Psychological reactance: Theory and applications. Advances in Consumer Research, 16,72–75.

Brendl,C.M.,Markman,A.B.,&Messner,C.(2003).Thedevaluationeffect:Activatinganeeddevaluesunrelatedobjects.Journal of Consumer Research, 29(4),463–473.

Brewer,M.B.(1991).Thesocialself:Onbeingthesameanddifferentatthesametime.Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 17(5),475–478.

Cantor,N.,&Blanton,H.(1996).Effortfulpursuitofpersonalgoalsindailylife.InP.M.Gollwitzer & J. A. Bargh (Eds.), The psychology of action: Linking cognition and motivation to behavior(pp.338–359).NewYork:Guilford.

Carver,C.S.,&Scheier,M.F.(1998).On the self-regulation of behavior.Cambridge,UK:CambridgeUniversityPress.

Chartrand, T. L. (2001). Mystery moods and perplexing performance: Consequences of succeeding and failing at a nonconscious goal.Unpublishedmanuscript,TheOhioStateUniversity.

Chartrand,T.L.,Cheng,C.M.,Dalton,A.N.,&Tesser,A.(2010).Consequencesoffailureatnonconsciousgoalsforself­enhancement.Social Cognition, 28,569–588.

Chartrand,T.L.,Dalton,A.,&Fitzsimons,G.J.(2007).Nonconsciousrelationshipreac­tance:Whensignificantothersprimeopposinggoals.Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 43,719–726.

Chen,M.,&Bargh,J.A.(1997).Nonconsciousbehavioralconfirmationprocesses:Theself­fulfillingconsequencesof automatic stereotype activation. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 33(5),541–560.

Chen, S., Lee­Chai, A. Y., & Bargh, J. A. (2001). Relationship orientation as a modera­torof theeffectsofsocialpower.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 80, 173–187.

Cheng,M.,&Bargh,J.A.(1997).Nonconsciousbehavioralconfirmationprocesses:Theself­fulfilling consequences of automatic stereotype activation. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 33,541–560.

Cunningham,M.R.,Barbee,A.P.,&Druen,P.B.(1997).Socialallergensandthereactionsthattheyproduce:Escalationofannoyanceanddisgustin loveandwork.InR.M.Kowalski(Ed.),Aversive Interpersonal Behaviors(pp.189–214).NewYork:PlenumPress.

Cunningham,M.R.,Shamblen,S.R.,Barbee,A.P.,&Ault,L.K.(2005).Socialallergiesinromanticrelationships:Behavioralrepetition,emotionalsensitization,anddissatisfac­tionindatingcouples.Personal Relationships, 12,273–295.

Davis,M.S.,&Schmidt,C.J.(1977).Theobnoxiousandthenice:Somesociologicalcon­sequencesoftwopsychologicaltypes.Sociometry, 40(3),201–213.

Page 116: the Psychology of Social Conflict and Aggression Sydney Symposium in Social Psychology

nonconSciouS BattleS of Will 99

Dijksterhuis,A.,Spears,R.,Postemes,T.,Stapel,D.,Koomen,W.,Knippenberg,A.,etal.(1998).Seeingonethinganddoinganother:Contrasteffectsinautomaticbehavior.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 75,862–871.

Dill,J.C.,&Anderson,C.A.(1995).Effectsoffrustrationjustifactiononhostileaggression.Aggressive Behavior, 21,359–369.

Duckworth,K.L.,Bargh,J.A.,Garcia,M.,&Chaiken,S.(2002).Theautomaticevaluationofnovelstimuli.Psychological Science, 13(6),513–519.

Fast,N.J.,&Tiedens,L.Z.(2010).Blamecontagion:Theautomatictransmissionofself­servingattributions.Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 46(1),97–106.

Felmlee,D.H. (1995). Fatal attractions: Affection and disaffection in intimate relation­ships.Journal of Social and Personal Relationships, 12(2),295–311.

Ferguson, M. J. (2005). Automatic evaluation as an implicit mechanism of goal pursuit.PaperpresentedattheSPSPAnnualMeeting,NewOrleans,LA.

Ferguson,M.J.,&Bargh,J.A.(2004).Likingisfordoing:Theeffectsofgoalpursuitonautomaticevaluation.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 87(5),557–572.

Fitzsimons,G.M.,&Bargh,J.A.(2003).Thinkingofyou:Nonconsciouspursuitofinter­personalgoalsassociatedwithrelationshippartners.JournalofPersonalityandSocialPsychology, 84(1),148–164.

Fitzsimons,G.M.,&Shah,J.Y.(2008).Howgoalinstrumentalityshapesrelationshipevalu­ations. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 95(2),319–337.

Forster, J.,Liberman,N.,&Higgins,E.T. (2005).Accessibility fromactiveand fulfilledgoals.Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 41,220–239.

Gilbert,D.T.,&Malone,P.S. (1995).Thecorrespondencebias.Psychological Bulletin, 117(1),21–38.

Gillath,O.,Mikulincer,M.,Fitzsimons,G.M.,Shaver,P.R.,Schachner,D.A.,&Bargh,J.A.(2006).Automaticactivationofattachment­relatedgoals.Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 32(10),1375–1388.

Hassin,R.R.,Aarts,H.,&Ferguson,M.J.(2005).Automaticgoalinferences.Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 41,129–140.

Jefferis,V.E.,&Chartrand,T.L.(2010).Aggression following failure at nonconscious goals.Unpublishedmanuscript,DukeUniversity,Durham,NC.

Kawada, C. L. K., Oettingen, G., Gollwitzer, P. M., & Bargh, J. A. (2004). The projec­tionofimplicitandexplicitgoals.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 86(4),545–559.

Kay,A.C.,Wheeler,S.C.,Bargh,J.A.,&Ross,L.(2004).Materialpriming:Theinflu­enceofmundanephysicalobjectsonsituationalconstrualandcompetitivebehav­ioralchoice.Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 95(1),83–96.

Kelly,G.A.(1955).A theory of personality: The psychology of personal constructs.NewYork:Norton.

Leander,N.P.,&Chartrand,T.L.(2010).On making a lasting impression: When behavioral mimicry elicits motivational conformity. Manuscriptinpreparation,DukeUniversity,Durham,NC.

Leander,N.P.,Chartrand,T.L.,&Wood,W.(2010).Mind your mannerisms: When mimicry elicits stereotype conformity.Manuscriptsubmittedforpublication,DukeUniversity,Durham,NC.

Leander,N.P.,&Shah,J.Y.(2010).Losing the will: Implicit regulatory reactions to the per-ceived indifference of others.Manuscriptsubmittedforpublication,DukeUniversity,Durham,NC.

Leander,N.P.,Shah,J.Y.,&Chartrand,T.L.(2009).Momentsofweakness:Theimplicitcontext­dependencies of temptations. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 35(7),853–866.

Page 117: the Psychology of Social Conflict and Aggression Sydney Symposium in Social Psychology

n. pontuS leander and tanya l. chartrand100

Leander,N.P.,Shah,J.Y.,&Chartrand,T.L.(2010).Pushing back pursuits: Regulatory resistance to the implicit motivational influence of others.Manuscriptsubmittedforpublication,DukeUniversity,Durham,NC.

Leander,N.P.,Shah,J.Y.,Chartrand,T.L.,&Fitzsimons,G.J.(2010).Feeling the pressures: Reactance motivation among peers in underage alcohol consumption.Manuscriptinpreparation,DukeUniversity,Durham,NC.

Leary,M.R.,Twenge,J.M.,&Quinlivan,W.(2006).InterpersonalRejectionasadeter­minant of anger and aggression. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 10(2),111–132.

Lewin,K.(1935).Adynamictheoryofpersonality:Selectedpapers(D.E.A.K.E.Zener,Trans.).NewYork:McGrawHill.

Lockwood,P.,&Kunda,Z.(1997).Superstarsandme:Predictingtheimpactofrolemodelsontheself.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 73,91–103.

Loersch,C.,Aarts,H.,Payne,B.K.,&Jefferis,V.E.(2008).Theinfluenceofsocialgroupsongoalcontagion.Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 44,1555–1558.

Maddux,W.W.,Mullen,E.,&Galinsky,A.D.(2008).Chameleonsbakebiggerpiesandtake bigger pieces: Strategic behavioral mimicry facilitates negotiation outcomes.Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 44,461–468.

Maner,J.K.,Kenrick,D.T.,Becker,D.V.,Robertson,T.E.,Hofer,B.,Neuberg,S.L.,etal.(2005).Functionalprojection:Howfundamentalsocialmotivescanbiasinterpersonalperception.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 88(1),63–78.

Moskowitz,G.B.,Gollwitzer,P.M.,Wasel,W.,&Schaal,B.(1999).Preconsciouscontrolof stereotypeactivation throughchronicegalitariangoals. Journal of Personality & Social Psychology, 77(1),167–184.

Ratelle,C.F.,Baldwin,M.W.,&Vallerand,R.J.(2005).Onthecuedactivationofsitu­ationalmotivation.Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 41,482–487.

Shah, J. Y. (2003). Automatic for the people: How representations of significant othersimplicitly affect goal pursuit. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 84(4),661–681.

Shah,J.Y.,Friedman,R.S.,&Kruglanski,A.W.(2002).Forgettingallelse:Ontheanteced­entsandconsequencesofgoalshielding.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 83(6),1261–1280.

Shah,J.Y.,&Kruglanski,A.W.(2002).Primingagainstyourwill:Howaccessiblealterna­tivesaffectgoalpursuit.Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 38, 368–383.

Sinclair,S.,Huntsinger,J.,Skorinko,J.,&Hardin,C.D.(2005).Socialtuningoftheself:Consequences for the self­evaluations of stereotype targets. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 89(2),160–175.

Spears, R., Gordijn, E., Dijksterhuis, A., & Stapel, D. A. (2004). Reaction in action:Intergroupcontrastinautomaticbehavior.Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 30(5),605–616.

Stapel,D.A.,&Koomen,W.(2005).Competition,cooperation,andtheeffectsofothersonme.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 88(6),1029–1038.

Tesser, A. (1988). Toward a self­evaluation maintenance model of social behavior. In L.Berkowitz(Ed.),Advances in Experimental Social Psychology(Vol.21,pp.181–227).SanDiego,CA:AcademicPress.

Tiedens,L.Z.,&Jimenez,M.C.(2003).Assimilationforaffiliationandcontrastforcontrol:Complementaryself­construals.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 85(6),1049–1061.

Todorov,A.,&Bargh,J.A.(2002).Automaticsourcesofaggression.Aggression and Violent Behavior, 7,53–68.

Page 118: the Psychology of Social Conflict and Aggression Sydney Symposium in Social Psychology

nonconSciouS BattleS of Will 101

Winter,L.,&Uleman,J.(1984).Whenaresocialjudgmentsmade?Evidenceforthespon­taneousnessoftraitinferences.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 47(2),237–252.

Zelli,A.,Huesmann,L.R.,&Cervone,D.(1995).Socialinferenceandindividualdiffer­ences in aggression: Evidence for spontaneous judgments of hostility. Aggressive Behavior, 21(6),405–417.

Page 119: the Psychology of Social Conflict and Aggression Sydney Symposium in Social Psychology
Page 120: the Psychology of Social Conflict and Aggression Sydney Symposium in Social Psychology

103

7UsingBothYourHead

andYourHeartThe Role of Perspective Taking and

Empathy in Resolving Social ConflictADAMD.GALINSKY

Northwestern University

DEBRAGILINSaint Mary’s University

WILLIAMW.MADDUXINSEAD

Ifyouknowtheenemyandknowyourself,youneednotfeartheresultofahundredbattles.Ifyouknowyourselfbutnottheenemy,foreveryvictorygainedyouwillalsosufferadefeat.

SunTzu

S uccessinstrategicconflictsituationsoftennecessitatesaclearunderstand­ingoftheunderlyingmotivesandlikelybehaviorsofone’sopponent.InTomClancy’sThe Hunt for Red October, for example, the captainof aSoviet

nuclear submarine enters U.S. waters and engages a new technology to avoiddetection.AlthoughU.S.militarycommanderssuspectheispreparingtoattacktheUnitedStates,aCentralIntelligenceAgency(CIA)analystnamedJackRyan

Page 121: the Psychology of Social Conflict and Aggression Sydney Symposium in Social Psychology

adam d. galinSky, deBra gilin, and William W. maddux104

isconvincedthat thecaptain isactually tryingtodefect.Theresultingstandoffmirrors a classic prisoner’s dilemma: Should the United States preventively useforce,ensuringashort­termvictory,ortrytheriskierbutpotentiallymorereward­ingrouteofmutualcooperation?Intheend,Ryanisprovedright:theU.S.mili­tarydelaystheattack,theSovietcommanderdoesswitchallegiance,andAmericagainsastalwartallyintheColdWar.

Ryan’sdeductioncameaboutbecausehehadthoroughlyresearchedhisoppo­nent’spersonalandmilitarybackgroundand thusclearlyunderstood theSovietcaptain’sdistastefortheSovietUnionandlikelydesiretodefect.Intherealworld,thesuccessfulresolutionoftheCubanmissilecrisishasbeencreditedtoPresidentJohn F. Kennedy’s ability to take the perspective of his Soviet counterpart. Byactively appreciating Soviet Premier Nikita Khrushchev’s core interests of sav­ing face and retaining power, Kennedy was able to devise a strategic plan thatsteeredthetwopowersawayfromtheprecipiceofnuclearwar,withoutsacrificingthe long­term interests of theUnited States. Whilepublicly refusing to removeanyofAmerica’smissilesplacedneartheSoviets(i.e.,noquidproquoonmissileremoval),KennedyofferedthatifallnuclearweaponswereremovedfromCubatheUnitedStateswouldpledgenottoinvadeCubainthefuture,termsthatsatis­fiedU.S. interestswhilealsoallowingKhrushchevtodeclare thathehadsavedCubafromattack.

Theseexamples,fromfictionandfact,illustratethepowerfuladvantageofhav­ingadeepunderstandingofone’sopponentinconflictsituationsthatcansome­times prevent escalation of the conflict to outright aggression. In disparate butrelateddomainssuchaschess,poker,andbusiness,knowingthemotivesandlikelybehaviorsofanadversarycanilluminatestrategiesthatwillbringaboutpersonalgain,thedownfallofone’snemesis(Findler,1990;Lopes,1976;Thagard,1992),andevenlong­termpeace(Axelrod,1987).Similarly,instrategicinteractionssuchasnegotiations,whichinvolveconflictinginterests,negotiatorsmustoftenunder­standandsatisfytheotherparty’sinterestsandneedstoobtainthebestoutcomeforthemselves(Thompson,1990;Thompson&Hastie,1990;Fisher,Ury,&Patton,1991). In contrast, close­mindedness is often the foundation for aggression andimpulsiveretaliation(Chapter10inthisvolume).Byunderstandinganadversary’sexplicitandimplicit interests,anticipatingtheirwordsandactions,andthinkingthroughways to structure solutions that satisfy their ownand theotherparty’sinterests,individualscandevelopcreativesolutionsthatreaptherewards—bothcompetitiveandcooperative—ofstrategicsocialinteractions.

Becauseunderstandinginterestsandmotivesisvaluableforcompetitivesuc­cess,itseemslikelythatindividualcharacteristicsassociatedwithunderstandingofandappreciationforotherindividualsmayproveadvantageousinstrategic,mixed­motives situations, suchasnegotiationsandconflictmanagement. Inparticular,tworelatedbutdistinctinterpersonalsocialcompetences—perspectivetakingandempathy—havebeenshowntomotivatesocialunderstandingacrossavarietyofcontexts.Althoughthetermsperspective takingandempathyareoftenusedinter­changeably,thereisclearevidenceoftheirdifferences(Coke,Batson,&McDavis,1978;Davis,1980,1983;Deutsch&Madle,1975;Hoffman,1977;Oswald,1996).Ontheonehand,perspectivetakingisacognitivecapacitytoconsidertheworld

Page 122: the Psychology of Social Conflict and Aggression Sydney Symposium in Social Psychology

uSing Both your head and your heart 105

fromotherviewpoints.It“allowsanindividualtoanticipatethebehaviorandreac­tionsofothers,thereforefacilitatingsmootherandmorerewardinginterpersonalrelationships” (Davis, 1983, p. 115). Empathy, in contrast, is an other­focusedemotional response that allows one person to affectively connect with another.Sometimeslabeledsympathyorcompassion,empathyisacongruentemotionofconcernexperiencedwhenwitnessinganotherperson’ssuffering(Batson,Fultz,&Schoenrade,1987).

Davis(1983,p.113)eloquentlydescribedthehistoricalrootsofthedistinc­tionbetweenperspective­takingandempathy:“Smith(1759)andSpencer(1870),writing centuries ago and a century apart, drew a nearly identical distinctionbetweentwobroadclassesofresponse:acognitive, intellectualreactionontheone hand (an ability simply to understand the other person’s perspective), andamorevisceral,emotionalreactiontotheother.”Althoughbothcharacteristicsare basic building blocks of social competence early in life (Piaget, 1932) andhavebroadsocialbenefitslaterinlife(Bengtsson&Johnson,1992;Davis,1983;Johnson,1975),wereviewtheextenttowhichitismorebeneficialtogetinsidethehead(perspectivetaking)versustheheart(empathy)ofone’spartnerinstrategic,mixed­motiveinteractions.

Inthecurrentchapter,weexaminethesetwoconstructsinmixed­motiveset­tingsthathaveexplicitimplicationsforourunderstandingofconflict.Wedescriberesearch examining the differential effects of perspective taking and empathyinnegotiationcontexts(Galinsky,Maddux,Gilin,&White,2008)aswellastheeffectsofperspectivetakingandempathyindifferenttypesofmixed­motivestra­tegicinteractions,suchaswargamesandsocialcoalitiongames(Gilin,Maddux,&Galinsky,2010).Allofthesestrategictasksinvolveanunderlyingconflictofinter­estbetweenselfandother,creatingopportunitiesformutualgainononehand,andforimpasse,conflictescalation,orlostopportunitiesontheotherhand.Eachthereforemirrorskeydynamicsofinterpersonalandintergroupconflict.

perSpective takingResearchonperspective takingsuggests it is avaluable social skill in threekeyways:socialcoordination,cognitiveflexibility,andassertiveness.First,perspectivetakingincreasesbehavioralmatchingandfacilitatessocialcoordination(Chartrand&Bargh,1999;Galinsky,Ku,&Wang,2005;Galinsky,Wang,&Ku,2008).Asearlyas1934,GeorgeMeadspeculatedthatconsideringothers’viewpointsallowsindividuals to anticipate others’ behavior and reactions, increasing their socialmaturity.Recentresearchhasdirectlysupportedthisidea,showingthatindividu­alshigheronperspective­takingabilityaremorelikelytomimicothers’nonverbalbehavior(Chartrand&Bargh,1999),whichinturnengendersliking(Chartrand&Bargh),goodwill(VanBaaren,Holland,Kawakami,&VanKnippenberg,2004),andassistance(VanBaarenetal.).Thus,asimultaneousgiveandtakeofgoodwillinsocialinteractionshelpsperspectivetakerscoordinatewithothers(Galinskyetal.,2005,2008).

Second, perspective taking involves cognitive flexibility. Perspective takersareabletostepoutsidetheconstraintsoftheirownimmediatebiasedframesof

Page 123: the Psychology of Social Conflict and Aggression Sydney Symposium in Social Psychology

adam d. galinSky, deBra gilin, and William W. maddux106

referenceandstepintoalternatemindsets.Perspective­takinginstructions(com­paredwithcontrolconditions)havereducedavarietyofbiases,includingthemyo­pictendencytobelieveoneismorehinderedbysituationalconstraintsthanothers(Moore,2005),toactuallyignoreothers’situationalconstraints(Regan&Totten,1975; Vescio, Sechrist, &Paolucci, 2003), and to rely on stereotypic and preju­dicialassumptionsaboutout­groupmembers(Galinsky&Ku,2004;Galinsky&Moskowitz,2000;Galinskyetal.,2006;Vescioetal.).Thisincreaseincognitiveflexibility leads togreater problem solving by perspective takers—the ability tocognitivelyswitchbetweendivergentviewpoints,eventhosewithwhichtheper­spectivetakerdisagrees(Richardson,Hammock,Smith,Gardner,&Signo,1994;Tetlock,Skitka,&Boettger,1989).Perspectivetakingseemstopromptanexternalvantagepoint,allowinganescapefromone’sownlimitingmentalsets.

Finally,perspective takersareassertive.Themostcommon laydefinitionofassertivenessis“standingupforlegitimatepersonalrights”(Wilson&Gallois,1993,p.48).Here,weadoptasimilardefinitionofassertivenessespousedbyTwenge(2001,p.134):atargeteduseoffirmnesstoprotectself­interestinresponsetooth­ers’aggressivetactics(seealsoChapter5inthisvolumeforadifferentdefinitionandperspective).Thus,perspectivetakersstrivetosatisfytheirownandothers’interestswithoutbeingoverlyconcessionary.Overall,dispositionallyhighperspec­tive takers are lower in their use of dominating conflict behaviors and chronicaggressionthanothers,relyingheavilyonjointproblemsolvinganddiscussioninthefaceofconflict(Richardsonetal.,1994).Theysuccessfullymaintain“mindovermatter”whenmildlyormoderatelyprovoked,resistingretaliationwhenitisunnec­essaryandcounterproductive(Richardson,Green,&Lago,1998;seeChapter4inthis volume).However,when facedwith an immediate threat to their interests,perspective takerswillaggressandretaliate. Inan intergroupcompetitiontask,perspectivetaking,bygaininganunderstandingthatthetwosides’positionsaretrulyincompatible,increasesappropriatelycompetitivebehavior(Johnson,1967).Similarly,perspectivetakersretaliateinresponsetoanunambiguous,strongprov­ocation (Richardsonetal.).We interpret these results tomean thatperspectivetakersavoidinitiatingaggressivebehavior(Richardsonetal.)andstrivetosatisfytheirownandothers’interestsbutwillnotbackdowninthefaceofclearcompeti­tivecontingenciesorastrongattack.Thispatternofresponsesindicatesastrategysimilartothatofreciprocalaltruism(Trivers,1971),ahighlysuccessfulstrategyinmixed­motiveenvironments(Axelrod,1984)inwhichapreferenceforcooperationisbalancedwithamechanismforretaliationfollowingexploitation.

Adaptiveflexibilityandassertivenessembedded incoordinated social interac­tion shouldhelpperspective takers achievegreatergains at thebargaining table.Perspectivetakinghasbeenlinkedtogreateruseofjointproblemsolvinganddiscus­sionduringinterpersonalconflict(Richardsonetal.,1994),behaviorsthathaveinde­pendentlybeenshowntobeassociatedwithpositivenegotiationsoutcomes.Sinceappreciatingdifferent interests is essential forfindingwin–winsolutions innego­tiations (Thompson&Hastie,1990),negotiators’perspective­takingabilitieshaveproventoprovidesomebenefitincraftingintegrativedeals(Kemp&Smith,1994).

Inadditiontojointandintegrativegains,perspectivetakingshouldenhancedis­tributivegainsinnegotiation.Forexample,waitresseswhomimictheircustomers

Page 124: the Psychology of Social Conflict and Aggression Sydney Symposium in Social Psychology

uSing Both your head and your heart 107

receivebiggertips(VanBaaren,Holland,Steenaert,&VanKnippenberg,2003).Notsurprisinglythen,perspectivetakerselicitgreaterconcessionsfromanoppos­ing negotiator (Neale &Bazerman,1982) and can protect themselves from theanchoringeffectsofanopponent’sfirstoffer(Galinsky&Mussweiler,2001).

Thus, perspective taking allows advantages in adapting to others, and weproposethishelpsperspectivetakersdiscoverunderlyingcommoninterestswithan adversary when they are not obvious. Yet perspective takers also seem toprotecttheirownturf,beingassertiveindefendingtheirowninterestsandturn­ing aggressive when necessary and improving others’ outcomes only so far astheirown interests arenot sacrificed.We thereforeexpected thatperspectivetakingwould improve individualand jointoutcomes inmixed­motivestrategicinteractions.

empathyWhereasperspectivetakingisprimarilyacognitiveability,empathyisprimarilyanaffectivestateofconcernforothers(Davis,1983)andincludes“feelingsthataremoreother­focusedthanself­focused”(Batsonetal.,1987,p.2).Empathydoesnotcorrelatewiththesameconstellationofpersonalitycharacteristicsasperspectivetaking.Empathypredictsmoreintenseexperienceofemotions(Davis;Eisenberget al., 1994; Okun, Shepard, & Eisenberg, 2000), greater sensitivity to others(Davis),andhelpingothersevenatone’sownexpense(seereviewsinBatson,1991;Batson & Oleson, 1991). Empathy benefits others by prompting prosocial help­ing(Archer,Diaz­Loving,Gollwitzer,Davis,&Foushee,1981;Batson,O’Quinn,Fultz,Vanderplas,&Isen,1983;Batsonetal.,1987;Cokeetal.,1978).Forinstance,empathizersvolunteermoretimetohelpotherscomparedwithperspectivetakers(Oswald,1996),andempathyresultsinparticipantsassigninganunknownpartneramoredesirabletaskwhileacceptingalessdesirabletaskforthemselves(Batsonetal.,2003).Assuch,empathyisahighlyeffectivemeansof inducingconsider­ationandhelpingofothers.However,empathy­inducedhelpingisoftendoneattheexpenseofone’sownconcerns.Allocationdecisionsbyempathizerscanbesoother­servingastoharmone’sself­interest(Batsonetal.).

Instrategicinteractions,empathycansimilarlyresultinanoverconsiderationofanadversary.Forexample,empathizerstendtocooperateinprisoner’sdilemmagames(Batson&Moran,1999),eveniftheyknowthattheiropponenthasprevi­ouslydefectedand therefore cooperation is likely to be to theirown detriment(Batson&Ahmad,2001).Lendingsupporttotheideathatempathymaynotbeanassetbutaliabilityinnegotiationsisevidenceshowingthatagreeablenessisassoci­atedwithworsedistributiveoutcomes(Barry&Friedman,1998).Empathyisasso­ciatedwithincreasedperceivedclosenessormentalmergingwithothers(Cialdini,Brown,Lewis,Luce,&Neuberg,1997).Althoughthis“oneness”mayexpandtheboundaries of the self to include the other, a healthy appreciation of differentpriorities necessary to create win–win outcomes may not result from empathicconcern. In addition, close personal relationships increase attention to others’outcomes (Sally, 2000) but reduce concentration on economicgain (Ligthart&Lindenberg,1994).Asaresult,romanticpartners,comparedwithstrangers,arrive

Page 125: the Psychology of Social Conflict and Aggression Sydney Symposium in Social Psychology

adam d. galinSky, deBra gilin, and William W. maddux108

atlessintegrativeoutcomesbecausetheysetloweraspirations,makefeweroffers,andengageinlessassertivebehavior(Fry,Firestone,&Williams,1983).Similarly,friendsareoftenlessassertivebecauseofagreaterconcernwithmaintainingtherelationship(Peterson&Thompson,1997).

Becauseempathycreatessuchastrongother­focus,empathizersmaybeunabletoenlargethenegotiationpieortoclaimashareofthepieforthemselves.Becauseempathymotivatesalowfocusonprotectingone’sowninterestsandproducesapassivestance,weexpectedempathytoleadtolessefficientintegrationofinterestsandafailuretoclaimafairshareofindividualgainsatthenegotiationtable.

the dual concern model: predicting the effectS of perSpective taking and empathy

TheDualConcernModelofnegotiationsprovidesausefulframeworkformakingclearpredictionsabout the relativeadvantagesofperspective takingandempa­thyinstrategic,mixed­motiveinteractions.AccordingtotheDualConcernModel(Pruitt&Rubin,1986),negotiatorscanchoosetodividetheirattentionbetweenthemselvesandtheotherside:Negotiatorscanbeattentiveonlytotheirowncon­cerns,onlywiththeconcernsoftheotherside,orhaveamixofattentionfocusedonselfandotherconcerns.Whenattentionisfocusedonlyonself­interests,nego­tiators tend to be overly aggressive, displaying obstinate behavior designed toincreaseindividualordistributivegainsattheother’sexpense.However,focusingonlyontheinterestsofothersencouragesself­destructiveandspinelessconcessionmaking.Instead,abalanceofattentiontobothself­interestsandtheinterestsofothersandconcernsfacilitatescreativeproblemsolving.Assuch,effectivenego­tiatorsmustfindatenuousbalancebetweenfacilitatingpositiveandcooperativeinteractionswithinacompetitiveandoftendistrustfulenvironment; thisneces­sarybalancingofcompetitionandcooperationhasbeendubbedthe“negotiator’sdilemma”(Lax&Sebenius,1986).

Extrapolatingfromthismodel,empathycouldtipthebalanceofattentiontoofartowardcooperationandtheotherside’sconcerns,leadingnegotiatorstosacri­ficeself­interestand,bynotpushingone’sowninterests,evenpreventingnegotia­torsfromdiscoveringinsightsthatcouldbenefitbothsides.Incontrast,perspectivetakingmayleadtoamorebalancedfocusonappreciatingothers’interestswithoutforfeiting one’s own claims and therefore produce beneficial outcomes for boththe self and other. Indeed,AdamSmith (1759) suggested inhisworkon moralsentimentsthatperspectivetakingwasmoreessentialthanempathyinachievingefficientoutcomes,thatlookingatthingsfromanoutsideperspectiveallowsindi­vidualstooverridepassionssuchasexcessivesympathythatcanimpairinsightandcreativity.Overall,wepredictthatperspectivetakingwillenhancebothjointandindividualoutcomesinnegotiations,whereasempathymayleadtodisadvantagesonboththesefronts.

However, we believe there are important mixed­motive situations in whichempathizersshouldhaveamarkedadvantage,suchascoalitionbuilding,inwhichthereisabenefittorecognizingsubtleemotionalreactionsthatarediagnosticof

Page 126: the Psychology of Social Conflict and Aggression Sydney Symposium in Social Psychology

uSing Both your head and your heart 109

other’semotionalconnectionswithoneself.Thatis,weexpectthatempathicindi­vidualswillreadothers’interpersonalcuesmoreaccurately.Asaresult, intaskswheresuchunderstandingconfersastrategicadvantage,affectiveaccuracymightleadtoaperformanceadvantageofempathyoverperspectivetaking.

perSpective takerS are Better negotiatorSGalinsky et al. (2008) both measured and manipulated perspective taking andempathytoexploretheirinfluenceintwonegotiationtasksthatrepresentcommonandchallengingbarrierstounderstanding:(1)compatibilityofunderlyinginterestsinthefaceofconflictingpositions(Studies1and2);and(2)differingpreferencesand priorities (Study3). Indeed, perceivingone hasdissimilarpersonal goals isoftenthefoundationofincreasedratesoffightingincloserelationships(Chapter12 in this volume). These twobarriers to mutualunderstanding areunderlyingcontributorstomostinterpersonalconflicts.Theysoughttoanswerthefollowingquestion:Forindividualsinvolvedinsuchmixed­motivesituations,isitmoreeffec­tivetoempathizewithanopponent(havetheminsideyourheart)ortounderstandtheirthoughtsandperspective(getinsidetheirhead)?

Twoof theirstudiesusedanegotiationoverthesaleofthe“Texoil”gassta­tion (Goldberg, 2008), where a deal based solely on sale price was impossible.Specifically,thebuyer’sreservationprice(themaximumheorshewasauthorizedtopay)waslowerthantheseller’sreservationprice(theminimumheorshewaswillingtoaccept),resultinginanegativebargainingzoneforsaleprice.However,bothparties’underlyinginterestswerecompatible:Thebuyerwantedtohireman­agerstorunthestation,andthesellerneededhelpfinancingasailboattripandtoobtainemploymentafterreturning.Thus,partiescouldagreetoasalepricebelowtheseller’sreservationprice,butwithastipulationoffutureemployment.Toreachasuccessfuldeal,participantshadtodiscoverthisalternativesolutionthemselvesduringthecourseofthenegotiation.

Inthisstudy,dyadiclevelsofperspectivetakingandempathy(controllingforthe Big Five traits, and gender) predicted the likelihood of negotiating a deal.However,onlydyads’perspective­takingtendenciesactedasasignificantpositivepredictorofwhetherasuccessfuldealwasreached.Incontrasttherewasanega­tiverelationshipbetweenempathyanddealdiscovery.Follow­upanalysesattheindividuallevelfoundthatonlythebuyer’schronicperspectivetakingsignificantlypredictedwhetheradealwasreached,whereasforsellersonlytheiropennesstoexperiencesignificantlypredictedwhetheradealwasreached.

Inotherwords,perspective­takingtendencies(particularlyinthebuyer)helpednegotiatorsovercometheirapparentlyconflictingpositionsandgenerateacreativeresolutiontoamutualproblemthatmetbothparties’needs.Empathy,incontrast,proveddetrimentaltodiscoveringasolution.Importantly,theadvantagesofper­spectivetakingwereindependentoftheBigFivepersonalityvariables,providingdiscriminatevalidityforitsroleinnegotiations.

Inthisresearchonlythebuyer’sperspective­takingtendencymadeadifferenceinproducinga deal.However, this reasoning is consistentwith recent researchshowing the importance of the buyer’s role in soliciting information in this gas

Page 127: the Psychology of Social Conflict and Aggression Sydney Symposium in Social Psychology

adam d. galinSky, deBra gilin, and William W. maddux110

stationnegotiation(Maddux,Mullen,&Galinsky,2008).Althoughthesellerneedsto reveal personal information (not surprisingly, the seller’s openness toexperi­encematteredinthecurrentnegotiation),adealcannotbeachievedunlessthebuyerplays anactive role in soliciting andappreciating thevalueof the seller’sdisclosuresincraftingasolution.Thus,onlythebuyer’sperspective­takingabilitypredictedcreativeproblemresolution.

AsecondstudymanipulatedtheperspectivetakingandempathyofthebuyerinthesameTexoilnegotiation.Buyersintheempathyconditionweregiventhefol­lowinginstructions:“Inpreparingforthenegotiationandduringthenegotiation,taketheperspectiveoftheservicestationowner.Try tounderstandwhatheorsheisfeeling,whatemotionsheorshemaybeexperiencinginsellingthestation.Trytoimaginewhatyouwouldbefeeling inthatrole.”Buyersintheperspective­taking condition were told the following: “In preparing for the negotiation andduringthenegotiation,taketheperspectiveoftheservicestationowner.Trytounderstandwhatheorsheisthinking,whathisorherinterests andpurposesareinsellingthestation.Trytoimaginewhatyouwouldbethinking inthatrole.”Theresultsreplicatedtheoverallpatternfromthecorrelationalstudy:perspectivetak­ers achieved significantly moredeals thanempathizers andcontrolparticipants(whodidnotdifferfromeachother),overcomingseeminglyconflictinginterests.

Thisstudyalsomeasuredanotheroutcomewithimplicationsforconflictresolu­tion:theseller’ssatisfactionwithhowheorshefelttreatedduringthenegotiation.Hereempathyprovedadvantageous;beingempathizedwithledtothehighestlevelofinterpersonalsatisfaction.Althoughperspectivetakersinspiredsignificantlylesssatisfactionthandidempathizers,theystillproducedsignificantlymoresatisfactionthancontrolparticipants.Thus,althoughempathyhadimmediateaffectivebenefitsfortheotherside,empathizersdidnothaveanadvantageovercontrolparticipantsinproducingmoredeals,whichwouldprovidelong­termvalueforthemselvesandtheiropponentandresolvetheirconflictofinterest.Incontrast,perspectivetakerssecuredthemostagreementswithsufficientopponentsatisfaction.

Galinskyetal. (2008)nextexaminedwhetherperspective takingandempa­thywouldhelpnegotiatorsnavigatemulti­issuenegotiations.Wheneveranegotia­tioninvolvesmultipleissues,negotiatorscanhavedifferentpriorities;negotiatorscanimprovetheiroutcomesbyconcedingonlow­priority issues inexchangefortheirhigh­priorityones,a techniquecalled logrolling (Froman&Cohen,1970).Logrollingisanexcellentconflictresolutiontool,evenincloseandongoingrela­tionships,becauseitallowsthepartiestotradeoff“wins”onissuesonwhichtheywantopposite things for the sakeof theoverall dealor relationship (Sheppard,1999).Merecompromise,orsimply“splitting”all issuesdownthemiddle, isanimpediment to reaching efficient agreements compared with making mutuallybeneficialtrade­offs(Thompson,1990;2001;Tripp&Sondak,1992).Multi­issuenegotiationsalsohighlightadilemmanegotiatorsface:findingabalancebetweencapturingvalueforoneself(value­claiming)andmaximizingtheavailableresourcesforbothparties(value­creating;Lax&Sebenius,1986).Tobemosteffective,nego­tiatorsmustbothcreateaslargeapieofresourcesaspossible(toproducethemosteconomicallyefficientagreements)andalsoclaimasmuchofthatpieaspossible(to satisfy their self­interest). In the context of ongoing business and personal

Page 128: the Psychology of Social Conflict and Aggression Sydney Symposium in Social Psychology

uSing Both your head and your heart 111

relationships, soundnegotiationdeals thatcreatea lotofvalueanddistribute itfairlyshouldalsohelppreventfuturedisputes(Sheppard,1999).

Inamulti­issuenegotiationGalinskyetal.(2008)foundthattakingtheper­spectiveofone’sopponentproducedboththegreatestamountofjointgainsandmoreprofitableindividualoutcomes.Perspectivetakersachievedthehighestlevelofeconomicefficiencywithout sacrificing theirownmaterialgains. In contrast,empathizersreceivedthelowestindividualoutcomes,withincreasesinjointgainsgoingmostlytotheempathizer’sopponent(seealsoChapter14inthisvolumeforwhenforgivenesscanerodeself­respect).Interestingly,itappearsintheirstudiesthat the negotiator who would achieve the best individual outcome is one whotakestheperspectiveofanempathizingopponent,suggestingthatnegotiationout­comesmaybedrivenbytheinteractionbetweenthesetwosocialcompetencies.

Overall,theinitialstudiesbyGalinskyetal.(2008)suggestthatitisbetterto“thinklike”ratherthanto“feelfor”one’sadversaries.Inotherwords,itismorebeneficialtogetinsidetheirheadthanhavetheminsideone’sownheart.

perSpective takerS navigate mixed-motive SituationS BecauSe they are more accurate

in predicting their opponent’S moveSTheprevious studies focused on negotiations and not conflictper se. However,theresultsarehighlysuggestiveofperformanceinconflict­relatedsettings.TheTexoilexerciseinparticularpresentsaclassicdilemmainresolvinginterpersonalconflicts.Whenanobvioussolutionisnotpossible(inthiscase,amutuallybenefi­cialprice),partiesoftenbecomeangryandfrustrated.Thekeytoasolutionandtopreventillwillistodiscovermutuallycompatibleunderlyinginterests.Indeed,researchershaverepeatedlyshownthatidentifyingoverlappinginterestsiscriticalinsolvinginterpersonalconflictsaswellasnegotiations(Brett,2007;Ury,Brett,&Goldberg,1988).Thus,theextenttowhichperspectivetakingandempathypredicttheabilitytoidentifybothparties’underlyinginterestsshouldlikelyapplyacrossanarrayofmixed­motivecontexts,includingsituationsinvolvingbargainingaswellasthoseinvolvinginterpersonalorstrategicdisputes.

Toseeiftheirfindingsgeneralizedtosettingsinvolvingamoreexplicitdegreeof conflict, Gilin, Maddux, & Galinsky (2010) followed up these initial studiesbyexploring thedifferentialeffectsofperspective takingandempathy inothertypesofmixed­motivesettings.Theypredicted thatperspective taking(butnotempathy)wouldprovideanaccurateunderstandingoftheopponents’strategyandinterestsincognitivelybasedcompetitiveinteractions.Incontrast,theypredictedthatempathywouldaidperformanceincompetitivetasksrequiringanemotionalappreciationofandconnectionwiththeotherside.

Theirfirststudyusedasimulated“wargame”thatinvolvedmultipleroundsofapotentialarmsracewithan“enemy”country.Thistasktendstogeneraterealisticemotionalconflictresponses, suchasanger, frustration,andadesireforretalia­tioninthefaceofattack,eventhoughitoccursoverarelativelybriefinteractionperiod.Participantshadtomakerepeateddecisionsaboutwhethertodisarmone’s

Page 129: the Psychology of Social Conflict and Aggression Sydney Symposium in Social Psychology

adam d. galinSky, deBra gilin, and William W. maddux112

bombsortousethemtobombtheopponent.Gamesendedassoonasoneoftwooutcomesoccurred:either(1)attack:oneopponentbombedtheother;or(2)peace:10bomb­free(peaceful)roundswereconcluded.Also,atvariouspoints,face­to­facenegotiationsweremandatorysoeachplayerwasabletocommunicatedirectlywithhisorheropponent.Therearetworoadstosuccessinthisgame.First,onepotentiallywinningstrategyistodisarmfewerweaponsthanone’sadversaryandthenattack.Second,ifneitherplayerattackedinthe10roundsofagame,“peace”was declared, and payments were paid out by a neutral third party, the WorldBank.Partieswerethenrewardedaccordingtotheextenttowhichtheymetorsurpassedthegoalof50%disarmament.Successatthisgameinvolvesusingcom­plexcognitivestrategy,anticipatingthestrategyoftheopponent,persuadingtheopponenttomakemutuallybeneficialmoves,andavoidinggratuitousretaliationthatwillescalatedistrust,valuedestruction,andstalematewithapartner.Inotherwords,thetaskmodelsreal­worldstrategicconflictsituationsinwhichonemustkeepone’sangerandfrustrationinchecktoavoidacycleofincreasingprovocationandaggression.

Gilin et al. (2010) tested how individual differences in perspective takingandempathypredictedindividualprofit,jointgainprovidedbytheWorldBank,andthepercentoftotalgamesinwhichthedyadachievedpeace(bothpartiescooperatedthroughall10roundswithoutattack).Withregardtojointgainandattainmentofpeace, thepatternor resultswerevery similar to those found inthepreviouslydescribedTexoilstudy.Dyadic­levelperspectivetakingwasassoci­atedwithdyadsreachingpeacemorefrequently(i.e.,moreoftheirgamesendedwithouta“bombattack”),butdyadicempathyactuallypredictedasignificantlylowerpercentageofpeacefulsolutions.Inaddition,theamountofjointintegra­tive gains resulting from peaceful resolutions (reward money from the “WorldBank”),indicatingtheextentofcooperationinpeacefulgames,waspositivelyandsignificantlyrelatedtodyadicperspectivetakingbutnegativelyandsignificantlyrelatedtodyadicempathy.

This pattern of results suggests that, perhaps surprisingly, there was moreretaliationwhenthecollectiveempathyamongtheadversarieswashigh.Researchshows that regulating anger is a key factor in successfully navigating conflicts(Chapter9inthisvolume).Game­by­gameanalysesprovidesomeinsightintohowperspectivetakerswereabletosucceed:Thosehigheronperspectivetakingnotonlydisarmedtheirownarsenalstoagreaterextentbutalsowereabletoconvincetheiropponentstodothesameandtherebycreate jointgain.Incontrast,high­empathyindividualsanddyadshadmoreaggressiveattacksandfewersuccessfullycooperativeinteractions.High­empathydyadsseemedtogetlockedinspiralsofescalatingconflictinvolvingattackandcounterattack.Wespeculatethatthismaybeafunctionofthegreateremotionalityofempathicindividuals.Perhapsunderdirectthreatandattack,thisemotionalitycanleadtobeing“carriedaway”byangerorspite, leadingtocounterproductiveconflictescalation(Pillutla&Murnighan,1996).With regard to individual gains,higher levelsofperspective takingwererelatedtosignificantlygreaterprofit,whereasempathypredictedfewerindividualprofits.Thus,perspectivetakersnotonlyachievedpeacebutalsosecuredhigherprofitsforthemselves.

Page 130: the Psychology of Social Conflict and Aggression Sydney Symposium in Social Psychology

uSing Both your head and your heart 113

AsecondstudybyGilinetal. (2010) testedwhetherperspective takingandempathywouldpredictperformanceinasimpleultimatumbargaininggameandwhetheraccurate inferencesabout theopponent’sstrategywouldmediate theseeffects.Anultimatumbargaininggameinvolvestworoles:a“Proposer,”whoseroleistomakeasingleofferofapoolofresources(e.g.,$10)toaResponder,whomustsimplyacceptorrejecttheoffer(Pillutla&Murnighan,1995).AcceptancemeanspartieskeepthedistributionofmoneyofferedbytheProposer;rejectionmeansthatneitherpartyreceivesanyamountoftheresource.

Theultimatum task thereforemodelsa simpleconflict inwhichoneoppo­nenthasdecisionpowerwhile theotherhasthepowertorespondtoandpro­tectagainstpotentiallyunfairdecisions.Itprovidesanidealcontextfortestingwhethercognitive understanding of thepartner’sperspectivedrives successfuloutcomesbecausetheProposer’soutcomelargelydependsonparticipants’per­ceptions of the likely responses of the other person. For example, a Proposerwhocompletelydisregardswhattheopponentmightviewasafavorableorfairoutcome(i.e.,bytakingmostofthemoneyforhimself)islikelytohavetheofferrejectedandwindupwithnomoneyatall.Ontheotherhand,choosinganofferthat the responder perceives as fair will result in an accepted agreement andmoney for both sides. But this is a classic mixed­motive situation because thesender wants to send as little money as possible that will still be accepted tomaximizehisorherowngain.

Intheirstudy,onlytheperspectivetakingbutnottheempathyoftheProposerpredictedbothwhetheraResponderacceptedanofferandtheamountofmoneysecured.Inaddition,theProposerswhowerehigheronperspectivetakingweremoreaccurateindeterminingwhethertheiropponentwouldaccepttheirultima­tumoffersandthisaccuracymediatedtheirabilitytosecureacceptances.

empathizerS are more effective in predicting emotional connectionS

Allofthestudiesdescribedinthischaptersuggestthatperspectivetakingissupe­rior to empathy in mixed­motive settings: perspective takers can get inside theheadofopponentsandunderstandtheir interestsandprioritiesandcanpredictand influence their likelybehavior.However, in some strategic interactions andcoalition–formationsituations,thekeyisdeterminingwithwhomonehasemotion­allyoraffectivelyconnected.Totestthishypothesis,Gilinetal.(2010)designedathree­personsocialcoalitiongameinwhichparticipantscouldwinacashprizebutonlyiftheyselectedastheircoalitionpartnersomeonewhoalsosimultaneouslychosethem(ratherthanathirdperson).However,participantswerenottoldtheywereplayingastrategiccoalitiongameuntilafteranintroductorysessioninwhichtheygottoknowthetwootherparticipants.Thegamewasthereforenotprimarilyacognitivetask,butratheramoreintuitiveandaffectivetaskinwhichparticipantsneededtoretroactivelyassesstheirsocialconnectionswithothersduringthepre­viousinteraction;successdependedonhavinggatheredthecorrectsenseoftheirpreviousemotionalconnectionwithothers.

Page 131: the Psychology of Social Conflict and Aggression Sydney Symposium in Social Psychology

adam d. galinSky, deBra gilin, and William W. maddux114

Inthisgame,empathywasabenefittobothwinningthegameandaccuracyabout the other players’ game choices. In contrast, perspective taking was notassociatedwitheitherwinningoraccuracy,althoughperspectivetakingwasneverdetrimental toeitheroutcome. In this study, empathic tendencies conferred anadvantage, predicting successful coalition building. These results indicate thatempathy can be an asset when conflicts are fundamentally based on affective,interpersonal connections, suchas interpersonal disputes with spouses, friends,family,andcolleagues.

concluSionSWehave reviewedanumberof studiesexploring the roleof twosocial compe­tencies—perspectivetakingandempathy—inpredictingsuccessinmixed­motivesituations that model key underlying characteristics of conflict. In negotiations,wargames,andultimatumgames,wheresuccessistypicallyachievedbyunder­standing the likelystrategicmovesofone’sopponent,perspective takingprovesadvantageous.Perspectivetakersarebetterabletouncoverunderlyingintereststogeneratecreativesolutionsandtocraftmoreefficientdealsinmulti­issuenegotia­tions.Theyarealsobetterabletoanticipateandsteerlikelybehaviorinwar­likeclashes, leadingthemtoachievehigherlevelsofpeace.Notonlydoperspectivetakersreachagreementsandpeacewhilemaximizingthesizeofthebargainingpie, but they also take nice big pieces of that pie, garnering the highest levelsof individualprofit.TheyseemtotakeSunTzu’sexhortationthattoknowone’senemyistheroadtosuccessonthebattlefield.

However,whensuccessinastrategicinteractionrequiresdeterminingifyoupsy­chologicallyconnectwithanotherperson,empathizersweremorelikelytowininacoalition­formationgame.Theseresultssuggestthatempathycanpromotenotjustclosenesstoothersbutalsoanaccurateassessmentofinterpersonalconnection,lead­ingtosuccessinstrategictasksthatrequireaffectiveunderstanding.

Overall,thesestudiessuggestthatperspectivetakingandempathycaneachpromotesuccessfulresolutionofcompetitiveinteractionsdependingonthetypeofconflict.Ingeneral,theidentificationofmutuallycompatibleunderlyinginterests,akeyconflictresolutionstrategy,seemstobeattheheartofperspective­takingability,andwefoundanadvantageforperspectivetakerswheninteractions(e.g.,negotiations, war games) required an appreciation of counterparts’ underlyinginterests.Ontheotherhand,conflictsinvolvingmoreaffective,interpersonaldis­putesmaybebestapproachedwithanempatheticmindset.

At thesametime,however,weacknowledge thatmanyreal­worldconflictsinvolvebothaffectiveandcognitiveelements,andwebelievethathavingbothskills is likelyuseful: empathy for appreciating anddiffusing the affectiveele­ments that aredefining featuresof conflict; andperspective taking fordeduc­ingcompatibleinterestsandpossiblesolutionstoachieveanenduringresolution.Indeed, in our opening examples, both Jack Ryan and President Kennedy notonly identifiedkeyunderlying interestsof theirSovietcounterparts that ledtowin–win outcomes but were also able to diffuse heated interpersonal tensionsamongtheirowndecision­makingteams(inbothcases,governmentandmilitary

Page 132: the Psychology of Social Conflict and Aggression Sydney Symposium in Social Psychology

uSing Both your head and your heart 115

officialswhopushedforamilitaryresponse)togettheirproposed,peacefulsolu­tionsimplemented.Thus,althoughfutureresearchisneededforempiricalcon­firmation,webelievethereislikelyanidealbalancebetweenbothmindsetsinmanyconflictsituations,suggestingperhapsthattheidealistostriveforsome­thinglikeAristotle’s“goldenmean.”Indecidingwhenandhowbesttouseone’sheadandone’sheartinconflictsituationsalittleofbothmaygoalongway.LikethefamouscharactersfromThe Wizard of Oz,successfulresolutionofconflictsrequiresbrains,aheart,andalittlecourage.

referenceS

Archer,R.L.,Diaz­Loving,R.,Gollwitzer,P.M.,Davis,M.H.,&Foushee,H.C.(1981).Theroleofdispositionalempathyandsocialevaluationintheempathicmediationofhelping.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 40, 786–796.

Axelrod,R.(1984).The evolution of cooperation. NewYork:BasicBooks.Axelrod,R.(1987).Theevolutionofstrategiesintheiteratedprisoners’dilemma.InL.Davis

(Ed.),Genetic algorithms and simulated annealing(pp.32–42).LosAltos,CA:MorganKaufmann.

Barry,B.,&Friedman,R.(1998).Bargainercharacteristicsindistributiveandintegrativenegotiation:Theroleofpersonalityandcognitiveability.Journal of Personality and SocialPsychology,74,345–359.

Batson,C.D.,&Ahmad,N.(2001).Empathy­inducedaltruisminaprisoner’sdilemmaII:Whatifthetargetofempathyhasdefected?European Journal of Social Psychology, 31,25–36.

Batson,C.D.,Fultz,J.,&Schoenrade,P.A.(1987).Distressandempathy:Twoqualita­tivelydistinctvicariousemotionswithdifferentmotivationalconsequences.Journal of Personality, 55(1),19–39.

Batson, C. D., & Moran, T. (1999). Empathy­induced altruism in a prisoner’s dilemma.European Journal of Social Psychology, 29, 909–924.

Batson,C.D.,O’Quinn,K.,Fultz,J.,Vanderplas,M.,&Isen,A.(1983).Influenceofself­reportdistressandempathyonegoisticversusaltruisticmotivationtohelp.Journal of Personality and SocialPsychology, 45, 706–718.

Batson,C.D.,Lishner,D.A.,Carpenter,A.,Dulin,L.,Harjusola­Webb,S.,Stocks,E.L.,etal.(2003).Asyouwouldhavethemdountoyou:Doesimaginingyourselfintheother’splacestimulatemoralaction?Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 29,1190–1201.

Bengtsson,H.,&Johnson,L.(1992).Perspectivetaking,empathy,andprosocialbehaviorinlatechildhood.Child Study Journal,22, 11–22.

Brett,J.M.(2007).Negotiating globally: How to negotiate deals, resolve disputes, and make decisions across cultural boundaries.SanFrancisco:Jossey­Bass.

Chartrand,T.L.,&Bargh,J.A.(1999).Thechameleoneffect:Theperception­behaviorlinkandsocialinteraction.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 76,893–910.

Cialdini,R.B.,Brown,S.L.,Lewis,B.P.,Luce,C.,&Neuberg,S.L.(1997).Reinterpretingthe empathy­altruism relationship: When one into one equals oneness. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 73,481–494.

Coke,J.S.,Batson,C.D.,&McDavis,K.(1978).Empathicmediationofhelping:Atwo­stagemodel.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 36,752–766.

Davis,M.(1980).Amultidimensionalapproachtoindividualdifferencesinempathy.JSAS Catalog of Selected Documents in Psychology, 10,85.

Page 133: the Psychology of Social Conflict and Aggression Sydney Symposium in Social Psychology

adam d. galinSky, deBra gilin, and William W. maddux116

Davis,M.(1983).Theeffectsofdispositionalempathyonemotionalreactionsandhelping:Amultidimensionalapproach.Journal of Personality, 51,167–184.

Deutsch, F., & Madle, R. A. (1975). Empathy: Historic and current conceptualizations,measurement, and a cognitive theoretical perspective. Human Development, 18,267–287.

Eisenberg,N.,Fabes,R.A.,Murphy,B.,Karbon,M.,Maszk,P.,Smith,M.,etal.(1994).Therelationsofemotionalityandregulationtodispositionalandsituationalempathy­relatedresponding.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 66,776–797.

Findler, N. V. (1990). Contributions to a computer-based theory of strategies. NewYork:Springer­Verlag.

Fisher,R.,Ury,W.,&Patton,B.(1991).Getting to yes: Negotiating agreement without giv-ing in (2nded.).NewYork:Penguin.

Froman,L.A.,Jr.,&Cohen,M.D.(1970).Compromiseandlogroll:Comparingtheeffi­ciencyoftwobargainingprocesses.Behavioral Science, 15,180–183.

Fry,W.R.,Firestone,I.J.,&Williams,D.L.(1983).Negotiationprocessandoutcomeofstrangerdyadsanddatingcouples:Doloverslose?Basic & Applied Social Psychology, 4,1–16.

Galinsky, A. D., & Ku, G. (2004). The effects of perspective­taking on prejudice: Themoderating role of self­evaluation. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 30,594–604.

Galinsky, A. D., Ku, G., & Wang, C. S. (2005). Perspective­taking and self­other over­lap: Fostering social bonds and facilitating social coordination. Group Processes & Intergroup Relations, 8,109–124.

Galinsky,A.D.,Magee,J.C.,Inesi,M.E.,Gruenfeld,D.H.(2006).Powerandperspectivesnottaken.Psychological Science, 17,1068–1074.

Galinsky,A.D.,Maddux,W.W.,Gilin,D.,&White,J.B.(2008).Whyitpaystogetinsidetheheadofyouropponent:Thedifferentialeffectsofperspectivetakingandempathyinnegotiations.Psychological Science,19,378–384.

Galinsky, A.D., & Moskowitz, G.B. (2000). Perspective­taking: Decreasing stereotypeexpression, stereotype accessibility, and in­group favoritism. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 78,708–724.

Galinsky,A.D.,&Mussweiler,T.(2001).Firstoffersasanchors:Theroleofperspective­tak­ingandnegotiatorfocus.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 81,657–669.

Galinsky,A.D.,Wang,C.S.,&Ku,G.(2008).Perspective­takersbehavemorestereotypi­cally.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 95,404–419.

Gilin,D.,&Maddux,W.W.,&Galinsky,A.D.(2010).When to use your head, and when to use your heart: The differential value of perspective taking versus empathy in mixed-motive interactions.Unpublishedmanuscript,St.Mary’sUniversity.

Goldberg,S.(2000).Texoil.InJ.M.Brett(Ed.)Negotiation and decision making exercises.Evanston,IL:DisputeResolutionResearchCenter,NorthwesternUniversityCD.

Hoffman,S.(1977).AnAmericansocialscience:Internationalrelations.Daedalus 106(3),41–60.

Johnson,D.W.(1967).Useofrolereversalinintergroupcompetition.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 7,135–141.

Johnson, D. W. (1975). Affective perspective taking and cooperative predisposition.Developmental Psychology, 11,869–870.

Kemp,K.E.,&Smith,W.P.(1994).Informationexchange,toughness,andintegrativebar­gaining:Therolesofexplicitcuesandperspective­taking.The International Journal of Conflict Management, 5,5–21.

Lax,D.A.&Sebenius,J.K.(1986).The manager as negotiator: Bargaining for cooperation and competitive gain.NewYork:TheFreePress.

Page 134: the Psychology of Social Conflict and Aggression Sydney Symposium in Social Psychology

uSing Both your head and your heart 117

Ligthart, P.E.M., & Lindenberg, S. (1994). Ethical regulation of economic transactions:Solidarity frameversusgain­maximization frame.InA.LewisandK.E.Warneryd(Eds.),Ethics and economic affairs(pp.215–230).London:Routledge.

Lopes, L. L. (1976). Model­based decision and inference in stud poker. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 105,217–239.

Maddux,W.W.,Mullen,E.,&Galinsky,A.D.(2008).Chameleonsbakebiggerpiesandtake bigger pieces: Strategic behavioral mimicry facilitates negotiation outcomes.Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 44, 461–468.

Moore,D.A.(2005).Myopicbiasesinstrategicsocialprediction:Whydeadlinesputevery­one under more pressure than everyone else. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 31,668–679.

Neale,M.A.,&Bazerman,M.H.(1982).Theroleofperspective­takingabilityinnegotiat­ingunderdifferentformsofarbitration.Industrial and Labor Relations Review, 36,378–388.

Okun,M.A.,Shepard,S.A.,&Eisenberg,N. (2000).Therelationsofemotionalityandregulationtodispositionalempathy­relatedrespondingamongvolunteers­in­training.Personality and Individual Differences, 28,367–382.

Oswald,P.A.(1996).Theeffectsofcognitiveandaffectiveperspective­takingonempathicconcernandaltruistichelping.Journal of Social Psychology, 136(5),613–623.

Peterson,E.,&Thompson,L.(1997).Negotiationteamwork:Theimpactofinformationdis­tributionandaccountabilityonperformancedependsontherelationshipamongteammembers.Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 72,364–383.

Piaget,J.(1932).The moral judgment of the child.Glencoe,IL:FreePress.Pillutla,M.M.,&Murnighan,J.K.(1995).Beingfairorappearingfair:Strategicbehavior

inultimatumbargaining.Academy of Management Journal, 38,1408–1426.Pillutla,M.M.,&Murnighan,J.K.(1996).Unfairness,anger,andspite:Emotionalrejec­

tionsofultimatumoffers.Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 68,208–224.

Pruitt,D.G.,&Rubin,J.Z.(1986).Social conflict: Escalation, stalemate, and settlement.NewYork:McGraw­Hill.

Regan,D.T.,&Totten,J.(1975).Empathyandattribution:Turningobserversintoactors.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 32,850–856.

Richardson,D.R.,Green,L.R.,&Lago,T.(1998).Therelationshipbetweenperspective­takingandnonaggressiverespondinginthefaceofattack.Journal of Personality, 66,235–256.

Richardson, D. R., Hammock, G. S., Smith, S. M., Gardner, W., & Signo, M. (1994).Empathy as a cognitive inhibitor of interpersonal aggression. Aggressive Behavior, 20, 275–289.

Sally,D.(2000).Ageneraltheoryofsympathy,mind­reading,andsocialinteraction,withanapplicationtotheprisoners’dilemma.Social Science Information, 39,567–634.

Sheppard,B.H.(1999).Negotiatinginlong­termmutuallyinterdependentrelationshipsamongrelativeequals.InR.J.Lewicki,D.M.Saunders,&J.W.Minton(Eds.).Negotiation: Readings, exercises, and cases (3rded.,pp.278–293).Boston,MA:Irwin/McGraw­Hill.

Smith,A.(1759).Theory of moral sentiments.London:A.Miller.Spencer,H.(1870).The principles of psychology.London:WilliamsandNorgate.Tetlock,P.E.,Skitka,L.,&Boettger,R.(1989).Socialandcognitivestrategiesforcoping

with accountability: Conformity, complexity, and bolstering. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 57,632–640.

Thagard,P.(1992).Adversarialproblemsolving:Modelinganopponentusingexplanatorycoherence.Cognitive Science, 16,123–149.

Thompson,L.(1990).Negotiationbehaviorandoutcomes:Empiricalevidenceandtheo­reticalissues.Psychological Bulletin, 108,515–532.

Page 135: the Psychology of Social Conflict and Aggression Sydney Symposium in Social Psychology

adam d. galinSky, deBra gilin, and William W. maddux118

Thompson,L.(2001).The mind and heart of the negotiator (2nded.).UpperSaddleRiver,NJ:PrenticeHall.

Thompson, L., & Hastie, R. (1990). Social perception in negotiation. Organizational Behavior & Human Decision Processes, 47,98–123.

Tripp,T.,&Sondak,H.(1992).Anevaluationofdependentvariablesinexperimentalnego­tiation studies: Impasse rates and Pareto efficiency. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 51,273–295.

Trivers,R.L. (1971).Theevolutionof reciprocalaltruism.Quarterly Review of Biology, 46, 35–57.

Twenge, J.M. (2001).Changes inwomen’sassertiveness in response tostatusandroles:A cross­temporal meta­analysis, 1931–1993. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 81,133–145.

Ury,W.,Brett,J.M.,&Goldberg,S.B.(1988).Getting disputes resolved.SanFrancisco:Jossey­Bass.

VanBaaren,R.B.,Holland,R.W.,Kawakami,K.,&VanKnippenberg,A.(2004).Mimicryandprosocialbehavior.Psychological Science, 15, 71–74.

VanBaaren,R.B.,Holland,R.W.,Steenaert,B.,&VanKnippenberg,A.(2003).Mimicryfor money: Behavioral consequences of imitation. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 39, 393–398.

Vescio,T.K.,Sechrist,G.B.,&Paolucci,M.P.(2003).Perspectivetakingandprejudicereduction: The mediational role of empathy arousal and situational attributions.European Journal of Social Psychology, 33,455–472.

Wilson,K.,&Gallois,C.(1993).Assertion and its social context.Oxford,UK:PergamonPressLtd.

Page 136: the Psychology of Social Conflict and Aggression Sydney Symposium in Social Psychology

119

8AffectiveInfluencesonthe

Perception,Management,andResolutionofSocialConflicts1

JOSEPHP.FORGASandHUIBINGTANUniversity of New South Wales

introduction

I maginethatyouarehearingamarchingbandperformingacheerful,upbeattunewhileyouarethinkingaboutaseriousconflictyouneedtoresolvewithyourpartner.Wouldthemoodinducedbythemusicinfluenceyourthoughts,

plans,andeventualconflictmanagementstrategies?Thischapterwillexplorethepsychologicalmechanismsresponsibleforsucheffects,describingaseriesofexper­imentsdemonstratingtheinfluenceofmoodonvariousconflictbehaviors.Itiswellknownthataffectisanintegralcomponentofmostsocialconflictsandalsoplaysacrucialrole inmanyaggressiveencounters (Forgas,2002,2007;Zajonc,2000;seealsoChapter9inthisvolume).Affectivestatesarelikelytoinfluenceavarietyofstrategicconflict­relatedbehaviors,suchasassertiveness(seeChapter5inthisvolume),forgiveness(seeChapter14inthisvolume),goalsetting(Chapters6and12 in thisvolume),perspective taking(Chapter7 in thisvolume),andreactionstoostracism (Chapters3 and13 in this volume).Returning toour introductoryexample,militarymusichasbeenusedeversinceantiquitytoinfluencesoldiers’moodstates,inthehopethatupbeat,energeticmusiccreatesamoreassertiveandconfident mindset that can influence behavior in conflict situations. In another

1 ThisworkwassupportedbyaProfessorialFellowshipfromtheAustralianResearchCouncilandtheResearchPrizebytheAlexandervonHumboldtFoundationtoJosephP.Forgas.Forfurtherinformationonthisresearchproject,seealsowww.psy.unsw.edu.au/users/jforgas.htm.

Page 137: the Psychology of Social Conflict and Aggression Sydney Symposium in Social Psychology

JoSeph p. forgaS and hui Bing tan120

literaryexample,ThomasManninashortstorydescribeshowchessplayers’deci­sionswhethertouseadefensiveoraggressivestrategyismarkedlyinfluencedbytheupbeatordownbeatmoodofthebackgroundmusicplayedbyanorchestrainthebackground.

Althoughthelasttwodecadessawsomethinglikean“affectiverevolution”inpsychologicalresearch(seealsoForgas,2002,2006),wearestillalongwayfromfullyunderstandingtheage­oldpuzzleaboutthelinksbetweenaffectandcognition,feelingandthinkingasthetwocomplementaryfacultiesofthehumanmind (Hilgard, 1980). There is compelling recent evidence from evolutionarysocialpsychology,neuropsychology,andpsychophysiologysuggestingthataffectis an essential component ofmotivated social thinkingand behavior (Adolphs&Damasio,2001;Blascovich&Mendes,2000).However,wedonotyet fullyunderstandwhen,how,andwhytheseeffectsoccur.Thischapterwilldescribeaseriesofrecentstudiesshowinghowpositiveandnegativemoodstatescaninflu­enceboththecontentandtheprocessofhowpeoplethinkaboutconflictsitu­ations, resulting in significantconsequences forconflictbehaviorsandconflictresolutionstrategies.

Wewillbeginwithabriefoverviewofearlyresearchonaffectandsocialcon­flictandwillsurveyrecentcognitivetheoriesrelevanttounderstandingthislink.Next, two converging lines of research will be described demonstrating affec­tive influenceson thecontentandprocessof thinkingandbehavior in conflictsituations.First,experimentsdemonstratingaffectcongruencewillbedescribed,showing that affective states may color the way people interpret and evaluateconflictsituations,influencingtheirnegotiatingstrategiesandthewayinterper­sonaldemandsare formulatedandrespondedto.Asecondlineofexperimentsexplores affective influences in information­processing strategies, showing thataffectimpactsonhowpeopleprocessconflict­relevantinformation.Inparticular,mildnegativemoodsoftentriggeramoresystematic,accommodativeinformationprocessingstylethatresultsinmoreeffectiveandmoresuccessfuljudgmentsandbehaviorsinconflictsituationsandgreatersensitivitytosocialnorms.

affect, mood, and emotionThereisasyetlittlegeneralagreementintheliteratureabouthowbesttodefinetermssuchasaffect, feelings, emotions,ormood(Fiedler&Forgas,1988;Forgas,1992,1995,2002).Wehavearguedelsewherethataffectmaybeusedasagenericlabel to refer to both moods and emotions. Moods in turn could be describedas“low­intensity,diffuseandrelativelyenduringaffectivestateswithoutasalientantecedentcauseandthereforelittlecognitivecontent(e.g.,feelinggoodorfeel­ing bad),” whereas emotions “are more intense, short­lived and usually have adefinitecauseandclearcognitivecontent” (e.g.,angerorfear) (Forgas,1992,p.230).Thisdistinctionishighlyrelevanttounderstandingthefunctionsofaffectinconflictbehaviors.Thereismuchevidencefortheinfluenceofspecificemotionssuchasanger,shame,guilt,andprideinconflictbehaviors(seeChapter9inthisvolume).Inaddition,subtle,nonspecificmoodsmayoftenhaveapotentiallymoreenduringandinsidiousmotivationalinfluenceonsocialcognitionandbehaviorsin

Page 138: the Psychology of Social Conflict and Aggression Sydney Symposium in Social Psychology

reSolution of Social conflictS 121

conflictsituations(Fiedler,1991;Forgas,1992,1995,2002;Sedikides,1992,1995).Accordingly,ourprimaryconcernhereiswiththeeffectsoflow­intensitymoodsratherthandistinctemotions.

affect and Social conflictSThekeyroleofaffectinthewaypeoplethinkaboutandrespondtosocialcon­flictshasbeensuggestedinanumberofearlystudies.Psychoanalytictheoriesassumed that affecthas adynamic, invasivequality andcan “takeover” judg­ments unless adequate psychological resources are deployed to control theseimpulses (Feshbach & Singer, 1957). Conditioning and associationist theoriesprovided an alternative account, suggesting thatpreviously “neutral” conceptscanbecomeaffectivelyloadedasaresultofincidentalassociationswithaffect­elicitingstimuli.AccordingtoradicalbehavioristssuchasJohnWatson,allaffec­tive reactions acquired throughout life are the product of such a cumulativepatternofassociations.

More recent work showed that implicit representations of common socialencounters, including conflict situations, are largely determined by the feelingsarousedbytheseevents,ratherthantheirobjectivefeatures(Forgas,1979,1982).Feelings of anxiety, confidence, intimacy, pleasure, or discomfort are critical indefiningimplicitrepresentationsofsocialencounters.Severaldecadesago,Pervin(1976)notedthatwhat is striking is theextent towhich interpersonalsituationsare“describedintermsofaffects(e.g.,threatening,warm,interesting,dull,tense,calm,rejecting)andorganizedintermsofsimilarityofaffectsarousedbythem”(p.471).Morerecently,NiedenthalandHalberstadt(2000)showedthatsuchemo­tionalcategorizationisextremelycommon.Manysocialstimuliandeventsareper­ceived,categorized,andrespondedtonotbasedontheirobjectivecharacteristicsbutintermsoftheemotionalreactionstheyelicit.

Affect also has a dynamic influence on how social information—includinginformationaboutconflictsituations—isinterpreted,processed,andremembered(Bower,1981;Forgas,1995a,2001,2002).Contemporarycognitivetheoriesfocusontheinformation­processingmechanismsthatallowaffectivestatestoinfluenceboththecontentandtheprocessesofthinkingandjudgments.

cognitive mechaniSmS of affect congruenceAffectivestatescanhavetwokindsofeffectsonsocialthinkingandbehavior.Theymayinfluence(1)thecontentofthinkingbyselectivelyprimingaffect­congruentthoughtsandresponses,andtheymayalsoinfluence(2)theprocessofthinking,that is, thewaypeopleprocess social information.We shall consider these twokindsofeffectsinturn.

Content Effects

TheassociativenetworkmodelbyBower(1981)proposedthataffectandcogni­tionareintegrallylinkedwithinanassociativenetworkofmentalrepresentations.

Page 139: the Psychology of Social Conflict and Aggression Sydney Symposium in Social Psychology

JoSeph p. forgaS and hui Bing tan122

Selectively primed affect­congruent constructs are more likely to be used inconstructivecognitive tasks—forexample,whenperceiving, interpreting,andconstructing responses to a conflict situation. In several experiments, Bowerfound that happy or sad people were likely to selectively remember positive(ornegative)detailsoftheirchildhoodandoftheirsocialactivitiesduringthepreceding weeks, consistent with the predicted selective recall of affect­con­gruent information.Betteraccess toaffect­congruent informationshouldalsobiasperceptionsandbehaviorsinconflictsituations,andsuchmoodeffectsaremostreliablyfoundwhenthesituationiscomplexanddemandingasistypicallythecasewithconflictscenarios(Forgas,1994,1999a,1999b,2002;Sedikides,1995).

An alternative theory of content effects was proposed by Schwarz andClore(1983)whoarguedthat“insteadofcomputingajudgmentonthebasisofrecalledfeaturesofatarget,individualsmaysimplyjustaskthemselves:“Howdo I feel about it?” and when doing so, they may mistake feelings due to apre­existingmoodstateasindicativeoftheirreactiontothetarget”(Schwarz,1990, p. 529). This simplistic “how­do­I­feel­about­it” heuristic suggests thatpeopleoftenmisreadtheirmoodasinformativeoftheirreactionstoanunre­latedsituation.AsearlierconditioningtheoriesbyCloreandByrne(1974),thismodelalsopositsanincidentalandsubconsciouslinkbetweenaffectandunre­latedstimuliandresponses.Researchnowsuggeststhatpeopleseemtorelyonaffectasaheuristiccueonlywhentheyareunfamiliarwiththetask,whentheyhave no priorevaluations to fallbackon, when theirpersonal involvement islow,andwhentheyhaveinsufficientcognitiveresourcesormotivationtocom­puteamorethoroughresponse(Forgas,2006).Althoughaffect­as­informationmay influencequick, superficial judgments (Forgas&Moylan,1987;Schwarz&Clore),itisunlikelythatmorecomplexanddemandingreactionsinconflictsituationswouldbebasedonsuchasuperficialandtruncatedstrategy.

Processing Effects

Affectcanalsoinfluencetheprocessofcognition,thatis,howpeoplethink(Clark& Isen, 1982; Schwarz, 1990). According to the mood­maintenance hypothesis(Clark&Isen),people in apositivemoodshould try toavoideffortful thinkingtomaintainthispleasantstate.Incontrast,thoseinnegativemoodmightengagein vigilant, effortful processing as an adaptive response to improve an aversivestate.OtherssuchasSchwarz,andWegenerandPetty(1994),offeredafunction­alist“cognitivetuning”account,suggestingthatpositiveandnegativeaffecthavea signaling–tuning function, informing the person of whether a relaxed, effort­minimizing(inpositiveaffect)oravigilant,effortful(negativeaffect)processingstyleisappropriate.

Morerecent integrative theories suggestamore subtlepattern (Bless,2001;Bless&Fiedler,2006;Fiedler,2001),arguingthattheevolutionarysignificanceofaffectivestatesisnotsimplytoinfluenceprocessingeffortbuttotriggerqualitativelydifferentprocessingstylesaswell.Thus,positiveaffectrecruitsamoreassimila­tive,schema­based,top­downprocessingstyle,whenpreexistingknowledgeguides

Page 140: the Psychology of Social Conflict and Aggression Sydney Symposium in Social Psychology

reSolution of Social conflictS 123

informationprocessing.Incontrast,negativeaffectproducesamoreaccommoda­tive,bottom­up,andexternallyfocusedprocessingstrategywhereattentiontositu­ationalinformationdrivesthinking(Bless;Fiedler).Theseprocessingstylescanbeequallyeffortfulyetproducequalitativelydifferentoutcomesinconflictsituations(Tan&Forgas,2010).Interestingly,themorevigilantprocessingmoodpromotedbynegativeaffectcanproducesomesurprisingprocessingadvantages,improvingperformanceontasksthatrequiredetailedattentiontonewinformationandlead­ingtomoresuccessfulandadaptiveconflictbehaviors.

Toward an Integration: The Affect Infusion Model

An integrative theory, the Affect Infusion Model (AIM; Forgas, 1995a, 2002)predictsthataffectinfusionshouldoccuronlyincircumstancesthatpromoteanopen,constructiveprocessingstyle(Fiedler,1991;Forgas,1995b).TheAIMthusassumesthat(1)affectinfusionshoulddependonthekindofprocessingstrategypeopleuse,and(2)all thingsbeingequal,peopleshouldusetheleasteffortfulandsimplestprocessingstrategy.Themodelidentifiesfouralternativeprocessingstrategies:(1)directaccess;(2)motivated; (3)heuristic;and(4)substantivepro­cessing.Thesestrategiesdifferintermsoftwobasicdimensions:(1)thedegreeofeffort;and(2)thedegreeofopennessandconstructivenessoftheinformation­searchstrategy.

The combination of these two processing features—quantity (effort) andquality (openness)—produces fourdistinctprocessingstyles (Fiedler,2001): (1)substantiveprocessing(higheffort/open,constructive);(2)motivatedprocessing(higheffort/closed);(3)heuristicprocessing(loweffort/open,constructive);and(4)direct­accessprocessing(loweffort/closed).Direct­accessandmotivatedpro­cessinginvolvehighlytargetedandpredeterminedpatternsofinformationsearchandselection,strategiesthatlimitthescopeforincidentalaffectinfusion.Moodcongruence and affect infusion are likely only when constructive processing isused, suchassubstantiveorheuristicprocessing(seealsoFiedler,1991,2001).TheAIMalso specifies a range of contextual variables related to the task, theperson,andthesituation thatjointlyinfluenceprocessingchoices.AnimportantfeatureoftheAIMisthatitrecognizesthataffectitselfcanalsoinfluencepro­cessing choices. The implications of this model have now been supported in anumberofstudies.

affect congruence in dealing With conflictSAccordingtotheAIM,affectivestatesshouldhaveamood­congruent influenceondealingwithconflictsituationsthatrecruitconstructive,substantiveprocess­ing(Forgas,1995a,2001;Sedikides,1995).Onthemostbasiclevel,theremaybeaffect­congruentdistortionsonthewaypeopleinterpretobservedsocialbehaviors(Forgas,Bower,&Krantz,1984).Happysubjectstendtoseemorepositiveskilledbehaviors,whereassadmoodproducesmorecritical,negativebehaviorinterpreta­tionsevenwhenobjective,videotapedevidenceisreadilyavailablewithobviousimplicationsforconflictsituations.

Page 141: the Psychology of Social Conflict and Aggression Sydney Symposium in Social Psychology

JoSeph p. forgaS and hui Bing tan124

Affective Bias in Explaining Relationship Conflicts

Affectmay also influence theway peopleevaluate their partners and real­lifesocialconflicts(Forgas,2002;Forgas,Levinger,&Moylan,1994).Moodeffectsondealingwithrelationshipconflictscanhaveparticularlyimportantconsequencesforthesuccessandlongevityoftherelationship.Inaseriesofexperiments,weaskedhappyorsadparticipantstomakecausalattributionsforrecenthappyandsadconflictsintheircurrentrelationships(Forgas,1994,Experiment1).Therewassignificantmoodcongruence,withmoreself­blamingandpessimisticattri­butionsbysadsubjectsthanbyhappysubjects.Inafurtherstudyexplanationsforsimpleversuscomplexrelationshipconflictswerecompared(Experiment2).Again,sadmoodproducedmorenegative,pessimisticattributions.Moodeffectsweregreaterwhenexplanationsweregivenforseriousratherthansimplecon­flictsasseriousconflictsrequiredmoresubstantiveprocessing,andwereassoci­atedwithlongerprocessinglatencies(Experiment3).ConsistentwiththeAIM,theseresultsconfirmthatparadoxically,extendedprocessingrecruitedbyseri­ousconflictsincreasedmoodeffects(Forgas).

Affective Influences on Negotiating Strategies

Oneofthemostcommonmethodsfordealingwithconflictisnegotiation.Effectivenegotiationisacriticalskillinresolvingpersonalandrelationshipproblemsandisalsoroutinelyusedinorganizations.Inseveralexperiments(Forgas,1998a),posi­tive,control,ornegativemoodwasinducedbygivingparticipantspositive,nega­tive,orneutralfeedbackabouttheirperformanceonapriorverbaltest.Next,theyengagedineither(a)aninformalinterpersonal,or(b)aformal,intergroupnego­tiatingtask.Participantsinapositivemoodsetthemselvesmoreambitiousgoals,formulatedmoreoptimistic action plans, andengaged inmore cooperative andintegrativenegotiationthandidcontrol,ornegativemoodparticipants.Theywerealsomorewillingtomakeandreciprocatedeals(Figure 8.1)andactuallyachievedbetteroutcomes.Theseresultsprovideclearevidencethatevenslightchangesin

Negotiating StrategyCooperation Competition Make deals

HappySad

Ove

rall

Use

5

4

3

2

2

0

figure 8.1 Mood­congruent influences on negotiation: happy persons plan, use morecooperativeand lesscompetitivebargainingstrategies,andaremore likely tomakeandhonordealsthandonegotiatorsexperiencingnegativeaffect.(DatabasedonForgas,J.P.,Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,74,565–577,1998.Withpermission.)

Page 142: the Psychology of Social Conflict and Aggression Sydney Symposium in Social Psychology

reSolution of Social conflictS 125

moodcan significantly influencepeople’sperceptions,plans,andbehaviors inanegotiatingtask.

IntermsoftheAIM,thesemoodeffectsonconflictresolutioncanbeexplainedin terms of affectpriming mechanisms. Positive mood should selectively primemorepositivethoughtsandassociations,leadingtotheformulationanduseofmoreoptimistic,cooperative,andintegrativebargainingstrategies.Incontrast,negativemoodshouldprimemorepessimistic,negativethoughtsandassociations,leadingtolessambitiousgoalsandlesscooperative,morecompetitive,andultimatelylesssuccessfulbargainingstrategies.

Interestingly, further experiments in this series showed that mood effectswerereducedforindividualswhoscoredhighonmeasuressuchasMachiavellismandtheneedforapproval.Theseindividualsmayhaveapproachedthebargain­ing task from a strongly predetermined, motivated perspective, reducing open,constructiveprocessingandthuslimitingtheextentofaffectinfusion.Individualdifferences intendencytouseopen,constructiveversusguided,motivatedpro­cessingmaysignificantlymediateaffectinfusionintobehaviorinconflictsituations(Rusting,2001).

Mood Effects on Making Requests and Demands

Inseveralexperimentsweexploredtheeffectsofmoodonthewaypeoplefor­mulateandrespondtodemandsandrequests.Requestingisanintrinsicallycom­plexbehaviorcharacterizedbypotentialinterpersonalconflictandpsychologicalambiguity.Requestsmustbeformulatedwithjusttherightdegreeofassertive­ness versuspoliteness to maximize compliance without risking giving offense.Weexpectedhappypeopletoadoptamoreconfident,assertiverequestingstyle,due to thegreateravailabilityofpositively valenced thoughts andassociations(Forgas,1998b,1999a,1999b).Further,intermsoftheAIM,thesemoodeffectsshouldbeparticularly strongwhen theconflict situation ismorecomplex anddemandingandrequiresmoresubstantiveandelaborateprocessing.Moodwasinducedbyaskingpeopletorecallandthinkabouthappyorsadautobiographi­calepisodes(Forgas,1999a,Experiment1).Next,participantsselectedamoreorlesspoliterequestformulationthattheywoulduseinaneasyandadifficultrequestsituation.

Happy participants preferred more direct, assertive requests, whereas sadpersons used indirect, polite requests, and these effects were greatest in themoredifficult,demandingrequestsituation.Inafollow­upexperiment,similareffectswerefoundwhenparticipantsproducedtheirownopen­endedrequests,whichweresubsequentlyratedforpolitenessandelaborationby two indepen­dent raters (Forgas, 1999a, Experiment 2). This pattern was confirmed in athirdstudy,whereparticipantswereaskedtoproducemoreorlesspoliteversusassertiverequestalternativesinavarietyofdifferentrealisticsituations(Forgas,1999b,Experiment1)followinganaudiovisualmoodinduction(watchinghappyorsadfilms).

Afurtherunobtrusivefieldexperimentlookedatnaturallyproducedrequests(Forgas, 1999b, Experiment 2). After an audiovisual mood induction, the

Page 143: the Psychology of Social Conflict and Aggression Sydney Symposium in Social Psychology

JoSeph p. forgaS and hui Bing tan126

experimentercasuallyaskedparticipants togetafile fromaneighboringoffice.Theactualwordsinrequestingthefilewererecordedbyaconcealedtaperecorderandweresubsequentlyanalyzedforpolitenessandassertiveness.Sadpeopleusedmore polite, friendly, and more elaborate request forms, whereas happypeopleusedmoreassertiveandlesspoliteforms(Figure 8.2).Ananalysisofrecalldataconfirmed that unconventional requests were also recalled significantly better,indicatingtheirmoreelaborate,constructiveprocessing.

Mood Effects on Responding to Interpersonal Demands

Wehavesofarseenthatmoodstatescanhaveaprofoundinfluenceonhowpeo­pleapproachcomplexconflictsituations.Moodsmayalsoinfluencerespondingtomoreorlessassertivedemands,suchasbeingconfrontedbyanunexpectedrequestfromastranger(Forgas,1998b).Studentsenteringalibraryfoundpicturesortextplacedontheirdesksdesignedtoinducegoodorbadmoods.Afewminuteslater,they were approached by another student (in fact, a confederate) and receivedanunexpectedpoliteorassertive,impolitedemandforseveralsheetsofpaper.Ashorttimeaftertheincidentasecondconfederateaskedthemtocompleteabriefquestionnaire evaluating their perceptions, recall, and reactions to the demandandtherequester.

Peopleinanegativemoodreactedmorenegatively,formedmorecritical,nega­tiveviewsofrequests,andwerelessinclinedtocomplythanwerepositivemoodparticipants. In aparticularly interesting result (Figure 8.3),moodeffectsweregreaterwhentherequestwasassertiveandimpoliteandsorequiredmoresubstan­tiveprocessing,asalsoconfirmedbybetterrecallmemoryforthesemessageslateron.Itseemsthatassertive,unconventionaldemandswereprocessedmoresubstan­tivelyandresultedinstrongermoodeffects.AsimpliedbytheAIM,affectinfusionintoconflictbehaviorsseemenhancedwhencomplex,unusualtasksrequiremoreelaborateprocessing.

Request QualityPoliteness Elaboration Hedging

HappyControlSad

Mea

n Ra

ting

5

4

3

2

2

0

figure 8.2 Moodeffectsonnaturallyproducedrequests:positivemoodincreasesandnegativemooddecreases thedegreeofpoliteness, elaboration, andhedging in strategiccommunications.(AfterForgas,J.P.,Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 25,850–863,1999.Withpermission.)

Page 144: the Psychology of Social Conflict and Aggression Sydney Symposium in Social Psychology

reSolution of Social conflictS 127

the information-proceSSing conSeQuenceS of affect in conflict SituationS

In addition to producing affect congruence, affect can also influence the wayinformation is processed. Although it is commonly claimed that feeling goodpromotesbetterthinkingintermsofcreativity,flexibility,andintegrativethink­ing (Ciarrochi, Forgas, & Mayer, 2006; Forgas, 1994, 2002), this is only partofthestory.Inthissectionwepresentseveralexperimentsshowingthatnega­tiveaffectmayalsoproducedesirableandbeneficialcognitiveconsequences.Infunctional terms, negative affect may operate as an adaptive signal recruitingmoreattentiveandaccommodativethinkingthatmayhelppeopletocopewiththe requirements of demanding social situations (Forgas, 2007). For example,negativeaffectproducesathinkingstyle thathelpsreducecertain judgmentalbiases(Forgas,1998c)andpromotesmoresuccessfulsocialinfluencestrategies(Forgas,2007).

Negative Affect Reduces the Fundamental Attribution Error

Interpretingthebehaviorofpartnersandadversariesinaconflictisoftensubjectto the fundamental attribution error (FAE) when people see intentionality andinternalcausationdespiteevidencefortheinfluenceofsituationalforces(Gilbert&Malone,1995).TheFAEoccursbecausepeoplefocusonsalientandconspicu­ous information—the actor—and fail to process information about situational

Polite Impolite

Positive mood effect

Negative mood effectResp

onse

to re

ques

t

Request type

0.8

0.6

0.4

0.2

0

–0.2

–0.4

–0.6

–0.8

–1

–1.2

figure 8.3 Moodeffectsonreactionstoanunexpecteddemandinapublicplace(thelibrary): those inapositivemoodrespondmorepositivelyandthose inanegativemoodrespond more negatively (higher values indicate more positive reactions). These moodeffectsweregreaterwhentherequestwasimpoliteandatypicalandthusrequiredmoresubstantive processing. (Data based on Forgas, J.P., Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 24,173–185,1998.Withpermission.)

Page 145: the Psychology of Social Conflict and Aggression Sydney Symposium in Social Psychology

JoSeph p. forgaS and hui Bing tan128

constraints(Gilbert,1991).Ifnegativemoodpromotesthemoredetailedprocess­ing of situational information, the incidence of the FAE and other judgmentalbiasesmaybereduced(Forgas,1998c).Inoneexperiment,happyorsadpartici­pantsreadandmadeattributionsaboutthewriterofanessayadvocatingapopu­lar or unpopular position (for or against nuclear testing), which they were toldwaseitherassignedorfreelychosen,usingtheprocedurepioneeredbyJonesandHarris(1967).HappymoodincreasedandsadmoodreducedtheincidenceoftheFAE,consistentwiththemoreattentivethinkingstylerecruitedbynegativeaffect.Similareffectscanalsooccurinreallife.

Inafield study,happyor sadparticipantsmadeattributionsabout thewrit­ersofpopularandunpopularessaysarguing fororagainst recycling (cf.Forgas&Moylan,1987).Onceagain,negativemoodreducedtheFAE.Recallmemorydata confirmed that these effects were due to the more attentive processing ofsituational information in negative mood (Forgas, 1998c, Experiment 3). Theseeffectsareconsistentwiththesuggestedevolutionarybenefitsofnegativeaffectinrecruitingmoreaccommodativeprocessingstyles.

Negative Affect Increases Skepticism and Interpersonal Accuracy

Believingor notbelieving apartneror an adversary is another crucialdecisionpeopleoften face inconflict situations.Howdoweknow if the informationwereceivefromothersisaccurate?Acceptinginvalidinformationastrue(falseposi­tives,excessivegullibility)canbejustasdangerousasrejectinginformationthatisvalid(falsenegatives,excessiveskepticism).Negativemoodsmightproducemorecriticalandskepticaljudgments,whereashappypeoplemayacceptinterpersonalmessagesat“facevalue,”asgenuineandtrustworthyduetotheinformation­pro­cessingconsequencesofaffectwediscussedpreviously.Toexplorethis,weaskedhappyandsadparticipantstojudgethegenuinenessofpeopledisplayingpositive,neutral,andnegativefacialexpressions.

Aspredicted,thoseinanegativemoodweresignificantlylesslikelytoacceptfacialexpressionsasgenuinethanwerethoseintheneutralorhappycondition.Curiously,happyparticipantswerealsomoreconfident in their judgments thanwereothergroups.Inanotherstudynegativemoodreducedandpositivemoodincreasedpeople’stendencytoacceptothers’facialdisplaysasgenuine,consistentwiththemoreattentiveandaccommodativeprocessingstyleassociatedwithnega­tivemoods.

Negative mood may also improve perception accuracy and the detection ofdeception.Inonestudyhappyorsadparticipantshadtodeterminethetruthful­nessofvideotapedstatementsbypeoplewhowereinterrogatedafterastagedtheftandwereeitherguiltyornotguilty(East&Forgas,2008).Thoseinapositivemoodweremore likely toacceptdeceptivestatementsas truthful.Sadparticipants inturnformedsignificantlymoreguilty judgmentsandweresignificantlybetteratcorrectlydetectingdeception(Figure 8.4).Asignaldetectionanalysisconfirmedthatsadjudgesweremoreaccurateindetectingdeception(identifyingguiltytar­getsasguilty) thanwereneutralorhappy judges,consistentwith thepredictedmood­inducedprocessingdifferences.

Page 146: the Psychology of Social Conflict and Aggression Sydney Symposium in Social Psychology

reSolution of Social conflictS 129

Negative Affect Improves the Efficacy of Persuasive Messages

Accommodativeprocessingpromotedbynegativeaffectmayalsoresult inmoreconcreteandfactualthinkingandresultintheproductionofmoreeffectivepowerstrategiesandpersuasivemessages.Weexploredthis(Forgas,2007,Experiment1)byaskinghappyorsadparticipantstoproducepersuasiveargumentsfororagainstan increase in student fees and for or against Aboriginal land rights. Negativemoodresultedinargumentsthatwereofsignificantlyhigherquality,weremoreconcrete,andweremorepersuasivethanthoseproducedbyhappyparticipants.A mediational analysis established that mood­induced variations in argumentconcreteness improvedargumentquality.Ina furtherexperiment,happyorsadparticipantsproducedpersuasiveargumentsfororagainstAustraliabecomingarepublicandfororagainstaradicalright­wingparty.Sadmoodagainresultedinhigherqualityandmorepersuasivearguments,consistentwiththepredictionthatnegativemoodshouldpromoteamorecareful,systematic,bottom­upprocessingstyle(Bless,2001;Bless&Fiedler,2006;Fiedler,2001;Forgas,2002;Figure 8.5).

To further test the actual effectiveness of negative mood arguments, inExperiment3 the argumentsproduced by happy or sad participants were pre­sentedtoanaïveaudienceofundergraduatestudents.Argumentswrittenbynega­tivemoodparticipants inExperiments1and2wereactuallysignificantlymoresuccessfulinproducingarealchangeinattitudesthanwereargumentsproducedby happy participants. Finally, in Experiment 4 persuasive attempts by happyandsadpeopleweredirectedata“partner”tovolunteerforaboringexperimentusingemailexchanges(Forgas,2007).Themotivationtobepersuasivewasalsomanipulatedbyofferingsomeof themasignificantreward if successful (moviepasses).Peopleinanegativemoodproducedhigher­qualitypersuasivearguments.However,theofferofarewardreducedmoodeffects,confirmingakeypredic­tion of the Affect Infusion Model (Forgas, 1995a, 2002): that mood effects on

HappyNeutralSad

100908070605040302010

0

Aver

age

gui

lt ju

dgm

ents

Truthful DeceptiveTarget condition

figure 8.4 Theeffectsofmoodandthetarget’sveracity(truthful,deceptive)onjudg­mentsofguiltoftargetsaccusedofcommittingatheft(averagepercentageoftargetsjudgedguilty in each condition). (After East, R. & Forgas, J.P., Manuscript, University of NewSouthWales,2008.Withpermission.)

Page 147: the Psychology of Social Conflict and Aggression Sydney Symposium in Social Psychology

JoSeph p. forgaS and hui Bing tan130

informationprocessing—andsubsequentsocialinfluencestrategies—arereducedby motivated processing. Mediational analyses confirmed that negative moodinduced longerandmoreaccommodative thinkingandmoreconcreteandspe­cificarguments.

Theseexperimentsconfirmthatpersuasiveargumentsproduced innegativemoodarenotonlyofhigherqualityasjudgedbyratersbutarealsosignificantlymoreeffective inproducinggenuineattitudechange inpeople.However,whenmotivation is already high, mood effects tend to diminish, as predicted by theAffectInfusionModel(Forgas,2002).Thisfindingmayhaveinterestingappliedimplicationsformanagingpersonalandorganizationalconflictsthatalsoinvolveagreatdealofpersuasivecommunication.Itisanintriguingpossibilitythatmildnegativeaffectmayactuallypromoteamoreconcrete,accommodative,andulti­matelymoresuccessfulcommunicationstyleinsomeconflictsituations.

When Positive Affect Increases Aggressive Tendencies

After the London bomb attacks, in a tragic mistake British police shot deada Brazilian man who looked like a Muslim. Could it be that merely appearingMuslimmay functionasasubliminalcuefacilitatingaggressiveresponses?Inarecentexperimentweinvestigatedtheinfluenceofpositiveandnegativemoodsonaggressiveresponsesintheshoot–don’tshootparadigmwithtargetswhodidordidnotappeartobeMuslim.Usingthistechnique(Correll,Park,Judd,&Wittenbrink,2002),U.S.participantsrevealedastrongbiastoshootmoreatBlackratherthan

PositiveNeutralNegative

8

7

6

5

4

Mea

n ar

gum

ent q

ualit

y

Quality Concreteness

figure 8.5 Moodeffectsonthequalityandconcretenessofthepersuasivemessagespro­duced:negativeaffectimprovesthequalityandthedegreeofconcretenessofpersuasivearguments.(AfterForgas,J.P.,Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 43,513–528,2007,Experiment2.Withpermission.)

Page 148: the Psychology of Social Conflict and Aggression Sydney Symposium in Social Psychology

reSolution of Social conflictS 131

Whitetargets.Inourstudy,weexpectedMuslimtargets toelicitasimilarbias.Inessence,theshooter’sbiastaskisanunobtrusivebehavioralmeasureassessingaggressivereactionstonegativestereotypes(Forgas,1976,2003).

Partipantswereinstructedtoshootattargetsonacomputerscreenonlywhentheywerecarryingagun.WeusedmorphingsoftwaretocreatetargetsthatdidordidnotappearMuslimbywearingaturbanorthehijab(seeFigure 8.6).WeexpectedandfoundthatpeopletendedtoshootmoreattargetswithMuslimhead­gear,andthiseffectwasgreaterafterapositivemoodinduction(Figure 8.7).Thisresultconfirmsthatpositiveaffectfacilitatedanaggressivebehavioralresponsetoanegativestereotype,eveninagroupofotherwiseliberalandtolerantAustralianundergraduates.AsAustraliahasnotbeensubjecttoMuslimterroristattacksonitsterritory,othercountriesintheforefrontofMuslimterrorismsuchastheUnitedStates and Britain may showaneven stronger “turbaneffect” than the onewedemonstratedhere.Themost intriguingfindinghere is thatpositiveaffect trig­geredasignificantselectivebiasagainstMuslims,consistentwithrecenttheoriessuggesting that positive affect promotes top­down, assimilative processing thatfacilitates the influenceof stereotypesonautomatic responses (Bless&Fiedler,2006;Forgas,1998,2007).

Affective Influences on Interpersonal Strategies in the Dictator Game

Ifsomebodygaveyou$50todividebetweenyourselfandanotherpersonanywayyoulike,howmuchwouldyoukeepforyourself?Doesbeinginagoodorabadmoodinfluencesuchconflictdecisions?Peoplefaceaconflictbetweenbeingself­ishandbeingfairinmanyeverydaysituations,andthedilemmainherentinthesechoiceshasbeenamajortopicforphilosophersandwritersfordecades.Recentresearch in evolutionary psychology suggests that humans and other primates

figure 8.6 Theturbaneffect:stimulusfiguresusedtoassesstheeffectsofmoodandwearingornotwearinga turbanonautomatic aggressiveresponses.Participantshad tomakerapidshoot–don’tshootdecisionsinresponsetotargetswhodidordidnotholdagunanddidornotwearaMuslimheaddress(aturban).

Page 149: the Psychology of Social Conflict and Aggression Sydney Symposium in Social Psychology

JoSeph p. forgaS and hui Bing tan132

evolveda senseof justiceand fairnessasanadaptive strategy to constrain self­ishness and to maintain social cohesionandharmony (Forgas, Haselton, & vonHippel,2007).Doesmoodinfluencehowassertiveandselfishweareininterper­sonal situations?Weexplored thepossibility that a positivemood may increaseassertivenessandselfishnesswhereasasadmoodmayproducegreaterfairnessinthedictatorgame.Thisquestionhadnotbeeninvestigatedpreviously.Unlikepriorresearchonaltruism,thedictatorgameallowstheexplorationofmoodeffectsonpureselfishnessinasimpleallocationtask.

Traditionaleconomictheoriespredictthatarationalallocatorinthedictatorgameshouldmaximizeearningsandkeepmostoftheresources.Actualresearchsuggestsafarmorecomplexpattern.Infact,allocatorsoftengive30%,andeven50%,toothers(Bolton,Katok,&Zwick,1998;Forsythe,Horowitz,Savin,&Sefton,1994),suggestingthatbehaviorisgovernedbyasubtlecombinationoftheconflict­ingdemandsofself­interestandthenormoffairness(Haselhuhn&Mellers,2005;PillutlaandMurninghan,1995).Inthissituation,moodsmayinfluencebehaviorbysubtlyshiftingthewayallocatorsfocusonandinterpret internal(selfish)andexternal(fairnessnorm)information.Aswehaveseen,positivemoodsmaypro­moteamoreinternallyoriented,selfishprocessingstyle(Bless&Fiedler,2006).Incontrast,negativemoodseemstopromoteamoreexternallyfocused,accommoda­tiveprocessingstyle,withgreaterattentiontotheexternalnormsoffairness.

In the first experiment, volunteer students approached on campus receiveda false­feedback mood induction, and then they played the dictator game andmadeallocationseithertoanin­groupmember(studentintheirownfaculty)oranunknownperson.Moodwasinducedbygivingparticipantsabogussix­item“test

0.200

0.160

0.120

0.080

0.040

0.000

Diff

eren

tial R

espo

nse

Bias

Bet

a

Happy AngryNeutral

Mood

figure 8.7 Theeffectsofpositiveandnegativemoodonpeople’srelianceonstereotypesintheshooters’biastask:thoseinapositivemoodweremorelikelyandthoseinanegativemoodwere lesslikelytoselectivelyshootat targetswearingaturban.(AfterUnkelbach,Forgas&Denson,2008.Withpermission.)

Page 150: the Psychology of Social Conflict and Aggression Sydney Symposium in Social Psychology

reSolution of Social conflictS 133

ofcognitive­spatialabilities,”estimatingthesurfaceareaofrandomlysizedgeo­metricfiguresandprovidingpositiveornegativemanipulatedfeedbackdescrib­ing their performance as “outstanding” or “poor” to induce good or bad mood(e.g., Forgas, 2007). They were thenasked to allocate 10 raffle tickets betweenthemselvesandanotherperson,witha$20voucherastheultimateprize.Resultsshowed thathappy studentskeptmore raffle tickets thandid sad students, andtherewasalsoanonsignificanttrendforgreaterselfishnesstowardastrangerwheninapositivemood(Figure 8.8).Theseresultsconfirmthattransientmoodhadasignificantlyinfluenceonassertivenessandselfishness.

Experiment2replicatedthiseffectusingadifferentmoodinduction(affect­inducing films) and a more realistic allocation task in the laboratory, with thenamesandphotosofpartnersalsodisplayedforeachtasktoincreaserealism.Afterviewingfilmsdesignedto inducehappyorsadmoods,participantsperformedaseriesofallocationtasksdescribedasan“interpersonalgame”witheightrandomlyassignedothers,eachinvolvingtheallocationof10points.Happyindividualswereagainmoreselfishandkeptmorepointstothemselves thandidsad individuals,andtherewasalsoasignificantinteractionbetweenmoodandtheeighttrials.Asthetrialsprogressed,happyindividualsbecamemoreselfish,andsadindividualsbecamemorefair(Figure 8.9).

Inafurtherexperimentweexplicitlymanipulatedfairnessnormsbyprovidingallocatorswithinformationaboutthefairorunfairbehaviorsofpreviousplayerstoreinforceorunderminethesocialnormof fairness.Informationaboutunfairallocations should weaken the social norm and should increase the latitude forindividualdeliberations,thusincreasingthescopeformoodeffectstooccur.Afterviewingaffect­inducingfilms,participantsplayedtheallocationtask,afterbeingexposedtoinformationaboutfairorunfairoffersof“pastproposers”toemphasize

In-group member Stranger

Mea

n al

loca

tion

76.86.66.46.2

65.85.65.45.2

54.84.64.44.2

4

Positive moodNegative mood

figure 8.8 The effects of mood (good, bad) and relationship (in­group member vs.stranger)onthefairnessofallocations inadictatorgame,showingthemeannumberoftickets,outof10,individualskepttothemselvesineachcondition.

Page 151: the Psychology of Social Conflict and Aggression Sydney Symposium in Social Psychology

JoSeph p. forgaS and hui Bing tan134

ordeemphasizethefairnessnorm.Happyallocatorsweresignificantlymoreself­ishthanthesadgroup,andmoodeffectsonselfishnessaregreatestwhenfairnessnormwasundermined,allowinggreater scope forallocators toengage inopen,constructiveprocessingabouttheirchoices.

These experiments consistently show that happy mood increased assertive­ness and selfishness when allocating resources in the dictator game, an almostpuremeasureof selfishness.Moodeffectsweregreaterwhen thenormof fair­nesswasdeemphasized,asallocatorsweremorelikelytoprocess thetask inanopen,constructivemanner.Thesefindingsareconceptuallyconsistentwithpriorevidenceshowingthatpositiveaffectproducesmoreassertive,confident,andopti­misticinterpersonalstrategies,whereasnegativemoodtriggersmorepessimistic,cautiousresponsessensitivetoexternaldemands(Bless&Fiedler,2006;Fiedler,2001;Forgas,1999,2002).

This account is also broadly consistentwith functionalist evolutionary theo­riessuggestingthataffecthasasignalingfunctionaboutsituationalrequirements(Clore&Storbeck,2006;Forgasetal.,2007;Schwarz,1990),withnegativeaffectrecruitingamoreexternallyfocused,accommodativeorientation(Bless&Fiedler,2006).Positiveaffectinturnpromotesmoreassimilative,internallyfocusedstrate­gies,furtherenhancingthetendencyforselfishness(Bless,2001;Bless&Fiedler;Fiedler, 2001). Many conflict situations in our private as well as working livesinvolve decisions between acting assertively and selfishly and acting fairly. Thekindofmoodeffectsonassertivenessandselfishnessdemonstratedheremayhaveimportant implications for real­life conflict behaviors in personal relationships,organizationaldecisions,andmanyothereverydaysituationswheredecisionsbyonepersonhaveincontestableconsequencesforothers.

Summary and concluSionSThereislittledoubtthataffectivestatesplayanimportantroleininfluencinghowpeopleperceive,interpret,andrespondtoconflictsituations—aconnectionthat

1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 Number of Trials

Happy

Sad

Num

ber o

f poi

nts k

ept t

o se

lf 876543210

figure 8.9 Theeffectsofmoodonselfishnessversusfairness:happypersonskeepmorepointstothemselvesthandosadpeople,andtheseeffectsaremorepronouncedasalloca­tiontrialsprogress.

Page 152: the Psychology of Social Conflict and Aggression Sydney Symposium in Social Psychology

reSolution of Social conflictS 135

has been intuitively recognized since time immemorial. Theories suchas AIM(Forgas,1992,1995)offerasimpleandparsimoniousexplanationofwhenandhowaffectivestatesmayinfuseconflict­relatedthoughtsandbehaviorssuchasnego­tiation and making and responding to demands (Forgas, 1998a, 1998b, 1999a,1999b).Dealingwithsocialconflictrequirescomplexandelaborateinformationprocessingstrategies.Itistheveryrichnessandelaboratenessofconflictsituationsthatmakesmoodeffectsparticularlylikely,asevenaminorselectiveprimingofpositiveandnegativememory­basedinformationmayhavelargeconsequencesforwhatisperceived,howitisinterpreted,andthekindofresponsesthatarecon­structed.Criticaldecisionsandjudgmentsaboutconflictepisodesaremorelikelytobeassertive,confident,andoptimisticwhenapersonisinapositivemoodstateandaremorelikelytobeaccommodating,negative,orcriticalwhenthepersonisinadysphoricmood.

Inaddition,morerecentresearchalsoshowsthataffectivestatesalsoinfluencehowpeopledealwith social information.It turnsout thatmildnegativemoodscanhaveabeneficialeffectbyrecruitingmoreaccommodativeprocessingstyles,byreducingjudgmentalerrors,byimprovingthequalityofpersuasivearguments,byprovidingtheabilitytodetectdeception,andalsobyleadingtomoresensitiveandfairerallocationstrategies.Theprocessingeffectsofnegativemooddescribedhereseemparticularlyintriguing,sincethesestudiessuggestthatmilddysphoriacouldactuallyimprovecognitivestrategiesandevenresultinsuperioroutcomes(Forgas,2007).

Interestingly, these results also challenge the common assumption in muchof applied, organizational, clinical, and health psychology that positive affecthasuniversallydesirablesocialandcognitiveconsequences.Togetherwithotherrecentexperimentalstudies,ourfindingsconfirmthatnegativeaffectoftenpro­ducesadaptiveandmoresociallysensitiveoutcomes.Forexample,negativemoodscanreduce judgmentalerrors (Forgas,1988c),canimproveeyewitnessaccuracy(Forgas,Vargas,&Laham,2005),andcanimproveinterpersonalcommunicationstrategies(Forgas,2007),anditseems,asthepresentexperimentsshow,alsocanincrease fairness andsensitivity to theneedsofothers.There ismuch scope infuture work to explore mood effects on other kinds of strategic conflict­relatedbehaviors,suchasforgiveness(Chapter14inthisvolume),assertiveness(Chapter5inthisvolume),goalpreferences(Chapter6inthisvolume),perspectivetaking(Chapter7inthisvolume),andreactionstoostracism(Chapters3and13inthisvolume).

Although much has been discovered about the information­processing andrepresentationalfunctionsofaffectivestates,notenoughofthisevidencehassofarcomefromresearchdirectlyconcernedwithstudyingsocialconflicts.Thisisallthemoresurprisingasaffectandconflictarecloselyintertwined,andconflictbehaviorspresentaparticularlypromisingandecologicallyvalidresearchdomaintostudyaffectiveinfluencesonthoughtandbehavior.Giventhegrowingsophisti­cationofthetheoriesandmethodsnowemployedinresearchlookingattheinter­faceofaffectandcognition,thetimeseemsripetoapplythesestrategiestotheinvestigationoftheroleofaffectinthewayreal­lifeconflictisperceived,managed,andresolved.

Page 153: the Psychology of Social Conflict and Aggression Sydney Symposium in Social Psychology

JoSeph p. forgaS and hui Bing tan136

referenceS

Adolphs,R.,&Damasio,A.(2001).Theinteractionofaffectandcognition:Aneurobiologi­calperspective.InJ.P.Forgas(Ed.),The handbook of affect and social cognition(pp.27–49).Mahwah,NJ:Erlbaum.

Blascovich,J.,&Mendes,W.B.(2000).Challengeandthreatappraisals:Theroleofaffec­tivecues.InJ.Forgas(Ed.),Feeling and thinking: The role of affect in social cognition(pp.59–82).CambridgeUK:CambridgeUniversityPress.

Bless,H. (2001).Moodandtheuseofgeneralknowledgestructures.InL.L.Martin&G.L.Clore(Eds.),Theories of mood and cognition: A user’s guidebook(pp.9–26).Mahwah,NJ:LawrenceErlbaumAssociates.

Bless,H.,&Fiedler,K. (2006).Moodand theregulationof informationprocessingandbehavior.InJ.P.Forgas(Ed.),Affect in social thinking and behavior(pp.65–84).NewYork:PsychologyPress.

Bolton,G.E.,Katok,E.,&Zwick,R.(1998).Dictatorgamegiving:Rulesoffairnessversusactsofkindness.International Journal of Game Theory, 27(2),269–299.

Bower,G.H.(1981).Moodandmemory.American Psychologist,36,129–148.Ciarrochi,J.,Forgas,J.,&Mayer,J.(Eds.)(2006).Emotional intelligence in everyday life.

(2nded.).NewYork:PsychologyPress/Taylor&Francis.Clark,M.S.,&Isen,A.M.(1982).Towardunderstandingtherelationshipbetweenfeel­

ingstatesandsocialbehavior.InA.H.Hastorf&A.M.Isen(Eds.),Cognitive social psychology(pp.73–108).NewYork:Elsevier­NorthHolland.

Clore,G.L.,&Byrne,D.(1974).Areinforcement­affectmodelofattraction.InT.L.Huston(Ed.),Foundations of interpersonal attraction(pp.143–170).NewYork:AcademicPress.

Clore,G.L.,&Storbeck,J.(2006).Affectasinformationforsocialjudgment,behavior,andmemory.InJ.P.Forgas(Ed.),Affect in social cognition and behavior(pp.154–178).NewYork:PsychologyPress.

Correll,J.,Park,B.,Judd,C.M.,&Wittenbrink,B.(2002).Thepoliceofficer’sdilemma:Using ethnicity to disambiguate potentially threatening individuals. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,83,1314–1329.

East,R.,&Forgas,J.P.(2008).Mood effects on scepticism and the detection of deception.Manuscript,UniversityofNewSouthWales.

Feshbach,S.,&Singer,R.D.(1957).Theeffectsoffeararousalandsuppressionof fearuponsocialperception.Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology,55,283–288.

Fiedler,K. (1991). On the task, themeasures and the mood in research onaffect andsocialcognition.InJ.P.Forgas(Ed.),Emotion and social judgments(pp.83–104).Oxford,UK:Pergamon.

Fiedler,K.(2001).Affectiveinfluencesonsocialinformationprocessing.InJ.P.Forgas(Ed.),The handbook of affect and social cognition(pp.163–185).Mahwah,NJ:Erlbaum.

Fiedler,K.,&Bless,H.(2001).Theformationofbeliefsintheinterfaceofaffectiveandcognitiveprocesses.InN.Frijda,A.Manstead,&S.Bem(Eds.),Emotions and beliefs (pp.144–170).NewYork:CambridgeUniversityPress.

Fiedler,K.,&Forgas,J.P.(1988).Affect, cognition, and social behavior: New evidence and integrative attempts.Toronto:Hogrefe.

Forgas, J. P. (1976). An unobtrusive study of reactions to national stereotypes in fourEuropeancountries. Journal of Social Psychology,99,37–42.

Forgas,J.P.(1979).Social episodes: The study of interaction routines.London:AcademicPress.Forgas,J.P.(1982).Episodecognition:Internalrepresentationsofinteractionroutines.In

L.Berkowitz(Ed.),Advances in experimental social psychology(Vol.15,pp.59–100).NewYork:AcademicPress.

Page 154: the Psychology of Social Conflict and Aggression Sydney Symposium in Social Psychology

reSolution of Social conflictS 137

Forgas, J. P. (1992). On bad mood and peculiar people: Affect and person typicality inimpressionformation.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,62,863­875.

Forgas,J.P.(1994).Sadandguilty?Affectiveinfluencesontheexplanationofconflictepi­sodes.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,66,56–68.

Forgas,J.P.(1995a).Moodandjudgment:Theaffectinfusionmodel(AIM).Psychological Bulletin,117(1),39–66.

Forgas,J.P.(1995b).Strangecouples:Moodeffectsonjudgmentsandmemoryaboutproto­typicalandatypicaltargets.Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin,21,747–765.

Forgas, J.P. (1998a).Onfeelinggoodandgettingyourway:Moodeffectsonnegotiatorcognitionandbargainingstrategies.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,74, 565–577.

Forgas, J.P. (1998b).Askingnicely?Theeffectsofmoodonresponding tomoreor lesspoliterequests.Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin,24,173–185.

Forgas,J.P.(1998c).Onbeinghappyandmistaken:Moodeffectsonthefundamentalattri­butionerror.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,75,318–331.

Forgas,J.P.(1999a).Onfeelinggoodandbeingrude:Affectiveinfluencesonlanguageuseandrequestformulations.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,76,928–939.

Forgas,J.P.(1999b).Feelingandspeaking:Moodeffectsonverbalcommunicationstrate­gies.Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin,25,850–863.

Forgas,J.P.(Ed.).(2001).Handbook of affect and social cognition.Mahwah,NJ:Erlbaum.Forgas, J. P. (2002). Feeling and doing: Affective influences on interpersonal behavior.

Psychological Inquiry,13,1–28.Forgas,J.P.(2003).Whydon’twedoitintheroad…?Stereotypingandprejudiceinmun­

danesituations.Psychological Inquiry,14,251–257.Forgas,J.P.(Ed.).(2006).Affect in social thinking and behavior.NewYork:PsychologyPress.Forgas,J.P.(2007).Whensadisbetterthanhappy:Negativeaffectcanimprovethequality

andeffectivenessofpersuasivemessagesandsocial influencestrategies.Journal of Experimental Social Psychology,43,513–528.

Forgas,J.P.,Bower,G.H.,&Krantz,S.(1984).Theinfluenceofmoodonperceptionsofsocialinteractions.Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 20,497–513.

Forgas,J.P.,Haselton,M.G.,&vonHippel,W.(Eds.)(2007).Evolutionary psychology and social cognition.NewYork:PsychologyPress.

Forgas,J.P.,Levinger,G.,&Moylan,S.(1994).Feelinggoodandfeelingclose:Affectiveinfluences on the perception of intimate relationships. Personal Relationships, 1,165–184.

Forgas,J.P.,&Moylan,S.J.(1987).Afterthemovies:Theeffectsoftransientmoodstatesonsocialjudgments.Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin,13,478–489.

Forgas,J.P.,Vargas,P.,&Laham,S.(2005).Moodeffectsoneyewitnessmemory:Affectiveinfluences on susceptibility to misinformation. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology,41,574–588.

Forsythe,R.,Horowitz,J.L.,Savin,N.E.,&Sefton,M.(1994).Fairnessinsimplebargain­ingexperiments.Games and Economic Behavior,6(3),347–369.

Gilbert,D.T.(1991).Howmentalsystemsbelieve.American Psychologist,46,107–119.Gilbert,D.T.,&Malone,P.S. (1995).Thecorrespondencebias.Psychological Bulletin,

117,21–38.Haselhuhn,M.P.,&Mellers,B.A.(2005).Emotionsandcooperationineconomicgames.

Cognitive Brain Research,23,24–33.Hilgard,E.R.(1980).Thetrilogyofmind:Cognition,affection,andconation.Journalof the

History of the Behavioral Sciences,16,107–117.Jones,E.E.,&Harris,V.A.(1967).Theattributionofattitudes.Journal of Experimental

Social Psychology,3,1–24.

Page 155: the Psychology of Social Conflict and Aggression Sydney Symposium in Social Psychology

JoSeph p. forgaS and hui Bing tan138

Niedenthal,P.,&Halberstadt,J. (2000).Groundingcategoriesinemotionalresponse.InJ. P. Forgas (Ed.), Feeling and thinking: The role of affect in social cognition (pp.357–386).NewYork:CambridgeUniversityPress.

Pervin,L.A.(1976).Afree­responsedescriptionapproachtotheanalysisofperson­situa­tioninteraction.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,34,465–474.

Pillutla,M.M.,&Murnighan,J.K.(1995).Beingfairorappearingfair:Strategicbehaviorinultimatumbargaining.Academy of Management Journal,38,1408–1426.

Rusting,C.(2001).Personalityasamoderatorofaffectiveinfluencesoncognition.InJ.P.Forgas(Ed.),The handbook of affect and social cognition (pp.371–391). Mahwah,NJ:Erlbaum.

Schwarz,N.(1990).Feelingsasinformation:Informationalandmotivationalfunctionsofaffectivestates.InE.T.Higgins&R.Sorrentino(Eds.),Handbook of motivation and cognition: Foundations of social behavior(Vol.2,pp.527–561).NewYork:GuilfordPress.

Schwarz,N.,&Clore,G. L. (1983).Mood,misattributionand judgmentsofwellbeing:Informative and directive functions of affective states. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,45,513–523.

SedikidesC.(1992).Moodasadeterminantofattentionalfocus.Cognition and Emotion,6,129–148.

Sedikides,C.(1995).Centralandperipheralself­conceptionsaredifferentially influencedbymood:Testsof thedifferentialsensitivityhypothesis.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,69(4),759–777.

Tan,H.B.,&Forgas,J.P.(2010).Whenhappinessmakesusselfish,butsadnessmakesusfair:Affectiveinfluencesoninterpersonalstrategiesinthedictatorgame.Journal of Experimental Social Psychology,46(3),571–576.

Unkelbach,C.,Forgas,J.P.,&Denson,T.F.(2008).Theturbaneffect:TheinfluenceofMuslimheadgearandinducedaffectonaggressiveresponsesintheshooterbiaspara­digm. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 44,1409–1413.

Wegener,D.T.,&Petty,R.E.(1997).Theflexiblecorrectionmodel:Theroleofnaïvetheo­riesofbiasinbiascorrection.InM.P.Zanna(Ed.),Advances in experimental social psychology(Vol.29,pp.141–208).NewYork:AcademicPress.

Zajonc,R.B.(2000).Feelingandthinking:Closingthedebateovertheindependenceofaffect.InJ.P.Forgas(Ed.),Feeling and thinking: The role of affect in social cognition(pp.31–58).NewYork:CambridgeUniversityPress.

Page 156: the Psychology of Social Conflict and Aggression Sydney Symposium in Social Psychology

139

9TheEffectsofAngerandAnger

RegulationonNegotiationTHOMASF.DENSONandEMMAC.FABIANSSON

University of New South Wales

N egotiation is a means of resolving social and economic conflict, whichsometimes evokes negative emotions. Recent theoretical approachesacknowledge the consequences that emotions and mood can have on

negotiations(Bazerman,Curhan,Moore,&Valley,2000;Morris&Keltner,2000;Shapiro,2002;seealsoChapter8inthisvolume).Thisreviewfocusesontherolethat anger plays in negotiations. Anger is important to regulate because it canleadtoanescalationofconflict(Allred,Mallozzi,Matsui,&Raia,1997).Wefirstexplorewhyangerregulationisimportantdespitethesometimespositiveeffectsofexpressingangerduringnegotiation.Nextweexaminetheeffectivenessofdiffer­entemotionregulationstrategiessuchasreappraisal,rumination,anddistractionanddiscusshowthesecanbeappliedtothenegotiationcontext.Wethenpresenttheresultsfromtwoexperimentsusingemotionregulationtoexplorewhatimpacttheseangerregulationstrategieshaveonself­reportedemotionandonaggressivebehaviorinnegotiations.

expreSSing anger iS a limited But at timeS effective Strategy

Whetheranegotiatorsimplyexpressesorexperiencesangercanresultinverydif­ferentnegotiationoutcomes.Angercanbeexaminedfromanintrapersonalper­spective(i.e.,feltanger)oraninterpersonalperspective(i.e.,theeffectsofangerexpressiononothers;VanKleef,VanDijk,Steinel,Harinck,&VanBeest,2008).Generally,intrapersonalangerinnegotiationsisthoughttoresultinpoorernego­tiationoutcomesthaninterpersonalanger(VanKleefetal.).Forexample,intraper­sonalangercanproducestalemates,conflict,andeconomicallyirrationalbehavior

Page 157: the Psychology of Social Conflict and Aggression Sydney Symposium in Social Psychology

thomaS f. denSon and emma c. faBianSSon140

(Allredetal.,1997;Liu,2009;Pillutla&Murnighan,1996).Bycontrast,expressingangercanresultinfinancialgainbyencouragingopponentstomakeconcessions.Forexample,asalespersonmaybelikelytogiveintoanangrycustomerdemand­ingadiscounttoavoidfurtherescalationofconflictandtominimizedisruptiontoothercustomers.

Despite these sometimes positive benefits, strategically using anger toobtaindemandsisa limitedshort­termstrategy.Forexample,angercanneg­atively impact relationship quality and make people less willing to negotiateagaininthefuture(Allredetal.,1997).Overtimenegotiatingcounterpartsmayhabituate toangerexpressionsandtheymayno longerbeeffective (Tiedens,2001). For example, an angry outburst may be effective the first time; how­ever,thesecondtimeonetriesthisstrategy,theothernegotiatormayresistthedemands.Furthermore,over timeanangrynegotiatormaydevelopanargu­mentativereputation thatcouldnegatively influencesubsequentnegotiations.Therefore,expressingangerisdoubtfulasaneffectivelong­termstrategyandmaybeeffectiveonlyinsingleinstancesofnegotiation.Moreover,evenduringone­timenegotiationsamongstrangers,researchsuggeststhatexpressingangerrequiresveryspecificconditionstobeeffective.Thesevariables includehow,when,who,andwheretheangerisexpressed.ToquoteAristotle,“Anyonecanbecomeangry.Thatiseasy.Buttobeangrywiththerightperson,totherightdegree,at theright time, for therightpurposeand in therightway—that isnoteasy.”

How Anger Is Expressed

Angercanbeexpressedinanumberofdifferentways.Gibson,Schweitzer,Callister,andGray(2009)examinedwhichcharacteristicsareneededforexpressingangerto result in constructive negotiation outcomes in organizational settings. Angerepisodesfromavarietyoforganizationswereanalyzed,andtheconsequencesoftheseepisodeswereexaminedbyanalyzingtherespondent’sperceivedimpactoftheepisodeonoutcomesatthe individualandorganizational level.Theauthorsalsoexaminedtheeffectofangerexpressionontherelationshipbetweenthepar­tiesinvolvedintheevent.Positiveoutcomesweremorelikelyiftheangerepisodeswerelowinintensity,wereexpressedverballyratherthanphysically,andweredis­playedinorganizationswhereexpressingangerisconsideredthenorm.However,expressinglow­intensityangerisdifficult,requirescontrol,andifdisplayedincor­rectlymayresultinconflictescalationorstalemate(Gibsonetal.).Therefore,bothhow anger is expressed and the context in which it is expressed are importantdeterminantsofitseffectiveness.

When Anger Is Expressed

Theeffectivenessofexpressingangermayalsodependonwhen in thenegotia­tionangerisexpressed(e.g.,atthebeginningofthenegotiation,duringtheposi­tioning phase,during problem solving,or at theend of thenegotiation; Morris&Keltner,2000).Thepositioningstageiswhenoneorbothnegotiatorsexpress

Page 158: the Psychology of Social Conflict and Aggression Sydney Symposium in Social Psychology

the effectS of anger and anger regulation on negotiation 141

theirnegotiationstancetotheirpartner—forexample,whatpointstheywouldbewillingtoconcedeandrefusetocompromise.Angerexpressedduringtheposition­ingstagemayresultingaininganupperhandthroughcoercivepressure.Angerexpressedduringthebargainingstagemaysignaldissatisfactionwiththeofferandencouragetheircounterparttoofferamoresatisfactoryoffer.However,negativeeffectsmayoccurwhenangerisexpressedduringotherstages.Forexample,angerexpressedduringtheinitialstagesofthenegotiationmayproduceastalemateorunwillingnesstonegotiateinthefirstplace.

Who Expresses Anger

Whether anger is an effective strategy for claiming value also depends on whoexpressestheangerandtheirbargainingposition.Themoderatingeffectsofgen­der, status, and the number of alternative offers have been investigated in thisregard. Individuals in a high­power position (operationalized as the number ofavailablealternatives)aremorelikelytogainfromexpressingangerwhentheyarepairedwithalow­poweropponentwhopossesseslimitedalternatives(Friedmanetal.,2004;Sinaceur&Tiedens,2006;VanKleef&Côté,2007).Forexample,participantswithfewormanyclientstochoosefromreadvignettesandratedhowmuchtheywouldconcedetoaclientthatexpressedangerordidnotexpressanger.Whereasparticipantswithgoodalternativesconcededsimilaramountstononan­gry and angry clients, when participants had poor alternatives they were morelikelytoconcedetoanangryclientthananonangryclient(Sinaceur&Tiedens).

Similarly,VanKleefandCôté(2007)observedthatwhenparticipantshadfewalternatives to choose from and their fictitious opponent expressed anger usingwrittenstatementssuchas“Thisoffermakesmereallyangry;Iexpectabetteroffer,” participants demanded less from angry opponents than a nonemotionalnegotiator. Participants tend to concede more to angry opponents because theexpressed anger is thought to signal that opponents have higher demands (VanKleef,DeDreu,&Manstead,2004;VanDijk,VanKleef,Steinel,&VanBeest,2008).

Expressingangercanalsobeusedtoexertanillusionofpowerandcompetence.Inaseriesofexperiments,Tiedens(2001)illustratedthatangerexpressionsinflu­encetheextenttowhichpeopleconferorbestowstatustoothers.Coworkerswhoratedtheircolleaguesashighlylikelytodisplayangeralsotendedtoconferthesecolleagueswithhigherstatus(i.e.,highersalariesandgreaterlikelihoodofpromo­tion).However,additionalresearchrevealsthattheassociationbetweenangerandstatusisdifferentformenandwomen(Brescoll&Uhlmann,2008).Incontrasttoanangryprofessionalman,professionalwomenwhoexpressedangerwere con­ferredlowerstatusregardlessoftheiroccupationalstatus(chiefexecutiveofficerorassistanttrainee).Womenwhoexpressedangerwereallocatedlowerwages,sta­tus,andperceivedaslesscompetentthanunemotionalwomenorangrymen.Inaddition, theextent towhich womenwere conferred lower status depended onwhethertheangerwasattributedasduetointernalcharacteristics(e.g.,personal­ity)orexternalcharacteristics(e.g.,thesituation).Whenexternalattributionswereprovidedforexpressionsofangerinprofessionalwomentheywereawardedhigher

Page 159: the Psychology of Social Conflict and Aggression Sydney Symposium in Social Psychology

thomaS f. denSon and emma c. faBianSSon142

statusthanwomenwithoutanexternalattributionbutnothigherstatusthannon­emotionalwomen.Therefore,theadvantagesassociatedwithexpressingangerdonotextendtoeveryone,andtheeffectivenessofexpressingangerisconstrainedbyvariablesincludinggenderandpower.

Where Anger Is Expressed

Theeffectivenessofexpressingangeralsodependsonwhereitisexpressed.Displayrules dictate how acceptable expressing anger in the workplace is, and organi­zationalculturepartiallydetermineswhichemotionsareconsidereddesirabletodisplay(Barsade&Gibson,2007;Gibsonetal.,2009;Morris&Feldman,1996).Forexample, incustomer service–basedoccupationssuchasairlines, telephoneservices, and health care, expressing anger is discouraged (Barsade & Gibson;Morris&Feldman),suggestingthatindividualsintheseoccupationsaretypicallyeffective at regulating anger displays. By contrast, some occupations encouragethe expression of anger (e.g., ice hockey player, lobbyist, opposition politician,radioshockjock).Thus,organizationalnormsforexpressingangercandeterminewhetherexpressingangerhasbeneficialordetrimentalconsequences(Gibsonetal.). Collectively, the limitations associated with expressing anger illustrate thatexpressingangertocreategainsisconstrainedbynumerousboundaryconditions,suggestingthatmoreoftenthannotexpressingangerislikelytobeineffectiveorevendetrimentalinmanycontexts.

hoW can anger Be regulated?Studiesexaminingtheexpressionofangerhavetypicallyinvestigatedthephenom­enonfromtheperspectiveofthereceiverratherthanexaminingthebidirectionaleffectsofthesenderandreceiveronangerexperience.Computersimulationsofnegotiationsareoftenusedtoexaminethesevariables,whichmanipulateangerthroughtheuseofwrittencommentsusedtocommunicateangertothepartici­pantduringthedeal­makingstagesofthenegotiation(VanKleef&Côté,2007;VanDijketal.,2008).Althoughmanyof thesestudies illustrate thatexpressingangerresultsinbetterindividualnegotiationoutcomes,theydonottakeintocon­siderationtheimpactthatexpressingangermayhaveonthesender.Forexample,theperipheralfeedbackeffect(alsoknownasthefacialfeedbackhypothesis)sug­gests that expressing emotions can lead to experiencing those emotions (Laird,1984).Ifexpressingangerresultsinexperiencinganger,thentheimpactofexpress­ingangermayhavethesamedetrimentalconsequencesassociatedwithstudiesofintrapersonalangerinnegotiations.Experiencingangermayinterfereinnegotia­tionsbecauseangercanleadtogreatercompetitivenessanddesireforretaliation(Allredetal.,1997;Pillutla&Murnighan,1996).Negotiatorswhoseekretaliationtendtobenarrow­mindedandmorefocusedontheirowninterestswhilebeinglessawareofalternativesolutionsorjointgains(Liu,2009;seealsoChapter10inthisvolume).

Together, the literature reviewedherein suggests that regulating angerdur­ingnegotiationsmightproveefficacious.Previousrecommendationsforreducing

Page 160: the Psychology of Social Conflict and Aggression Sydney Symposium in Social Psychology

the effectS of anger and anger regulation on negotiation 143

angerhaveincludedstrategiessuchasremovingoneselffromtheconflictorvent­ing anger (Fisher&Ury,1991). However, avoidingor leaving the situationmaybe impractical and does nothing to resolve the issue. Methods such as ventinganger tend to increase rather than decrease aggression (Brown, Westbrook, &Challagalla,2005;Bushman,2002;Bushman,Baumeister,&Stack,1999).Instead,anumberofemotionregulationstrategiesareavailable,andthesedifferintermsoftheireffectiveness.Emotionregulationcanbedefinedas“theprocessesbywhichindividualsinfluencewhichemotionstheyhave,whentheyhavethem,andhowtheyexperienceandexpresstheseemotions”(Gross,1998b,p.275).Therearetwogeneralclassesofemotionregulationstrategies:antecedentfocusedandresponsefocused.

Cognitive reappraisal is a widely studied example of an antecedent­focusedstrategy.Reappraisalinvolvesinterpretingananger­elicitingeventbyadoptinganeutralorobjectiveperspectivetoreducetheemotionalimpactoftheevent(Ray,Wilhelm,&Gross,2008).Forexample,insteadoffixatingonwhatwentwronginanegotiation,reappraisalmayinvolvefocusingonfuturechangesthatcanbemadetoimprovesubsequentnegotiations.Asreappraisaliseffortfulandinvolvescognitivechange,reappraisalhasalsobeenlabeleda“deepactingstrategy”(Grandey,2000).Reappraisalworksbestwhenit isappliedbeforethefullonsetoftheemotionalresponse(Gross,1998a)andshouldthereforebemosteffectivewhenengagedinpriortonegotiation.

Oneresponse­focusedstrategythathasbeeninvestigatedwithinthecontextofangerisrumination.Ruminationinvolvesfocusingonone’semotionsandfeel­ingswithoutconstructiveproblemsolving(Nolen­Hoeksema,1991).Forinstance,thismayincludedwellingontheinflexibilityofone’snegotiationpartnerandtheirreluctancetoyieldinanegotiationratherthanfocusingonalternativewaysofcre­atingvalue.Ruminationalsomaintainsangerandincreasesaggression(Bushman,Bonacci,Pedersen,Vasquez,&Miller,2005;Rusting&Nolen­Hoeksema,1998).There are several different types of rumination. Analytical rumination involvesthinkingaboutwhyanangerprovocationoccurredandtheconsequencesofthisevent(Wimalaweera&Moulds,2008).Experientialruminationisatypeofpro­cessingthat focusesoncurrentandconcreteexperiences, forexample,howyoucurrentlyfeel(Watkins&Teasdale,2008).

Otherexamplesofresponse­focusedstrategies includefakingandsuppress­ingemotions.Thesestrategiestendnottoproducecognitivechangebutinsteadfocusonmaskingfeltemotions(Grandey,2000).Usingacallcentersimulationwithahostilecustomer,Goldberg andGrandey (2007) found thatmoreerrorsoccurredwhenplacingorderswhenresponse­focusedstrategieswereusedthanwhenantecedent­focusedstrategieswereused.Emotionregulationnotonlyinflu­ences how we experience emotions but can also negatively impact job perfor­mance.Response­focusedstrategiessuchasruminationtendtobelesseffectiveinreducingangerandaggressivebehaviorbecausetheyareoftenassociatedwiththedepletionofself­regulatoryresourceswhereasantecedent­focusedstrategiessuch as reappraisal tend not to result in depletion when initiated prior to thefullemotionalresponse(Denson,2009;Goldberg&Grandey;Grandey,Fisk,&Steiner,2005).

Page 161: the Psychology of Social Conflict and Aggression Sydney Symposium in Social Psychology

thomaS f. denSon and emma c. faBianSSon144

In emotion regulation studies, mental distraction is often used as a controlcondition.However,distractioncanbeconceptualizedasanemotionregulationstrategyinitsownright.Insteadoffocusingonfeelings,distractioninvolvesdraw­ingattentiontoneutralorpositivestimuliunrelatedtotheanger­inducingevent(Nolen­Hoeksema,1991).Distractionismoreeffectivethanruminationinreduc­ingangerandaggression(Bushman,2002;Bushmanetal.,2005;Denson,White,& Warburton, 2009; Rusting & Nolen­Hoeksema, 1998). For instance, RustingandNolen­Hoeksemaexperimentallymanipulatedruminationanddistractionandfoundthatcomparedwithruminationdistractiondecreasedanger(Experiment3)orhadnoimpactonself­reportedanger(Experiment1).Furthermore,participantswhoengagedindistractionwrotelessangrystoriesincontrasttoparticipantsintheruminationcondition.

Anumberofadditionalvariables influencewhatemotionregulationstrategyis used. For example, how stress is appraised can impact whether one engagesin adaptive or maladaptive emotion regulation strategies (Grandey, Dickter, &Sin,2004).Grandeyetal.examinedhowcallcenteremployeesappraisedangrycustomers and what emotion regulation strategies individuals used. Employeesrecalledarecenteventinwhichacustomerwasaggressiveandratedthelevelofstress and the emotion regulation strategy theyused.Ventingor surface actingstrategiesweremorelikelytobeusedwhenemployeesappraisedthecustomerasthreatening.Engagingincognitivereframingordeepactingstrategiessuchasper­spectivetakingwasmorelikelywhenthecustomerwasinterpretedasnotthreat­ening(seeChapter7 in thisvolume).Anotherfactorthat influencesthetypeofemotionregulationstrategyusedisthelevelofcontrolindividualspossesswithintheiroccupation(Grandeyetal.).

Onlyafewstudieshavespecificallyexaminedtheeffectivenessofreappraisalasanangerregulationstrategy(Denson,Moulds,&Grisham,2009;Mauss,Cook,Cheng,&Gross,2007;Rayetal.,2008).Maussetal.examinedwhetherindividualdifferencesinreappraisalwererelatedtoself­reportedangerandcardiovascularresponses.TheEmotionRegulationQuestionnaire(Gross&John,2003)wasusedtoclassifyhighandlowreappraisers.Participantswereprovokedusingabackwardcountingtaskwithfalsefeedback.Highreappraisersdisplayedmoreadaptivecar­diovascular responses and less self­reported anger in contrast to low reapprais­ers.However,thisexperimentreliedonpreexistingdifferencesinreappraisalandcouldnotdetermineifgenerallyinducingreappraisalwouldbeassociatedwiththesamebenefitsorwhetherreappraisal trainingwouldbeeffective for individualswhotendnottonaturallyreappraise.

By contrast, Ray et al. (2008) induced reappraisal and rumination by usingguided instructions. For example, when participants in the reappraisal condi­tionrecalledanangryepisodetheywere instructedtothinkabouttheeventasif they were an objective observer. Reappraisal was associated with less angeranddecreased physiological respondingcomparedwith rumination (Ray et al.).Collectivelytheseresultsillustratethebenefitsofreappraisalcomparedwithotheremotionregulationstrategies.

Densonetal.(2009)examinedtheeffectivenessofdifferentangerregulationstrategiesinresponsetorecallinganangrymemory.Participantswereallocated

Page 162: the Psychology of Social Conflict and Aggression Sydney Symposium in Social Psychology

the effectS of anger and anger regulation on negotiation 145

tooneoffourconditions:(1)cognitivereappraisal;(2)analyticalrumination;(3)distraction;and(4)ano­instructioncontrolconditioninwhichtheywroteaboutananger­inducingautobiographicalevent for20minutes.Aspredicted,rumina­tionwasassociatedwithmaintenanceof self­reportedanger,whereas theotherconditionsdecreasedanger.Reappraisalwasthemosteffectiveofthefourstrate­giesforreducingangeramongindividualshighinanger­relatedtraits.Tofurtherexamine thecognitiveprocessesunderlyingtheemotionregulation strategies, aquantitativecontentanalysisofthewrittenresponsesfromthewritingtaskwereanalyzedusingtheLinguisticInquiryWordCountprogram(Pennebaker,Chung,Ireland,Gonzales,&Booth,2007).Participantswhoreappraisedusedincreasedfuture tense and positive words, whereas participants in the rumination condi­tionusedincreasedpasttenseandgreaternegativeemotionwords.Theseresultsconvergewithotherstudiesexaminingangerregulation(Maussetal.,2007;Rayetal.,2008).

neural evidence for emotion regulation during negotiation

Thereisevidencetosuggestthatangerregulationmaybeespeciallybeneficialinsituationswhereacceptingapooroffer isobjectivelybetterthanrejectingapooroffer.However,acceptingapooroffer isparticularlydifficult todowhenangryasangerproducesadesiretopunishunfairnegotiationpartners(Pillutla&Murnighan,1996).PillutlaandMurnighanfoundthattherejectionoflowoffersinaneconomicbargaininggamewasnotstrictlyduetounfairnessoftheofferbutrathertotheinsultingconnotationsassociatedwithacceptingthepooroffersuchas the threat toparticipants’ self­worth.Effectiveanger regulationmighthelpreducethenegativeemotionalexperienceandbehavioralconsequencesassoci­atedwithexperiencingangerwhennegotiating.Toourknowledgeemotionregu­lationstrategieshavenotyetbeensystematicallyinvestigatedwithinthecontextofnegotiations.

Evidencefortheeconomicbenefitofregulatingangercomesfromtworecentfunctional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) studies using the UltimatumGame(Sanfey,Rilling,Aronson,Nystrom,&Cohen,2003;Tabibnia,Satpute,&Lieberman,2008).IntheUltimatumGame,oneplayerchooseshowtodivideamonetaryamount(typically$10)betweenhimselforherselfandanotherplayer.The second player chooses whether to accept or reject the offer. If the offer isaccepted,themoneyisdividedbetweenthetwoparticipants.However,ifthepro­posalisrejectedthenbothparticipantsreceivenothing(Güth,Schmittberger,&Schwarze,1982).Therefore,thelatterplayerhasthepossibilityofpunishinghisorheropponentforchoosingtodividethemoneyunfairlybutatthesametimesuf­fersacost.Thistypeofbehaviorisknownasaltruistic punishmentbecauseonechoosestopunishatapersonalcost(Fehr&Gächter,2002).InthecontextoftheUltimatumGame,itismoreeconomicallyrationaltoacceptanofferregardlessofhowlowtheofferis.Thisisbecauseacceptingasmallamountisobjectivelybetterthanrejectingtheofferandreceivingnothing.

Page 163: the Psychology of Social Conflict and Aggression Sydney Symposium in Social Psychology

thomaS f. denSon and emma c. faBianSSon146

Sanfeyetal.(2003)andTabibniaetal.(2008)illustratethatreceivingunfairofferssuchas$1and$2(outofapossible$10)isassociatedwithincreasedactiv­ity in theanterior insula,aregion implicated innegativeemotionalexperiencesincludingangerandrumination(Denson,Pedersen,Ronquillo,&Nandy,2009).Accepting unfair offers requires the ability to regulate negative emotions. Theventrolateralprefrontalcortex(VLPFC)hasbeenimplicatedinemotionregula­tion(Lieberman,2007).Accordingly,acceptingmoreunfairofferswasassociatedwith increasedactivity in the right VLPFC and decreased activity in the ante­rior insula. Similarly, adults with damage to the ventromedial prefrontal cortex(VMPFC),whichisassociatedwithemotionregulationandsocialfunctioning,aremorelikelytorejectunfairoffersintheUltimatumGamethanmatchedcontrolswithoutVMPFCdamage(Koenigs&Tranel,2007).Thesefindingsconvergeonthenotionthatemotionregulationisimportantinpromotingrationalthinkingandtheavoidanceofaggressiveretaliationduringbargaining.

emotion regulation in negotiationSFabiansson and Denson (2010) examined whether regulating anger using reap­praisalwouldnotonlydecreaseself­reportedangerbutalsoimprovenegotiationperformance.Participantsinthereappraisalconditionweretoldpriortothespeechtaskthattheirpartnerwasinabitofabadmoodandnottotakeitpersonallyiftheydoappeartobeinabadmood.Thistimingwasimportantastheoreticallyreappraisalshouldoccurbeforethefullonsetofanemotionalresponsetochangethe experience of the emotion (Gross, 1998a). Indeed, late reappraisal is moreeffortfulthanearlyreappraisal,andlatereappraisalwaslesseffectiveinreducingasadmoodinducedbyafilmthanlatedistraction(Sheppes&Meiran,2007).

Participantsengagedinaspeechtaskcalled“unilink”withaconfederateviawebcamandspokeaboutpersonaltopicssuchastheirlifegoals.Followingthis,participantswereprovokedwithinsultingfeedbackstatingthattheirspeechwasofpoorqualityforauniversitystudentandthatlisteningtotheirspeechwasawasteoftime.Participantswereledtobelievethatthiswassentfromtheirspeechpart­ner.Followingthis,participantsreappraised,ruminated,orengagedindistractionfor 20 minutes. Participants who reappraised were given the following instruc­tions:“Describeyourexperienceoftheunilinktaskinawaythatmakesyouadoptaneutralattitude.”Participantsintheruminationconditionwereaskedto“writeaboutthefeelingsyouhaveabouttheotherpeopleyouhaveencounteredinthestudy.”Instructions inthedistractionconditionconsistedofemotionallyneutralitemssimilartoRustingandNolen­Hoeksema(1998;e.g.,“Writeaboutthelayoutoftheaislesatyourlocalsupermarket”).

Followingtheemotionregulationinduction,participantsplayedtheUltimatumGamewiththreebogusparticipants.Thesenegotiationcounterpartsconsistedoftheprovocativeparticipantfromthepriorspeechtaskandtwootherparticipantsnotpreviouslyencountered.First,participantsproposedanoffertoallthreecoun­terpartspriortobeginningthegame.Second,participantsplayedtheroleoftheresponderandreceivedmultipleoffersfromtheseparticipants.Halfoftheofferswerefair (e.g.,$5,$4),andtheotherhalfwereunfair (e.g.,$1,$2).Participants

Page 164: the Psychology of Social Conflict and Aggression Sydney Symposium in Social Psychology

the effectS of anger and anger regulation on negotiation 147

decidedwhethertoacceptorrejecttheseoffers.Oneachtrialparticipantssawapictureoftheircounterpart(asinSanfeyetal.,2003).Thetwoplayersnotencoun­teredbeforeandtheinsultingopponentgaveexactlythesameoffers inrandomorderssothatthesecouldbecompared.Nextparticipantsratedtheirmoodatthebeginningofthestudy,postfeedback,duringthewritingandnegotiationtask,andattheconclusionoftheexperiment.Participantsalsoratedtheiropponentsonavarietyofnegotiation­relevanttraits(e.g.,trustworthy,competitive).

As expected, there were no significant differences in self­reported anger atbaseline among the three conditions. Manipulationchecks showed that partici­pants in the ruminationcondition felt emotional more often and more stronglyduring the writing task than participants in the other conditions. In addition,participantsinthereappraisalconditionreportedreflectingmoreonthepositivefeaturesof the speech taskand thinkingabout it fromanobjectiveperspectivecomparedwiththeremainingconditions.

Importantly,reappraisalwasassociatedwiththemostadaptiveemotionalresponse.Earlyreappraisalwasusedinthisexperiment,andtheresultsshowedthatparticipantswhoreappraisedwerelessimpactedbytheinitialinsultandsustainedlowerlevelsofangerthroughouttheexperiment.Specifically, followingtheinsultingfeedback,allconditions reported increasedanger;however, reappraisal was associatedwith thesmallestincreaseinanger.Duringthewritingtask,bothreappraisalanddistractionwereassociatedwithadecreaseinanger.Interestingly,duringthenegotiationtask,participantsinthedistractionconditionreportedincreasedangertoalevelequivalenttothoseintheruminationcondition.Reappraisalwasassociatedwithlessangercom­paredwiththedistractionandruminationconditionsduringthenegotiationphase.Similarly,attheconclusionoftheexperiment,reappraisalwasassociatedwithsignifi­cantlylessangercomparedwiththedistractionandruminationconditions.

These findings illustrate the effectiveness of reappraisal and distraction forreducing anger within a negotiation context. Furthermore, our results suggestthatdistractionmightserveasa“quickfix”forreducinganger,butitseffectsdonot last. Once the negotiation began, participants in the distraction conditionbecame angry again. This is presumably because distraction does not facilitateeffectiveprocessingoftheanger­inducingeventasreappraisalispresumedtodo.Presumably, participants in the reappraisal and rumination conditions thoughtabouttheirspeechtaskopponentandtheprovocationduringthewritingtaskinadaptiveandmaladaptivemanners,respectively.Bycontrast,participants inthedistraction conditionwere asked to think about topics entirelyunrelated to thespeech task. Thus, when participants in the distraction condition encounteredtheir speechcounterpartagain in thenegotiation task, thismayhave remindedthemoftheinitialprovocationandtherebyresultedinanincreasedsenseofangeratseeingtheircounterpart’spicture.

Participants proposed offers before playing the role of the responder. Asexpected, participants who reappraised proposed more generous offers to theperson that insulted them than either the ruminationordistraction conditions.Theruminationconditionproposedoffers thatwere inbetweenthe reappraisalanddistraction conditions.Whenparticipants played the roleof the responder,theemotionregulationstrategythatparticipantsengagedindidnotsignificantly

Page 165: the Psychology of Social Conflict and Aggression Sydney Symposium in Social Psychology

thomaS f. denSon and emma c. faBianSSon148

influencethenumberofoffersaccepted.Ascanbeexpected,participantsproposedfaireroffersandacceptedagreaternumberofoffersfromthetwoopponentsnotencounteredbeforeregardlessofemotionregulationcondition.Thisisnotsurpris­inggiventhatthesetwoopponentsdidnotinsulttheparticipantpreviously.Forthepositivenegotiationrelevanttraits(e.g.,cooperative),participantsinthedis­tractionconditionratedtheirspeechtaskopponentmorenegativelythanboththeruminationandreappraisalconditions.Collectivelythesefindingssuggestwhichformsofemotionregulationmaybemostbeneficialinnegotiations;however,theeffectofdifferentemotionregulationstrategiesonthenumberofoffersacceptedremainslessclear.

concluding remarkSResearchsuggeststhatexpressingangerasanegotiationstrategyhaslimitedeffec­tiveness.Theprimarypurposeofthischapterwastopresentemotionregulationstrategiesasameans tomanagethedetrimentaleffectsofangeronnegotiationoutcomes.Specifically,workbyourselvesandotherssuggeststhattheapplicationofreappraisaltonegotiationsettingsmightproveusefulinreducinganger,aggres­sivebehavior,andconflictinnegotiations.Thefirsttwostudiesweconductedcon­vergedwithpriorwork in that reappraisalwas associatedwithdecreasedangerrelative toruminationordistraction(Fabiansson&Denson,2010;Maussetal.,2007;Rayetal.,2008).

Althoughthesestudiesillustratedthatreappraisaliseffectiveinreducingexpe­riencedanger,furtherresearchisnecessarytoinvestigatetheabilityofreappraisalto curb retaliatory negotiation behavior. For example, we found that differentforms of emotion regulation influenced how much participants proposed; how­ever,participantsacceptedasimilarnumberofoffersregardlessoftheemotionregulationstrategytheyengagedin(Fabiansson&Denson,2010).Applyingemo­tionregulationstrategiessuchasreappraisaldoeshavebenefits;however,thefullextentofthesestrategiesinimprovingnegotiationbehaviorremainstobefurtherinvestigated.Reappraisal isaneffortfulemotionregulationstrategyandmaybedifficult touse for individualswhodonotnaturally tendtoreappraise.Itmightbepossibletotrainnegotiatorsineffectivereappraisaloverseveralsessions.Suchtrainingcouldmakereappraisallesseffortfulandmaybeparticularlybeneficialforindividualswhotendtouseotheremotionregulationstrategies.Byimprovingtheabilitytoreappraise,thisnotonlymaychangeself­reportedemotionbutalsomayinfluencenegotiationbehavior.

Anotherwayreappraisalmayinfluencenegotiationbehaviorisbyusinginvolvednegotiationtaskssimilartothatcommonlyencounteredinreallife.Forinstance,intheUltimatumGameitisclearwhatisconsideredanunfairandfairofferandpar­ticipantscanautomaticallychoosewhethertoacceptorrejectoffersbasedonfair­nessratherthanallowingforemotionregulationstrategiestoinfluencetheirdecisionmaking.Usingmoreeffortfulnegotiationtaskssuchasnegotiationscenariosthatrequireproblemsolvingmaybemoreamenable toemotionregulationstrategies.Usingproblemsthataremoreabstractandlessconcretemaymeanthatparticipantsarelesslikelytosimplyapplyafairness­baseddecisionrulewhennegotiating.Future

Page 166: the Psychology of Social Conflict and Aggression Sydney Symposium in Social Psychology

the effectS of anger and anger regulation on negotiation 149

researchmightalsofurther investigatethepsychologicalmechanismsresponsibleforanger’sadverseeffectsinnegotiationsettings.Topicsdiscussedinthisvolumesuchasclose­mindednessandlackofperspective­takingabilitysuggestpromisingavenuestoexplore(seeChapters7and10inthisvolume).

Theabilitytoeffectivelyregulateemotionsinnegotiationshasseveralimpor­tantpracticalimplications.Regulatingangerisimportantforhealth.Angerisasso­ciatedwithdecreasedwell­beingandproblemsincludinghypertension(Diamond,1982). Regulating anger can reduce conflict and prevent aggression and mayreduceworkplaceviolence.Effectivelyregulatingangercanhelpimproverelation­shipsbetweennegotiatorsandfacilitate futurenegotiationsandmayreducethelikelihoodofstalemates.

referenceS

Allred,K.G.,Mallozzi,J.S.,Matsui,F.,&Raia,C.P.(1997).Theinfluenceofangerandcompassion on negotiation performance. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 70,175–187.

Axelrod,R.(1984).The evolution of cooperation.NewYork:BasicBooks.Barsade,S.G.,&Gibson,D.E.(2007).Whydoesaffectmatterinorganizations?Academy

of Management Perspectives, 21,36–59.Bazerman,M.H.,Curhan,J.R.,Moore,D.A.,&Valley,K.L.(2000).Negotiation.Annual

Review of Psychology, 51,279–314.Brescoll,V.L.,&Uhlmann,E.L.(2008).Cananangrywomangetahead?Statusconfer­

ral,gender,andexpressionofemotion intheworkplace.Psychological Science, 19,268–275.

Brown,S.P.,Westbrook,R.A.,&Challagalla,G.(2005).Goodcope,badcope:Adaptiveandmaladaptivecopingstrategiesfollowingacriticalnegativeworkevent.Journal of Applied Psychology, 90,792–798.

Bushman,B.J.(2002).Doesventingangerfeedorextinguishtheflame?Catharsis,rumina­tion,distraction,anger,andaggressiveresponding.Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 28, 724–731.

Bushman,B.J.,Baumeister,R.F.,&Stack,A.D.(1999).Catharsis,aggression,andpersua­siveinfluence:Self­fulfillingorself­defeatingprophecies?Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 76,367–376.

Bushman, B. J., Bonacci, A. M., Pedersen, W. C., Vasquez, E. A., & Miller, N. (2005).Chewingonitcanchewyouup:Effectsofruminationontriggereddisplacedaggres­sion.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 88,969–983.

Denson,T.F.(2009).Angryruminationandtheself­regulationofaggression.InJ.P.Forgas,R.F.Baumeister,&D.M.Tice(Eds.),The psychology of self-regulation.(pp.233–248).NewYork:PsychologyPress.

Denson,T.F.,Moulds,M.L.,&Grisham,J.R. (2009).The effects of rumination, reap-praisal, and distraction on anger experience.Manuscriptsubmittedforpublication.

Denson,T.F.,Pedersen,W.C.,Ronquillo,J.D.,&Nandy,A.S.(2009).Theangrybrain:Neuralcorrelatesofanger,angryrumination,andaggressivepersonality.Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 21, 734–744.

Denson, T. F., White, A. J., & Warburton, W. A. (2009). Trait displaced aggression andpsychopathy differentially moderate the effects of acute alcohol intoxication andruminationontriggereddisplacedaggression.Journal of Research in Personality, 43, 673–681.

Page 167: the Psychology of Social Conflict and Aggression Sydney Symposium in Social Psychology

thomaS f. denSon and emma c. faBianSSon150

Diamond,E.L.(1982).Theroleofangerandhostilityinessentialhypertensionandcoro­naryheartdisease.Psychological Bulletin, 92,410–433.

Fabiansson,E.C.,&Denson,T.F.(2010).Regulating anger in negotiations.Unpublishedmanuscript.

Fehr,E.,&Gächter,S.(2002).Altruisticpunishmentinhumans.Nature, 415,137–140.Fisher,R.,&Ury,W.(1991).Getting to yes: Negotiating agreement without giving in(2nd

ed.).London:CenturyBusiness.Friedman,R.,Anderson,C.,Brett,J.,Olekalns,M.,Goates,N.,&Lisco,C.C.(2004).The

positiveandnegativeeffectsofangerondisputeresolution:Evidencefromelectroni­callymediateddisputes.Journal of Applied Psychology, 89,369–376.

Gibson,D.E.,Schweitzer,M.E.,Callister,R.R.,&Gray,B.(2009).Theinfluenceofangerexpressions on outcomes in organizations. Negotiation and Conflict Management Research, 2,236–262.

Goldberg,L.S.,&Grandey,A.A.(2007).Displayrulesversusdisplayautonomy:Emotionregulation,emotionalexhaustion,and taskperformance inacallcentersimulation.Journal of Occupational Health Psychology, 12,301–318.

Gouldner, A. W. (1960). The norm of reciprocity. American Sociological Review, 25,161–178.

Grandey,A.A.(2000).Emotionregulationintheworkplace:Anewwaytoconceptualizeemotionallabor.Journal of Occupational Health Psychology, 5,95–110.

Grandey, A. A., Dickter, D. N., & Sin, H.­P. (2004). The customer is not always right:Customer aggression and emotion regulation of service employees. Journal of Organizational Behavior, 25,397–418.

Grandey,A.,Fisk,G.,&Steiner,D.(2005).Must“servicewithasmile”bestressful?ThemoderatingroleofpersonalcontrolforAmericanandFrenchemployees.Journal of Applied Psychology, 90,893–904.

Gross, J. J. (1998a). Antecedent­ and response­focused emotion regulation: Divergentconsequencesforexperience,expression,andphysiology.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74, 224–237.

Gross,J.J.(1998b).Theemergingfieldofemotionregulation:Anintegrativereview.Review of General Psychology, 2,271–299.

Gross,J.J.,&John,O.P.(2003).Individualdifferencesintwoemotionregulationprocesses:Implicationsforaffect,relationships,andwell­being.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 85,348–362.

Güth,W.,Schmittberger,R.,&Schwarze,B.(1982).Anexperimentalanalysisofultimatumbargaining.Journal of Economic Behavior and Organization, 3,367–388.

Koenigs,M.,&Tranel,D.(2007).Irrationaleconomicdecision­makingafterventromedialprefrontaldamage:Evidencefromtheultimatumgame.Journal of Neuroscience, 27,951–956.

Laird,J.D.(1984).Therealroleoffacialresponseintheexperienceofemotion:AreplytoTourangeauandEllsworth,andothers.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 47,909–917.

Lieberman,M.D.(2007).Socialcognitiveneuroscience:Areviewofcoreprocesses.Annual Review of Psychology, 58,259–289.

Liu,M.(2009).Theintrapersonalandinterpersonaleffectsofangeronnegotiationstrate­gies:Across­culturalinvestigation.Human Communication Research, 35,148–169.

Mauss,I.B.,Cook,C.L.,Cheng,J.Y.J.,&Gross,J.J.(2007).Individualdifferencesincog­nitivereappraisal:Experientialandphysiologicalresponsestoanangerprovocation.International Journal of Psychophysiology, 66,116–124.

Morris,J.A.,&Feldman,D.C.(1996).Thedimensions,antecedents,andconsequencesofemotionallabor.Academy of Management Review, 21,986–1010.

Page 168: the Psychology of Social Conflict and Aggression Sydney Symposium in Social Psychology

the effectS of anger and anger regulation on negotiation 151

Morris,M.,&Keltner,D.(2000).Howemotionswork:Thesocialfunctionsofemotionalexpressioninnegotiations.Research in Organizational Behavior, 22,1–50.

Nolen­Hoeksema,S.(1991).Responsestodepressionandtheireffectsonthedurationofdepressiveepisodes.Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 100,569–582.

Pennebaker, J. W., Chung, C. K., Ireland, M., Gonzales, A., & Booth,R. J. (2007). The LIWC manual: The development and psychometric properties of LIWC2007.Austin,TX:LIWC.net.

Pennebaker, J. W., Colder, M., & Sharp, L. K. (1990). Accelerating the coping process.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,58,528–537.

Pillutla,M.M.,&Murnighan,J.K.(1996).Unfairness,anger,andspite:Emotionalrejec­tionsofultimatumoffers.Organizational Behavior & Human Decision Processes, 68,208–224.

Ray,R.D.,Wilhelm,F.H.,&Gross,J.J.(2008).Allinthemind’seye?Angerruminationandreappraisal.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 94,133–145.

Rusting, C. L., & Nolen­Hoeksema, S. (1998). Regulating responses to anger: Effectsof rumination and distraction on angry mood. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74,790–803.

Sanfey,A.G.,Rilling,J.K.,Aronson,J.A.,Nystrom,L.E.,&Cohen,J.D.(2003).Theneuralbasisofeconomicdecision­makingintheultimatumgame.Science, 300,1755–1758.

Shapiro,D.L.(2002).Negotiatingemotions.Conflict Resolution Quarterly, 20,67–82.Sheppes,G.,&Meiran,N.(2007).Betterlatethannever?Onthedynamicsofonlineregu­

lationofsadnessusingdistractionandcognitivereappraisal.Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 33,1518–1532.

Sinaceur,M.,&Tiedens,L.Z.(2006).Getmadandgetmorethaneven:Whenandwhyangerexpressioniseffectiveinnegotiations.Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 42,314–322.

Tabibnia, G., Satpute, A. B., & Lieberman, M. D. (2008). The sunny side of fairness:Preferencefor fairnessactivates rewardcircuitry (anddisregardingunfairnessacti­vatesself­controlcircuitry).Psychological Science, 19,339–347.

Tiedens,L.Z.(2001).Angerandadvancementversussadnessandsubjugation:Theeffectofnegativeemotionexpressionsonsocialstatusconferral.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 80,86–94.

Van Dijk, E., Van Kleef, G. A., Steinel, W., & Van Beest, I.  (2008). A social functionalapproachtoemotions inbargaining:Whencommunicatingangerpaysandwhenitbackfires.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 94, 600–614.

VanKleef,G.A.,&Côté,S.(2007).Expressingangerinconflict:Whenithelpsandwhenithurts.Journal of Applied Psychology, 92,1557–1569.

VanKleef,G.A.,DeDreu,C.K.W.,&Manstead,A.S.R.(2004).Theinterpersonaleffectsofangerandhappinessinnegotiations.Journal ofPersonality and Social Psychology, 86, 57–76.

VanKleef,G.A.,VanDijk,E.,Steinel,W.,Harinck,F.,&VanBeest,I.(2008).Angerinsocialconflict:Cross­situationalcomparisonsandsuggestionsforthefuture.Group Decision and Negotiation, 17,13–30.

Watkins,E.,&Teasdale,J.D.(2004).Adaptiveandmaladaptiveself­focusindepression. Journal of Affective Disorders, 82,1–8.

Wimalaweera, S. W., & Moulds, M. L. (2008). Processing memories of anger­elicitingevents:Theeffectofasking‘why’fromadistance.Behaviour Research and Therapy, 46,402–409.

Page 169: the Psychology of Social Conflict and Aggression Sydney Symposium in Social Psychology
Page 170: the Psychology of Social Conflict and Aggression Sydney Symposium in Social Psychology

153

10TheRoleoftheQuestforPersonal

SignificanceinMotivatingTerrorismARIEW.KRUGLANSKI

University of Maryland

EDWARDOREHEKUniversity of Groningen

Thesecretofhappinessis:findsomethingmoreimportantthanyouareanddedicateyourlifetoit.

DanielDennett,Philosopher

O ne particularly striking form of aggression is the attacking of civiliansto reachpoliticalobjectives, labeled terrorism.Theuseof terrorism isanextreme formof aggressionbecause it targets individuals tradition­

allyviewedas innocentbystanders.Thus,psychologists studying terrorismhavefocused theiraimatunderstanding the reasonsapersonbecomesa terroristorsupportsterroristactivity.Inshort,researchersandtheoristshavebeenconcernedwiththefactorsthatdriveapersontoattackcivilians.Threegeneralcategoriesofexplanationshavebeenoffered:(1)ideologicalreasons;(2)personalcauses;and(3)socialpressures.

Ideologies constitute belief systems in which some ideal is envisioned andcompared with the current status. When a discrepancy between the ideal andtheactualstatusofaffairs isperceived,theindividual ismotivatedtoreduceit.Terroristideologiesmustidentifyaculpritbelievedtoberesponsibleforthedis­crepancy.Inadditiontoidentifyingaculprit,theideologymustbelievethatengag­inginviolenceagainsttheculpritwouldreducethediscrepancybetweentheactualandidealconditions.Finally,tocarryoutterrorism,theideologymustprovidea

Page 171: the Psychology of Social Conflict and Aggression Sydney Symposium in Social Psychology

arie W. kruglanSki and edWard orehek154

justification for theattackingofcivilians.Oneexampleof suchan ideologywasoutlinedbyPape(2005),whonotedthatterroristsoftenviewforeignoccupationoftheirlandasthestateofaffairstobecorrected,theoccupierastheculprit,andterroristactionasthemeansofremedyingthesituation,ultimatelyhopingtoforcetheoccupiertoleavetheland.

Personalcausesincludeanyexperiencesthatmaymotivateapersontoacceptthe previously outlined type of ideology. Researchers have proposed a plethoraof such experiences, including social rejection and exclusion (Sageman, 2004;Stern,2003;Chapter3inthisvolume),personallossandtrauma(SpeckhardandAkhmedova,2005),humiliationand injustice(Bloom,2005;Stern),andpoverty(Stern).Eachofthese,alongwithmanyotherpersonalexperiences,maypredis­poseandmotivateapersontoperceiveaninjusticeandtojustifytheuseofvio­lenceagainstciviliansasanappropriatemeansofretaliation.

Socialpressuresintheformofdutyandobligationtothegroupaswellastheacceptanceof terrorismasa socialnormmotivateandallow theviolence tobecarriedout.Thesesocialpressurescanbeinternalizedorinducedbypeerpres­sure.Evidence for sucha roleof suchsocialpressurescanbe found indataonJapaneseKamikazepilots(e.g.,Ohnuki­Tierney,2006)andalsoappliestopresent­dayterrorism(Bloom,2005;Gambetta,2005;Stern,2003).Consistentwiththis,TomFriedman(2010)arguedthatthelackofoutrageamongMuslimpopulationsregardingtheuseofterrorismbymembersoftheircommunityhasplayedacriti­calroleinallowingterroristactivitytocontinue.Forterrorismtobeused,itmustbeviewedasnormativelyacceptableamongapopulationofpeopleforwhomtheterroristsbelievetheyarefighting.Withoutsuchacceptance,theterroristactivitywouldbeatoddswiththosewhomtheyclaimtobehelping.

Althoughthesethreecomponentsofterroristmotivationsneatlyorganizetheabundanceofexplanationsforterrorism,theyfallshortofexplicatingthepsycho­logicalmechanismsforviolence.TheQuest forSignificanceTheoryattempts todojustthatbyoutliningafundamentalhumanmotivationthatleadsonetoattachoneself toagroupandtofighton itsbehalf.Inthischapter,wewill reviewtheQuestforSignificanceTheoryandpresentrecentdatainsupportofthetheorythatwasnotavailableatthetimeitwasoriginallyproposed.

the QueSt for Significance aS the underlying motivation for terroriSm

Thequestforsignificancehasbeenidentifiedasafundamentalhumanmotivationbymanypsychologicaltheorists(Becker,1962;Frankl,2000;Maslow,1943,1967).Maslowplacedself­actualizationconcernsattheapexofhismotivationalhierar­chy.AccordingtoFrankl,suchself­actualizationisencapsulatedinandattainedthroughattemptstoserveacausehigherthantheself.Suchself­transcendencecan be attained only through attachment to the social group. The recent bur­geoningfieldofpositivepsychologyhasalsoarguedthatthequestformeaningis central toauthentichappiness andcanbeattainedbyattachingoneself to alargercause(Seligman,2002).AsnotedbyBecker(1973)andTerrorManagement

Page 172: the Psychology of Social Conflict and Aggression Sydney Symposium in Social Psychology

the QueSt for perSonal Significance in motivating terroriSm 155

theorists(Greenberg,Koole,&Pyszczynski,2004),theultimatethreattopersonalsignificanceisone’sownimminentmortality.Towardoffthethreatofpersonalinsignificance,individualsaremotivatedtoattachthemselvestosocialgroups,todefendthegroup’sworldview,andtoworkinservicetothegroup.

Oneimportantprincipleofthecurrentframeworkisthatperceptionsofinjus­ticeandpersonalsignificancearebasedonrelativedeprivation.Accordingtothisview,theinjusticeorlackofpersonalsignificanceisnotnecessarilyrealorobjec­tive.Indeed,poverty,pooreducation,andpoliticaloppressiondonotconstituterootcauses ofterrorism(Atran,2003;Berrebi,2003;Krueger&Maleckova,2002).Moreover,knownperpetratorsofterrorismsuchasMuhammadAttaandhis9/11coconspiratorswereneitherlivinginpovertynorlackingeducation.Yetitseemslikelythattheyperceivedthattheyhadlessthantheydeserved,perhapsbecausetheywerelackingthefinancial,religious,orsocialopportunitiesgrantedtotheirpeers.Suchaperceiveddiscrepancyshouldthreatenone’ssenseofpersonalsignifi­cance,motivatingsignificancerestoration.

Becausegroupmemberships functionasan importantaspectof individuals’social identity, a perceived loss of significance to the groups to which a personbelongsmaymotivateasimilarquestforsignificancerestoration.Theperceivedrelative deprivation of a social class, sector, or group has been identified as anunderlying factor in large­scale socialmovements, including those thatuse vio­lence suchas riots and terrorism (Gurr, 1970).Wewouldexpect,basedon thisaccount,thatindividualswhodefinethemselvesaccordingtotheirgroupmember­shipswouldbemoresupportiveofaggressionagainstout­groups,includingtheuseofterrorism.

Collectivism and Support for Terrorism

Theforegoinganalysissuggeststhatindividualsidentifystronglywithgroups,valuegroupmemberships,andactonbehalfofthegrouptogainpersonalsignificance.Individualsforwhomgroupidentificationsarecentraltotheirworldviewaremorelikely to perceive the boundaries between groups as rigid and clearly defined.Whenlinesaredrawnbetweengroups,membersoftheout­grouparederogated,andaggressionagainstout­groupsismorelikelytobeviewedasjustifiable(Staub,2002).Assuch,acollectivistorientationcanleadtoaggressionandviolencetowardout­groupsperceivedtobeinconflictwiththein­group(Triandis,2003).

Ifcollectivismisgenerallyrelatedtosupportforviolenceagainstout­groups,then it should also be related to support for terrorism. To test this notion, twosurveystudieswereconductedinMuslimnations(Orehek,Fishman,Kruglanski,Dechesne,&Chen,2010).Thefirstsurveywasconductedin12Arabcountries,Pakistan,andIndonesiaviatheInternet.Respondentswereaskedwhethertheyprimarilyidentifyasbeing(1)amemberoftheirreligion,(2)amemberoftheirnation,or(3)anindividual.Participantswhoidentifyprimarilywiththeirnationorreligionhavecollectivegoals,whereasparticipantswhoidentifyasanindividualhavepersonalgoals.Hence,weexpectedthosewhoidentifiedwith theirnationor religion would be more supportive of terrorism against the West than thosewho identifiedprimarily as an individual.Toassess their support for terrorism,

Page 173: the Psychology of Social Conflict and Aggression Sydney Symposium in Social Psychology

arie W. kruglanSki and edWard orehek156

theywereaskedfourquestionstappingtheirsupportforviolenceagainstciviliancitizensfromtheUnitedStatesandEurope.Participantswhoprimarilyidentifiedwiththeirnationorreligionweresignificantlymoresupportiveofterrorismthanwereparticipantswhoprimarilyidentifiedasanindividual.Thesedifferenceswerefoundevenwhencontrollingforage,gender,andlevelofeducation.Therewerenosignificantdifferencesbetweenthosewhoidentifiedwiththeirnationandthosewhoidentifiedwiththeirreligion.

Whilethefirststudywassupportiveofthehypothesisthatcollectivisticiden­tificationswouldbeassociatedwithgreatersupportforterrorism,wecollecteddataaspartofasecondsurveytoreplicatethefindingsusingaslightlydiffer­ent methodology. Because the first survey sample was limited to individualswithInternetaccess,thesecondstudyusedrepresentativesamplesfromEgypt,Indonesia,andPakistantoensurethattheresultswouldgeneralizetotherestofthepopulation.Second,wemeasuredcollectiveidentificationsusingadiffer­entquestion,moredirectlytappingthegoalsoftherespondents.Inthissurvey,respondentswereaskedtochoosewhichofthreestatementstheyagreedwithmost: (1) “aparent’s major goal should be ensuring that their children have agoodeducationandachancetosucceedinlife”;(2)“aparent’smajorgoalshouldbeensuringthattheirchildrenservetheirnation”;or(3)“aparent’smajorgoalshouldbeensuringthattheirchildrenservetheirreligion.”Replicatingthefind­ingsfromthefirststudy,wefoundthatthosewhoidentifiedprimarilywiththeirnationorreligionweremoresupportiveofterrorismagainsttheWestthanwererespondentswho identifiedprimarilywiththeirnation.Again,wefoundthesedifferencesevenwhencontrollingforage,gender,andlevelofeducation.Therewasnodifferencebetweenthosewhoidentifiedwiththeirnationandthosewhoidentifiedwiththeirreligion.

Wecanseethenthatcollectivismisassociatedwithgreatersupportforterror­ism.Theredoesnotseemtobeanydifferencebetweenthecollectiveofanationand the collective of a religion in supporting violence. Both groups representpotentialsourcesofsocialidentity.Whenindividualsviewthemselvesaccordingtosuchgroupmemberships,itincreasesthelikelihoodthattheywillbesupportiveoftheuseofviolence,includingwhentheviolenceisaimedatciviliantargets.

Suicidal Terrorism and the Quest for Significance

Perhapsanevenmorestrikingformofterrorisminvolvestheintentionaltakingofone’sownlifeintheprocess.Becausesuicidalterrorismisanextrememeansandtheperpetratorsarehailedasgivingtheultimatesacrifice,ithasthepotentialofbestowinggreatersignificanceupontheactor.Oneimportantimplicationoftheimportanceplacedonthesocialgroupingainingpersonalsignificanceisthat “thewillingnesstodieinanactofsuicidalterrorismmaybemotivatedbythedesiretoliveforever”(Kruglanskietal.,2009,p.335).Thatis,thesignificancegainedbykillingoneselfforthesakeofthegroupmayleadthepersontoacquiremoreper­sonalsignificancethroughgainingprestige,andthepotentialtoberememberedbythegroupmembersforalongtimemaymakeitpossiblefortheindividualtogainmorepersonalsignificanceindeaththanheorshecouldduringanextended

Page 174: the Psychology of Social Conflict and Aggression Sydney Symposium in Social Psychology

the QueSt for perSonal Significance in motivating terroriSm 157

life.ConsistentwiththisideaisthepropositionofferedbythephilosopherDanielDennett(2002),whostatesthathumansarewillingtoengagein“thesubordina­tionofourgeneticintereststootherinterests.Nootherspeciesdoesanythinglikeit.”Onepossibleimplicationoftheseobservationsisthathumansarenotactingintheirowngeneticinterestandinsteadthatideasandcultureareevolvingratherthangeneticmaterial(seeChapter15inthisvolume).

Yet analternative account could suggest that ideas are the fabricof a sharedsocial reality that defines the group. This notion is posited by the Quest forSignificanceTheoryandisacceptedinpsychologicaltheorymoregenerally(Hardin&Higgins,1996;Kruglanski,Pierro,Mannetti,&DeGrada,2006).Accordingtosuchanaccount,evolutionthencanoccuratthegrouplevel,meaningthatanactof suicide terrorismmay in factbestowanevolutionary advantageonto theclosegeneticrelativesofthemartyr.Indeed,recentadvancesinevolutionarytheoryhavesuggestedthatevolutioncanoccuratthegrouplevel,andspecificmechanismsforsuchevolutionhavebeenproposed(Wilson&Wilson,2008;seealsoChapter15inthisvolume).Thisspecifictheoreticaladvancementhasbeenappliedtothestudyof suicide terrorism, suggesting that it may bestowanevolutionary advantage ontheirkin(Victoroff,2009).Accordingtosuchanaccount,suicidalterrorismmaybeoneexampleofaltruisticsuicide(Durkheim,2007;Pedahzur,Perliger,&Weinberg,2003).EarlyresearchsuggeststhatPalestiniansuicidebombersdidindeedproduceevolutionarybenefitsfortheirkin(Blackwell,2005).Futureresearchcouldprofit­ablyexploresuchclaims,investigatingwhetherthegeneticrelativesofsuicideterror­istsarereallybetteroffthantheywouldhavebeenhadthepersonremainedalive.

teStaBle tenetS of the QueSt for Significance theory

TheoriginalformulationoftheQuestforSignificanceTheorypositedseveraltest­able tenets thathave sincemotivated research in anattempt to test theclaims.Herewewill reviewevidence in supportof threeprimary implicationsderivedfromthequestforsignificanceargument.Thefirstsuchimplicationhasbeenthor­oughlytestedinresearchontheeffectsofmortalitysalience.Thesecondandthirdimplicationshaveonlyrecentlybeenempiricallytested,andthedatainsupportofthemwerenotavailablewhentheoriginaltheorywaspresented.

Mortality Salience as a Threat to Personal Significance

ThefirsttestabletenetoftheQuestforSignificanceTheorystatesthat“ifremind­ersofone’sownmortalityconveyone’spotentialinsignificancethensuchremind­ersshouldaugmentthequestforsignificanceasdefinedbyone’sculturalnormsand accepted ideological frames” (Kruglanski et al., 2009, p. 338). Indeed,research in support of Terror Management Theory has consistently found thatremindersofone’smortality lead todefenseofone’sworldview, includingmorefavorable attitudes toward those who follow group norms (Greenberg, Porteus,Simon,&Pyszczynski,1995),andsupportforharshertreatmentofdeviants(e.g.,

Page 175: the Psychology of Social Conflict and Aggression Sydney Symposium in Social Psychology

arie W. kruglanSki and edWard orehek158

Greenberg et al., 1990;Rosenblatt, Greenberg, Soloman, Pyszczynski, & Lyon,1989).Particularlyrelevanttothestudyofterrorism,IraniansremindedoftheirownmortalityratedapersonwhosupportedmartyrdomattacksagainsttheUnitedStatesmorefavorablythanapersonwhodidnotsupportsuchattacks(Pyszczynskietal.,2006).Yetparticipantswhowerenotremindedoftheirownmortalityratedthepersonwhodidnotsupportmartyrdomattacksmorefavorablythanthepersonwhodid.Wecansee,then,thatthethreattopersonalsignificanceintheformofremindingpeoplethattheirexistenceistemporaryleadsthemtoattempttoregainsignificancethroughdefenseoftheirsocialgroup,includingtheuseofterrorism.

The Collectivistic Shift Hypothesis

ThesecondtestableimplicationoftheQuestforSignificanceTheory,asstatedbyKruglanskietal. (2009,p.338) is thata“perceived lossofsignificancethrougheventsotherthanmortalityremindersshouldfueleffortsatsignificancerestora-tion.”Specifically,thetheoryproposedanovel“collectivisticshifthypothesis”inwhichalossofpersonalsignificancewouldleadtoashifttowardamorecollectiv­isticorientation.Whenindividualsarefacedwithnegativefeedbackthreateningtheirpersonalsignificance,theycanrestoretheirlostsignificancebyviewingtheselfasinterdependentwithothers.

Fourstudieshavebeenconductedthatdirectlytestthishypothesis.Inthefirststudy, representative samples fromEgypt, Indonesia, andPakistancompletedasurveyinwhichtheywereaskedtheextenttowhichtheyhadexperiencedpersonalsuccessandwereaskedtoselectwhethertheyidentifiedprimarilyasamemberoftheirnation,amemberoftheirreligion,orasanindividual(Orehek,Kruglanski,etal.,2010).Theseitemswereembeddedinalarger,unrelatedsurvey.Aspredictedbythecollectivisticshifthypothesis,participantswhoidentifiedwiththeirnationorreligion(eachrepresentingcollectiveidentities)reportedlowerpersonalsuccessthanparticipantswhoidentifiedprimarilyasanindividual.

Althoughthepreviousstudyisconsistentwiththecollectivisticshifthypothesis,theresultsaresubjecttoanumberofalternativeinterpretationsbecauseofthecorre­lationalnatureofthestudy,includingthedirectionofcausalityissue.Toaddressthisspecifically,wedesignedthreelaboratoryexperimentstofurthertestthehypoth­esis(Orehek,Belcher,Fishman,Goldman,&Kruglanski,2010).Inthefirststudy,participantscompletedalanguagetest,whichtheyweretoldwasagoodpredictoroftheirfutureacademicandcareersuccess.Participantswererandomlyassignedto receive false feedback indicating that theyeither succeededor failed the test.Participantsthencompletedaself­reportmeasureofinterdependentself­construal(Singelis,1995).Participantsinthefailureconditionscoredsignificantlyhigherontheinterdependencescalethandidparticipantsinthesuccesscondition.Itseemsthatthethreattopersonalsignificanceengenderedbythefailureonanimportantlifeskillsdomainledparticipantstoincreasetheirinterdependentorientation.

Asecondstudywasdesignedtotesttheadditionalpredictionthatparticipantswhoexperiencefailurewouldnotonlyincreasetheirlevelofinterdependencebutwould also show decreased independence. Participants in this study were ran­domlyassigned toeitherwriteabouta time in thepast that theysucceededon

Page 176: the Psychology of Social Conflict and Aggression Sydney Symposium in Social Psychology

the QueSt for perSonal Significance in motivating terroriSm 159

animportantpersonalgoaloratimeinthepastwhentheyfailedatanimportantpersonal goal.Participants thencompleted self­reportmeasuresof independentandinterdependentself­construal(Singelis,1995).Consistentwiththeresultsofthefirststudy,participantsinthefailureconditionscoredsignificantlyhigherontheinterdependencescaleandsignificantlylowerontheindependencescalethanparticipants in the successcondition.These results suggesta true shiftingawayfromanindependentorientationtowardaninterdependentorientationinthefaceoffailure.

Toextendtheresultsfromthefirsttwostudies,ourthirdstudyinvestigatedthepossibilitythatafterfailureparticipantswouldelecttoworkinagroupratherthanalone.Totestthisprediction,participantsfirstengagedinavideogameonthecomputer.Participantsweretoldthattheirperformanceonthistaskhasbeendemonstratedtobeareliablepredictoroftheirintelligenceandfuturelifesuccess.Thevideogamewasriggedsothatparticipantswererandomlyassignedtoeithersucceedor fail at the task.Following this task,participantswere told that theywouldengageinanothertaskwiththechancetowinareward(achocolatebar).Theyweretoldthattheyhadtheoptionofworkingaloneonthistaskorworkinginagroup.Participantsinthesuccessconditionweresignificantlylesslikelytoelecttoworkinagroupthanwereparticipantsinthefailurecondition.Thisstudydem­onstratesthatfailurenotonlyshiftstheindividuals’mindsetfromanindependentwayofthinkingtoaninterdependentwayofthinkingbutalsoleadstoeffortstoengageincollectiveaction.

Theresultsfromthesefourstudiesprovidetheempiricalevidenceforthecol­lectivisticshifthypothesis.Individualswhoexperienceadeclineintheirpersonalsignificanceasaresultofpersonalfailureseemtoattempttorestoretheirpersonalsignificanceby shifting toacollectivisticorientationandbyengaging in collec­tiveaction.Inthisway,individualsareattachingthemselvestoasocialgrouptoattemptsignificancerestoration.

Thisinitialsetofdataonthecollectivisticshiftispromising.Yetmanyques­tionsremaintobeanswered.Forexample,dataareneededmeasuringthedeclineinpersonal significance followingthefailureandsubsequent restoration inper­sonalsignificancefollowingtheshift.Wecouldalsotestwhetherthecollectivisticshiftisespeciallylikelywhenone’sgroupmembershipismadesalient.Inaddition,itispossiblethatthetypeofgrouptowhichonebelongsmoderatesthetendencytoshifttocollectivisticgoals.Forinstance,itmightbethecasethatgroupscharacter­izedbycohesionmightbemorelikelytopromptacollectivisticshiftthangroupscharacterizedbyinternalconflict.Finally,onecouldinquirewhetherthecollectiv­isticshiftmaybemorelikelyforindividualsunderaheightenedneedforcognitiveclosure,knownfortheirproclivityforgroupcentrism(Kruglanskietal.,2006).

Extending the Self Through Time: Interdependent Self-Construals

ThethirdtestableimplicationoftheQuestforSignificanceTheory,asstatedbyKruglanskietal.(2009,p.338),isthatthe“adoptionofculturalcausesthatlendoneasenseofpersonalsignificanceshouldreducedeath­anxiety.”Inotherwords,a person who views the self as interdependent with others in the social group

Page 177: the Psychology of Social Conflict and Aggression Sydney Symposium in Social Psychology

arie W. kruglanSki and edWard orehek160

shouldexperiencelessdeathanxietythanshouldapersonwhoviewstheselfinde­pendently.Byviewingtheselfinterdependently,thepersonisabletoextendtheselfthroughtime(Castano&Dechesne,2005).Thinkingaboutoneselfaspartofagroup reduces the threatofdeathbecause, although the individual’s lifemaybe temporary, thegroupcan liveon.Themore important the interdependencegleaned from group membership becomes relative to the independent self, themoreimportantthegroup’sexistenceshouldbecomeandthelessimportanttheindividual’sexistenceshouldbecome.Therefore,priminganinterdependent(vs.independent)wayofthinkingshouldreducetheaversiontowarddeathoftheindi­vidual.Wetestedthispredictioninthreelaboratoryexperiments(Orehek,Sasota,Ridgeway,Dechesne,&Kruglanski,2010).

Inourfirstexperiment,participantswererandomlyassignedtooneoftwoexperi­mentalconditions,designedtomanipulateindependentversusinterdependentself­construal.Inbothconditions,participantswereinstructedtocircleallthepronounsinanessay.Participantsintheindependentconditioncircledpersonalpronouns(e.g.,I, me, my),andparticipantsintheinterdependentconditioncircledinterpersonalpro­nouns(e.g.,we, us, our).Thismanipulationhasbeenshowntoincreaseindependentversusinterdependentself­construalsintheappropriatecondition(Brewer&Gardner,1996).Participantsthencompletedaself­reportscaleofdeathanxiety(Templer,1970).Participantsintheinterdependentconditionscoredsignificantlyloweronthedeathanxietyscalethanparticipantsintheindependentcondition.Thisfindingsupportsourpredictionregardingthelinkbetweenself­construalanddeathanxiety.

Inoursecondstudy,wemeasureddeathanxietyusingameasuredesignedtotap implicitbehavioraldispositions (Fishbach&Shah,2006) following thesameexperimentalmanipulationofself­construalasinthefirststudy.Inonecondition,participantswereaskedtopushmeaningfulwords(e.g.,pint)awayfromthemandtopullmeaninglesswords(e.g.,pind)towardthem.Inanothercondition,partici­pantswereaskedtopullmeaningfulwordstowardthemandtopushmeaninglesswordsawayfromthem.Inbothconditions,wordsrelatedtodeath(e.g.,coffin)wereembeddedintothetask.Basedonpreviousresearch(Fishbach&Shah),weassumedthatfasterpullingofdeath­relatedwordstowardtheparticipantreflectedgreaterwillingnesstoapproachdeath,andfasterpushingofdeath­relatedwordsawayfromtheparticipantreflectedgreateravoidanceofdeath.Wefoundthatparticipantsintheinterdependentconditionpulleddeath­relatedwordstowardthemselvesfasterandpusheddeath­relatedwordsawayslower(controllingforspeedonneutraltrials)thandidparticipantsintheindependentcondition.Thus,itseemsthatwhenpeopleareinaninterdependentmindsettheyavoiddeathtoalesserextentandapproachdeathtoagreaterextentthandopeoplewithanindependentmindset.

Ourthirdstudywasdesignedtoextendthesefindingstoanadditionalmanipu­lationofself­construal.Inthisstudy,participantsintheindependentself­construalconditionwereaskedtothinkaboutthewaystheyweredifferentfromtheirfriendsand family. In the interdependent self­construal condition, they were asked tothinkaboutthethingsthattheyhadincommonwiththeirfriendsandfamily.Asinthesecondstudy,participantsintheinterdependentconditionwerefastertopulldeath­relatedwordstowardthemselvesandslowertopushthemaway(controllingforspeedofrespondingtoneutralwords).

Page 178: the Psychology of Social Conflict and Aggression Sydney Symposium in Social Psychology

the QueSt for perSonal Significance in motivating terroriSm 161

Acrossfourstudies,wefoundaconsistentpatternofresultsattestingtotheabil­ityofaninterdependentself­construaltomitigatethefeartowarddeath.Futureresearchcouldfurtherextendtheseresultsinimportantways.Forexample,wedonothavedatademonstratingthataninterdependentself­construalshiftsthefocustothegroup’slifeovertheindividual’slife.Inaddition,ouranalysiswouldsuggestthereversepatternforanxietyregardingthegroup’sexistence,yetthesedatahavenotyetbeencollected.

Summary of Empirical Support

WehaveoutlinedthreeresearchprogramsinsupportofthemajortenetsoftheQuestforSignificanceTheory.Ithasbeenshownthat(1)collectivistssupportter­rorismtoagreaterextentthandoindividualists,(2)remindersofone’sownmortal­ityaugmenttheadherencetoone’sculturalnormsandacceptedideologicalframes,(3)threatstopersonalsignificanceintheformofpersonalfailureleadstoacol­lectivisticshift,and(4)acollectivistorientationreducesdeathanxietycomparedwithanindividualistorientation.Takentogether,thesedataprovideinitialsupportfortheQuestforSignificanceTheory.Threatstoone’ssignificance,whetherfromimpendingdeathorpersonalfailure,leadtoattemptstorestorepersonalsignifi­cance. Individuals who attach themselves to a social group are more willing toattackout­groupcivilians.Finally,construingtheselfininterdependentwaysleadstodecreasedanxietyaboutdeath,whichmayserveasacriticalwayofovercominginhibitionsrelatedtomartyrdomaction.

concluSionWehavesummarizedthetheoryrelatedtothequestforpersonalsignificancetoterroristactivityandtheempiricalsupportforitsmajorimplications.Inshort,wehavearguedthatindividualswhoexperienceathreattotheirpersonalsignificanceattempttorestorelostsignificancethroughtheirattachmenttoasocialgroupanddefenseof thatgroup.Thesignificancemotiveimprovedonprevioustheorizingon terroristmotivations by tying the categoriesof ideological reasons,personal causes,andsocial pressures togetherandexplicating theunderlyingpsychologi­calmotivationforterroristactivity.Inthischapterwehavealsooutlinedhowthistheoryfitsmoregenerallywithevolutionarytheoryandmayexplainsuicidalter­rorismasaformofaggressivealtruism.

Thequestforsignificancehasbeenpostulatedasafundamentalhumanmoti­vation,presentinallpeopleanduniversalacrosscultures.Yetonlyaminorityofpeopleintheworldsupportterrorism,eveninregionsfromwhichterrorismmorecommonlyoriginates.Personalsignificancecanbegainedfromavarietyofaccom­plishmentsandgroupmemberships.Yetwhenpersonalgoalsandgroupidentitiesareperceivedasrelativelydeprived,effortstorestorepersonalsignificanceshouldbeenacted.Whenthedeprivationisperceivedtobeunjust,aculpritcanbeidenti­fied,andviolencecanbejustified;onlythenarewelikelytoseeterrorismpursuedasameansofrestoringsignificance.

Page 179: the Psychology of Social Conflict and Aggression Sydney Symposium in Social Psychology

arie W. kruglanSki and edWard orehek162

Thisframeworksuggestspotentialwaystoreducetheincidenceofterrorismintheworld.Ifterrorismismotivatedbythequestforsignificance,thenopeningalternativeopportunitiesforsignificancerestorationthatdonotincludeviolenceshouldreducetheuseofterrorism­justifyingideologies.Thiscanoccuronboththeindividualandgrouplevels.Onanindividuallevel,providingsupportforanindividual’spersonalaspirationsandsocialmobilityshouldprovidealternativeave­nuesforthegainingofpersonalsignificance.Onagrouplevel,reducingperceivedinjusticesthroughdiplomacyandnegotiationshouldreducetheneedforviolenceasameansofachievingone’sobjectives.

While the early results of studies in support of the Quest for SignificanceTheoryareconsistentwithitstenets,futureresearchisneeded.Wehavealreadyoutlinedmultiplewaystheclaimscouldbefurthertested.Oneimportantlimita­tionofthedatasofaristhatmuchofthemhavebeencollectedoncollegestudentsamplesinlaboratorieslocatedintheUnitedStates.Futuretestsofthepredictionswillneedtotesttheclaimsinotherculturesamongdiversesamples.Becausethetheoryispurportedtoexplainterroristbehavior,testingeachtenetamongterroristsampleswouldsignificantlybolsterthecredibilityoftheclaims.Forthetheorytobeconfidentlyappliedtocounterterrorismefforts,empiricaltestsofinterventionsrelevanttothetheoryareneeded.

referenceS

Atran,S.(2003).Genesisofsuicideterrorism.Science, 299,1534–1539.Becker,E.(1973).The denial of death.NewYork:TheFreePress.Becker,W.C.(1962).Developmentalpsychology.Annual Review of Psychology,13,1–34.Berrebi,C.(2003).Evidence about the link between education, poverty, and terrorism among

Palestinians.PrincetonUniversityIndustrialRelationsSectionWorkingPaper477.Blackwell, A. D. (2005). Terrorism, heroism, and altruism: Kin selection and socio-reli-

gious cost benefit scaling in Palestinian suicide attack.Presentedatthe2005HumanBehaviorandEvolutionSocietyMeeting.

Bloom,M.(2005).Dying to kill: The allure of suicide terror.NewYork:ColumbiaUniversityPress.

Brewer,M.B.,&Gardner,W.(1996).Whoisthis“we”?Levelsofcollectiveidentityandselfrepresentations.Journal of Personality andSocial Psychology, 71, 83–93.

Castano, E., & Dechesne, M. (2005). On defeating death: Group reification and socialidentificationas immortality strategies. European Review of Social Psychology,16,221–255.

Dennett,D.On dangerous memes.TEDTalk,February2002.Durkheim,E.(2007).On suicide (Le suicide)(R.Sennett,Contrib.,A.Riley,Ed.,R.Buss,

Trans.).NewYork:PenguinClassics.(Originalworkpublished1897)Fishbach,A.,&Shah,J.(2006).Self­controlinaction:Implicitdispositionstowardgoalsand

awayfromtemptations.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,90,820–832.Frankl,V.E.(2000).Man’s search for ultimate meaning.NewYork:BasicBooks.Friedman,T.L.(2010,January6).Fatherknowsbest.New York Times,p.A23.Gambetta, D. (2005). Making sense of suicide missions. Oxford, UK: Oxford University

Press.Greenberg,J.,Koole,S.L.,&Pyszczynski,T.(2004).Handbook of experimental existential

psychology.NewYork:GuilfordPress.

Page 180: the Psychology of Social Conflict and Aggression Sydney Symposium in Social Psychology

the QueSt for perSonal Significance in motivating terroriSm 163

Greenberg,J.,Porteus,J.,Simon,L.,Pyszczynski,T.,&Solomon,S.(1995).Evidenceofaterrormanagementfunctionofculturalicons:Theeffectsofmortalitysalienceontheinappropriateuseofcherishedcultural symbols.Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin,21,1221–1228.

Greenberg,J.,Pyszczynski,T.,Solomon,S.,Rosenblatt,A.,Veeder,M.,Kirkland,S.,etal.(1990).EvidenceforterrormanagementtheoryII:Theeffectsofmortalitysalienceon reactions to those who threaten or bolster the cultural worldview. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,58,308–318.

Gurr,T.R.(1970).Why men rebel. Princeton,NJ:PrincetonUniversityPress.Hardin,C.D.,&Higgins,E.T.(1996).Sharedreality:Howsocialverificationmakesthe

subjectiveobjective.InR.M.Sorrentino&E.T.Higgins(Eds.),Handbook of motiva-tion and cognition (Vol.3,pp.28–84).NewYork:GuilfordPress.

Krueger.A.B.,&Maleckova,J.(2002,June).Doespovertycauseterrorism?New Republic, 226,27–33.

Kruglanski, A., Chen, X., Dechesne, M., Fishman, S., & Orehek, E. (2009). Fully com­mitted:Suicidebombersmotivationandthequestforpersonalsignificance.PoliticalPsychology, 30(3),331–357.

Kruglanski,A.,Pierro,A.,Mannetti,L.,&DeGrada,E.(2006).Groupsasepistemicpro­viders:Needforclosureandtheunfoldingofgroup­centrism.Psychological Review,113(1),84–100.

Maslow,A.H.(1943).Atheoryofhumanmotivation.Psychological Review, 50, 370–396.Maslow,A.H.(1967).Atheoryofmetamotivation:Thebiologicalrootingofthevalue­life.

Journal of Humanistic Psychology,7,93–127.Ohnuki­Tierney, E. (2006). Kamikaze diaries: Reflections of Japanese student soldiers.

Chicago:UniversityofChicagoPress.Orehek,E.,Belcher,J.,Fishman,S.,Goldman,L.,&Kruglanski,A.W.(2010).The collec-

tivistic shift hypothesis.Unpublisheddata.Orehek,E.,Fishman,S.,Kruglanski,A.W.,Dechesne,M.,&Chen,X.(2010).The role of indi-

vidualistic and collectivistic goals in support for terrorist attacks.Unpublisheddata.Orehek,E.,Kruglanski,A.W.,Fishman,S.,Dechesne,M.,&Chen,X.(2010).The link between

perceptions of personal success and collective identification.Unpublisheddata.Orehek,E.,Sasota,J.,Ridgeway,L.,Dechesne,M.,&Kruglanski,A.W.(2010).Extending

the self through time: The effects of self construal on death anxiety. Unpublisheddata.

Pape, R. A. (2005). Dying to win: The strategic logic of suicide terrorism. New York:RandomHouse.

Pedahzur,A.,Perliger,A.,&Weinberg,L.(2003).Altruismandfatalism:ThecharacteristicsofPalestiniansuicideterrorists.Deviant Behavior, 24,405–423.

Pyszczynski,T.,Abdollahi,A.,Solomon,S.,Greenberg,J.,Cohen,F.,&Weise,D.(2006).Mortalitysalience,martyrdom,andmilitarymight:ThegreatSatanversustheaxisofevil.Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin,32,525–537.

Rosenblatt,A.,Greenberg,J.,Solomon,S.,Pyszczynski,T.,&Lyon,D.(1989).Evidenceforterrormanagementtheory:I.Theeffectsofmortalitysalienceonreactionstothosewhoviolateorupholdculturalvalues.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 57,681–690.

Sageman, M. (2004). Understanding terror networks. Philadelphia: University ofPennsylvaniaPress.

Seligman,M.E.P.(2002).Authentic happiness: Using the new positive psychology to realize your potential for lasting fulfillment.NewYork:SimonandSchuster.

Singelis,T.M.(1994).Themeasurementofindependentandinterdependentself­constru­als.Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin,20,580–591.

Page 181: the Psychology of Social Conflict and Aggression Sydney Symposium in Social Psychology

arie W. kruglanSki and edWard orehek164

Speckhard,A.,&Akhmedova,K.(2005).Talkingtoterrorists.Journal of Psychohistory,33,125–156.

Staub,E.(2002).Notesonterrorism:Originsandprevention.Peace and Conflict: Journal of Peace Psychology, 8, 207–214.

Stern, J. (2003).Terror in the name of God: Why religious militants kill.NewYork:Ecco/HarperCollins.

Templer,D. (1970).The construction and validationof adeath anxiety scale. Journal of General Psychology,82(2),165–177.

Triandis,H.C.(2000).Cultureandconflict.International Journal of Psychology,Special issue: Diplomacy and Psychology,35,145–152.

Victoroff,J.(2009).Suicideterrorismandthebiologyofsignificance.Political Psychology,30,397–400.

Wilson,D.S.,&Wilson,E.O.(2008).Evolution“forthegoodof thegroup.”American Scientist, 96,380–389.

Page 182: the Psychology of Social Conflict and Aggression Sydney Symposium in Social Psychology

Section IIIConflict and Aggression

in Relationships

Page 183: the Psychology of Social Conflict and Aggression Sydney Symposium in Social Psychology
Page 184: the Psychology of Social Conflict and Aggression Sydney Symposium in Social Psychology

167

11IntimatePartnerViolenceCognitive, Affective, and

Relational FactorsCHRISECKHARDT

Purdue University

I ntimatepartnerviolence(IPV)isacriticalpublichealthproblem,asrecentresearchhasdocumentedalarminglyhighprevalenceandincidencerates.IntheUnitedStates,population­basedsurveysindicatethatasmanyas20%of

womenarephysicallyassaultedbytheirintimatepartnerinagivenyear(Schafer,Caetano,&Clark,1998;Straus&Gelles,1990;Tjaden&Thoennes,1998).Relativetowomenwhohavenotbeenvictimized,abusedwomenareatsubstantiallyhigherrisk fordepression,suicide,posttraumatic stressdisorder,alcoholordrugabuseordependence,andpoorphysicalhealth(Golding,1999),andthefinancialcostsof intimate partner violence exceed $5.8billioneach year (Centers for DiseaseControlandPrevention,2003).ThesedatastronglyindicatetheneedtodevelopclearandtestablemodelsofIPVetiologyandmaintenanceandtoelucidateallpos­siblemechanismsthroughwhichIPVperpetratorscanbeidentified,prosecuted,andrehabilitated.

Researchers from a variety of professional backgrounds, including socialpsychology (Finkel,2007; seealsoChapter4 in thisvolume),haveexplored theindividualandrelationalriskfactorsthatdistinguishIPVperpetratorsfromnon­perpetrators (Hotaling & Sugarman, 1986) and established theoretical contextsthat have fostered the development of causal models of IPV (O’Leary, Slep, &O’Leary,2007)andapproachesforIPVperpetrators (e.g.,Murphy&Eckhardt,2005). Together, these research efforts have provided critical information thatcouldpotentiallybeputtogreatusebycriminaljusticeprofessionals,treatmentproviders,advocatesatthegrass­rootslevelworkingonbehalfofIPVvictims,andlegislatorsseekingtosetempiricallyinformedpoliciesdesignedtoreduceratesof

Page 185: the Psychology of Social Conflict and Aggression Sydney Symposium in Social Psychology

chriS eckhardt168

IPV.Inthischapter,Iwill reviewresearchfindings,primarilyrelyingonNorthAmerican samples, regarding three broad IPVperpetration risk factors of rele­vancetotheseoutcomes:(1)distortionsinsocialinformationprocessing;(2)affec­tivedysregulation;and(3)disturbancesinrelationaldynamics.Iwillsuggestthatbecause of long­standing ideological biases among advocacy groups against theroleofindividualandrelationalvariablesasriskfactorsforIPV,researchfindingsregardingpredictorsofIPVarenotactuallyhavinganysubstantiveinfluenceinthefieldasawhole,especiallythosethatruncountertotraditionalIPV ideolo­gies.Asaresult,theseresearchfindinghavefailedtoinfluencethoseonthe“frontlines,”includingvictims’advocacygroups,counselorsworkinginagenciesthatdealwithIPVperpetrators,criminaljusticeprofessionals(e.g.,police,judges,attorneys)whoworkwithIPVoffendersdaily,andlegislatorstaskedwithsettingIPV­focusedsocialpolicies.Becauseofthisdivide,IwillarguethatIPVvictimsarebeingputatgreaterriskinpartbytheveryorganizationsdedicatedtoassistthem.

ipv: the ideological BackdropResearchershaveinvestigatedadiverserangeofdeterminantsofaggressionandviolence; indeed, it is the very diversity of perspectives that makes the field ofaggression research so dynamic and broadly influential (Anderson & Bushman,2002;Baron&Richardson,1994;Daly&Wilson,1988; seealsoChapter16 inthisvolume).However,unlikeotherareasofaggressionresearch,theIPVfieldisunusuallyinfluencedbyideologicalfactorsconcerningthepresumed“appropriate”causesofspousalabuse.ThedominantperspectivethatguidescurrentIPVpolicyandinterventionprogrammingisbasedonearlyprofeministtheoriesofdomesticviolencethatarosefollowingthecreationofsheltersforabusedwomenintheearly1970sinDuluth,Minnesota,andfromresultingprogramsformaleIPVperpetra­torsdevelopedasoffshootsofshelterstorehabilitatemaleabusersinearly1980s(Pence&Paymar,1993).

This“DuluthModel”positsthatWesternsocietyisbuiltonpatriarchy,definedas“asystemofsocialorganizationthatcreatesandmaintainsmaledominationoverwomen”(Sugarman&Frankel,1996,p.14).Malesarethereforesocializedfromanearlyagebyotherpowerfulmalestoholdattitudesthatjustifyorsupportthepatri­archalsystem(seealsoChapter15inthisvolumeonthe“malewarrior”hypoth­esis).Theseattitudesresultinovertandcovertdesiresformentodominateandcontrolwomenand,whencombinedwithpatriarchalpracticesinthelegalsystem,religiousinstitutions,andothersocialsystems,resultinthecollectivemaintenanceof male power structures to dominate women across social domains, includingcloserelationships.Thisnotionissimilartofeministanalysesofrape,whichalsoviewsuchviolenceaspower­orcontrol­basedenforcementofmaleprivilege(forareview,seeBaumeister,Catanese,&Wallace,2002).Notsurprisinglygiventhisgender­focusedanalysis,proponentsofthismodelmaintainthat thispowerandcontrolpatternisexclusivetomalesandthatfemales’useofviolenceinrelation­shipsisrestrictedtoself­defensivepre­emptivestrikes(Walker,1984).Thisclaimpersistsdespitemeta­analyticfindingsshowingthatratesofIPVperpetrationarehigheramongfemalesrelativetomales,evenwhentakingintoaccountviolence

Page 186: the Psychology of Social Conflict and Aggression Sydney Symposium in Social Psychology

intimate partner violence 169

initiation (Archer, 2000,2002).Mentalhealthproviders andproponentsofpsy­chologicalorinteractionalmodelsofIPV,inturn,havebeencriticizedbyfeministscholars andvictims’advocates for their theoriesand interventionsdisempowerwomenandblamevictimsfortheirexperiencesofabuse(Adams,1988;Bograd,1984;Gondolf,2007).

Asanintellectualstartingpoint,thepatriarchalideologyperspectivefollowslogically given historically relevant cultural shifts regarding the role of gendersocializationinunderstandingvariousculturalphenomena(e.g.,Dobash&Dobash,1979),and,fromamorepracticalperspective,illustratesthefactthatsociologistsandsocialworkerswerethedrivingforcesbehindtheearliesteffortstounderstandIPVandintervenewithvictimsofabuse.However,itisproblematicthatsome35yearslaterthis“powerandcontrolmodel”hasremainedthedominantperspectiveinmany,ifnotmost,areasoftheIPVfield,despitevastamountsofdisconfirmingdata(forreviews,seeDutton&Corvo,2006;Dutton&Nicholls,2005).

Thismodelhasexpandedfromausefultheorythatprovidedsomeearlystruc­turetoanascentfieldofstudytoanoverarchingideologythatwieldsenormouspolitical influenceintermsofsocialpolicyandcriminaljusticepractice(Stuart,2005).InterventionsassociatedwiththeDuluthModel(Pence&Paymar,1993)focus exclusively on male power and control dynamics caused by faulty genderrolesocialization;littletonofocusisdirectedtowardpsychologicalfactors,nega­tiveemotions,orrelationalprocessesthatresearchershaveempiricallylinkedwithIPVperpetration(forareview,seeStith,Smith,Penn,Ward,&Tritt,2004).Intheirmeta­analyticreviewofinterventionprogramsforabusers,Babcock,Green,andRobie (2004)noted that theDuluthModel is the “unchallenged treatmentof choice formostcommunities” (p.1026).Of the45 states that currentlyhavestandardsoutliningthestructureofinterventionprogramsforIPVoffenders,themajorityincludestatementsofetiologyorprinciplesofpracticethatreferencethepatriarchalideologymodel(Dutton&Corvo,2006).Suchassumptionsservenotonlytoguideinterventionprogrammingbutalsotorestrictthenarrativeofwhatmightbeconsideredallowableetiologicfactors.However,itisalsoworthnotingthatdespitethesepronouncements,comprehensivemeta­analyticreviewsindicatethatDuluthModelprogramsareassociatedwithonlynegligiblesuccessinreduc­ingIPV(Babcocketal.,2004;Feder&Wilson,2006),witheffectsizesinverselyrelatedtothelevelofmethodologicalrigorofeachstudy.

With this brief, and somewhat distressing, backdrop, the remainder of thischapterwillreviewselectedareasassociatedwithIPVperpetrationthathighlightthecognitive,affective,andrelationalriskfactorsforIPV.

cognitive, affective, and relational riSk factorS for ipv

Cognitive Factors

DiversemodelsofIPVetiologyhaveconsistentlysuggestedthatcertainattitudes,beliefs,andcognitivedistortionsareimplicatedatsomelevelintheonsetandmain­tenanceofabusivebehavior.Asaresult,manytheoreticalmodelsandintervention

Page 187: the Psychology of Social Conflict and Aggression Sydney Symposium in Social Psychology

chriS eckhardt170

programs that focus on IPV­related cognitive factors claim to be “cognitive” or“cognitive­behavioral” in their orientation (Gondolf, 2004). As noted already, ini­tialresearchontheroleofcognitivefactors inIPVgrewoutofprofeministtheo­riesofdomesticviolence(e.g.,Dobash&Dobash,1979),suggestingthatlong­termexposuretopatriarchalcommunitiesinstilledadeeplyheldbeliefinmaleprivilegeandsuperioritythatcovertlyandovertlycondonesanymeansnecessarytomaintainthisunequalpowerarrangement,includingtheuseofcoercionandaggressiveforce(Pence,1983).EarlyfindingsusinginterviewswithfemaleIPVvictimssupportedthenotionthatabusewasdrivenbypowerandcontroldynamics(Straus,1976).

Analternativetothepatriarchalideologymodelasastartingpointforestab­lishingcognitivevariablesasriskfactorsforIPVemergedfromtheapplicationofsocial learningtheoryto interpersonalviolence,whichfocusesonprocess­levelinteractionsof theindividualwith thebroadersocialand interpersonalcontext(Bandura,1973).Thesociallearningapproachsuggeststhataggressivebehaviorsareacquiredthroughbasicprinciplesoflearning,andasaresultofthesedirectandvicariouslearningexperiencesviolentindividuals’processingofsocialinfor­mation is systematically biased toward negative assumptions of others’ behav­iorandpositiveassociationsregardingtheacceptabilityandvalueofaggressivebehaviors (Dodge, 1991). Long­standing cognitive distortions further degradeindividuals’ ability to self­regulate their emotional responses to interpersonalconflictandimpairthedevelopmentofsecureattachmentswithromanticpart­ners(Dutton,Saunders,Starzomski,&Bartholomew,1994;seealsoChapter9inthisvolume,onaself­regulatoryapproachtoaggression).Togetherthesedeficitsresultinadeficientsetofbasicrelationshipskillsthatfavortheuseofcontrollingandabusivebehaviors,includingbelligerentandcoercivecommunicationpatterns(Jacobson,Gottman,Waltz,Rushe,Babcock,&Holtzworth­Monroe,1994).Thus,a centraldifference between the social learning and feminist accountsofhowcognitivevariablesrelatetoIPVisthatthesociallearningmodeladdressesbothcognitivecontentandcognitiveprocessespresumedtoberelatedtoIPV,whereasthefeministaccountfocusesalmostentirelyonbiasedcognitivecontent.

evidence The profeminist patriarchal ideology model’s conceptualization ofgender­focusedcognitive–attitudinaldisturbancesastheyrelatetoIPVisincom­pleteforanumberofreasons.First,thereislittleevidencetosupportthenotionthat patriarchal attitudes and power­related beliefs represent specific proximal contributorstotheenactmentofIPV(Malik&Lindahl,1998;Stithetal.,2004;Sugarman&Frankel,1996).Second,partnerabuseisquiteprevalentin lesbianandgayrelationships(e.g.,Lie,Schilit,Bush,Montague,&Reyes,1991),afactthatisdifficulttoexplainifabuseisapurelygender­basedsystemofoppression(Burke&Follingstad,1999).Third,literaturereviewsindicatethatmenintreatmentfordomesticabusearenomorelikelythannonabusivementoendorsesexistbeliefsinmaleprivilegeorregardingwomen’srolesandrights,asindicatedbyoveradozencasecontrolstudies(Dutton&Corvo,2006;Eckhardt&Dye,2000;Sugarman&Frankel).Thus,whilemisogynisticbeliefsarecharacteristicofagreatmanymenacrossmanysocieties,theydonotappeartobeconsistentorspecificriskfactorsforIPVperpetration.

Page 188: the Psychology of Social Conflict and Aggression Sydney Symposium in Social Psychology

intimate partner violence 171

Overthelast20years,researchershaverefinedthesociallearningapproachintermsofunderstandingandassessingcognitivemechanismsthatmaybeinvolvedinIPV,againwithaneyetowardabroaderunderstandingofbothcognitivecontentandprocessthatmaytranslateintointerventionadvancements.Researchershavedeveloped social information processing models of IPV that have hypothesizeddecoding,decisionmaking,andenactmentdeficienciesassociatedwithIPVper­petration(e.g.,Holtzworth­Munroe,1992).Ampleevidencesupportspredictionsfromthismodel(Eckhardt&Dye,2000;Holtzworth­Munroe,2000;Murphy&Eckhardt,2005;Stithetal.,2004). Relativetononviolentmales,IPVperpetratorsexhibit(1)decoding,interpretation,andhostileattributionbiasesonquestionnairemeasures(Fincham,Bradbury,Arias,Byrne,&Karney,1997) andduringimag­inedconflictscenarios(Eckhardt,Barbour,&Davison,1998;Eckhardt&Jamison,2002;Holtzworth­Munroe&Hutchinson,1993);(2)lesscompetentdecisionmak­ing (i.e., greater generation of aggressive response options) on questionnaires(Field,Caetano,&Nelson,2004;Sugarman&Frankel,1996)andduringconflictsimulations(Anglin&Holtzworth­Munroe,1997;Barbour,Eckhardt,Davison,&Kassinove,1998;Jacobsonetal.,1994) ;and(3)positiveevaluationsofviolenceincloserelationships(Kaufman­Kantor&Straus,1990).

Importantly, thesefindings involvetheuseofmethodsthatgobeyondmereself­reportsofcognitivevariablesandhaveusedmethodsofcognitiveassessmentinformedbydevelopments ingeneralsocial cognition theoryandresearch (e.g.,Abelson,1981;Nisbett&Wilson,1977)aswellasinotherareasofinterpersonalviolence(Baumeister,Catanese,&Wallace,2002;Huesmann,1988).Forexample,ourownworkhassuggestedthatcognitionsrelatedtoIPVare“hot”cognitionsthattendtoaccompanyintenseaffectivestatessuchasangerratherthan“cold”cogni­tionsthatcanbecalmlydiscussedduringaface­to­faceintervieworendorsedonapaper­and­pencilmeasure.Wehaveusedauniquecognitiveassessmentmethodtoassessabuse­relatedcognitiveprocessing,thearticulatedthoughtsinsimulatedsituationsparadigm(ATSS;Davison,Robins,&Johnson,1983).IntheATSSpara­digm,participants are asked to imaginea seriesof audiorecorded interpersonalscenariosinvolvingtheirwivesorgirlfriends,toimaginethatthescenestheyarehearingarehappening“rightnow,”thattheyareinvolvedineach,andto“talkoutloud”abouttheirthoughts,feelings,andanythingthey’dliketodowhenpromptedbyatoneevery30seconds.Wehaveusedseveraldifferentscenariostoserveascontextsforthoughtarticulation,includingajealousy­themedscriptinwhichthesubject imagines that he has come home early to find his wife having a ratherromanticdinnerandmoviewithamaleacquaintanceandaninsultscriptinwhichthe subject imagines that he is overhearing a conversation between his wife orgirlfriendandherfemalefriendwhereinthetwowomenproceedtodenigratehisprofessional aspirations, intelligence, and sexualprowess. Articulations are latercodedbytrainedratersforthepresenceofcognitivedistortions linkedtoangerarousalandaggression(Beck,1999).

Ourresults(e.g.,Eckhardtetal.,1998;Eckhardt&Jamison,2002;Eckhardt&Kassinove,1998)haveconsistentlyfoundthatwhileabusivemendidnottypicallydifferfromnonabusivemenonpaper­and­pencil, self­reportmeasuresofcogni­tive distortions, IPV perpetrators articulated more cognitive distortions during

Page 189: the Psychology of Social Conflict and Aggression Sydney Symposium in Social Psychology

chriS eckhardt172

ATSS than nonviolent men. Specifically, IPV perpetrators exhibited cognitionsthat demeaned their partners’ worth, placed absolutistic demands that peopleact appropriately (demandingness), magnified the importance of situations, cat­egorizedtheimaginedscenariosintopolarextremes(dichotomousthinking),andestablished conclusions in the absence of confirming evidence (arbitrary infer­ence).Inaddition,severelyviolentmenweremorelikelythanmildlyviolentmentoarticulatecognitionsreflectiveofdemandingnessandawfulizing(characterizinganeventastheworstpossibleoutcome).

Otherresearchfromourlabsuggeststhatdecoding­relatedbiasesfavoringanangryandaggressiveresponsemayoccurquiteearlyinsocialinformationprocess­ing. Using a paradigm borrowed from social psychological researchers (Nisbett,1993),wehaveshownthathigh­trait­angercollegestudentsperforminganemo­tionalStroop­like taskexhibitautomaticattentionalbiases favoringanger­relatedstimulibutonlyduringconcurrentmoodinduction(Eckhardt&Cohen,1997).Inasubsequentstudyusingavisualsearchtask,wefoundthatthisbiasamonghigh­anger individuals diminished with repeated exposure to anger stimuli, suggest­ingasensitization–habituationprocessuniquetodispositionallyangryindividuals(Cohen,Eckhardt,&Schagat,1998).Morerecently,weexaminedautomaticpro­cessingbiasesamongIPVperpetratorsusingthreeimplicitassociationtests(IATs)designedtoexaminepreferences forgender,violence,andthelinkbetweenvio­lenceandgender(Eckhardt,Samper,Holtzworth­Munroe,&Suhr,2010).Resultsindicated that IPV perpetrators were faster than nonabusive men to categorizewordpairsinvolvingpositivewordswithviolencewords,andviolencewordswithfemalenames.NogroupdifferenceswerefoundontheIATassessingpreferencesformaleorfemalegender.Thesefindingsindicatethatindividualswhopositivelyendorse the use of aggressive conflict resolution strategies in close relationshipsmaybemorelikelytousesuchstrategiestomanagetheirownrelationshipconflicts(Archer&Graham­Kevan,2003)andtoautomaticallylinkthepresenceofafemale(butnotamale)withaggression­relatedbehavioralintentions.

Nevertheless, thesedatahavehad little influenceonhowcounselorson thefrontlinesofIPVtreatmentconceptualizetheoffendersintheirprogramsordesigntreatmentstoreducetheIPVthatbroughtthemthereinthefirstplace.Why?Theydonotlendthemselveswelltoagender­themedapproachtounderstandingIPV(Dutton&Corvo,2006).Traditionally, DuluthModelproponentshave focusedonthenotionofaccountabilityandpersonalresponsibilityascausesofviolence(Adams,1988)andhavebeenideologicallyopposedtothenotionthataffective,personality, or psychopathological variables can also be proximal causes of IPVperpetration(Gondolf,2004;Healey,Smith,&O’Sullivan,1998).Thus,ourasser­tionthatcognitivefactorsrelateonlytoviolenceinthecontextofangerarousalisnotparticularlycompellingfromthestandpointofthepatriarchalideologymodel,giventheapparentinvolvementofanger(seethenextsectionforreasonsunderly­ingantiangersentimentsamongDuluthModelproponents).Likewise,suggestingthatcertainbiasesmayoperateatanautomaticleveltoaffectIPVisnotlikelytobeawarmlyreceivedfinding,sinceitsuggestsatacitorimplicitprocessthatisnotunder theconsciouscontrolof theperpetrator.Promotingblameaccountabilityandpersonalresponsibilityisthemostcommonthemeamongstatestandardsfor

Page 190: the Psychology of Social Conflict and Aggression Sydney Symposium in Social Psychology

intimate partner violence 173

IPVinterventionprograms(Maiuro,Hagar,Lin,&Olson,2001),andIPVperpe­tratorsarecommonlyassumedtoengageinahighlevelofdenialandminimizationoftheiractions(Pence&Paymar,1993);offenderscouldpresumablyblametheirabuseonprocessesoutsidetheircontrolratherthanacknowledgethattheirvio­lenceistheproductofmaleprivilege(i.e.,“accountability”).However,theselargelyautomaticandoverlearnedassociationsareexactlythekindsofprocessesknowntounderliepropensitytowardgeneralaggression(e.g.,Berkowitz,1993)andthushaveenormouspotentialtoinformetiologicconceptualizationsofIPVandinflu­encethedevelopmentofIPVinterventions.

Regulation of Negative Emotions

Whiledecadesoftheoretical(Ellis,1962)andempirical(Haagaetal.,1991)worksupportthegeneralpropositionthatcognitivedisturbancesintensifytheexperi­enceofnegativeemotionsanddisrupthowtheseemotionsareexpressedinterper­sonally(seealsoChapter9inthisvolume),muchcontroversyexistswithinelementsoftheIPVfieldconcerningtherelevanceofemotionalvariablesinexplainingandtreatingIPV(e.g.,Gondolf,2002).Indeed,whilegivingarecenttalktoagroupofbatteredwomen’sadvocatesandinterventionprogramworkersaboutriskfac­tors for male­to­female IPV, I was met with a chorus of boos and rather nastycomments from theaudience the moment I concluded that thedata supportedangerdisturbances andpsychopathology as important risk factors for IPV.Whythenegativereaction?Generally,thereappearstobeaconcernamongmanybat­teredwomen’sadvocatesandprogramstaffthatinvokinginternalconstructssuchaspsychopathologyoremotionalproblemswillleadtoa“medicalmodel”approachtoIPVthatmayleadtoafocusawayfromwhattraditionallyhavebeenviewedastherootcausesofviolence(e.g.,communitysupportsthatovertlyorcovertlycon­doneabusivebehaviorandmen’slackofaccountabilityandresponsibility).WhileitwouldindeedbecounterproductivetoseethecausesofIPVasrestingsolelywiththepsychologicaldisturbancesofthemaleperpetrator,itseemssimilarlyunpro­ductivetoblithelydismisssuchfactorswhenampleempiricalevidenceexiststosubstantiatethesevariablesaslegitimateriskfactors.

InthecontextofIPV,thenegativeemotionthathasgarneredthemostatten­tion (favorable and not) is anger. The role of anger arousal in intimate partnerviolenceseemsobvious,foritisoftenassumedthatangerandaggressionare“inex­tricably, biologically linked” (Tavris, 1989, p. 24), andone can easily imagine ascenariowhereinanabusivemalebecomesintenselyangryandassaultshisfemalepartner.Indeed,dataarelargelysupportiveoftheassociationbetweenangerprob­lemsandIPVperpetration(Norlander&Eckhardt,2005).However,therearefewareasmorecontroversialwithinareasofdomesticviolenceresearchandadvocacyareas than the issue of anger and IPV. Part of this resistance reflects concernsabouttheextensionofamedicalmodelapproachintointerventionprogramsandwhatthismayimplyaboutetiology.Thatis,ifangercontrolinterventionsworkforIPVperpetrators,thenthismightsuggestthatanger­relatedfactorsmayindeedbeinvolvedintheetiologyofIPV;asnotedalready,suchaconclusionisunpopularamongalargesectorofthegrass­roots,feminist­advocacycommunity.

Page 191: the Psychology of Social Conflict and Aggression Sydney Symposium in Social Psychology

chriS eckhardt174

Echoingthesesuspicions,Gondolf(Gondolf,2002;Gondolf&Russell,1986)suggestedthat“angermanagement”interventions(1)implythatthevictimistoblame,(2)donotaccountforabusemeanttoexertpowerandcontrol,(3)per­petuatethebatterer’sdenial,(4)mayputthefemalepartneratfurtherriskforviolence,(5)givecommunitiesareasontoshuncollectiveresponsibilityforIPV,and(6)giveperpetratorsnewtoolstocoerceandcontrolwomen.Thesesenti­ments are reflectedbymanyadvocates forbatteredwomenand statedomes­ticviolencecoalitions(seeHealeyetal.,1998),whichhavelobbiedeffectivelyagainsttheuseofangercontroltreatmentsformenmandatedtoattendbattererinterventionprograms(BIPs).However,asnotedbyMaiuroetal.(2001),statestandards governing BIP content typically lack any empirically justification,callingintoquestionthebasisforthebanonangercontrolinterventions.Thenet result of these assumptions hasnot only been a resistance toward anger­based interventions but also a steadfast dismissal of anger as a potential riskfactor for IPV. Ultimately, however, all of these concerns must be answeredempiricallyratherthanideologically.So,istherearelationshipbetweenangerarousalandIPV?

evidence Theanswerisyes,althoughtherelationshipismoderateinstrength.Fromanempiricalstandpoint,recentquantitativereviews(Norlander&Eckhardt,2005;Schumacher,Feldbau­Kohn,Slep,&Heyman,2001)haveindicatedthatdis­turbancesinangerexperienceandexpressiondistinguishbetweenpartnerviolentandnonviolentmen(effectsize:d=.50).Studiesusingself­reportquestionnairesconsistentlyindicatethatpartner­violentmalesshowelevatedtraitanger,hostil­ity,increasedtendencytoexpressangeroutwardly,anddecreasedangercontrol(Eckhardt,Barbour,&Stuart,1997;Norlander&Eckhardt).Inaddition,angerproblemsare directly related tomore severe and frequentperpetrationof IPV(Holtzworthetal.,2000).Inobservationalresearchexaminingsequentialpatternsofcoupleinteraction,violentcouplesdemonstrateincreasedusageof“destructive”formsofanger,involvingexpressionsofcontempt,disgust,andbelligerence(e.g.,Jacobsonetal.,1994).Priorresearchonangerinsubtypesofpartner­violentmensuggests that some, although not all, partner­abusive men exhibit symptoms ofexcessiveanddysregulatedanger(e.g.,Chase,O’Leary,&Heyman,2001;Dutton,1988; Hershorn & Rosenbaum, 1991; Saunders, 1992; Holtzworth­Munroe &Stuart,1994;Waltz,Babcock,Jacobson,&Gottman,2000). Holtzworth­Munroeandcolleagues(2000)foundthatthetwomostseveresubtypesofpartner­violentmen (labeled Generally Violent and Borderline/Dysregulated) had significantlyhigherangerlevelsthanlessseveresubtypes.OtherresearchsuggeststhatangerinteractswithalcoholintoxicationtoincreasethelikelihoodofIPVduringrela­tionshipconflicts(Eckhardt,2007).Finally,recentfindingsusingforensicsamplesofIPVperpetratorssuggestthatapproximately20–25%ofpartner­abusivemenjudiciallymandatedtoattendbattererinterventionprogramshaveclinicallysig­nificant problems with anger experience and expression (Eckhardt, Samper, &Murphy,2008) and that abuserswithproblematic anger are less likely to com­pletesuchprogramsandmorelikelytoreassaultfemalepartners(Murphy,Taft,&Eckhardt,2007).

Page 192: the Psychology of Social Conflict and Aggression Sydney Symposium in Social Psychology

intimate partner violence 175

Butthereareinconsistenciesaswell.Severalstudiesusingself­reportquestion­nairesofangerandhostilityhavenotfounddifferencesbetweenpartner­violentandnonviolentmales(seeNorlander&Eckhardt,2005).Inaddition,researchersusingobservationalmethodshavetypicallyfoundthatdirectstatementsofanger(e.g.,“I’mreallymadatyou”)donotreliablydifferentiateviolentfromnonviolentcouples(Barbouretal.,1998;Gottmanetal.,1995).Thus,whiletheaccumulateddataindicatethatIPVperpetratorsshowdysfunctionallevelsoftraitangerandangercontrolrelativetononviolentmales,evenaftercontrollingforrelationshipdistress,andthatangerproblemsportendriskfortreatmentattritionandcrimi­nalrecidivism,itisunlikelythatpartner­violentmalescanbedifferentiatedfromtheirnonviolentcounterpartssolelyonthebasisofangerproblems;indeed,IPVperpetrators constitute aheterogeneous group of individuals (e.g., Holtzworth­Munroe & Stuart, 1994) who act abusively as a function of a diverse array ofcausesandsituations.Thus,ratherthanassumingthatangeris“always”or“never”involvedinIPV,itismoreimportanttoconsiderwhetherandforwhomspecificpatternsofangerproblemsmaybefactorsdeservingofclinicalattention(Murphyetal.,2007).

However,onceagain,thesefindingshaveseenonlyverylimitedacceptanceintheIPVfield.Oneofthemoredistressingaspectsofhowthepatriarchalideologymodelhaslimitedscientificinquiryisthatcurrentlythereisnoevidenceregardingwhether changingperpetrators’ negative emotions (e.g., anger) in IPV interven­tionprogramsspecificallypredictnonviolentoutcomes.Suchastudywouldfaceintenseideologicalresistancefromcriminaljusticefundingagenciesandwouldbepractically impossibletoconductusingcriminal justicesamplesgivenprevailingideologicallybasedopinionsthatopposeanger­themedresearchandtreatmentinthosesettings.Giventhatnorelevantstudiesexistconcerningwhetheranger­oremotion­focusedtechniquesspecificallyreduceIPVrisk,itissurprisinganddisap­pointingtoseesuchvehementpronouncementsagainsttheusageofanger­focusedinterventionsforIPVperpetrators(Adams,1988;Gondolf,2002),foronewouldassumethatsuchstronglynegativeevaluationswouldbebasedonactualevidence.Important research needs tobe conducted to investigate whether interventionsthathaveanemotionregulationcomponentaremoreeffectiverelativetostandardinterventionswithoutsuchacomponentorwhether interventionswithanangerfocuscouldbecometheinterventionofchoiceforperpetratorswithspecificemo­tionregulationdifficulties(Eckhardtetal.,2008;Murphyetal.,2007).

Relational Factors

One of the hallmark assumptions of feminist­informed models of IPV etiologyandinterventionisthatrelationshipdisturbancesareaconsequence,ratherthanacause,ofIPV(Gondolf,2002).Asnotedpreviously,thecentralthemeofthesemodels is that the patriarchal society in which we live provides an enormouslyinfluential and reinforcing context for men to use power and control tactics tosubjugatetheirfemalepartnersandpromotemaleprivilege.Thus,popularmod­elsofIPVsuggestthataggressivemanifestationsofabusearebutoneexampleofpowerandcontroltactics,asmenmayalsousepsychologicaloremotionalabuse,

Page 193: the Psychology of Social Conflict and Aggression Sydney Symposium in Social Psychology

chriS eckhardt176

economic coercion, and restriction of social contacts to intimidate, isolate, andcontroltheirpartners.

IsthereevidencethatrelationshippowerdynamicsarerelatedtoIPV?Surveyssuggest that couples wherein both partners acknowledge that the male is thedominantpartnercompriseaverysmallnumber(9.6%)ofU.S.couples(Coleman&Straus,1985).Inaddition,researchhasnotbeenconsistentlysupportiveofthespecificlinksbetweenrelationshippowerandIPV(Malik&Lindahl,1998).Forexample,Babcocketal.(1993) foundnorelationshipsbetweenpowerbases(i.e.,education,income,socioeconomicstatus[SES])andIPVandonlyamodestrela­tionamongpower­relatedoutcomes(i.e.,controloverdecisionmaking)andIPV.However,violenthusbandsreportedgreaterpursuitanddemandtacticsduringconflictdiscussions,whereaswivesreportedwithdrawingorshuttingdown(seealsoHoltzworth­Munroe,Smutzler,&Stuart,1998).Thus,whilethepower­and­controlmodelprovidesanimportantdistalcontextfromwhichtoexplorepart­nerabuse,theproximalmotivationsunderlyingviolentactsareusuallycomplexandmultideterminedratherthanstraightforwardexpressionsofdominanceandcontrol.

Priorreviewsof the literatureregardingriskfactorsforIPVhaveconcludedthat thecontextof IPV is indeed the relationship—violent couples also tend tobedistressedandunhappycouples(Dobash&Dobash,1979;Schumacheretal.,2001).Overall,thedatasuggestthatproblematiccouplecommunicationpatternsarestrongdeterminantsofrelationshipdistressandthatlowerlevelsofrelation­shipsatisfactiondifferentiateviolentfromnonviolentcouples(O’Learyetal.,1989;Rogge&Bradbury,1999).IPVtendstoaccompanyrelationaldistressandverbalarguments(O’Leary,1999)andisitselfastrongpredictorofrelationshiptermina­tion.Itfollows,then,thatthemanyexistingstrategiesforthetreatmentofrelation­shipdysfunctioncanbeusefullyappliedtothispopulation.

Researchon themutualnatureof IPV further illustrates the importanceof contextual factors in relationship conflict and abusive behavior (see alsoChapter12inthisvolume). Whenonepartnerhasbeenphysicallyaggressiveinarelationship, it ishighly likelythattheotherpartnerhasbeenphysicallyaggressiveaswell(Archer,2000).Therefore,itbecomescriticaltounderstandtheusualwaysthatcoupleswithaviolentpartnerinteractaboutmattersbothmundaneandseriousandto integrate this information intoeffectiveclinicalinterventions. An important area of research in this regard is based on theanalysis of the sequential behavioral interaction patterns of violent couples.Researchers have found that, relative to nonviolent couples, violent couplesexhibit more offensive negative behaviors during conflict discussions as wellas more reciprocal patterns of negative communication (Berns, Jacobson, &Gottman,1999; Burmanetal.,1993; Cordova,Jacobson,Gottman,Rushe,&Cox,1993;Jacobsonetal.,1994;Margolin,John,&Gleberman,1988).Inpar­ticular,violentcouplesseemtobelockedinapatternofreciprocatedbelliger­ence,contempt,disgust,andoverthostility,withbothpartnersrespondingtotheother’snegativebehaviorwithsimilarlynegativereactions(Gottman,1994).IncontrasttoDuluthModelassumptions,fewdifferenceshavebeenobservedonthesevariablesbetweenhusbandsandwiveswithinviolentcouples.These

Page 194: the Psychology of Social Conflict and Aggression Sydney Symposium in Social Psychology

intimate partner violence 177

data suggestwhathas longbeenobserved in clinical settings: amongviolentcouples,bothpartnersarelikelytobenegative,reactive,andlockedinacom­petitivebattletodefeattheother.Thiscontextualrealityneitherabsolvestheperpetratorfromhisorherdecisiontoactabusivelynorblamesthevictimforhis or her victimization. But it seems reasonable to suggest that a completeunderstandingofIPVrequiresknowledgeofthecontextinwhichitoccursandthatthiscontextalsoincludesthebehaviorofbothpartners(Jacobson,1994;Murphy&Eckhardt,2005).

There isnotopicmorecontentiousamongproponentsoftheDuluthModelthanthenotionofrelationalriskfactorsforIPV.Giventhatthemodelemergedasaconsequenceofintensiveadvocacyeffortstoimprovethelivesofabusedwomen,itfollowsthattherewouldbelittleinterestamongDuluthModelproponentstoexamine how behaviors from both partners, including female victims, providethecontextforIPV.Advocacygroupsthatcreateandenforcestandardsforabuseintervention programs do not include relational risk factors in lists of variablesthat predict IPV, since doing so, from the standpoint of the Duluth Model, istantamounttoblamingthevictim.Indeed,almostallstateswithsuchstandardscautionagainstorexplicitlyprohibit interventionstrategiesthat involvecouples’basedtreatment.

But is there evidence that modifying interpersonal and communicationskills in partner violence interventions is associated with nonviolent change?Yes. Recent research indicates that interventions based on improving couplecommunicationandrelationshipskillsareatleastaseffectiveatpreventingnewIPVepisodesasstandardinterventionprogramsorothercomparisoninterven­tions(foramoredetailedreviewseeMurphy&Eckhardt,2005).Forexample,Dunford(2000)examinedtheeffectsofa26­weekcognitive­behavioralgroupBIP,a26­weekcouples’ therapygroup,arigorousmonitoringgroup,orano­treatmentcontrolgroupforIPVperpetratorsinthemilitary.Follow­upreportsfromfemalepartnersofmaleparticipantsgathered6and12monthspost­treat­mentindicatedthatindividualsassignedtoalltreatmentsexhibitedreductionsin IPV; no differences in recidivism were found in male­to­female physicalaggressionacross the fourgroups. Inaddition,usingcouplesvolunteering fortreatmentatauniversitymaritaldistressclinic,O’Leary,Heyman,andNeidig(1999)foundnodifferenceinrecidivismbetweenmenassignedtoeithercou­ples’treatmentversusagroupDuluthModelintervention.Similarresultsusingacourt­referredsamplewerereportedbyBrannenandRubin(1996).Thus,onecaneitherconcludethattreatmentthatfocusesonimprovingrelationshipskillsisunwarrantedsinceitdoesnobetterthanmoretraditionalgrouptreatments,oronecanperhapsseecouples’treatmentasausefulalternativeforsomevio­lentcouples(especiallythosewhoareclearlyplanningonstayingtogether)sinceitappearstoworkjustaswellastraditionalinterventions.However,theclini­cianinterestedinimplementingcouples’treatmentmusttakeextremecaretomake sure thecouple is indeedappropriate for the interventionand that theinterventiondoesnotexacerbateexistingproblemsinwaysthatincreaserisksoffutureIPVvictimization(formore,seeLaTaillade,Epstein,&Werlinich,2006;Murphy&Eckhardt,2005).

Page 195: the Psychology of Social Conflict and Aggression Sydney Symposium in Social Psychology

chriS eckhardt178

concluSionSSubstantialprogresshasbeenmadeinthedevelopmentofetiologicmodelsofIPVandinterventionsforindividualswhoassaulttheirrelationshippartners.Despitethedominatingpresenceof thepatriarchalsocialization (“Duluth”)model, research­ershavedevelopedtheoriesofIPVthathavebroadenedthefactorsthataccountforabusivebehaviorincouples,withlessideologicallybasedandmoreempiricallybasedfindingsconcerningrisk factors for IPV(e.g.,Finkel,2007;O’Learyetal.,2007),withaneyetowardtranslatingthisworkintomorefocusedinterventionsforperpetrators(e.g.,Murphy&Eckhardt,2005).Usingmethodsheavily influencedbysocialpsychologicalresearchers,thecurrentstateofIPVresearchisrobustandinformative.Relativetononperpetrators,partner­violentindividualsexhibitavari­etyofsocialinformationprocessingdisturbancesandshowmorefavorableattitudestowardviolenceasanacceptableconflictresolutionstrategy.Intermsofemotionregulation,thelimitedresearchavailableindicatesthatIPVperpetratorsshowmoredisturbancesinangerexperienceandexpressionrelativetononviolentcomparisonsamplesandthatproblemsrelatingtoangercontrolarelinearlyrelatedtothesever­ityandfrequencyofIPVperpetration.LaboratorystudiesindicatethatrelativetononviolentmalesIPVperpetratorsinducedtofeelangryaremorelikelytorespondtorelationshipconflictsituationswithexpressionsofverbalaggression,belligerence,and hostile conflict strategies. Data also clearly indicate the relational nature ofviolentconflicttactics:abusivebehavior,whilealwaystheresponsibilityoftheindi­vidualperpetrator,emergesinparticularrelationshipcontextsandfollowsasome­timespredictablepatternofreciprocatedandescalatinginterpersonalprocesses.

Together, thesefindingsmake for a compelling frameworkaroundwhich tostructureinterventionprogramsfornonviolentchangeandtoinformpolicymak­erstaskedwithpreventingtheoccurrenceofsuchviolence.However,despitetheproductivityofIPVresearchersandtheincreasingsophisticationofthemethodsusedtoexamineIPVdynamics,thegulfbetweenresearchersandpractitioners–policymakersremainsenormous.Asdiscussedinthisreview,acentralreasonfortheever­wideningnatureofthisdivideistheideologicalresistanceonthepartofprofeministgroupsadoptingtheDuluth“powerandcontrol”ModelofIPVtowardthetypesofvariablesandthespecificconclusionsofferedbybehavioralscienceresearchconcerningtheetiologyofIPV.AsnotedinacritiqueofthismodelbyDuttonandCorvo(2006),researchthatcouldotherwiseimproveourunderstand­ingofthecausesofpartnerabuseandinformtreatmenteffortsisderided,reinter­preted,orignored:

Againstanationalmovementtowardevidence­basedandbest­practicecriteriaforassessingprogramcontinuance,interventionswithperpetratorsofdomes­tic violence remain immune to those evaluative criteria. The strangleholdon theoryandpolicydevelopment that theDuluthmodelexerts confoundseffortstoimprovetreatment.Thereisnorationalreasonfordomesticviolenceto be viewed outside of the broad theoretical and professional frameworksusedtoanalyzeandrespondtomostcontemporarybehavioralandpsychologi­calproblems.Onthecontrary,thisisolationofdomesticviolencehasresultedinabackwateroftautologicalpseudo­theoryandfailedinterventionprograms.

Page 196: the Psychology of Social Conflict and Aggression Sydney Symposium in Social Psychology

intimate partner violence 179

No other area of established social welfare, criminal justice, public health,or behavioral intervention has such weak evidence in support of mandatedpractice.(p.478)

Thus,thereisstillquiteadistancelefttotravel ifthesocialandbehavioralsciences are to make the paradigm shift toward an approach to understandingIPVthattakesadvantageofthemostthatoursciencehastooffer.Oneadvantageof an approach informed by science, asopposed to ideology, is anopenness tonovelfindings,tonewconstructs,andtonewcollaborationsthatmayeventuallyproveuseful (ornot) toourunderstandingof a givenphenomenon. “Givenourawarenessofthelimitationsofcurrentapproaches…,itisourobligationtoapplywhatweknowabout thecomplexityofpartner abuse to improve theprogramsintendedtoendit”(Stuart,2005,p.262).Forattheendoftheday,nomatterhowmuchideologicalresistanceoneexperiences,ourallegiancetoanopen,scientifi­callydrivenapproachtoknowledgediscoveryprovidesthebesthopetosolvetheonegoalthateveryoneassociatedwiththeIPVfieldcanagreeon:toreducethelikelihoodthatinvolvementinaromanticrelationshipmeansanincreasedriskforviolentvictimization.

referenceS

Abelson,R.P.(1981).Psychologicalstatusofthescriptconcept.American Psychologist, 36,715–729.

Adams,D.(1988).Treatmentmodelsofmenwhobatter:Aprofeministanalysis.InK.Yllö&M.Bograd(Eds.),National conference for family violence researchers (pp.176–199).ThousandOaks,CA:Sage.

Anderson,C.A.,&Bushman,B.J.(2002).Humanaggression.Annual Review of Psychology, 53,27–51.

Anglin,K.,&Holtzworth­Munroe,A. (1997).Comparing the responsesofmaritally vio­lent andnonviolentspousestoproblematicmaritalandnonmaritalsituations:Aretheskilldeficitsof physically aggressivehusbandsandwivesglobal? Journal of Family Psychology, 11,301–313.

Archer,J.(2000).Sexdifferencesinaggressionbetweenheterosexualpartners:Ameta­ana­lyticreview.Psychological Bulletin, 126,651–680.

Archer,J.(2002).Sexdifferencesinphysicallyaggressiveactsbetweenheterosexualpart­ners.Ameta­analyticreview. Aggression and Violent Behavior, 7(4),313–351.

Archer,J.,&Graham­Kevan,N.(2003).Dobeliefsaboutaggressionpredictphysicalaggres­siontopartners?Aggressive Behavior, 29,41–54.

Babcock,J.C.,Green, C.E.,&Robie,C. (2004).Doesbatterers’treatmentwork?Ameta­ana­lyticreviewofdomesticviolencetreatment.Clinical Psychology Review, 23,1023–1053.

Babcock,J.C.,Waltz,J.,Jacobson,N.S.,&Gottman,J.M.(1993).Powerandviolence:The relationbetweencommunicationpatterns,powerdiscrepancies, and domesticviolence.Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 61,40–50.

Bandura,A.(1973).Aggression: A social learning analysis. Oxford,England:Prentice­Hall.Barbour,K.A.,Eckhardt,C.I.,Davison,G.C.,&Kassinove,H.(1998).Theexperience

andexpressionofangerinmaritallyviolentandmaritallydiscordant­nonviolentmen.Behavior Therapy, 29, 173–191.

Baron,R.A.,&Richardson,D.R.(1994).Human aggression (2nded.).NewYork:Plenum.

Page 197: the Psychology of Social Conflict and Aggression Sydney Symposium in Social Psychology

chriS eckhardt180

Baumeister,R.,Catanese,K.,&Wallace,H.(2002).Conquestbyforce:Anarcissisticreac­tancetheoryofrapeandsexualcoercion.Review of General Psychology, 6, 92–135.

Beck,A.T.(1999).Prisoners of hate: The cognitive basis of anger, hostility, and violence. NewYork:HarperCollins.

Berkowitz,L.(1993).Aggression: Its causes, consequences, and control. NewYork:McGraw­Hill.

Berns,S.B.,Jacobson,N.S.,&Gottman,J.M.(1999).Demand­withdrawinteractionincoupleswithaviolenthusband.Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 67,666–674.

Bograd, M. (1984). Family systems approaches to wife battering: A feminist critique.American Journal of Orthopsychiatry, 54,558–568.

Brannen, S. J., & Rubin, A. (1996). Comparing the effectiveness of gender specific andcouples groups in court­mandated spouse abuse treatment programs. Research on Social Work Practice, 6, 405–424.

Burke,L.K.,&Follingstad,D.R.(1999).Violenceinlesbianandgayrelationships:Theory,prevalence,andcorrelationalfactors.Clinical Psychology Review, 19, 487–512.

Burman,B.,Margolin,G.,&John,R.S.(1993).America’sangriesthomevideos:Behavioralcontingenciesobservedinhomereenactmentsofmaritalconflict.Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 61, 28–39.

Centers for Disease Control and Prevention. (2003). Costs of intimate partner violence against women in the United States.Atlanta,GA:Author.

Chase,K.A.,O’Leary,K.D.,&Heyman,R.E.(2001).Categorizingpartner­violentmenwithin the reactive­proactive typology model. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 69, 567–572.

Cohen,D.J.,Eckhardt,C.I.,&Schagat,K.D. (1998).Attentionallocationandhabitu­ation to anger­related stimuli during a visual search task. Aggressive Behavior, 24,399–409.

Coleman,D.,&Straus,M.A.(1985).Maritalpower,conflictandviolenceinanationallyrepresentativesampleofAmericancouples.Violence and Victims, 1(2), 141–157.

Cordova,J.V.,Jacobson,N.S.,Gottman,J.M.,Rushe,R.,&Cox,G.(1993).Negativereci­procityandcommunicationincoupleswithaviolenthusband.Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 102, 559–564.

Daly,M.,&Wilson,M.(1988).Homicide. NewYork:AldinedeGruyter.Davison, G. C., Robins, C., & Johnson, M.K. (1983). Articulated thoughtsduring simu­

latedsituations:Aparadigmforstudyingcognitioninemotionandbehavior.Cognitive Therapy and Research, 7, 17–40.

Dobash,R.E.,&Dobash,R.(1979).Violence against wives: A case against the patriarchy. NewYork:FreePress.

Dodge,K.A.(1991).Thestructureandfunctionofreactiveandproactiveaggression.InD.J.Pepler&K.H.Rubin(Eds.),Earlscourt symposium on childhood aggression (pp.201–218).Hillsdale,NJ:Erlbaum.

Dunford,F.W. (2000).TheSanDiegoNavyexperiment:Anassessmentof interventionsformenwhoassaulttheirwives.Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 68, 468–476.

Dutton,D.G.(1988).Profilingofwifeassaulters:Preliminaryevidenceforatrimodalanaly­sis.Violence and Victims, 3,5–29.

Dutton,D.G.,&Corvo,K.(2006).Transformingaflawedpolicy:Acalltorevivepsychol­ogy andscienceindomesticviolenceresearchandpractice.Aggression and Violent Behavior, 11, 457–483.

Dutton,D.G.,&Nicholls,T.L.(2005).Thegenderparadigmindomesticviolenceresearchandtheory:Part1—Theconflictoftheoryanddata.Aggression and Violent Behavior, 10, 680–714.

Page 198: the Psychology of Social Conflict and Aggression Sydney Symposium in Social Psychology

intimate partner violence 181

Dutton,D.G.,Saunders,K.,Starzomski,A.,&Bartholomew,K. (1994). Intimacy­angerandinsecureattachmentasprecursorsofabuseinintimaterelationships.Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 24, 1367–1386.

Eckhardt,C.I.(2007).Effectsofalcoholintoxicationonangerexperienceandexpressionamong partner assaultive men. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 75, 61–71.

Eckhardt,C.I.,Barbour,K.A.,&Davison,G.C.(1998).Articulatedthoughtsofmaritallyviolentandnonviolentmenduringangerarousal.Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 66, 259–269.

Eckhardt,C.I.,Barbour,K.A.,&Stuart,G.L.(1997).Angerandhostilityinmaritallyvio­lentmen:Conceptualdistinctions,measurementissues,andliteraturereview.Clinical Psychology Review, 17, 333–358.

Eckhardt,C.I.,&Cohen,D.J.(1997).Attentiontoanger­relevantandirrelevantstimulifollowingnaturalisticinsult.Personality and Individual Differences, 23,619–629.

Eckhardt,C.I.,&Dye,M.L.(2000).Thecognitivecharacteristicsofmaritallyviolentmen:Theoryandevidence. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 24, 139–158.

Eckhardt, C. I., Holtzworth­Munroe, A., Norlander, B., Sibley, A., & Cahill, M. (2008).Readinesstochange,partnerviolencesubtypes,andtreatmentoutcomesamongmenintreatmentforpartnerassault.Violence and Victims, 23, 446–475.

Eckhardt,C.I.,&Jamison,T.R.(2002).Articulatedthoughtsofmaledatingviolenceper­petratorduringangerarousal.Cognitive Therapy and Research, 26, 289–308.

Eckhardt,C.I.,&Kassinove,H.(1998).Articulatedcognitivedistortionsandcognitivedefi­cienciesinmaritallyviolentmen.Journal of Cognitive Psychotherapy, 12, 231–250.

Eckhardt,C.,Samper,R.,Holtzworth­Munroe,A.,&Suhr,L.(2010).Implicit associations towards violence among intimate partner violence perpetrators.Manuscriptsubmit­tedforpublication.

Eckhardt,C.I.,Samper,R.,&Murphy,C.(2008).Angerdisturbancesamongperpetratorsofintimatepartnerviolence:Clinicalcharacteristicsandoutcomesofcourt­mandatedtreatment.Journal of Interpersonal Violence, 23,1600–1617.

Ellis,A.(1962).Reason and emotion in psychotherapy.NewYork:CitadelPress.Ellis,A.(1994).Reason and emotion in psychotherapy (2nded.).NewYork:CitadelPress.Feder,L.,&Wilson,D.(2005).Ameta­analyticreviewofcourt­mandatedbattererinter­

vention programs: Can courts affect abusers’ behavior? Journal of Experimental Criminology, 1, 239–262.

Field,C.A.,Caetano,R.,&Nelson,S.(2004).Alcoholandviolencerelatedcognitiveriskfactorsassociatedwiththeperpetrationofintimatepartnerviolence.Journal of Family Violence, 19,249–253.

Fincham,F.D.,Bradbury,T.N.,Arias,I.,Byrne,C.A.,&Karney,B.R.(1997).Maritalvio­lence,maritaldistress,andattributions.Journal of Family Psychology, 11, 367–372.

Finkel,E.J.(2007).Impellingandinhibitingforcesintheperpetrationofintimatepartnerviolence.Review of General Psychology, 11, 193–207.

Golding, J.M. (1999). Intimate partner violenceas a risk factor formentaldisorders:Ameta­analysis.Journal of Family Violence, 14, 99–132.

Gondolf,E.W.(2002).Batterer intervention systems. ThousandOaks,CA:SagePublications.Gondolf,E.W.(2004).Evaluatingbatterercounselingprograms:Adifficulttaskshowing

someeffectsandimplications.Aggression and Violent Behavior, 9, 605–631.Gondolf,E.W.(2007).TheoreticalandresearchsupportfortheDuluthmodel:Areplyto

DuttonandCorvo.Aggression and Violent Behavior, 12, 644–657.Gondolf,E.W.,&Russell,D.(1986).Thecaseagainstangercontroltreatmentprogramsfor

batterers.Response to the Victimization of Women & Children, 9,2–5.

Page 199: the Psychology of Social Conflict and Aggression Sydney Symposium in Social Psychology

chriS eckhardt182

Gottman,J.M.,Jacobson,N.S.,Rushe,R.H.,Shortt,J.W.,Babcock,J.C.,LaTaillade, J. J.,et al. (1995). The relationship between heart rate reactivity, emotionally aggres­sive behavior, and general violence in batterers. Journal of Family Psychology, 9, 227–248.

Haaga, D. A., Dyck, M. J., & Ernst, D. (1991). Empirical status of cognitive theory ofdepression.Psychological Bulletin, 110, 215–236.

Healey,K.,Smith,C.,&O’Sullivan,C.(1998).Batterer intervention: Program approaches and criminal justice strategies.Washington,DC:NationalInstituteofJustice.

Hershorn,M.,&Rosenbaum,A.(1991).Over­vs.undercontrolledhostility:Applicationoftheconstructtotheclassificationofmaritallyviolentmen.Violence and Victims, 6, 151–58.

Holtzworth­Munroe,A.(1992).Social skilldeficits inmaritallyviolentmen:Interpretingthe datausingasocialinformationprocessingmodel.Clinical Psychology Review, 12,605–617.

Holtzworth­Munroe,A.(2000).Socialinformationprocessingskillsdeficitsinmaritallyvio­lentmen:Summaryofaresearchprogram.InJ.P.Vincent&E.N.Jouriles(Eds.),Domestic violence: Guidelines for research-informed practice (pp.13–36).London:JessicaKingsleyPublishers.

Holtzworth­Munroe,A.,&Hutchinson,G.(1993). Attributingnegativeintenttowifebehav­ior:Theattributionsofmaritallyviolentversusnonviolentmen. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 102, 206–211.

Holtzworth­Munroe, A.Rehman,U., & Herron, K. (2000). General and spouse specificangerandhostilityinsubtypesofmaritallyviolentmenandnonviolentmen.Behavior Therapy, 31,603–630.

Holtzworth­Munroe,A.,&Smutzler,N. (1996).Comparing theemotional reactionsandbehavioralintentionsofviolentandnonviolenthusbandstoaggressive,distressed,andotherwifebehaviors.Violence and Victims, 11,319–339.

Holtzworth­Munroe,A.,Smutzler,N.,&Stuart,G.L.(1998).Demandandwithdrawcom­municationamongcouplesexperiencinghusbandviolence.Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 66,731–743.

Holtzworth­Munroe,A.,&Stuart,G.L.(1994).Typologiesofmalebatterers:Threesub­typesandthedifferencesamongthem.Psychological Bulletin, 116,476–497.

HotalingG.,&Sugarman,D.(1986).Ananalysisofriskmarkersinhusbandtowifevio­lence:Thecurrentstateofknowledge.Violence and Victims, 2,101–124.

Huesmann,L.R.(1988).Aninformationprocessingmodelforthedevelopmentofaggres­sion.Aggressive Behavior, 14,13–24.

Jacobson,N.(1994).Rewardsanddangersinresearchingdomesticviolence.Family Process, 33, 81–85.

Jacobson,N.S.,Gottman,J.M.,Waltz,J.,Rushe,R.,Babcock,J.,&Holtzworth­Monroe,A.(1994).Affect,verbalcontent,andpsychophysiologyintheargumentsofcoupleswithaviolenthusband. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 62,982–988.

Kaufman­Kantor,G.,&Straus,M.A.(1990).The“drunkenbum”theoryofwifebeating.InM.Straus&R.Gelles(Eds.),Physical violence in American families(pp.203–224).NewBrunswick,NJ:Transaction.

LaTaillade,J.J.,Epstein,N.B.,&Werlinich,C.A.(2006).Conjointtreatmentofintimatepartnerviolence:Acognitivebehavioralapproach.Journal of Cognitive Psychotherapy, 20,393–410.

Leonard,K. (2001).Alcoholandsubstanceabuse inmaritalviolenceandchildmaltreat­ment.InC.Wekerle&A.Wall(Eds.),The violence and addiction equation(pp.194–219).NewYork:Brunner­Routledge.

Page 200: the Psychology of Social Conflict and Aggression Sydney Symposium in Social Psychology

intimate partner violence 183

Leonard,K.E.,&Roberts,L.J.(1998).Theeffectsofalcoholonthemaritalinteractionsof aggressive and nonaggressive husbands and their wives. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 107,602–615.

Lie, G., Schilit, R., Bush, J., Montague, M., & Reyes, L. (1991). Lesbians in currentlyaggressiverelationships:Howfrequentlydotheyreportaggressivepastrelationships?Violence and Victims, 6,121–135.

Maiuro,R.D.,Hagar,T.S.,Lin,H.,&Olson,N.(2001).Arecurrentstatestandardsfordomesticviolenceperpetratortreatmentadequatelyinformedbyresearch?Aques­tionofquestions.Journal of Aggression, Maltreatment & Trauma, 5, 21–44.

Malik,N.M.,&Lindahl,K.M.(1998).Aggressionanddominance:Therolesofpowerandcultureindomesticviolence.Clinical Psychology: Science and Practice, 5,409–423.

Margolin,G.,John,R.S.,&Gleberman,L.(1988).Affectiveresponsestoconflictualdiscus­sionsinviolentandnonviolentcouples.Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 56,24–33.

McMurran,M.,&Gilchrist,E.(2008).Angercontrolandalcoholuse:Appropriateinterven­tionsforperpetratorsofdomesticviolence?Psychology, Crime, & Law, 14, 107–116.

Murphy,C.M.,&Eckhardt,C.I.(2005).Treating the abusive partner: An individualized cognitive-behavioral approach.NewYork:Guilford.

Murphy,C.M.,Taft,C.T.,&Eckhardt,C.I.(2007).Angerproblemprofilesamongpartnerviolentmen:Differencesinclinicalpresentationandtreatmentoutcome.Journal of Counseling Psychology, 54,189–200.

Nisbett, R. (1993). Violence and U.S. regional culture. American Psychologist, 48,441–449.

Nisbett,R.,&Wilson,T.D.(1977).Tellingmorethanwecanknow:Verbalreportsonmen­talprocesses.Psychological Review, 84,231–259.

Norlander,B.,&Eckhardt,C.(2005).Anger,hostility,andmaleperpetratorsofintimatepartnerviolence:Ameta­analyticreview.Clinical Psychology Review, 25,119–152.

O’Leary,K.D.(1988).Physicalaggressionbetweenspouses:Asociallearningtheoryper­spective.InV.B.VanHasselt,R.Morrison,A.Bellack,&M.Hersen(Eds.),Handbook of family violence(pp.31–55).NewYork:Plenum.

O’Leary,K.D.(1999).Psychologicalabuse:Avariabledeservingcriticalattentionindomes­ticviolence.Violence and Victims, 14,3–23.

O’Leary, K. D., Barling, J., Arias, I., Rosenbaum, A., Malone, J., & Tyree, A. (1989).Prevalenceandstabilityofphysicalaggressionbetweenspouses:Alongitudinalanaly­sis.Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 57, 263–268.

O’Leary,K.D.,Heyman,R.E.,&Neidig,P.H.(1999).Treatmentofwifeabuse:Acompari­sonofgender­specificandconjointapproaches.Behavior Therapy, 30,475–505.

O’Leary,K.,Smith­Slep,A.,&O’Leary,S.(2007).Multivariatemodelsofmen’sandwom­en’spartneraggression.Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 75,752–764.

Pence,E.(1983).TheDuluthdomesticabuseinterventionproject.Hamline Law Review, 6,247–275.

Pence,E.,&Paymar,M.(1993).Education groups for men who batter: The Duluth model.NewYork:Springer.

Rogge,R.D.,&Bradbury,T.N.(1999).Tillviolencedoesuspart:Thedifferingrolesofcommunication and aggression in predicting adverse marital outcomes. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 67,340–351.

Saunders,D.(1984).Helpinghusbandswhobatter. Social Casework, 65,347–353.Saunders,D.G.(1992).Atypologyofmenwhobatter:Threetypesderivedfromcluster

analysis.AmericanJournal of Orthopsychiatry, 62,264–275.Schafer, J.,Caetano,R.,&ClarkC.L. (1998).Ratesof intimatepartnerviolence in the

UnitedStates.American Journal of Public Health, 88,1702–1704.

Page 201: the Psychology of Social Conflict and Aggression Sydney Symposium in Social Psychology

chriS eckhardt184

Schumacher,J.A.,Feldbau­Kohn,S.,Slep,A.M.S.,&Heyman,R.E.(2001).Riskfac­torsformale­to­femalepartnerphysicalabuse.Aggression and Violent Behavior, 6,281–352.

Stith,S.M.,Smith,D.B.,Penn,C.E.,Ward,D.B.,&Tritt,D.(2004).Intimatepartnerphysical abuse perpetration and victimization risk factors: A meta­analytic review.Aggression and Violent Behavior, 10, 65–98.

Straus,M.A.(1976).Sexualinequality,culturalnorms,andwife­beating.Victimology: An International Journal, 1,54–76.

Straus,M.A.,&Gelles,R.J.(1990).HowviolentareAmericanfamilies?InM.A.Straus& R. J. Gelles (Eds.), Physical violence in American families (pp. 95–108). NewBrunswick,NJ:TransactionPublishers.

Stuart,R.B.(2005).Treatmentforpartnerabuse:Timeforaparadigmshift.Professional Psychology: Research and Practice, 36,254–263.

Sugarman,D.B.,&Frankel,S.L.(1996).Patriarchalideologyandwife­assault:Ameta­analyticreview.Journal of Family Violence, 11,13–40.

Tavris,C.(1989).Anger: The misunderstood emotion(rev.ed.).NewYork:Touchstone.Tjaden,P.,&Thoennes,N. (1998).Prevalence, incidenceandconsequencesof violence

against women: Findings from the National Violence Against Women Survey.Washington,DC:U.S.DepartmentofJustice.

Walker,L.(1984).The battered woman syndrome.NewYork:Springer.Waltz,J.,Babcock,J.C.,Jacobson,N.S.,&Gottman,J.M.(2000).Testingatypologyof

batterers.Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 68,658–669.Wathen, C. N., & MacMillan, H. L. (2003). Interventions for violence against women:

Scientificreview.Journal of the American Medical Association, 289, 589–600.

Page 202: the Psychology of Social Conflict and Aggression Sydney Symposium in Social Psychology

185

12InterdependentGoalsand

RelationshipConflictGRáINNEM.FITZSIMONSandJOANNAE.ANDERSON

University of Waterloo

I ncloserelationships,partners’outcomesaremutuallydependent.Thisexten­sive everyday interdependence is what brings much of what people desirefromrelationships(e.g.,intimacy,understanding,support,andstability)but

is also what bringsmuchof what people fear (e.g., hurt, pain, obstruction, andstrife; see Chapters 3, 11, 13, and 14 in this volume). From its very inception,InterdependenceTheoryhasexplicitlyconnectedinterdependenceandthelikelyoccurrenceof interpersonal conflict (Braiker&Kelley, 1979;Kelley&Thibaut,1978),notingthatwithgreaterinterdependencecomesgreateropportunitiesforpartnerstobothfacilitateandobstructeachother’sgoals.Becausegoalobstruc­tionisacommontriggerofconflictandaggression(seeChapter10inthisvolume),thegreateropportunitiesforobstructionthataccompanyinterdependencecaninturngeneratemoreoccurrencesofstrifeinrelationships.

Ifindividualgoalpursuitsareenmeshedintheeverydayinterdependenceofromanticrelationships(seeChapters2and6inthisvolume),thenitmaybefruit­fulfortheunderstandingofbothself­regulatoryandrelationshipprocessestoelu­cidatetherelationsofonepartner’sgoalstotheother’s.Inthecurrentresearch,wesuggestthatthesimilarityofthepartners’personalgoalssuchascareergoals,financialgoals,andhealthandfitnessgoalsmaybeanimportantfactorinpredict­ingbothindividuals’progressontheirgoalsaswellasrelationshipcoordination,compatibility,andconflict.

Inthischapter,wepresentrecentfindingsexaminingthelinksbetweengoalsimilarityandrelationshipconflict,testingthehypothesisthatromanticpartnerswhopursuedissimilarpersonalgoalsexperiencemoreconflictintheirrelationship.Dissimilarpersonalgoalsarelikeliertobeincompatiblegoals,interferingwitheachother’sprogress,andwethussuggestthatdissimilarpersonalpursuitswillleadto

Page 203: the Psychology of Social Conflict and Aggression Sydney Symposium in Social Psychology

gráinne m. fitzSimonS and Joanna e. anderSon186

moreconflictintherelationshipoverall,viaadirecteffectofgoalobstructiononconflict.Dissimilargoalsarealsolikelytocreateconflictviamoreindirectroutes,suchasperceptionsthatone’spartnerisnotresponsivetoone’sneeds(Reis,Clark,&Holmes,2004)andfeelingsofbeingunsupported(Brunstein,Dangelmayer,&Schultheiss,1996).

Background: Similarity and likingAccordingtothesimilarity–attractionhypothesis,individualsevaluateotherswhosharetheirattitudesmorepositivelythantheyevaluateotherswhopossessdissimi­larattitudes(Byrne,1971;Byrne,Clore,&Worchel,1966).Inaclassicprogramofresearch,Byrnedemonstratedthatindividualsevaluatedabogusstrangermorepositivelyafterreadingthatthestrangerpossessedattitudes(aboutissuessuchasGodandpremarital sex)thatweresimilar tothosereportedbytheparticipantsthemselves (Byrne, 1961). Byrne theorized that similarity produced its positiveeffectsbyvalidatingindividuals’ownviewsandattitudesabouttheworld,whichisinherentlyrewarding.Becauseindividualsassociatetherewardofvalidationwiththestranger,theylikehimorhermore(Byrne,1961).Thisfindinganditsinterpre­tationhavebeenchallenged(e.g.,Rosenbaum,1986),butthepositivelinkbetweenattraction and similarity is largely viewed as abasic principle underlying initialattraction(e.g.,Berscheid&Walster,1978).

Althoughthelinkbetweensimilarityandinitialattractionamongstrangersiswidelyaccepted,thelinkbetweensimilarityandsatisfactionwithexistingrelation­shippartners is lessclear.Findingshavebeenmixed (Buunk&Bosman,1986;Montoya,Horton,&Kirchner,2008;Morry,2005,2007),andmanystudieshavesuffered from data­analytic weaknesses that limit their interpretability (Griffin,Murray, & Gonzalez, 1999; see also the review in Karney & Bradbury, 1995).Despitetheseconcerns,themostoftenreportedfindingisstillthatsimilarpart­nersaremoresatisfiedpartners(e.g.,Gaunt,2006;Luo&Klohnen,2005).Lessworkhasdirectlyexaminedconflictasanoutcome;however,somefindingshavesuggestedthatsimilarityalsoreducestheoccurrenceofeverydayconflict(Surra&Longstreth,1990),andmanyauthorshavepositedthatreducedconflictisoneroutethroughwhichthesimilarity–satisfactioneffectmayoccur(e.g.,Esterberg,Moen,&Dempster­McCain,1994).

Mostoftheresearchtodateonsimilarityhasexaminedpersonalitytraitsandattitudes,withnostudiesonsimilarpersonalgoalpursuitsperse.However,sev­eralstudieshaveexaminedsimilarityincharacteristicsrelatedtopersonalgoals,findingthatsimilaractivitypreferences,atleastinsomedomains,predictreducedconflictandincreasedrelationshipsatisfaction(Surra&Longstreth,1990)andthatsimilarneedsforautonomyandaffiliationpredicthigherreportsofmaritaladjust­ment(Meyer&Pepper,1977).

current reSearchThus,buildingonpriorwork,weexploredtheinfluenceofsimilarpersonalgoalson the frequency and intensity of relationship conflict. Because this chapter is

Page 204: the Psychology of Social Conflict and Aggression Sydney Symposium in Social Psychology

interdependent goalS and relationShip conflict 187

inspiredbyInterdependenceTheoryandtheideasaredrivenbyour interestinlinkinginterdependencetheorizingaboutrelationshipswithworkonself­regula­tion,ouremphasisdiffersfrompriorworkonsimilarity.Whereaspastworkhasfocused on links between similarity of personality traits, attitudes, values, anddemographic characteristics, this chapter focuses on similarity in personal goalpursuits.Personalgoalshavenothingtodowithrelationships—theyaretheindi­vidual’sownquests—andthusaretheidealcharacteristictostudytotestinterde­pendencetheory’spredictionsaboutsimilarityandconflict.Inoneofourstudies,we test theroleofotherkindsofsimilaritywithin relationships todetermine iftheseinterdependentgoalsproduceanyuniqueeffectsonconflict.

Similarly,whereaspastworkhasexaminedtheimpactofsimilarityonattrac­tion, liking, or satisfaction, weemphasize the moredirect outcome that shouldstem from interdependencecosts—conflict.Finally,while themostwell­knownfindingsonsimilarityhavestudiedattractiontostrangers,wefocusonsimilaritywithinestablishedlong­termromanticrelationships.Becauseoftheinherentinter­dependenceincloserelationships,wespeculatethattheprocessesthatpromoteanypositiveeffectsof similarityon reduced conflict are probably not the samesimplepositivereinforcementprinciplesthoughttodrivetheeffectsofsimilarityoninitialattractionamongstrangers.Itsurelyfeelsgoodtoknowthatone’sroman­ticpartneragreeswithone’sopinionsbutthatsenseofself­validationisonlyoneofmanypossibleprocessesthatcouldexplainasimilarity–conflictlinkwithinthismorecomplicatedinterpersonalcontext.

Forexample,HolmesandMurray(1996),intheirreviewofresearchoncon­flictinrelationships,suggestthatonemajorcauseofrelationshipconflictisthatpartners lack understanding of each other’s “untransformed” preferences—thatis,thepreferencestheywouldpossessintheabsenceofexternalinfluence.Beforepartnerscandecidehowtoact inagivensituation, theymustunderstandtheirpartner’s preferences in the situation (Kelley, 1979; Messick & Brewer, 1983).Indeed,accurateperspectivetakinghasbeenshowntobecrucialtonegotiatingandresolvingconflicts(seeChapter7inthisvolume).Unfortunately,partnersareknown to be quite inaccuratewhen it comes to perspective taking—they oftenmisreadeachother’spreferences(Kenny,1994).Inaccuraciesarethoughttostemboth frompeople’s tendencies toproject theirown preferencesonto their part­ners(a tendencythat isevenlikelier tohappenwithcloserelationshipsbecausepeopleassumemoresimilarity)andfrompeople’soverrelianceonpartners’pastovertbehaviorsascuesfortheirpreferences.Overtbehaviorscanoftenbemis­leading reflections of people’s real motivations, both because people behave inlinewith“transformed”motivations(i.e.,theyaltertheirpreferencestobettersuittheirpartner’s;Holmes&Murray,1996)andbecausepeopleoftenfailtobehaveinlinewiththeirgoals,duetoself­controlfailures(Baumeister,1998).Ifpartnersbasetheirbeliefsabouteachother’spreferencesonwhattheyseeintheirpartner’sbehaviors,thentheymaywellmissthemarkwhenitcomestounderstandingwhattheirpartnersreallywant.

Wesuggestthatbothofthesetypesofmisunderstandingsarelikeliertohap­pen when partners hold dissimilar personal goals. Partners who pursue similargoalsmayhavenobetterknowledgeoftheirpartner’spreferences;however,their

Page 205: the Psychology of Social Conflict and Aggression Sydney Symposium in Social Psychology

gráinne m. fitzSimonS and Joanna e. anderSon188

assumptionofsimilaritywillbewellfounded,andthustheywillprojectaccuratepreferencesontotheirpartners.Similarly,whenpartnershavesimilargoals,pastbehaviorislesslikelytoreflecttransformedmotivationsandmorelikelytoreflecteachpartner’sactualuntransformed interests, simplybecausethere is lessneedfortransformationwhenbothpartnerssharethesamegoals.Incontrast,partnerswithdissimilarpersonalgoalswillbelikeliertomakeparticularlyinaccuratepro­jectionsandtohavebehavedinwaysthatdon’treflecttheiruntransformedinter­ests,outofdesiretocompromiseorgetalongwitheachother.

Theseperspective­takingissuesareonepossibleroutethroughwhichdissimi­larpersonalgoalscouldgeneraterelationshipconflict.Itisalsopossiblethatpart­nerswhoholddissimilarpersonalgoalsmayobstructeachother’sgoalprogress,whichmaydirectlyleadtonegativeemotionsandconflict(Berscheid,1983,1991;Berscheid & Ammazzalorso, 2001; Fehr & Harasymchuk, 2005; Fitzsimons &Shah,2008).Whengoalsareobstructed,peopletendtofeelfrustratedwitheachother(Berscheid&Ammazzalorso,2001),andmayavoideachotherandseekmoreindependence (Fitzsimons & Shah, 2008), all of which would promote conflict.Incontrast,partnerswhoholdsimilarpersonalgoalsmay(whetherintentionallyor incidentally) facilitate each other’s goal progress,which in turn may directlyleadtopositiveemotions,closeness,andcooperation(Berscheid&Ammazzalorso;Fehr&Harasymchuk;Fitzsimons&Shah).Finally,anotherroutethroughwhichgoalsimilaritymayimpactconflictisthroughthenatureofeverydayinteractionswithinrelationships.Ifpartnersarepursuingsimilargoals,theirinteractionsmaybesmootherandmoreefficientthaninteractionsbetweenpartnerswhoarepur­suingdissimilargoals.Ifso,theymayexperiencemoreharmonyandsynchrony,whichagainmaypromotepositive,cooperativeresponses(Dalton,Chartrand,&Finkel,2010;Wiltermuth&Heath,2008). Incontrast, ifpartners arepursuingdissimilargoals,theirinteractionsmaybediscordantandinefficient,whichmayleadtomoreconflict.

Inthischapter,wedescribeseveralstudies—correlational, longitudinal,andexperimental—thatprovidepreliminary support for the importanceofpersonalgoal similarity for the experience of effortless, harmonious interactions withininterpersonal interactionsandforreducedconflict.Thefirst twostudies lookatperceptionsofgoalsimilarityandcompatibility,andtheremainingstudiescompareresponsesfrombothpartnerstogetamore“objective”measureofgoalsimilarity.

hoW doeS goal Similarity affect conflict and goal progreSS over time?

Inaninitialexplorationoftheroleofsimilarpersonalgoalsinconflict,wecon­ductedalongitudinalsurveystudyamongNewYorkUniversityundergraduatestu­dents.Wesoughttoexaminewhetherparticipantswhoperceivedtheycaredaboutanimportantgoaltothesamedegreeastheirpartnersreportedlowerfrequencyofconflictsoverthenextmonth.Becauseourparticipantswerecollegestudents,weexaminedperceivedsimilarityinthegoalofacademicachievement,whichisthemostcommonlynotedgoalinthissample.

Page 206: the Psychology of Social Conflict and Aggression Sydney Symposium in Social Psychology

interdependent goalS and relationShip conflict 189

AtTime1,aspartofabroadersurvey,participantsansweredquestionsabouttheextenttowhichtheyandtheirbestfriendorromanticpartnervaluedacademicachievement to the same degree. Specifically, they rated their agreement withthreedirectstatementslike,“Mypartner/friendandIcareequallyaboutacademicachievement.”Participantsalso rated theiragreementwith the item, “Academicachievementisveryimportanttomerightnow,”whichweusedasameasureoftheirowngoalcommitment.Asameasureofconflict,participantsreportedhowmanyfights,arguments,orconflictstheyhadhadwiththeirpartnerorfriendoverthepastmonth.Theyalsoratedtheirrelationshipsatisfactionusingtheitem,“Iamfullysatisfiedwithmyrelationship/friendship,”andtheirgoalprogressusingtheitem,“IfeelImadegoodprogressonmyacademicachievementgoalsthismonth.”AtTime2,1monthlater,participantsonceagainindicatedhowmanyfights,argu­ments,orconflictstheyandtheirpartnerorfriendhadhadoverthepastmonth,ratedtheirrelationshipsatisfactionandgoalprogressusingthesameitems,andprovidedthegradestheyhadreceivedontheirmidtermexaminations.

Aspredicted,perceivedgoalsimilaritypredictedlowerreportsofconflictoverthefollowingmonth,arelationthatheldwhencontrollingforinitialratingsofcon­flictandrelationshipsatisfactionandparticipants’ownratingsofgoalimportance.Ideally,wewouldalsohaveassessedparticipants’perceptionsoftheother’sgoalcommitmentasanadditionalcontrol: it isconceivablethatconflictisnotneces­sarilyrelatedtodyads’similarityonthisgoalbuttosomethingabouttheother’sowngoalpursuit.Becauseofthewaythegoalsimilarityitemswereworded,itisunclearwhethertheratedsimilarityresultsfromthepartnerorfriendvaluingthegoalmoreorlessthantheparticipant.Thatis,theitemsmayhaveunintentionallycaptureddissatisfactionwithaparticularlyambitiousorunambitiouspartner.Insubsequentstudies,wewerecarefultoincludeallsuchvariables,whichallowsustobemoreconfidentaboutthepreciserolethatdissimilargoalsmayplayingener­atingconflictwithinrelationships.

Nonetheless,becauseofthelongitudinalnatureofthecurrentstudy,inwhichperceivedsimilaritypredicted changeinconflictfromTime1toTime2,wecantentatively report evidence that (the perception of) similarity reduces conflict.Thatis,althoughtheavailabledatacannotruleoutthirdvariableinfluenceswiththisstudy,theydosuggestthedirectionalityofthelinkbetweensimilargoalsandconflict.Findingswereidenticalforthefrienddyadsandromanticpartnerdyads;accordingtoInterdependenceTheory,theprocessesshouldworkinthesamewayforallestablished,long­term,closerelationships.Thatis,thereisnothingquali­tativelydifferentaboutromanticrelationships.However,ifromanticrelationshipswerehigherininterdependence,astheywouldbeinmostadultsamples,wewouldexpecttoseestrongereffectsofsimilarityonconflict.Becauseoftheheightenedopportunityforobstructedgoalsthatemergesincloserrelationshipswithgreaterinterdependence,similarityshouldbemorepredictive.Withinacollegesample,though,closesame­sexfriendshipsarenotnecessarilylesscloseorinterdependentthanromanticrelationships,withmanystudentslivingwiththeirbestfriends.

Turning to examine goal progress, we found that perceived goal similaritypositivelypredictedperceptionsofprogress1monthlateraswellasbetterperfor­manceonmidtermexaminations,effectsthatheldwhilecontrollingforinitialgoal

Page 207: the Psychology of Social Conflict and Aggression Sydney Symposium in Social Psychology

gráinne m. fitzSimonS and Joanna e. anderSon190

progress.Therearemanyreasonsgoalsimilaritycouldleadtobettergoalprog­ress.Forexample,ifbothpartnerssharethesamegoals,thiscouldpromotetheconstructionofagoal­encouragingenvironment.Asanotherexample,friendsandpartnerswhoalsovalueacademicachievementmayactuallybemoreinstrumentaltogoalpursuit,throughprovidingeitherpracticalhelporemotionalsupport.

However,itisalsopossiblethatgoalsimilaritycouldaffectprogressbydecreas­ingconflictorincreasingcoordination.Indeed,theseresultsalsoindicatethattherelationshipbetweengoal similarity andgoalprogress is significantly (partially)mediatedbyconflict.Wehavesuggestedthatwhenpartnersarepursuingsimilarpersonalgoalstheireverydayinteractionsshouldbesmootherandmoreharmoni­ous.Forexample,theyshouldunderstandeachother’sperspectivesbetter,shouldhavesimilarexpectationsaboutthoseinteractions,andshouldbemorerespectfulofeachother’sgoalpursuits.Iftheireverydayinteractionsareindeedsmoother,theyarelikelyalsomoreefficientorlessdepleting.Incontrast,whenpartnersarepursuing dissimilar personal goals, they may have misunderstandings, differentexpectations,andlessrespectforeachother’sgoals,whichmaymaketheirinterac­tionslessefficientandmoreresourceconsuming(Finkeletal.,2006).Ifso,thesesubtleclashescoulddrainself­regulatoryresources,leavinglessenergyandfocusforgoalpursuit, and thusnegativelyaffectinggoalprogress.Thenext studywedescribeincludesadditionalmeasuresthatallowustoexaminethislinkbetweenhighmaintenanceinteractions(Finkeletal.)andgoalsimilarity.

What mechaniSmS drive the goal Similarity–conflict link?

Inthisnextstudy, wesoughttoreplicatethelinkbetweenperceivedgoalsimilar­ityandconflictwhileincludingsomeadditionalmeasurestoincreaseclarityabouthowgoalsimilaritymayaffectthefrequencyandintensityofrelationalconflict.Inparticular,weexaminedthepotentialroleof(1)high­maintenanceinteractionsand (2)partner instrumentality.First, asexplainedalready,partner interactionsthatarerifewithmisunderstandingsandinefficienciesmaybeoneresultofhav­ingdissimilarpersonalgoals;ifso,thesedraininginteractionsmayproducemoreconflict.Second,pastresearchhasdemonstratedthatpeoplefeelclosertopartnerswhoarehelpfulorinstrumentalforongoinggoals(Fitzsimons&Shah,2008),andwewonderedwhetherinstrumentalitycouldplayamediatingroleintheeffectsofsimilarityonrelationshipsatisfactionandconflict.That is, itmaybethecasethatgoalsimilarityproducespositiveoutcomesbecauseofastronglinkbetweengoalsimilarityandinstrumentality.Partnerswithsimilargoalsmaybemorelikelytobehelpfultoeachotherthanpartnerswithdissimilargoals.However,itisalsopossible that instrumentality may reduce conflict—people may be more agree­ableandaccommodatingtohelpfulothers—butthatitmaynotactasamediatingmechanismfortherelationbetweengoalsimilarityandconflict.

MarriedanddatingcouplesattheUniversityofWaterlootookpartinalargerinvestigation consistingof multiple sessions and spanning a 4­monthperiod. Inthesessionrelevanttothecurrentstudy,allparticipantscompletedanhour­long

Page 208: the Psychology of Social Conflict and Aggression Sydney Symposium in Social Psychology

interdependent goalS and relationShip conflict 191

seriesofonlinequestionnairesduringwhichtheyansweredquestionsabouttheirpersonalgoalpursuits, individualdifferences inmotivationalandself­regulatoryvariables,andfeaturesoftheirrelationships,includingqualityandoutcomes.

One of the measures was a new scale created to assess perceptions of per­sonalgoalsimilarity.Thisscale,modifiedfromanearliermeasure(Bohnsetal.,2010),consistedof14itemsdesignedtobeface­validmeasuresofgoalsimilarity.Participantsratedtheiragreementwithitemslike,“MypartnerandIhaveverysimilarpersonalgoals”and,“Wehavealotincommonwhenitcomestowhatper­sonalgoalswecareaboutrightnow.”Thisnewscalewaspositivelycorrelatedwithmeasuresofclosenessandsatisfaction;alleffectswedescribeherecontrolforthepotentialeffectsofthosevariablesonconflict.

Participantsalsoscoredtheamountofconflictintheirrelationship,ratingtheiragreementwithaone­itemmeasurethatread,“MypartnerandIdisagreeaboutalotofthingsdaytoday.”Amongtheothermeasures,participantscompletedcom­mitmentandsatisfactionsubscalesoftheInvestmentModelScale(Rusbult,Martz,&Agnew,1998).

Inaddition,wewantedtoexaminetheextenttowhicheverydayinteractionswithin the relationship were depleting or inefficient in nature, to determine ifhavinghigh­maintenanceinteractionsmaybeoneroutethroughwhichdissimilargoalsarerelatedtoconflict.Todoso,weincludedafour­itemmeasuretakenfromFinkeletal.(2006)thatasksparticipantstoratetheiragreementwithstatementslike,“Maintainingefficient,well­coordinatedinteractionwithmypartnerrequiresalotofenergy”and,“Interactionswithmypartnergenerallygosmoothly.”

Finally,wewere interested in the roleofperceivedpartner instrumentality,or the perception that one’s partner is helpful or useful for one’s goal progress(Fitzsimons&Shah,2008,2009).Wemeasured instrumentalitywitha13­itemscaleaskingparticipantstoratetheiragreementwithstatementslike,“Ingeneral,Ifindmypartnertobeveryhelpfulwithmygoalpursuits”and,“Ingeneral,mypartnerisarealsourceofstrengthformeinpursuingmygoals.”

Replicatingthepatternestablishedinthelongitudinalstudy,thenewmeasureofperceivedgoalsimilaritywasnegativelyrelatedtoconflictratings,arelationthatheldwhenwecontrolledforrelationshipsatisfactionandcommitment.

Next,welookedattheroleofinstrumentality.Wefoundthatperceptionsofpartnerinstrumentalitydidpredictconflict,evencontrollingforsatisfaction,suchthatindividualswhosawtheirpartnersasmorehelpfulfortheirpersonalgoalpur­suitsreportedfewerincidencesofconflict.Thedirectionalityofthiseffectremainsunclear:itisconceivableeitherthatpartnerswhohavelowerratesofconflictmayseetheirpartnersasmoreinstrumentalfortheirgoalsorthatindividualsfightlessoftenwithinstrumentalpartners.Instrumentalitywasalsorelatedtogoalsimilar­ity,aswepredicted, suchthat similarpartnerswere seenasmore instrumentalforgoalprogress.Whenbothinstrumentalityandsimilaritywereenteredintoaregressionpredictingconflict,however,wefoundnoevidenceofamediatingrolefor instrumentality. Instead,bothvariables significantlypredictedconflict,evenwhencontrollingforrelationshipsatisfaction.Thus,itseemsthatinstrumentality,thoughrelatedtobothgoalsimilarityandtoconflict,doesnotaccountforthelinkbetweenthesetwovariables.

Page 209: the Psychology of Social Conflict and Aggression Sydney Symposium in Social Psychology

gráinne m. fitzSimonS and Joanna e. anderSon192

However,thisstudyalsofoundsignificant(albeitpartial)mediationofthelinkbetweengoalsimilarityandconflictbythehigh­maintenanceinteractionmeasure.Thispatternsupportsthepossibilitythatpartnerswhopursuedissimilarpersonalgoalsmayfindtheireverydayinteractionsdraininganddifficult,whichmaycauseconflicttoarise.Becausethereversemediationalpathwayisalsosignificant(i.e.,similaritynegativelypredictsdepletion,withconflictasasignificantpartialmedia­tor),itmayalsobethecasethatpartnerswhopursuedissimilarpersonalgoalsmayhavemorefrequentconflicts,whichmayleadthemtofindtheireverydayinterac­tionsmoredraininganddifficult.

doeS oBJective goal Similarity affect conflict?

Thus,thefirsttwostudiesfoundevidencethatpartnerswhobelievetheysharesimilarpersonalgoalsreportlessconflict.Ofcourse,giventhecorrelationalnatureofthesestudies,itwouldbeprematuretodrawconclusionsaboutthedirectional­ityoftherelationship.Indeed,itseemsquiteplausiblethatparticipantswhoareengagedinfrequentconflictwiththeirpartnersmayinferfromthatconflictthattheymusthavedifferentorincompatiblegoalsthantheirpartners.Althoughthenexttwostudieswedescribearealsocorrelational(thelaststudywedescribeisexperimental),webelieveweminimizetheplausibilityofthisalternativeaccountbymeasuringactualgoalsimilarityratherthanperceivedgoalsimilarity.Thatis,welookatbothtargetandpartnerratingsoftheirowngoalsandusecompari­sonsofthosegoalsaspredictorsoftargetratingsofconflict.Becauseparticipantsarenotprivytotheresponsesoftheirpartnerstothesegoalquestionnaires,thereversecausaldirection—thatconflictleadstogoaldissimilarity—isunlikely.Itremainspossiblethatfrequentconflictcouldcausepartnerstobegintopursuedifferent personal goals, but we believe that such an explanation is lessparsi­monious than our suggestion that pursuing different personal goals generatesfrequentconflict.

DalCin,Anderson,Holmes,andYoung(2010)collectedself­reportdatafrombothpartnersofdatingcouplesattheUniversityofWaterloo.Participantsratedthe importanceofa seriesofgoals,which rangedfromspecificandconcrete togeneralandabstractandwerediverse incontent, includingpersonalgoals (aca­demicachievement,finances,health,leisure)andrelationalgoals(communication,sex).Goalsimilaritywasmeasuredbycomputinganaverageoftheabsolutedif­ferencebetweenpartnergoalimportanceratingsandselfgoalimportanceratings.ParticipantsalsoratedthedegreeofconflictintheirrelationshipsusingitemstakenfromBraikerandKelley(1979).Thescalemeasuredparticipants’perceptionsofthefrequencyandseriousnessofconflictsintheirrelationshipaswellasthefre­quencyandintensityofnegativeaffect(anger,frustration)ineverydayinteractions.Itemsincluded,“MypartnerandIfrequentlyarguewitheachother”and,“WhenmypartnerandIargue, theargumentsorproblemswehavearequiteserious.”Finally,participantsratedtheirrelationshipsatisfactionusingfourstraightforwarditems(e.g.,“Iamextremelyhappywithmycurrentromanticrelationship”).

Page 210: the Psychology of Social Conflict and Aggression Sydney Symposium in Social Psychology

interdependent goalS and relationShip conflict 193

Aspredicted,resultsshowedthatcouplesthatreportedsimilargoalsreportedless conflict in the relationship and more relationship satisfaction. Because wemeasuredbothpartners’goals,insteadofonepartner’sperceptions,wecanruleouttheroleofperceptualillusionsandbiasesinproducingthelinkbetweengoalsimilarityandconflict.

iS there anything uniQue aBout goalS? goal Similarity verSuS other typeS of compatiBility

Wenext returned toexamineanother component from the larger longitudinalstudyofdatingandmarriedcouplesdescribedearlier,whichincludedmeasuresofpersonalityandattitudesandallowedustocomparetheeffectsofgoalsimilar­itywithotherkindsofsimilarity.Twoweeksaftercompletingtheonlinepremea­sure,couplesattendeda laboratorysession inwhichthey individuallyreportedon the frequency of disagreements in their relationships and provided ratingsoftheircommitmenttoandidentificationwithacademicachievementgoalsandhealth and fitness goals, goals that undergraduate students commonly pursue.Thegoalmeasurewasanaverageoffouritemsassessingthecommitmenttoandimportanceofthegoal.Forexample,participantsindicatedtheiragreementwithstatementslike,“Thisgoalisveryimportanttome.”Toconstructameasureofgoalsimilarity,wecalculatedanabsolutedifferencescoretorepresentthemag­nitudeofthedifferencebetweenbothpartners’ratingsoftheircommitmenttoacademic achievement and to fitness and health goals and averaged those twomeasures.Participantsalsoprovidedmeasuresofotherkindsofpersonalvari­ables,suchasinpersonality,religiousbeliefs,andattitudes.Itisverylikelythatpartnerswithdifferenttypesofpersonalitieswouldhavegreaterconflict(imagineanextravertandanintrovertmakingsocialplans)andthatpartnerswithdiffer­entsocialattitudeswouldalsohavemoredisagreements(imagineasocialistandalibertariandiscussingcurrenteventsoverdinner).Indeed,asbrieflydiscussedintheintroduction,thereisevidencefortheeffectsofsimilarityinpersonalityandattitudesonrelationshipsatisfaction(Gaunt,2006).Includingthesemeasuresin our analyses allowed us to determine whether similarities in personal goalpursuitshaveanyremainingpredictivepowerwhenaccountingfortheseotherimportantfactors.

Overall, our results supported the importance of goal similarity in conflict.Aspredicted,andreplicatingtheeffectfromthelastdescribedstudy,usingthisnew(moreobjective)measureofgoalsimilaritydidnotchangetheresults.Whenpartners reported similar levels of commitment to and identification with theirimportantpersonalgoals,theyalsoreportedlowerincidenceofconflictwiththeirpartner.This relationheldwhenwecontrolled for relationship satisfactionand,mostimportantly,whenwecontrolledfortheabsolutelevelsofgoalcommitmentofbothpartners.Because theeffectholdsevenwhencontrolling for individualreportsofgoalcommitment,wecanbemoreconfidentthatitisindeedthedis­crepancy between the partners’ goals, rather than something about one of thepartners’goalpursuitsitself,thatpredictsconflict(seeKenny&Acitelli,1994).

Page 211: the Psychology of Social Conflict and Aggression Sydney Symposium in Social Psychology

gráinne m. fitzSimonS and Joanna e. anderSon194

Next,wewereinterestedinexaminingwhetherthereisauniqueroleforgoalsimilarityor compatibility inparticularorwhetherwhatwe are capturingwithourgoalsimilaritymeasuresisanothertypeofsimilarity,suchassimilarattitudes,values,orpersonalitytypes.Toexaminethisidea,weanalyzedthelinkbetweenourdifferencescoremeasureofgoalsimilarityandconflictwhilecontrollingforanumberofotherpotentiallyusefulpredictors.Goalsimilarityturnedouttobeastrongandrobustpredictorofconflict,evenwhencomparedwithconsequentialvariablessuchassimilarityinpoliticalandsocialattitudes,religiousbeliefs,values,andBigFivepersonality traits.That is, itcontinuedtobeasignificantnegativepredictorofconflict.

hoW doeS manipulating goal incompatiBility affect conflict?

Inthefinalstudythatwedescribe,weaimedtofindexperimentalevidencetosupporttheideathatdissimilarpersonalgoalscouldaffectinterpersonalconflicts.Weactivatedthementalrepresentationofahealthandfitnessgoalandexamineditsinfluenceonparticipants’responsestohypotheticalrelationshipscenariosinwhichtherewaspotentialforconflict.Toactivatethegoal,wereliedonadvancesin the understanding of nonconscious goal pursuit (Chapter 6 in this volume),using a subtle priming technique. We were interested in whether participantswouldindicatethattheywouldbemoreorlessaccommodatingandcooperativeinthesehypotheticalscenariosdependingon(1)whethertheywereprimedwiththehealthandfitnessgoaland(2)whethertheyhadindicated(earlierintheses­sion)thattheyandtheirpartnerweresimilarordissimilarinthisgoaldomain.Wepredictedthatparticipantswhoreporteddissimilaritieswiththeirpartnerintheimportanceofhealthandfitnessgoalswouldrespondtotheprimedgoalbybeinglesscooperativeandaccommodatinginthehypotheticalscenarios.

Participants were adultU.S. women whowere currently in a romantic rela­tionship.Theybeganbyansweringquestionsabouttheirpersonalgoalpursuits.Onesuchquestionaskedparticipantstoindicatewhethertheyandtheirpartnervaluedseveralgoalsequally,includingcareergoals,familygoals,andhealthandfitnessgoals.Thisitemread,“Whenitcomestohealthandfitnessgoals…,”andparticipantscouldchooseoneofthreeitems:“MypartnercaresmuchlessthanIdo”;“MypartnerandIcareequally”;and“MypartnercaresmuchmorethanIdo.”Approximately40%ofparticipantsindicatedthattheycaredmoreaboutthegoalthantheirpartner;another40%indicatedtheyandtheirpartnervaluedthegoalequally;andthefinal20%indicatedthattheirpartnercaredmoreaboutthegoal.Wecombinedparticipantswhosaidtheirpartnercaredeithermoreorlessaboutthegoalintoonegroupwetermthe“dissimilar”group.Participantswhoreportedequalgoalimportanceweretermedthe“similar”group.(Resultsforthetwodif­ferent“dissimilar”groupsdidnotdiffer.)

Participantsalsocompletedashortscrambledsentencetask(Srull&Wyer,1979),inwhichtheywererandomlyassignedtobeexposedtowordsrelatedtoeitherhealthandfitnessgoal­relevantwords(e.g.,fit, healthy)orcontrolwords

Page 212: the Psychology of Social Conflict and Aggression Sydney Symposium in Social Psychology

interdependent goalS and relationShip conflict 195

matchedforvalence(e.g.,book, artistic)embeddedinlargerstringsofneutralwords.Theyalsocompletedascaledesignedtoassesstheirdesiretobecoop­erativeandaccommodatingtotheirpartners’preferences.Theitemsweremod­eledafterpriorresearchonaccommodativebehaviorsinrelationships(Arriaga&Rusbult,1998;Rusbult,Verette,Whitney,Slovik,&Lipkus,1991)butwereshortenedtomeettherequirementsforonlinedatacollection.Participantsreada scenario thatpresented themwithadecision toeithercooperatewith theirpartners’interestsortorefusetodoso(i.e.,tobeother­versusself­interested)andthenchoseoneoffourresponseoptionsthatvariedintheextenttowhichtheywerepositiveandconstructive.Forexample,onescenarioread,“Imaginethatyourpartnerinsistedoninvitingafriendovertodinner,eventhoughyoudon’tlikethisfriend.Howlikelywouldyoubetoengageineachofthefollow­ingactions?”Participantschosefromthefollowingoptions:“Iwouldrefuse”;“Iwouldprotestbutgivein”;“Iwouldgrudginglyagree”;and“Iwouldcheerfullyagree.”Theotherscenariosaskedaboutparticipants’responseswhentheirpart­nersrentedamovietheydidn’twanttosee,forgottorunanimportanterrand,and made a big mess at home. The scenarios were designed to be mundaneexamplesofdecisionsmembersofcouplesmakeeachdayabouthowtorespondwhentheirinterestsdonotcoincidewiththeirpartners’interests.

Aspredicted,wefoundthatforparticipantswhoreportedthattheyandtheypartnerhelddissimilarvaluestowardhealthandfitnessgoals,thegoalprimeledtolessaccommodatingresponsestothehypotheticalscenarios.Forparticipantswhoreportedsimilarlevelsofcaringabouthealthandfitnessgoals,thegoalprimedidnotaffecttheirresponses.Statedanotherway,withinthecontrolcondition,therewasnoeffectofsimilarityonaccommodating responses;within thehealthgoalprimecondition,therewasaneffectofsimilarity,suchthatparticipantswhoper­ceivedgoalsimilaritywiththeirpartnersreportedmoreaccommodatingresponsesthandidpartnerswhoperceivedgoaldissimilarity.

Thisstudyprovidesexperimentalsupportfortheroleofpersonalgoalsimilar­ity ineverydayrelationship interactionsandsuggestsonepossibleexplanationofthelinkbetweensimilarityandconflictinthefirstfourstudies.Whenparticipantswereremindedofapersonalgoal—inthiscase,healthandfitness—theywerelessaccommodatingtothepreferencesofdissimilar(vs.similar)partners,indecisionscompletelyunrelatedtothehealthandfitnessdomain.Theywerelesslikelytoagreetodecisionsthatwereintheirpartners’butnotintheirowninterests,andtheyweremorelikelytosaytheywouldfeelangryandgetinafightwiththeirpartnerswhenthosepartnersengagedinmundanenegativeacts.Bydemonstratingthattheeffectofsimilargoalsisstrongerwhenthepersonalgoalitselfisactivated,and,relyingonpriorreportsofthegoalsimilarity,thisstudyprovidesevidenceforthecausalroleofpersonalgoalsintheseeffects—whenthegoalwasnotcurrentlyactive,participantswereequallyaccommodatingtopartnerswhosharedanddidn’tsharethegoal.

future directionS and concluSionSThus, the current research provides support for the interdependence theorynotionthatdissimilargoalpursuitscanpredictdailyconflictwithininterpersonal

Page 213: the Psychology of Social Conflict and Aggression Sydney Symposium in Social Psychology

gráinne m. fitzSimonS and Joanna e. anderSon196

relationships.Fivestudiesusingvariedmeasuresandmethodologiesdemonstratedthatwhenpartnersdonotvaluepersonalgoalstothesamedegreetheytendtoreport more occurrences of conflict within the relationship and more negativeresponsestopotentialconflictsanddisagreements.Althoughthesefindingsdem­onstratethatgoalsimilarity,apreviouslyneglectedtopicwithinresearchonrela­tionshipconflicts,isrelatedtotheoccurrenceofconflict,theyleavemanyquestionsunanswered.Most importantly, thefindingspresentedhereoffer intriguingevi­denceofsomeofthepotentialmechanismsunderlyingthislink,likethedepletingnatureofinteractingwithsomeonewhopursuesdifferentgoals,butitisclearthattheremaybemanyotherprocessesatwork.Futureresearchthatexperimentallymanipulatessomeofthepositedmechanismswouldbeparticularlyvaluable.

Oneinterestingextensionofthecurrentfindingswouldbetoexaminehowgoalsimilarity and conflict relate across thedurationof close relationships. Becausepersonal goals are likely to change over the lifespan of an average relationship,itmaybeparticularlyenlighteningtoexaminethelinksbetweengoalsandrela­tionshipsasgoalschange(Fitzsimons&Fishbach,2010).Forexample,itislikelythatsomecoupleswhooncesharedsimilargoalswillencounterconflictsthathavearisenduetochangesinoneorbothpartners’goalsacrosstime.AsHolmesandMurray(1996)note,“Thecompatibilityofimportantgoalsisbestthoughtofasamovingwindowratherthanafixedqualityofarelationship.”Asanotherexample,couples that start out their relationship with dissimilar goals may well grow topossess more similar goals over time, either because both partners’ goals growtogetherorbecauseonepartneradoptstheother’sgoals.

Anotherimportantdirectionforfutureresearchistoexaminetheroleofdis­similarrelationshipgoals,likegoalsforincreasedintimacyversusindependence,and dissimilar joint goals, like goals to buy a bigger house versus save money.Becauseoutcomesarelikelytobeevenmoreinterdependentforsuchgoalsandbecause relationship­level factors have been shown to have an importanteffectonconflictbehaviors(Simpson,Rholes,&Phillips,1996)theeffectsofdissimilarrelationshipgoalsmaybebothpervasiveandpowerful.

Finally,althoughwepresentedthehypothesisthatsimilarpersonalgoalswillreduceconflict,itisalsoquiteconceivablethatsharedgoalscouldpromotecon­flictundersomecircumstances.Iftwopartnerssharethesamegoal,thereisanincreasedopportunity for social comparisonandcompetition. Imaginethat twopartnersbothvolunteerforthesamecharityandthatbothhavegoalstobecomeleaderswithinthecharity.Help,understanding,andsupportmayincrease,butsotoomightcompetition,resentment,andstrife.Onemoderatingvariablethatwoulddeterminethepresenceofthesenegativeoutcomesforsimilarityinpersonalgoalswouldbethelinkbetweenthepartners’personalgoals—whethersuccessbyonepartnerwouldimplyfailurebyanother.Suchalinkcouldbetheresultofobjec­tivecharacteristicsofthesituation(e.g.,therecanbeonlyonecharitypresident)orsubjectivecharacteristicsbroughtbythepartners(e.g.,competitiveorinsecurepartnersmaybelikeliertofeelnegativelyabouttheirpartner’ssuccesses).Futureresearchexaminingthesepotentialmoderatorswouldbeparticularlyvaluable.

Theseareonlyafewofthemanyavenuesavailableforfutureresearchinthisarea.Theeffectsofpersonalgoalpursuitsoninterpersonalrelationships,and,in

Page 214: the Psychology of Social Conflict and Aggression Sydney Symposium in Social Psychology

interdependent goalS and relationShip conflict 197

turn,theeffectsofrelationshipsonpersonalgoalpursuits,areonlybeginningtobeunderstood(Finkel&Fitzsimons,forthcoming;Fitzsimons&Finkel,inpress;seealsoChapters2and6inthisvolume).However,thecurrentresearchpresentsanimportantstepforwardinourresearchonthisinterplayofself­regulatoryandrela­tionalprocesses.Inallofourpriorwork(e.g.,Fitzsimons&Bargh,2003;Fitzsimons&Shah,2008),wehavefocusedononlyonepartner’sgoalpursuitsandfeelingsabouttherelationshipwhileacknowledgingthatsuchanapproachisseriouslylim­ited,asrealinteractionsinvolvetwoindividuals’goalpursuits(seeRusbult,Finkel,&Kumashiro,2009).Inthecurrentresearch,wetryforthefirsttimeinourlabtoexaminethedyadiclinksbetweenbothpartners’goals,afirststeptowardbuildingarichunderstandingofhowtheseeffectsplayoutinrealrelationships.

referenceS

Arriaga,X.B.,&Rusbult,C.E.(1998).Standinginmypartner’sshoes:Partnerperspective­takingandreactionstoaccommodativedilemmas.Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 24,927–948.

Baumeister, R. F. (1998). The self. In D. T. Gilbert, S. T. Fiske, & G. Lindzey (Eds.),Handbook of social psychology (4thed.,pp.680–740).NewYork:McGraw­Hill.

Berscheid,E.(1983).Emotion.InH.H.Kelley,E.Berscheid,A.Christensen,J.H.Harvey,T.L.Huston,G.Levinger,etal.(Eds.), Close relationships(pp.110–168).NewYork:Freeman.

Berscheid,E.(1991).Theemotion­in­relationshipsmodel:Reflectionsandupdate.InW.Kessen,A.Ortony,&F.I.M.Craik(Eds.),Memories, thoughts, and emotions: Essays in honor of George Mandler(pp.323–335).Hillsdale,NJ:Erlbaum.

Berscheid,E.,&Ammazzalorso,H. (2001).Emotionalexperience incloserelationships.InG.Fletcher&M.Clark(Eds.),Blackwell handbook of social psychology(Vol.2:InterpersonalProcesses,pp.308–330).Oxford,UnitedKingdom:Blackwell.

Berscheid, E., & Walster, E. (1978). Issues in studying close relationships:Conceptualizingandmeasuringcloseness.InC.Hendrick(Ed.),Close relation-ships: Review of personality and social psychology (Vol.10,pp.63–91).NewburyPark,CA:Sage.

Bohns,V.K.,Lucas,G.M.,Molden,D.C.,Finkel,E.J.,Coolsen,M.K.,Kumashiro,M.,etal.(2010).When opposites fit: Increased relationship well-being from partner comple-mentarity in regulatory focus.Unpublishedmanuscript,UniversityofToronto.

Braiker,H.B.,&Kelley,H.H.(1979).Conflictinthedevelopmentofcloserelationships.InR.L.Burgess&T.L.Huston(Eds.),Social exchange in developing relationships (pp.135–168).NewYork:AcademicPress.

Brunstein,J.C.,Dangelmayer,G.,&Schultheiss,O.C.(1996).Personalgoalsandsocialsupportincloserelationships:Effectsonrelationshipmoodandmaritalsatisfaction.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,71,1006–1019.

Buunk,B.,&Bosman,J.(1986).Attitudesimilarityandattractioninmaritalrelationships.Journal of Social Psychology, 126,133–134.

Byrne,D.(1961).Interpersonalattractionandattitudesimilarity.Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 62,713–715.

Byrne,D.(1971).The attraction paradigm.NewYork:AcademicPress.Byrne,D.,Clore,G.L.,&Worchel,P. (1966).Theeffectofeconomicsimilarity­dissim­

ilarity on interpersonal attraction. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 4,220–224.

Page 215: the Psychology of Social Conflict and Aggression Sydney Symposium in Social Psychology

gráinne m. fitzSimonS and Joanna e. anderSon198

DalCin,S.,Anderson,J.E.,Holmes,J.G.,&Young,S.B.(2010).Accordance in personal goals and relationship outcomes.Unpublisheddata,UniversityofWaterloo.

Dalton,A.,Chartrand,T.L.,&Finkel,E.(2010).Thedepletedchameleon:Self­regulatoryconsequencesofsocialasynchrony.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,98,605–617.

Esterberg, K., Moen, P., & Dempster­McCain, D. (1994). Transition to divorce: A life­courseapproachtowomen’smaritaldurationanddissolution.Sociological Quarterly, 35,289–307.

Fehr, B., & Harasymchuk, C. (2005). The experience of emotion in close relationships:Towardanintegrationoftheemotion­in­relationshipsandinterpersonalscriptmod­els. Personal Relationships, 12,181–196.

Finkel,E.J.,Campbell,W.K.,Brunell,A.B.,Dalton,A.N.,Scarbeck,S.J.,&Chartrand,T. L. (2006). High­maintenance interaction: Inefficient social coordination impairsself­regulation.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,91,456–475.

Finkel,E.J.,&Fitzsimons,G.M.(forthcoming).Effectsofself­regulationoninterpersonalrelationships.InK.D.Vohs&R.F.Baumeister(Eds.),Handbook of self-regulation(2nded.).

Fishbach,A.,Friedman,R.S.,&Kruglanski,A.W.(2003).Leadingusnotintotemptation:Momentaryallurementselicitoverridinggoalactivation.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,84,296–309.

Fitzsimons,G.M.,&Bargh,J.A.(2003).Thinkingofyou:Nonconsciouspursuitofinter­personalgoalsassociatedwithrelationshippartners.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 84, 148–163.

Fitzsimons,G.M.,&Finkel,E.J.(forthcoming).Effectsofinterpersonalrelationshipsonself­regulation.InK.D.Vohs&R.F.Baumeister(Eds.),Handbook of self-regulation,2ndedition.

Fitzsimons,G.M.,&Finkel,E.J. (inpress).Interpersonal influencesonself­regulation.Current Directions in Psychological Science.

Fitzsimons,G.M.,&Fishbach,A.(2010).Shiftingcloseness:Interpersonaleffectsofper­sonalgoalprogress.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 98,535–549.

Fitzsimons,G.M.,&Shah,J.Y.(2008).Howgoalinstrumentalityshapesrelationshipevalu­ations.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,95, 319–337.

Fitzsimons,G.M.,&Shah,J.Y.(2009).Confusingoneinstrumentalotherforanother:Goaleffectsonsocialcategorization.Psychological Science,20,1468–1472.

Gaunt,R.(2006).Couplesimilarityandmaritalsatisfaction:Aresimilarspouseshappier?Journal of Personality, 74,1401–1420.

Griffin,D.,Murray,S.,&Gonzalez,R.(1999).Differencescorecorrelationsinrelationshipresearch:Aconceptualprimer.Personal Relationships,6,505–518.

Holmes,J.G.,&Murray,S.L.(1996).Conflictincloserelationships.InE.T.Higgins&A.W.Kruglanski(Eds.),Social psychology: Handbook of basic principles (pp.622–654).NewYork:Guilford.

Karney,B.R.,&Bradbury,T.N.(1995).Thelongitudinalcourseofmaritalqualityandsta­bility:Areviewoftheory,method,andresearch.Psychological Bulletin, 118,3–34.

Kelley, H. H. (1979). Personal relationships: Their structure and processes. Hillsdale,NJ:Erlbaum.

Kelley,H.H.,&Thibaut,J.(1978).Interpersonal relations: A theory of interdependence.NewYork:Wiley.

Kenny,D.A.(1994).Interpersonal perception: A social relations analysis.NewYork:Guilford.Kenny,D.A.,&Acitelli,L.K.(1994).Measuringsimilarityincouples.Journal of Family

Psychology,8,417–431.Luo,S.,&Klohnen,E.C.(2005).Assortativematingandmaritalqualityinnewlyweds:Acou­

ple­centeredapproach.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,88,304–326.

Page 216: the Psychology of Social Conflict and Aggression Sydney Symposium in Social Psychology

interdependent goalS and relationShip conflict 199

Messick,D.M.,&Brewer,M.B.(1983).Solvingsocialdilemmas.InL.Wheeler&P.R.Shaver (Eds.), Review of Personality and Social Psychology (Vol. 4, pp. 11–44).BeverlyHills,CA:Sage.

Meyer,J.P.,&Pepper,S.(1977).Needcompatibilityandmaritaladjustmentinyoungmar­riedcouples.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,35,331–342.

Montoya,R.M.,Horton,R.,&Kirchner,J.(2008).Isactualsimilaritynecessaryforattrac­tion?Ameta­analysisofactualandperceivedsimilarity.Journal of Social and Personal Relationships,25,889–922.

Morry,M.M.(2005).Relationshipsatisfactionasapredictorofsimilarityratings:Atestoftheattraction–similarityhypothesis.Journal of Social and Personal Relationships, 22, 561–584.

Morry, M. M. (2007). The attraction–similarity hypothesis among cross­sex friends:Relationshipsatisfaction,perceivedsimilarities,andself­servingperceptions.Journal of Social and Personal Relationships, 24, 117–138.

Reis, H. T., Clark, M. S., & Holmes, J. G. (2004). Perceived partner responsiveness asanorganizingconstructinthestudyofintimacyandcloseness.InD.Mashek&A.Aron(Eds.),The handbook of closeness and intimacy (pp.201–225).Mahwah,NJ:LawrenceErlbaumAssociates.

Rosenbaum,M.E.(1986).Therepulsionhypothesis:Onthenondevelopmentofrelation­ships.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 51, 1156–1166.

Rusbult,C.E.,Finkel,E. J.,&Kumashiro,M. (2009).TheMichelangeloPhenomenon.Current Directions in Psychological Science,18,305–309.

Rusbult,C.E.,Martz,J.M.,&Agnew,C.R.(1998).Theinvestmentmodelscale:Measuringcommitment level, satisfaction level, quality of alternatives, and investment size.Personal Relationships, 5,357–391.

Rusbult,C.E.,Verette,J.,Whitney,G.A.,Slovik,L.F.,&Lipkus,I.(1991).Accommodationprocessesincloserelationships:Theoryandpreliminaryempiricalevidence.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 60,53–78.

Simpson,J.A.,Rholes,W.S.,&Phillips,D.(1996).Conflictincloserelationships:Anattach­mentperspective.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,71,899–914.

Srull, T. K., & Wyer, R. S. (1979). The role of category accessibility in the interpreta­tionof informationaboutpersons:Somedeterminantsandimplications.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,37,1660–1672.

Surra,C.A.,&Longstreth,M.(1990).Similarityofoutcomes,interdependence,andconflictindatingrelationships.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,59,501–516.

Wiltermuth,S.S.,&Heath,C.(2009).Synchronyandcooperation.Psychological Science,20,1–5.

Page 217: the Psychology of Social Conflict and Aggression Sydney Symposium in Social Psychology
Page 218: the Psychology of Social Conflict and Aggression Sydney Symposium in Social Psychology

201

13SilentRage

When Being Ostracized Leads to Aggression

LISAZADROUniversity of Sydney

Paincanbealleviatedbymorphinebutthepainofsocialostracismcannotbetakenaway.

DerekJarman(1994,p.113)

introductionOverthepastdecade,therehasbeenconsiderabledebateastowhetherbeingostracized(beingexcludedandignored;Williams,2007)leadstargetstobehaveineitheraprosocialorantisocialmannertowardothers.Whereassomeresearch­ershavefoundthatbeingexplicitlyrejectedleadstargetstobehaveinanaggres­sivemannertowardeitherthesourceofostracismorinnocentbystanders(e.g.,Twenge, Baumeister, DeWall, Ciarocco, & Bartels, 2007), others have foundevidence suggesting that being excluded prompts targets to respond in waysthatincreasetheiropportunitiesforreinclusion(e.g.,conforming;seeWilliams,Cheung,&Choi,2000). Unravelingthesecontradictoryfindingshasbeenthefocusofrecentresearch.

Thischapterexamines thepossiblepsychological (i.e.,primaryneed threat),contextual(i.e.,relationshiptypeandstatus),andemotional(i.e.,negativeaffectandfeelingsofanger)factorsthatmaymotivatetargetstoenactpunitiveandvengefulbehaviorsasaconsequenceofbeingexcludedandignored.Factorsthatpotentiallymoderatetheconsequencesofexclusionarediscussedin termsofwhether they

Page 219: the Psychology of Social Conflict and Aggression Sydney Symposium in Social Psychology

liSa zadro202

ameliorateorexacerbateaggressivereactions.Finally,thechapterintroducesnewexperimental researchusinganovelostracismparadigm, “O­Cam,”a simulatedWebconferencethatspecificallyinvestigatestheformsofvengeancethattargetsofostracismarewillingtoimposeonsources.

the functionS of oStraciSmOstracismisoftendescribedasoneofthemostinnocuousformsofinterpersonalconflictandapreferablealternativetoverbalorphysicalabuse(Williams,2007;seealsoChapters4and6inthisvolume).Thevirtuesofsilencearepreachedinproverbs,informingusthat“silenceisgolden”andthat“ifyouhavenothingnicetosay,saynothingatall.”Theveryactofostracizinganotherindividual(i.e.,thetargetofostracism)simplyinvolvesnotspeakingtothemoracknowledgingtheirpresence—therearenoraisedvoicesorphysicalblows.Infact,ostracismcanbeusedinthepresenceofotherswithonlookersbeingnonethewiser.The“benign”appearanceofthistacticiswhymanyinstitutionsuseformsofostracismaspunish­mentinpreferencetoothermethodsofinterpersonalconflict.Forinstance,schoolstypicallyadvocateusingtime­out(i.e.,physicallyremovingthestudentfromtheirpeers)todisciplinestudentsasopposedtocorporalpunishment.Similarly,solitaryconfinement—aformofphysicalostracism—isusedasameansofpunishingpris­onersforinfractionscommittedbehindbars.

Ifweexaminethewaysthatostracismisusedacrossdifferentagegroups,cul­tures,andspecies,itisapparentthatallformsofostracismhavetwobroadgoals.Thefirstgoalistoremoveundesirablemembersfromthegroup,particularlythosewhomayharmor jeopardizethesafetyandwell­beingoftherestofthegroup.TheverytermostracismcomesfromtheancientAthenian(488–487B.C.)practiceof exiling citizenswhosedictatorial ambitionsposeda threat to thedemocraticnatureofthestate(Zippelius,1986).Inmoderntimes,westillremovemembersofsocietywhoharmothersthroughimprisonment,whichisaninstitutionalizedformofexile.Abeneficialconsequenceofremovingundesirablesisthattheremainingmembersofthegroupoftenbecomemorecohesive—theyfunctionasastrongerunitthatbenefitsthegroupasawhole(Gruter&Masters,1986;Williams,2001).Thus,removingundesirablemembersensuresthatthegroup(andindeeditsval­ues)remainssafeandintact.

Thesecondpotentialgoalofostracismiscorrective,seekingtoensurethatthetargetofostracismchangestheirthoughts,feelings,orbehaviorstobecomealignedwiththoseoftherestofthegroup.Inthisregard,ostracismisusedasaformofdis­ciplineorpunishment;itgivesrecalcitrantmembersaglimpseofwhatitwouldbelikewithoutthesupportofthesourcesandhencewhatisinstoreforthemiftheydonotcomplywiththegroup.Therearemanyexamplesofostracismbeingusedasmeansofdiscipliningwaywardgroupmembers.Forinstance,withinromantic(orindeedothercloseinterpersonal)relationships,individualsmayuse“thesilenttreatment”ontheirpartnertopunishthemforactualorperceivedwrongdoing,therebydiscouragingthepartnerfrombehavinginthesamewayinfuture.

However, regardless of the reason, ostracism can have devastating conse­quencesforthetarget.Foranimals,beingostracizedbythegroup—andthusbeing

Page 220: the Psychology of Social Conflict and Aggression Sydney Symposium in Social Psychology

Silent rage 203

removedfromtheprotectionofothermembers—willoftenleadtostarvationanddeath (Goodall, 1986). Forhumans, the results ofbeing ostracized by a source(individualorgroup)maynothavesuchgraveconsequencesonsurvivalbutmaystillhaveotherdevastatingeffects.Forinstance,thereisconsiderableevidencetosuggestthatalossofsocialsupportcanhaveanadverseimpactonhealthandwell­beingcomparabletodamaginghealthfactorssuchasobesity,smoking,andhighblood pressure (Kiecolt­Glaser, Cacioppo, Malarkey, & Glaser, 1992). A lack ofsocialsupportcanalsodelayrecoveryfromillnessesandsurgeryandevenhindercompliancewithprescribedmedicalregimens(seeCobb,1976).

Given the negative ramifications of losing group membership or social sup­port—particularly if thesourceofostracismisapartneror lovedone—it isnotsurprisingthattargetstendtorespondbychangingtheirbehaviortoregainfavorwiththesources.Inaseriesofinterviewswithtargetsandsourcesoflong­termostracism,Zadro(2004,2009)foundthattargetsoftenbehavedinaprosocialandconciliatory fashion toward the sources of ostracism. Prosocial and conciliatoryresponsesaredesignedtorectifyorrelievetheostracismsituation.Suchresponsesincludeforgivenessseeking(i.e.,apologizingtothesourceforanyactionthatmayhavewarrantedtheostracism;foradiscussiononwhysuchtacticsmayhavenega­tiveeffectsontargets,seeChapter14inthisvolume),discussion(i.e.,tryingtoelicitaresponsefromsourcesbyspeakingtotheminanonconfrontationalmanner),andingratiation(i.e.,attemptstoelicitaconversationthroughflattery,panderingtothesource’sneedsorwants,orpurchasingitemssuchasflowersorgifts).Bycarryingoutprosocialandconciliatorystrategies,thetargetaimstorepairtherelationshipwiththesourcesandtherebytoputanendtoanyemotionalorphysiologicalpaintheymayhaveexperiencedduringtheostracismepisode.

Yet, despite the potentially adverse consequences to the target’s health andwell­beingthatmayresultfrombeingostracized,Zadro(2004,2009)foundthatseveraltargetsoflong­termostracismrespondedinanantisocialandreactionarymannertobeingexcludedandignored.Specifically,thesetargetsreportedactinginavengefulmannertowardthesourcesofostracismandevenrecountedinstanceswheretheyhadrespondedinanantisocialmannertowardinnocentbystanders.

Thus,thereisanecdotalevidencethatostracismcanleadtoprosocialoranti­socialresponsesindifferentsituations.Yetwhydoesostracismleadtosuchdiversebehavioralresponses?Whydosometargetstrytoappeasethesourcewhereasoth­ersretaliateandevenattackinnocentothers?Toanswerthesequestions,itisfirstnecessarytoexaminethepotentialpsychologicalandemotionalresponsestoostra­cism;afterall,itistheseresponsesthatprovideaninvaluablepieceofthepuzzlewhentryingtodeterminewhethertargetswillrespondinapro­orantisocialman­nerwhentheyarerejectedandignored(seealsoChapter3inthisvolume).

pSychological effectS of Being oStracized: the effectS of oStraciSm on primary needS

Williams’s(1997,2001)modelofostracismpredictsthatostracism(comparedwithotherformsofinterpersonalconflict)hasthepotentialtothreatenfourfundamental

Page 221: the Psychology of Social Conflict and Aggression Sydney Symposium in Social Psychology

liSa zadro204

human needs: belonging, control, self­esteem, and meaningful existence—anassertionthathasbeensupportedbyoveradecadeofempiricalinvestigation(seeWilliams,2007forreview).Targetsaremotivated(behaviorally,emotionally,andcognitively)torefortifytheselostorthreatenedneeds.However,iftheostracismepisode is prolonged or if the target is repeatedlyexcluded and ignored by thesources,thethreatenedneedsbecomeinternalizedleadingtodetrimentalpsycho­logicalandhealth­relatedconsequences(seeZadro,2004).

Thedesiretoregaintheprimaryneedsthreatenedduringostracismplaysanimportantmotivatingforceindeterminingwhethertargetsrespondinaprosocial/conciliatoryor an antisocial/reactionarymanner. Regaininga sense ofbelong­ingness,forinstance,wouldfirstentailtryingtoregainone’smembershipinthegroup(or,ifthetargetisbeingostracizedbyapartnerorlovedone,toregainone’splaceinthepartnership).Tobestpursuethisgoal,targetsmayfirsttrytobehaveinanaffiliativemannertowardsources(i.e.,byenactingtacticssuchasingratia­tion,discussion,andforgivenessseeking).Iftheseattemptstorepairtherelation­shipareunsuccessful,thetargetcouldalsotrytoaffiliatewithnewindividualsorgroupstoregainasenseofbelongingness.Incontrast,attemptstoregaintheremainingneeds(i.e.,control,self­esteem,andmeaningfulexistence)couldeasilylendthemselvestomoreantisocialandreactionaryactions.Forinstance,targetsmaylashoutverballyorphysicallytoregaincontroloverothersortheirenviron­ment;targetswhoseself­esteemhasbeenadverselyaffectedbybeingignoredmaytry tobolster theirown feelingsof self­worthbydenigratingothers; if a targetfeels invisible,as isoftenthecasewhenone’s senseofmeaningfulexistence isthreatened,actinginanaggressivefashion—pickingafight,yellingan insult—willensurethatthepersonwill receiveattention,eventhoughthisattentionisessentiallynegative.

Severalfactorsmaydeterminewhethertargetsregaintheirneedsusinganti­social methods. Although all four primary needs are threatened, there may beindividual differences in the way that targets prioritize the reestablishment oftheseneeds.Forinstance,pictureanostracismsituationwherebyatargetisbeingexcludedandignoredattheirworkplacebyafellowemployee.Althoughtheostra­cismepisodemaythreatenallfourofthetarget’sprimaryneeds,thetarget’sdesiretoregaincontrolofthesituation—andhenceregainsocialstandingamongothersin the office—may actually be a greater priority to the target than regaining asenseofbelonging(i.e.,byrepairingtherelationshipwiththesource),particularlyiftherelationshipwiththesourceisfairlysuperficialandthetargethasastrongsupportnetworkoutsideoftheoffice.Thisdesiretoregaincontrolmaypossiblylenditselftobehavingantisociallytowardthesource.

Theprioritizationofneedsmayalsobeaproductofpreexisting traitdiffer­encesamongtargets.Forinstance,thosewithpreexistinglowlevelsofoneormoreoftheprimaryneeds(e.g.,lowself­esteem)maytryhardertoregainthatneedonceithasbeenfurtherthreatenedbyostracism.Otherindividualdifferences,suchasattachmentstyle,mayalsocausesometargetstoprioritizetheregainingofcer­tainneedsafterbeingostracized;thismaybeparticularlythecasewithanxiouslyattachedindividualswhohaveaparticularfearofrejection(seeChapter2inthisvolume).

Page 222: the Psychology of Social Conflict and Aggression Sydney Symposium in Social Psychology

Silent rage 205

Gendermayalsoplayaroleintheextenttowhethertargetsregaintheirneedsin an antisocial fashion. For instance, Williams and Sommer (1997) found thatostracizedfemalesworkedcomparativelyharderonacollectivetaskpostostracismratherthanacoactivetaskwhereasostracizedmalestendedtosociallyloafmoreduring the collective task thanduring the coactive task.Williams andSommerconcludedthatbeingostracizedmotivatedtargetstotryandregaintheirthreat­enedneeds;however,ostracizedfemalesattemptedtoregainasenseofbelongingwhereasmalesinsteadgaveprioritytoregainingself­esteemorpossiblyasenseofcontrolovertheirenvironment.Indoingso,malesrespondedinalessthanproso­cialfashiontowardthegroup(i.e.,socialloafing).

Giventhegenderdifferences,andpossiblyalsotraitdifferences,thatinfluencetheways inwhichtargetsprioritizeregainingtheirthreatenedprimaryneeds, itisnotsurprisingthattherearearangeofpossibleanti­andprosocialreactionstoostracism.

the role of emotionS in determining proSocial or antiSocial reSponSeS to oStraciSm

JeanJacquesRousseau(1712–1778)wasoncequotedassaying,“Absolutesilenceleadstosadness.Itistheimageofdeath.”Thissentimentisechoedbytargetsoflong­termostracismwhoreportpowerfulemotionalresponsestobeingostracized(Zadro,2004,2009).Inadditiontofeelingsadnesswhilebeingsociallyexcluded,targetsoflong­termostracismreportedfeelingarangeofnegativeemotionsinclud­ing despair, loneliness, horror, anguish, helplessness, pain, shame, and anxiety.Almostalltargetsreportedfeelingangryafterostracism.Forinstance,onetargetwhowasrepeatedlyignoredbyherdaughteralmostresortedtoviolenceduringafamilyholidayduringwhichthedaughterrefusedtospeakorparticipate inanyactivities.Shestated:

I’mnotviolent.Well,Iavoidviolence liketheplague.Igrabbedherbythewaist,andIthought,“GeewhatamIdoing?”Iwasactuallygoingtothrowheracrosstherestaurant—Iwasthatangry.That’showIwasthewholetime—Iwasangrythewholetime.

The anecdotal findings suggesting a link between ostracism and negativeaffecthavereceivedsomeempiricalsupport,includingevidencethatsocialexclu­sionleadstofeelingsofsadnessandhurtfeelings(e.g.,Buckley,Winkel,&Leary,2004; Chow, Tiedens, & Govan, 2008; however, for an exception, see Twenge,Cantanese,&Baumeister,2002).

Thereisgrowingevidencethatnegativeaffectivestatesoftentriggeradaptivecognitive,motivational,andbehavioralreactions(seeChapter8 inthisvolume).Yethowdoesnegativeaffectfuelantisocialbehaviorspostostracism?AccordingtoBerkowitz(1990),negativeaffecthasaprimaryroleintheactivationofthoughtsandmemoriesassociatedwithangeraswell as “rudimentary” feelingsofanger.Indeed, targets in experimental studies of ostracism often reported feeling sig­nificantlyangrierthanthosewhowereincludedintheostracismparadigm(e.g.,

Page 223: the Psychology of Social Conflict and Aggression Sydney Symposium in Social Psychology

liSa zadro206

Chowetal.,2008;Zadro,Williams,&Richardson,2004).Recently, researchershavebeguntoexaminethelinkbetweenangerandaggressionpostostracism.Forinstance,Chowetal. foundthatparticipantswhoreportedfeelingangrierafterbeingostracizedduringanInternetball­tossinggame(Cyberball;seeWilliamsetal.,2000)weremorelikelytobehaveinanantisocialmannertowardthesourcesofostracismthanthosewhoreportedfeelingsad.Thusitseemsthatangermaypro­moteantisocialreactionswhereassadnessseemstoproducemorevigilant,atten­tive,andadaptivestylesofresponding.

Giventhepsychologicalandemotionaltraumathattargetssufferwhilebeingostracized,itisnotsurprisingthattheymayexpresstheirfrustration,anger,andpainbybehavinginanantisocialfashion.Thequestionthenbecomes—whomdotargetschoosetobethefocusoftheirantisocialactions?

“all are puniShed”?: Who iS the focuS of poStoStraciSm aggreSSion?

Whentargetsrespondtoostracisminanantisocialandreactionaryfashion,theycanpotentiallydirecttheiractionstowardthreeparties:(1)thesourceofostracism(i.e.,revengeorretaliation);(2)innocentbystanders(i.e.,physicalorverbalaggres­sion);or(3)themselves(i.e.,self­harmorself­defeatingbehaviors).

Aggressing Against Sources of Ostracism

Antisocial andreactionaryacts that targetscommit against sourcesofostracismcouldbe construedas actsof revengeor retaliation forbeingostracized.Thereareseveralreasonstargetsmaybemotivatedtoseekoutvengeanceagainstthosewhohaveostracizedthem.Walster,Walster,andBerscheid(1978)statethatactsof revengewithin interpersonalrelationshipsmaybemotivatedby thedesire torestoreequityintherelationship.Inanostracismsituation,thetargetisatthecom­pletemercyofthesource;thesourcechooseswhentoostracizethetargetandiforwhenheorshewillstoptheepisode.Bychoosingtoostracizethetarget,thesourcehas achieved complete power over the target and the relationship. Acts of ven­geancemaybeoneoftheonlywaysthetargetcouldtopplethesourcefromhisorherpositionofpower,therebyredressingtheequityimbalanceintherelationship.

Revenge may also assist targets to regain lost or threatened primary needs.Specifically,revengemayallowtargetstoregaintheirsenseofcontrolbythrow­ingofftheirroleof“passivevictim”(seeFrijda,2007)andinstrumentallyengag­ing in behaviors that increase control over the situation and over other people.Retaliatoryactionmayalsogivethetargetsomelevelofcontroloverthefutureoftheirrelationshipwiththesources,inthesensethatactinginaretaliatoryfashionmayhelptodiscouragesourcesfromostracizingthetargetinsubsequentconflictsituations(seePinker,1997).Vengefulactsaredesignedtoattracttheattentionofthesource,whichinturnwillmakethetargetfeellessinvisibleandhencehelprestoreasenseofmeaningfulexistence(seeYoshimura,2007).Revengeresearch­ershavealsofoundthatvengeanceisoftenmotivatedbyadesiretoregainself­

Page 224: the Psychology of Social Conflict and Aggression Sydney Symposium in Social Psychology

Silent rage 207

worth, whichwouldbe attractive to targets ofostracismwhose self­esteem hasbeen thwartedbybeing socially excluded (e.g.,McCullough,Bellah,Kilpatrick,&Johnson,2001).However,whilerevengemayallowthetargettoregainasenseof control, self­esteem, and meaningful existence, depending on the nature ofthevengefulact,itmayfurthererodethetarget’srelationshipwiththesource—therebyfurtherthwartingthetarget’ssenseofbelonging—andmayevenleadtofurtherretaliationonbehalfofthesource.

Interviews with targets of long­term ostracism revealed that targets oftenengagedinantisocialorvengefulacts,usuallyasameansofelicitingaresponsefrom the source or as a way of venting their frustrations (Zadro, 2004, 2009).Althoughseveraltargetsreportedusingverbalabuseonsources,actsofphysicalabusewere(thankfully)rare;however,targetsmayhavebeenreluctanttoadmitbeingviolentevenifithadoccurred.Targetswhodidadmittousingphysicalabuseresortedtothistacticafterothertactics(suchasforgivenessseekingordiscussion)hadfailed.

The interviewsprovideda few,mildexamplesofphysicalaggression towardsources,but themediaoftenhighlightsepisodesofostracism thathave serious,andeven lethal,consequences.Schoolyard shootings, suchas thatconductedatColumbineHighSchool,areoftenretaliatorystrikesbystudentswhohavebeenwidelyrejectedbytheirpeers.InacasestudyofschoolshootingsintheUnitedStatesbetween1995and2001,Leary,Kowalski,Smith,andPhillips(2003)foundthat individualswhohad rejected the shooterwere typically targetedandwereoftenamongthevictims.

Althoughfewtargetsoflong­termostracismadmittedtorespondinginaphysi­callyaggressivemanner,itbecameapparentthattargetsrarelyjustsatbackandallowedthemselves toberepeatedlyrejectedand ignoredwithoutretaliating insomefashion.Sometargetsactivelyspokeabout“gettingrevenge”forbeingsub­jectedtolengthyepisodesofsilence,particularlywhentheyperceivedtheepisodestobeunwarranted.Forsome,gettingrevengeamountedtosimpleactsofreputa­tion defamation (i.e., they spoke badly about the source to others). In contrast,othertargetsdetailedelaborateandpotentiallyharmful,revengescenarios.Zadro(2009)conductedafocusgroupwithfourgenerationsofSicilianwomentodiscusstheirexperiencesastargetsofrelationalostracism(i.e.,ostracismbytheirrelation­shippartner).TherewerecleardifferencesbetweenthewaysyoungerandolderSicilianwomencopedwiththesilenttreatment.YoungerSicilianwomentendedtoquicklycurtailanyostracismattemptsonthepartoftheirspouse,usuallythroughstronglanguageandanexplicitthreattoleavetherelationshipifthesilenttreat­mentcontinued.However,fortworeasons,thesetacticswerenotavailabletomostoftheolderwomen.First,theywereinpatriarchalrelationshipswheretheirhus­bands held all power; in many cases, these women experienced physical abusefromtheirspouseandhencedidnotwanttoactinawaythatwouldincitefurtherabuse.Second,leavingtherelationshipwasnotanoption;therewerestrongsocialsanctions against divorce, and the “shame” of such an act would then lead thewomentobeadditionallyostracizedbytheirfriendsandfamily.

Instead, older Sicilian women pursued active and very creative campaignsofrevengethatoperatedunderthesources’radar(whichcouldbeconstruedas

Page 225: the Psychology of Social Conflict and Aggression Sydney Symposium in Social Psychology

liSa zadro208

campaignsofpassiveaggression;seeChapter4inthisvolume).Inmanyinstances,these campaigns continued for years—well after the ostracism episode. Forinstance, one target—an Italian woman in her late 50s—said that her husbandhadrejectedhersexually20yearsearlierandhadinstigatedabriefaffairwithawoman inhisworkplace.Inreturn, the targetpunishedherhusbandbyaddingmoreandmorebuttertohismealsinthehopesofslowlyelevatinghischolesteroltopainful(andpossiblylethal?)levels.Anotherwomanreportedmovingherhus­band’spossessionsaroundthehome—keys,watch,tools—tothepointwherehestartedtowonderabouthissanity.OlderSicilianwomenalsospokeofacting“lesspositively”(ratherthannegatively)asameansofactingoutagainstostracism.Suchactsincludedgivingtheirhusbandasmallerpieceofdessertthanthosereceivedbytherestofthefamilyratherthansimplydeprivinghimofdessertentirelyorfailingtoinvitehisfamilyaroundtovisitasoftenasexpected—acardinalsininanItalianhouseholdwherefamilymeanseverything.Theseactswerenotconductedtogainthenoticeofthesource;rather,theywereperformedsothatthetargetwouldhavethe satisfactionof regaining their senseof control, andpossiblyeven their self­esteem,aftermultipleactsofongoingneglectandexclusion.

Forsometargets,“fightingfirewithfire”becomes thebest typeofdefense.Thesetargetschoosetosimplyostracizethesources:eithertogivethesourceatasteoftheemotionalpainthatthetargethasexperienced(“[thesilenttreatment]isnotsomethingthatIwouldusuallydo,butif[thesource]isgoingtoactlikethattowardyou,wellIcangiveasgoodasaIget”)ortosimplygetthesource’satten­tion.Othertargetsresortedtoretaliatoryostracismwhentheynolongercaredtopursuearelationshipwiththesource.

Therehasbeenconsiderableexperimentalresearchexaminingthewaysthattargetsperceivesourcesofostracism.Typically,sourcesofostracismwereviewedas less likeable (Pepitone & Wilpizeski, 1960) and generally rated unfavorably(Geller,Goodstein,Silver,&Sternberg,1974)andwere rarely rewardedby thetargetinsubsequenttasks(Gelleretal.).

Recentstudieshavefocusedonexaminingwhetherbeingostracizedleadstoantisocialresponsestowardostracizers.Forinstance,BourgeoisandLeary(2001)found that participants who were rejected tended to derogate their ostraciz­ers,which supports anecdotal evidenceof reputationdefamationpostostracism.AccordingtoBourgeoisandLeary,derogationofostracizerscanserveanadaptivefunctionbecauseitdiminishesthedesiretobeacceptedbythesourceandhencereducesthepotentialimpactofostracismonthetarget’spsychologicalwell­being.

Aggressing Against Bystanders and Observers

Targets of ostracism do not always take out their anger and frustration on thesourceofostracism.Instead,thereisanecdotalevidencesuggestingthattargetswilloftenlashoutatinnocentbystanders.Thismayseemcounterproductive;afterall,affiliatingwithanewpersonprovidestargetswiththeopportunitytoregainprimaryneeds,particularly theirsenseofbelonging.However,accordingtotheinterviewswithlong­termtargetsandsourcesofostracism(Zadro,2004),targetsoftendirecttheiriretowardnonsources.Behavinginanantisocialmannertoward

Page 226: the Psychology of Social Conflict and Aggression Sydney Symposium in Social Psychology

Silent rage 209

othersratherthanthesourcemayoccurforavarietyofreasons.Forinstance,iftargetsdonotwanttofurtherjeopardizetheirrelationshipwiththesource,thentheymaytakeouttheirfrustrationonothers.Similarly,targetsmayabuseinnocentthirdpartiesifthereisapowerdisparitybetweenthetargetandthesourceandthe cost of direct retaliation is far too high. For instance, one long­term targetreportedthathisemployerhadignoredhimforseveralweeks,refusingtospeaktohimdirectlyandexcludinghimfrommemosandinterofficeemails.Althoughthetargetwasangryandfrustratedbyhisemployer’sbehavior,hecouldnotactonhisfeelingsbecausehewasterrifiedthathewouldlosehisjob.Instead,hisfamilyborethebruntofhisanger;hewasverballyabusiveandshort­temperedtohiswifeandchildren(Zadro,2009).

Behavinginanantisocialandreactionarymannertowardthirdpartiesmayallowatargetnotonlytoventnegativeemotionssuchasangerbutalsotoregainthwarted primary needs. For instance, targets may regain a sense of controloverothersandtheirenvironmentby takingout theirangeron thosearoundthem. They may also increase their feelings of self­worth by focusing on theshortcomingsofothers.Aswellasbeingcathartic,suchbehaviortowardoth­erswillalsogetthemattention,whichwillhelpthemtoregaintheirsenseofmeaningfulexistence.

Despitethepossibilityofregainingprimaryneeds,thereareevidentlydrawbackstolashingoutagainstathirdparty.Ifwereturntotheexampleofthebusinessmanwhotookouthisfrustrationandhelplessnessoverbeingostracizedonhiswifeandfamily,it isclearthatthetargetisjeopardizinghisrelationshipwithhiswifeandchildrenbymistreatingthem.Heisalsofurtherweakeninghissenseofbelongingandhissocialsupportnetworkingeneralbyerodinghistiestohislovedones.

Several experimental studies have examined the ways that targets respondtostrangerspostostracism.Forthemostpart,thesestudies indicatethattargetsofostracismtendtoexhibitprosocialorcooperativebehaviorstowardneutralornovelindividuals(e.g.,conformingtoincorrectgroupjudgmentstobetterfitwiththegroup;Williams&Sommer,1997;Williamsetal.,2000),suggestingthattheyarebehavinginamannerthatwillpromoteinclusionandsubsequentsocialcon­nection (see Maner, DeWall, Baumeister, & Schaller, 2006). However, in thesestudies,targetswerenotgiventheopportunitytoactinanaggressivefashion.

Muchrecentostracismresearchexamineswhethertargetswillactinananti­social and reactionary manner toward innocent bystanders (e.g., Twenge et al.,2001;Warburton,Williams,&Cairns,2006.Typically,thesestudieshaverevealedthat the link between ostracism and aggression is not clear­cut. For instance,Warburtonetal.(2006)foundthattargetsofostracismonlyaggressedtowardaninnocentthirdpartywhenthetargets’senseofcontrolhadbeenfurtherthwartedinaprevioustask.Targetswhosesenseofcontrolhadbeenrestoredwerenotmorelikelytoaggress.Thesefindingssuggestthatostracismpersemaynotbesufficienttocausetargetstoaggress,particularlyagainstsomeonewhoisnotthesourceofostracism.Instead,atriggerisneeded—inthisinstance,afurtherlossofcontrol—toelicitanantisocialandreactionaryresponse.

In other studies assessing antisocial behavior postostracism, the “innocentbystander”isnotallthatinnocent—thatis,thebystanderoftenprovokesthetarget

Page 227: the Psychology of Social Conflict and Aggression Sydney Symposium in Social Psychology

liSa zadro210

insomewaythatinturnelicitsanantisocialandreactionaryresponse.Inaseriesofstudies,Twenge,Baumeister,Tice,andStucke(2001)examinedwhethersocialexclusionleadstoformsofaggressivebehavior.Theyfoundthatparticipantswhohadbeenrejectedactedaggressivelytowardanotherparticipantwhohadinsultedorprovokedthem.Inonlyonestudydidtheresearchersfindthatthetargetsalsoacted aggressively toward an innocent bystander. Yet, tellingly, targets of socialexclusionwerenotmoreaggressivetoabystanderwhopraisedthem.

Whenexaminingreal­lifeinstancesofviolenceprecipitatedbyrejection,bothbystanders and sources may be subjected to antisocial acts. For instance, thejournalentriesoftheColumbineHighSchoolmassacregunmenEricHarrisandDylanKleboldrevealedthatbeingcontinuallyostracizedandrejectedbyfellowclassmateswasakeycausalfactor intheirdecisiontoopenfireonstudentsandstaff.Althoughsourceswouldhavebeen involved in theostracism,someof thepeopleshotwereprobablyinnocentofanywrongdoing(seeLearyetal.,2003).

Recentstudieshaveaimedtocomparetheresponseoftargetstowardbothsourcesandinnocentbystanders.Forinstance,inastudybyZadroetal.(2010),participantswereostracized,included,orostracizedthenreincludedduringagameofCyberball.TheythenparticipatedinaResourceDilemmatask(seeHardy&vanVugt,2006;seealsoChapter15inthisvolume),wherebyparticipantswereaskedtoindicatehowmuchofa100­centresourcetheywouldallocatetotheotherplayersandtothem­selves.Thistaskallowedparticipantstobehaveingratiatingly,cooperatively,oranti­sociallytowardeithertheindividualstheyhadjustplayedCyberball withortwonewplayers.WhencomparedwithincludedparticipantsintheCyberballtask,ostracizedparticipantsbehavedmoreantisociallywhenplayingwiththesourcesofostracism(i.e.,theyallocatedalmosttwo­thirdsoftheresourcetothemselves,leavinglittleintheresourcetosplitamongthesources)butinaningratiatingmannerwhenplay­ingwithnewplayers(i.e.,theytooklessthanathirdoftheresourceforthemselves,leavingalotmoreoftheresourcetosplitamongthenewplayers).Neitherofthesebehaviorswasobservedinreincludedtargets,astheirthwartedneedsweresome­whatrefortifiedwhensourcesreacceptedthemintothegame.

Aggression Against the Target: Self-Harm Postostracism

Theliteratureoftenfailstoacknowledgethattargetsofostracismcanturntheirnegativefeelings,theirfrustrations,andtheirthwartedneedsinward,internaliz­ingthecauseofostracismandeffectivelypunishingthemselvesfortheostracismincident.Thisreactioniscommonamongtargetswhohaveexperiencedprolongedperiods of ostracism or who have been repeatedly ignored by multiple sources(Zadro, 2004, 2009). These threatened needs often manifest in self­destructivethoughts(“Ioftenthinktomyself,‘Whenisthisgoingtoend?’”;“I’vethoughtofsuicide”).Unfortunately,manytargetsoftenactontheseself­destructivethoughts,leadingtoahostofself­harmbehaviorsincludingpromiscuity,alcoholismanddrugaddiction,self­mutilation,andevensuicideattempts(Zadro,2004).

The negative, self­defeating behaviors demonstrated by long­term targets ofostracismarealsoevident—inlesserform—inlaboratorymanipulationsofostra­cismandrejection.Forinstance,inaseriesofstudies,Twengeetal.(2002)found

Page 228: the Psychology of Social Conflict and Aggression Sydney Symposium in Social Psychology

Silent rage 211

thatparticipantswhoweretoldthattheywouldhaveafuturedevoidofsocialbondsweremorelikelytoengage invariousformsofself­defeatingbehavior includingincreasedrisktaking(e.g.,bettingonalongshotratherthanasaferoption)andengaging in fewerhealth­enhancingbehaviors (e.g., choosing toeatacandybarratherthanamueslibar).Thus,thethreatofsocialexclusionledtothepursuitofactivities that have pleasurable short­term effects but, ultimately, aversive long­termconsequences.

the moderatorS of oStraciSm: factorS that may precipitate or curtail the

deSire to act aggreSSivelyThere is considerable evidence suggesting that being ostracized has an adverseeffectonprimaryneedsandaffect.AccordingtoBerkowitz(1990),oncenegativeaffecthasactivatedthecognitiveconstructsassociatedwithangerandaggression,individualsbegin to thinkaboutpossible“attributions,appraisals, andschematicconceptionsthatcanthenintensify,suppress,enrich,ordifferentiatetheinitialreac­tion”(p.494).Thissecondphase—wherebytargetsgothroughthe“suppression”or“enrichment”of thenegativeoraggressiveaffect thathasarisenfromtheostra­cismincident—isessentialforunderstandingwhysometargetschoosetoactinanantisocialandreactionarymannerpostostracism;specifically,itisatthisstagethatvariousmoderatingfactorscomeintoplay,whethersingularlyorincombination.

Numerous factors may, either singularly or in combination, moderate theeffectsofostracism.First,specificaspectsoftheostracismepisodemaydeterminewhetheratargetrespondsinanantisocialandreactionaryfashion.Forinstance,thephysicallocationoftheepisodemayfacilitateorinhibitantisocialresponses.Unlikephysicalorverbalabuse,ostracismcanbeusedbysourcesinpublic,oftenwithoutobserversevennoticingthatitisoccurring.Ifanostracismepisodeoccursinpublic,however,itwouldbedifficultforatargettorespondinanantisocialfash­ion(particularlyinaphysicallyaggressivefashion)withoutattractingtheattentionof onlookers—unless the target is so desperate for attention that even negativeattentionisconsideredpreferabletobeingignored.

Second,theidentityofthesourcemayalsobeanimportantmoderatingvariable.Manytargetsstatedthatitwas“easier,”andfarlessaversive,tobeignoredbyastrangerthana lovedone (Zadro,2004).Whenostracizedbya lovedone, targets typicallyexpressedadesiretopreservetheirrelationshipwiththesource.Hence,theytypicallytriedtoactinanaffiliativemannertoensurethattheepisodeendedasquicklyaspos­sibleandtherelationshipremainedintact.Targetsweretypicallylessmindfuloftheirrelationshipwithanostracizingstrangeroracquaintance;thus,ifgivenasufficienttrigger,targetsmaybemorelikelytoactinanantisocialfashionwhenostracizedbystrangersratherthanbylovedones.Laboratorystudieshavefoundthattheidentityofthesourcetypicallydoesnotmoderatetheimmediateconsequencesofostracism;thatis,beingignoredbyacomputerversusahuman(Zadro,Williams,&Richardson,2004)orbyadespisedout­group(i.e.,theKKK;Gonsalkorale&Williams,2007)doesnotmoderatethedeleteriouseffectsofostracismonthefourprimaryneeds.

Page 229: the Psychology of Social Conflict and Aggression Sydney Symposium in Social Psychology

liSa zadro212

Thereis,however,someevidencethattheidentityofthesourcemaymoderateaffectiveresponses.Forinstance,Zadroetal.(2004)foundthattargetswhowereostracizedduringCyberballbyacomputer­generatedplayerreportedfeelingsig­nificantlyangrierthanthosewhohadbeenostracizedbyahumanplayer.Iftargetsfeelangrierwhenostracizedbyspecificsources,thenitispossiblethatthisangermaymanifestinantisocialreactions,particularlyiftherearefurtherenvironmen­taltriggers.

Third,thenumberofsourcesmayalsobeamoderatingfactor.Whenostra­cizedbyasinglesource,targetshavetheopportunitytoregainthreatenedneedsbyaffiliatingwithothersintheirsocialsupportnetwork.Whenostracizedbymul­tiplesources,orsources indifferentenvironments(e.g., if targetsaregiventhesilenttreatmentbytheirpartnerathomeaswellasbytheircolleaguesatwork),theremaybefeweropportunitiestoregainthwartedneedsand,hence,agreaterriskthatlostneedswillbeinternalized.Long­termtargetswhowereostracizedbymultipletargetsweremorelikelytomakeinternalattributionsfortheostra­cismepisodes(“It’sallmyfault!”;Zadro,2004).Thispromptedsometargetstorespondinanantisocialanddestructivefashion;thatis,theyattemptedtorestorelost social bonds by aligning themselveswith unsavory or unscrupulous others(e.g.,joininggangs).Insomeinstances,targetsofmultiplesourcesfeelsufficientlyangryanddisenfranchisedtoretaliateaggressivelyagainsttheirostracizers(e.g.,inthecaseofthevariousU.S.schoolyardshootingswhereshootersfeltrejectedandexcludedbytheirpeers;seeLearyetal.,2003).Targetsmayalsoturntheiraggressioninwardandcommitactsofself­harm—forinstance,engaginginpro­miscuousbehaviorasameansoffeelingwantedandlovedorindulginginalcoholandrecreationaldrugsasameansofescapingtheproblem.Thedevastationofbeingignoredbysomanyisclearlyevidentinthefollowinglettersentbyayoungwomaninher20swhowasignoredbyherschoolpeers(Zadro,2004):

Inhighschool,theotherstudentsthoughtmeweirdandneverspoketome.Itellyouinallhonestythatatonestagetheyrefusedtospeaktomefor153days,notonewordatall….Thatwasaverylowpointformeinmylifeandonthe153rdday,Iswallowed29Valiumpills.(p.61)

Fourth, the causal clarity of the ostracism episode may also fuel antisocialresponses.Forinstance,iftargetsknowwhytheyarebeingignored,theycanfocustheirattentiononpursuingtacticsthatrectifythesituationandthataremorelikelytoleadtoreacceptance(e.g.,ingratiationordiscussionthatfocusesonapologizing).However,iftargetsareunawareofwhytheyarebeingignored,thenthehelpless­nessandfrustrationfeltinsuchasituationmayfuelanaggressiveresponse,par­ticularlyifthereisafurthertriggertoelicitaggressivebehaviortowardthesourcesorinnocentbystanders(e.g.,theyexperienceafurtherlossofcontrol).

Finally,thelengthoftheostracismepisodemayalsocontributetoantisocialresponsespostostracism.Accordingtothesocialreconnectionhypothesis,exclu­sionmotivatestargetstoforgesocialbondswithothersonlytotheextentthattheycanrealisticallyprovidesocialreconnection(Maneretal.,2006).Attheonsetofostracism,targetstypicallyusestrategiesthattheybelievewillappeasethesource

Page 230: the Psychology of Social Conflict and Aggression Sydney Symposium in Social Psychology

Silent rage 213

(i.e.,discussionor ingratiation).Iftheseareunsuccessfulandtheostracismepi­sodestretchesindefinitely,targetsmayviewtheprospectoffutureinteractionasincreasinglyhopelessandmaythusactinavengefulfashioninadesperateattempttohavetheirexistenceacknowledged.

Just as ahostof situational factorsmaymoderate antisocial and reactionaryresponsestoostracism,individualdifferencesaresuretoplayaroleinpostostra­cismresponses.Althoughlittleresearchtodatehasspecificallyexaminedtheroleof individualdifferences inmoderatingantisocial responses toostracism(foranexception,seeBuckleyetal.,2004),researchershavefoundthatindividualdiffer­encessuchassocialanxiety(e.g.,Oaten,Jones,Williams,&Zadro,2008;Zadro,Boland, & Richardson, 2006), and self­esteem (e.g., Nezlek, Kowalski, Leary,Blevins,&Holgate,1997),domoderatetargets’responsestobeingsociallyexcludedandrejected.Futureresearchneedstoexploretheextentandwaysindividualdif­ferences,coupledwithsituationalchanges,moderateresponsestoostracism.Byunderstanding the roleofmoderating factors (singularlyand incombination) indeterminingpostostracismaggression,wecanbegintodevelopstrategiesthatwillnotonlycurtailtheeffectsofostracismbutpotentiallywillveertargetsawayfromrespondingantisocially.

oStraciSm and revenge: a neW program of reSearch

Recently,wehavebegun to conduct a series of studies that examine the rangeof vengeful and retaliatorybehaviors that targets arewilling to conductagainstsourcesofostracism(seeGoodacre,2007;Goodacre&Zadro,2010).Unlikeprevi­ousstudies,wedonotfocussolelyonphysicalaggressionbutratherseeitasonlyone aspect of the possible arsenal of antisocial and retaliatory behaviors at thetarget’sdisposal.

Toassesstheeffectsofostracismonrevengeandretaliation,wecreatedanewparadigm—O­Cam—a simulated Webcam conference that takes place betweenthetargetandtwostudentsfromalocaluniversity.DuringthisWebconference,thetargetisinformedthateachstudentwillgiveabrief,prewrittenspeechaboutuni­versitylife.AlthoughtheparadigmhastheappearanceofarealWeb­basedinterac­tion,the“students”areactuallyactorswhoseactionshavebeenprerecorded.TwoO­Cam conditionsareprerecorded:onewherethestudentsappeartolistentothetargetasthetargetmakesaspeech(theinclusioncondition);andanotherwherethestudentsappeartolistentothetarget’sspeechfor30secondsandthenturntoeachotherandbeginhavingaconversation,completelyignoringthetarget(theostracismcondition).Ademonstrationoftheparadigmcanbeseenathttp://www.psych.usyd.edu.au/research/ostracism/(Username:guest;Password:Bach).Unlikeothersocialexclusionandostracismparadigms,O­Camallowsparticipantstobeostracizedinthephysicalpresenceofthesourcesofostracismyetrequiresnoconfederatestoparticipateduringthetask.Wehypothesizedthatbeingabletowatchthesourcesofostracismastheyinteracttogetherduringtheostracismepisode(all thewhileignoringthetarget)wouldelicitstrong,emotionalresponsesinthetarget.

Page 231: the Psychology of Social Conflict and Aggression Sydney Symposium in Social Psychology

liSa zadro214

Unlike previous studies, which typically use a single indicator of antisocialbehavior(e.g.,the“hotsauceallocation”measureofphysicalaggressionusedbyWarburtonetal., 2006), thecurrent studyexamined several aspectsof revengeandretaliation.TheconstructofrevengeandretaliationwasassessedusinganewmeasurebasedonresearchbyYoshimura(2007)onthedifferentcategoriesofven­geancebehaviors.Specifically,thequestionnaireexaminedparticipants’desiretoengageinfourcommonrevengeandretaliationtypebehaviors:(1)activedistanc­ing(i.e.,removingoneselffromthephysicalpresenceofthesources);(2)reputationdefamation(i.e.,attemptstoreducethetarget’spositivepublicimage);(3)physicalaggressiveness(i.e.,attemptstocausethetargetphysicaldiscomfort,emotionaldis­tressorpain);and(4)resourceremoval(i.e.,withholdingrewardsfromthesources;forpsychometricpropertiesofthequestionnaire,seeGoodacre,2007).

Overall, the findings suggest that ostracized participants endorsed acts ofactivedistancing,reputationdefamation,andresourceremovalsignificantlymorethantheirincludedcounterparts.Yettheydidnotwishtobehaveinamorephysi­callyaggressivemannertowardthosewhoexcludedthem.Thissupportspreviousresearch that has found that ostracism alone is not sufficient to elicit physicalaggression;rather,itrequiresafurthertrigger,suchasafurtherlossofcontrol,toelicitresponsesthatinduceadesiretocausebodilyharm(e.g.,Warburtonetal.,2006).

Overall,thesefindingsindicatedthatevenasinglebriefexposuretoostracism,instigatedbypreviouslyunknownpeersacrossanelectronicmedium, ispower­fulenough toelicit antisocialbehaviorwithoutprovocation. It shouldbenoted,however,thatalthoughostracizedtargetssoughttodistancethemselvesfromthesourcesoftheirrejectiontheyalsoexpressedaninterestinconnectingwithanewgroupofpeople,indicatingthatostracizedtargetsarerespondinginamorecom­plexfashionthanpreviouslyexpected,simultaneouslyactinginapro­andantiso­cialmannerasameansofattainingshort­termbenefits(i.e.,vengefulactsthatmaygivethemasenseoftemporarysatisfaction)andlong­termgains(i.e.,seekingoutnewaffiliativeties).

Our future studies will continue the search for triggers and moderators ofaggressionpostostracism.Moreover,wehopetorefineourbehavioralmeasures;oftentheaggressionmeasuresusedinsocialexclusionstudiescouldbeviewedasmeasures of condonedaggression—that is, participants are given permission toaggressbybeingtoldthattheycangiveasmuchhotsauceorasmanynoiseblasts(atwhatevervolume)thattheywish.Thetargetcanrationalizethatiftheexperi­menter is allowing them toperform theseactions thennorealharmcancometothetarget.Theaimistofindnewwaysofassessingantisocialandaggressiveresponsesthatarelesscontrivedandparallelreal­worldtoattainaricherunder­standingofthemotivationsbehindpostostracismaggression.

referenceS

Berkowitz,L.(1990).Ontheformationandregulationofangerandaggression:Acognitive–neoassociationisticanalysis.American Psychologist, 45,494–503.

Page 232: the Psychology of Social Conflict and Aggression Sydney Symposium in Social Psychology

Silent rage 215

Bourgeois, K. S., & Leary, M. R. (2001). Coping with rejection: Derogating those whochooseuslast.Motivation and Emotion, 25,101–111.

Buckley,K.E.,Winkel,R.E.,&LearyM.R.(2004).Reactionstoacceptanceandrejection:Effectsoflevelandsequenceofrelationalevaluation.Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 40,14–28.

Chow,R.M.,Tiedens,L.Z.,&Govan,C.(2008).Excludedemotions:Theroleofangerinantisocialresponsestoostracism.Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 44,896–903.

Cobb,S.(1976).Socialsupportasamoderatoroflifestress.Psychosomatic Medicine, 38,300–314.

Frijda,N.(2007).The laws of emotion.Cambridge,UK:CambridgeUniversityPress.Geller, D. M., Goodstein, L., Silver, M., & Sternberg, W. C. (1974). On being ignored:

The effects of the violation of implicit rules of social interaction. Sociometry, 37,541–556.

Goodacre,R.(2007).O-Cam: A new social ostracism paradigm.Unpublishedhonor’sthesis,UniversityofSydney.

Goodacre,R.,&Zadro,L.(2010).“O­Cam”:Anewparadigmforinvestigatingtheeffectsofostracism.Behavior Research Methods,42, 768–774.

Gonsalkorale,K.,&Williams,K.D.(2007).TheKKKwon’tletmeplay:Ostracismevenbyadespisedoutgrouphurts.European Journal of Social Psychology, 37,1176–1186.

Goodall,J.(1986).Socialrejection,exclusion,andshunningamongtheGombechimpan­zees.Ethology and Sociobiology, 7,227–236.

Gruter,M.,&Masters,R.D.(1986).Ostracismasasocialandbiologicalphenomenon:Anintroduction.Ethology and Sociobiology, 7,149–158.

Hardy,C.,&VanVugt,M. (2006).Giving forglory in socialdilemmas:Thecompetitivealtruismhypothesis.Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 32,1402–1413.

Jarman, D. (1994). At your own risk: A saint’s testament. Minneapolis: University ofMinnesotaPress.

Kiecolt­Glaser,J.K.,Cacioppo,J.T.,Malarkey,W.B.,&Glaser,R.(1992).Acutepsycho­logicalstressorsandshort­termimmunechanges:What,why,forwhom,andtowhatextent?Psychosomatic Medicine, 54,680–685.

Leary,M.R.,Kowalski,R.M.,Smith,L.,&Phillips,S.(2003).Teasing,rejection,andvio­lence:Casestudiesoftheschoolshootings.Aggressive Behavior, 29,202–214.

Maner,J.K.,DeWall,C.N.,Baumeister,R.F.,&Schaller,M.(2006).Doessocialexclusionmotivateinterpersonalreconnection?Resolvingthe“porcupineproblem.”Journal of Personality & Social Psychology, 92,42–55.

McCullough,M.E.,Bellah,C.G.,Kilpatrick,S.D.,&Johnson,J.L.(2001).Vengefulness:Relationshipswithforgiveness,rumination,well­being,andtheBigFive.Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 27,601–610.

Nezlek,J.B.,Kowalski,R.M.,Leary,M.R.,Blevins,T.,&Holgate,S.(1997).Personalitymoderatorsofreactionstointerpersonalrejection:Depressionandtraitself­esteem.Personality and SocialPsychology Bulletin, 23,1235–1244.

Oaten, M., Jones, A., Williams, K. D., & Zadro, L. (2008). The effects of ostracism onself­regulationinthesociallyanxious.Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 27,471–504.

Pepitone, A., & Wilpizeski, C. (1960). Some consequences of experimental rejection.Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 60,359–364.

Pinker,S.(1997).How the mind works. NewYork:W.W.Norton.Twenge,J.M.,Baumeister,R.F.,DeWall,C.N.,Ciarocco,N.J.,&Bartels,J.M.(2007).

Social exclusion decreases prosocial behavior. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 92,56–66.

Page 233: the Psychology of Social Conflict and Aggression Sydney Symposium in Social Psychology

liSa zadro216

Twenge,J.M.,Baumeister,R.F.,Tice,D.M.,&Stucke,T.S.(2001).Ifyoucan’tjointhembeatthem:Effectsofsocialexclusiononaggressivebehavior.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 81,1058–1069.

Twenge,J.M.,Catanese,K.R.,&Baumeister,R.F.(2002).Socialexclusioncausesself­defeatingbehavior.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 83,606–615.

Walster,E.,Walster,G.W.,&Berscheid,E.(1978).Equity: Theory and research.Boston:AllynandBacon.

Warburton,W.A.,Williams,K.D.,&Cairns,D.R.(2006).Whenostracismleadstoaggres­sion:Themoderatingeffectsofcontroldeprivation.Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 42,213–220.

Williams,K.D.(1997).Socialostracism.InR.M.Kowalski(Ed.),Aversive interpersonal behaviors(pp.133–170).NewYork:Plenum.

Williams,K.D.(2001).Ostracism:The power of silence.NewYork:GuilfordPress.Williams,K.D.(2007).Ostracism.Annual Review of Psychology, 58,425–452.Williams,K.D.,Cheung,K.T.,&Choi,W.(2000).Cyberostracism:Effectsofbeingignored

overtheInternet.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 79,748–762.Williams,K.D.,&Sommer,K.L.(1997).Socialostracismbycoworkers:Doesrejectionlead

toloafingorcompensation?Personality & Social Psychology Bulletin, 23,693–706.Yoshimura,S.(2007).Goalsandemotionaloutcomesofrevengeactivitiesininterpersonal

relationships.Journal of Social and Personal Relationships, 24,87–98.Zadro,L.(2004).Ostracism:Empirical studies inspired by real-world experiences of silence and

exclusion.Unpublisheddoctoraldissertation,UniversityofNewSouthWales,Sydney.Zadro, L. (2009). Further interviews with targets and sources of long-term ostracism.

Unpublishedmanuscript,UniversityofSydney.Zadro,L.,Boland,C.,& Richardson,R. (2006).How long does it last?The persistence

of the effects of ostracism in the socially anxious. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 42,692–697.

Zadro,L.,Dale,E.,VanVugt,M.,Griffiths,B.,Richardson,R.,Williams,K.D.,etal.(2010).To ingratiate or retaliate? An empirical investigation of the conditions that promote prosocial versus antisocial behaviours in ostracised targets.Unpublishedmanuscript,UniversityofSydney.

Zadro,L.,Williams,K. D.,&Richardson,R. (2004).How low canyou go?Ostracismbyacomputerissufficienttolowerself­reportedlevelsofbelonging,control,self­esteem,andmeaningfulexistence.Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 40,560–567.

Zippelius,R. (1986).Exclusionandshunningas legalandsocial sanctions.Ethology and Sociobiology, 7,159–166.

Page 234: the Psychology of Social Conflict and Aggression Sydney Symposium in Social Psychology

217

14TheDoormatEffect

On the Dangers of Resolving Conflict via Unilateral Forgiveness

LAURAB.LUCHIESNorthwestern University

ELIJ.FINKELNorthwestern University

G ivenenough time, close relationship partners arebound to experienceconflicts in which one person hurts, angers, or upsets the other. Howcan they resolve such conflicts? Scholars and clinicians have designed

and implemented several interventions to bolster victims’ forgiveness of inter­personal transgressions (e.g., Hebl & Enright, 1993; Rye & Pargament, 2002;Worthington,Kurusu,Collins,Berry,Ripley,&Baier,2000;forareview,seeWade&Worthington,2005).Theseinterventionssharetheassumptionthatbolsteringvictims’forgivenesswillbenefitthevictims.Inotherwords,forgivenessinterven­tionsassumethatvictimshavecontrolovertheirownoutcomes: if theyforgive,theywillexperiencebetteroutcomesthaniftheydonotforgive.

Pastresearchshowssomesupportforthisassumption.Forgivenesshasbeenlinkedtoimprovedpsychologicalhealth,physicalhealth,andrelationalwell­being.Forexample,thosewhoforgivetendtoexperiencepsychologicalhealthbenefitssuchasgreaterlifesatisfactionandfewerpsychologicaldistresssymptoms(Bono,McCullough,&Root,2008;Orcutt, 2006).Theyalso tend toexperiencephysi­calhealthbenefitssuchasbettercardiacfunctioningandlessphysiologicalstress(McCullough,Orsulak,Brandon,&Akers,2007;Waltman,Russell,Coyle,Enright,Holter,&Swoboda,2009).Finally,theytendtoexperiencerelationalbenefitssuchasgreater closeness andcommitment to theirperpetrators aswell as enhanced

Page 235: the Psychology of Social Conflict and Aggression Sydney Symposium in Social Psychology

laura B. luchieS and eli J. finkel218

conflict resolution, which predicts subsequent relationship quality (Hannon,Rusbult,Finkel,&Kumashiro,2010;Tsang,McCullough,&Fincham,2006).

However,mightperpetratorsalsohavecontrolovervictims’outcomes?Thatis,mightperpetrators’behavior,intandemwithvictims’behavior,affectthequalityofvictims’outcomesfollowingbetrayalssuchasostracism(seeChapters3and13inthisvolume),harmtowardalovedone(seeChapter10inthisvolume),ornastyfeed­back(seeChapter10inthisvolume)?McCullough(2008)recentlyarguedthatfor­givenessevolvedtohelppeoplepreservetheirvaluablerelationships.Wepositthat,whenforgivenesshelpsvictimspreservearelationshipthatislikelytobevaluabletotheminthefuture,itleadstopositiveoutcomesforthevictim,butwhenitpreservesarelationshipthatisunlikelytobevaluableitleadstonegativeoutcomes.

Whatdetermineswhetheracontinuedrelationshipbetweenthevictimandtheperpetratorislikelytobevaluable?Theperpetrator’sbehavior.Atadispositionallevel,perpetratorscanindicatethatacontinuedrelationshipislikelytobevaluablefortheirvictimsbybehavinginanagreeablemanner.Ataconflict­specificlevel,onewayperpetratorscanindicatethatacontinuedrelationshipislikelytobevalu­ablefortheirvictimsisby“makingupfor”theiroffenses.Indeed,pastresearchhasshownthatagreeablenesspredictsperpetrators’amend­makingbehavior:highlyagreeableindividualsactinaprosocial,constructivemannerduringinterpersonalconflicts(Jensen­Campbell&Graziano,2001)andaremorelikelythantheirlessagreeablecounterpartstoacceptresponsibilityandmakereparationaftercommit­tingabetrayal(Chiaramello,Sastre,&Mullet,2008).Accordingtothisanalysis,scholarsandpractitionerswhohave,explicitlyorimplicitly,suggestedthatforgive­ness is uniformly good for victims might have oversimplified the story becausevictimsdonothavecompletecontrolovertheirownoutcomes.Rather,theconse­quencesofvictims’forgivenesshingeontheirperpetrators’behavior.

interdependence theory: three typeS of control over outcomeS

InterdependenceTheory(Kelley&Thibaut,1978;Kelleyetal.,2003;Thibaut&Kelley,1959)providesaframeworkforunderstandingthecontroltwoindividualshaveovertheirownandeachothers’outcomes,andthisframeworkcanbeappliedto the control victims and perpetrators have over victims’ outcomes. Followingabetrayal,perpetratorsmayormaynotmakeamends andvictimsmayormaynotforgive.Victims’outcomesforeachcombinationoftheirownandtheirper­petrators’behaviorcanbeplotted ina2×2table,as illustratedinFigure 14.1.(Perpetrators’outcomescanbe includedin thetableaswell,althoughwefocusonlyonvictims’outcomesbecauseweseektoaddresstheextantliterature’sfocusonvictims’outcomes.)

In interdependenceterminology(Kelleyetal.,2003),actor control (formerlycalled“reflexivecontrol”)istheamountofcontrolonehasoverone’sownoutcomes.Theamountofactorcontrolvictimshaveovertheirownoutcomescanbederivedbycalculatingtheaveragedifferencebetweenthevictims’outcomesinthe“Forgive”columnandthevictims’outcomesinthe“DoNotForgive”column—thatis,((A+

Page 236: the Psychology of Social Conflict and Aggression Sydney Symposium in Social Psychology

the doormat effect 219

C)−(B+D))/2.Actorcontrolisanalogoustothemaineffectvictimforgivenesshas on victims’ outcomes. Partner control (formerly called “fate control”) is theamountofcontrolone’spartnerhasoverone’soutcomes.Theamountofpartnercontrolperpetratorshaveovervictims’outcomescanbederivedbycalculatingtheaveragedifferencebetweenthevictims’outcomesinthe“MakeAmends”rowandthevictims’outcomesinthe“DoNotMakeAmends”row—thatis,((A+B)−(C+D))/2.Partnercontrolisanalogoustothemaineffectperpetratoramendshasonvictims’outcomes.Joint control (formerlycalled“behaviorcontrol”)istheamountofcontrolone’sselfandone’spartnerjointlyhaveoverone’soutcomes.Theamountofjointcontrolvictimsandperpetratorshaveovervictims’outcomescanbederivedbycalculatingtheaveragedifferencebetweenthevictims’outcomesintheupper­leftandlower­rightcellsandthevictims’outcomesintheupper­rightandlower­leftcells—thatis,((A+D)−(B+C))/2.Jointcontrolisanalogoustotheinteractioneffectbetweenvictimforgivenessandperpetratoramendsonvictims’outcomes.Thisframeworkcanbeusedtodeterminetheamountofactor,partner,andjointcontrolvictimsandperpetratorshaveovervictims’outcomesandcantherebyshedlight on the potential dangers of unilateral forgiveness interventions, which fre­quentlyassumethatvictims’outcomesaredeterminedprimarilybyactorcontrol.

a revieW of recent evidence of Joint control over victimS’ poStconflict outcomeS

A series of four recent studies investigated the interactive effects of victims’and perpetrators’ behavior on victims’ outcomes (Luchies, Finkel, McNulty, &Kumashiro,2010);allfourstudiesexaminedtheseconflictdynamicsbetweenpart­nersinclose,attachment­bondedrelationships(seeChapters2,6,and12inthisvolume)ratherthaninnegotiationsbetweennoncloseinteractants(seeChapters5and7inthisvolume).Wereviewthisprogramofresearch,whichincludestwo

Actor Control = ((A + C) – (B + D)) / 2Partner Control = ((A + B) – (C + D)) / 2Joint Control = ((A + D) – (B + C)) / 2

Forgive

MakeAmends

Do NotMake

Amends

Do Not ForgiveVictim

Perpetrator

A B

C D

figure 14.1 Howtocalculateactorcontrol,partnercontrol,andjointcontrolovervic­tims’postconflictoutcomes.

Page 237: the Psychology of Social Conflict and Aggression Sydney Symposium in Social Psychology

laura B. luchieS and eli J. finkel220

longitudinal studies (the first and fourth studies) and two experimental studies(thesecondandthirdstudies)thatexaminetheeffectsofvictimforgivenessandperpetratoramendsonvictims’postconflictself­respectandself­conceptclarity.Asexplainedalready,forgivenessinterventionsassumethatvictims’outcomesareprimarilysubjecttoactorcontrol.Incontrast,weexpectthatvictimsandperpe­tratorssharejointcontrolovervictims’outcomes.Thatis,wehypothesizethattheeffect of forgiving on one’s self­respect and self­concept clarity dependson theperpetrator’s behavior:when theperpetratorhasmadeamends,weexpect thatforgivenesswillbolsterone’s self­respect and self­concept clarity.Butwhen theperpetratorhasnotmadeamends,weexpectthatforgivenesswilldiminishone’sself­respectandself­conceptclarity.

forgiveneSS tendencieS and partner agreeaBleneSS Jointly predict traJectorieS of Self-reSpect

The first study was a longitudinal investigation in which both members of 72recentlymarriedcouplescompleteduptoninequestionnairesoverthefirst5yearsofmarriage.Atthebeginningofthestudy,participantsreported(1)theirtendencytoforgivetheirspousebyimaginingthemselves infivesituationsthatdescribedtheir spouse transgressingagainst them(e.g., snappingatand insulting the self,lyingaboutinappropriatebehaviorswithsomeoneoftheoppositesex)andindi­catedtheextenttowhichtheywouldfeelandexpressforgivenessineachsituation;(2)theiragreeableness(e.g.,“Itaketimeoutforothers,”“Ifeellittleconcernforothers”[reversed]);and(3) theirself­respect (“IwishIcouldhavemorerespectformyself” [reversed]).Every6–8months following the initial assessment,par­ticipantscompletedadditionalreportsoftheirself­respect.Althoughtheextenttowhichperpetratorsactinanagreeablemannerisnotourfocalmeasureofperpe­tratorbehavior,agreeablenesshasbeenlinkedwithactinginaprosocial,construc­tivemannerduring interpersonalconflicts (Jensen­Campbell&Graziano,2001)and with seeking forgiveness (Chiaramello et al., 2008), which includes accept­ing responsibility and making reparation after committing a betrayal (Sandage,Worthington,Hight,&Berry,2000).Becauseagreeableindividualstendtomakeamends,weuseagreeablenessasaproxyforamendsinthisstudy.

Weconductedgrowthcurveanalyses(cf.Singer&Willett,2003)toassesstheassociationsofforgivenessandpartneragreeablenesswithlinearself­respecttrajec­tories.Specifically,wepredictedchangesinparticipants’self­respectovertimefromtheirtendencytoforgivetheirspouse,theirspouse’sagreeableness,time,andtheinteractiontermsamongthesevariables.Lookingfirstatthemaineffectsofvictims’andperpetrators’behavior,inturn,onvictims’outcomes,therewerenosignificantmaineffectsofforgivenessorspouseagreeablenessontrajectoriesofvictims’self­respect.Thus,therewasnoevidencethatvictimshaveactorcontrolorthatperpe­tratorshavepartnercontroloverchangesinvictims’self­respectovertime.

Turningtotheinteractioneffectofvictims’behaviorandperpetrators’behavioronvictims’outcomes,thetrajectoryofself­respectforparticipantswhoreporteda

Page 238: the Psychology of Social Conflict and Aggression Sydney Symposium in Social Psychology

the doormat effect 221

strongtendencytoforgivetheirspousedependedontheirspouse’sagreeableness.Highlyforgivingparticipantswhosespousereportedhighlevelsofagreeablenessexperiencedincreasesinself­respectovertime.Incontrast,highlyforgivingpar­ticipantswhosespousereportedlowlevelsofagreeablenessexperienceddecreasesinself­respectovertime.Thus,victimsandperpetratorssharedjointcontroloverchangesinvictims’self­respectovertime.

Althoughthesefindingsareconsistentwiththeideathatvictimsandperpetra­tors share jointcontrolovervictims’ self­respect, this studydidnotprovide theexperimental evidence necessary to conclude that forgiveness and perpetratorbehaviorcausedtheobservedchangesinself­respectovertime.Inaddition,itdidnotexaminewhethervictims’self­conceptclarityfollowsthesamepatternastheirself­respect.Finally,itusedanindirectmeasureofamends.Wedesignedthenextstudytoaddresstheselimitations.

experimentally manipulated perceptionS of forgiveneSS and amendS Jointly affect

Self-reSpect and Self-concept clarityThesecondstudywasanexperimentinwhich49undergraduatesreceivedfalsefeedback(usingaprocedureweadaptedfromKarremans,VanLange,Ouwerkerk,&Kluwer,2003)regardingtheextenttowhichtheyhaveforgivenandtheextentto which their perpetrator has made amends for a specific, real­life betrayal.Participants were asked to recall a recent incident in which a close other hurt,angered,orupsetthem.Afterprovidingadescriptionoftheincident,participantstypedinthefirstnameoftheperpetratorandansweredquestionsabouttheextenttowhichtheperpetratorhadmadeamends.

Then,participants readabout thebogus “forgiveness test,”which theyweretoldwouldassesstheextenttowhichtheyhadforgiventheirperpetrator.Thefor­givenesstestcapitalizedontheexperientialvalidityoftheImplicitAssociationTest(IAT;Greenwald,McGhee,&Schwartz,1998),whichwasoriginallydevelopedtoassesspeople’simplicitassociationsbetweencategoriesbycomparingtheirreactiontimeswhencategorizingwordsorimagesfromtargetcategoriesindifferentblocksoftrials.Thecategoriesusedintheforgivenesstestwere(1)theperpetrator’sfirstnameandotherfirstnamesand(2)wordswithpositivevalence(e.g.,love, accep-tance)andwordswithnegativevalence(e.g.,hate, rejection).Inoneblockoftrials,participantswereinstructedtopressthesamekeywhenpresentedwithpositivewordsandtheperpetrator’sname.Inanotherblock,theywereinstructedtopressthesamekeywhenpresentedwithnegativewordsandtheperpetrator’sname.

After completing this bogus forgiveness test, participants read that, when aperson has forgiven a perpetrator, associations between positive words and thenameof theperpetrator are stronger thanassociationsbetweennegativewordsandthenameoftheperpetrator.Butwhenapersonhasnotcompletelyforgiventheperpetrator, associationsbetweennegativewords and thenameof theper­petrator are stronger. Then, they read that these associations can be measuredthroughreactiontimes.Next,ratherthanscoringparticipants’actualperformance

Page 239: the Psychology of Social Conflict and Aggression Sydney Symposium in Social Psychology

laura B. luchieS and eli J. finkel222

on the forgiveness test, we instead gave them false feedback regarding theirreactiontimes.Participants in thelowforgivenessconditionweretold that theyrespondedfasterintheblockoftrialsinwhichtheyrespondedwiththesamekeytonegativewordsandthenameoftheperpetratorthanintheblockoftrialsinwhichtheyrespondedwiththesamekeytopositivewordsandthenameoftheperpetrator,whichindicatesthattheyhavenotcompletelyforgiventheperpetra­tor.Participantsinthehighforgivenesscondition weretoldthattheyrespondedfasterintheblockoftrialsinwhichtheyrespondedwiththesamekeytopositivewordsandthenameoftheperpetratorthanintheblockoftrialsinwhichtheyrespondedwiththesamekeytonegativewordsandthenameoftheperpetrator,whichindicatesthattheyhavelargelyforgiventheperpetrator.

Next, participants received false feedback regarding their responses to thequestionstheyhadansweredearlierinthestudyabouttheextenttowhichtheirperpetratorhadmadeamends.Participants intheweakamendscondition weretoldthat,comparedwithotherswhohadpreviouslyparticipatedinthestudy,theirresponsesindicatedthattheextenttowhichtheirperpetratorhadmadeamendswasinthe17thpercentile,whichmeansthattheirperpetratorhasmadeonlyweakamends.Participantsinthestrongamendscondition weretoldthattheirresponsesindicatedthattheextenttowhichtheirperpetratorhadmadeamendswasinthe83rdpercentile,whichmeansthattheirperpetratorhasmadestrongamends.

Followingthesemanipulations,participantscompletedmeasuresofself­respectandself­conceptclarity(“Ihavealotofrespectformyself”and“IhaveaclearsenseofwhoIamandwhatIam,”respectively).Next,participantscompletedmanipu­lationchecksassessingtheextenttowhich(1)theyhadforgiventheperpetratorand(2)theperpetratorhadmadeamends.Finally,theywereprobedforsuspicionand debriefed. The manipulation checks indicated that the manipulations weresuccessful:participantsinthehighforgivenessconditionreportedhavingofferedgreaterforgivenessthanthoseinthelowforgivenesscondition,andparticipantsinthestrongamendsconditionreportedhavingreceivedgreateramendsthanthoseintheweakamendscondition.

We conducted two analyses of variance (ANOVAs) with forgiveness andamendsfeedbackconditionsasthebetween­subjectsfactorsandwithself­respectandself­conceptclarity, inturn,asthedependentvariable.Lookingfirstat themaineffectsofvictims’andperpetrators’behavior,inturn,onvictims’outcomes,therewerenosignificantmaineffectsofforgivenessoramendsonself­respectorself­conceptclarity.Thus,therewasnoevidencethatvictimshaveactorcontrolorthatperpetratorshavepartnercontrolovervictims’postconflictself­respectorself­conceptclarity.

Turningtotheinteractioneffectofvictims’behaviorandperpetrators’behav­ioronvictims’outcomes,althoughthedescriptivepatternsofself­respectwereinthepredicteddirections,theforgiveness×amendsinteractioneffectonself­respectdidnotreachconventionallevelsofsignificance.However,theeffectofforgivenessonself­conceptclaritydiddependonwhethertheperpetratormadeamends.Descriptivelyspeaking,participantswhowereledtobelievetheyhadforgiven a perpetrator who made strong amends reported higher self­conceptclaritythanthosewhowereledtobelievetheyhadnotforgivenaperpetrator

Page 240: the Psychology of Social Conflict and Aggression Sydney Symposium in Social Psychology

the doormat effect 223

whomadestrongamends.Incontrast,participantswhowereledtobelievetheyhadforgivenaperpetratorwhomadeweakamendsreportedlowerself­conceptclarity than those who were led to believe they had not forgiven a perpetra­torwhomadeweakamends.Thus,totheextentthattheeffectofparticipants’experimentallymanipulatedperceptionsofforgivenessandamendsontheirself­respectandself­conceptclarityparallelstheeffectofactuallevelsofforgivenessandamends,victimsandperpetratorssharedjointcontrolovervictims’postcon­flictself­conceptclarity.

Thisstudyextendedthefirststudybyexaminingtheeffectsofexperimentallymanipulatingparticipants’perceptionsoftheirownforgivenessofandperpetratoramendsmadeforactualbetrayalsonbothself­respectandself­conceptclarity.Wedesignedthefollowingstudytoprovideanadditionaltestofthecausaleffectsofforgivenessandamendsonself­respectandself­conceptclarity.

Well-controlled levelS of forgiveneSS and amendS Jointly affect anticipated Self-reSpect and Self-concept clarity

Thethirdstudywasanexperimentinwhich247undergraduatesimaginedthem­selvesasthevictimofapartnerbetrayal.Specifically,participantswereaskedtoimaginethemselvesinascenario(whichweadaptedfromBoon&Sulsky,1997)inwhichtheirromanticpartnerbetrayedtheirtrustbytellingamutualfriendveryprivatedetailsabouttheparticipant’spast.Participantsinthestrongamendscon­ditionreadthattheirpartneradmittedhisorhermistake,apologized,andtriedveryhard tomakeup for it,whereas those in theweakamends condition readthattheirpartnerdidnotadmithisorhermistake,didnotapologize,anddidnottryat all tomakeup for it.Next,participants in thehigh forgivenessconditionreadthattheydecidedtoforgivetheirpartner,whereasthoseinthelowforgive­nessconditionreadthattheydecidednottoforgivetheirpartner.Afterimaginingthemselvesinthescenario,participantscompletedmeasuresassessingthelevelsofself­respectandself­conceptclaritytheyanticipatedtheywouldhaveiftheyhadjustgonethroughthedescribedsituation(“Iwouldhavealotofrespectformyself”and“IwouldhaveaclearsenseofwhoIamandwhatIam,”respectively).

Weconducted twoANOVAswith forgivenessandamendsconditions as thebetween­subjects factors and with self­respect and self­concept clarity, in turn,asthedependentvariable.Lookingfirstatthemaineffectsofvictims’andperpe­trators’behavior,inturn,onvictims’outcomes,thereweremarginallysignificantmaineffectsofforgiveness,suchthatgreaterforgivenesscausedloweranticipatedself­respect and self­concept clarity. There were also significantmaineffectsofamends,suchthatgreateramendscausedhigheranticipatedself­respectandself­conceptclarity.Thus,therewassomeevidencethatvictimshaveactorcontrolovertheiranticipatedpostconflictself­respectandself­conceptclaritybutthatforgivingmayhaveanegativeeffectonvictims’outcomes.Andtherewasevidencethatper­petrators havepartner control over victims’ anticipated postconflict self­respectandself­conceptclarity.

Page 241: the Psychology of Social Conflict and Aggression Sydney Symposium in Social Psychology

laura B. luchieS and eli J. finkel224

Turning to the interaction effect of victims’ behavior and perpetrators’behavior on victims’ outcomes, the effect of forgiveness on both self­respectand self­concept clarity depended on whether the perpetrator made amends.Descriptively speaking, participants who imagined offering forgiveness whentheirpartnermadeamendsreportedtheywouldexperiencehigherself­respectandself­conceptclaritythanthosewhoimaginedwithholdingforgivenesswhentheir partner made amends. In contrast, participants who imagined offer­ing forgivenesswhen theirpartnerdidnotmakeamends reported theywouldexperiencelowerself­respectandself­conceptclaritythanthosewhoimaginedwithholdingforgivenesswhentheirpartnerdidnotmakeamends.Thus,victimsandperpetratorssharedjointcontrolovervictims’anticipatedpostconflictself­respectandself­conceptclarity.

Although these results established that forgiveness and amends caused theobserveddifferencesinanticipatedlevelsofself­respectandself­conceptclarity,hypotheticalscenariosmayseemartificial,andparticipants’anticipatedself­respectandself­conceptclarityscoresmayreflecttheirtheoriesofhowtheyshouldviewthemselves in thedescribed situation rather thanhow they actuallywouldviewthemselves.Therefore,itremainsimportanttoexamineassociationsamongforgive­ness,amends,self­respect,andself­conceptclarityastheynaturallyoccurfollow­ingactualbetrayals.Wedesignedthefinalstudytoexaminetheseassociations.

actual levelS of forgiveneSS and amendS Jointly predict Self-reSpect

and Self-concept clarityThe fourth study was a longitudinal investigation in which 69 undergraduatesinvolvedindatingrelationshipscompleted14biweeklyonlinequestionnairesover6months.Oneachquestionnaire,participantsreportedtheirself­respectandself­conceptclarity(“Irespectmyself”and“Ingeneral,IhaveaclearsenseofwhoIamandwhatIam,”respectively).Later inthequestionnaire,participantsansweredyesornotothefollowingquestion:“Hasyourpartnerdoneanythingoverthepast2weeksthatwasupsettingtoyou?”Participantswhoanswerednomovedontoanunrelatedsetofquestions.Thosewhoansweredyescompletedmeasuresassessingforgiveness(“Ihaveforgivenmypartnerforthisbehavior”),amends(“Mypartnertried tomakeamends tomefor thisupsettingbehavior”), andbetrayal severity(“Thisbehaviorwashighlydistressingtome”).

Weconductedtwosetsofmultilevelregressionanalysespredictingself­respectandself­conceptclarity,inturn,fromforgiveness,amends,andbetrayalseverity.Lookingfirstatthemaineffectsofvictims’andperpetrators’behavior,inturn,onvictims’outcomesafterseverebetrayals,therewerenosignificantmaineffectsofforgiveness.Butthereweremarginallysignificantmaineffectsofamends,suchthatgreateramendspredictedhigherself­respectandself­conceptclarity.Thus,therewasnoevidencethatvictimshaveactorcontrolovertheirpostconflictself­respectorself­conceptclarity.However,therewassomeevidencethatperpetratorshavepartnercontrolovervictims’postconflictself­respectandself­conceptclarity.

Page 242: the Psychology of Social Conflict and Aggression Sydney Symposium in Social Psychology

the doormat effect 225

Turningtotheinteractioneffectofvictims’behaviorandperpetrators’behav­ioronvictims’outcomesafterseverebetrayals,theassociationofforgivenesswithbothself­respectandself­conceptclaritydependedontheextenttowhichtheper­petratormadeamends.Increasinglevelsofforgivenesspredictedmoreself­respectandself­conceptclaritywhenthepartnermadestrongamendsforhighlydistress­ingbetrayals.Incontrast,descriptivelyspeaking,increasinglevelsofforgivenesspredictedlessself­respectandself­conceptclaritywhenthepartnermadeweakamendsforseverebetrayals.Thus,victimsandperpetratorssharedjointcontrolovervictims’postconflictself­respectandself­conceptclarity.

This study complemented the previous studies by examining prospectivereports of forgiveness, amends, self­respect, and self­concept clarity followingactualbetrayalsinongoingrelationships,andtheseresultsshowedthattheassocia­tionsofforgivenesswithself­respectandself­conceptclaritydependontheextenttowhichtheperpetratorhasmadeamends.Acrossthefourstudies,ourhypothesisthatvictim’sbehaviorandperpetrators’behaviorwieldjointcontrolovervictims’self­respectandself­conceptclaritywassupportedstronglyandconsistently.Thefirststudydemonstratedthattheassociationofmaritalforgivenesswithtrajectoriesofself­respectdependsonspouseagreeableness,whichisassociatedwithmakingamends.Thethreesubsequentstudiesdemonstratedthattheeffectofforgivenessonself­respectandself­conceptclaritydependsonperpetratoramends.Inaddi­tion,ourtwosubhypothesesweresupported:forgivingbolstersone’sself­respectand self­concept clarity if the perpetrator tends to act in a generally agreeablemannerormakesamends,butdiminishesone’sself­respectandself­conceptclar­ityiftheperpetratortendstoactinagenerallydisagreeablemannerordoesnotmakeamends.All14simpleeffectswereinthepredicteddirections,butnotallofthemachievedstatisticalsignificance.Weconductedameta­analysistoformallytest whether the simple effects garnered reliable support across studies in thisresearchprogram.(Thefirststudywasnotincludedinthemeta­analysisbecausechangeinself­respectovertime,ratherthanabsolutelevelsofself­respect,wastheprimaryunitofanalysis.)

meta-analySiSWe calculated meta­analytic (1) main effects of forgiveness on self­respect andself­concept clarity, (2)main effectsof amendson self­respect and self­conceptclarity,(3)interactioneffectsofforgivenessandamendsonself­respectandself­conceptclarity, (4)simpleeffectsof forgivenessonself­respectandself­conceptclaritywhentheperpetratormadestrongamends,and(5)simpleeffectsoffor­givenessonself­respectandself­conceptclaritywhentheperpetratormadeweakamends.Because themeta­analytic effectscombine the resultsof studiesusingexperimentallymanipulatedperceptionsof,hypotheticallevelsof,andactuallevelsofforgivenessandamends,andbecausetheseeffectsmaydifferfromoneanother,theyshouldbeinterpretedwithcaution.Butbecausethepatternofresultswassimilarforallthreestudies,themeta­analyticresultslikelyreflecttheeffectsofactual levels of forgiveness and amendson self­respect and self­concept clarity.Lookingfirstatthemaineffectsofvictims’andperpetrators’behavior,inturn,on

Page 243: the Psychology of Social Conflict and Aggression Sydney Symposium in Social Psychology

laura B. luchieS and eli J. finkel226

victims’outcomes,themeta­analysisrevealedthat,acrossstudies,therewerenosignificantmaineffectsofforgivenessonself­respectorself­conceptclarity.Thus,across studies, therewasnoevidence that victimshaveactor controlover theirpostconflictself­respectorself­conceptclarity.Thisnullresultcontrastswiththeliteraturelinkingforgivenesstoavarietyofpositiveoutcomesandfailstosupportthenotionthatforgivenessisapanacea.Butthereweresignificantmaineffectsofamends,suchthatgreateramendscausedhigherself­respectandself­conceptclar­ity.Thus,acrossstudies,therewasevidencethatperpetratorshavepartnercontrolovervictims’postconflictself­respectandself­conceptclarity.

Turningtotheinteractioneffectofvictims’behaviorandperpetrators’behavioronvictims’outcomes,themeta­analysisrevealedthatthereweresignificantforgive­ness×amendsinteractioneffectsforbothself­respectandself­conceptclarity.Themeta­analysisalsoprovidedstrongsupportforbothsimpleeffects.AcrossStudies2–4,forgivenesssignificantlybolsteredself­respectandself­conceptclaritywhentheperpetratormadestrongamends,butforgivenesssignificantlydiminishedself­respect and self­concept claritywhen theperpetratormadeonlyweakamends.Thus,victimsandperpetratorssharedjointcontrolovervictims’postconflictout­comes,suchthatiftheperpetratorhasmadeamendsthenforgivingincreasesone’sself­respectandself­conceptclarity,butiftheperpetratorhasnotmadeamendsthenforgivingdecreasesone’sself­respectandself­conceptclarity.

Thepredictedmeansfromthemeta­analysisforvictims’self­respectandself­conceptclarityarepresentedinFigures 14.2and14.3,respectively.Calculatingtheamountofactorcontrol,partnercontrol,andjointcontrolusingtheformulaspresentedintheIntroductionconfirmsthatvictimsdonothavecompletecontrolovertheirownoutcomes.Rather,victimshaveasmallandnonsignificantamountof actor control (−.21 and −.20 for self­respect and self­concept clarity, respec­tively);collapsingacrosslevelsofperpetratoramends,victimswhoforgivereport

Actor Control = ((4.63 + 3.53) – (4.23 + 4.34)) / 2 = –.21Partner Control = ((4.63 + 4.23) – (3.53 + 4.34)) / 2 = .49Joint Control = ((4.63 + 4.34) – (4.23 + 3.53)) / 2 = .60

Forgive

MakeAmends

Do NotMake

Amends

Do Not ForgiveVictim

Perpetrator

4.63(0.25)

4.23(0.03)

3.53(–0.37)

4.34(0.09)

figure 14.2 Actorcontrol,partnercontrol,andjointcontrolovervictims’meta­analyzedpostconflictself­respect.Tablevaluesinboldarerawscoresona1–7scale.Tablevaluesinparenthesisarestandardizedscores.

Page 244: the Psychology of Social Conflict and Aggression Sydney Symposium in Social Psychology

the doormat effect 227

anaverageoftwo­tenthsofascalepointlessself­respectandself­conceptclaritythanthosewhodonotforgive.Incontrast,perpetratorshaveasignificantamountof partner control (.49 and .46 for self­respect and self­concept clarity, respec­tively); collapsing across levelsof victim forgiveness, victimswho receive strongamendsreportanaverageoffour­tofive­tenthsofascalepointmoreself­respectandself­conceptclaritythanthosewhoreceiveonlyweakamends.Importantly,victimsandperpetratorsalsoshareasignificantamountofjointcontrol(.60and.65forself­respectandself­conceptclarity,respectively);victimswhoeitherfor­giveaperpetratorwhomadestrongamendsordonotforgiveaperpetratorwhomadeonlyweakamendsreportanaverageofsix­toseven­tenthsofascalepointmoreself­respectandself­conceptclaritythanthosewhoeitherforgiveaperpetra­torwhomadeonlyweakamendsordonotforgiveaperpetratorwhomadestrongamends.

additional evidence of Joint control over victimS’ poStconflict outcomeS

Twoexperimentsandtwolongitudinalstudiesprovidedconsistentevidencethatvictimsandperpetratorssharejointcontrolovervictims’postconflictself­respectandself­conceptclarity.Isthereevidencethatvictimsandperpetratorssharejointcontrolnotonlyovervictims’psychologicalhealthoutcomes,suchasself­respectandself­conceptclarity,butalsoovervictims’relationalwell­beingandphysicalhealthoutcomes?Theanswerappearstobeyes.Inalongitudinalstudyofmarriedcouples, McNulty (2008) found that, although individuals whose spouses rarelybehavednegativelyexperienced more stablemarital satisfactionover thefirst 2yearsofmarriagetotheextenttheyweremoreforgiving,individualswhosespousesfrequentlybehavednegativelyexperiencedsteeperdeclinesinmaritalsatisfaction

Actor Control = ((4.86 + 3.74) – (4.40 + 4.59)) / 2 = –.20Partner Control = ((4.86 + 4.40) – (3.74 + 4.59)) / 2 = .46Joint Control = ((4.86 + 4.59) – (4.40 + 3.74)) / 2 = .65

Forgive

MakeAmends

Do NotMake

Amends

Do Not ForgiveVictim

Perpetrator

4.86(0.29)

4.40(0.00)

3.74(–0.42)

4.59(0.12)

figure 14.3 Actorcontrol,partnercontrol,andjointcontrolovervictims’meta­analyzedpostconflictself­conceptclarity.Tablevaluesinboldarerawscoresona1–7scale.Tablevaluesinparenthesisarestandardizedscores.

Page 245: the Psychology of Social Conflict and Aggression Sydney Symposium in Social Psychology

laura B. luchieS and eli J. finkel228

totheextenttheyweremoreforgiving.Thatis,whethergreatermaritalforgivenesspredictedgreaterstabilityorsteeperdeclinesinmaritalsatisfactiondependedonhowfrequentlyone’sspousebehavedbadly,indicatingthatperpetratorsandvic­timssharejointcontrolovervictims’relationalwell­being.

Anotherstudyindicatedthatperpetratorsandvictimsalsomaysharejointcon­trol over victims’ physical health outcomes. In a study of women at a domesticviolenceshelter,Gordon,Burton,andPorter(2004)foundthatthosewhoreportedthe greatest forgiveness of their abusive partner were the most likely to reporttheyintendedtoreturntotheirpartner.Returningtoanabusivepartnermaywellheightentheriskofbeingabusedagain,butwhetherornotreturningtoapreviouslyabusivepartnerleadstofurtherabusedependsontheperpetrator’sbehavior.

Thefindingsofthepreviouslyreviewedstudies,togetherwiththefindingsofMcNulty(2008)andGordonetal.(2004),suggestthatvictimsandperpetratorssharejointcontroloveranarrayofvictims’outcomes,includingtheirself­respect,self­conceptclarity,maritalsatisfaction,andriskofbeingphysicallyabused.Yetanotherbodyofresearchsuggeststhatvictimshaveactorcontroloverotherout­comes, including their life satisfaction (Bonoetal.,2008),commitment to theirperpetrators(Tsangetal.,2006)andphysiologicalstress(McCulloughetal.,2007).Itmaybethatsomeoutcomesaresubjectprimarilytojointcontrolwhereasotheroutcomesaresubjectprimarilytoactorcontrol.Forinstance,avictimwhoforgivesaperpetratorwhohasnotmadeamendsmightexperiencedecreasedself­respectandself­conceptclarityatthesametimeasincreasedcommitmenttotheperpe­trator.Byexaminingmultipleoutcomesof forgiveness inthesamestudy, futureworkcouldexplorewhetherthecostsofforgivingintheabsenceofamendsout­weighthebenefitsofdoingso.

concluding remarkSGiventhatvictimsandperpetratorssharejointcontrolovervictims’postconflictoutcomes,ourdatasuggestthatconflictresolutionstrategiesdesignedtopromotevictims’forgivenessshouldaimtoheightenvictims’sensitivitytowhetherforgive­nessislikelytobebeneficialintheirparticularsituation.Furthermore,forgivenessinterventionsshouldbesupplementedwithstrategiesdesignedtopromoteperpe­trators’amendmaking(e.g.,theVictimOffenderReconciliationProgram;see,e.g.,Green, 1984; Ristovski & Wertheim, 2005). Such “amends interventions” couldadaptmanyof themethodsused in forgiveness interventions, includinghelpingperpetratorsdevelopempathyfortheirvictims,havingperpetratorsrecall timestheywerehurtbyothers,andencouragingperpetratorstomakeacommitmenttomakeamendsfortheirmisdeeds.

Moreover,becausereceivingamendsfacilitatesforgiveness(e.g.,McCullough,Worthington,&Rachal,1997),interventionsthatsuccessfullyincreasetheextenttowhichperpetratorsmakeamendsmayalsoincreasetheextenttowhichvictimsforgive.Pastresearchhasshownthat,whenperpetratorsnotonlyapologizebutalsooffertocompensatetheirvictimsfortheiroffenses,victimsareespeciallylikelytoforgive(Bottom,Gibson,Daniels,&Murnighan,2002;Darby&Schlenker,1982).Moreover, in ananalysisof videotapedconflictdiscussions,perpetratoramends

Page 246: the Psychology of Social Conflict and Aggression Sydney Symposium in Social Psychology

the doormat effect 229

expressed during one 2­minute segment were positively associated with victimforgiveness expressed during the following segment, controlling for forgivenessexpressedintheinitialsegment(Hannonetal.,2010).

Conflict resolution strategies that successfully promote both perpetratoramends and victim forgiveness are optimal because they are likely to yield themostfavorableoutcomes.Inallfourstudiesexaminingvictims’postconflictself­respectandself­conceptclarity,victims’self­viewswerethemostpositivewhentheyforgaveperpetratorswhohadmadeamends.Byrecognizingthat,justastwopeopleareinvolvedwhenarelationshipruptures,so,too,aretwopeopleinvolvedinmendingthoseruptures,individualswhoseektohealtheirownorothers’bro­kenrelationshipsmightdosomoresuccessfully.

referenceS

Bono,G.,McCullough,M.E.,&Root,L.M. (2008).Forgiveness, feelingconnected toothers,andwell­being:Twolongitudinalstudies.Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 34,182–195.

Boon, S. D., & Sulsky, L. M. (1997). Attributions of blame and forgiveness in romanticrelationships:Apolicy­capturingstudy.Journal of Social Behavior and Personality, 12,19–44.

Bottom, W. P., Gibson,K., Daniels, S. E.,&Murnighan, J. K. (2002). When talk is notcheap: Substantive penance and expressions of intent in rebuilding cooperation.Organization Science, 13(5),497–513.

Chiaramello,S.,Sastre,M.T.M.,&Mullet,E.(2008).Seekingforgiveness:Factorstruc­ture,andrelationshipswithpersonalityandforgivingness.Personality and Individual Differences, 45,383–388.

Darby, B. W., & Schlenker, B. R. (1982). Children’s reactions to apologies. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 43(4),742–753.

Gordon, K. C., Burton, S., & Porter, L. (2004). Predicting the intentions of women indomesticviolenceshelterstoreturntopartners:Doesforgivenessplayarole?Journal of Family Psychology, 18,331–338.

Green,S.(1984).Victim­offenderreconciliationprogram:Areviewoftheconcept.Social Action & the Law, 10,43–52.

Greenwald,A.G.,McGhee,D.E.,&Schwartz,J.L.K.(1998).Measuringindividualdiffer­encesinimplicitcognition:TheImplicitAssociationTest.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74,1464–1480.

Hannon, P. A., Rusbult, C. E., Finkel, E. J., & Kumashiro, M. (2010). In the wake ofbetrayal:Amends,forgiveness,andtheresolutionofbetrayal.Personal Relationships,17, 253–278.

Hebl,J.H.,&Enright,R.D.(1993).Forgivenessasapsychotherapeuticgoalwithelderlyfemales.Psychotherapy, 30,658–667.

Jensen­Campbell,L.A.,&Graziano,W.G.(2001).Agreeablenessasamoderatorofinter­personalconflict.Journal of Personality, 69,323–362.

Karremans,J.C.,VanLange,P.A.M.,Ouwerkerk,J.W.,&Kluwer,E.S.(2003).Whenforgivingenhancespsychologicalwell­being:Therolesofinterpersonalcommitment.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 84,1011–1026.

Kelley,H.H.,Holmes,J.G.,Kerr,N.L.,Reis,H.T.,Rusbult,C.E.,&VanLange,P.A.M.(2003).An atlas of interpersonal situations.NewYork:CambridgeUniversityPress.

Page 247: the Psychology of Social Conflict and Aggression Sydney Symposium in Social Psychology

laura B. luchieS and eli J. finkel230

Kelley,H.H.,&Thibaut,J.W.(1978).Interpersonal relations: A theory of interdependence.NewYork:Wiley.

Luchies,L.B.,Finkel,E.J.,McNulty,J.K.,&Kumashiro,M.(2010).Thedoormateffect:When forgiving erodes self­respect and self­concept clarity. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 98, 734–749.

McCullough,M.E.(2008).Beyond revenge: The evolution of the forgiveness instinct.SanFrancisco:Jossey­Bass.

McCullough,M.E.,Orsulak,P.,Brandon,A.,&Akers,L.(2007).Rumination, fear,andcortisol:An invivostudyof interpersonal transgressions.Health Psychology, 26(1),126–132.

McCullough,M.E.,Worthington,E.L.,&Rachal,K.C.(1997).Interpersonalforgivingincloserelationships.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 73, 321–336.

McNulty,J.K.(2008).Forgivenessinmarriage:Puttingthebenefitsintocontext.Journal of Family Psychology, 22,171–175.

Orcutt,H.K.(2006).Theprospectiverelationshipofinterpersonalforgivenessandpsycho­logicaldistresssymptomsamongcollegewomen.Journal of Counseling Psychology, 53,350–361.

Ristovski,A.,&Wertheim,E.H.(2005).Investigationofcompensationsource,traitempa­thy, satisfaction with outcome and forgiveness in the criminal context. Australian Psychologist, 40,63–69.

Rye,M.S.,&Pargament,K.I.(2002).Forgivenessandromanticrelationshipsincollege:Canithealthewoundedheart?Journal of Clinical Psychology, 58,419–441.

Sandage,S. J.,Worthington,E.L.,Hight,T.L.,&Berry, J.W. (2000).Seeking forgive­ness:Theoretical context andan initial empirical study. Journal of Psychology and Theology, 28,21–35.

Singer,J.D.,&Willett,J.B.(2003).Applied longitudinal data analysis.NewYork:OxfordUniversityPress.

Thibaut,J.W.,&Kelley,H.H.(1959).The social psychology of groups.NewYork:Wiley.Tsang, J., McCullough, M. E., & Fincham, F. D. (2006). The longitudinal association

between forgivenessandrelationshipclosenessandcommitment.Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 25(4),448–472.

Wade,N.G.,&Worthington,E.L.(2005).Insearchofacommoncore:Acontentanalysisofinterventionstopromoteforgiveness.Psychotherapy: Theory, Research, Practice, Training, 42,160–177.

Waltman,M.A.,Russell,D.C.,Coyle,C.T.,Enright,R.D.,Holter,A.C.,&Swoboda,C.M.(2009).Theeffectsofaforgivenessinterventiononpatientswithcoronaryarterydisease.Psychology and Health, 24,11–27.

Worthington,E.L.,Kurusu,T.A.,Collins,W.,Berry, J.W.,Ripley, J.S.,&Baier,S.B.(2000).Forgivingusually takes time:A lesson learnedby studying interventions topromoteforgiveness.Journal of Psychology and Theology, 28,3–20.

Page 248: the Psychology of Social Conflict and Aggression Sydney Symposium in Social Psychology

Section IVSocial, Cultural, and Evolutionary Factors

in Social Conflict and Aggression

Page 249: the Psychology of Social Conflict and Aggression Sydney Symposium in Social Psychology
Page 250: the Psychology of Social Conflict and Aggression Sydney Symposium in Social Psychology

233

15TheMaleWarriorHypothesis

MARKVANVUGTVU University Amsterdam, The Netherlands

A lienbiologistscollectingdataaboutdifferent life formsonPlanetEarthwouldnodoubtcomeupwithcontradictoryclaimsabouthumannature.They would witness the human capacity to help complete strangers in

sometimes largegroups,yettheywouldalsoobservemanyincidentsofextremeviolence,especiallybetweengroupsofmales.Tomakesenseofthedata,thealienresearcherswouldprobablyconcludethathumansareafiercelytribalsocialspe­cies.Sometimeago,CharlesDarwinspeculatedabouttheoriginsofhumantribalnature:“Atribeincludingmanymemberswho,frompossessinginahighdegreethespiritofpatriotism,fidelity,obedience,courage,andsympathy,werealwaysreadytoaidoneanother,andtosacrificethemselvesforthecommongood,wouldbevictoriousovermostothertribes;andthiswouldbenaturalselection”(1871,p.132).UnfortunatelyDarwin’sbrilliantinsightwasignoredformorethanacen­turybyfellowscientists,yetitisnowgainingimpact.HereIofferanevolutionaryperspectiveonthesocialpsychologyofintergroupconflict,offeringnewinsightsand evidence about the origins and manifestation of coalitional and intergroupaggression.1

Socialscientistsareincreasinglyadoptinganevolutionaryapproachtodevelopnovelhypothesesandtointegratedataonvariousaspectsofhumansocialbehavior(Buss,2005;VanVugt&Schaller,2008).Theevolutionaryapproachisbasedonthepremisethatthehumanbrainisaproductofevolutionthroughnaturalselec­tioninthesamewayourbodiesaretheproductsofnaturalselection.Evolutionary­mindedpsychologistsfurtherproposethatthehumanbrainisessentiallysocial,comprisingmanyfunctionalizedmechanisms—oradaptations—tocopewiththe

1 Iwillusethetermscoalitionalandintergroup aggressioninterchangeablythroughoutthischapter.Althoughthereisadifferenceinscale,bothtypesofaggressioninvolveindividualswhoasmembersofgroupscommitactsofaggressionagainstmembersofothergroups(Brewer&Brown,1998).

Page 251: the Psychology of Social Conflict and Aggression Sydney Symposium in Social Psychology

mark van vugt234

variouschallengesofgroupliving(VanVugt&Schaller,2008).Onesuchspecial­izedmechanismiscoalitionformation.Formingallianceswithotherindividualsconfers considerable advantages inprocuring andprotecting reproductively rel­evant resources (e.g., food, territories, mates, offspring) especially in large anddiversesocialgroups.Coalitionalpressuresmayhaveledinhumanevolutiontotheemergenceofsomeratheruniquehumantraitssuchaslanguage,theoryofmind,culture, andwarfare. Ithasbeenargued thatultimately the need to formeverlargercoalitionsspurredtheincreaseinhumansocialnetworksizeandledtoaconcomitantbrainsizetoholdthesenetworkstogetherandtodealeffectivelywithanintensifiedcompetitionforresources—thishasbeendubbedtheMachiavellianIntelligencehypothesis,theSocialBrainhypothesis,ortheSocialGluehypothesis(Byrne & Whiten, 1988; Dunbar; Van Vugt & Hart, 2004). According to thesehypotheses,oursocialbrainisthereforeessentiallyatribalbrain.

Insearchingfortheoriginsofthehumantribalbrainitisusefultomakeadis­tinctionbetweenproximateandultimatecauses.Anactofintergroupaggressionsuchaswar,terrorism,gang­relatedviolenceorhooliganismcouldbeexplainedattwodifferentlevelsatleast.First,whydidthisparticulargroupdecidetoattacktheother?Thisproximatequestioninterestsmostsociologists,politicalscientists,his­torians,andsocialpsychologistsstudyingsocialconflict.Second,onecouldaskwhyhumanshaveevolved thecapacity toengage in intergroupaggression—thisulti­matequestioninterestsmostlyevolutionary­mindedpsychologistsandanthropolo­gists.Addressingquestionsatdifferentlevelsproducesamorecompletepicture,buttheselevelsshouldnotbeconfused(Buss,2005;VanVugt&VanLange,2006).

Intermsofultimatecausesofintergroupaggression,threeclassesofexplanationsaregenerallyinvoked(Kurzban&Neuberg,2005;VanVugt,2009;seealsoChapters10and18inthisvolume).Thefirsttreatsitasaby­productofanadaptivein­grouppsychology.Beingahighlysocialandcooperativespecies,humanslikelypossessten­denciestofavorhelpingmembersof in­groups(Brewer,1979;Brewer&Caporael,2006;Tajfel& Turner, 1986). As a result of this in­group favoritism, people showeitherindifferenceor(perhapsworse)adislikeformembersofout­groups.Analter­nativeby­producthypothesisviewsintergroupaggressionasanextensionofinterper­sonalaggression.Theargumentisthathumanshaveevolvedspecializedmechanismstoengageinaggressionagainstconspecificsandthatthesemechanismshavebeenco­opted to cope with a relatively novel evolutionary threat, namely, aggressionbetweengroups(Buss,2005).Thethirdclassfocusesexplicitlyonanadaptiveinter­grouppsychology.Theargumentisthathumanslikelyevolvedspecificpsychologicalmechanismstointeractwithmembersofout­groupsbecausesuchsituationsposeda significant reproductive challenge for ancestral humans. This latter hypothesisaccountsforthehighlytexturedsocialpsychologyofintergrouprelationsandisthere­foremorepersuasive.Forinstance,peopledonothavesomehazynegativefeelingtowardanout­group;insomeinstancesout­groupsmotivateadesiretoapproachoravoidandinotherinstancestofight,dominate,exploit,orexterminate.

Recent work on prejudice and intergroup relations recognizes this texturednatureofintergrouppsychologyandhasgeneratedmanynewinsightsandempiri­calfindingsconsistentwiththisview(Cottrell&Neuberg,2005;Kurzban&Leary,2001; Schaller, Park, & Faulkner, 2003; Sidanius & Pratto, 1999; Van Vugt, De

Page 252: the Psychology of Social Conflict and Aggression Sydney Symposium in Social Psychology

the male Warrior hypotheSiS 235

Cremer, & Janssen, 2007; Van Vugt, 2009). Given the complexity of intergrouprelations,thereareprobablymanydifferentadaptiveresponsespertainingtothenature and type of intergroup challenge. From an evolutionary perspective, itbecomesclearthatnotallintergroupsituationsareequalbecausenotallout­groupsareequal.Forinstance,notallout­groupsconsistofcoalitionsofindividualswhoengageincoordinatedaction—thinkofthehomeless, theelderly,orpeoplewithblue eyes. Humans are likely to have evolved coalition­detection mechanismsthatareresponsive tovarious indicatorsof tribalalliances (Kurzban,Tooby,andCosmides,2001).AsKurzbanandLearynote,“Membershipinapotentiallycoop­erativegroupshouldactivateapsychologyofconflictandexploitationofout­groupmembers—afeaturethatdistinguishesadaptationsforcoalitionalpsychologyfromothercognitivesystems”(p.195).Inmodernenvironments,heuristiccuessuchasskincolor,speechpatterns,andlinguisticlabels—regardlessofwhethertheyactu­allysignaltribalalliances—mayengagethesemechanisms(Kurzbanetal.;Schalleretal.).Perhapsequallyimportant,manyothersalientcuessuchasgender,age,oreyecolormaybefarlesslikelytoengagethistribalpsychology.Weshouldnotethatalthoughthistribalpsychologylikelyevolvedintheevolutionarycontextofcompe­titionforresources(e.g.,territories,food,andmates),thisdoesnotimplythatitiscontemporarilyactivatedonlywithincontextsinvolvingactualintergroupconflictasproposed,forinstance,byrealisticconflicttheory(Campbell,1999).

The specific psychological reactions of individuals in intergroup contextsshouldfurtherdependonwhetherone’sgroupistheaggressor.Fortheaggressors,desires todominateandexploit—and theassociatedpsychological tendencies—wouldbefunctional.Forthedefendingparty,desirestoyield,toavoid,ortomakepeace,alongwiththeassociatedpsychologicaltendencies,wouldbefunctional.Ofcourse,inmanysituations,agroup’spositionasbeingthedominantorsubordinatepartyistransientorambiguoussoitislikelythatthetwopsychologicaltendenciesareactivatedinsimilarsituationsbysimilarcuesandmoderatedbysimilarvari­ables(socialdominancetheory;Sidanius&Pratto,1999).

the male Warrior hypotheSiSAnimportantimplicationofthisevolutionarytribalbrainhypothesisisthatinter­groupconflictmayhaveaffectedthepsychologiesofmenandwomendifferently.Intergroupconflicthashistoricallyinvolvedrivalcoalitionsofmalesfightingoverscarcereproductiveresources,andthisistrueforearlyhumansaswellaschimpan­zees,ourclosestgeneticrelative(Chagnon,1988;DeWaal,2006;Goodall,1986).Menarebyfarthemostlikelyperpetratorsandvictimsofintergroupaggression,nowandinthepast.Asaconsequence,thisaspectofcoalitionalpsychologyislikelytobemorepronouncedamongmen,whichwedubbedthemale warrior hypothesis(MWH,seeTable15.1;VanVugtetal.,2007;VanVugt,2009).Thishypothesispos­itsthatduetoalonghistoryofmale­to­malecoalitionalconflictmenhaveevolvedspecializedcognitivemechanisms thatenable themto formallianceswithothermentoplan,toinitiate,toexecute,andtoemergevictoriousinintergroupconflictswiththeaimofacquiringorprotectingreproductivelyrelevantresources.

Page 253: the Psychology of Social Conflict and Aggression Sydney Symposium in Social Psychology

mark van vugt236

Evolutionary Models

TheMWHfitsintoatraditionofevolutionaryhypothesesaboutgenderdifferencesinsocialbehavior.Thereisalreadyconsiderableevidenceforgenderdifferencesinmorphology,psychology,andbehaviorthatarefunctionallyrelatedtodifferentselectionpressuresoperatingonmenandwomenthroughouthuman,primate,andmammalevolution(Campbell,1999;Eagly&Wood,1999;Geary,1998;Tayloretal.,2000).Duetoacombinationofdifferencesinparentalinvestmentandparentalcertaintymenandwomenpursuesomewhatdifferentmatingstrategies(Buss&Schmitt,1993;Trivers,1972).Inhumans—asinmostothermammals—mothersinvestmoreheavilyintheiroffspring;consequently,itwillbephysiologicallyandgeneticallycostlierforwomentobeopenlyaggressive(Archer,2000;Campbell,1999;Tayloretal.,2000).Yet,asthelessinvestingsexandundertherightcondi­tions, it canbeattractive formen to formaggressivecoalitionswith theaimofacquiringandprotectingvaluablereproductiveresources.

Tooby and Cosmides’s (1988) risk contract hypothesis specifies four condi­tionsfortheevolutionofcoalitionalaggression,whichunderscorestheevolution­arylogicofthehypothesizedgenderdifferencesinwarriorpsychology.First,the

taBle 15.1 the male Warrior hypothesis: domains of evidence, hypothesized mechanisms, predictions, and Support for gender differences

Domain of Evidence

HypothesizedMechanism

Prediction About Gender Difference Supported

1.Intergroupaggression

Propensitytoengageinintergroupaggression

Menaremorelikelytomakeunprovokedout­groupattacks

Yes

Menreporthavingmore(competitive)intergroupexperiences

Yes

2.Intergroupprejudice

Infrahumanizationordehumanizationofmembersofantagonisticout­groups

Menaremorelikelytoinfrahumanizemembersofout­groups

Yes

3.Intragroupdynamics

In­groupcooperationinresponsetooutgroupthreat

Mencontributemoretogroupduringintergroupcompetition

Yes

In­grouployaltyduringintergroupconflict

Menshowmorein­grouployaltyduringintergroupconflict

Maleleadershipbiasinintergroupconflict

Groupsshowstrongerpreferenceformaleleadersduringintergroupcompetition

4.Tribalpolitics Politicalsupportforintergroupaggression

Menshowstrongerpoliticalsupportforwarfareinopinionpolls

Yes

Preferencesforsocialdominancehierarchies

Menscorehigheronsocialdominanceorientationscale

Yes

5.Tribalsocialidentity

Affiliationtotribalgroups Menaremorelikelytomakespontaneoustribalassociationswhendefiningthemselves

Yes

Page 254: the Psychology of Social Conflict and Aggression Sydney Symposium in Social Psychology

the male Warrior hypotheSiS 237

averagelong­termgainsinreproductivesuccess(i.e.,matingopportunities)mustbesufficientlylargetooutweightheaveragecosts(i.e.,injuryordeath).Second,membersofwarfarecoalitionsmustbelieve that theirgroup is likely toemergevictoriousinbattle.Third,theriskthateachmembertakesandtheimportanceofeachmember’scontributiontovictorymusttranslateintoacorrespondingshareof benefits (cf. the free­rider problem). Fourth, when individuals go into battletheymustbecloakedina“veilofignorance”aboutwhowillliveordie.Thus,ifanintergroupvictoryproduces,onaverage,a20%increaseinreproductivesuccess,thenaslongastheriskofdeathforanyindividualcoalitionmemberislessthan20%(e.g.,1in10die)suchwarriortraitscouldbeselectedfor.Thismodelassumesthatthespoilsofanintergroupvictoryarepaidoutinextramatingopportunitiesfortheindividualmalesinvolved,andthusitisessentiallyanindividualselectionmodelbasedonsexualselection.

Alternatively,aspecificmalewarriorpsychologycouldhaveevolvedviagroup­levelselection.Multilevelselectiontheoryholds that if there is substantialvari­ance inthereproductivesuccessamonggroups thengroupselectionbecomesagenuine possibility (Wilson, Van Vugt, & O’Gorman, 2008). As Darwin (1871)noted,groupsofselfless individualsdobetter thangroupsofselfishindividuals.Althoughparticipatinginintergroupconflictispersonallycostly—becauseoftheriskofdeathor injury—genesunderlyingpropensity to serve thegroupcanbepropagatedifgroup­servingactscontributetogroupsurvival.Inarecentempiricaltestofthismodel,ChoiandBowles(2007)showedviacomputersimulationsthataltruistictraitscanspreadinpopulationsaslongasthereiscompetitionbetweengroupsandaltruisticactsbenefitin­groupmembersandharmout­groupmembers(parochialaltruism).

Oneconditionconducivetogroup­levelselectionoccurswhenthegeneticinterestsofgroupmembersarealigned,suchas inkingroups.Inkin­bondedgroups, individuals benefit not just from their own reproductive success butalso from the success of their family members (inclusive fitness; Hamilton,1964).Ancestralhumangroupsarelikelytohavebeenbasedaroundmalekinmembers,withfemalesmovingbetweengroupstoavoidinbreeding(so­calledpatrilocal groups). This offers a complementary reason for the evolution ofmalecoalitionalaggression:becausethemenaremoreheavilyinvestedintheirgroup,theyhavemoretolosewhenthegroupceasestoexist.Inaddition,thecollectiveactionproblemunderlyingcoalitionalaggressionis lesspronouncedwhengroupmembers’genetic interestsarealigned.Incidentally (butperhapsnotcoincidentally),thesamepatrilocalstructureisfoundinchimpanzees:malechimpanzeesalsoengageincoalitionalaggression(Goodall,1986;Wrangham&Peterson,1996).

Theseevolutionarymodelsdonotprecludethepossibilityofculturalprocessesatworkthatcouldexacerbateorunderminemalewarriorinstincts(Richerson&Boyd,2005).Infact,manyoftheevolvedpropensitiesforcoalitionalaggressionarelikelytobetranslatedintoactualpsychologicalandbehavioraltendenciesbysocializationpracticesandculturalnorms.Thus,itisentirelypossiblethatincer­tainenvironmentsitcouldbeadvantageousforsocietiestosuppressmalewarriortendencies(so­calledpeacefulsocieties)ortoturnfemalesintodedicatedwarriors.

Page 255: the Psychology of Social Conflict and Aggression Sydney Symposium in Social Psychology

mark van vugt238

Amodern­dayexampleof the latter is thestateofIsrael,whichis involvedinacontinuouswarwith itsArabneighbors.To increase itsmilitary strength,Israelhasaconscriptionarmyofbothmenandwomenandcurrentlyhasthemostliberalrulesregardingtheparticipationoffemales inactualwarfare(Goldstein,2003).WewouldexpectthesocializationpracticesamongIsraeligirlstomatchthoseofboys,potentiallyattenuatinganyinnatepsychologicaldifferences.

Evidence for the MWH From Across the Behavioral Sciences

Evidence for various aspects of this male warrior phenomenon can be foundthroughout the behavioral science literature, for instance, in anthropology, his­tory,sociology,politicalscience,biology,psychology,andprimatology.Asstated,acrossallcultures,almostanyactofintergroupaggressionisperpetratedbycoali­tionsofmales,forinstance,insituationsofwarfare,genocide,rebellion,terrorism,streetgangs,andhooliganviolence (Goldstein,2003;LivingstoneSmith,2007).Evidenceofmale­to­malecoalitionalaggressiongoesbackasfaras200,000years(e.g.,massgravescontainingmostlymaleskeletonswithevidenceofforce;Keeley,1996).Menarealsothemostlikelyvictimsofintergroupaggression.Onaverage,maledeath ratesdue towarfareamonghunter­gatherersare13%(according toarchaeologicaldata)and15%(accordingtoethnographicdata;Bowles,2006),sug­gestingarelativelystrongselectionpressureonmalewarriortraits.Thefigureissometimesevenhigher.AmongtheYanomamöintheAmazonBasin,anestimated20–30% of adult malesdie through tribal violence (Chagnon, 1988), comparedwithlessthan1%oftheU.S.andEuropeanpopulationsinthetwentiethcentury.Finally,theprimateliteraturerevealsthat,amongchimpanzees,adultmalesformcoalitionstoengageinviolenceagainstmembersofneighboringtroops.Thissug­geststhatthereisphylogeneticconsistencybetweenhumansandoneofourmostcloselyrelatedspecies(Wilson&Wrangham,2003).

Male warriors in traditional societies have higher status, more sexual part­ners,andmorechildren(Chagnon,1988),suggestingadirectreproductiveben­efit;RichardDawkins(1976)labeledthisthe“DukeofMarlborough”effect.Thesexualattractivenessofthemalewarriormightstillbeoperativeinmodernsoci­ety.AU.S.studyrevealedthatmaleyouthstreetgangmembershavemoresexualpartnersthanordinaryyoungmales(Palmer&Tilley,1995).Werecentlyfoundthatmilitarymenhavegreatersexappeal,especiallyiftheyhaveshownbraveryincombat(Leunissen&VanVugt,2010).Thus,theremaybereputationalbenefitsassociatedwith“warrior”behaviorsinmen(cf.competitivealtruism;Hardy&VanVugt,2006).

InlightofthesupportfortheMWH,itisnoteworthythatmanypublishedinter­groupstudiesinsocialpsychologydonotreporttheresultsformenandwomensep­aratelyandthatsomeuseonlymalesamples.Oneoftheclassicsocialpsychologicalstudies,theStanfordprisonexperiment(Zimbardo,1971),whichhighlightedsomedisturbingaspectsofhumancoalitionalaggression,usedanall­malesample.Teamgameexperimentsalsooftenuseallmalegroups(e.g.,Bornstein,2003).Inaper­sonalcommunication,oneoftheauthorsofthisstudy(Bornstein,2006)suggestedthatpilotresearchshowedthatfemalegroupswerelesscompetitive.

Page 256: the Psychology of Social Conflict and Aggression Sydney Symposium in Social Psychology

the male Warrior hypotheSiS 239

pSychological mechaniSmS underlying male Warrior phenomenon

TheMWHoffersanintegrative,conceptualframeworkinwhichfindingsfromdiverseliteraturescanbewovenintoacoherentstory.However,thisapproachruns theriskofbeinga“justso”storyabout the roleofcoalitionalaggressioninhumanevolution.Itwouldbemuchbetterifwecouldmakespecificpredic­tionsaboutgenderdifferencesinthepsychologicalmechanismsunderlyingthiswarriorpsychologyandcouldtestthesepredictionsincarefullycontrolledstud­ies.Ifmenhaveamorepronouncedwarriorpsychology,weshouldexpectthemtothinkandfeeldifferentlyabout intergroupconflictandtobemorelikelytoplan, support, and commit acts of intergroup aggression (Van Vugt, 2009). Inaddition,meningroupsshouldmakeadaptiveintergroupchoicesdependingoninformationaboutthesex,size,andformidabilityoftheout­group.Forinstance,they should respond with anger and aggression toward a numerically weakerout­groupandwithfearandavoidancetoastrongerout­group(especiallyanall­malegroup).Finally,thesereactionsarelikelytobeproducedautomaticallyandspontaneously.

To test various aspectsof themalewarriorhypothesis and tofindevidenceforgenderdifferencesinevolvedpsychologicalmechanisms,Iwillpresentsomeresearchfindingspertainingtovariousdomainssuchas(1)frequencyandlikeli­hoodofaggressiontowardout­groups;(2)protectionofin­groupsagainstexternalthreats;(3)likelihoodofpoliticalsupportforintergroupaggression;and(4)tribalsocialidentifications.Byandlarge,thesestudiesprovidepreliminarysupportforthemalewarriorhypothesis,yetmuchworkstillremainstobedone.

Propensity for Intergroup Aggression

Afirstprediction fromtheMWHis thatmenshould,onaverage,havea lowerthresholdtoengageinactsofintergroupaggressionwhengiventheopportunity.Wetestedthisinvariousways.First,weexaminehowmenandwomenmakedeci­sionsinwargamessimulatedinthelaboratory.AstudybyJohnsonetal.(2006)foundthat,onbeingtoldthattheyaretheleaderofafictitiouscountryinteractingwithleadersofothercountries,menaresignificantlymorelikelytoattackanothercountrywithoutprovocation(i.e.,“preemptivestrike”).Moreover,warfareismostintensewhenmenareplayingagainstothermendespitenotknowingthesexoftheirrivals.Thelowerthresholdforintergroupaggressionmaybeduetoexpecta­tionsofsuccess.Indeed,menheldmorepositiveillusionsaboutwinningthesesim­ulatedintergroupconflicts,abeliefthatincreasedtheprobabilitythattheywouldattacktheiropponent(Johnsonetal.).Anotherstudyanalyzingthesamedatasetfoundthatmoremale­typical2D:4Ddigitratios,whicharethoughttoindexpre­natal testosterone exposure, predicted aggression in the war­game experimentsoverandabovesex.Thesegenderdifferencesalsoemergewhenindividualsplayeconomic games between groups: all­male groups tend to be more competitivethanall­femalegroupsormixed­sexgroups (Wildschut,Pinter,Vevea, Insko,&Schopler,2003).

Page 257: the Psychology of Social Conflict and Aggression Sydney Symposium in Social Psychology

mark van vugt240

Second,thereisampleevidencethatmenandwomendifferintheirinvolve­ment inactsof intergroupaggressionoutside the laboratory (Pemberton,Insko,&Schopler,1996).Whenaskedtoindicatethefrequencyofvariouscategoriesofsocialinteractionsoverthepastmonth,menreportedmoregroup­to­groupinter­actions (mean [M]=18.47, standarddeviation [SD]=73.48) thanwomen(M=12.77,SD=59.68).Furthermore,menratedtheseinteractionsasmorecompeti­tive(Mformalevs.female=3.17vs.2.31,SD=2.50formalevs.2.22forfemale;scaleis1=verycooperative,5=verycompetitive).

Thus,consistentwiththeMWH,menexperienceintergroupcompetitionmoreoften,havealowerthresholdtostartanintergroupconflict,andaremoreoptimis­ticaboutwinningsuchconflicts.

Intergroup Prejudice and Stereotyping

The MWH further predicts that men are more likely to be prejudiced and toopenlydiscriminateagainstmembersofout­groups,especiallythosethatcanbeviewedascoalitionalthreats.Onemanifestationofout­groupprejudiceisinfrahu­manization,thetendencytoconsidermembersofout­groupssubhumanoranimallike,whichisoftenaprecursorofintergroupviolence(Haslam,2006;Leyensetal.,2001).Theevolutionarylogicisthatbyconsideringout­groupsaspsychologicallyinferioritwillbepsychologicallyeasiertotreatthembadly.Inarecentstudy(VanVugt,2009),menandwomen—allChristians—wereaskedtodescribeaChristianor Muslim target using either human­typical (e.g., civil) or animal­typical (e.g.,feral) words. Christian men were more likely to describe the Muslim target inanimal­typicalways,thusshowingevidenceofinfrahumanization.TheMWHalsopredicts that infrahumanizationstrategiesaremost likely inmale­to­maleinter­groupcontests,butthisremainstobetested.

Menalsoshowother intergroupbiasessuchasracismandxenophobiamorereadilyandespeciallyinthreateningsituationsSeveralexperimentsyieldagreatersensitivityofout­groupstereotypesforin­groupmen,especiallyunderconditionsofintergroupconflict(Gerard&Hoyt,1974;Sidanius,Cling,&Pratto,1991).Schaller,Park,andMueller(2003)showedthatmenusedanger­relevantstereotypestowardout­groupmembersmorewheninfluencedbycuesofambientdarkness.Finally,thenotoriousout­grouphomogeneityeffectdisappearswhen in­groupmembersareshownangryfacesofout­groupmalesbutnotfemales(Ackermanetal.,2006),whichisconsistentwiththeideathatout­groupmalesposeaheightenedthreat.

ThesefindingssupporttheMWHinthatmenaremorelikelytobeprejudicedagainst members of out­groups, especially when these constitute a coalitionalthreat;inaddition,out­groupmenaremorelikelytobediscriminatedagainst.

Protecting the Group Against External Threats

TheMWHalsoexpectsthepresenceofpsychologicalmechanismsthatenablementoprotect their in­groupagainstexternal threats.Todefend thegrouprequirespeople tobondtogetherand tohelp the in­group (Brewer&Brown,1998;VanVugtet al., 2007).Basedon theMWH,wehypothesize thatduring intergroup

Page 258: the Psychology of Social Conflict and Aggression Sydney Symposium in Social Psychology

the male Warrior hypotheSiS 241

conflictparticularlymenwillstepuptheireffortstohelpthein­group.Consistentwiththisprediction,inpublic­goodgameswefoundthatmenraisedtheirgroupcontributionsbutonlywhenweactivatedcompetitionbetweengroups(VanVugtetal.).InExperiment1,VanVugtetal.foundthatduringintergroupcompetition92%ofmen(butonly53%ofwomen)contributedtothepublicgood.Inaddition,menshowedgreaterin­grouployaltybystickingwiththegroupevenifitwasmore(financially) attractive to leave (Van Vugt et al., 2007). As a proxy for in­groupcohesion,menwerealsomorelikelytoincreasetheiridentificationwiththegroupunderconditionsofintergroupconflict.Itremainstobeseenwhethermenarealsomorelikelyaltruisticpunishersoffree­ridinggroupmembersduringintergroupconflict,astheMWHwouldpredict.

Malesarealsomorelikelytobechosenasgroupleadersduringintergroupcon­flict.VanVugtandSpisak(2008)foundthatwhentwoequallysuitablecandidatesofdifferentsexes,SarahandJohn,viedforthepositionofgroupleaderinaninter­groupconflictgroupspreferredthemaleleader(78%).Themaleleaderwasalsomoreeffectiveinelicitingfollowers’groupcontributionsduringintergroupthreat.(Interestingly,whentheproblemshiftedtowardconflictwithinthein­groupvirtu­allyallgroupspreferredthefemaleleader.)

Preference for Hierarchies

There is some evidence that male groups have different group dynamics thatmake them more suitable to engage in coalitional aggression. Whereas femalegroupsaremoreegalitarian,groupsofmalesformmorehierarchicalgroups,andthesehierarchiestendtobemorestableovertime.Thedifferenceingroupstruc­turecorrespondswithgenderdifferences in leadershipstyle(Eagly&Johnson,1990; Van Vugt, 2006). Military specialists assume that hierarchy formation isaneffectiveresponseindealingwithintergroupconflictthatrequiresanurgent,coordinatedresponse.

Researchondevelopmentaldifferencesinsocialplayreflectsthemalewarriortendencies.Boysplayinlargergroupsthangirlsandmoreoftenplaycomplexcom­petitiveteamgames,whichsometimesinvolvetheuseofweaponssuchastoygunsandswords(Geary,1998).Boysalsoputgreatersocialpressureonteammemberstoconformtogroupnormsduringplayactivities(Sherifetal.,1961),andtheyhavemoretransientfriendshipswithalargernumberofpeersthangirls(Geary).Thus,consistentwiththeMWH,menhavepsychologicalmechanismsthatenablethemtoworkinandfunctionbetterinlargerandmorehierarchicallystructuredgroupsandtheprimaryfunctionofsuchgroupstructuresistocompetewithothergroups.

Support for Tribal Politics

TheMWHfurtherpredictsgenderdifferencesinpoliticalattitudestowardinter­group conflict. We hypothesize that men would show relatively stronger politi­cal support forwarfareasa solution to internationalconflictbecause theyhavemoretogainpotentially(atleastinancestraltimes)fromintergroupconflict.Wetestedthispredictionusingdatafromarandomselectionof10recentnationaland

Page 259: the Psychology of Social Conflict and Aggression Sydney Symposium in Social Psychology

mark van vugt242

internationalopinionpollsthatwewereabletofindontheInternetandfoundcon­sistentgenderdifferences(sometimeslarge,othertimessmall,butalwaysinthesamedirection).Forinstance,aWashington Postpollin2003(N=1,030)askedthequestion,“DoyousupporttheUShavinggonetowarinIraq?”,towhich82%ofmenagreedversus72%ofwomen.Asanotherexample,arecentpollbyGallup News(N=7,074)foundthat46%ofmen(vs.37%ofwomen)disagreedwiththestatement,“DoyouthinktheIraqwarwasamistake?”

TheMWHalsoexpectsmentohaveastrongerpreferenceforbetween­groupdominance hierarchies, the inevitable outcome of intergroup conflict. To testthisprediction,weaskedaninternationalsurveyofpeopletocompletetheshort10­itemsocialdominanceorientationscale(Pratto,Sidanius,Stallworth,&Malle,1994).Thisseven­pointscalecontainsitemssuchas,“Somegroupsofpeoplearesimplyinferiortoothers”;“Weshoulddowhatwecantoequalizeconditionsfordifferentgroups”;and“Togetaheadinlife,itissometimesnecessarytosteponothergroups.”Consistentwithotherdata(Prattoetal.)wefoundthatmenweresignificantlymoresociallydominant(M=2.56,SD=1.13)thanwomen(M=2.28,SD=1.0).

Thus, in agreement with the MWH, men are generally more belligerent intheirtribalpolitics.

Tribal Social identity

AfinalpredictionfromtheMWHisthatmen’spersonalself­conceptshouldbeaffectedmorestronglybytheiraffiliationstotribalgroups.Incontrast,women’sself­concept should be influenced primarily by having meaningful connectionswithcloseothers.Menhave indeedamorecollectivesenseofself that ismorestrongly derived from their group memberships and affiliations (Baumeister &Sommer,1997).GabrielandGardner(1999)askedstudentstodescribethemselvesbycompletingthestatement,“Iam….”Theyfoundthatmalestudentsweretwiceaslikelytomakestatementsreferringtoatribalassociation(e.g.,“Iamamemberofafraternity”).

In a recent study (Van Vugt et al., 2007) we asked 100 people around theUniversityofKentcampustoindicatetheirfavoritecolorandtoexplainwhytheypickedthisparticularcolor.Amongmen,almost30%mentionedatribalassocia­tion(e.g.,theirfavoritefootballteam,thecolorsoftheflagoftheircountryofori­gin);noneofthewomendidso.

Thus, men’s social identity seems to be more strongly based on their tribalaffiliationsthanwomen’s,whichisconsistentwiththeMWH.

implicationS for intergroup relationSThis chapter presented a framework for studying the psychology of intergroupaggression from an evolutionary perspective. This analysis suggests that not allintergrouprelationsarealikebecausenotallout­groupsarealike.Howgroupsinter­actwitheachotherisdeterminedbythespecificcontextualthreatsandopportuni­ties.Whensuchchallengescorrespondtoevolutionarilyrelevantthreats—threats

Page 260: the Psychology of Social Conflict and Aggression Sydney Symposium in Social Psychology

the male Warrior hypotheSiS 243

thatweresignificantenoughinancestralsocialenvironments thathumanshaveevolvedtodealwiththem—theyactivateaspecifictribalpsychology.HereIhavearguedthatahistoryofcoalitionalaggressionhasproducedadistincthumantribalbrainincludinganinterrelatedsetoffunctionalcognitiveandbehavioralreactionstoattackanddefendagainstmembersofout­groups.Furthermore, as themostlikelyperpetratorsandvictims,Ihavehypothesizedthatthemalepsychologyhasbeenparticularlyaffectedbyintergroupconflictepisodesandhavedubbedthisthemalewarriorhypothesis. I reviewed the literatureongenderdifferences inintergrouppsychologyinlightofpredictionsfromthemalewarriorhypothesisandfoundthemtobegenerallysupportive.Furthertestsareneeded.

In addition to intergroup conflict there might be a host of other signifi­cantancestralchallengesinvolvingothergroups,whicharenotdiscussedhere.Disease avoidance is one such threat, and we would expect a different set offunctionalresponsestoacontagionthreatratherthanaphysicalthreatfromanout­group; for instance, behavioral avoidance rather than aggression. When adiseasethreatissalient,perhapswomenrespondmorestrongly.Thereissomeevidencethatwomenaremoreprejudicedtowardstrangerswhenintheirmostfertilemenstrualphase(Navarette,Fessler,&Eng,2007).Ingeneral,weknowvery littleabout the intergrouppsychologyof females.Inaddition, theneuro­scienceunderpinninggenderdifferences in intergrouppsychologyought tobeexamined—forinstance,whichhormonaldifferencesdrivethesegenderdiffer­encesintribalpsychology?

Theevolutionaryframeworkmakesvarioussuggestionsforinterventionstoimproveintergrouprelations.Whenout­groupsposeacoalitionalthreat,inter­ventions might be targeted specifically at male­to­male interactions becausetheyarethemost likelyperpetratorsandvictimsof intergroupaggression.Interms of their objectives, interventions will be particularly successful whentheyeliminatethesenseofthreatassociatedwithaparticularout­groupalto­gether.Attemptsmustbemadetoindividuatemembersofsuchout­groups,forinstance,byaccentuatingtheirpersonalachievementsratherthantheachieve­mentsoftheirgroup.Asecondaimof interventions istoaltertheperceptualcuesthatelicitthreatresponsestowardparticularout­groupssuchasnewimmi­grantgroups.Forinstance,language,dresscode,andparticularritualsorcus­tomsserveas tribalmarkers,and the lessnoticeable theyare themoretheseout­groupswillreceivepositivetreatment.Thus,forthesakeofattenuatingtheeffectsofcoalitionalpsychology, it is importantforsocietiestomake iteasierfornewimmigrantgroupstoadoptthelanguageandcustomsofthein­group.Third, interventionsmightbefocusedonchangingthe specificcognitiveandaffectiveresponsestowardout­groups.However,ifitistruethattheseresponsesare evolved, then the link between threat and response might be difficult toinhibitorextinguish(cf. fearofsnakesandspiders;Ohman&Mineka,2001).Nevertheless,wesuspect that frequentpositive interactionswithmembersofout­groupswill,overtime,reduceinitialaversionorhostility.Forinstance,theJigsawclassroomexperiments(Aronson&Bridgeman,1979)demonstratethatcooperativerelationsbetweenmembersofdifferentethnicgroupsareagoodmeansofreducingprejudice.

Page 261: the Psychology of Social Conflict and Aggression Sydney Symposium in Social Psychology

mark van vugt244

referenceS

Abrams,D.,Ando,K.,&Hinkle,S.(1998).Psychologicalattachmenttothegroup:Cross­culturaldifferencesinorganizationalidentificationandsubjectivenormsaspredictorsofworkers’turnoverintentions.Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 24,1027–1039.

Ackerman,J.M.,Shapiro,J.R.,Neuberg,S.L.,Kenrick,D.T.,Becker,D.V.,&Griskevicius,V.(2006).Theyalllookthesametome(unlessthey’reangry),Psychological Science, 17,836–840.

Archer, J. (2000). Sex differences in aggression between heterosexual partners: A meta­analyticreview.Psychological Bulletin, 126,651–680.

Aronson,E.,Blaney,N.,Stephan,C.,Sikes,J.,&Snapp,M.(1978).The jigsaw classroom.BeverlyHills,CA:Sage.

Aronson,E.,&Bridgeman,D.(1979).Jigsawgroupsandthedesegregatedclassroom:Inpursuitofcommongoals.Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 5,438–446.

Batson,C.D.,Sager,K.,Garst,E.,Kang,M., Rubchinsky,K.,& Dawson,K. (1997). Isempathy­induced helping due to self–other merging? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 73,495–509.

Baumeister,R.F.,&Sommer,K.L.(1997).Whatdomenwant?Genderdifferencesandtwospheresofbelongingness:CommentonCrossandMadson(1997).Psychological Bulletin, 122,38–44.

Becker,S.W.,&Eagly,A.H.(2004).Theheroismofwomenandmen.American Psychologist, 59,163–178.

Bornstein, G. (2003). Individual, group, and collective interests. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 7, 129­145.doi:10.1207/S15327957PSPR0702_129­145

Bornstein,G.(2006).Personalcommunication.Bowles,S.(2006).Groupcompetition,reproductivelevelling,andtheevolutionofhuman

altruism.Science, 314,1569–1572.Branscombe, N. R., & Wann, D. L. (1991). The positive social and self­concept conse­

quencesofsportsteamidentification.Journal of Sport & Social Issues, 15,115–127.Brewer,M.B.(1979).In­groupbiasintheminimalintergroupsituation:Acognitive–moti­

vationalanalysis.Psychological Bulletin, 86,307–324.Brewer,M.B.,&Brown,R.J.(1998).Intergrouprelations.InD.T.Gilbert,S.T.Fiske,&

G.Lindzey(Eds.),The handbook of social psychology(4thed.,pp.554–594).NewYork:McGraw­Hill.

Brewer,M.B.,&Campbell,D.T. (1976).Ethnocentrism and intergroup attitudes: East African evidence.NewYork:Sage.

Brewer, M. B., & Caporael, L. (2006). An evolutionary perspective on social identity:Revisitinggroups.InM.Schaller,J.A.Simpson,&D.T.Kenrick(Eds.),Evolution and social psychology(pp.143–161).NewYork:PsychologyPress.

Burnstein,E.,Crandall,C.,&Kitayama,S.(1994).Someneo­Darwiniandecisionrulesforaltruism:Weighingcuesforinclusivefitnessasafunctionofthebiologicalimportanceofthedecision.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 67,773–789.

Buss,D.M.(2005).Handbook of evolutionary psychology.Hoboken,NJ:Wiley.Buss,D.M.,&Schmitt,D.P.(1993).Sexualstrategiestheory:Anevolutionaryperspective

onhumanmating.Psychological Review, 100,204–232.Byrne,D.,&Whiten,A.(1988).Machiavellian intelligence: Social expertise and the evolu-

tion of intellect in monkeys, apes, and humans.NewYork:OxfordUniversityPress.Campbell A. (1999). Staying alive: Evolution, culture, and intra­female aggression.

Behavioural and Brain Sciences, 22,203–252.Campbell, D.T. (1965). Ethnocentric and other altruistic motives. In D. Levine, (Ed.),

Nebraska symposium on motivation.Lincoln:UniversityofNebraskaPress.

Page 262: the Psychology of Social Conflict and Aggression Sydney Symposium in Social Psychology

the male Warrior hypotheSiS 245

Chagnon,N.A.(1988).Lifehistories,bloodrevenge,andwarfare inatribalpopulation.Science, 239,985–992.

Choi,J.­K.,&Bowles,S.(2007).Thecoevolutionofparochialaltruismandwar.Science,318(5850),636–640.

Cialdini,R.B.,Brown,S.L.,Lewis,B.P.,Luce,C.,&Neuberg,S.L.(1997).Reinterpretingthe empathy­altruism relationship: When one into one equals oneness. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 73,481–494.

Cosmides,L.,&Tooby,J.(1992).Cognitiveadaptationsforsocialexchange.InJ.Barkowetal.(Eds.),The adapted mind: Evolutionary psychology and the generation of culture(pp.163–228).NewYork:OxfordUniversityPress.

Cottrell,C.A.,&Neuberg,S.L.(2005).Differentemotionalreactionstodifferentgroups:A sociofunctional threat­basedapproach to “prejudice.” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 88,770–789.

Daly,M.,&Wilson,M.(1988).Homicide.Hawthorne,NY:deGruyter.Darwin,C.(1871).The descent of man, and selection in relation to sex.London:Murray.Dawkins,R.(1976).The selfish gene.Oxford:OxfordUniversityPress.DeCremer,D.,&VanVugt,M.(1999).Socialidentificationeffectsinsocialdilemmas:A

transformationofmotives.European Journal of Social Psychology, 29,871–893.DeWaal,F.(2006).Our inner ape.London:GrantaBooks.Dovidio,J.F.,Piliavin,J.A.,Schroeder,D.A.,&Penner,L.A.(2006).The social psychology

of prosocial behavior.London:LawrenceErlbaum.Dunbar,R.(2004).The human story: A new history of mankind’s evolution.London:Faber

andFaber.Eagly, A. H., & Johnson, B. T. (1990). Gender and leadership style: A meta­analysis.

Psychological Bulletin, 108,233–256.Eagly,A.H.,&Wood,W.(1999).Theoriginsofsexdifferencesinhumanbehavior:Evolved

dispositionsversussocialroles.American Psychologist, 54,408–423.Faulkner,J.,Schaller,M.,Park,J.H.,&Duncan,L.A.(2004).Evolveddisease­avoidance

mechanismsandcontemporaryxenophobicattitudes.Group Processes and Intergroup Relations, 7,333–353.

Fehr,E.,&Gächter,S.(2002).Altruisticpunishmentinhumans.Nature, 415,137–140.Fiske,S.T.(2002).Whatweknownowaboutbiasandintergroupconflict,theproblemof

thecentury.Current Directions in Psychological Science, 11,123–128.Gabriel,S.,&Gardner,W.L. (1999).Are therehis andhers typesof interdependence?

Theimplicationsofgenderdifferencesincollectiveversusrelationalinterdependenceforaffect,behaviorandcognition.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 77,642–655.

Gaertner,S.L.,&Dovidio,J.F.(2000).Reducing intergroup bias: The common ingroup identity model.Philadelphia:PsychologyPress.

Geary,D.C.(1998).Male, female: The evolution of human sex differences.Washington,DC:AmericanPsychologicalAssociation.

Gerard,H.B.,&Hoyt,M.F.(1974).Distinctivenessofsocialcategorizationandattitudetowardingroupmembers.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 29,836–842

Goldstein,J.(2003).War and gender.Cambridge,UK:CambridgeUniversityPress.Goodall, J. (1986). The chimpanzees of Gombe: Patterns of behavior. Cambridge, MA:

HarvardUniversityPress.Hamilton,W.D.(1964).Thegeneticalevolutionofsocialbehaviour.Journal of Theoretical

Biology, 7,1–52.Hardy,C.L.,&VanVugt,M.(2006).Niceguysfinishfirst:Thecompetitivealtruismhypoth­

esis.Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 32,1402–1413.Haslam, N. (2006). Dehumanization: An integrative review. Personality and Social

Psychology Review, 10,252–264

Page 263: the Psychology of Social Conflict and Aggression Sydney Symposium in Social Psychology

mark van vugt246

Hewstone,M.,Rubin,M.,&Willis,H.(2002).Intergroupbias.Annual Review of Psychology, 53,575–604.

Insko,C.A.,Schopler, J., Graetz,K.A., Drigotas,S.M., Currey,D.P.,Smith, S.L., etal.(1994).Interindividual–intergroupdiscontinuityintheprisoner’sdilemmagame.Journal of Conflict Resolution, 38,87–116.

Insko,C.A.,Schopler,J.,Hoyle,R.H.,Dardis,G.J.,&Graetz,K.A.(1990).Individual–groupdiscontinuityasafunctionoffearandgreed.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 58,68–79.

Johnson,D.D.P.,McDermott,R.,Barrett,E.S.,Crowden,J.,Wrangham,R.,McIntyre,M.H.,etal.(2006).Overconfidenceinwarames:Experimentalevidenceonexpecta­tions,aggression,genderandtestosterone.Proceedings of the Royal Society B,273,2513–2520.

Judd,C.M.,&Park,B.(1988).Out­grouphomogeneity:Judgmentsofvariabilityattheindi­vidualandgrouplevels.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 54,778–788.

Keeley,L.(1996).War before civilization.NewYork:OxfordUniversityPress.Kenrick,D.T.,Li,N.P.,&Butner,J.(2003).Dynamicalevolutionarypsychology:Individual

decisionrulesandemergentsocialnorms.Psychological Review, 110,3–28.Kurzban,R.,&Leary,M.R.(2001).Evolutionaryoriginsofstigmatization:Thefunctionsof

socialexclusion.Psychological Bulletin, 127,187–208.Kurzban,R.,&Neuberg,S.(2005).Managingingroupandoutgrouprelationships.InD.

M.Buss(Ed.),The handbook of evolutionary psychology(pp.653–675).Hoboken,NJ:Wiley.

Kurzban,R.,Tooby,J.,&Cosmides,L.(2001). Canracebeerased?Coalitionalcomputa­tionandsocialcategorization.Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 98,15387–15392.

Leach, C. W., Spears, R., Branscombe, N. R., & Doosje, B. (2003). Malicious pleasure:Schadenfreudeat thesufferingofanothergroup.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 84,932–943.

Leunissen,J.,&VanVugt,M.(2010).I love the man in the uniform: Why women prefer male warriors.Unpublishedmanuscript,UniversityofKent.

Leyens,J.P.,Rodriquez­Perez,A.,Rodriguez­Torres,R.,Gaunt,R.,Paladino,M.,Vaes,J.,etal. (2001).Psychologicalessentialismand thedifferentialattributionofuniquelyhumanemotionstoingroupsandoutgroups.European Journal of Social Psychology, 31,395–411.

LivingstoneSmith,D.(2007).The most dangerous animal in the world: Human nature and the origins of war.NewYork:St.MartinsPress.

Maner,J.K.,Kenrick,D.T.,Becker,D.V.,Robertson,T.E.,Hofer,B.,Neuberg,S.L.,etal.(2005).Functionalprojection:Howfundamentalsocialmotivescanbiasinterpersonalperception.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 88,63–78.

Marques,J.M.,Yzerbyt,V.Y.,&Leyens,J.P.(1988).The“blacksheepeffect”:Extremityofjudgmentstowardsingroupmembersasafunctionofgroupidentification.European Journal of Social Psychology, 18,1–16.

Navarrete,C.,Fessler,D.,&Eng,S.(2007).Elevatedethnocentrisminthefirsttrimesterofpregnancy.Evolution and Human Behavior, 28,60–65.

Neuberg,S.L.,&Cottrell,C.A.(2006).Evolutionarybasesofprejudice.InM.Schaller,J.A.Simpson,&D.T.Kenrick(Eds.),Evolution and social psychology(pp.163–187).NewYork:PsychologyPress.

Öhman,A.,&Mineka,S.(2001).Fear,phobia,andpreparedness:Towardanevolvedmod­uleoffearandfearlearning.Psychological Review, 108,483–522.

Palmer,C.T.,&Tilley,C.F.(1995).Sexualaccesstofemalesasamotivationfor joininggangs:Anevolutionaryapproach.Journal of Sex Research, 32,213–217.

Page 264: the Psychology of Social Conflict and Aggression Sydney Symposium in Social Psychology

the male Warrior hypotheSiS 247

Pemberton,M.B.,Insko,C.A.,&Schopler,J.(1996).Memoryforandexperienceofdif­ferentialcompetitivebehaviorofindividualsandgroups.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 71,953–966.

Pratto, F., Sidanius, J., Stallworth, L. M., & Malle, B. F. (1994). Social dominance ori­entation: A personality variable predicting social and political attitudes. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 67,741–763.

Richerson, P., & Boyd, R. (2005). Not by genes alone: How culture transformed human evolution. Chicago:UniversityofChicagoPress.

Schaller, M. (2003). Ancestral environments and motivated social perception: Goal­likeblastsfromtheevolutionarypast.InS.J.Spencer,S.Fein,M.P.Zanna,&J.M.Olson(Eds.),Motivated social perception: The Ontario Symposium(pp.215–231).Mahwah,NJ:Erlbaum.

Schaller, M., & Abeysinghe, A. M. N. D. (2006). Geographical frame of reference anddangerous intergroup attitudes: A double­minority study in Sri Lanka. Political Psychology, 27,615–631.

Schaller,M.,&Neuberg,S.L.(inpress).Intergroupprejudicesandintergroupconflicts.InC.Crawford&D.L.Krebs(Eds.),Foundations of evolutionary psychology: Ideas, issues, and applications.Mahwah,NJ:Erlbaum.

Schaller,M.,Park,J.H.,&Faulkner,J.(2003).Prehistoricdangersandcontemporaryprej­udices.European Review of Social Psychology, 14,105–137.

Schaller,M.,Park,J.H.,&Kenrick,D.T.(2007).Humanevolutionandsocialcognition.InR.I.M.Dunbar&L.Barrett(Eds.),Oxford handbook of evolutionary psychology(pp.491–504).Oxford,UK:OxfordUniversityPress.

Schaller,M.,Park,J.H.,&Mueller,A.(2003).Fearofthedark:Interactiveeffectsofbeliefsabout danger and ambient darkness on ethnic stereotypes. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 29,637–649.

Sherif,M.,Harvey,O.J.,White,B.J.,Hood,W.R.,&Sherif,C.W.(1961).Intergroupcon­flictandcooperation:The Robbers Cave experiment.Norman,Oklahoma:UniversityBookExchange.

Sidanius,J.,Cling,B.J.,&Pratto,F.(1991).Rankingandlinkingasafunctionofsexandgenderroleattitudes.Journal of Social Issues, 47,131–149.

Sidanius,J.,&Pratto,F.(1999).Social dominance: An intergroup theory of social hierarchy and oppression.NewYork:CambridgeUniversityPress.

Tajfel,H.,&Turner, J.C. (1979).An integrative theoryof intergroupconflict. InW.G.Austin&S.Worchel(Eds.),The social psychology of intergroup relations(pp.33–47).Monterey,CA:Brooks/Cole.

Tajfel, H., & Turner, J. C. (1986). The social identity theory of intergroup behavior. InS. Worchel & W. G. Austin (Eds.), Psychology of intergroup relations (pp. 7–24).Chicago:Nelson­Hall.

Taylor,S.E.,Klein,L.C.,Lewis,B.P.,Gruenewald,R.A.R.,Gurung,R.A.,&Updegraff,J.A.(2000).Biobehavioralresponsestostressinfemales:Tend­and­befriendnotfight­or­flight. Psychological Review, 107, 411–429.

Tooby, J., & Cosmides, L. (1988). The evolution of war and its cognitive foundations(InstituteforEvolutionaryStudiesTech.Rep.No.88­1).PaloAlto,CA:InstituteforEvolutionaryStudies.

Trivers,R.L. (1971).Theevolutionof reciprocalaltruism.Quarterly Review of Biology, 46,35–57.

Trivers,R.L.(1972).Parentalinvestmentandsexualselection.InB.Campbell(Ed.),Sexual selection and the descent of man(pp.136–179).Chicago:Aldine.

Turner,J.C.,Hogg,M.A.,Oakes,P.,Reicher,S.,&Wetherell,M.(1987).Rediscovering the social group: A self-categorization theory.Oxford,UK:Blackwell.

Page 265: the Psychology of Social Conflict and Aggression Sydney Symposium in Social Psychology

mark van vugt248

VanVugt,M.(2006).Theevolutionaryoriginsofleadershipandfollowership.Personality and Social Psychology Review, 10,354–372.

VanVugt,M. (2009). Sexdifferences in intergroupcompetition, agreesion, andwarfare.AnnalsoftheNewYorkAcademyofSciences,1167,124–134.DOI:10.1111/j.1749­6632.2009.04539.x

VanVugt,M.,&Chang,K.(2008).Group reactions to loyal and disloyal members: The mod-erating role of member criticality.Unpublishedmanuscript,UniversityofKent.

VanVugt,M.,DeCremer,D.,&Janssen,D.P.(2007).Genderdifferencesincooperationandcompetition:Themale­warriorhypothesis.Psychological Science, 18,19–23.

VanVugt,M.,&Hart,C.M.(2004).Socialidentityassocialglue:Theoriginsofgrouploy­alty.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 86, 585–598.

Van Vugt, M., & Schaller, M. (2008). Evolutionary perspectives on group dynamics: Anintroduction.Group Dynamics,12, 1–6.

VanVugt,M., Snyder, M.,Tyler, T. R., & Biel, A. (Eds.). (2000).Cooperation in modern society: Promoting the welfare of communities, states and organizations. New York:Routledge.

VanVugt,M.,&Spisak,B.(2008).Sexdifferencesinleadershipemergenceduringcom­petitions within and between groups. Psychological Science, 19, 854–858. doi:10.1111/j.1467­9280.2008.02168.x

Van Vugt, M., & Van Lange, P.A.M. (2006). The altruism puzzle: Psychological adapta­tionsforprosocialbehavior:InM.Schaller,J.A.Simpson,&D.T.Kenrick(Eds.),Evolution and social psychology(pp.237–261).NewYork:PsychologyPress.

Wildschut,T.,Pinter,B.,Vevea,J.L.,Insko,C.A.,&Schopler,J.(2003).Beyondthegroupmind: A quantitative review of the interindividual–intergroup discontinuity effect.Psychological Bulletin, 129,698–722.

Wilson, E. O. (1975). Sociobiology: The new synthesis. Cambridge, MA: HarvardUniversityPress.

Wilson,D.S.,VanVugt,M.,&O’Gorman,R.(2008).Multilevelselectiontheoryandmajorevolutionarytransistions:Implicationsforpsychologicalscience.Current Directions in Psychological Science, 17, 6–9.

Wilson,M.,&Wrangham,R.(2003).Intergrouprelationsinchimpanzees.Annual Review of Anthropology, 32,363–392.

Wrangham,R.W.,&Peterson,D.(1996).Demonic males: Apes and the origins of human violence.Boston,MA:HoughtonMifflinCo.

Zdaniuk,B.,&Levine,J.M.(2001).Grouployalty:Impactofmembers’identificationandcontributions.Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 37,502–509.

Zimbardo,P.(1971).Thepowerandpathologyof imprisonment.Congressional Records, 15,10–25.

Page 266: the Psychology of Social Conflict and Aggression Sydney Symposium in Social Psychology

249

16ImplicationsofGlobalClimate

ChangeforViolenceinDevelopedandDevelopingCountries

CRAIGA.ANDERSONandMATTDELISIIowa State University

R apidglobalclimatechange,takingplaceoverdecadesratherthanmillen­nia, is a fact of twenty­first­century life. Human activity, especially thereleaseofgreenhousegases,has initiatedageneralwarmingtrend.The

10warmestyearsonrecordbetween1880and2008werethelast10.Thistrendisexpectedtocontinueuntiltheatmosphericcompositionreturnstoapreindustrial­eranorm.

Climate change effects on specific regions are expected to vary consider­ably.Thoughmostpartsoftheglobearewarming,afewplacesmayexperiencecooler climates asoceanandwindcurrents shift.Some regions areexperienc­ing increased rainfall,whereasmanyothers arehavingprolonged droughts. In2007,theIntergovernmentalPanelonClimateChange(IPCC)releasedareportthat includednumerousprojectionsof likelyeffectsby theendof thiscentury,under varying assumptions of how world governments, industries, and peoplerespond.Thebest­casescenarioassumeshugereductionsinnetgreenhousegasproduction,beginningalmostimmediately.Inthisscenario,climatemodelspre­dict an average global temperature increase of 1.8°C (3.24°F) and an averagesea­levelincreaseof28cm(11inches).Theworst­casescenario,whichassumesabusiness­as­usualapproach,predictsincreasesof4.0°C(7.2°F)and43cm(17inches).Otherprojections,someofwhichhavealreadybecomeapparent,includeincreases in heat waves and heavy precipitation; decreases in precipitation insubtropical areas; and increases in tropical cyclones. More specific projectionsinclude(1)5–8%increaseintheproportionofAfricathatisaridandsemiarid;(2)majorfloodingofheavilypopulatedareasofAsia fromrisingsea levelsand

Page 267: the Psychology of Social Conflict and Aggression Sydney Symposium in Social Psychology

craig a. anderSon and matt deliSi250

storms;(3)inundationoflow­lyingislands;(4)severewatershortagesinAustraliaandNewZealand;(5)drought insouthernEurope; (6)decreasedsoilmoistureandfoodcropsinLatinAmerica;and(7)increasedwinterfloodingandsummerheatwavesinNorthAmerica.MorerecentresearchbeingpreparedforthenextIPCCreportsuggeststhatthenewbest­casescenariowillbeworsethantheoldworst­casescenario,withsealevelsrisingaleast1meter(Vermeer&Rahmstorf,2009).Because13%oftheworld’spopulation—hundredsofmillionsofpeople—live in low­lying coastal areas (Engelman,2009,p.41), this latterprojection isparticularlydisturbing.Indonesiamayloseasmanyas2,000smallislandsinthenext20yearstorisingsealevels(Engelman,p.3).

In addition to the changes in average temperature and rainfall, climatemodelsalsopredictanincreaseinextremeweatherevents.Recentdatasuggestthat this increase has already begun, with dramatic increases in floods, windstorms,anddroughtdisastersinthelast20years(Engelman,2009,pp.16,30).Hurricanes,cyclones,andothertropicalstormsalsoareincreasinginintensity.Theproblemwithrisingsealevelsconcernsnotjusttheheightofhightidesbutalso stormsurge.Aonce­a­century storm inNewYorkCity, for example,willoccuraboutonceevery3years(Rahmstorf,2009)ifaveragesealevelincreasesby1meter.

Researchfrompsychology,sociology,politicalscience,economics,history,andgeographysuggestthatrapidglobalwarmingcanincreasetheincidenceofviolentbehaviorinatleastthreeways.Oneinvolvesdirecteffectsofuncomfortablywarmtemperatureson irritability,aggression,andviolence.Asecondinvolves indirecteffectsofglobalwarmingonfactorsthatputchildrenandadolescentsatriskfordevelopingintoviolence­proneadults.Thethirdinvolvesindirecteffectsofrapidclimate change on populations whose livelihoods and survival are suddenly atrisk,effectsthatinfluenceeconomicandpoliticalstability,migration,andviolentintergroupconflict.Forexample,variousgovernmentalandscientificreportshavenotedthatclimatechangehasexacerbatedexistingtensionsandconflictscenteredintheDarfurregionofSudanandinBangladesh.

Heat and Aggression

Much research has established that uncomfortably warm temperatures canincreasethelikelihoodofphysicalaggressionandviolence(Anderson&Anderson,1998;Anderson,Anderson,Dorr,DeNeve,&Flanagan,2000;foraconcisereviewseeAnderson,2001).Threetypesofstudieshavetestedandfoundconsiderablesupportforthisheathypothesis:experimentalstudies,geographicregionstudies,andtimeperiodstudies.

experimental Studies of the heat effect Earlyexperimentalstudiesofheateffectsyieldedconsiderable inconsistency inoutcomes,perhapsbecauseofparticipantsuspicionandmeasurementissues.Laterstudiesprovidedbettertestsandcleanerresults.Forexample,Vrij,vanderSteen,andKoppelaar(1994)con­ductedafieldexperimentinwhichDutchpoliceofficerswererandomlyassignedtoperformatrainingsessioninvolvingasimulatedburglaryunderhotorcomfortable

Page 268: the Psychology of Social Conflict and Aggression Sydney Symposium in Social Psychology

implicationS of gloBal climate change 251

conditions.Officersinthehotconditionreportedmoreaggressiveandthreateningimpressionsofthesuspectandweremorelikelytodrawtheirweaponandtoshootthesimulatedsuspect.

Anderson et al. (2000) reported a series of laboratory experiments on bothhotandcoldtemperatureeffects.Inseparateexperiments,uncomfortablywarmtemperatures(relativetocomfortabletemperature)increasedparticipants’feelingsof anger and hostility, their perceptions of hostility in observed dyadic interac­tions,andtheirinitialretaliatoryaggressivebehavioragainstapersonwhosepriorharmfulbehaviorwasofanambiguousnature.RecentexperimentsbyWilkowski,Meier,Robinson,Carter,&Feltman(2009)linkedheat­relatedimagerytoahostofangerandaggression­relatedperceptionsandjudgments.

Geographic Region Studies of the Heat Effect

Studiesdatingbacktothenineteenthcenturysuggestthathotterregionshavehigherviolentcrimeratesthancoolerregions(Anderson,1989).However,evenwithinthesamecountryregionsdifferinmanywaysotherthanclimate.Someoftheseotherdifferences(e.g.,poverty,unemployment,agedistribution,culture)areriskfactorsforviolence.Thebestgeographicregionstudiesincludestatisticalcontrolsforsuchfactors.Evenwhensuchfactorsarecontrolled,temperaturepredictsviolentcrimerates.Forexample,hotterU.S.citieshavehigherviolenceratesthancoolercities,evenafterstatisticallycontrollingfor14riskfactorsincludingage,education,race,economic, and culture of violence factors (Anderson & Anderson, 1996). RecentworkbyVandeVliert(2009;inpress;underreview)furthersuggeststhatclimateandeconomicconditionsjointlyinfluencecultureinwaysthatencourageordiscourageaggressionandviolence.Particularlyvulnerablearepopulationsthatliveinregionsthatarebothclimaticallychallenging(hot,cold,orboth)andimpoverished.

time period Studies of the heat effect “Timeperiod”studiescompareaggressionrateswithinthesameregionbutacrosstimeperiodsthatdifferintem­perature.Studiesvaryconsiderablyintermsofthetimeperiodsforwhichviolenceandtemperatureareassessed.Overall,resultsareremarkablyconsistent.Hottertimeperiods (e.g.,days, seasons,years) areassociatedwithhigher levelsof vio­lence.Forexample,riotsintheUnitedStatesarerelativelymorelikelyonhotterthancoolerdays(Carlsmith&Anderson,1979).Similarly,violentcrimesacrossawiderangeofcountriesandmeasuresoccurmorefrequentlyduringhotterseasonsthancoolerones(Anderson,1989).

Ofcourse,otherviolence­relatedfactorsmaydifferbetweenhotterversuscoolertimeperiods,evenwithinthesameregion.Forexample,intheUnitedStateslargenumbersofyouthareoutofschoolduringthesummermonths,soonecouldarguethattheroutineactivitiesofthepopulationmightaccountforseasonaldifferencesinviolence.Severalstudieshaveaddressedthisandotheralternativeexplanationsofheat­relatedtimeperiodeffects.Althoughit isclearthatroutineactivitiesdoinfluenceaggressivebehavior,itisalsoclearthatsuchalternativeexplanationsdonotparsimoniouslyaccountformanyobservedeffects.Forexample,intwostud­iesAndersonandAnderson(1984)foundsignificantday­of­weekeffectsondaily

Page 269: the Psychology of Social Conflict and Aggression Sydney Symposium in Social Psychology

craig a. anderSon and matt deliSi252

violentcrimerates,inadditiontoheateffects.Othertime­relatedroutineactivities,suchasyouthbeingoutofschoolinthesummer,cannotaccountfortheheateffectfoundinStudy1(Chicago),becausethatstudyincludedonlythesummermonths.Similarly,routineactivitytheorycannotaccountforthefindingthatMajorLeagueBaseballpitchersaremorelikelytohitbatterswithapitchedballonhotdaysthanoncooldays,evenafterstatisticallycontrollingforthepossibilityofsweatinfluenc­ingthepitcher’scontrol(Reifman,Larrick,&Fein,1991).

Differences in violent crime rates for hotter versus cooler days have beenfoundwithincitiesasvariedasHouston,Chicago,andMinneapolis(Anderson&Anderson,1984,1998).Evenaftercontrollingforroutineactivityeffectsoftimeofdayanddayofweek,violentcrimesarerelativelymorefrequentinhotterweather(e.g.,Anderson&Anderson,1998;Bushman,Wang,&Anderson,2005a,2005b).Interestingly, nonviolent crime (burglary, motor vehicle theft) rates are largelyunrelatedtoheat.

Whenthetimeperiodisyears(insteadofdaysorseasons),thekindsofpoten­tiallyconfoundedvariableschange.Forexample,U.S.youthareoutofschoolinthesummerregardlessofwhethertheyearisslightlywarmerorcooler.Whencon­sideringyear­basedstudiesandglobalwarmingeffects,onemightbeconcernedaboutwhetheraggression­relatedfactorssuchasagedistribution(e.g.,proportionofthepopulationthatisinthehigh­crimeagerange)andincomeinequality(e.g.,LaFree&Drass,1996)mightbeconfoundedwithtimeorsystematictemperaturechanges.Weconductedtwonewstudiestoexaminetheeffectsofyearlychangesin temperatureonviolentandnonviolentcrime in theUnitedStates,beginningwith1950.

Study 1: Hot Years and Violent Crime

method

Data ThisstudyextendsAnderson,Bushman,andGroom’s(1997)Study1.Majoradditionsare13yearsofnewdataandseveralaggression­relatedcontrolvariables.Datafortheyears1950–2008wereobtainedfromU.S.governmentsources.FromtheFBI’sUniformCrimeReportswecreatedtwocrimemeasures.Violent crimewasdefinedasthesumofthehomicideandassaultratesper100,000population.Nonviolent crimewasdefinedasthesumoftheburglaryandmotorvehicletheftratesper100,000population.1

Theprimarypredictor variable, annual average temperature,was computedfromdata fromtheNationalOceanicandAtmosphericAdministration.Controlvariableswere year, age (proportionof the population in the 15–29 high­crimeagerange),prison(numberofincarceratedstateandfederalinmatesper100,000population),poverty (percentof families livingbelow thepoverty line), and theGiniindexofincomedistributioninequality(perfectlyequaldistributionyieldsa

1 Asinpriorstudies,robberyandrapewereexcludedfortheoreticalreasons.Bothappeartohaveagreatermixtureofaggressivemotives(intenttoharm)andnonaggressivemotives.SeeAndersonetal.,1997.

Page 270: the Psychology of Social Conflict and Aggression Sydney Symposium in Social Psychology

implicationS of gloBal climate change 253

Giniindexof0,perfectinequality=1.0).Yeareffectsmightreflectahostofcul­turalandpopulationchanges,suchasincreasedreportingofassaultsandimprove­mentsintraumacare.Theothercontrolvariableshaveobvioustheoreticallinkstoviolence.

Correlated Residuals Time­seriesdataoftenhaveaprobleminwhichtheresidu­alsarecorrelatedwithtime.Themostcommonversioniswhentheresidualsatanygiventimeperiod(T)arecorrelatedwiththeresidualsatthesubsequenttimeperiod(T+1).Such“autocorrelations”makeordinaryleastsquares(OLS)proce­duresinappropriateforestimatingregressionparameters.Withasufficientlylargesample of time periods, autoregression (AR) techniques can be used to reduceoreliminateautocorrelationsamongresidualsandcanthusyieldmoreaccurateresults.Inallregressionanalyses,chi­squaretests(Ljung&Box,1978)wereusedto assess goodness of model fit regarding the presence of correlated residuals.Whenthechi­squarestatisticsuggestedthatthemodelprovidedapoorfittothedata,autoregressiveparameterswereadded.Thisprocesswasiterativelyrepeateduntilthechi­squareteststatisticindicatednonsignificantautocorrelations inthenewresiduals.

Thepresentstudyaddressesfivealternativeexplanationsforheat­relatedtimeperiodeffectsonviolentbehavior:(1)seasonalfluctuations;(2)correlatedresiduals;(3)coincidentalcrime,year,andglobalwarmingtrends;(4)coincidentalagedistri­butionshifts;and(5)coincidentalincomeandpovertyshifts.Thefirstalternativeexplanation isdealtwithbyusing the year as theunit of analysis.The remain­ingalternativesarehandledbystatisticalcontrols.Nonviolentcrimeanalysesareincludedasapointofcomparison.

results Table  16.1 presents descriptive statistics and zero­order correlationsamongthevariables.Averageannualtemperaturehasincreasedduringthis59­yearperiod(r =.54).Notethesubstantialzero­ordercorrelationsamongviolentcrime,temperature,andyear.Thissuggeststhatinadditiontocheckingforautocorrelatedresidualsaconservativestatisticalprocedurewouldalsocontrol foryeareffects.Finally,notethatnonviolentcrimewasnotstronglycorrelatedwithtemperature.

Table 16.2presentstheresultsofOLSandARanalysesonviolentcrime(topsection)andnonviolentcrime(bottomsection).OLSregressionrevealeda largeeffectoftemperatureonviolentcrime;each1°Fincreaseinaverageannualtemper­aturewasassociatedwith79moreseriousanddeadlyassaultsper100,000people.However,theARtestrevealedsignificantautocorrelationsamongtheresiduals(χ2

(6)=196,p<.05).WeaddedARparameterstothemodeluntiltheautocorrelationtestbecamenonsignificant(threeparameterswereneeded).Thisgreatlyreducedthesloperelatingtemperaturetoviolentcrime,butthisheateffectremainedsta­tisticallyandpracticallysignificant.Inthenextstepwecontrolledforyear.Thetemperature effect on violent crime remained essentially unchanged. The yeareffectalsowassignificant;eachyearadded4.90violentcrimesper100,000people.Weexaminedahostofmodelswiththeothercontrolvariables(age,prisonrate,poverty,Gini).Onlyprisonrateyieldedasignificanteffect.Withthreeautoregres­siveparameters—temperature,year,andprison—in themodel thetemperature

Page 271: the Psychology of Social Conflict and Aggression Sydney Symposium in Social Psychology

craig a. anderSon and matt deliSi254

taBle 16.2 destructive testing results using auto-regressive parameters and competitor variables, Study 1, 1950–2008

Violenta AR Test Temperature Effect

Model χ2 df b SE t

OLS 196* 6 79 19.1 4.15* Year Effect

AR­3 3.80 3 4.11 1.25 3.30* b SE t Prison Effect

AR­3 4.07 3 4.16 1.25 3.33* 4.90 1.68 2.91* b SE t

AR­3 3.27 3 4.19 1.21 3.47* 8.34 1.88 4.43* ­.42 .203 ­2.07*

NonViob AR Test Age EffectModel χ2 df b SE t

OLS 186* 6 16,646 1778 9.37*

AR­3 4.85 3 9645 3321 2.90*

Temp,annualaveragetemperature.Age,proportionofU.S.population in the15–29agerange.AR,autoregression.OLS,ordinaryleastsquares.

a Seriousanddeadlyassault:Assault+homicide.b Nonviolentcrime:Burglary+motorvehicletheft.* p<.05.+p<.10ift>1.67.

taBle 16.1 correlations among the predictor and outcome variables, Study 1, 1950–2008

Year Temp Age Prison Pov Gini Vio NVio

Year 1.00 0.54 0.04 0.90 –0.75 0.88 0.87 0.52Temp 0.54 1.00 –0.19 0.61 –0.18 0.61 0.48 0.11Age 0.04 –0.19 1.00 –0.36 –0.44 –0.30 0.25 0.78Prison 0.90 0.61 –0.36 1.00 –0.43 0.97 0.69 0.13Pov –0.75 –0.18 –0.44 –0.43 1.00 –0.36 –0.71 –0.79Gini 0.88 0.61 –0.30 0.97 –0.36 1.00 0.72 0.16Vio 0.87 0.48 0.25 0.69 –0.71 0.72 1.00 0.75Nvio 0.52 0.11 0.78 0.13 –0.79 0.16 0.75 1.00

Descriptive StatisticsMean 1979 57.85 0.227 231 16.1 0.382 239.1 1300St.Dev. 17.2 0.78 0.025 153 5.6 0.027 127.7 544Min. 1950 56.60 0.195 93 11.1 0.348 55.7 385Max. 2008 59.70 0.272 512 32.5 0.432 451.3 2163

Notes:N=59.Ifr>.25thenp<.05.Temp,annualaveragetemperature.Age,proportionofU.S.population in the 15–29 age range. Prison, number of incarcerated state and federalinmatesper 100,000population.Pov,percent of families livingbelow thepoverty line.Gini,indexofincomedistributionequality(perfectlyequaldistributionyieldsaGiniindexof0;perfectinequality=1.0).Vi,seriousanddeadlyassaultsper100,000population.Nvio,burglariesandmotorvehicletheftsper100,000population.

Page 272: the Psychology of Social Conflict and Aggression Sydney Symposium in Social Psychology

implicationS of gloBal climate change 255

effect remainedsignificant (b=4.19).Finally, thegreater theproportionof theU.S.populationthatwasimprisoned,thesmallertheviolentcrimerate.

AhostofOLSandARmodelsonnonviolentcrimedidnotyieldasinglesig­nificanttemperatureeffect.Indeed,afterappropriateARparameterswereinthemodel,onlyagewasasignificantpredictorofnonviolentcrimerates.Forevery1%increaseintheproportionofhigh­crimeageindividualsinthepopulation,therewasanincreaseof96nonviolentcrimesper100,000people.

Study 2: Hot Summers and Violent Crime

method

Data This study extends Anderson et al.’s (1997) Study 2. It examines violentcrimeinthesummermonthsintheUnitedStatesrelativetononsummermonths.Majoradditionsare9yearsofnewdataandseveralaggression­relatedcontrolvari­ables.Datafortheyears1950–2004werecollectedfromnumerousgovernmentalsources.Seasonaldatawereunavailableafter2004.

Basically,thedatasetisthesameasforthepriorstudy,withtwomajorexcep­tions. First, the outcome variable is the difference between the percent of theyear’scrimesthatwerecommittedduringthesummermonths(June,July,August)andtheaverageoftheotherthreeseasons,adjustedfornumberofdaysineachseason.Ifviolentcrimeswereequallylikelytooccurregardlessofseason,thenthesummermonthswouldaccountforexactly25%ofthem,andthesummereffectscoreusedinthisstudywouldbezero.Ifviolentcrimeswererelativelymore(less)likelyinthesummermonths,thesummereffectwouldbegreater(less)thanzero.Asimilarsummereffectwascomputedfornonviolentcrimes.

The second major difference from Study 1 was the temperature measure.Acrossasampleofcities,werecordedthenumberofhotdays(maximumtempera­turewas≥90°F)peryear.ThevastmajorityofhotdaysinthecontinentalUnitedStatesoccurduringthesummermonths,sothismeasureisagoodindicatorofthehotnessofeachofthe55summers.

Predictions Weexpectedthesummereffectonviolentcrimetobesignificantlygreaterthanzero,whenaveragedacrossyears.Furthermore,weexpectedyearswithmorehotdaystoyieldlargersummereffectsonviolentcrimethanyearswithfewerhotdays.

results Asexpected,theaveragesummereffectonviolentcrimewassignifi­cantly greater than zero (M = 2.57, t(54) = 18.52, p < .05). Violent crimes areoverrepresentedinthesummermonths.Infact,inonly2ofthe55yearswasthesummereffectnegative.

Concerningthesecondhypothesis,therewasnoevidenceofautocorrelationsamongtheresidualsinanyoftheanalysesofviolentcrime,soOLSanalyseswereappropriate.Theonlyvariablethatsignificantlypredictedthesizeofthesummereffectonviolentcrimewasthenumberofhotdays(b=.068,t(53)=3.07,p <.05).Noneofthecontrolvariables(includingyear)hadasignificanteffect,nordidthey

Page 273: the Psychology of Social Conflict and Aggression Sydney Symposium in Social Psychology

craig a. anderSon and matt deliSi256

substantially reducethesizeof thehotdayseffect.Nonviolentcrimewasunaf­fectedbynumberofhotdays.

General Discussion of the Heat Effect on Aggression

Insum,theheathypothesishasbeenrepeatedlyconfirmed.Laboratorystudiessuggestthatthisislargelytheresultofheat­inducedincreasesinirritabilityandinhostileinterpersonalperceptionbiases.Thereisadditionalevidencethattheseeffectscanbefurthertracedtothermoregulationandemotionregulationareasof thebrain (Anderson, 1989; Boyanowsky,1999, 2008;Boyanowsky,Calvert­Boyanowsky,Young,&Brideau,1981).Theimplicationforglobalwarmingisthatat the level of the individualperson increasedexposure to uncomfortably hottemperatureswillincreasethelikelihoodofinterpersonalconflictandviolence.Itisdifficulttoestimatewithconfidencehowbiganimpactglobalwarmingwillhaveonviolentcrimeinmodernsocieties,butFigure 16.1providessomeroughestimatesbasedontheresultsofStudy1.IfaverageannualtemperatureintheUnitedStates increasesby8°F (4.4°C), thebest estimate of the effecton thetotalmurderandassaultrateisanincreaseofabout34per100,000people,orover100,000moresuchseriousanddeadlyassaultsperyearinapopulationof305million.

Oneresponsetohighheat in industrializedcountries is increaseduseofairconditioning in buildings, cars, buses, and trains. Although such actions mightmitigate heat­induced increases in aggression, they increase the production ofgreenhousegases.

Therearenocomparabledaily,seasonal,orannualdataontheheateffectonviolentcrimein lessdevelopedcountries.However,thefindingssummarizedinprevioussectionssuggest thatuncomfortablyhot temperaturescanhavea fairlydirecteffectonaggressiveandviolenttendencies,perhapsthroughneuroandhor­monalpathwaysthatarecommontothermoregulationandemotion(seeChapter9inthisvolume).

84.4

°F°C

63.3

b =5.40 +1 S.E.b =4.19 Best Estimateb =2.98 +1 S.E.

42.2

21.1

0

Incr

ease

in M

urde

r/A

ssau

lt Ra

te

Increase in Average Annual Temperature

Increase in # of Murder/A

ssaults

50

40

30

20

10

0

140,000

120,000

100,000

80,000

60,000

40,000

20,000

0

figure 16.1 HeateffectonviolentcrimeintheUnitedStates,1950–2008.

Page 274: the Psychology of Social Conflict and Aggression Sydney Symposium in Social Psychology

implicationS of gloBal climate change 257

development of violence-prone individualSGlobalclimatechangewill likely increase theproportionofchildrenandyouthexposedtoriskfactorsknowntoincreasethelikelihoodofbecomingaviolence­prone individual—someonewho frequentlyusesphysicalaggressionor violencetodealwithconflict,togetdesiredresources,andtoimpulsivelyandshortsight­edlysatisfyone’swants(Gottfredson&Hirschi,1990;Moffitt,1993).Studiesofviolent youth and criminals reveal a host of psychological, neuropsychological,genetic,andenvironmentalriskfactorsthatplayamajorroleindeterminingwhobecomesaviolence­proneperson.Theseinterrelatedriskfactorsincludemalegen­der;stronglyheritableantisocialtraitsincludingimpulsivity,sensationseeking,lowintelligence,andpoorself­regulation;poverty;poorprenatalandchildhoodnutri­tion;familialdysfunction;growingupinviolentneighborhoods;psychopathy;loweducation;anddisorganizedandunstableneighborhoods(DeLisi,2005).

Food, Violence, and Antisocial Behavior

Potentiallyoneof themostcatastrophiceffectsof rapid climatechangecentersonfoodavailability.Today,oneineightU.S.householdswithinfantsisfoodinse­cure—the family has limited or uncertain availability of nutritionally adequateandsafefoods.Inmanypartsoftheworld,foodinsecurityisamuchlargerprob­lem.Thismeansthatarobustproportionofimpoverishedchildren(notwithstand­ing themultifaceted independenteffectsofpovertyonantisocialbehavior) facethespecterofpoornutritionormalnutrition—conditionswithsevere long­termconsequencesforcrimeandviolence.Arecentstudyisillustrative.JianghongLiuandcolleaguesexaminedthelongitudinalrelationshipbetweenmalnutritionandsubsequentexternalizingandantisocialbehaviorsusingabirthcohortofchildrenfromtheislandofMauritius,offthecoastofAfrica.Childrenwhoweremalnour­ishedatage3weresignificantlymoreaggressiveandhyperactiveatage8,moreaggressiveandpronetoexternalizing(actingout)behaviorsatage11,andmorehyperactiveandmorelikelytoexhibitsymptomsofconductdisorderatage17(Liu,Raine,Venables,&Mednick,2004).

It isnotmerely armchair conjecture to assert that foodscarcitywill result inincreased violence­prone individuals; history has already told such a story. FromOctober1944toMay1945,residentsofthewesternNetherlandsexperiencedmod­erate to severe food scarcity caused by a German army blockade. Over 100,000Dutchmenbornbetween1944and1946were studied toexamine theeffectsofgestationalnutritionaldeficiencyonsubsequentpronenesstoviolence(Neugebauer,Hoek,&Susser,1999).Menexposedtoseverematernalnutritionaldeficiencydur­ingthefirstandsecondtrimesterswere2.5timesmorelikelythanmennotexposedtoseverematernalnutritionaldeficiencytodevelopantisocialpersonalitydisorder,apsychiatricdiagnosischaracterizedbyrecurrentuseofviolenceandotherantiso­cialbehaviors.Otherstudies linkingpovertytopoordevelopmentaloutcomesarereviewedbyHustonandBentley(2010).Similarly,recentworkbyChen,Cohen,andMiller(2010)revealsthatpovertyeffectsonchildren’sstresslevels(assessedbycorti­sol)isexacerbatedbyperceivedthreatandbychaosintheirdailylivingconditions.

Page 275: the Psychology of Social Conflict and Aggression Sydney Symposium in Social Psychology

craig a. anderSon and matt deliSi258

Environmental–Genetic Interplay

Childreninregionsoffamine,prolongeddroughts,civilunrest,andwars(seenextsection)areexposedtomanyknownriskfactorsforthedevelopmentofviolence­prone adolescents and adults. Longitudinal studies have shown that even fairlybriefexposures(e.g.,afewmonths)tosomeoftheseriskfactorscanputtheindi­vidualchild(orfetus)onahigh­riskdevelopmentaltrajectory.

Caspiandcolleagues(2002)examinedtheinteractionbetweenmonoamineoxi­daseA(MAOA)—anenzymaticdegraderthatmodulatesneurotransmitters—andchildhoodmaltreatmentonlaterantisocialoutcomes.Forallantisocialoutcomes,theassociationbetweenmaltreatmentandantisocialbehaviorwasconditionalontheMAOAgenotype.Just12%ofthesamplehadboththegeneticrisk(low­activityMAOAlevels)andmaltreatment;theyaccountedfor44%ofthetotalconvictionsfor violent crime.Moreover, 85%of thosewhohadboth risk factorsdevelopedsomeformofantisocialbehavior.Intheabsenceofmaltreatment,thegenotypicriskfactordidnotmanifestitselfbehaviorally.Similargene–environmentinterac­tionshavebeenfoundforearlylife,environmentaladversity,andpsychiatricout­comes(Caspietal.,2003;Uher&McGuffin,2010).

If global warming brings about a world of dramatically increased environ­mentalriskandanunknownnumberofenvironmentalpathogens,thenitislikelythataproportionalproliferationofbehavioralriskswillresultasthesepathogenicenvironmentsmoderategeneticandneuropsychological riskswithinindividuals.Recalltheperniciousandlong­termeffectsofmalnutritionandviolentandanti­social behavior. Malnutrition, particularly when it is endured during gestation,causesahostofneuropsychologicaldeficitsrelatingtoneuronalreduction,braintoxicity,alteredneurotransmission,andotherphysiologicaleffects.Theseneurop­sychological deficits also interact with genes to predict antisocial behavior. Forexample,Beaver,DeLisi,Vaughn,andWright(2010)foundthatneuropsychologi­caldeficits(suchasthoseimplicatedbyprenatalnutritionaldeficiency)interactedwiththelow­activitypolymorphismintheMAOAgenetopredictviolentbehavior,delinquency,andlowself­controlacrosstwotimeperiods.

Terrorism Susceptibility

Recentresearchintoterrorismandsuicidebombershasledtoabetterunder­standingofthesocialandenvironmentalconditionsthatareconducivetothedevelopment of individuals willing to use such extreme tactics (Kruglanski,Chen,Dechesne,Fishman,&Orehek,2009;seealsoChapter10 in thisvol­ume).Briefly,theseresearchershaveshownthatsuchextremelyviolenttacticscan emerge from a “quest for personal significance,” triggered by failure tosatisfybasichumanmotivestobelongtoasignificantgroupandtocontributetoitswelfare.Avarietyofeventscanleadtofeelingsoffailureandexclusion,events such as personal trauma, loss of family through violence, and socialhumiliation.Undertheright(orwrong)circumstances,including(1)anavail­ableideologytojustifyviolenceagainsttheperceivedperpetratorsoftrauma,humiliation, and violent loss; and (2) social pressures to engage in violence

Page 276: the Psychology of Social Conflict and Aggression Sydney Symposium in Social Psychology

implicationS of gloBal climate change 259

againsttheperpetratorsasameansofgainingorrestoringone’sownsignifi­cance toone’sgroup,evensuicidal terrorismbecomesa viableoption to theindividual(seeChapter3inthisvolumeforarelateddiscussionofaggressionandostracism).

civil unreSt, ecomigration, genocide, and WarBoththeheateffectandthedevelopmentofviolence­proneindividualsfocusonviolenceattheindividuallevel.Thisthirdlinkbetweenclimatechangeandvio­lencefocusesonlargergroupsofpeople—communities,tribesorclans,societies,andcountries.Thisisaparticularlycomplexsetofphenomena.Emergingresearchfromseveralfieldssuggests thatrapidclimatechange(heatingorcooling)oftenleadstoincreasesinviolence.Thereareseveralwaysthiscanhappen.Forexample,insubsistenceeconomiesrapidchangesinclimateleadtoadecreasedavailabilityof food,water, and shelter.Dependingon the levelof social–political organiza­tion,suchshortagescanleadtocivilunrestandcivilwar,tomigrationtoadjacentregions and conflict with the people who already live in that region, and eventogenocideandwar.AlthoughitwouldbeoverlysimplistictoblamethebloodyconflictsinAfricaandAsiaduringthelattertwentiethandthisfirstdecadeofthetwenty­firstcenturyonclimatechangeandenvironmentaldisasters,italsowouldbeincorrecttoignoretheroleplayedbytheeconomichardships(includingstarva­tion)wroughtby theprolongeddroughtsandresultingresourceshortages.Civilunrest,revolutions,andwarsrequirerecruitsandleaderswhoarewillingtoriskmuchtogainvaluableresources.

Case Studies

Historicalresearchshowsthatenvironmentaldisasters,manylinkedtorelativelyrapidclimatechanges,canleadtoincreasesingroup­levelviolence.Ofcourse,not all environmental disasters are caused by climate change. For example,earthquakes,tsunamis,andvolcanoescananddocauseenvironmentaldisastersbut are not directly related to climate change. However, floods due to exces­siverainfallormeltingglaciers,droughts,hurricanes,andcyclonesareclimate­changerelated.

Thissectionconcernswhetherenvironmentaldisastersincreaseviolenceratesandseverity,regardlessofwhethertheenvironmentaldisasterwasthedirectresultofclimatechange.

Intherecentpast,evidenceofsucheffectscomesfromtheU.S.DustBowlofthe1930s,clashesinBangladeshandIndiasincethe1950s,andHurricaneKatrinaintheUnitedStatesin2005(Reuveny,2008).Thecasesdifferinmanyways,includ­ingpoliticalorganizationandstrength.Butineachcase,thereisevidencethatenvi­ronmentaldisasterledtoincreasedinterpersonalviolence,aresultofecomigration(migrationofalargenumberofpeopleasaresultofecologicaldisaster).

hurricane katrina WhenKatrinahitLouisianaandMississippiinfall2005,itfloodedabout80%ofNewOrleansanddestroyedmuchoftheBiloxi–Gulfport

Page 277: the Psychology of Social Conflict and Aggression Sydney Symposium in Social Psychology

craig a. anderSon and matt deliSi260

area.Morethanamillionpeopleleftthearea.Thisecomigrationwastoatleast30differentstates,withTexas(especiallyHouston)absorbingthemost,atleastinitially.Texasofficialsran20,000criminalchecksandfoundminimalcriminaldataontheirKatrinaimmigrants.Nonetheless,Houstonrecordedhugeincreasesinhomicides in thefollowingmonths,relative tothesamemonths in theyearprior to Katrina (Reuveny, 2008). There were other indicators (e.g., polls) oftension between the long­time residents and the newcomers. However, therewasnooutbreakofcivilwarandnoevidenceofarmedintergroupconflict.Thisseemstobegenerallytrueofecomigrationsinwell­organizedhighlyindustrial­izedcountries.

u.S. dust Bowl Inthe1930s,poorfarmingpracticescombinedwithapro­longeddroughtandstrongwindstoproduceanenvironmentaldisasterintheGreatPlains,particularlyOklahoma.About2.5millionpeopleleftthearea,primarilyforadjacentstates,butabout300,000wenttoCalifornia.Therearenumerousreportsof hostility and violence between the residents and the ecomigrants, includingpoliceeffortstoblockthemigrantsortoscatterthemfromtheirsettlements,beat­ings,andshackburnings(Reuveny,2008).

Bangladesh Population pressures from a very high fertility rate combinedwithunsustainablefarmingpracticesandenvironmentaldisasters(possiblyrelatedtoclimatechange)ledtolarge­scalemigrationstoadjacentregionsinBangladeshandacrossthebordertoIndia.From1976to2000about25millionpeoplewereaffectedbydroughts,270millionbyfloods,andanother41millionbyrainandwindstorms.Makingmattersworse, in1975theIndianFarakkaBarragebegandiverting water from the Ganges River to other parts of India, decreasing theamountflowingintoitshistorictributariesinBangladesh.Theresultingsalt­waterintrusionfromtheIndianOceanandincreasedsiltingoftheriverbedresultedinadditionalfloods,erosion,andenvironmentaldegradation.

Anestimated12to17millionBangladeshishavemigratedtoadjacentstatesin India since the1950s.Clashesbetween the residents and themigrantshaveoccurredalong socioeconomic, religious, ethnic, and national lines, resulting inthousandsofdeaths,especiallyafter the1983elections. Indeed,1,700Bengaliswerekilledina5­hourrampagein1983.

1967 arab–israeli War There is historical evidence of water issues con­tributingtoconflict in theMiddleEastat leastasearlyas theseventhcenturyB.C.(Gleick,1993).SincetheestablishmentofIsraelin1948theregionhasperi­odically been at war, for a variety of political and religious reasons. But waterissuesalsoplayanimportantroleintheconflicts,especiallyissuesconcerningtheJordanRiverbasin.Thisbasin is sharedbyIsrael, Jordan,Lebanon,andSyria.AccordingtoGleick(p.85),“oneofthefactorsdirectlycontributingtothe1967WarwastheattemptbymembersoftheArabLeagueintheearly1960stodiverttheheadwatersoftheJordanRiverawayfromIsrael.”(Foradditionalexamplesofimportantwaterconflicts,historicalaswellascontemporary,seeGleick,1993;Postel&Wolf,2001).

Page 278: the Psychology of Social Conflict and Aggression Sydney Symposium in Social Psychology

implicationS of gloBal climate change 261

Time-Period Studies

little ice age effects FollowingtheMedievalWarmPeriod,theLittleIceAge(roughly1300–1850)usheredincoolertemperatures,shortergrowingseasons,andahostofotherclimate­relatedchanges.Scholarsfromavarietyofdisciplineshavebegunexamining the relationshipsamongrelatively rapid shifts inclimateandahostofhumanpopulationevents,includingwar.Fagan(2000)weavesacare­fulstoryofclimateshiftsandtheirimpactonEuropeans,linkingfarmingpracticesandoutcomes,socialandculturalchanges,civilunrest,andwar.Thoughcarefultoavoidextremeclaimsofenvironmentaldeterminism,hemakesa strongcaseforviewingrapidclimatechange(inthiscase,cooling)ascontributingtowarandotherformsofviolence.Briefly,rapidclimatechangedisruptedfoodproduction,leadingtofoodshortages,famines,civilunrest,andwar.Thisprocessseemspar­ticularlyimportantinagrariansocietiesthatdonothavethepoliticalandeconomicresourcestoeffectivelydealwithfoodshortagesandfamine.Indeed,accordingtoFagantheFrenchRevolutionwasfueledinpartbyfoodshortagesthatwerelargelytheresultofthefailureoffarmingpracticestoadapttothechangedclimate.

Zhangandcolleagues(Zhang,Brecke,Lee,He,&Zhang,2007;Zhang,Zhang,Lee,&He,2007)tookamorestatisticalapproachtoexaminingthequestionofwhetherrapidshiftsinclimatefrom1000to1900werelinkedtowars.UsingdatafromtheNorthernHemisphereandfromChina,theyfoundstatisticalsupportfortheirmodel,whichisverysimilartoFagan’s(2000).

It might seem strange to include studies of rapid cooling in a work that isfocusedonglobalwarmingandviolence.However, thebasicmodel is thesameregardlessofwhetherarapidshift inclimateiswarmingorcooling,floodingordrought.Asystematicchangeinclimatethatthreatensbasichumanresourcesputsstressoneconomicandsocialsystems.Thatstresscanleadtoecomigrationandconflictordirectlytowaroverresources.

civil War in africa Burke,Miguel,Satyanath,Dykema,andLobell(2009)recentlyanalyzedcivilwarsinAfricafrom1981to2002.Somemodelsincludedper capita incomeand formofgovernment aswell as temperature andprecipi­tation. Overall, the results showed a strong positive relation between tempera­tureincreasesandcivilwar.Fora1°Cincreaseintemperature,therewasa5.9%increaseincivilwar.Giventhebaserateofcivilwarinthisdataset(11%),thisrep­resentsa54%relativeincreaseinthelikelihoodofcivilwarforeach1°Cincreaseintemperature.Theauthorsnotedthata1°Cincreaseisprojectedby2030andthatiffuturewarsareasdeadlyaspastonesanadditional393,000battledeathscanbeexpectedinthisregion.

Additional Ecomigration and War-Related Forms of Violence

Arecent reportby theUnitedNations (Engelman,2009)highlightedanumberofadditionalwaysglobalclimatechangecanleadtoincreasedviolence.Perhapsthemostnotableisthelikelyincreaseinviolentcrimescommittedagainstwomenand children as a consequence of their increased vulnerability in subsistence

Page 279: the Psychology of Social Conflict and Aggression Sydney Symposium in Social Psychology

craig a. anderSon and matt deliSi262

economiesthatsufferanecologicaldisaster.Withthebreakdownofsocietalnormsandincreasedeconomicstresscomeincreasesinrape,assault,andhomicide.Asfarasweknow,therearenostudiesdirectlylinkingglobalwarmingtosucheffects,butsuchoutcomeshavebeendocumentedintheaftermathofseverefloods,foodshortages,andwar(“civil”orotherwise).

implicationSCollectively, these three ways global climate change increases human violencesuggest a rather dire future. We prefer to end on a more positive note. Actioncanbetaken,byindividuals,groups,andgovernments.Oneobviousactionistoreducegreenhousegasemissions,therebyreducingthemagnitudeandspeedofclimate change. Many individuals, groups, and governments are taking actions,albeitsomewhatbelatedly.

Inadditiontothetechnologicalandlifestylechangesbeingactivelydeveloped,discussed,andimplemented,italsoseemsworthwhiletoconsideraninfrequentlydiscussedoption,thepotentialbenefitsofbetterpopulationcontrol.Onethousandyearsagotheworldpopulationwasabout300million.Currentlyitisabout7bil­lion.Somehaveestimatedthattheworldpopulationwillpeakataround10billion.Mostofthatincreasewilltakeplaceindevelopingcountries,withhugeincreasesingreenhousegasemissionsasaresultofcarbon­intensiveindustrializationandincreasingconsumption.Generallyspeaking,asacountrybecomesmore indus­trializedandwealthy,thecarbonfootprintperpersonincreasesdramatically,andpopulationgrowtheventuallyslows.Theconundrumwefaceishowtoreducetotalgreenhousegasemissionswhileimprovingthequalityoflifeofthelargepropor­tionofpeoplecurrentlylivinginpoverty.Onerecentstudyfoundthat,“dollar­for­dollar,investmentsinvoluntaryfamilyplanningandgirls’educationwouldalsointhelongrunreducegreenhouse­gasemissionsatleastasmuchasthesameinvest­mentsinnuclearorwindenergy”(Engelman,2009,p.26).

Developed and developing countries will be affected differently by globalwarming.Insomeways,developedcountrieswillbelessaffected,inpartbecauseof their locations but more importantly because they have more resources percapitatodealwiththechanges.Itisunlikelythatfamineswillstriketherichestcountries,forexample.However,nocountrywillbeimmunetotheviolencecon­sequencesofglobalclimatechange.Theheateffectonindividuallevelsofaggres­sion and violence applies to all countries. Similarly, it seems obvious that evenwealthycountriesarelikelytoseeincreasesintheproportionofchildrenexposedtoknownriskfactorsforthedevelopmentofviolence­proneyouthandadults.Itislessobvioushowwealthycountrieswillbeaffectedbythethirdprocess,whichleads to increases in civil unrest, ecomigration, genocide, and war. But even ifdeveloped countries do not experience sufficient economic and social stress toinducewar(civilorinternational),civilunrestandecomigrationwithinthemwilllikelyleadtoincreasesinviolentcrime,especiallyafterecologicaldisasterssuchasfloods.Furthermore,increasedpoverty,civildissolution,andwarsindevelop­ingcountrieshaveanimpactondevelopedcountries.Insomecases,theimpactderives from the global economy and the need for resources. Also, differences

Page 280: the Psychology of Social Conflict and Aggression Sydney Symposium in Social Psychology

implicationS of gloBal climate change 263

between the have and have­not countries create breeding grounds for interna­tionalterroristgroups.

Whatactionscouldreducethelikelihoodofclimatechange–inducedviolence?Thereissomelimitedevidencethattheheat–aggressioneffectonindividualscanbereducedbysimplymakingpeopleawarethatwhentheyareuncomfortablyhottheytendtoreacttominorprovocationsininappropriatelyhostileways.However,giventheimmediacyandsubtletyoftheheateffectonirritability,hostilepercep­tionbiases,andaggression,itisdoubtfulthatsuchaneducationalinterventionwillhavealargeimpact.

Ontheotherhand,theothertwowaysglobalwarmingincreaseshumanvio­lenceappeartobegoodcandidatesforintervention.Ifgovernmentsbeganprepar­ingnowtofeed,shelter,educate,andmoveat­riskpopulationstoregionsinwhichthey can maintain their livelihoods and their cultures, we could dramaticallyreduceboththedevelopmentofviolence­proneindividualsandthecivilunrest,ecomigration,andwarproblems.Thiswillcosthugeamountsofmoneyandwillrequiremoreinternationalcooperationthanourplanethaseverseen.Failuretodosowillresultinadditionaldisastersformillionsofpeople.

referenceS

Anderson,C.A.(1989).Temperatureandaggression:Ubiquitouseffectsofheatonoccur­renceofhumanviolence.Psychological Bulletin, 106,74–96.

Anderson,C.A. (2001).Heatandviolence.Current Directions in Psychological Science, 10,33–38.

Anderson,C.A.,&Anderson,D.C.(1984).Ambienttemperatureandviolentcrime:Testsofthelinearandcurvilinearhypotheses.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 46,91–97.

Anderson,C.A.,&Anderson,K.B.(1996).Violentcrimeratestudiesinphilosophicalcon­text:Adestructivetestingapproachtoheatandsoutherncultureofviolenceeffects.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 70,740–756.

Anderson,C.A.,&Anderson,K.B. (1998).Temperatureandaggression:Paradox, con­troversy,anda (fairly)clearpicture. InR.Geen&E.Donnerstein (Eds.),Human aggression: Theories, research, and implications for social policy.(pp.247–298).SanDiego,CA:AcademicPress.

Anderson, C. A., Anderson, K. B., Dorr, N., DeNeve, K. M., & Flanagan, M. (2000).Temperature and aggression. Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, 32,63–133.

Anderson,C.A.,Bushman,B.J.,&Groom,R.W.(1997).Hotyearsandseriousanddeadlyassault: Empirical tests of the heat hypothesis. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 73,1213–1223.

Beaver, K. M., DeLisi, M., Vaughn, M. G., & Wright, J. P. (2010). The intersection ofgenes and neuropsychological deficits in the prediction of adolescent delinquencyand low self­control. International Journal of Offender Therapy and Comparative Criminology,54, 22–42.

Boyanowsky,E.O.(1999).Violenceandaggressionintheheatofpassionandincoldblood.International Journal of Law and Psychiatry, 22, 257–271.

Page 281: the Psychology of Social Conflict and Aggression Sydney Symposium in Social Psychology

craig a. anderSon and matt deliSi264

Boyanowsky, E. O. (2008). Explaining the relationship among environmental tempera-tures, aggression and violent crime: Emotional-cognitive stress under thermoregula-tory conflict (The ECS-TC Syndrome).Presentedat theBiannualWorldMeetingof theInternationalSociety forResearchonAggression,Budapest,Hungary, July8–13.

Boyanowsky.E.O.,Calvert­Boyanowsky,J.,Young,J.,&Brideau,L.(1981).Towardather­moregulatorymodelofviolence.Journal of Environmental Systems, 11,81–87.

Burke,M.B.,Miguel,E.,Satyanath,S.,Dykema,J.A.,&Lobell,D.B.(2009).Warmingincreases the risk of civil war in Africa. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 106(49), 20670–20674.

Bushman,B.J.,Wang,M.C.,&Anderson,C.A.(2005a).Isthecurverelatingtemperaturetoaggressionlinearorcurvilinear?AssaultsandtemperatureinMinneapolisreexam­ined.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 89,62–66.

Bushman,B.J.,Wang,M.C.,&Anderson,C.A.(2005b).Isthecurverelatingtemperaturetoaggressionlinearorcurvilinear? AresponsetoBell(2005)andtoCohnandRotton(2005).Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 89, 74–77.

Carlsmith,J.M.,&Anderson,C.A.(1979).Ambienttemperatureandtheoccurrenceofcollectiveviolence:Anewanalysis.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 37,337–344.

Caspi,A.,McClay,J,Moffitt,T.E.,Mill,J.,Martin,J.,Craig,I.W.,etal.(2002).Roleofgenotypeinthecycleofviolenceinmaltreatedchildren.Science,297,851–854.

Caspi,A.,Sugden,K.,Moffitt,T.E.,Taylor,A.,Craig,I.W.,Harrington,H.,etal.(2003).Influenceoflifestressondepression:Moderationbyapolymorphisminthe5­HTTgene.Science,301,386–389.

Chen,E.,Cohen,S.,&Miller,G.E.(2010).Howlowsocioeconomicstatusaffects2­yearhormonaltrajectoriesinchildren.Psychological Science, 21, 31–37.

DeLisi,M.(2005).Career criminals in society.ThousandOaks,CA:Sage.Engelman,R.(Ed.).(2009).The state of world population 2009.NewYork:UnitedNations

PopulationFund.Fagan, B. (2000). The Little Ice Age: How climate made history 1300–1850. New York:

BasicBooks.Gleick,P.H.(1993).Waterandconflict.International Security, 18(1),79–112.Gottfredson,M.R.,&Hirschi,T.(1990).A general theory of crime.Stanford,CA:Stanford

UniversityPress.Huston, A. C., & Bentley, A. (2010). Human development in societal context. Annual

Review of Psychology, 61,411–437.IntergovernmentalPanelonClimateChange.(IPCC).(2007).Parry,M.L.,Canziani,O.F.,

Palutikof,J.P.,vanderLinden,P.J.&Hanson,C.E.(Eds.).Intergovernmental Panel on Climate Change.Cambridge,UK:CambridgeUniversityPress.

Kruglanski,A.W.,Chen,X.,Dechesne,M.,Fishman,S.,&Orehek,E.(2009).Fullycom­mitted:Suicidebombers’motivationandthequestforpersonalsignificance.Political Psychology, 30,331–357.

LaFree,G.,&Drass,K.A. (1996).Theeffectsof changes in intraracial income inequal­ityandeducationalattainmentonchangesinarrestratesforAfricanAmericansandWhites,1957–1990.American Sociological Review, 61,614–634.

Ljung,G.M.,&Box,G.E.P.(1978).Onameasureoflackoffit intimeseriesmodels.Biometrika, 65,297–303.

Liu, J.,Raine,A.,Venables,P.H.,&Mednick,S.A. (2004).Malnutritionatage3yearsandexternalizingbehaviorproblemsatages8,11,and17years.American Journal of Psychiatry, 161,2005–2013.

Moffitt,T.E.(1993).Adolescence­limitedandlife­course­persistentantisocialbehavior:Adevelopmentaltaxonomy.Psychological Review, 100,674–701.

Page 282: the Psychology of Social Conflict and Aggression Sydney Symposium in Social Psychology

implicationS of gloBal climate change 265

Neugebauer,R.,Hoek,H.W.,&Susser,E.(1999).Prenatalexposuretowartimefamineanddevelopmentofantisocialpersonalitydisorderinearlyadulthood.Journal of the American Medical Association, 282,455–462.

Postel,S.L.,&Wolf,A.T.(2001).Dehydratingconflict.Foreign Policy, 126,60–67.Rahmstorf, S. (2009). Climate seminar. Presented at the COWI conference, May 11,

Kongens,Lyngby,Denmark.DownloadedJanuary4,2010fromhttp://www.youtube.com/v/9bILFEdLy28

Reifman,A.S.,Larrick,R.P.,&Fein,S.(1991).Temperandtemperatureonthediamond:Theheat­aggression relationship inMajorLeagueBaseball.Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin,17,580–585.

Reuveny,R.(2008).Ecomigrationandviolentconflict:Casestudiesandpublicpolicyimpli­cations.Human Ecology. 36,1–13.

Uher,R.,&McGuffin,P.(2010).Themoderationbytheserotonintransportergeneofenvi­ronmentaladversityintheetiologyofdepression:2009update.Molecular Psychiatry, 15, 18–22.

Van de Vliert, E. (2009). Climate, affluence, and culture. New York: Cambridge UniversityPress.

VandeVliert,E. (inpress).Climato­economicoriginsofvariation in ingroup favoritism. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology.

VandeVliert,E.(underreview).Bullyingthemedia:Culturalandclimato­economicread­ingsofpressrepressionversuspressfreedom.

Vermeer, M., & Rahmstorf, S. (2009). Global sea level linked to global temperature.Proceedings of the National Academy of Science, 106, 21527–21532,downloadedJanuary4,2010fromhttp://www.pnas.org

Vrij,A.,vanderSteen,J.,&Koppelaar,L.(1994).Aggressionofpoliceofficersasafunc­tionoftemperature:AnexperimentwiththeFireArmsTrainingSystem.Journal of Community and Applied Social Psychology,4,365–370.

Wilkowski,B.M.,Meier,B.P.,Robinson,M.D.,Carter,M.S.,&Feltman,R.(2009).“Hot­headed”ismorethananexpression:Theembodiedrepresentationofangerintermsofheat.Emotion, 9,464–477.

Zhang,D.D.,Brecke,P.,Lee,H.F.,He,Y.Q.,&Zhang,J.(2007).Globalclimatechange,war, and population decline in recent human history. Proceedings of the National Academy of Science, 104,19214–19219.

Zhang,D.D.,Zhang,J.,Lee,H.F.,&He,Y.Q.(2007).ClimatechangeandwarfrequencyinEasternChinaoverthelastmillennium. Human Ecology, 35,403–414.

Page 283: the Psychology of Social Conflict and Aggression Sydney Symposium in Social Psychology
Page 284: the Psychology of Social Conflict and Aggression Sydney Symposium in Social Psychology

267

17TheMediaandAggression

From TV to the InternetEDDONNERSTEIN

University of Arizona

R ecentlytheAmericanAcademyofPediatrics(AAP)issuedapolicystate­mentonmedia violence (AmericanAcademyofPediatrics, 2009a).Thestatementwasclear in termsof itsfindings,position,andrecommenda­

tions.InratherstraightforwardtermstheAAPnoted:

Exposuretoviolenceinmedia,includingtelevision,movies,music,andvideogames,representsasignificantrisktothehealthofchildrenandadolescents.Extensiveresearchevidenceindicatesthatmediaviolencecancontributetoaggressivebehavior,desensitizationtoviolence,nightmares,andfearofbeingharmed. Pediatricians should assess their patients’ level of media exposureandinterveneonmedia­relatedhealthrisks.(p.1495)

Therecommendationsforparents,practitioners,andtheindustrywereequallyfrankandsuggestedsomeofthefollowing:

• Remove televisions, Internet connections, and video games from chil­dren’sbedrooms.

• Avoidscreenmediaforinfantsortoddlersyoungerthan2years.• Avoidtheglamorizationofweapon­carryingandthenormalizationofvio­

lenceasanacceptablemeansofresolvingconflict.• Eliminatetheuseofviolenceinacomicorsexualcontextorinanyother

situationinwhichtheviolenceisamusing,titillating,ortrivialized.• Eliminategratuitousportrayalsofinterpersonalviolence.• Ifviolenceisused,itshouldbeusedthoughtfullyasseriousdrama,always

showingthepainandlosssufferedbyvictimsandperpetrators.

Page 285: the Psychology of Social Conflict and Aggression Sydney Symposium in Social Psychology

ed donnerStein268

• Videogamesshouldnotusehumanorotherlivingtargetsorawardpointsforkilling,becausethisteacheschildrentoassociatepleasureandsuccesswiththeirabilitytocausepainandsufferingtoothers.

IwouldexpectthattheAAP’sstatementandrecommendationswouldprob­ablybeacceptedbyasubstantialmajorityofresearchersintheareaofmediavio­lence,andaggressioningeneral,includingthoseparticipatinginthisSymposium(Anderson, Gentile, & Buckley, 2007; Huesmann, 2007; Strasburger, Jordan, &Donnerstein,2010).AndasapublichealthorganizationtheAAPisnotaloneinitsrecommendation;groupsliketheAmericanMedicalAssociation,theAmericanAcademy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, and the American PsychologicalAssociationhaveallissuedstatementsovertheyearspertainingtothe“harmful”impactofmediaviolenceonchildren.

Inthischapterwewillprovideanoverviewforthisandotherresearchrelatedtotheseharmfuleffectsbutwithanadditionalfocusonhowviolenceisdissemi­natedthroughthelensofnewermediatechnology.Muchoftheresearchonmediaviolencehastraditionallybeenonthemediaoftelevisionandofcourseonvideogames.Thismediaformisnotobsolete;rather,formanychildrenandadolescentsthemediumforthisviewingmightnotbethetime­honoredtelevisionscreenbutinsteadtheInternet,whichoffersanarrayofnewissuestoconsider.ThefollowingsectionexaminestheroleoftheInternetasitrelatestotheconcernswehaveabouttheinfluenceofmediaviolence.

the internet aS a medium for media violenceUnliketraditionalmediasuchasTV,therearerelativelyfewstudiesontheimpactofInternetviolence.Thesearereviewedlaterinthechapter,butitisinterestingtonoteattheoutsettherecentcommentaryofresearchersaboutthepotentialandfar­reachinginfluencesofthis“newer”technology:

TheInternetisfastbecomingthetelephoneofthe21stcentury,withanesti­mated97%ofyoungpeoplebetweentheagesof12and18yearsusingonlinecommunication. Almost all youths now have online access, and this accessmayincreaseopportunitiesforchildrenandyouthstobeexposedtoviolence.(Ybarra,West,Markow,Leaf,Hamburger,&Boxer,2008,p.930)

Formanyyouthithasbecomethemajorsourceofinformationandentertain­ment.Itisperhapstheonemediumwherechildrenmaycomeacrossnon­inten­tionallycontentthatislessavailableintraditionalmediasuchassevereviolence,violentpornography,childpornography,hategroups.(Feilitzen,2009)

TheInternetbecomesthemediuminwhichtraditionalmedia likeTV,film,andvideogamescanbedownloaded,viewed,andprocessedcognitively.Themostrecent survey of children and adolescent media use by the Kaiser Foundation(2010)indicatedthattheamountoftimeviewingTVcontenthadincreasedoverthe lastdecadebut that this increase isaccounted forprimarilyby theviewingofsuchprogrammingovertheInternetandmobiledevices.TheInternetisalso

Page 286: the Psychology of Social Conflict and Aggression Sydney Symposium in Social Psychology

the media and aggreSSion 269

a vehicle, however, for the creation of aggressive images and the acting out ofaggressivebehavior.It isbothpassiveandactive.Itincorporatesourconceptionofhowchildrenandadolescentscognitivelyprocessconventionalmediaviolence,butitaddsanewdimension—actuallybeingaggressive.WecanseeinFigure 17.1(adaptedfromcommentsbyFeilitzen,2009)howthismightbeconceptualized,both in terms of the Internet generally and in the use of new technologies asmobilephones.

TheInternetallowstheindividualtoviewtraditionalTVandfilmandvideogamesthroughlivestreamingordownloads.Forthechildoradolescent,accesstowhatmightbeconsideredrestrictedmaterials(adultrated)ismucheasierviabothlegaland“illegal”outlets.Websitesofferanotherdimensiontotheviewingofviolence:thecreationanduploadingofviolentmaterials.Websitesoffernotonly theprospectof viewingmore severe violence (e.g., realdecapitations andexecutions)butalsoaccesstohateandterroristgroups.Theviewer,however,cannowbecome thecreator of violent images in an almost formulaicmanner andcanplacethatmaterialacrosstheglobeinstantaneously.Finally,Websitesandinparticularsocialnetworkingsites,blogs,chatrooms,andemailnotonlyallowforthecreationofaggressionbutalsoprovidetheabilitytoactuallypersistentlyaggressagainstanother(i.e.,cyberbullying).Onephenomenon,whichbringsthistoa“strange”confluence,is“happyslapping,”whereavictimisassaultedandthedepictionisuploadedtotheInternet.AsCalveteetal.(2008)noted,adolescentswhousetheirmobilephoneswiththisaimarecharacterizedbyseveraltypesofaggressiveness,by justificationofviolencebeliefs,andbyhighexposuretovio­lenceinthefamilyandmedia.

Consequently,unliketraditionalmediasuchasTV,theInternetandthesenewtechnologiesgivechildrenandadolescentsaccesstojustaboutanyformofcontenttheycanfind(e.g.,Livingstone&Haddon,2009).Forthefirsttime,theseindividu­alswillbeable(oftenwithlittleeffort)tohavetheabilitytoviewalmostanyformofsexualbehavior,violentcontent,orotherrisk­relatedcontent(Donnerstein,2009;Strasburgeretal.,2010).Unlikeyearspast,thiscanbedoneintheprivacyoftheirownroomwith littleknowledgeor supervisionof theirparents.The interactive

Traditional massmedia• Receive and consume “traditional” film and TV violence• Video games

Web sites• Traditional mass media available• Severe violence, hate groups• Send, create, and upload violent content

Socialnetworkingsites, blogs,chat, email• Receive aggression (e.g., cyber-bullying)• Create and send aggression

figure 17.1 TheInternet,newtechnology,andmassmediaviolence.

Page 287: the Psychology of Social Conflict and Aggression Sydney Symposium in Social Psychology

ed donnerStein270

natureoftheInternet,whichcanleadtomorearousalandmorecognitiveactivity,wouldsuggestthatinfluencessuchasthosefoundfrommediaviolencewouldbefacilitated(seeHuesman,2007).

media uSe and the child audienceAmerican children and adolescents spend significant time in front of a screen(Strasburgeretal.,2010).IntheirnationalsurveyofmediauseintheUnitedStates,theKaiserFamilyFoundation (2006,2010) foundthatchildrenandadolescentswerespendingonaveragemoretime in frontofascreen thanreadingorbeingactiveoutdoors.Moreinterestingwasthefindingthatofallthoseundertheageof3,anagemanywouldsuggestismorevulnerabletoeffects,overtwo­thirdsusesomescreenmediaaday,andathirdarealreadyusingacomputer.Achild’sbedroomisnolongeraplaceofisolationinthatmediatechnologyispartofthefurniture.Two­thirdshadatelevisionset,one­halfhadaVCRorDVDplayerorvideogameconsole,andnearlyone­thirdhadInternetaccessoracomputer.Moreimportantly,theuseoftheInternetandmobiledevicesformediaconsumptionhasincreasedsignificantly,particularlywiththerapidexpansionofbroadbandavailability.

WhileittookdecadesforTVtobecomepartofthefamilyhousehold,Internetusehasachievedthisinashorttimeframe.RecentresearchbythePewInternetandAmericanLifeProject (PewFoundation,2009)revealed that93%ofyouthaged12 to17 areonline sometimeduring theday, and71%havea cellphone.Whetheritiswatchingvideos(57%),usingsocialnetworkingsites(65%),orplayingvideogames (97%),childrenandadolescentshave incorporatednewtechnologyintotheirdailylives.Thesefrequenciesarealsoobservedacross21differentcoun­trieswithinEurope.TheEUKidsOnlineProjectfoundthatin2005onaverage70%of6–17­year­oldsusedtheInternet.By2008,itwasat75%withthelargestincreaseoccurringamongyoungerchildren(6–10)inwhich60%werenowonline(Livingstone&Haddon,2009).

Inthinkingaboutthisuseofthemassmedia,weneedtorememberthatveryoften those who are the most vulnerable to negative impacts (e.g., aggression,deceptiveadvertising)arechildren,andtheirprocessingofthemediaisdifferentfromadults.Inthefirstplace,youngerchildreninterpretmediamessagesdiffer­ently.Theypaymoreattentiontoperceptualfeaturesandmoresalientcontextualfeaturesratherthantoplot.Theyarelesscognizantofconsequencesandmotives.Childrenalsohavedifficultydistinguishingbetweenfantasyandreality.Thisplaysanextremelyimportantroleinchildren’smediaviewinginthatwhenweexaminetheeffectsfromexposuretomediaviolenceonecriticalcontextualvariableistheperceivedrealityoftheaggression.Finally,childrenhavedifficultiesandarelesscapableoflinkingscenestogether.Adultsrecognizethattheperpetratorofaggres­sion was caught, punished, or reprimanded for his or her behavior in the finalscene.Ayoungchilddoesnotalwaysperceivethisrelationship.

Weneedtorealizethatnotallviolentportrayalsposethesameriskofharmtoviewers.Researchindicatesthatcertaindepictionsofviolenceincreasetheriskofantisocialeffectsmorethanothers(seeStrasburger,Wilson,&Jordan,2009).Simplyput,thecontextorwayviolenceispresentedinfluencesitsimpactonthe

Page 288: the Psychology of Social Conflict and Aggression Sydney Symposium in Social Psychology

the media and aggreSSion 271

audience.Basedonanextensivereviewofstudiesinthisarea,researchers(Smith&Donnerstein,2003;Wilsonetal.,2002)haveidentifiedarangeofcontextualfea­turesthatinfluencehowaudienceswillrespondtotelevisedviolence(e.g.,adver­tisements;Tajimaetal.,2008)andalsotovideogameviolence(Horiuchiet.al.,2008).Themostimportantcontextualfactorsareattractiveperpetrators,thepres­enceofweapons,graphicandextensiveviolence,consequencesforaggressionsuchaspunishmentsorpain,andrealism.

Muchof theviolencepresented in themedia isoften sanitizedandglamor­ized,andinchildren’sprogrammingitoftenispresentedashumorous(Strasburgeretal.,2009).Thesecontextualfactorsareimportantindeterminingthe“risk”ofexposure primarily to young children. Portrayals with an attractive perpetratorandthatarerealistic, justified,gounpunished,andshownoharmare themostproblematicforyoungchildren.Thesetypesofmediadepictionsoccurmostofteninthetypesofshowsviewedbychildren(e.g.,Wilsonetal.,2002).

Interactive media seems to be no different from traditional television when itcomestodepictionsofconcernforchildrenandadolescents.Arecentanalysisofvideogamesrevealedthatmorethanhalfofallgames—including90%ratedasappropri­ateforchildrenaged10yearsandolder(Andersonetal.,2007)—containviolence.Contextualcharacteristicsthatareconsideredmoreofariskforsubsequentaggressivebehaviorhavealsobeenfoundinvideogames.Inarecentanalysisjustifiedviolencewasfoundin60%ofthegames,violencewithweaponswasfoundin65%,andrewardforviolencein89%.Factorswemightconsiderasinhibitingaggressionsuchaspun­ishmentsforviolencewerefoundinonly26%ofthegames(Horiuchietal.,2008).

Bydefinition, theInternetencompassesall the typesofviolencedepicted intraditionalmedia andvideogames.While there isnomajorcontentanalysis forvideogameslikethereisfortraditionalmedia,researchersacknowledgethatmorereal­worldviolence,hategroups,violentpornography,andotherformsofviolencearemoreprevalent(Ybarraetal.,2008).ThereiscurrentlyalackofdataonhowmanychildrenoradolescentsareintentionallyviewingthesesitesandwhethertheInternet’sincreasedeaseofaccesstothesetypesofdepictionshasresultedinhighratesofexposureamongyoungviewers(Hamburger,Ybarra,Leaf,&West,2009).

media violence effectS: televiSion, video gameS, and the internet

In this section we will brieflyexamine theknown effects of media violence onaggressivebehavior.Eventhoughthereviewisseparatedintothreecategories(TV,videogames,Internet),theinterrelationshipsareevident.Sinceonemajorgoalofthischapterwastoexaminetheuniqueaspectsofnewertechnologies,inparticu­lartheInternet,thisclassificationseemsmorefunctional.

Television

Inareportonyouthviolence,thesurgeongeneraloftheUnitedStates(2001)iden­tifiedaseriesofriskfactorsthatwereconsideredto(1)increasetheprobabilitythat

Page 289: the Psychology of Social Conflict and Aggression Sydney Symposium in Social Psychology

ed donnerStein272

ayoungpersonwillbecomeviolent,and(2)predicttheonset,continuity,orescala­tionofviolence.Inconsideringthesevaryingfactors,thesurgeongeneral’sreportnotedthatmoreimportantthananyindividualriskfactoristheaccumulationoffactors.Whenconsideringthemost importantfactors inyouthviolence,beingamalehadthehighesteffectsize.Amongothermajorfactorsandtheireffectsizeswerethefollowing:

Substanceabuse=.30Antisocialparents=.23Weaksocialties=.15Mediaviolence=.13LowIQ=.12Brokenhome=.09Abusiveparents=.07Antisocialpeers=.04

Thoughitisnotthemajorcontributor,mediaviolenceisconsidered,especiallywhenotherfactorsarepresent,asignificantforceinthedevelopmentandonsetofaggressivebehaviorinyouth.Asresearchershavestronglysuggested,exposuretoviolentmedianeedstobepartofthemeasurestakenwhenriskfactorsforaggres­sive behavior are considered (Boxer,Huesmann,Bushman, O’Brien,& Moceri,2009;Bushman,Huesmann,&Whitaker,2009).

Anycriticalexaminationoftheliteraturewouldindicatethatexposuretomediaviolencecancontributetoarangeofantisocialeffectsonviewers(Huesmann&Kirwil,2007).Theconclusionthatviolenceontelevisioncontributestonegativeeffectsonviewersishardlynovel.Theeffectsthatseemtobemostpronouncedarethelearningofaggressiveattitudesandbehaviors,desensitizationtoviolence,andincreasedfearofbeingvictimizedbyviolence(seeHuesmann,2007;Huesmann&Taylor,2006).

Overthelast40years,severalgovernmentalandprofessionalorganizationshaveconductedexhaustive reviewsof the scientific literature toascertain the relation­shipbetweenexposuretomediaviolenceandaggression.Theseinvestigationshavedocumentedconsistentlythatexposuretomediaviolencecontributestoaggressivebehaviorsinviewersandmayinfluencetheirperceptionsandattitudesaboutvio­lenceintherealworld.Heavyviewingofmediaviolenceiscorrelatedwithaggres­sivebehaviorand increasedaggressiveattitudes(seeAndersonetal.,2003foranextensivereview).Thecorrelationbetweenviewingviolenceinthemediaandexhib­iting aggressivebehavior is fairly stable over time, place, anddemographics (e.g.,Huesmann,Moise,Podolski,&Eron,2003).Experimentaland longitudinalstud­ies also support theposition that viewing televised violence is relatedcausally toaggressivebehavior(Andersonetal.).Evenmoreimportant,naturalisticfieldstudiesand cross­national investigations reveal that viewing televised aggression leads toincreasesinsubsequentreal­lifeaggressionandthatsuchbehaviorcanbecomepartofalastingbehavioralpattern(Bushmanetal.,2008;Huesmann,Boxer,&Bushman,2009).Thesestudieshavebeenconsistentinresearchconductedinanumberofdif­ferentcountries(seeBushman&Huesmann,2006;Huesmann,2007).

Page 290: the Psychology of Social Conflict and Aggression Sydney Symposium in Social Psychology

the media and aggreSSion 273

Froma theoretical perspective, Huesmann (2007; see also Chapters16 and19inthisvolume)andothers(e.g.,Anderson&Bushman,2002)wouldagreethattheshort­termeffectsofexposuretomediaviolencearemostlydueto(1)primingprocesses, (2) arousalprocesses, and (3) immediatemodelingof specificbehav­iors(observationallearning).Long­termeffectsseemtobedueto(1)longer­termlearningofcognitionsandbehavioralscriptsand(2)theactivationanddesensitiza­tionofemotionalprocesses.

Video Games

In terms of demonstrating increased aggressive behavior from exposure, theresearch on video games is as consistent as that with television violence (seeAnderson et al., 2007, 2010 for an extensive review). Meta­analyses (Anderson,2004;Anderson&Bushman,2001;Andersonetal.,2010)havebeenconsistentintheirfindings.Whilesomehavedisagreedwiththesestudies(Ferguson,2010),theconsensusofresearchersisthateffectsfromplayingviolentvideogameshavebeenshownforthefollowing:

1.Increasedaggressivebehavior(Andersonetal.,2008) 2.Hostileaffect(Carnagey&Anderson,2005) 3.Physiologicalarousal(Anderson&Bushman,2001) 4.Aggressivecognitions(Anderson&Huesmann,2003;Bluemke,Friedrich,

&Zumbach,2010) 5.Reductionsinprosocialbehaviorfromdesensitization(e.g.,Bushman&

Anderson,2009)

Theseresultshavebeenobservedbothinshort­termandlongitudinalstudiesaswellascross­culturally(e.g.,AndersonetAl.,2008,2010).

Anderson (2000, 2007) noted that there are strong compelling reasons toexpectthatviolentvideogames,duetotheirinteractivenature,wouldhavestron­gereffectsonaggressionthanmoretraditional formsofmediaviolencesuchasTV.Invideogamestheprocessofidentificationwiththeaggressor,activepartici­pation,repetitiveactions,ahostilevirtualreality,andreinforcement foraggres­siveactionsareallstrongmechanismsforthelearningandretentionofaggressivebehaviorsandattitudes(Gentile&Anderson,2003).

The Internet

AnumberofareasdeserveconsiderationwhendiscussingtheeffectsoftheInternetasapotentiallyuniquecontributortoaggressivebehavior.Aswenotedearlier,theInternetnotonlyactsasaplatformfortheviewingofmediabutalsoisavehicleforactingoutaggression.Itisthislaterfunctionthatformanyhealthprofessionalshasbeenofimportance.Forthatreasonwewillexaminefirsttheconcernsaboutcyberbullyingandsexualexploitation.Followingthisdiscussionwewilllookatthecontentofviolentmediathatmightbeconsideredunique,orcertainlymoreacces­sible,ontheInternet.

Page 291: the Psychology of Social Conflict and Aggression Sydney Symposium in Social Psychology

ed donnerStein274

cyberbullying Theoneareathatseemstobeofprimeimportanceiscyber­bullying.Ithasbecomeasignificantsocialissueprimarilyamonghealth­careandotherprofessionals(AAP,2009b).Olweus(1993)definedbullyingasfollows:

Apersonisbulliedwhenheorsheisexposed,repeatedlyandovertime,tonegativeactionsonthepartofoneormoreotherpersons,andheorshehasdifficultydefendinghimselforherself.(p.14)

Thisdefinitionincludesthreeimportantcomponentsinthatitis(1)aggres­sivebehaviorthatinvolvesunwanted,negativeactions,(2)involvesapatternofbehaviorrepeatedovertime,and(3)involvesanimbalanceofpowerorstrength.Cyberbulling,often referred toas Internetharassmentor Internetaggression,incorporates these components and in many ways is similar to other forms ofbullying(e.g.,Heirman&Walrave,2008;Williams&Guerra,2007)but takesplaceovertheInternettorepeatedlyharass,threaten,ormaliciouslyembarrass.Research also suggests that Internet bullying shares common predictors withverbal and, to some extent, physical bullying (see Williams & Guerra). Whiletherearecertainlydebatesabouttheusageoftheterm,bothpolicymakersandthepublicacrossmanycountrieshaveincorporatedthetermcyberbullyingintotheirlexicon,anditwillbeusedinthischapter.Itinvolvesthefollowingbehav­iors(NCPC,2009):

• Sendingunsolicitedorthreateningemail• Encouragingotherstosendthevictimunsolicitedorthreateningemail• Sendingvirusesbyemail(electronicsabotage)• Spreadingrumors• Making defamatory comments about the victim in public discussion

areas• Sendingnegativemessagesdirectlytothevictim• Impersonating the victim online by sending an inflammatory message

thatcausesotherstorespondnegativelytothevictim• Harassingthevictimduringalivechat• LeavingabusivemessagesonWebsiteguestbooks• Sending the victim pornography or other knowingly offensive graphic

material• CreatingaWebpagethatdepictsthevictiminnegativeways

Those conducting research in this area would acknowledge that victims ofcyberbullyingaswellaschildrenwhoarebulliedinpersonmayexperiencemanyof the sameeffects, such as adrop ingrades, lowered self­esteem, a change ininterests, ordepression (see Journal of Adolescent Health, 2007; NCPC, 2009).Chapter3inthisvolumediscussescyberostracism—theeffects ofbeingignoredandexcludedovertheInternet,whichcanleadtosimilareffects(e.g.,anger,aggres­sion)as“face­to­face”ostracism.However,cyberbullyingcanseemmoreextremetoitsvictimsbecauseofseveralfactors:

Page 292: the Psychology of Social Conflict and Aggression Sydney Symposium in Social Psychology

the media and aggreSSion 275

• Itoccursinthechild’shome.Theplacethechildoradolescentoftenseesassecurenowrepresentsbeingavictim.

• Itcanbeharsherbecauseoftheanonymityoftheaggressorandinabilitytoseethevictim’sreactions. Theabilitytoempathizewiththevictimismuchmoredifficultinthesesituations(seeChapter7inthisvolume).

• Itcanbefar­reachinginthatoncepostedonaWebsiteitis“forever”incyberspace.

• It may seem inescapable since not going online takes away one of themajorplaceschildrenandadolescentssocialize.

TherecentU.S.DepartmentofJustice(2009)NationalSurveyofChildren’sExposuretoViolencefoundthatInternetharassmentwaslesscommonthanotherformsofbullying.Itwasfoundthat6%reportedInternetharassmentwithinthepastyearand8%duringtheir lifetimes.Whilethismayappearatfirstglanceasmallpercentage,weneedtorememberthatthiswasanationalsurveywithintheUnitedStatesandthatthesepercentagescertainlyrepresentedasignificantnum­berofyouthwhoareimpacted.

Surveys reported by the Pew Foundation (2007) and Hinduja and Patchin(2009) indicate that, although cyberbullying is less common than school bully­ing,anywherebetween15and35%ofyouth reporthavingexperienced it.Thedata also indicate that 10–20% of students admit to cyberbullying others, andgirlsare justas likely, ifnotmore so, tobe involved in this typeofbehaviorasboys(e.g.,Kowalski&Limber,2007).TheseeffectsseemtobeconsistentintheUnitedStates,Australia,andEurope(e.g.,Brandtzæg,Staksrud,Hagen,&Wold,2009).Mostofthesesurveysindicatethatinvolvementincyberbullyingseemstopeakinthemiddleschoolyears(grades6–8).Thereisalsorecentresearchtosug­gestoverlapamongvictimsofschoolbullyingandonlineharassmentbothwithintheUnitedStatesandGermany(e.g.,Wolak,Mitchell,&Finkelhor,2007;Katzer,Fetchenhauer,&Belschak,2009).

Anumberofresearchersarebeginningtoexaminetheplatformforcyberbul­lying.Althoughthisresearchisjustemerging,thereissomeevidencetosuggestthatatleastwithintheUnitedStateschatroomsandinstantmessaging(IM)aremorefrequentlyemployedwhereasemailisthepreferredtechnologyinEurope(Brandzægetal.,2009).

Sexual exploitation Anothermajorconcernwith theInternet isthesex­ual exploitation of children and adolescents. Sending sexual information overemailorpostingsonbulletinboardshasbeenalong­termissue.OneofthemostcomprehensiveseriesstudiesontheseissueshascomefromtheCrimesAgainstChildren Research Center at the University of New Hampshire (http://www.unh.edu/ccrc).This excellent seriesof studies ( Wolak,Finkelhor,Mitchell,&Ybarra,2008)involvedarandomnationalsampleof1,500childrenages10–17interviewedin2000andthenanadditionalsampleof1,500interviewedin2005.Thisprocedureallowedtheresearchestolookatthechangesinyouths’experi­enceswiththeInternet.Themajorfindingsfromthisstudycanbesummarizedasfollows:

Page 293: the Psychology of Social Conflict and Aggression Sydney Symposium in Social Psychology

ed donnerStein276

1.Therewasan increaseover the5­yearperiodfrom25%to34%of theyouthwhoindicatedthattheywereexposedtounwantedsexualmateri­als.ItisinterestingtonotethatthisincreaseoccurredinspiteofthefactthatmorefamilieswereusingInternetfilteringsoftware(over50%)dur­ingthisperiod.AEuropeanstudyof21countries(Livingstone&Haddon,2009)indicatesthatabout40%ofyouthreportexposuretopornography.

2.Atotalof15%ofalloftheyouthreportedanunwantedsexualsolicita­tiononlineinthepreviousyear,with4%reportinganincidentonasocialnetworkingsitespecifically.Perhapsmoreimportantly,about4%ofthesewere considered “aggressive” in that the solicitor attempted to contactthe user offline. These are the episodes most likely to result in actualvictimizations.

3.Additionally,inthisstudy4%ofthosesurveyedwereaskedfornudeorsexuallyexplicitpicturesofthemselves.Ofmoreconcernmaybethefind­ingthatlessthan5%ofthesewerereportedtolawenforcementofficialsortheInternetprovider.Inmanyjurisdictions,theseconstitutecriminalrequeststoproducechildpornography(Wolaketal.,2008).

4.Inthestudy,4%saidtheywereupsetordistressedasaresultoftheseonlinesolicitations.Thesearetheyouthmostimmediatelyharmedbythesolicitationsthemselves.

5.Theseresearchersalsoreportedanincreaseinonlineharassmentandbully­ing.Manyoftheseepisodesoccurfromconfrontationsinschoolfromindivid­ualswhoknoweachother.Mostofthosewhowereharassedwerefemales.

Two interesting questions have been raised about the Internet and sexualexploitation(Wolaketal.,2008;Ybarra&Mitchell,2008).First,doestheInternetmakechildrenmoreaccessible tooffenders?And if offendersparticipate in sexsitesandhaveeasieraccesstochildpornography,doesthis“trigger”theoffense,orwouldithaveoccurredanyway?Bothanswersarespeculative,andadditionalresearchisimperativetomorefullyunderstandthecomplexitiesoftheInternetandchildexploitation.

Withrespecttothefirstquestion,theInternetcanmakechildrenmoreacces­sibletooffendersthroughsocialnetworkingsites,email,andtextinginamannerthatismoreanonymousandoutsidethesupervisionofparents.Childrenmayalsofindthe“privacy”andanonymityofelectroniccommunicationmoreconducivetodiscussionsofintimaterelationshipsthaninaface­to­facesituation.

Withrespecttothesecondquestion,therearesuggestionsthattheInternetcanfacilitatesexualoffendingsuchaspedophiliaviatherapidexchangeofimages,thelocatingofvictims,anddevelopmentofnetworks(e.g,Beech,Elliott,Birgden,&Findlater,2008;McDonald,Horstmann,Strom,&Pope,2009).Therationalesuggestedisasfollows(seeWolaketal.,2008):

• Thereiseasieraccesstochildpornography,whichcanevokeorpromoteinterestsinchildren.

• There are Web sites and Internet groups that explicitly encourage andlegitimizesexualbehaviorswithyouth.

Page 294: the Psychology of Social Conflict and Aggression Sydney Symposium in Social Psychology

the media and aggreSSion 277

• Thereistheinitialanonymityforcontactandsolicitationofachild.Socialnetworkingsitesandchatroomsmakeaccesseasier.

AsWolaketal.(2008)noted,alternativehypothesesshouldbeconsidered.Intheend,muchmorein­depthresearch isneeded,particularly inanareathat isbothnewandoftentimesmoredifficulttoinvestigate.

exposure to violence on the internet Concernsaboutchildrenandado­lescents’useoftheInternetarenotlimitedtosexualcontent.Exposuretoviolentorhatefulcontenthasalsobeenamongthe typesofmaterialsconsideredrisky.AmongthesetypesofcontentareWebsitesforterrorismandotherradicalvio­lentorganizations.Someonlinearchivesprovide instructions formakingbombsorotherweapons.SincetheeventsofSeptember11,terroristgroupsmakeexten­siveuseoftheInternettorecruitandspreadpropaganda.Chapter10inthisvol­umeoffersanexcellentinsightintothistypeofterroristactivity.TheproliferationofhatespeechandhategroupshasalsobecomeeasilyaccessibleontheWeb.AreportbytheSimonWiesenthalCenter(2009)indicatesthatinthepastdecadetherehasbeenatenfoldincreaseofInternet­basedhategroupsthatmakeexten­siveuseofsocialnetworkingsitesforrecruitment.

In an extensive survey of European countries, the EU Kids Online project(Livingstone&Haddon,2009) found that seeingviolentorhateful contentwasexperiencedbyapproximatelyone­thirdofteenagers,makingitoneofthehigherriskconcerns.Oneproblem,however,wasthattheseverityandnatureofthevio­lentcontentencounteredwasnotwellresearched,mainlyforethicalreasons.

Thisisoneofthemajorresearchproblemsinthisarea.ItisdifficultinthesestudiestoseparateoutthecontentonecouldseeofflineanywayandsimplyusetheInternetasamediumforviewing.ItistruethattheInternetallowschildrenandadolescentseasieraccesstomaterialswealreadyconsiderriskrelated(violenceonTVandfilm,videogameviolence),butamoreimportantquestionistherolethatmaterial“unique”totheInternetmightplayinaggressivebehaviors.Theresearchinthisareaislimited,andassomehavesuggestedwedonotreallyyetknowhowmanyyouthare“intentionally”viewingviolentWebsitesorarebeingexposedtographicrealisticviolence(Hamburgeretal.,2009).Aswealreadynotedregardingchildpredators,wearestillinneedoffurtherresearchontheuniqueroleoftheInternetcontentintheseareas.

However,somerecentresearchdoessuggestthatthetypesofmaterialsfoundexclusively on the Internet may have a relationship to aggressive behavior. In anational surveyofyouth,Ybarraetal. (2008) foundanassociationbetween theviewingofInternetviolenceandself­reportedseriouslyaggressivebehavior.Whileexposuretoviolenceinthemediaoverallwasrelatedtoaggressivebehavior,youthwhoreportedthatmanyoralloftheWebsitestheyvisiteddepictedrealpeoplefighting,shooting,orkillingwerefivetimesmorelikelytoreportengaginginseri­ouslyviolentbehavior.ThesetypesofsitesseemedtobeuniquetotheInternetandincluded(1)hatesites,(2)Websitesshowingpicturesofdeadpeopleorpeopledying,ora“snuff”site,(3)Websitesshowingsatanicrituals,(4)Websitesshowingpicturesofwar,death,or“terrorism,”or(5)Websitesshowingcartoons,suchas

Page 295: the Psychology of Social Conflict and Aggression Sydney Symposium in Social Psychology

ed donnerStein278

stickpeopleoranimals,beingbeatenup,hurt,orkilled.TheauthorsspeculatedthattheinteractiveenvironmentoftheInternetandthedepictionofrealpeopleengagedinviolencemayexplainthestrongerassociationwithreportedseriouslyviolentbehavior.Weneedtokeepinmindthatthisisacross­sectionaldesignstudyanddoesnotestablishcausality.

AnothernationalsurveyconductedinTaiwanalsosuggestssomeuniquecon­tributionoftheInternettoyouthaggression.Inasurveyofover9,000adolescents,Ko,Yen,Liua,Huang,andYen(2009)foundthatheavyusersoftheInternetweremorelikelytoself­reportaggressivebehaviorduringthepastyear.Thiswasthecaseaftercontrollingfortheviewingofviolenttelevisionprograms.Thisreportedaggressionalsooccurredmoreofteninstudentswhowereinvolvedinonlinechat­ting, adult sex Web sites, online gaming, online gambling, and bulletin boardsystems.Theauthorssuggestthattheselateractivitiesofferbothanonymityandgroupidentification.

Another concern, suggested earlier, is the proliferation and access to hategroupsandotherpotentiallyviolentorganizationsthroughWebsites,chatrooms,andotherInternetplatformsthathavethepotentialtorecruit,organize,andrein­forceindividualsforaggressive­relatedbehaviors.InhisbookondemocracyandtheInternet,Republic.com,Sunstein(2001)acknowledgestherisksweencounterwithanopenanduncensoredInternet.UsingoneexampleforthegroupUnorganizedMilitias,henoted:

AcrucialfactorbehindthegrowthoftheUnorganizedMilitiahasbeentheuse of computer networks, allowing members to make contact quickly andeasilywithlike­mindedindividualstotradeinformation,discusscurrentcon­spiracytheories,andorganizeevents.(p.22)

Anumberofexcellentdiscussionsinthisvolumeonin­groupattachments(seeChapters2and10)speaktoSunstein’s(2001)assertion.Thequestionconsideredearlier, however, about theuniqueness of the Internet from offline exposure toviolentmaterialscanalsoberaisedwithregardtohateorradicalgroups.Whilethereisanincreaseintheproliferationofthesegroupsaswellas(1)examinationsoftheircontent(e.g.,Douglas,McGarty,Bliuc,&Lala,2005)and(2)speculationof their influences, there is little systematic research on the specific influencesoftheseonlinesitesanddiscussiongroupsforsubsequentofflinebehaviors.Inarecentreviewofthisliterature,McDonald,Horstmann,Strom,andPope(2009)notedthattheefficacyofWeb­basedhategroupsisstillunclear.

ArecentstudybyWojcieszak(2009)doessuggestthatparticipationinradicalonlinegroups,suchasneo­Nazis,increasesofflineactionsthatsupportneo­Nazimovementsaswellasinactionsthatpromotethesemovements.Thisstudycer­tainlyhasanumberoflimitationssuchascausality,self­selection,andvalidityofbehavioraloutcomes.Nevertheless,itexaminestheroleoftheInternetasanalter­nativetotraditionalface­to­facesocializationinunderanalyzedcommunities.

Sexual violence WhileitisimportanttoexaminetheuniquecharacteristicsofInternetcontent,weshouldnotsummarilydismisstheinadvertent(orperhaps

Page 296: the Psychology of Social Conflict and Aggression Sydney Symposium in Social Psychology

the media and aggreSSion 279

intentional) exposure to materials children or adolescents would have difficultyviewingoffline.Oneconcern raisedabout childrenandadolescents’ interactionwiththeWebistheinadvertentexposurenotonlytoextremeformsofviolencebutalsotosexualviolence(e.g.,Donnerstein,2009;Feilitzen,2009;Strasburgeretal.,2010).Todate,therehasbeenvirtuallynoresearchontheeffectsofexpo­sure to sexualviolenceonadolescentviewers, althoughresearchershavespecu­latedonitsimpact(e.g.,Malamuth&Impett,2001;Donnerstein&Smith2001;Wright,Malamuth,&Donnerstein,2010).Forethical reasons these studiesarenearlyimpossibletoundertake.However,numerousstudiesinvolvingcollege­agestudentshaverevealedthatdepictionsofsexualviolenceinthemediacanpromoteantisocial attitudesandbehavior.Given that someof this researchhas involvedR­ratedfilms,thereiseveryexpectationthatadolescentsandchildrenwouldbeexposedtothesetypesofmaterialsviatheInternet.Particularlydetrimentalareviolentimagesinpornographyandelsewherethatportraythemyththatwomenenjoyorinsomewaybenefitfromrape,torture,orotherformsofsexualviolence(e.g.,Donnerstein,2000,2008;Harris,2009).Ifanything,wemightexpectevenstrongereffectsofsuchcontentonyoungerviewerswhomaylackthenecessarycriticalviewingskillsandtheexperiencetodiscounttheseportrayals.Toanado­lescentwho is searching theWeb for informationabout relationships, the inad­vertentexposuretosexualviolencemaybeapotentsourceofinfluenceoninitialattitudestowardsexuality.

Summary Inmanyways the issueof Internetviolence isperhapsataplacethatvideogameviolencewasadecadeago.ThereisagooddealofspeculationandtheoreticalassumptionstoassumethattheInternetwillbeasubstantialfactorinthedevelopmentofaggression.Whatbroughtvideogameviolencetotheforefrontwassolidempiricalandtheory­drivenresearch(e.g.,Andersonetal.,2007,2010).WearebeginningtoseethiswithintherealmofresearchontheInternetandvio­lence.Inconsideringthefutureofthisresearch,anumberofindividualsreview­ingthisresearchacrossvariouscountrieshavepointedtoareasthatneedspecificconsideration(e.g.,Livingstone&Haddon,2009;McDonaldetal.,2009).Someoftheserecommendationsareasfollows:

1.Longitudinalresearchtoexaminethecausalrelationshipsbetweenonlineparticipationandengagingincriminalacts

2.The major risk factors (i.e., individual, environmental, social) that arerelatedtosomeone“acting”onthisInternetexposure

3.GiventheincreasinguseoftheInternetbyyoungerchildren(undertheageof12),thereisaneedforspecificresearchonthispopulation.Inpar­ticulararestudiesonthoseintheunder­6­year­oldrangewhowillhavelesscapacityto“cope”withriskieronlinecontent.

4.Researchonexpandingplatforms likemobilephonesandvirtualgameenvironmentsaswellaspeer­to­peerexchanges

5.Increasedresearchonpublichealthissueslikeself­harm,suicide,drugs,andaddiction

Page 297: the Psychology of Social Conflict and Aggression Sydney Symposium in Social Psychology

ed donnerStein280

concluSionInthischapterwesetouttoaddresstheissuessurroundingtheeffectsofexposuretomediaviolenceonprimarilychildrenandadolescents.Thisisnotanewendeavor,asmanyinthepsychologicalcommunityhavewrittenaboutthesevaryingeffectsfordecades(e.g.,Andersonetal.,2010;Huessman,2007).Formany,therehasbeentheoverallassumptionthatexposuretomassmediaviolencecanbeinfluentialinthebehaviorandattitudesofchildren,adolescents,andevenadults.Myintentinthisreviewwasnottoreiteratewhathasbeenfocusedonin thepastbutrathertoexpandthediscussiontonewertechnologies,inparticulartheInternet.Inthismanner the focusbecomesoneonmediatedviolenceandexamines the varyingmediumsyouthhaveattheirdisposalforbeingexposedtowhatwewouldconsiderrisk­relatedcontent.

Inreflectingonthisbriefreview,itwouldbesafetoconcludethatthemassmedia,inallitsdomains,isacontributortoanumberofantisocialbehaviorsandhealth­relatedproblemsinchildrenandadolescents.Wemustkeepinmind,how­ever,thatthemassmediaisbutoneofamultitudeoffactorsthatcontributeand,inmanycases,isnotalwaysthemostsignificant.Nevertheless,itisoneofthefactorsinwhichproper interventionscanmitigate its impactand, furthermore, canbecontrolledwithreasonableinsight(Strasburgeretal.,2010).

Unlikethemoretraditionalmediumsforexposuretomediaviolence,thereisgeneralagreementthatconsiderablymoreresearchisneededwithregardtotheInternetinitsroleasatechnologyforthelearning,social,andcognitivedevelop­mentofchildrenandadolescents.Thereisnoquestionthatweneedtoenrichourunderstandingofthesenewtechnologiesasmoreandmorechildrencomeonlineandthetechnologyitselfchangesandexpands.

WhenthinkingaboutthesenewertechnologiesweshouldkeepinmindwhatHuesmann(2007)notedaboutthedecadesofresearchandtheoryontraditionalmedia.Thisextensiveresearchandtheorydevelopmenthasprovideduswithsig­nificant insights into the role new technology will play in the development andmitigationofaggressivebehavior.Assomehavesaid,“Thetechnologyconduitmaybechanging,buttheinfluentialprocesses(e.g.,priming,activationanddesensiti­zation)maybethesame”(Ferdon&Hertz,2007,p.55).

referenceS

American Academy of Pediatrics (AAP). (2009a). Policy statement—media violence.Council on Communications and Media. http://www.pediatrics.org/cgi/doi/10.1542/peds.2009­2146doi:10.1542/peds.2009­2146

AmericanAcademyofPediatrics.(AAP).(2009b).Policy statement—role of the pediatrician in youth violence. CommitteeonInjury,Violence,andPoisonPrevention.http://www.pediatrics.org/cgi/doi/10.1542/peds.2009­0943doi:10­1542/peds.2009­0943

Anderson, C. A. (2000). Violent video games increase aggression and violence. U.S.Senate CommitteeonCommerce,Science,andTransportationhearingontheimpactofinteractiveviolenceonchildren,March21.

Page 298: the Psychology of Social Conflict and Aggression Sydney Symposium in Social Psychology

the media and aggreSSion 281

Anderson,C.A.(2004).Anupdateontheeffectsofplayingviolentvideogames.Journal of Adolescence, 27,113–122.

Anderson,C.A.(2007).The bottom line on violent video games: What we know and what we don’t know.PaperpresentedatthemeetingoftheAmericanAcademyofPediatrics.

Anderson,C.,Berkowitz,L.,Donnerstein,E.,Huesmann,R.,Johnson,J.,Linz,D.,etal.(2003).Theinfluenceofmediaviolenceonyouth.Psychological Science in the Public Interest, 4,81–110.

Anderson,C.A.,&Bushman,B. J. (2001).Effectsofviolentvideogamesonaggressivebehavior,aggressivecognition,aggressiveaffect,physiologicalarousal,andprosocialbehavior:Ameta­analyticreviewofthescientificliterature.Psychological Science, 12,353–359.

Anderson,C.A.,&Bushman,B.J.(2002).Humanaggression.Annual Review of Psychology, 53,27–51.

Anderson,C.A.,&Huesmann,L.R.(2003).Humanaggression:Asocial–cognitiveview.In M. A. Hogg & J. Cooper (Eds.), The Sage handbook of social psychology (pp.296–323).London,England:Sage.

Anderson,C.A.,Gentile,D.A.,&Buckley,K.E. (2007).Violent video game effects on children and adolescents.Oxford,UK:OxfordUniversityPress.

Anderson,C. A.,Sakamoto, A.,Gentile, D. A., Ihori, N.,Shibuya, A.,Yukawa,S., et al.(2008).Longitudinaleffectsofviolentvideogamesonaggression inJapanand theUnitedStates.Pediatrics, 122,1067–1072.

Anderson,C.A.,Shibuya,A.,Ihori,N.,Swing,E.L.,Bushman,B.J.,Sakamoto,A.,etal.(2010).Violentvideogameeffectsonaggression,empathy,andprosocialbehaviorineasternandwesterncountries:Ameta­analyticreview.Psychological Bulletin,136(2),151–73.

Beech,A.R.,Elliott,I.A.,Birgden,A.,&Findlater,D.(2008).TheInternetandchildsexualoffending:Acriminologicalreview.Aggression and Violent Behavior, 13,216–228.

Bluemke,M.,Friedrich,M.,&Zumbach,J.(2010).Theinfluenceofviolentandnonvio­lent computer games on implicit measures of aggressiveness. Aggressive Behavior, 36, 1–13.

Boxer,P.,Huesmann,L.R.,Bushman,B.J.,O’Brien,M.,&Moceri,D.(2009).Theroleofviolentmediapreferencesincumulativedevelopmentalriskforviolenceandgeneralaggression.Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 38,417–428.

Brandtzæg,P.B.,Staksrud,E.,Hagen,I.,&Wold,W.(2009).Norwegianchildren’sexpe­riences of Cyberbullying when using different technological platforms. Journal of Children and Media, 3,349–365.

Bushman, B. J., & Anderson, C. A. (2009). Comfortably numb: Desensitizing effects ofviolentmediaonhelpingothers.Psychological Science, 20,273–277.

Bushman,B.J.,Boxer,P.,Johnson,T.,Huesmann,R.,O’Brien,M.,&Moceri,D.(2008).Relation of young adult violent and criminal behavior to habitual exposure to violent media during childhood and adolescence.PresentedatthemeetingoftheInternationalSocietyforResearchonAggression.Budapest,Hungary.

Bushman,B. J.,&Huesmann,L.R. (2006).Short­termandlong­termeffectsofviolentmedia on aggression in children and adults. Archives of Pediatrics & Adolescent Medicine, 160,348–352.

Bushman,B.J.,Huesmann,L.R.,&Whitaker,J.(2009).Violentmediaeffect.InM.B.Oliver&R.Nabi(Eds.),The Sage handbook of media processes and effects(pp.361–376).ThousandOaks,CA:Sage.

Calvete,E.,Orue,I.,Estevez,L.V.,Garcia,A.,Mendiola,J.,Garramiola,J.,etal.(2008).Violence through mobile phones in adolescents. Presented at the meeting of theInternationalSocietyforResearchonAggression.Budapest,Hungary.

Page 299: the Psychology of Social Conflict and Aggression Sydney Symposium in Social Psychology

ed donnerStein282

Carnagey,N.L.,&Anderson,C.A.(2005).Theeffectsofrewardandpunishmentinviolentvideogamesonaggressiveaffect,cognition,andbehavior.Psychological Science, 16,882–889.

CrimesAgainstChildrenResearchCenter.(CCRC).(2006).Second youth Internet safety survey(YISS­2).Availablefromhttp://www.unh.edu/ccrc

Donnerstein,E.(2000).Pornography:research,theory,implications.InA.E.Kazdin(Ed.),Encyclopedia of Psychology (pp.788–795).NewYork:OxfordUniversityPress.

Donnerstein,E.(2008).Massmedia:Ageneralview.InL.Kurtz(Ed.),Encyclopedia of violence, peace, and conflict(2nded.,pp.1184–1192).Oxford:Elsevier.

DonnersteinE.(2009).TheInternet.InV.Strasburger,B.Wilson,&A.Jordan,Children, ado-lescents and the media(2nded.,pp.471–498).ThousandOaks,CA:Sage.

Donnerstein,E.,&Smith,S. (2001).Sex inthemedia:Theory, influencesandsolutions.InD.Singer&J.Singer(Eds.),Handbook of children and the media (pp.289–308).ThousandOaks,CA:Sage.

Douglas,K.M.,McGarty,C.,Bliuc,A.,&Lala,G.(2005).Understandingcyberhate:Socialcompetitionandsocialcreativityinonlinewhitesupremacistsgroups.Social Science Computer Review, 23,68–76.

Feilitzen,C.(2009).Influences of mediated violence: A brief research summary.Guthenberg,Sweden:InternationalClearinghouseonChildren,YouthandMedia,NORDICOM,UniversityofGothenburg.

Ferdon, C. D., & Hertz, M. F. (2007). Electronic media, violence, and adolescents: Anemergingpublichealthproblem.Journal of Adolescent Health 41,S1–S5.

Ferguson,C. J. (2010). Ameta­analysisofnormal and disorderedpersonality across thelifespan.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 98(4),659–667.

Gentile,D.A.,&Anderson,C.A.(2003).Violentvideogames:Thenewestmediaviolencehazard.InD.Gentile(Ed.),Media violence and children (pp.131–152),Westport,CT:Praeger.

Hamburger,M.,Ybarra,M.,Leaf,P.,&West,M.(2009).Violence on the Internet: How many youth are really looking? Presented at the annual meeting of the AmericanSocietyofCriminology.

Harris, R. J. (2009). A cognitive psychology of mass communication (RoutledgeCommunicationSeries).London:Taylor&Francis.

Heirman,W.,&Walrave,M.(2008).Assessingconcernsandissuesaboutthemediationoftechnologyincyberbullying.Cyberpsychology: Journal of Psychosocial Research on Cyberspace,2.Availablefromhttp://www.cyberpsychology.eu/index.php

Hinduja, S., & Patchin, J. W. (2009).  Bullying beyond the schoolyard: Preventing, and responding to cyberbullying.ThousandOaks,CA:SagePublications(CorwinPress).

Horiuchi,Y.,Minamisawa,U.,Shibuya,A.,Suzuki,K.,Sakamoto,A.,Linz,D.,etal.(2008).Analysis of violent content in Japanese video games. PresentedatthemeetingoftheInternationalSocietyforResearchonAggression.Budapest,Hungary.

Huesmann,L.R. (2007).The impactofelectronicmediaviolence:Scientific theoryandresearch.Journal of Adolescent Health, 41,S6–S13.

Huesmann,L.R.,Boxer,P.,&Bushman,B.(2009).The relation between exposure to video violence in childhood and serious youth violence and delinquency. PresentedattheannualmeetingoftheAmericanSocietyofCriminology.Atlanta,Georgia.

Huesmann,L.R.,&Kirwil,L. (2007).Whyobservingviolence increasestheriskofvio­lentbehaviorbytheobserver. InD.J.Flannery,A.T.Vazsonyi,&I.D.Waldman(Eds.),The Cambridge handbook of violent behavior and aggression(pp.545–570).Cambridge,England:CambridgeUniversityPress.

Huesmann,L.R.,Moise,J.,Podolski,C.P.,&Eron,L.D.(2003).Longitudinalrelationsbetweenchildren’sexposuretoTVviolenceandtheiraggressiveandviolentbehaviorinyoungadulthood: 1977–1992.Developmental Psychology, 39,201–221.

Page 300: the Psychology of Social Conflict and Aggression Sydney Symposium in Social Psychology

the media and aggreSSion 283

Huesmann, L. R., & Taylor, L. D. (2006). Media effects in middle childhood. In A. C.Huston&M.N.Ripke(Eds.),Developmental contexts in middle childhood: Bridges to adolescence and adulthood (pp. 303–326). Cambridge, England: CambridgeUniversityPress.

Journal of Adolescent Health(2007).Volume41,Supplement:YouthViolenceandElectronicMedia:SimilarBehaviors,DifferentVenues?

KaiserFamilyFoundation.(2006).The media family: Electronic media in the lives of infants, toddlers, preschoolers, and their parents.MenloPark,CA:Author.

KaiserFamilyFoundation.(2010).Generation M2: Media in the lives of 8- to 18-year-olds.MenloPark,CA:Author.

Katzer,C.,Fetchenhauer,D.,&Belschak,F.(2009).Cyberbullying—whoarethevictims?A comparison of victimization in Internet chatrooms and victimization in school.Journal of Media Psychology, 21, 25–36.

Ko,C.H.,Yen,J.Y.,Liua,S.C.,Huang,C.F.,&Yen,C.F.(2009).Theassociationsbetweenaggressive behaviors and Internet addiction and online activities in adolescents.Journal of Adolescent Health, 44,598–605.

Kowalski,R.M.,&Limber,S.P.(2007).Electronicbullyingamongmiddleschoolstudents.Journal of Adolescent Health, 41,S22–S30.

Livingstone, S., & Haddon, L. (2009). EU Kids Online: Final report. London: EU KidsOnline.

Malamuth,N.,&Impett,E.A.(2001).Researchonsex inthemedia.InD.Singer&J.Singer(Eds.),Handbook of children and the media (pp.269–288).ThousandOaks,CA:Sage.

McDonald,H.S.,Horstmann,N.,Strom,K.J.,&Pope,M.W.(2009).The impact of the Internet on deviant behavior and deviant communities.TheInstituteforHomelandSecuritySolutions.Availablefromhttp://www.ihssnc.org

National Crime Prevention Council (NCPC). (2009). Homepage. Available from http://www.ncpc.org/

Olweus,D.(1993).Bullying at school: What we know and what we can do.Malden,MA:Blackwell.

PewFoundation.(2007).The Pew Internet Project: Teens online.Philadelphia,PA:ThePewCharitableTrusts.

PewFoundation.(2009).The Pew Internet & American Life Project.Philadelphia,PA:ThePewCharitableTrusts.

SimonWiesenthalCenter.(2009).Homepage.Availablefromhttp://www.wiesenthal.comSmith,S.,&Donnerstein,E.(2003).Theproblemofexposure:Violence,sex,drugs,and

alcohol.InD.Ravitch&J.Viteritti(Eds.),Kid stuff: Marketing sex and violence to America’s children (pp.65–95). Baltimore:JohnsHopkinsUniversityPress.

Strasburger,V.C.,Jordan,A.B.,&Donnerstein,E.(2010).Healtheffectsofmediaonchil­drenandadolescents.Pediatrics,125,756–767.

Strasburger,V.C.,Wilson,B.,&Jordan,A. (2009).Children, adolescents and the media(2nded.).ThousandOaks,CA:Sage.

Sunstein,C.R.(2001).Republic.com.Princeton,NJ:PrincetonUniversityPress.SurgeonGeneraloftheUnitedStates.(2001).Youth violence: A report of the surgeon gen-

eral.Washington,DC:DepartmentofHealthandHumanServices,U.S.GovernmentPrintingOffice.

Tajima,S.,Suzuki,K.,Sado,M.,Hasegawa,Y.,Horiuchi,Y.,Linz,D.,etal.(2008).Contextual features of violence depiction in television advertisings.PresentedatthemeetingoftheInternationalSocietyforResearchonAggression.Budapest,Hungary.

U.S.Departmentof Justice. (2009).National Survey of Children’s Exposure to Violence. OfficeofJuvenileJusticeandDelinquencyPrevention,Washington,DC.

Page 301: the Psychology of Social Conflict and Aggression Sydney Symposium in Social Psychology

ed donnerStein284

Williams,K.R.,&Guerra,N.G.(2007).PrevalenceandpredictorsofInternetbullying.Journal of Adolescent Health, 41,S14–S21.

Wilson, B., Smith, S., Potter, J. Kunkel, D., Linz, D., Colvin, C., & Donnerstein, E.(2002).Violenceinchildren’stelevisionprogramming:Assessingtherisks.Journal of Communication, 52, 5–35.

Wojcieszak, M. (2009). “Carrying online participation offline”—mobilization by radicalonline groups and politically dissimilar offline ties. Journal of Communication, 59,564–586.

Wolak,J.,Finkelhor,D.,Mitchell,K.J.,&Ybarra,M.L.(2008).Online“predators”andtheirvictims: Myths, realities, and implications for prevention and treatment. American Psychologist, 63,111–128.

Wolak,J.,Mitchell,K.J.,&Finkelhor,D.(2007).Doesonlineharassmentconstitutebul­lying?Anexplorationofonlineharassmentbyknownpeersandonline­onlycontacts.Journal of Adolescent Health, 41,S51–8.

Wright,P.,Malamuth,N.,&Donnerstein,E.(2010).Researchonsexinthemedia:Whatdoweknowabouteffectsonchildrenandadolescents?InD.Singer&J.Singer(Eds.),Handbook of children and the media(2nded.).ThousandOaks,CA:Sage.

Ybarra,M.L.,&Mitchell,K.J.(2008).Howriskyaresocialnetworkingsites?Acomparisonofplacesonlinewhereyouth sexual solicitationandharassmentoccurs. Pediatrics, 121,350–357.

Ybarra,M.L.,West,M.D.,Markow,D.,Leaf,P.J.,Hamburger,M.,&Boxer,P.(2008).LinkagesbetweenInternetandothermediaviolencewithseriouslyviolentbehaviorbyyouth.Pediatrics, 122,929–937.

Page 302: the Psychology of Social Conflict and Aggression Sydney Symposium in Social Psychology

285

18AreSupernaturalBeliefs

CommitmentDevicesforIntergroupConflict?

ROBERTKURZBANandJOHNCHRISTNERUniversity of Pennsylvania

A rguablythemostimportantpoliticaleventofthealbeitstillyoungtwenty­firstcenturywasacaseofintergroupconflictinwhichsupernaturalbeliefsplayedapivotalrole.TheattackontheWorldTradeCenterinNewYork

City,thePentagoninWashington,D.C.,andthefoiledattackbythehijackersofUnitedAirlinesFlight93onSeptember11,2001,wasmotivatedbyintergroupcon­flictbutwasmadepossibleinnosmallpartbecausetheperpetratorshadbeliefsabouttheafterlife.Whilewedonotattemptheretosortoutthemanycausalante­cedentsofthisattack,whichareundoubtedlycomplex(seealsoChapters2,10,and16inthisvolume),wedoproposeanexplanationforthebroaderphenomenon:whypeopleentertainsupernaturalbeliefsandtheirrelationshiptointergroupconflict.

introductionTruebeliefsareuseful, somuchso thatphilosophershavearguedthattheonlythingmindsaregoodforis“thefixationoftruebeliefs”(Fodor,2000,p.68),senti­mentsthathavebeenechoedbyothers(e.g.,Dennett,1987;Millikan,1984;forarecentdiscussion,seeMcKay&Dennett,2009).Thegeneralideaisintuitiveandcompelling:truebeliefsaidinaccomplishinggoalsand,withappropriateinferencemachines,ingeneratingadditionaltruebeliefs.

Symmetrically,falsebeliefsare,ingeneral,lessuseful.Actingonthebasisofbeliefsthatdonotcapturesomethingtrueabouttheworldcanleadtoanynumberofbadoutcomes.Falsebeliefsaboutwhat is edible can lead topoisoning, falsebeliefsaboutwhatissharpcanleadtocuts,andsoon.Falsesupernaturalbeliefs,

Page 303: the Psychology of Social Conflict and Aggression Sydney Symposium in Social Psychology

roBert kurzBan and John chriStner286

asWright(2009)recentlydocumented,causetheirbearerstoengageinanarrayof costly behaviors, including enduring—even self­inflicting—severe harm and,fromanevolutionary standpoint, themostcostly choiceofall,electing to forgoreproduction(seealsoIannaccone,1992).

Inlightofthesearguments,onewouldexpectminds—absentsomeselectiveforce—tobedesignedtoresistadoptingfalsebeliefs.Thereare,however,impor­tant exceptions. Consider binary decisions suchas fleeingor notfleeing from apotentialpredatorinwhichthecostsoferrors(misses,falsealarms)andtheben­efitsofbeingcorrect(hits,correctrejections)areasymmetrical.Insuchcases,ifthesystemisforcedtoadoptonebelieforotherandtoactonthebasisofthebelief,selection will not favor maximizing the probability of true belief; it will rathermaximizeexpectedvalue(Cosmides&Tooby,1987;Green&Swets,1966).Thatis,ifweassumethattheremustbeabeliefeitherthatthepredatorispresentorthatitisabsent(asopposedtosomeprobabilisticrepresentation),thenevenweakevidenceshouldgiverisetothe(likelyfalse)beliefthatthepredatorispresentsothattheappropriateaction(i.e.,fleeing)canbetaken.

Thisprincipleisreflectedinthedesignofbothhumanartifacts,suchasthesmoke detector, and human physiology (Nesse, 2001, 2005; Nesse & Williams,1994).Asmokedetectorcannotsignalthattheremightbeafire,soitsignalsthatit“believes”thereisoneevenonscantevidence.Inhumans,all­or­nonedefensessuchastheimmunesystem(Nesse,2001)reflectthesameidea.

Thisprinciplegovernsthedesignofevolvedmechanismsforinferencesaboutthestateoftheworldacrossanynumberofdomains.AsWiley(1994)putit,ratherthan maximize percent correct, “basic decision theory suggests that a criterionshould maximize the expected utility for the receiver…” (p. 172). Wiley shows,usingastandardsignaldetectionanalysis,thatselectioncanfavor“adaptivegull­ibility”(i.e.,erringonthesideoffalsepositivesinthecontextofmating)and“adap­tive fastidiousness” (i.e.,erringonthesideofmisses in thecontextofdetectingprey).Thepropensityforerror—false“beliefs”aboutwhatisandisnotamateorprey—isbuilt into thesemechanismsbecauseselectionwill sift indesignspacefordesignsthatmaximizefitnessratherthanaccuracy.Thisisastrueforevolvedhumansystemsasitisforotherorganisms’systems(Haselton&Buss,2000;Nesse&Williams,1994;Tooby&Cosmides,1987).

Thereisasecondimportantselectionpressurethatcancounteractthetendencyforevolutiontofavortruth­preservingbeliefsystems.Thispressurearisesinthecontextofstrategicinteractions,inwhichindividuals’payoffsareaffectedbyoth­ers’actions(vonNeumann&Morgenstern,1944;Smith,1982).Toseethepotentialadvantagesoffalsebeliefs,denotepasthetruestateoftheworldandp*asafalsebelief(p≠p*).Supposeegoisbetteroffintermsofsocialadvantagesifeveryonebelievedp*ratherthanp.(Supposep*isthategoishighlyintelligent,forexample.)Supposefurtherthatego,byherselfbelievingp*,increasesthechancesthatotherswilladoptp*.(Weassumethat“genuine”beliefcanhaveadvantagesoversimplydissembling,perhapsbyvirtueoftheprobabilityofpersuasion;Trivers,2000.)Insuchacase,byvirtueoftheeffectsp*hasonothers’behavior,itcanbeadvanta­geousforegotobelievep*(Nesse&Lloyd,1992;Trivers,2000).So­calledpositiveillusions(Taylor&Brown,1988)mightbesuchcases,inwhichfalsepositivebeliefs

Page 304: the Psychology of Social Conflict and Aggression Sydney Symposium in Social Psychology

are Supernatural BeliefS commitment deviceS? 287

aboutoneselfcanaidinpersuadingotherstoadoptthisstrategicallyadvantageousbeliefp*(Kurzban&Aktipis,2006,2007).Systemscancometobedesignedtogenerateandadoptp*’saslongasthecostsofthefalsebeliefdonotoutweighthestrategicbenefits(Kurzban,inpress).

Itisimportanttobearinmindboththepowerandthelimitsofthistypeofargument.Putativecasesofdesigntobringabout falsebeliefsmustrespect thedistinctionbetween,ononehand,whenthedecisiononemakes inandof itselfdeterminesone’spayoffand,ontheotherhand,whenthedecisiononemakesandwhatonecommunicatestootheragentsaffectsone’spayoff.

Thedistinctionisimportantbecausetherelentlesscalculusofdecisiontheoryandnaturalselectionpunishesmechanismsthatdonotmaximizeexpectedvalue.Holdingasidewhatiscommunicatedtoanotherindividual—andtherebypotentiallychanginghisorherbehaviorand,inconsequence,thedecisionmaker’sdownstreampayoff—amechanismthatmaximizesexpectedvaluecannotbebeaten.(Maximizingexpectedvalueis,ofcourse,notthesameasmaximizingpercentcorrect,asindicatedalready.)

Substantialconfusionsurroundsthispoint.Forexample,considertheputativebenefitsofbeing“too”optimistic.Systemsthatgenerateerrorsthatcauseonetotrymorethanone“should”—giventheexpectedvalueoftryingornottrying—willlosetheevolutionarygametosystemsthatmaximizeexpectedvalue.Thereisnowayaroundthis.Thecontemporaryemphasison“positivethinking”mayalsoinvolveavarietyofsignificantcostsforsimilarreasons(seealsoChapter8inthisvolume).

Somemodelsalsopurporttoshowthaterrorcanbeadvantageousevenwithoutconsiderationofthestrategicadvantagesofinfluencingothers’behavior.However,thesemodelssucceedonlybecausetheyartificiallypenalizestrategiesthatmaxi­mizeexpectedvalue.Nettle(2004),forexample,modelsadecisioninwhichcom­municationplaysnorole,soanalgorithmthatmaximizesexpectedvaluecannotbebeatenbyanyotherstrategywithoutgivingnonmaximizershelp.Inthemodel,“optimists”—whooverestimatethechanceofsuccess—aregivenexactlysuchhelp:themodel’s“rational”(nonoptimistic)agentsrelyonandusecompletelyinaccurateestimatesof the chanceof success.When (rational) agentshaveno informationatallaboutthechanceofsuccess,theyshouldusethedecision­theoreticcorrectestimateof.5inmakingtheirdecision.Itistruethatwhentheexpectedpayoffof trying ishigher than theexpectedcostof failing, then“optimists”arebetteroffthanthe“rational”agents(andsymmetricallyforpessimists;seealsoHaselton&Nettle,2006),but themodel’s “optimists”and“pessimists”winonlybecausetheythrowoutthemisleadinginformationthatthe“rational”agentsdonot.Asweexploresubsequently,oneprominentmodelofsupernaturalpunishmentrunsintothisproblemaswell.

Outsideofcasessuchasthese,asfarasweknow(McKay&Dennett,2009),inwhichthereisanadvantagetoerrorbecauseofconsiderationsofdecisiontheoryorthevalueofthecommunicativeeffectofone’sdecisions,onewouldnotexpecttofindmechanismsdesignedtoadoptfalsebeliefs.Further,onewouldexpecthumancomputationalarchitecturetobedesignedtorejectfalsebeliefs,giventheirpoten­tialcosts.

Fromthisperspective,thefactthathumansseemtohavemechanismsthatendorse supernatural beliefs—which are (by assumption) guaranteed to be

Page 305: the Psychology of Social Conflict and Aggression Sydney Symposium in Social Psychology

roBert kurzBan and John chriStner288

false—ispuzzling.Firstisthebarefactthathumansseemnotjustdisposedbutalsopositivelyeager toendorse supernaturalbeliefs (Dawkins,2006;Dennett,2006). Second, these beliefs seem to have high costs. Even holding aside therelationshipbetween supernatural beliefs and intergroup conflict—the subjecthere—supernatural beliefs seem to play a large role in any number of costlybehaviors.Thiswould include things like time­consuming (butuseless)prayer,buildingmonumentstononexistentgods,sacrificinggoatsorotheranimalswith­outconsumingthem,doingraindances,andtakingrisksbecauseofpredictionsofdivineintervention.

So,holdingasidethetwopreviousarguments,selectionshould,everythingelseequal,haveeliminatedbelief­generationmechanismsthathadthepropertyofgen­eratingandacquiringsupernaturalbeliefs.Why,then,aresupernaturalbeliefssopervasiveinourspecies?

theorieS of Supernatural BeliefManyscholarshaveaddressedtheissueoftheoriginofsupernaturalbelief.Herewediscussonlyafewprominentmodels,which,broadly,fallintotwoclasses.Thefirst class is by­product explanations. On this view, humans have mechanismsdesignedtoconstruct,transmit,andacquirerepresentationsforonefunction,andsupernaturalbeliefsemergeasasideeffectofthewaythesesystemsoperate.Wereviewthesefirstandthenturntothesecondpossibility:thatthemechanismsthatgeneratesupernaturalbeliefsaredesignedforpreciselythisfunction.

By-product Views

Oneofthemostprominentby­productmodelsofsupernaturalbeliefsbeginswiththebroad idea thatpeople transmit informationsocially.People learnfromoneanotherinpartbecausetherearetremendouscostsavingsinsociallyratherthanindividuallylearninginformation(Boyd&Richerson,1985).Further,givensociallearning,itfollowsthat,byvirtueofthewaythatlearningmechanismsoperate,some kinds of ideas, beliefs, and practices will be more likely to be generated,recalled,andtransmittedthanothers(e.g.,Sperber,1985).Thisisanaturalconse­quenceofanysociallearningsystem,andthisideaiseasilyseeninthedomainoflanguage,inwhichvariousrulesconstrainthegrammarentertainedbylanguagelearners(Pinker,1994).

Fromthis,itfollowsthat,byanevolutionaryprocess,certainideaswilltendtopersistandbeobservedovertimemorethanothers.Ideasthatare“sticky,”havingproperties thatmake themmemorableand transmitted (Bartlett,1932),willbeobservedmorethanthosethatdonot“fit”withhumancognition.

Oneofthemajormodelssurroundingsupernaturalbeliefs—the“ontologicalheresy”(OH)model—beginswiththisideaandturnsononeimportantelementoflearningsystems:thatthereseemtobecategoriesofentitiesthatthemindispre­paredtolearnabout.Eachofthesecategoriescomeswithasetofdefiningchar­acteristicsthatapplytoallentrieswithinit,sowhenanewentryisaddedmanyofitsfeaturesare“automatically”assigned,eliminatingtheneedtorelearnthem.For

Page 306: the Psychology of Social Conflict and Aggression Sydney Symposium in Social Psychology

are Supernatural BeliefS commitment deviceS? 289

example,categorieslikePERSON,ANIMAL,TOOL,PLANT,orOBJECTeachprovideascaffoldingofinferencesonwhichtobuildnewconcepts.Whenlearn­ingaboutanewanimal,peopledonotneedtorelearnthattheanimal’sinnardsresemblethoseofconspecifics,thatithasoffspringthatgrowintoadults,thatitmovesofitsownaccordandpursuesgoals,andsoon.Theseinferencesareauto­maticallyprovidedbytheANIMALcategory.

TheOHmodelhighlightsthatsupernaturalbeliefstendtoberepresentationsthatconformtoontologicaltemplatesbut,crucially,departfromtheminaparticu­larwayandthatthiscombination—conformityplusexception—givesrisetotheir“stickiness.”

Consideraghost,whichisaPERSONbutviolatestheusualtemplateinthatitpassesthroughobjectsand,most importantly,isnotalive,acriticalfeatureofaPERSON.Aghost, then,canbeunderstoodasaPERSON—preservingmostPERSON­relatedproperties (e.g.,hasamind,movesaround)plusviolations—aghostcanpassthroughsolidmatterwhereaspeoplecannot.

BoyerandBergstrom(2008)recentlywroteaboutideassuchasghosts:

Suchnotionsare salient and inferentiallyproductivebecause theycombinespecificfeaturesthatviolate somedefaultexpectationsforthedomainwithnonviolatedexpectationsheldbydefaultastrueoftheentiredomain(Boyer,1994).Thesecombinationsofexplicitviolationandtacitinferencearecultur­allywidespreadandconstituteamemoryoptimum(Barrett&Nyhof,2001;Boyer&Ramble,2001).Thismaybebecauseexplicitviolationsofexpectationsareattention­grabbing,whereaspreservednonviolatedexpectationsallowonetoreasonaboutthepostulatedagentsorobjects(Boyer,1994).(p.119)

Thekeypointisthenotionofa“memoryoptimum.”Onthisview,supernatu­ral beliefs persist as a by­product of the fact that humancomputational systems“like”representationsthatallowonetoreasonaboutthem(thePERSONpartofaghostorspirit)combinedwiththefactthatwealsoattendmoretoideasthatviolateourexpectations(thenonlivingcomponentofbeingaghost).Supernaturalbeliefs,on this view,persist as aby­productofmechanismsdesigned for inferences andattention.

Arelatedby­productviewisthatsomebeliefs,byvirtueoftheircontentandtheirtendencytomovefromoneheadtoanother,replicatethemselvesnotbecausethebeliefsareusefultothepeoplewhohavethembutsimplybecausetheyarethesortsofbeliefsthatleadtotheirownpropagation.Dennett(2006)arguesthatreligioussystemsofbeliefseemtohavepropertiesthatmakethemgoodatrepli­catingthemselves,includingtheinjunctiontotransmitinformationtochildren,toreproduce,andtoconquerandconvertothers.Thesefeaturesofabeliefsystem,heargues,contributetothespreadofthebeliefsthemselves.

There are three primary difficulties of these models. First, as the costs ofsupernaturalbeliefsincrease,sodoesthestrengthofselectionto“cleanup”thesystem,makingby­productclaimslessplausible.Thatis,by­productexplanationsareunlikelytotheextentthatcostsarehighandselectioncouldhaveselectedoutthese supernatural­belief­generation systems without compromising the systemthat thesebelief­generationsystemsareaby­productof.Webelieve that these

Page 307: the Psychology of Social Conflict and Aggression Sydney Symposium in Social Psychology

roBert kurzBan and John chriStner290

costsare, indeed,highandthat there isnoreasontothinkselectioncouldnothavemodifiedlearningsystemstoresist,ratherthanendorse,supernaturalbeliefs.Second,by­producthypothesesexplainwhysupernaturalbeliefsarememorablebutnotwhy supernaturalbeliefsareendorsed (Dennett,2006).Theseare twoimportantly distinct claims. Finally, models such as Dennett’s rest on largelydomain­generalandcontent­freelearningsystems,which,fromanevolutionaryview,areunlikelytocharacterizehumanpsychology(Tooby&Cosmides,1992).

Adaptationist Views

Thesecondclassofarguments suggests that themechanismsunderlyingsuper­naturalbeliefacquisitionaredesignedtoadoptthem.Onthisview,thereissomeadvantagetohavingsupernaturalbeliefs,andthisadvantageexplainstheexistenceofthemechanismsdesignedtogenerateandadoptthem.

Oneprominentaccount isthatsupernaturalbeliefs“steeredindividualsawayfromcostlysocialtransgressionsresultingfromunrestrained,evolutionarilyances­tral,selfishinterest(actswhichwouldrapidlybecomeknowntoothers,andtherebyincur an increased probability and severity of punishment by group members)”(Johnson&Bering,2006,p.219).Thatis,thosewithsupernaturalbeliefs—partic­ularlyfalsebeliefsaboutpunishmentandtheafterlife—wouldhaveavoidedactionsthatwouldhaveledtocostsintherealworld,thusmakingthembetteroff.

Thisargumentisagametheoreticalargument thatagentswiththesesuper­naturalbeliefs could invadeapopulationof agentswithout them. Inevaluatingthisargument,thekeyistoconsiderapopulationatequilibrium.Thiswouldbeapopulationofagentswhomaximizeexpectedvalue.Inaworldinwhichsomeacts are punished, maximizing expected value entails taking into account theprobability of detection and the costs of punishment. Maximizing individualsdo not take advantage of all opportunities for selfish, norm­violating gain; theytakeadvantageofopportunitieswithpositiveexpectedvalue.JohnsonandBering(2006)assumethisissueaway:“Aslongasthenetcostsofselfishactionsfromreal­worldpunishmentbygroupmembersexceededthenetcostsoflostopportunitiesfromself­imposednormabiding,thengod­fearingindividualswouldoutcompetenon­believers”(p.219).However,thereisnoreasontothinkthatthedefaultstateisadesignthatfavorsengagingin(selfish)actionswithnegativeexpectedvalue.Indeed,thereverseisthecase.Selectionshouldcontinuouslypushcomputationalmechanisms toward such optima, subject to all the usual constraints (see, e.g.,Dawkins,1982).Intheabsenceofanargumentaboutaconstraintthatispushingthedesignoffthisoptimum,gametheoreticmodelsmustassumeexpectedvaluemaximizationasthedefault.

Further,evenifoneweretoassumethatatsomepointapopulationwereoutofequilibriuminthisway,suchapopulationisalwaysinvadable—again,byagentswhodonotadoptoutcome­reducingsupernaturalbeliefs.Ifthesocialworldwerelikepoker,considerthecostofhavingtheviewthatthosewhobluffwillendureendlesspunishmentintheafterlife(and,therefore,neverbluff).Suchpeopleareatadisadvantageandwill lose,eventually, tothosewhousebluffingasatactic,unhinderedbyfalsebeliefsaboutthecosts.

Page 308: the Psychology of Social Conflict and Aggression Sydney Symposium in Social Psychology

are Supernatural BeliefS commitment deviceS? 291

Asecondadaptationistargumentforsupernaturalbeliefsturnsonthevalueofsuchbeliefsinthecontextofsignalingtoothers(seeChapter9inthisvolumeonthevalueofsignalinginthecontextofanger).Argumentsofthisnaturedrawonthebehavioralecology literature,especiallymodels thatshow thatsomesignalsevolvebecauseof,ratherthaninspiteof,theircost(Grafen,1990;Zahavi,1975).Thetypicalexampleisthepeacock’stail.Becausethelargetailhasgreatenergeticcostsandmakesonevulnerabletopredation,onlyveryhealthyandhigh­qualityorganismscanaffordtosupportthem.Forthisreason,peahensthatselectpea­cockswithsuchtailsasmatesareatanadvantage.

In the context of religion, it has been argued that enduring the high costsimposed by religions (e.g., physical harm, deprivation of food and water, laborrequirements)sendsignalstoothers(Irons,2001;Sosis&Alcorta,2003).Inpar­ticular,ithasbeenarguedthatthesecostscommitthosewhoendurethecoststothegroup.(SeeHenrich,2009forarecentrelatedbutdistinctidea.)

However,caremustbeexercisedintherelationshipbetweencostandsignal.Inthecaseofthepeacock’stail,thecostconveyssomethingaboutqualityasanintrinsicfeatureofthecost.Poor­qualitypeacockssimplycannotendurethecost.Thesameargumentdoesnotapplytocostsandcommitment.Enduringacosttoenteragroupdoesnot,asaninherentconsequenceofthecost,preventsomeonefromdefectingorleavingthegroup.Allcostsinthissensearesunk,asarecoststhatareimposedwhileoneisinthegroup(suchasatithe).

Performingritualscanindeedbecostly,andsuchritualsoftenincludesuper­naturalbeliefsasjustification.Enduringsuchcostsmightbesignalingsomething.However,itisnotclearthatthesecostshonestlysignalcommitment,giventhatitispossibletoendurecostsandthenleavethegroup.Havingsaidthat,somekindsofsignalsmight,infact,makeleavingmoredifficult.Wenowturntothisissueandourownviewofthefunctionofsupernaturalbeliefs.

Supernatural BeliefS aS commitment deviceS

The Value of Commitment

Difficultieswithexistingexplanationsforsupernaturalbeliefssuggestthatitmightbe worthwhile to look for alternatives. The idea sketched here requires severalinferentialstepsandisthereforeperhapsnotthemostelegantmodel,butitargu­ablysolvestheproblemswithpreviousmodels.

We begin with the premise that human evolutionary history was character­izedbyshiftingcoalitionsandalliances(DeScioli&Kurzban,2009;Kurzban&Neuberg,2005;Cosmides,Tooby,&Kurzban,2001;Sidanius&Kurzban,2003;Tiger,1969;Tooby,Cosmides,&Kurzban,2003).Thisisnottosaythatsomealli­ancesweren’trelativelystable,suchasthosearrangedalongkinlines,asobservedinotherspecies,suchasbaboons(Cheney&Seyfarth,2007).Theargumentturnsonlyonthenotionthattherewassomevolatilityinalliances.

Wefurtherassumethat,inaworldofalliances,beingamemberofanallianceisabenefit,and,symmetrically,notbeingamemberofanallianceisacost.Oncepeople can form alliances, individuals left out of the protection of a group are

Page 309: the Psychology of Social Conflict and Aggression Sydney Symposium in Social Psychology

roBert kurzBan and John chriStner292

subjecttoeasyexploitation.Evidencethatpeoplederivepleasurefrommember­ship ingroups (Baumeister&Leary,1995)andexperiencepainwhenexcludedfromthem(Chapters3and13inthisvolume)isindicativeofmotivationalsystemsexecutingthisfunction.

In this hypothetical world of shifting group memberships, there would, ofcourse,bemanydimensionsalongwhichpeopleareevaluatedforpossiblemem­bership in a group. These would presumably have to do with properties of theindividual,suchasskills,intelligence,physicalcondition,andsocialconnections.

Whilethesepropertiesareallnodoubtimportant,onekeyparametermightbe the extent to which an individual is viewed as likely to change sides as thefaultlinesofconflictshift.Whenalliancesaredynamic,amemberwhocan,whenopportunityarises,shifttothecompetinggroupisextremelydangerous.Thissug­geststhattheabilitytosignalthatonewillnot—or,evenbetter,cannot—switchalliancescanbeabenefit,ratherthanacost,becausecommittingcanmakeoneamorevaluablegroupmember(Frank,1988).Thisideaisaspecificcaseofthegeneralnotionthatremovingone’sownoptionscanbestrategicallyadvantageousifitissignaledtoothers(Schelling,1960).

Thisideamighthelptoexplainvariouspracticessurroundinggroupmember­ship.Scarification—thepracticeofmakingpermanentmarksonone’sskinwithcolorsorshallowcuts—mightbedesignedtohelppersuadeothersthatoneiscom­mittedtoone’sgroup(e.g.,Rush,2005).Totheextentthatrivalswouldnotacceptanindividualwiththesepermanentmarksintotheirgroup,thesesignalsarehon­estinthetechnicalsenseoftheterm.

Scarificationandtattoos (like falsebeliefs)canbedangerous, leading to thepossibilityofdamageorinfection.Despitethis,itisstillpracticedwidely,pointingtothepossibilityofanevolvedappetiteforvisiblesignalsofcommitment,whethertogroupsorromanticpartners.

Supernatural Beliefs as Loyalty Signals

Beliefs,unlikescarsandtattoos,areinvisibleandeasilyrevised.Spokenstatementsare themselves ephemeral, limiting their effectiveness as commitment devices.Havingsaidthat,givingrisetoabeliefinanotherperson’sheadcan,undercertaincircumstances, recruit thepowerofcommitment.Forexample,asFrank(1988)discusses,informationthatmakesonevulnerablecanbeusefulinthiscontext.IfAlfredtellsBobinformationthatwouldbedisastrousforAlfredshoulditgetout,Alfredhas,effectively,assuredBobthathewon’tactinsuchawaythatwouldmakeBobunfavorablydisposedtowardhim.WhenBobknowsinformationthatwouldcompromiseAlfred—perhapswheretofindevidenceofacrimethatAlfredhascommitted—BobcanbeassuredofAlfred’sloyalty.So,transmittingcertainkindsofinformationtootherscanincreasetheextenttowhichtheyarelikelytobelieveyouwillremainaloyalally,whichcanyieldimportantbenefits.

Broadcastingbeliefsmightallowcommitment.Forexample,publicstatementsofloyaltytoaparticulargroup—orantipathyforotherlocalgroups—mighthelpassure potential allies of one’s commitment. However, talk is cheap, and suchpronouncementsdonotbindone’sactionsinthesamewaythattattoos,scars,or

Page 310: the Psychology of Social Conflict and Aggression Sydney Symposium in Social Psychology

are Supernatural BeliefS commitment deviceS? 293

disclosing incrimination information does. Opinions can change; apologies andrestitutioncanbemade.

Somestatements,however,mightmakeonewhatBoyer(personalcommu­nication,October20,2007)hascalled“unclubbable,”meaningundesirableasamemberofagrouporcommunity.Suchstatements,accordingtothelogicofcommitmentabove,are, tobeclear,potentiallygoodthings: fromthe stand­point of commitment to a group, ways to disqualify oneself from alternativegroupmembershipsarethegoal.

Considerthefollowingstatements:

1.*ChristopherColumbusdiscoveredAmericain1215. 2.*Theearthisflat. 3.*Ienjoyeatingmyownfeces.

Statements1and2,inmoderntimes,would,itseemsreasonabletosay,inviterelativelynegativeevaluations.Everythingelseequal,peopleprefergroupmem­berswhodonothavebeliefsthatarethoughttobeobviouslyfalse.However,evenifitwereknownthatsomeonehadsuchfalsebeliefs,heorshewouldnotnecessar­ilybesubjecttosocialexclusion.

Statement3,incontrast,aslongasitisnotsaidinobviousjest,wouldbepar­ticularly likely to elicit negative evaluations. As the literature on social stigmasuggests,suchdeviationsfromnormalhumanbehaviorelicitverystrongnegativeevaluations(Kurzban&Leary,2001).

The problem with 1 and 2, then, is that they are not strong enough—theydon’tmakeyouunclubbableinanygroup.Statement3,incontrast,istoostrong;itmakesyouunclubbableineverygroup.

So,tosolvethecommitmentproblem,whatisrequiredisthesincereendorse­mentofabeliefthatmakesoneunclubbableineverygroupexceptthegrouptowhich one is trying to signal loyalty. What sort of belief will make one a poorcandidateforgroupmembershipinnearlyeverygroupexcepttheonethatoneiscurrentlyinorwishestocommitto?

ToreturntoStatement3,whatmakessomeoneunclubbableaboutthisisthedeparturefromcanonicalhumannature.Humansocialcognitivesystemsappeardesigned to sift through the social world, evaluating others as potential mates,allies,andgroupmembers.Departures fromthe skeletal structureofbasic fea­turesofhumannatureactascuesthatcountheavilyagainstcandidatesforsocialinteraction(Kurzban&Leary,2001).

RecallourdiscussionofBoyer’s (1994) ideas surrounding intuitiveontology.Toafirstapproximation,byvirtueofsharedhumancomputationalarchitecture,peopleshareintuitiveontologicalcommitments.Supernaturalbeliefsviolatethesecommitments.Inthissense,supernaturalbeliefsaresingularlygoodatmakingoneappeartohavebeliefsthatviolatefundamentalcausalintuitiveprinciples.Inthis,theyareverydifferentfromgarden­varietyfalsebeliefs.Beliefs1and2arefalse,buttheirfalsenessdoesnotcomebyvirtueofaconflictwithintuitiveontology.

Inthissense,supernaturalbeliefsmightbewellsuitedtomakingoneunclub­bablebecausetheyconnotedeviationfromthespecies­typicaldesign.Individuals

Page 311: the Psychology of Social Conflict and Aggression Sydney Symposium in Social Psychology

roBert kurzBan and John chriStner294

whodonotrespectthebasicprinciplesthatgoverncausalreasoningaboutfunda­mentalcategoriesintheworld—ARTIFACTS,ANIMALS,andPEOPLE—arebyandlargeseen(withakeyexception)asmentallyill.

TheDiagnostic and Statistical Manual(DSM) reflectsthisidea.IntheDSM, fourthedition,textrevised(DSM-IV-TR),adelusionisdefinedthisway:“Afalsebeliefbasedonincorrectinferenceaboutexternalrealitythatisfirmlysustaineddespitewhatalmosteverybodyelsebelievesanddespitewhatconstitutesincontro­vertibleandobviousprooforevidencetothecontrary”(APA,2000).Harris(2005,p.821)pointsoutthesimilaritybetweenasupernaturalbeliefandadelusion:“Wehavenamesforpeoplewhohavemanybeliefsforwhichthereisnorationaljusti­fication.Whentheirbeliefsareextremelycommonwecallthem‘religious’;other­wise,theyarelikelytobecalled‘mad,’‘psychotic,’or‘delusional’”(p.72).

Thekeypointisthatsupernaturalbeliefswillbeeasilyidentifiedbypeopleasfalsebecauseofpeople’sintuitiveontologicalcommitments.Thiswillleadpeopletoinferthatthepersonwhoendorsessuchbeliefs—and“firmlysustains”them—is, toafirstapproximation, insane.Thementally illareoneof themostheavilystigmatizedgroups(Corrigan,2005).

Thishasoneverylargeexception,asindicatedbythedefinitionintheDSM-IV-TR.Falsebeliefsthatthataresharedby“almosteverybodyelse”arenotcon­sidereddelusions.Considerthefollowing:

4.*Eatinganotherpersongivesyouaccesstohisorhersoul. 5.*Ifaspecialpersonsaysspecialmagicwordsinaspecialbuilding,certain

crackersturnintothebodyofapersonwhowasalivebutisnowdead. 6.*Acertainkindoftreecanbemadetofruitifaprettywomankicksit. 7.*Keepingyourdeadgrandmother’shairinajarkeepsherspiritaround.

First,itisworthaskingifonecanintuitwhichofthesebeliefsaresupernaturalbeliefs culled from the world’s cultures and which are delusional beliefs culledfromtheclinicalliterature.(Notethat4and7aredrawnfromclinicalaccounts,whereas5and6arereligiousbeliefs.)

Peoplewhoendorsesuchbeliefsmightbetakenforeithermentallyillornot,depending on the social context in which such beliefs are uttered, specificallywhetherthesupernaturalbeliefiscommonlyheldbytheotherpeopleinasocialgroup.Amongthosewhobelieveintransubstantiation,5willnotmakeoneappearmentallyill.Indeed,endorsingthisbeliefnotonlydoesnotelicitexclusionbut,infact,insomecommunitiesisalsoessentiallyarequirementforinclusion.Wright(2009)quotes an interesting observationof this generalphenomenon suggestedbytheApostlePaul,whoasks,if“thewholechurchcomestogetherandspeaksintongues,andoutsidersorunbelieversenter,willtheynotsaythatyouareoutofyourmind?”(p.270).

Theveryfirstcommandment,ofcourse,echoesthis idea.Thecalltomono­theism,andtheharshpunishmentsintheOldTestamentforpolytheism,iscon­sistentwiththeideathatsupernaturalbeliefsareforpreventingmembershipinothergroups.Thefirstcommandmentessentiallypreventedswitchinginaworldinwhichothergroupswereworshipingmultipledeities.Inthissense,themodern

Page 312: the Psychology of Social Conflict and Aggression Sydney Symposium in Social Psychology

are Supernatural BeliefS commitment deviceS? 295

practiceofreligioustolerancecanbeseenasevidencefor,ratherthanagainst,ourposition.Themassiveeffortsthatmustbemadetotrytogetpeopletobetolerantofothers’religiousviewssuggeststhatthisisnotthedefaultstate.

Relatedly,Iannaccone(1994),drawingonearlierargumentsbyKelley(1986),suggeststhatreligiousgroupsaresuccessfulbecausethethingsthatmakethemdistinctive “invite ridicule, isolation, and persecution” (p. 1182) and that suchgroups“demandofmemberssomedistinctive,stigmatizingbehaviorthatinhibitsparticipationorreducesproductivityinalternativecontexts…”(p.1188).Theseareideasthatresonatecloselywiththenotionthatsupernaturalbeliefsareeffectivewaystocommittoonegroupoverothers.NotethatIannacone,however,suggeststhatthebenefitofsuchcostshastodowithpublicgoodsratherthanthepresentargument.HequotesSingh(1953):“TheGuruwantedtoraiseabodyofmenwhowouldnotbeabletodenytheirfaithwhenquestioned,butwhoseexternalappear­ancewouldinvitepersecutionandbreedthecouragetoresistit”(p.31;seealsoIannaccone,1992).Thoughthepresentargumentfocusesonsupernaturalbeliefs,certainly it is plausible that there are other ways to make oneself unclubbable,through,forinstance,physicalappearanceorone’schoiceoffoods.Aslongasone’sbehaviorreducesthe(perceptionof)thechanceofswitchinggroups,thepresentargumentholds.

Summary

Tosummarize,ourargumentbeginswiththenotionthatsupernaturalbeliefsthatprecludemembershipinothergroupsarevaluablebecausetheyrepresentcom­mitment.Supernaturalbeliefs,whichviolatethebasicontologicalcommitmentsofevolvedintuitivetheories,makeoneappearmentallyilltothosewhodonotsharesuchbeliefs,anideareflectedinmodernpsychiatricclassification.Ifsupernaturalbeliefsdohavethisproperty,thentherecouldhavebeenselectionformechanismsdesignedtogenerateandendorselocallydistinctivesupernaturalbeliefs.Suchamechanismpotentially solves thecommitmentproblembyallowingone topre­cludemembershipinanygroupsotherthanthelocalone.

Supernatural beliefs have advantages over other potentially distinctive localbeliefs.Forexample,falsebeliefsabouthistory,althoughtheymightbelocallydis­tinctive,donotprecludemembershipinothergroups.Supernaturalbeliefs,unlikeotherbeliefs thatmightbe locally shared,have theparticularpropertyofcom­mittingonetothelocalgroupthatsharesthesupernaturalbelief,makingthemfunctionalinawaythatessentiallyanynonsupernaturalbeliefcouldnot.ThisgivesafunctionalexplanationforBoyer’s(1994)findingregardingsupernaturalbeliefsandmighthelptoexplainhowthecostsoffalsebeliefsmightbeoffset.

Itseemsplausible—thoughthisisnotcentraltothepresentargument—thatritualsmightbewaystosignalone’sendorsementofthefalsebeliefthatgoesbeyondsimplestatementstothateffect.Takingcommunion,forexample,mighthelptopersuadeothersthatoneendorsesBelief5.Otherrituals,insteadofbeingcostlysignals,mightbemeansofpersuadingothersthatonereallyendorsesparticularsupernaturalbeliefs.Thischangesthevalueofritualfromsignalingcostpersetosignalingbelief.

Page 313: the Psychology of Social Conflict and Aggression Sydney Symposium in Social Psychology

roBert kurzBan and John chriStner296

implicationS for intergroup conflictOnepuzzlingfeatureofreligiousconflictisthedegreeofantipathybetweengroupsthat share nearly all of their supernatural beliefs, with only a handful of suchbeliefsdistinguishingthem.Thevariousantipathiesoftheworld’smajormonothe­isticreligionsarewellknown,as isthebloodspilledoverdetailsofsupernaturalbeliefsamongthedivisionsofChristianity.Onemighthavepredictedthatsimilar­ity reduced hostility, with, say, monotheistic Catholics most fiercely antagonistictowardpolytheisticHindubutlesstowardMormons.Thisdoesnot,however,seemto be the case.Despite massiveoverlap in largenumbersof false beliefs, a tinynumberofsuchbeliefsthatdifferseemtobesufficientforstrikingnegativeemotionandhostility,asoneseesinfightsamongsects.(Forsomedataontherelationshipbetweenorganizedreligionandaggression,seeChapter19inthisvolume.)

Thereare,ofcourse,manypossibleexplanationsforthisphenomenon,includ­ingthefactthatgroupswithsimilarbeliefsmightbeengagedinconflictforthesameresources(Wright,2009)becauseoftheirproximity,butitsitswellwiththepresentview.Ifsupernaturalbeliefsaredesignedspecificallyforthepurposeofcommittingpeopletoparticulargroupsbecauseofthepotentialforconflict,thenitisnotsurprisingthatdifferencesinsupernaturalbeliefsbetweengroupsshouldbreedfearandhostility.

Alongsimilarlines,thepresentviewresonateswellwiththefactthatorganizedreligionsarethelocusoftrustandcooperation(Wilson,2002).Ifsharedsupernat­uralbeliefsareagoodcuetogroupcommitment,thentheyoughttobringaboutemotionsoftrustandsupport.Inthecontextofintergroupcompetition,mutuallybeneficialwithin­grouptransactionsareveryvaluable.Itisworthnotingthatthereisnothinginandofitselfthatsuggeststhatfalsebeliefsheldincommonwouldleadtotrustandstrongcommunityties.

Theforegoingsuggeststhatsupernaturalbeliefsshouldplayaspecialroleinbothwithin­andbetween­groupsocialrelationships(seealsoChapter10inthisvolume).Withingroups,sharedsupernaturalbeliefsandanyactsthatareindica­tiveofsuchsharedbeliefs(e.g.,particularrituals)shouldmakeothersfeelthatthepersoninquestionistrustworthyandaloyalmemberofthegroup.Thisshouldbeparticularlythecaseforpublicactivities,whichwouldservethefunctionofdis­qualifyingonefrommembershipinothergroups.Thisisdistinctfromotherkindsofbeliefs.Forexample,falsesharedhistoricalbeliefsshouldnotleadtoinferencesoftrustworthinessinthesamewaythatsupernaturalbeliefsmight.

Inshort,wearguethatsupernaturalbeliefsarenot, in themselves,accidentalconsequencesofdesign;neitheristhefactthattheyareatthecenterofintergroupconflictanaccidentalconsequenceofdesign.Onthepresentview,then,mechanismsthatgiverisetosupernaturalbeliefsthatcausetheirbearertobefearedandhatedbyotherswhodosharethebeliefarefunctioningpreciselyastheyweredesigned.

referenceS

AmericanPsychiatricAssociation.(APA).(2000).Diagnostic and statistical manual of men-tal disorders(4thed.,textrevision).Washington,DC:Author.

Page 314: the Psychology of Social Conflict and Aggression Sydney Symposium in Social Psychology

are Supernatural BeliefS commitment deviceS? 297

Barrett,J.,&Nyhof,M.(2001).Spreadingnonnaturalconcepts.Journal of Cognition and Culture, 1,69–100.

Bartlett, F. C. (1932). Remembering: A study in experimental and social psychology. Cambridge,UK:CambridgeUniversityPress.

Baumeister,R.F.,&Leary,M.R. (1995).Theneed tobelong:Desire for interpersonalattachments as a fundamental human motivation. Psychological Bulletin, 117,497–529.

Boyd, R., & Richerson, P. J. (1985). Culture and the evolutionary process. Chicago:UniversityofChicagoPress.

Boyer,P.(1994).The naturalness of religious ideas: A cognitive theory of religion.Berkeley:UniversityofCaliforniaPress.

Boyer,P.,&Bergstrom,B.(2008).Evolutionaryperspectivesonreligion.Annual Review of Anthropology, 37, 111–130.

Boyer,P.,&Ramble,C.(2001).Cognitivetemplatesforreligiousconcepts:Cross­culturalevidence for recall of counter­intuitive representations. Cognitive Science, 25,535–564.

Cheney,D.L.,&Seyfarth,R.M. (2007).Baboon metaphysics: The evolution of a social mind.Chicago:UniversityofChicagoPress.

Corrigan, J. (2005). Is the experimental auction a dynamic market? Environmental & Resource Economics, 31,35–45.

Cosmides,L.,&Tooby,J.(1987).Fromevolutiontobehavior:Evolutionarypsychologyasthemissinglink.InJ.Dupré(Ed.), The latest on the best: Essays on evolution and optimality (pp.277–306).Cambridge,MA:MITPress.

Cosmides,L.,Tooby, J.,&Kurzban,R. (2003).Perceptionsof race. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 7,173–179.

Dawkins,R.(1982).The extended phenotype: The gene as the unit of selection. Oxford,UK:W.H.Freeman&Co.

Dawkins,R.(2006). The God delusion. NewYork:BantamBooks.Dennett,D.C.(1987).The intentional stance.Cambridge,MA:MITPress.Dennett,D.C.(2006).Breaking the spell: Religion as a natural phenomenon.NewYork:

PenguinBooks.DeScioli,P.,&Kurzban,R.(2009).Mysteriesofmorality.Cognition, 112,281–299.Fodor,J.(2000).The mind doesn’t work that way. Cambridge,MA:MITPress.Frank,R.(1988).Passions within reason: The strategic role of the emotions.NewYork:W.W.

Norton&Co.Grafen, A. (1990). Biological signals as handicaps. Journal of Theoretical Biology, 144,

517–546.Green,D.M.,& Swets,J.A.(1966).Signal detection theory and psychophysics.NewYork:

JohnWileyandSons.Harris,S.(2005).The end of faith: Religion, terror, and the future of reason. NewYork:W.

W.Norton&Co.Haselton,M.G.,&Buss,D.M.(2000).Errormanagementtheory:Anewperspectiveon

biases incross­sexmindreading.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 78, 81–91.

Haselton,M.G.,&Nettle,D.(2006).Theparanoidoptimist:Anintegrativeevolutionarymodelofcognitivebiases.Personality and Social Psychology Review, 10,47–66.

Henrich,J.(2009).Theevolutionofcostlydisplays,cooperation,andreligion:Credibilityenhancingdisplaysandtheirimplicationsforculturalevolution.Evolution and Human Behavior, 30,244–260.

Iannaccone,L.R.(1992).Sacrificeandstigma:Reducingfree­ridingincults,communes,andothercollectives.Journal of Political Economy, 100,271–291.

Page 315: the Psychology of Social Conflict and Aggression Sydney Symposium in Social Psychology

roBert kurzBan and John chriStner298

Iannaccone,L.R.(1994).Whystrictchurchesarestrong. American Journal of Sociology 99,1180–1211.

Irons,W.(2001).Religionasahard­to­fakesignofcommitment.InR.Nesse(Ed.),Evolution and the capacity for commitment(pp.292–309).NewYork:RussellSageFoundation.

Johnson,D.D.P.,&Bering,J.M.(2006).HandofGod,mindofman:Punishmentandcognitionintheevolutionofcooperation.EvolutionaryPsychology, 4,219−233.

Kelley,D.M.(1986).Why conservative churches are growing: A study in sociology of reli-gion with a new preface for the ROSE edition.Macon,GA:MercerUniversityPress.

Kurzban,R.(inpress).Why everyone (else) is a hypocrite: Evolution and the modular mind.Princeton,NJ:PrincetonUniversityPress.

Kurzban,R.,&Aktipis,C.A.(2006).Modularminds,multiplemotives.InM.Shaller,J.Simpson, &D. Kenrick (Eds.),Evolution and social psychology (pp. 39–53). NewYork:PsychologyPress.

Kurzban,R.,&Aktipis,C.A.(2007).Modularityandthesocialmind:Arepsychologiststooself­ish?Personality and Social Psychology Review, 11,131–149.

Kurzban,R.,&Leary,M.R.(2001).Evolutionaryoriginsofstigmatization:Thefunctionsofsocialexclusion.Psychological Bulletin, 127, 187–208.

Kurzban,R.,&Neuberg,S.(2005).Managingingroupandoutgrouprelationships.InD.Buss(Ed.),Thehandbook of evolutionary psychology (pp.653–675).Hoboken,NJ:Wiley.

Kurzban,R.,Tooby,J.,&Cosmides,L.(2001).Can race be erased? Coalitional computa-tion and social categorization.ProceedingsoftheNationalAcademyofSciences,98,15387–15392.

McKay,R.T.,&Dennett,D.C.(2009).Theevolutionofmisbelief.Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 32,493–561.

Millikan,R.(1984).Language, thought, and other biological categories. Cambridge,MA:MITPress.

Nesse, R. M. (2005). Natural selection and the regulation of defenses: A signal detec­tion analysis of the smoke detector principle. Evolution and Human Behavior, 26, 88–105.

Nesse,R.M.(2001).Evolution and the capacity for commitment.NewYork:RussellSageFoundation.

Nesse,R.M.,&Lloyd,A.T.(1992).Theevolutionofpsychodynamicmechanisms.InJ.Barkow,L.Cosmides,&J.Tooby(Eds.),The adapted mind: Evolutionary psychology and the generation of culture(pp.601–624).NewYork:OxfordUniversityPress.

Nesse,R.M.,&Williams,G.C.(1994).Why we get sick: The new science of Darwinian medicine.NewYork:VintageBooks.

Nettle,D.(2004).Adaptiveillusions:Optimism,controlandhumanrationality.InD.Evans& P. Cruse (Eds.),Emotion, evolution and rationality (pp. 193–208). Oxford, UK:OxfordUniversityPress.

Pinker, S. (1994). The language instinct: How the mind creates language. New York:HarperCollins.

Rush,J.A.(2005).Spiritual tattoo: A cultural history of tattooing, piercing, scarification, branding, and implants.Berkeley,CA:Frog,Ltd.

Schelling,T.(1960).The strategy of conflict.Cambridge,MA:HarvardUniversityPress.Sidanius,J.,&Kurzban,R.(2003).Evolutionaryapproachestopoliticalpsychology.InD.

O.Sears,L.Huddy,&R.Jervis(Eds.),Oxford handbook of political psychology(pp.146–181).Oxford,UK:OxfordUniversityPress.

Singh,K.(1953).The Sikhs.London:Allen&Unwin.Smith, J. M.  (1982).  Evolution and the theory of games.  Cambridge, UK: Cambridge

UniversityPress.Sosis,R.,&Alcorta,C.(2003).Signaling,solidarity,andthesacred:Theevolutionofreli­

giousbehavior.Evolutionary Anthropology, 12,264–274.

Page 316: the Psychology of Social Conflict and Aggression Sydney Symposium in Social Psychology

are Supernatural BeliefS commitment deviceS? 299

Sperber,D.(1985).On anthropological knowledge.Cambridge,UK:CambridgeUniversityPress.

Taylor,S.E.,&Brown,J.D.(1988).Illusionandwell­being:Asocialpsychologicalperspec­tiveonmentalhealth.Psychological Bulletin, 103,193–210.

Tiger,L.(1969).Men in groups.NewYork:RandomHouse.Tooby,J.,&Cosmides,L.(1992).Thepsychologicalfoundationsofculture.InJ.Barkow,

L.Cosmides,&J.Tooby(Eds.),The adapted mind(pp.19–136).NewYork:OxfordUniversityPress.

Trivers,R. (2000).Theelementsofascientifictheoryofself­deception.Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 907,114–131.

Von Neumann, J., & Morgenstern, O. (1944).  Theory of games and economic behavior.Princeton,NJ:PrincetonUniversityPress.

Wiley,R.H.(1994).Errors,exaggeration,anddeceptioninanimalcommunication.InL.Real(Ed.),Behavioral mechanisms in evolutionary ecology(pp.157–189).Chicago:UniversityofChicagoPress.

Wilson, D. S. (2002). Darwin’s cathedral: Evolution, religion, and the nature of society. Chicago:UniversityofChicagoPress.

Wright,R.(2009).The evolution of God.NewYork:Little,BrownandCompanyHachetteBookGroup.

Zahavi,A.(1975).Mateselection:Aselectionforahandicap.Journal of Theoretical Biology, 53, 205–214.

Page 317: the Psychology of Social Conflict and Aggression Sydney Symposium in Social Psychology
Page 318: the Psychology of Social Conflict and Aggression Sydney Symposium in Social Psychology

301

19TheEffectofReligious

ParticipationonAggressionOverOne’sLifetimeand

AcrossGenerationsL.ROWELLHUESMANN

University of Michigan

ERICF.DUBOWUniversity of Michigan and Bowling Green State University

PAULBOXERRutgers University and University of Michigan

D uringthepastdecade,therehasbeenaburgeoninginterestintheroleofreligiosityinfamilyfunctioningandchildandadolescentadjustment(e.g.,Bridges&Moore,2002;Mahoney,Pargament,Swank,&Tarakeshwar,

2001)andasaresourceforadultscopingwithstress(Pargament,1997,2007).Thefocusofthischapterisontheroleofreligiosityacrossthelifespaninpredictingadulthood aggressiveness. We use data from a 40­year prospective longitudinalstudytoexamine(1)theextenttowhichparentalreligiositywhenachildis8yearsoldisrelatedtothechild’sreligiosityatages19,30,and48,andthegrandchild’sreligiosity;and(2)theextenttowhichgrandparental,parental,andchildreligiosityactaslong­termprotectivefactorsagainstaggressivebehaviorinchildhood,youth,andadulthood.

Page 319: the Psychology of Social Conflict and Aggression Sydney Symposium in Social Psychology

l. roWell hueSmann, eric f. duBoW, and paul Boxer302

the importance of religioSityAccording to theGallupConsultingOrganization (2008),93%ofAmericans18yearsofageandolderreportedthattheybelieveinGodorauniversalspirit;54%reportedthatreligionis“veryimportant”intheirlives,andanother26%reportedthatreligionis“fairlyimportant”intheirlives;61%saidthattheyareamemberofachurchorsynagogue,and38%saidthattheyhadattendedreligiousservicesinthepast7days;and57%agreedthatreligioncananswerallormostoftoday’sproblems. Inanationally representative sampleofeighth through twelfthgrad­ers,Wallace,Forman,Caldwell,andWillis(2003)foundthat60%ofadolescentsreportedthatreligionisanimportantpartoftheirlives,and50%saidtheyattendreligiousservicesregularly.Whilecomparablestatisticsaredifficulttoobtainforothercountries, the available statistics forotherWesterncountries arenot thatdifferent.Forexample,88%ofItalianssaytheybelongtoachurch,andabout30%saytheyattendregularly.Accordingtothe1996WorldValuesSurvey(1996),onlyabout36%ofEuropeanssaid theynever(orpracticallynever)attendedchurch.Thus,whilethecurrentstudyfocusesentirelyontheUnitedStates,wheremostofthedatarelatingreligiositytobehaviorhavebeenobtained,cautiousgeneraliza­tionstotherestoftheworldarepossible.

Inextensivereviewsoftheliteratureontheroleofreligioninchildandado­lescentadjustment,BridgesandMoore(2002)andMahoneyetal.(2001)reportedthathighlevelsofparentandchildreligiosity(mostoftenmeasuredbyparentalorself­reportsoffrequencyofchurchattendance,frequencyofprayer,andimportanceofreligiontoone’slife)werelinkedtolowerlevelsofdelinquency,behaviorprob­lems,andsubstanceuseandtohigherlevelsofadolescentresponsibility.Relatedly,Chapter14inthisvolumeshowsthatforgivingisrelatedtoreligion.Whilehistoryhasshownthatreligiousdevotioncanpromoteterrorismandaggressioninsomecases(e.g.,Chapter10inthisvolume),themajorityofempiricalresearchtodateseemstoindicatethatreligiousparticipationisrelatedtomorepositiveoutcomesinyouth.

empirical StudieS of the relation BetWeen religioSity and aggreSSion and delinQuency

Johnson,DeLi,Larson,andMcCullough(2000)reviewed40studiespublishedfrom 1985 to 1997 on the relation between religiosity and delinquency. A totalof30ofthe40studiesshowedanegativerelationbetweenreligiosityanddelin­quency.Onlyfivestudieshadalongitudinaldesign.

Severalstudieshaveassessedtherelationbetweenparentalreligiosityandchildaggressionanddelinquency, andmosthave shownnegativecorrelationsbetween parental church attendance and risk for aggression, delinquency,or criminality of their children (Ellis & Pettersson, 1996; Pettersson, 1991).Bartkowski,Xu,andLevin(2008)useddatafromthenationallyrepresentativeEarlyChildhoodLongitudinalStudy­Ksample(over20,000kindergartenandfirstgradersin1998–1999).Parentalreligiositywasmeasuredbyfrequencyof

Page 320: the Psychology of Social Conflict and Aggression Sydney Symposium in Social Psychology

the effect of religiouS participation on aggreSSion 303

churchattendance,religioushomogamy(similaritybetweenparentsintermsoffrequencyofchurchattendance),andfrequencyofdiscussionsofreligionwiththechild.Higherlevelsofeachparent’sfrequencyofattendanceandreligioushomogamy were related to most parent­ and teacher­rated measures of childdevelopment,includinghigherlevelsofself­controlandlowerlevelsofimpul­sivenessandexternalizingbehaviorproblems.Kim,McCullough,andCicchetti(2009)examineda sampleofmaltreatedandnonmaltreatedchildren.Amongnonmaltreatedchildren,parents’importanceoffaithwasrelatedtolowerlevelsofinternalizingandexternalizingbehaviorsinmiddlechildhood,andparentalreligious influence seemed tobe strongerwhen the child reported lower lev­elsof importanceof religion.Theseeffectswerenotobserved formaltreatedchildren;however, inaseparatestudyKim(2008) foundprotectiveeffectsofreligiosityoninternalizingsymptomsformaltreatedfemales.Finally,usingdatafrom our Columbia County Longitudinal Study (CCLS), we found that boyswhoseparentsattendedchurchmorefrequentlywhentheboyswere8yearsoldwerelessatriskforcriminalitybyage30thanwereequallyaggressive8­year­oldboyswhoseparentsattendedchurch less frequently (Huesmann,Eron,&Dubow, 2002). This finding held even after controlling for family interactionvariablesandthechild’sIQ.

Several recent studies assessedadolescents’ self­reportsof theirown religi­osity.Herrenkohl etal. (2003)useddata from theSeattleSocialDevelopmentStudy.Theparticipantswerechildrenwhowerehighinteacherratingsofaggres­sionatage10.Lowerprobabilityofviolenceatage18wasassociatedwithseveralage­15 variables: attendance at religious services, good family management byparents,andschoolbonding.Pearce,Jones,Schwab­Stone,andRuchkin(2003),inasampleofhigh­riskurbanadolescents, foundthatreligiousnessassessedbychurch attendance and self­rated religiousness was associated negatively withconductdisorderandthat“privatereligiousness”(e.g.,prayer,readingreligiouslit­erature)wasassociatedwithdecreasesover1yearinconductdisorder.RegnerusandElder(2003)useddatafromtheNationalLongitudinalStudyofAdolescentHealth to examine whether religiosity would be most important for high­riskyouthbecausereligioussupport“providesfunctionalcommunitiesamiddysfunc­tion”(p.635).Thechildrenwereingrades7–12attime1andwereassessed1yearlateraswell.Underconditionsofhigherpoverty,therewasindeedastrongerrela­tionbetweenfrequencyofchurchattendanceand“stayingontrackacademically,”whichincludedacompositeofgradepointaverage,gettinghomeworkcompleted,gettingalongwithteachers,notbeingsuspendedorexpelled,andnotskippingschool. Fowler,Ahmed,Tompsett, Jozefowicz­Simbeni, and Toro (2008) exam­inedasampleofover300low­incomeAfricanAmericanandCaucasianemerg­ing adults (average age 20 years old).Public religious affiliation (i.e., the valuetheparticipantsheldintheirchurchmembership)bufferedtherelationbetweenexposuretocommunityviolenceandsubstanceuse.Privatereligiousness(i.e.,theextenttowhichparticipantsindicatedthattheirreligiousbeliefsprovidedthemwithpersonalmeaning)buffered the relationbetweenexposure to communityviolenceanddeviantbehaviorandconductproblems,butthisfindingwaslimitedtoAfricanAmericanparticipants.

Page 321: the Psychology of Social Conflict and Aggression Sydney Symposium in Social Psychology

l. roWell hueSmann, eric f. duBoW, and paul Boxer304

theoretical explanationS for Why parental and child religioSity Should protect againSt aggreSSion and delinQuency

Researchers have reviewed theoretical explanations for potentially posi­tive effects of parental and child religiosity on family functioning and childandadolescentoutcomes(e.g.,Bridges&Moore,2002;Mahoneyetal.,2001;McCullough & Willoughby, 2009; Smith, 2003). We organize these explana­tionsintothreecategories.Thefirstexplanationisthatreligionisamarkerforother structural characteristics in the home, such as good parenting. BridgesandMooreandMahoneyetal.notedthatreligionmaydirectlyaffectparent­ingbyimbuingchildrearingwithamoralandspiritualsignificanceleadingtheparenttoseethechildasa“holygiftfromGod”whorequiresspecialattentionandcareorbyofferingspecificchild­rearingguidance.Theauthorsalsonotedindirect effects of religion on parenting; that is, religiosity may enhance thestabilityandqualityof,andsatisfactionwith,themaritalrelationship,aswellasparentalmentalhealth,whichinturncanpromotepositiveparenting.Mahoneyetal.reviewed94studiesontheeffectsofreligiononmaritalandfamilyfunc­tioningandfoundbroadsupportforthesedirectandindirecteffectsofparents’religiosity. For example, Gunnoe, Hetherington, and Reiss (1999) found thatparents’religiosity(howreligionismanifestedintheirinteractionswithothers)predictedhigherlevelsofobservedauthoritativeparenting(awarm,supportiveenvironmentcoupledwithhighage­appropriatedemands),which in turnpre­dictedadolescents’andparents’reportsoftheadolescents’socialresponsibility(perseverance,self­control,obediencetoparentsandteachers).Acrossseveralstudies,thecorrelationsbetweenparentalreligiosityandchildoutcomespersistevenaftercontrollingforvariablesthoughttoinfluencebothparentreligiosityandchildoutcomes(e.g.,socioeconomicstatus,thechild’scognitiveability).

Parentsalso imparttheirreligiousbeliefsandbehaviorstotheirchildren; inturn,asreviewedearlier,thechild’sreligiosityisrelatedtolowerlevelsofaggressionanddelinquency.KirkpatrickandShaver(1990)suggestedthatthechild’sdevelop­ingreligiousbeliefsandpracticesareinfluencedbythoseoftheirparents,andthistransmissionisaffectedbythequalityoftheparent–childrelationship.Ifthechildis securelyattached to theparent, thechild ismore likely toadopt theparent’sbeliefs(seeChapter2inthisvolume).GunnoeandMoore(2002),usingdatafromthe NationalLongitudinal Surveyof Youth, found that for late adolescents andearlyadults(ages17–22),frequencyoftheirchurchattendanceandimportanceofreligionwerepredictedbyearlierparentalreligiousinfluencessuchasattendingchurchasachild,maternalimportanceofreligion,andattendingreligiousschool.Thesefindingsheldevenaftercontrollingforfamilysocioeconomicstatusandthechild’scognitiveability.Acrossstudies,FlorandKnapp(2001)reportedcorrela­tionsinthe.50rangebetweenparentandoffspringreligiosity.

A second theoretical explanation for religion’s potentially positive effects onchilddevelopmentisthatthereligiousestablishmentprovidessupporttohelppar­entswithproblemchildrensuccessfullydealwiththeproblems.Thisexplanationstemsfromsocialcontroltheory(Hirschi&Stark,1969)andalsoisconsistentwith

Page 322: the Psychology of Social Conflict and Aggression Sydney Symposium in Social Psychology

the effect of religiouS participation on aggreSSion 305

researchandtheoryonsocialcapital(e.g.,Coleman,1988)andsocialsupport(e.g.,Dubow&Ullman,1989).Smith(2003)suggestedthatthereligiouscommunityisaformofsocialcapitalthatcansupportparentalvaluesandcanprovidecross­gen­erationalrelationshipsforthechildand“networkclosure”(densenetworksofindi­vidualswhoknowthechildandthechild’sparents,sotheycanprovideinformationtotheparentsaboutanynegativechildbehaviors).Similarly,religiouscommunitiesofpeersandreligiousleadersalsocanprovideformalaswellasinformalsocialsup­porttoparentsandchildren;forexample,parentsmightseekguidancefromclergyonhandlingchildproblems,whereaschildrenmightrelyonpeernetworksthroughtheirreligiousinstitutionsforadviceormorenondirectiveformsofsupport.

Athirdtheoreticalexplanationofreligion’spositiveeffectsonchilddevelop­mentisthatreligiousexposurebuildsstronginternalself­regulatingstandardsinachild,suchasnormativebeliefsopposingaggressionorfaiththatthisisGod’splanand“thingswillgetbetter”(e.g.,Smith,2003).McCulloughandWilloughby(2009)reviewedstudiespublishedthroughJuly2008totestkeypropositionsrel­evanttotherelationbetweenreligionandself­control.Acrossstudies,thereweresmallbutsignificantcorrelationsbetweenreligiosityandpersonalitytraitsindica­tiveofself­control(e.g.,agreeableness,conscientiousness)andself­controlmedi­ated the relationbetween religiosity and substanceuse inone study (Desmondetal.,2008,citedinMcCullough&Willoughby,2009).Inaseriesoffiveexperi­ments,Koole(2007)showedthatprayerhadasalutaryeffectonaffectregulation:praying foraperson inneedwasshowntopromoteamorepositivemood.Thepromotion of positive affect is hypothesized to beprotective against aggressionand antisocial behavior. Still, despite the important experimental evidence thathasbeenobtained,examiningthedevelopmentthroughchildhoodofinternalizedstandardsthatpromoteprosocialbehaviorandreducethelikelihoodofantisocialbehaviorasthefunctionofexposuretoreligiouspracticesandinstitutionsrequiresalongitudinaldesign.

the columBia county longitudinal StudyIntheremainderofthischapterweexaminetherelationbetweenreligiosityandaggressionwithdatafromthe40­yearColumbiaCountyLongitudinalStudy.TheCCLS isaprospective studyof8568­year­oldswhowere in the thirdgrade inColumbiaCounty,NewYork,in1960,whentheyandtheirparentswerefirstinter­viewed.Thechildrenweresubsequentlyreinterviewedatages19,30,and48.Weexaminethecontinuityofreligiosityfromyouthtoadulthoodandacrossthreegen­erations,howthisreligiosityrelatestoconcurrentandfutureaggressionwithinandacrossgenerations,andhowreligiositymodifiestheexpectedtrajectoryofaggres­sionfromchildhoodtoadulthoodandacrossgenerations.

Methods

TheColumbiaCountyLongitudinalStudywasinitiatedin1960(Eron,Walder,& Lefkowitz, 1971) when the original sample of 856 children, all of the thirdgradersinColumbiaCounty,NewYork,werefirstassessedatWave1ofwhathas

Page 323: the Psychology of Social Conflict and Aggression Sydney Symposium in Social Psychology

l. roWell hueSmann, eric f. duBoW, and paul Boxer306

nowbecamea40­year,four­wavelongitudinalstudy.Subsetsofthesamplewerereassessed10years later in1970when theparticipantswere19;11yearsafterthatin1981whentheparticipantswere30;and19yearsafterthatin2000whentheparticipantswere48onaverage.Thisprojecthasgenerateda largeamountofdataconcerninghowaggressiondevelopsfromchildhoodintoadulthood(seeEron,Huesmann,&Zelli,1991;Eronetal.,1971;Huesmann,Dubow,&Boxer,2009;Huesmann,Eron,Lefkowitz,&Walder,1984)aswellashowchildhoodandadolescentaggressionnegativelyaffectadulthoodsuccess(e.g.,Dubow,Boxer,&Huesmann,2008;Dubow,Huesmann,Boxer,Pulkkinen,&Kokko,2006).

participants and procedures Whenthestudybeganin1960,thesampleof856childrenwasdrawnfromallofthepublicandprivateschoolsinColumbiaCounty,NewYork.Over90%oftheoriginalsamplewasCaucasian;51%weremale,and49%werefemale.Inthisfirstwave,85%oftheparticipants’mothersand71%oftheirfathersalsowereinterviewed.Theparticipantscamefromabroadrangeofsocioeconomicbackgrounds(mean[M]=5.01,standarddeviation[SD]=2.23ona10­pointscaleoffather’soccupationalstatusderivedbyEronetal.,1971,basedonseven­pointscalefromWarner,Meeker,&Eells,1960;thismeanreflectsjobssuchascraftsmen,foremen,andskilledtradesmen)anddisplayedawiderangeofintelligence(meanIQof104,SD=14).

In1970,427participants(211boys,216girls)werereinterviewedforWave2.Theyhadamodalageof19yearsandhadcompleted12.6yearsofeducationonaverage.In1981,therewasathirdwaveofinterviews,butwewillnotbeusingdatafromthatwaveinthischapterasreligiositywasnotassessed.

In1999–2002,523 of theparticipants (268males, 255 females; 61%of theoriginalsample)werereinterviewedforWave4.Theirmeanagewas48.46yearsold(SD=.77);theiraverageeducationlevelwasbetweensomecollegeandacol­lege degree; their average occupational attainmentwas middle­class status (theaverageoccupationalprestigecodeusingStevens&Hoisington’s [1987]prestigescoresreflectedjobssuchassales,bookkeepers,secretaries);and69%oftheorigi­nalparticipantswerelivingwiththeirspouses.Theiraverageverbalachievementscoreon theWRATwas99.15 (SD=13.72).During this samewave,we inter­viewed536offspringofouroriginalsubjects.Theyweretheoffspringof325dif­ferent subjects. To keep the sample independent for this study we selected theoldestoffspringwhenmorethanonewasinterviewed.Thisgaveusasampleof349independentchildren,youth,andyoungadultswhowerechildrenofouroriginalsubjectsandgrandchildrenoftheparentsweinterviewedin1960.Thissamplewas51%femaleand49%male.Theagesoftheoffspringrangedfrom8to33withameanageof21.75.

Interviews Datacollectionproceduresforthefirstthreewavesofthestudyhavebeen reported elsewhere (e.g., Eron et al., 1971; Huesmann et al., 1984, 2002;Lefkowitz, Eron, Walder, & Huesmann, 1977). At age 8, two main sources ofdatawereused:classroom­basedpeernominationsandextensiveindividualpar­entinterviews.Atage19,participantswereadministeredavarietyofself­reportmeasures,aswell aspeernominations, in individual interviewsatafieldoffice.

Page 324: the Psychology of Social Conflict and Aggression Sydney Symposium in Social Psychology

the effect of religiouS participation on aggreSSion 307

Atage48,interviewswereconductedbycomputerinafieldofficeandbymailortelephoneforparticipantswhocouldnotcometotheoffice.TheoffspringofthesubjectswereinterviewedusingthesameproceduresasforthesubjectsinWave4exceptthatphoneandmailinterviewswerenotconductedwithanywhowereyoungerthan13.

Attrition Information Ofthe39%whowerenot interviewedatage48,37wereconfirmed dead, 112 had disappeared and could not be found despite intenseefforts,40couldnotbeinterviewedbecauseofdistanceandschedulingdifficul­ties,and144refused.Acomparisonofmeansonage­8scoresrevealedthat,com­paredwithparticipantswhowerereinterviewedatage48,participantswhowerenotreinterviewedhadhigherlevelsofaggression,t (854)=4.06,p<.001(Mdifference=.13,SEdifference=.03),lowerlevelsofpopularity,t(854)=4.19,p<.001(Mdifference=4.45,SEdifference=1.06),lowerpeercompliance,t(854)=3.86,p<.001(Mdifference=3.40,SEdifference=.88),andlowerIQatage8,t(852)=5.69,p<.001(Mdifference=5.70,SEdifference=1.00).However,analysesofthe1960datafromthe39%whodroppedoutalsorevealedthattherewasnosubstantialrestrictionofrangeonany1960variableduetotheattrition.

Measures

Specific aggression measures for all Waves Peer-nominatedaggres-sionwasassessedatages8and19usingapeer­nominationproceduredevelopedbyEronetal.(1971),whodefinedaggressionas“anactwhosegoalresponseisinjurytoanotherobject”(p.30).Their10peer­nominatedaggressionitemscoverphysical(e.g.,“Whopushesandshovesotherchildren?”),verbal(e.g.,“Whosaysmeanthings?”),acquisitive(e.g.,“Whotakesotherchildren’sthingswithoutask­ing?”),andindirect(e.g.,“Whomakesupstoriesandliestogetotherchildrenintotrouble?”)aggressiveacts.Thescore(α=.90)representstheproportionoftimesthechildwasnominatedbyclassmatesonthe10itemsoutof the timesthechildcouldhavebeennominated.Atage8thiswasthenumberofchildrenin theclassroom.Atage19,becauseparticipantsalreadyhad lefthighschool,the proportion for a participant was computed based on the number of otherparticipantswhosaidtheyknowthatparticipant“wellenoughtoanswersomequestionsaboutthem.”

Self-reports on peer-nomination questionswereobtainedforthechildrenwhowereinterviewedinWave4astheirwidegeographicdistributionmakeobtainingpeernominationsimpossible.Fortheyoungeroffspringthesamequestionswereusedashadbeenusedforthesubjectswhentheywere8yearsold;fortheolderoffspringweusedthequestionsthathadbeenusedwiththe19­year­olds.

Severe physical aggressionwasassessedforthesubjectatages19,30,and48andforthechildofthesubjectinWave4throughself­reportsofhowofteninthelastyeartheyengagedineachoffourbehaviors:(1)chokedsomeone;(2)slappedorkickedsomeone;(3)punchedorbeatensomeone;(4)knifedorshotatsomeoneorthreatenedtodoit(1=neverto4=alot).Scoreswerelog­transformedforanalysisduetoskewness(α=.66).

Page 325: the Psychology of Social Conflict and Aggression Sydney Symposium in Social Psychology

l. roWell hueSmann, eric f. duBoW, and paul Boxer308

Aggressive personality was measured at ages 19, 30, and 48 and amongchildrenof the subjectswhowere13orolder inWave4by taking the sumofscales4,9,andFfromtheMinnesotaMultiphasicPersonalityInventory(MMPI;Hathaway&McKinley,1940).Inearlierstudiesbyourgroup(e.g.,Huesmannetal.,1984;Huesmann,Lefkowitz,&Eron,1978),thesummedT­scoresofthesethreescalesreflectedareliableandvalidmeasureofantisocial–aggressivebehav­ior(α=.78).

Fortheanalyses,wefirstconvertedtheaggressionmeasuresobtainedineachwave (peer nomination, self­report of peer­nomination questions, self­report ofseriousphysicalaggression,MMPIF+4+9)tostandardizedz­scores.Ateachage, where more than one aggression measure exists (i.e., ages 19 and 30), wecomputedameasurementmodelforcombiningthemeasures.Thenacompositemeasureofaggressionwascomputedas theweightedmeanof theoneto threeaggression scores available for the subject during that wave or for the subject’schildduringWave4.Becausethesecompositescoresarestandardizedwithineachwaveofdata,theyprovideastandardscaleonwhichindividuals’locationscanbecomparedacrosswaves independentlyoftotalsampleshifts inaggressivenessordifferencesinmeasuresobtained.

religiosity In Wave 4, the subjects and their offspring both indicated their frequency of religious service attendance on anine­point scale (“Howoftendoyouattendreligiousservices?”,ratedas1=never,2=lessthanonceayear,3=1–2timesayear,4=severaltimesayear,5=aboutonceamonth,6=2–3timesamonth,7=nearlyeveryweek,8=everyweek,and9=severaltimesaweek).Additionally,boththesubjectsandtheiroffspringreportedontheirreligiouspref­erence;theirspirituality(“Towhatextentdoyouconsideryourselfaspiritualorreligiousperson?”,ratedas1=notspiritualorreligiousatall,2=slightlyspiritualorreligious,3=moderatelyspiritualorreligious,4=veryspiritualorreligious);andtheirfrequencyofpraying(“Howoftendoyouprayprivatelyinplacesotherthanachurch,mosque,orsynagogue?”,ratedas1=never,2=lessthanonceamonth,3=onceamonth,4=afewtimesamonth,5=onceaweek,6=afewtimesaweek,7=onceaday,8=morethanonceaday).

InWave1,theparentsofthesubjectswerealsoaskedtheirfrequency of reli-gious service attendance (responsescale:0=never,1=afewtimesayear,2=aboutonceamonth,3=afewtimesamonth,4=onceaweek,5=morethanonceaweek).Theywerealsoaskedtheirreligionpreference,buttheywerenotaskedanyquestionsaboutspiritualityorpraying.

SimilarlyinWave2,the19­year­oldsubjectsthemselveswereaskedtoreportontheirfrequency of religious service attendanceusingthesameprocedureandscaleasusedwiththeparentsinWave1.

other outcomes and covariates InWave4,wealsoassessedthenorma-tive beliefs about aggressionofboththesubjectsandtheiroffspring(Huesmann&Guerra,1997).Thenormativebeliefscaleisa20­itemscalethataskstherespon­dentabouthisorherapprovalofaggression,suchas,“Supposeamansayssome­thingbadtoanotherman,John.DoyouthinkitisOKforJohntohithim?(4=

Page 326: the Psychology of Social Conflict and Aggression Sydney Symposium in Social Psychology

the effect of religiouS participation on aggreSSion 309

perfectlyOK,3=sortofOK,2=sortofwrong,1=reallywrong).Thenormativebeliefsscalescore is themeanofall theresponsesandhasbeenshowntobeahighlyreliableassessmentofapprovalofaggressionbytherespondent(Huesmann&Guerra,1997).

InWave1,parents’ educational level(Eronetal.,1971)reflectstheparents’self­reportedlevelsofeducationalattainment(1=under7yearsto7=graduateorprofessionaltraining).Thefamilyscorewascomputedasthemeanofthemother’sandfather’seducationallevel.

Finally,inWave1wealsoobtainedthesubject’sIQ score.Thechild’sIQwasassessedwiththeCaliforniaShort­FormTestofMentalMaturity(Sullivan,Clark,&Tiegs,1957).Kuder–Richardsonreliabilitycoefficients rangefrom .87 to .89acrossgrades;thetotalscorecorrelatesapproximately.75withotherIQmeasures.

Results

religions of participants InFigure 19.1, thedistributionof self­reportedreligious affiliations is shown for the participants in the study when they were8 years old (as reported by their parents) and when they were 48 years old (asself­reported).InFigure 19.2,theirchurch(orsynagogueormosque)attendanceis graphed for the same two times. The sample was predominately Christian–ProtestantandChristian–Catholicin1960withasmallsampleofJewishandother(including“no”)affiliations.By2000,thesamplewasstillpredominatelyProtestantorCatholic,butamuchlargerproportionreported“other”or“no”affiliation.Also,asshowninFigure19.2,by2000whenthesubjectswere48yearsold,onaveragetheyattendedreligiousservicesmuchlessthantheirparentshadattendedthem40yearsearlier.

religiosity The three measures of religiosity that were assessed in Wave 4amongthe48­year­oldsubjectsandamongtheiroffspring(averageage=21.75)werehighlycorrelatedasshowninTable 19.1.Afactoranalysisofthethreemea­suresshowedthatonefactorcouldexplain69to71%ofthevarianceinthescales

Protest

Cath

Other

0.7

0.6

0.5

0.4

0.3

0.2

0.1

0Mother Father

Protest

Cath

Other

0.7

0.6

0.5

0.4

0.3

0.2

0.1

0Females Males

figure 19.1 Thereligionsoftheparentsofthesubjectswhenthesubjectswereage8(leftpanel)andthereligionsofthesubjectsthemselves40yearslateratage48(rightpanel).

Page 327: the Psychology of Social Conflict and Aggression Sydney Symposium in Social Psychology

l. roWell hueSmann, eric f. duBoW, and paul Boxer310

Pare

nts’

Chur

ch A

tten

danc

e19

60Su

bjec

t’s C

hurc

h At

tend

ance

2000

0.5 0.45 0.4 0.35 0.3 0.25 0.2 0.15 0.1 0.05 0

0.5 0.45 0.4 0.35 0.3 0.25 0.2 0.15 0.1 0.05 0

Nev

erFe

w ti

mes

/yea

rO

nce/

mon

thSe

vera

l/mon

thEv

ery w

eek

Nev

erFe

w tim

es/y

ear

Onc

e/m

onth

Seve

ral/m

onth

Ever

y wee

k

fig

ure

19.2

T

hec

hurc

hat

tend

ance

oft

hep

aren

tso

fthe

sub

ject

sw

hen

the

subj

ects

wer

eag

e8

and

the

chur

cha

tten

danc

eof

the

subj

ects

them

­se

lves

40

year

s la

ter

ata

ge4

8.

Page 328: the Psychology of Social Conflict and Aggression Sydney Symposium in Social Psychology

the effect of religiouS participation on aggreSSion 311

in both cases. Furthermore, all three scales had loadings of .77 to .88 in bothcases.Consequently,anyoneofthemeasurescouldbeusedtorepresentreligiosityadequately.AsonlythemeasureofreligiousparticipationwascollectedinWaves1and2aswellasinWave4onthesubjectsandontheirchildren,wedecidedtousethatmeasureforallanalyses.

continuity of religious participation The correlations over 40 yearsbetween the subject’s parent’s religious participation when the subject was 8yearsold, the subject’sown religiousparticipationatage19andatage48,andthesubject’schild’sreligiousparticipationwhenthesubjectwas48areshowninTable  19.2. Religious participation clearly displays continuity within and acrossgenerations.Mostnotably,perhaps,thesubject’schild’sreligiousparticipationcor­relates.52(p<.001)withthesubject’sconcurrentreligiousparticipationatage48,correlates.28(p<.001)withthesubject’sreligiousparticipation30yearsearlier,andcorrelates.21(p<.001)withthegrandparent’sreligiousparticipation40yearsearlier.Ofcourse,theselatercorrelationsrepresentonlymodesteffectsizes,andthereissubstantialvariabilityinthetrajectoriesofreligiousparticipationoverthelifecourseandacrossgenerations.Whenwepartitionedreligiousparticipationintoupper,middle,andlowertertiles(calledHigh,Medium,andLowParticipation),wefoundthatinonlyabout44%ofthecaseswasthelevelofparticipationthesamewithinthesubjectatages19and48;inonlyabout20%ofthecaseswasthelevelofreligiousparticipationthesameforthefamilywhenthesubjectwas8,19,and48;andinonlyabout10%ofthecaseswasthelevelofparticipationexactlythesameforthegrandparent,thesubjectatage8and48,andforthesubject’schildwhenthesubjectwas48.Additionally,ingeneraltheparticipationratesdeclinedoverthe40yearsfrom1960to2000,aswasshowninFigure 19.2.

the relation of religious participation to aggression over time and generations InTable 19.3,thecorrelationsareshownbetweenthereli­giousparticipationofthesubject’sparents,thesubject,andthesubject’schildandtheconcurrentandsubsequentaggressivebehaviorandbeliefsofthesubjectand

taBle 19.1 correlations Between different components of religiosity in 2000 at age 48 (Below diagonal) and at age 12 to 30 (average age 21.75; above the diagonal)

Subject’s Self-Reported Religious

Participation

Subject’s Self-Reported Frequency

of PrayerSubject’s Self-

Reported Spirituality

Subject’sself­reportedreligiousparticipation

.48*(N=303)

.56*(N=204)

Subject’sself­reportedfrequencyofprayer

.52*(N=481)

.67*(N=204)

Subject’sself­reportedspirituality

.46*(N=479)

.65*(N=480)

* p<.001.

Page 329: the Psychology of Social Conflict and Aggression Sydney Symposium in Social Psychology

l. roWell hueSmann, eric f. duBoW, and paul Boxer312

thesubject’schild.Thecorrelationsateachagebetweenaperson’sownreligiousparticipationandhisorherownaggressivebehaviorarenegativeandsignificant.Higherreligiousparticipationisrelatedtolowerconcurrentaggression.Theeffectsizesarenotlargebutaresignificant:–.20atage19and–.13atage48.Additionally,thegrandparents’religiousparticipationassessedin1960notonlycorrelatessig­nificantlynegatively(–.09,p<.05)withtheirchild’sconcurrentaggressionatage8butalsocorrelatessignificantlynegativelywiththeirgrandchild’saggressionandaggressivebeliefs 40 years later (–.15,p< .01; –.13, p < .05).This is true eventhough the grandparent’s religious participation does not correlate significantlywiththesubject’s(theirownchild’s)aggressionatage19or48,andthesubject’sreligiousparticipationatage48doesnotcorrelate significantlywith thegrand­child’sconcurrentaggressiveness.Allinall,thistableofnegativecorrelationspro­videsevidencethatnotonlyisaperson’saggressivenessnegativelyrelatedtotheirconcurrentreligiousparticipation,butitisalsorelatednegativelytohigherlevelsofreligiousparticipationwithinthefamilysystem.

It is illustrative toexamine these relationsover time in termsofhowpredic­tive very frequent religious participation is of lower aggression compared withveryinfrequentreligiousparticipation.Wepartitionedreligiousparticipationintoapproximatethirdswherehighparticipationmeansattendingservicesonceaweekormore,lowparticipationmeansattendingchurchneverornomorethanonceayear, andmediumparticipation iseverything inbetween.We thenanalyzed themeandifferencesforthehighandlowgroupsontheaggressionmeasuresateachpointintime.TheresultsareshowninFigure 19.3.Ineverysinglecase,thosehighinreligiousparticipationscoreloweronaggressionandaggressivebeliefsthanthoselowinreligiousparticipation.However,notalloftherelationsaresignificant.Mostly

taBle 19.2 correlations of religiosity over three generations and 48 years

Subject’s Parents’ Religious

Participation When Subject

Is Age 8

Subject’s Religious

Participation at Age 19

Subject’s Religious

Participation at Age 48

Subject’s Child’s

Religious Participation When Subject

Is Age 48

Subject’sparents’religiousparticipationwhensubjectisage8

Subject’sreligiousparticipationatage19

.36**(N=374)

Subject’sreligiousparticipationatage48

.17*(N =401)

.31**(N=305)

Subject’schild’sreligiousparticipationwhensubjectisage48

.21**(N=274)

.28** .52**(N=196) (N=294)

* p<.01.** p<.001.

Page 330: the Psychology of Social Conflict and Aggression Sydney Symposium in Social Psychology

the effect of religiouS participation on aggreSSion 313

theresultsareconsistentwiththecorrelationsinTable 19.3butshowthatthecorre­lationsreflectlargedifferencesbetweenfairlyhighandfairlylowlevelsofparticipa­tionratherthansmalldifferencesacrossthecontinuumofparticipationscores.

predicting adult aggression from youth aggression and religious participation Inanumberofpriorpublications,thecontinuityofaggressionwithin and across generations in the Columbia County Longitudinal Study hasbeenshowntobesubstantial(Huesmannetal.,1984,2009).Theanalysessofarhaveshownboththatthereiscontinuityofreligiousparticipationacrosstimeandgenerationsandthatreligiousparticipation is inverselyrelatedtoaggressivenessconcurrentlyandovertimeandgenerations.Giventheseresults, itmakessensetoexaminewhetherreligiousparticipationinyouthpredictsadultaggressivenesswhenonecontrolsforyouthaggression.Wecreatedacompositereligiouspartici­pationscore forWaves1and2by takingthemeanofthestandardizedpartici­pationscoresforeachwave(Wave1religiousparticipationreportedbysubject’sparentandWave2 religiousparticipation reportedby subject).WealsocreatedacomparablecompositeaggressionscoreforWaves1and2inthesameway.Weconductedaregressionanalysispredicting thesubject’sWave4aggressionfromthese twovariables and their interaction (productof their standardized scores).TheresultsareshowninTable 19.4.

taBle 19.3 correlations of religious participation With aggression over three generations

Subject’s Parents’ Religious

Participation When Subject

Is Age 8

Subject’s Religious

Participation at Age 19

Subject’s Religious

Participation at Age 48

Subject’s Child’s

Religious Participation When Subject

Is Age 48

Subject’saggressionatage8

–.09**(N=706)

–.04(N=427)

–.04(N=481)

–.05(N=325)

Subject’saggressionatage19

–.04(N=373)

–.20****(N=426)

–.21****(N=305)

–.14**(N=196)

Subject’saggressionatage48

–.05(N=399)

–.09(N=303)

–.13***(N=476)

–.16***(N=293)

Subject’sbeliefsapprovingofaggressionatage48

–.02(N=398)

–.11**(N=304)

–.22****(N=476)

–.12**(N=293)

Subject’schild’saggressionwhensubjectisage48

–.15***(N=292)

–.17**(N=215)

–.07(N=316)

–.13**(N=325)

Subject’schild’sbeliefsapprovingofaggressionwhensubjectisage48

–.13**(N=275)

–.07(N=208)

–.11*(N=304)

–.19****(N=307)

* p<.10.** p<.05.*** p<.01.**** p<.001.

Page 331: the Psychology of Social Conflict and Aggression Sydney Symposium in Social Psychology

l. roWell hueSmann, eric f. duBoW, and paul Boxer314

0.3

0.2

0.1 0

–0.1

–0.2

–0.3

Lo R

elig

Hi R

elig

Lo R

elig

Hi R

elig

Lo R

elig

Hi R

elig

Lo R

elig

Hi R

elig

Subj

’sAg

g at

Age 8

Subj

’sAg

g at

Age 1

9

Subj

’sAg

g at

Age 4

8 Subj

’sAg

g at

Age 4

8

Subj

’sAg

gBe

liefs

Age 4

8

Child

’sAg

g whe

nSu

bj is

48

Child

’sAg

gBe

liefs

when

Subj

is 48

Child

’sAg

g whe

nSu

bj is

48

Child

’sAg

gBe

liefs

when

Subj

is 48

Subj

’sAg

gBe

liefs

Age 4

8

Child

’sAg

gwh

enSu

bjis

48

Child

’sAg

gBe

liefs

when

Subj

is 48

Subj

’sAg

g at

Age 1

9

Subj

’sAg

g at

Age 4

8

Subj

’sAg

gBe

liefs

Age 4

8

Child

’sAg

gwh

enSu

bjis

48

Child

’sAg

gBe

liefs

when

Subj

is 48

p <

.03

p <

.02

p <

.01

p <

.02

p <

.01

p <

.06

n. s.

n. s.

n. s.

n. s.

p <

.02

p <

.001

p <

.05

p <

.02

n. s.

n. s.

n. s.

Aggression z-score

0.3

0.2

0.1 0

–0.1

–0.2

–0.3

–0.4

Aggression z-score

0.3

0.2

0.1 0

–0.1

–0.2

–0.3

–0.4

Aggression z-score

0.3

0.2

0.1 0

–0.1

–0.2

–0.3

Aggression z-score

Agg

ress

ion

as F

unct

ion

of P

aren

ts’ R

elig

ious

Part

icip

atio

n W

hen

Subj

ect W

as A

ge 8

Agg

ress

ion

as F

unct

ion

of S

ubje

ct’s

Age

19

Relig

ious

Par

ticip

atio

n

Agg

ress

ion

as F

unct

ion

of S

ubje

ct’s

Age

48

Relig

ious

Par

ticip

atio

nA

ggre

ssio

n as

Fun

ctio

n of

Chi

ld’s

Relig

ious

Part

icip

atio

n W

hen

Subj

ect’s

Age

48

fig

ure

19.3

M

ean

aggr

essi

veb

ehav

iora

ndm

ean

aggr

essi

veb

elie

fsfo

rthe

subj

ecta

tage

8,1

9,a

nd4

8an

dfo

rth

esu

bjec

t’sc

hild

whe

nth

esu

bjec

tis

48

asa

fun

ctio

nof

the

fre

quen

cyo

fre

ligio

usp

artic

ipat

ion

oft

hes

ubje

ct,t

hes

ubje

ct’s

pare

nts,

and

the

sub

ject

’sch

ildo

ver

the

40y

ears

of

the

stud

y.

Page 332: the Psychology of Social Conflict and Aggression Sydney Symposium in Social Psychology

the effect of religiouS participation on aggreSSion 315

Asexpected,youthaggressivenessisrevealedtobeahighlysignificantpredic­torofadultaggressiveness30to40yearslater(β=.38, p<.001).Theregressionalsorevealsthatayouth’slevelofreligiousparticipation30to40yearsearlierdoesnotaddatallsignificantlytothispredictioneventhoughtheirreligiousparticipa­tionatage8and19correlatednegativelywiththeirconcurrentaggressionatthattime.However,whilethatearlyreligiousparticipationdoesnothaveadirecteffecton adult aggression, it does significantly moderate the trajectory of aggressionfromyouthtoadulthoodas indicatedbythehighlysignificant interactioneffectofyouthparticipationandyouthaggressiononadultaggression(β=.15,p < .002).Tounderstandthemeaningofthisinteraction,weplotteditinFigure 19.4intwoways—firstasathree­dimensionalplotshowingthesurfacedefinedbythecom­plete regressionequationandsecondas a limitplot showinghowhighand lowyouthaggressionandhighandlowreligiousparticipationinyouth(asdefinedbyplusandminusoneSD)combinetopredictadultaggression.

Theresultsarestriking.Ifoneacceptsthatthedirectionofeffectsmustbefromreligiousparticipationtoaggression,theresultsindicatethathighreligiouspartici­pationexacerbatestheeffectsofyouthaggressiononadultaggression.Forthoseloweronyouthaggression,highreligiousparticipationispredictiveofevenloweradultaggressionandlowerparticipationofhigheraggression.Forthosehigheronyouthaggression,theeffectisreversed.Highreligiousparticipationispredictiveofevenhigheradultaggressionandlowerparticipationofloweraggression.

Onemaywonder if theseeffectsare independentofotherparticipant char­acteristics.ThethirdcolumnofTable 19.4showsthattheyare.Theparticipant’s

taBle 19.4 multiple regression predicting Subjects’ aggressive Behavior at age 48 from their aggressive Behavior at age 8 and 19 and their participation in religious Services at age 8 and 19 controlling for their gender, intelligence, and parents’ educational level

Step 1 Standardized Regression Coefficients

Step 2 Standardized Regression Coefficients

Step 3 Standardized Regression Coefficients

Subject’saggressionasyouth(meanofages8&19)

.35** .38** .38**

Subject’sreligiousparticipationasyouth(meanofages8&19)

–.04 –.04 –.02

Interactionofsubject’syouthaggressionandyouthparticipationinreligiousactivities

.15* .15*

Subject’sgender –.01Subject’sIQatage8 .04Subject’sparent’slevelofeducation –.17**R2 .123** .143** .169*** p<.01.** p<.001.

Page 333: the Psychology of Social Conflict and Aggression Sydney Symposium in Social Psychology

l. roWell hueSmann, eric f. duBoW, and paul Boxer316

genderandIQatage8donotchangetheresultsatall,and,while theparent’seducationlevelisasignificantpredictorofthesubject’saggression40yearslater,itsinclusioninthemodeldoesnotchangethemoderatingeffectofreligiouspar­ticipationinyouth.

A similar regression analysis was conducted to predict the subject’s child’saggressionwhenthesubjectwas48(meanageofchild=21.75).Whilethesub­ject’slifelongaggressionwasahighlysignificantpredictorofthesubject’schild’saggressionwhenthesubjectwas48(β=.25,p<.001),theanalysisrevealednosimilarinteractiveeffectofthesubject’slifelongreligiousparticipationoncross­generationaltransmissionofaggressionandnomaineffectofthesubject’sreligiousparticipationontheoffspring’saggression.

Finally,weconstructedalongitudinalstructuralmodeltorepresentboththeeffectsofreligiousparticipationonaggressionandthecontinuityofaggressionandreligiousparticipationwithinandacrossgenerations.ThefinalmodelthatbestfitthedataisshowninFigure 19.5.

Again,wemaketheassumptioninthismodelthattherelationbetweenreli­giousparticipationandaggression(totheextentanyrelationexists)isinthecausaldirectionofparticipationaffectingaggression.Themodelfitsquitewell(fullinfor­mationmaximumlikelihood[FIML]solution,N=856,chi­square=27.8,df=23,p>.22,comparativefitindex[CFI]=.99,rootmeansquareerrorofapproxima­tion[RMSEA]=.016).Themodelshowstheexpectedstrongcontinuityofbothaggressionandreligiosityoverthelifecourseandacrossgenerationswithstrongercontinuitywithingenerationsforaggressionandacrossgenerationsforreligiosity.Themodelalsoshowssignificantconcurrentdirectnegativeeffectsofthesubject’sreligiosityatage19tohisorheraggressionatage19andfromtheparent’sreligios­ityin1960tothechild’sconcurrentaggressionatage8.Theeffectsfromsubjects’age­48 religiosity to their age­48aggressionwereonlymarginally significant asweretheeffectsofchildren’sreligiosityontheirownaggressionin2000.However,takentogether,thefourconcurrentpathscertainlyindicatethatreligiosityhasadampening main effect on concurrent aggression. Furthermore, the interactiveeffectofsubjects’religiosityandaggressionintheiryouthontheiradultaggression

Hi Agg inYouthLo Agg inYouth

Lo Religiosityin Youth

Hi Religiosityin Youth

0.6

0.4

0.2

0

–0.2

–0.4

–0.6

–0.8

2

1

0

–1

Age 4

8 Ag

gres

sion

z-sc

ore

Adul

t-Agg

Youth-Religiosity

Youth-Agg –10

0

1

1

2

2–2

–2–1

figure 19.4 The moderating effect of youth religious participation on the relationbetweenyouthaggression(meanofages8and19)andadultaggressionatage48.

Page 334: the Psychology of Social Conflict and Aggression Sydney Symposium in Social Psychology

the effect of religiouS participation on aggreSSion 317

thatwehaddiscoveredwiththeregressionanalysisremainedsignificantandsub­stantialinthismodel(β=.12,p<.008).Beinghighonreligiosityinyouthseemstoexacerbatethetendencyoflowaggressiveyouthtowardlowaggressioninthefutureandhighaggressiveyouthtowardhighaggressioninthefuture.Themodelexplains24%ofthevariationinthesubject’sage­48aggression,12%ofthevaria­tioninthesubject’sage­48religiosity,7%ofthevariationinthesubject’schild’saggression, and27%of the variation in thechild’s religiosity.Thestandardizedtotaleffectsizesofpriorfamilyreligiositycombinedwithconcurrentself­religios­ityonaggressionwere.078forage­48subjectaggressionand.146forthesubject’schildreninWave4.Thoughthesearenotlargeeffectsizes,theyaresignificantandlargeenoughtobeimportant.

+ p < .20 * p < .10 ** p < .05 *** p < .01

Child Relig2000

–.07+

–.06+

–.13**

–.10**

–.15***–.13***

–.10** –.05

–.15***

–.10***

–.18***.19***

.30***

.12***

.50***

.22***

.21***

.43***

.49***

.13**

.38***

.20***

Subj ReligAge 482000

Subj ReligAge 191971

Par Relig1960

Sex0=F, 1=M

Subj AggAge 81960

Subj AggAge 191971

Subj AggAge 482000

Child Agg2000

InteractionRelig & Agg1960 & 71

figure 19.5 Structuralmodelshowingdirectandindirecteffectsofreligiousparticipa­tionthroughoutthelifecourseonsubsequentaggressionintheselfandinone’soffspring(fullinformationmaximumlikelihood[FIML]solution,N=856,chi­square=27.8,df=23,p> .22,comparativefitindex[CFI]=.99,rootmeansquareerrorofapproximation[RMSEA]=.016).

Page 335: the Psychology of Social Conflict and Aggression Sydney Symposium in Social Psychology

l. roWell hueSmann, eric f. duBoW, and paul Boxer318

Discussion

Ouranalysisofthesefourwavesofdataspanningthreegenerationsand40yearsshowedfirstthatreligiousparticipation,prayer,andspiritualityarehighlyintercor­relatedandcanberepresentedsubstantiallybyasingleconstruct.Becauseofthisresultandbecauseparticipationinreligiousservicesistheonlymeasurewehadinallfourwaves,webasedallouranalysesonthismeasure.Obviously,thisisapotentialweakness,andourresultsmustbeconsideredinthecontextthatspiritualindividualswhoneverparticipate in religious servicesaremisclassified in theseanalyses.Ofcourse,themostlikelyeffectofthisomissionwouldbetoweakenoureffectsizesforreligiosity.

We found clear evidence both thatparticipation in religious activities has amaineffectonreducingconcurrentaggressionatanyageandinyouthhasanaddi­tionaleffectofexacerbatingthetendenciesoflowaggressiveyouthtogrowuptobelowaggressiveadultsandofhighaggressiveyouthtogrowuptobehighaggres­siveadults.Theseeffectswerenotduetorelationsbetweenreligiosityandgender,IQ,ortheeducationallevelofthefamily.Theseeffectsremainedinthecontextofalongitudinalmodelthataccountedforthesubstantialcontinuityofreligiosityandaggressionbothoverthelifespanandacrossgenerations.

Althoughthisstudydemonstratestheseeffectsfairlyconclusively,itdoesnotexplainwhytheyoccur.Aswediscussedintheintroduction,religiosity,andpar­ticularlyparticipation inreligiousactivities,hasanumberofbenefitsthatcouldexplainthemaineffectsofreligiosityinreducingaggressioninadditiontoaffectingnormativebeliefsaboutaggression.Thethreemaintheoreticalideaswereviewedwereasfollows:

1.Parents’ religiosity is a marker of more proximal factors that influencechildoutcomes(e.g.,goodparenting,thechild’sdevelopingreligiosity).

2.Religiousorganizationsprovidesocialsupportwhenproblemsoccur. 3.Religious exposurebuilds strong internal self­regulating standards in a

child,suchasnormativebeliefsopposingaggression.

Relevanttothethirdexplanation,wedidshowthatanadult’sandyouth’snormative beliefs about the appropriateness of aggression were significantlyrelated to their religiosity in the direction that more religiosity predictedlower approval of aggression. However, the direct relation between religios­ityandconcurrentnormativebeliefswasmodest(–.19to–.22inTable 19.3).Consequently, while we could not directly test mediation models becausescores on normative beliefs were available only in Wave 4 of the study, it isunlikelythattheeffectsizesofreligiosityonnormativebeliefsarelargeenoughtocompletelyexplainthetotaleffectsofreligiosityonaggression.Similarly,thefactthattherelationsbetweenreligiosityandaggressionwerenotdiminishedmuchwhenwecontrolledforgender,childIQ,andparentallevelofeducationsuggeststhatnoassociationbetweenreligiosityandanyoftheseothervariablesrelatedtoaggressioncanaccountfortheeffectbyitselfasthefirstexplanationmightsuggest.

Page 336: the Psychology of Social Conflict and Aggression Sydney Symposium in Social Psychology

the effect of religiouS participation on aggreSSion 319

Undoubtedly,ourmostnotableresultwasthefindingthathighreligiosityexac­erbates the tendencyofhigh­aggressiveyouth togrowup tobehigh­aggressiveadultsandlow­aggressiveyouthtogrowuptobelow­aggressiveyouth.Thisresultwasnotexpectedandiscontrarytoouroriginalhypothesisthatthesocialsupportprovided through participating in religious activities might ameliorate the ten­dencyofyouthtorespondtostressorsandsocialproblemswithaggression.

We propose that this interactive exacerbating effect most likely reflects a“self­justification”process.Mostreligioustextscanbereadindifferentwaysandcanequallywellprovidejustificationforbehavingaggressivelyorprosocially(seealso Chapter 18 in this volume for the possible divisive effects of supernatu­ralbeliefs).Christianscanfocuson“turning theothercheek”whenprovokedoronobtaining“aneye foraneye.” Ifonehasalreadybeenbehavingaggres­sively in one’s youth, participating in religious activities and focusing on textssupportingaggressionmaymakeiteasiertoself­justifyone’saggressivenessbyprovidingconsensual validation for thebehavior.On theotherhand, ifone isalreadybehavinglessaggressively,onecanfindconsensualvalidationforthosebehaviors in religion as well. Thus, while religious participation can promotepeacefulbehavioramongalreadypeacefulyouth,itcanalsoincreasetheriskforviolence(andfundamentalist terrorism;seeChapter10in thisvolume)amongthoseyouthleaningtowardaggression.

Certain cognitive characteristics associatedwith high religiousparticipationmay also contribute to the exacerbating effect of religiosity on early behavioraltrends.Researchon“cognitiveclosure”(Kruglanskietal.,1996)suggeststhathighneedforclosureindividuals“freeze”morestronglyonearlyideasandnorms.If,asseemsplausible,familieswhoparticipateregularlyinreligiousactivitieshaveahigherneedforcognitiveclosure,thenitwouldnotbesurprisingthattheirchil­drentendtocontinuedownthebehavioralpathsofaggressivenessornonaggres­sivenessthatareestablishedearlyinlife.

Final determination of the processes through which religious participa­tioninfluencesaggressionmustawaitmoredevelopmentalstudiesassessingthehypothesizedfactorsinvolvedintheprocesses.Nevertheless,itseemsfairtocon­cludefromthisstudythattheviewthatreligiosityhasastraightforwardprotec­tiveeffectinreducingthedevelopmentofaggressionistoosimplistic.Itistruethatparticipating inreligiousactivitieshasageneralprotectivemaineffectonconcurrent aggression and promotes religious participation later in life and insubsequentgenerationsandthat theseeffectsarerelatively independentofIQ,educational level, and gender. However, these main effects are limited by thesignificant tendencyofparticipation in religious activities to turn thedevelop­mentaltrajectoryofaggressionupwardforyouthhighinaggressionanddown­ward for youth low in aggression. Intense religious participation may promotenonaggressivepeacefulbehavioramongyouthalreadytendinginthatdirection,butitalsoseemstoexacerbatethetendenciesofaggressiveyouthtodevelopintomoreaggressiveyoungadults.Whether thisexacerbatingeffect ismoreduetothe self­affirming support for behavior that religion can provide or due to thetendencyofthoseneedingcognitiveclosuretoparticipateinreligionremainstobeinvestigated.

Page 337: the Psychology of Social Conflict and Aggression Sydney Symposium in Social Psychology

l. roWell hueSmann, eric f. duBoW, and paul Boxer320

referenceS

Bartkowski,J.P.,Xu,X.,&Levin,M.L.(2008).Religionandchilddevelopment:EvidencefromtheEarlyChildhoodLongitudinalStudy.Social Science Research, 37, 18–36.

Bridges,L.,&Moore,K.A.(2002).Religion and spirituality in childhood and adolescence. Washington,DC:ChildTrends.

Coleman,J.S.(1988).Socialcapitalinthecreationofhumancapital.American Journal of Sociology, 94, S95–S120.

Dubow,E.F.,Boxer,P.,&Huesmann,L.R.(2008).Childhoodandadolescentpredictorsofearlyandmiddleadulthoodalcoholuseandproblemdrinking:TheColumbiaCountyLongitudinalStudy.Addiction, 103,36–47.

Dubow, E. F., Huesmann, L. R., Boxer, P., Pulkkinen, L., & Kokko, K. (2006). Middlechildhood and adolescent contextual and personal predictors of adult educationalandoccupationaloutcomes:A mediationalmodel in twocountries.Developmental Psychology, 42(5),937–949.

Dubow,E.F.,&Ullman,D.G.(1989).Assessingsocialsupportinelementaryschoolchil­dren:Thesurveyofchildren’ssocialsupport.Journal of Clinical Child Psychology, 18, 52–64.

Ellis,L.,&Pettersson,J.(1996).Crimeandreligion:Aninternationalcomparisonamongthirteenindustrialnations.Personality and Individual Differences, 20,761–768.

Eron, L. D., Huesmann, L. R., & Zelli, A. (1991). The role of parental variables inthe learning of aggression. In D. Pepler & K. Rubin (Eds.), The development and treatment of childhood aggression (pp. 169–188). Hillsdale, NJ: LawrenceErlbaumAssociates.

Eron,L.D.,Walder,L.O.,&Lefkowitz,M.M.(1971).Learning of aggression in children.Boston:Little,Brown.

Flor, D. L., & Knapp, N. F. (2001). Transmission and transaction: Predicting adoles­cents’internalizationofparentalreligiousvalues.Journal of Family Psychology, 15,627–645.

Fowler, P. J., Ahmed, S. R., Tompsett, C. J., Jozefowicz­Simbeni, D. M. H., & Toro, P.A. (2008). Community violence and externalizing problems: Moderating effects ofraceand religiosity inemergingadulthood. Journal of Community Psychology, 36,835–850.

GallupConsultingOrganization. (2008).Available fromhttp://www.gallup.com/poll/1690/religion.aspx.

Gunnoe,M.,Hetherington,M.,&Reiss,D.(1999).Parentalreligiosity,parentingstyle,andadolescentsocialresponsibility.Journal of Early Adolescence, 19,199–225.

Gunnoe,M.L.,&Moore,K.A.(2002).Predictorsofreligiosityamongyouthaged17–22:AlongitudinalstudyoftheNationalSurveyofChildren.Journal for the Scientific Study of Religion, 41,613–622.

Hathaway,S.R.,&McKinley,J.C.(1940).The MMPI manual.NewYork:PsychologicalCorporation.

Herrenkohl,T.I.,Hill,K.G.,Chung,I.J.,Guo,J.,Abbott,R.D.,&Hawkins,J.D.(2003).Protectivefactorsagainstseriousviolentbehaviorinadolescence:Aprospectivestudyofaggressivechildren.Social Work Research, 27,179–191.

Hirschi,T.,&Stark,R.(1969).Hellfireanddelinquency.Social Problems, 17, 202–213.Huesmann,L.R.,Dubow,E.F.,&Boxer,P.(2009).Continuityofaggressionfromchild­

hoodtoearlyadulthoodasapredictoroflifeoutcomes:Implicationsfortheadoles­cent­limitedandlife­course­persistentmodels.Aggressive Behavior, 35, 136–149.

Page 338: the Psychology of Social Conflict and Aggression Sydney Symposium in Social Psychology

the effect of religiouS participation on aggreSSion 321

Huesmann,L.R.,Eron,L.D.,&Dubow,E.F.(2002).Childhoodpredictorsofadultcrimi­nality:Areallriskfactorsreflectedinchildhoodaggressiveness?Criminal Behavior and Mental Health, 12,185–208.

Huesmann,L.R.,Eron,L.D.,Lefkowitz,M.M.,&Walder,L.O.(1984).Stabilityofaggres­sionovertimeandgenerations.Developmental Psychology, 20, 1120–1134.

Huesmann,L.R.,&Guerra,N.G.(1997).Children’snormativebeliefsaboutaggressionandaggressivebehavior.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 72(2), 408–419.

Huesmann,L.R.,Lefkowitz,M.M.,&Eron,L.D.(1978).ThesumofMMPIscalesF,4,and9asameasureofaggression.Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology,46(5),1071–1078.

Johnson,B.R.,DeLi,S.,Larson,D.B.,&McCullough,M.(2000).Asystematicreviewofthereligiosityanddelinquencyliterature:Aresearchnote.Journal of Contemporary Criminal Justice, 16,32–52.

Kim,J.(2008).Theprotectiveeffectsofreligiosityonmaladjustmentamongmaltreatedandnon­maltreatedchildren.Child Abuse & Neglect, 32, 711–720.

Kim,J.,McCullough,M.E.,&Cicchetti,D.(2009).Parents’andchildren’sreligiosityandchildbehavioraladjustmentamongmaltreatedandnon­maltreatedchildren.Journal of Child and Family Studies,18,594–605.

Kirkpatrick,L.A.,&Shaver,P.R.(1990).Attachmenttheoryandreligion:Childhoodattach­ments,religiousbeliefs,andconversion.Journal for the Scientific Study of Religion, 29,315–334.

Koole,S.L.(2007).Raising spirits: An experimental analysis of the affectregulation func-tions of prayer. Unpublishedmanuscript,VrijeUniversiteit,Amsterdam.

Kruglanski,A.W.,&Webster,D.M.(1996).Motivatedclosingofthemind:“Seizing”and“freezing.”Psychological Review, 103, 263–283.

Lefkowitz,M.M.,Eron,L.D.,Walder,L.O.,&Huesmann,L.R.(1977).Growing up to be violent: A longitudinal study of the development of aggression.NewYork:Pergamon.

Mahoney,A.,Pargament,K.,Swank,A.,&Tarakeshwar,N.(2001).Religioninthehomeinthe1980sand1990s:Ameta­analyticreviewandconceptualanalysisoflinksbetweenreligion,marriage,andparenting.Journal of Family Psychology, 15, 559–596.

McCullough,M.E.,&Willoughby,B.L.B.(2009).Religion,self­regulation,andself­con­trol:Associations,explanations,andimplications.Psychological Bulletin, 135,69–93.

Pargament,K.I.(1997).The psychology of religion and coping: Theory, research, practice.NewYork:Guilford.

Pargament,K.I.(2007).Spiritually integrated psychotherapy: Understanding and address-ing the sacred.NewYork:Guilford.

Pearce,M. J., Jones,S.M.,Schwab­Stone,M.E.,&Ruchkin,V. (2003).Theprotectiveeffectsofreligiousnessandparentinvolvementonthedevelopmentofconductprob­lemsamongyouthexposedtoviolence.Child Development, 74,1682–1696.

Pettersson, T. (1991). Religion and criminality: Structural relationships between churchinvolvement and crime rates in contemporary Sweden. Journal for the Scientific Study of Religion, 30,279–291.

Regnerus,M.D.,&Elder,G.H.(2003).Stayingontrackinschool:Religiousinfluencesinhigh­andlow­risksettings. Journal for the Scientific Study of Religion, 42,633–649.

Smith,C.(2003).TheorizingreligiouseffectsamongAmericanadolescents.Journal for theScientific Study of Religion, 42,17–30.

Stevens,G.,&Hoisington,E.(1987).Occupationalprestigeandthe1980U.S.laborforce.Social Science Research, 6,74–105.

Sullivan,E.T.,Clark,W.W.,&Tiegs,E.W.(1957).California short-form test of mental maturity. LosAngeles:CaliforniaTestBureau.

Page 339: the Psychology of Social Conflict and Aggression Sydney Symposium in Social Psychology

l. roWell hueSmann, eric f. duBoW, and paul Boxer322

WallaceJr.,J.M.,Forman,T.A.,Caldwell,C.H.,&Willis,D.S.(2003).ReligionandU.S.secondary school students: Current patterns, recent trends, and sociodemographiccorrelates.Youth & Society, 35,98–125.

Warner,W.L.,Meeker,M.,&Eells,K.(1960).Social class in America.NewYork:Harcourt.Werner,E.E.,&Smith,R.S.(1982).Vulnerable but invincible: A longitudinal and youth

study of resilient children.NewYork:McGraw­Hill.WorldValuesSurvey.(1996).Availablefromhttp://www.worldvaluessurvey.org/

Page 340: the Psychology of Social Conflict and Aggression Sydney Symposium in Social Psychology

323

Indexa

AbusiveBehaviorInventory,61Abusiveindividuals,13,60,167–179,209,228Adolescents,27,269,270,274,302–303,

313–316Affect,119–138

andaggression,130–131andcognition,12,114andconflict,4,121,123–124andconflictmanagement,119–120andnegotiation,124–125andostracism,201–214andpartnerviolence,167–184andrelationshipconflict,167–184andstereotyping,130–131asinformationmodel,122emotionandmood,120–121

AffectInfusionModel,123–124Affectiveinfluences

oninformationprocessing,122–123onjudgmentalerrors,127–128onpersuasivecommunication,129–130onrequesting,125–126

Affectiveriskfactorforaggression,169–173Aggression

againstbystanders,208–210andautomaticity,84–86andclimatechange,249–266andheat,250–252,255andhumannature,4,14andmodeling,8,9,273andostracism,37–52,201–216andpersonalsignificance,153–166andreligion,301–319andself–discrepancy,153andthemedia,267–284betweengroups,27–31ethologicalapproachto,5,7,8evolutionaryapproachesto,14,15,16,20,

23,38,122,128,131,134,157,161,233–243,286

indevelopedvs.developingcountries,249–266

instincttheoriesof,5,6–7,237inintimaterelationships,167–179measurementandoperationalizationof,

10–11motivationsfor,53–64passive,53–64physicalvs.psychological,61

Agreeableness,220–221,222–223Amends,seeForgivenessAngermanagement,174–175

Anger,ix,8,13,23,55,56,59,70,72,86,88,112,139–152,171–175,205–206,209

expressionof,140–141regulationof,142–145,173–175

Antisocialbehavior,41–42,206,209,212,257Anxiety,19,20,25,26,27,28,29,31,75,121,

160–161,Approvalseeking,24Arab–Israeliwar,260–261Assertiveness,ix,12,44,65–82,105–106,125,

131–132,134–135andperspectivetaking,106definitionof,66–67expectancies,70–73motivations,73–74Attachmentandgenderdifferences,26inadulthood,21–23ofintergroupconflict,27–31orientations,23–27theory,ix,11,19–36

Attributions,42,44,45,47,91,96,124,127,128,141–142,171,211–212,

Automaticityofsocialbehavior,5,84–86Avoidance,7,19,20,25,26,27,30,239,243

B

Bangladesh,260Beliefs,285–286

ascommitmentdevices,285–296Broaden­and­buildmodel,22

c

Catharsis,8Civilwar,261Climatechangeandaggression,x,14,249–266Closemindedness,104,149Cognitiveappraisal,143Cognitiveinfluencesonaggression,12,168–169Cognitivemechanismsofaffectcongruence,

121–122Collectivismandaggression,154–155,158–159ColumbiaCountyLongitudinalStudy,305–310Communicationskillsandaggression,173–177Competitivealtruism,238Conflict

andaggression,definitionof,5–6,10–11inrelationships,13,185–197andassertiveness,67–68andculture,4,8,9,161,234,238,251,263

Page 341: the Psychology of Social Conflict and Aggression Sydney Symposium in Social Psychology

index324

andevolutionarytheory,233–248andforgiveness,217–232andgoals,185–197andintergrouprelations,285–300ethologicaltheoryof,7managementandinsecurity,25–26measurementandoperationalizationof,

10–11resolutionandperspectivetaking,110

Cooperation,4,29,88,104,108,112,124,188,296

Crimeandheat,250–252,255Cubanmissilecrisis,104Culturaltheoriesofviolence,168–169Cultureandconflict,4,14,Cyberbullying,269,274–275

d

Decisiontheory,286–287Dehumanization,37,236Delinquency,302–303,304–305Dictatorgame,131–133Displacedaggression,208–210Distressedcouples,167–179Domesticviolence,26,27,167–179,228Doormateffect,217–229Dualconcernmodel,108–111

e

Economictheories,132Emotion

affectandmood,12–121andostracism,205–206regulation,140–145innegotiation,146–147

Empathy,4,12,25,103–118,228andemotionalprediction,113–114

Ethnography,238Ethologicaltheoryofconflict,7,8Evolutionarytheoriesofaggression,9,14,15,

233–248,285–296Expectancies,69–70Extremistgroups,46–47

f

Feminism,x,13,168–169,170,173,178Feministtheoryofmaritalviolence,168–169,

170Forgivenesstest,221Forgiveness,14,76,217–232

effectsof,217–218perceptionsof,221–223

FrenchRevolution,261Frustration–aggressionhypothesis,8Fundamentalattributionerror,127–128

g

Genderandostracism,205–206Genderdifferences

inaggression,168–169,233–248inattachment,26

Genetic–environmentalinteraction,258–259Genocide,259–260Goalconflict,13–14,83–102,185–197Goalincompatibility,194–195Goalsimilarity,186–189Goal­biasedinferences,87–89Goal­directedaggression,94–95Goals,185–200

andmotives,83–102andnon­consciousbehavior,84–86andpolarization,86–87andrelationshipconflict,185–197

Groupbehavior,233–243

h

Heatandaggression,250–252,255–256Historyofresearchonsocialconflictand

aggression,ix–x,3–5,3–19Hooliganism,30,234,238Hormonesandaggression,9,243,256Hostility,11,175,Humannatureandaggression,4–5,233Hunter–gatherers,238

i

Iceageeffects,261Ideologyvs.scienceinexplainingviolence,

167–179Implicitmotivation,89–90Indirectaggression,11,54,56,57,58Industrializedsocieties,4Insecurity,26,27–31Instincttheoriesofaggression,5,6–7,237Interdependencetheory,187,214–215Intergroupaggression,234,235,239–240,

242–243,285–296Intergroupconflict,285–296

attachmenttheoryof,15,27–31andsupernaturalbeliefs,296

Internetandaggression,267–284Interpersonalconflict,19–36Intimatepartnerviolence,167–179,228

J

Justicesystem,13

l

Languageinstinct,7

Page 342: the Psychology of Social Conflict and Aggression Sydney Symposium in Social Psychology

index 325

Leadership,68–69,236,240–241Loyalty,236,241,292–295

m

Maleaggression,233–248Malewarriorhypothesis,168,233–243

definitionof,236psychologicalmechanismsof,239–242

Massmediaandviolence,270–271Mediaviolenceandaggression,8,15,267–284MiddleEastconflict,29,30Mixed­motivesituations,111–112Modeling,8,9,273Moodcongruence,122,123,124Moodeffects

onconflictandaggression,12,119–138oninformationprocessing,122–123,

127–128onrequestsanddemands,125–126

Mood,emotion,andaffect,120–121Moralsentiments,108Mortalitysalience,157–158Motivation–preventionvs.promotionfocus,

88,89Motivationforaggression,ix,8,57–59,73,

153–166Motivationforassertiveness,73–74Motivation,implicit,89–90,92Motivesofothers,83–84

n

Need­fortificationandostracism,42–43Needs,42–43,45–46,203–204Negotiation,ix,11,12,13,24,32,65,67,70,71,

72,73,76,104,106,108,109,110,111,124,135,139–152,162,219

andaffect,124–125andemotionregulation,146–148andperspectivetaking,109–110

Neuralevidenceforemotionregulation,145–146

Neuropsychology,120Non–conscioussocialbehavior,84–86,95Nonverbalmessages,38,40,93,96,105,Norms,9,120,132,133,142,157,161,237,241,

262,319Nuclearwar,9,103,104

o

Organizationalleadership,68–69Ostracism,ix,x,14,55,37–52,201–216

andaffect,201–214andaggression,206–208andantisocialbehavior,41–42andattributions,45

andneedfortification,42–43andpredictivecontrol,40andretaliation,213–214deletionsystem,38–39effectsof,203–205functionsof,202–203moderatorsof,211–213researchparadigmsof,43

p

Parent–childinfluences,15,19,21–22,301–318Partnerviolence,x,13,59,72,167–184Passiveaggression,53–64

definitionof,53–54Patriarchalideology,168,170Perpetratorsofviolence,167–168,218,225–227Perspectivetaking,103–118

andassertiveness,106andempathy,12andmixed–motivesituations,111–112andnegotiation,109–110

Persuasivecommunication,13,129–130,135,234,286

Pornography,268,271,274,276–277,279Poverty,15,154,155,251,252,253,254,257,

262,303Prejudice,27,89,234,236,240–241,243Promotionvs.preventionmotivation,88Prototypicality,29Psychoanalysis,5,6,7,8,121Psychologicalaggression,59–61Psychopathology,173–175,177Pugnacity,6

Q

Questforsignificanceandaggression,154–155Questforsignificancetheory,157–160

r

Rejectionandviolence,210;seealsoostracismRelationshipconflict,x,13,26,167–184,

185–200Religion,15,301–319Religiosityandaggression,302–304,311–312,

313–316Religiosityandfamilyinteraction,301–302,

304–305Religiousparticipation,311Reproductivesuccess,237Requesting,125–126Retaliation,213–214Revenge,55,206–207,213–214Riskfactorsforpartnerviolence,169–173Romanticrelationships,19,185–197Rumination,144–145

Page 343: the Psychology of Social Conflict and Aggression Sydney Symposium in Social Psychology

index326

S

Scepticism,128Schoolviolence,46,202,207,210,212,275Secondaryattachmentstrategies,20Self­conceptclarity,221–223,224,225Self­construals,159–161Self­discrepancyandaggression,153–154Self­harm,210–211Self­regulation,9,13

ofanger,142–145ofnegativeemotions,173–175

Self­respect,220–220,221–223,224,225Sexualexploitation,275–277Sexualviolence,278–279Silenttreatment–seeostracismSimilarity–attractionhypothesis,186–188Socialconflict

andaffect,119–138historyofresearchon,3–19resolutionof,26,103–118

Socialidentitytheory,27–28Sociallearningapproachtoviolence,170–171Socialperception,83,87,88,89,95Socialstatus,141,142,238Socializationandviolence,169–170Spousalabuse,167–179Stanfordprisonexperiment,238Stereotypingandaffect,130–131Stereotyping,69,240–241Subconscious,6,7,122Subliminalexposure,28,29,30,85,88,89,91,

93,130Suicide,156–157,167,210,211,258,279Supernaturalbeliefsandintergroupconflict,

296Supernaturalbeliefs,15,285–300

ascommitmentdevices,291–29theoriesof,288–290

SydneySymposiumofSocialPsychology,x–xi,

t

Televisionandaggression,267–284Terrorism,x,13,46–47,153–166,238,258–259,

285andcollectivism,155–156andquestforsignificance,154–155andsuicide,156–157motivationsfor,153–154

Thanatos,6,7Tribalbehavior,x,14,233,234,235,236,238,

239,241–242,243

u

Ultimatumgame,85,113,114,145,146,148

v

Vicariousexperience,75Victimbehavior,218,219–220Victimself–respect,220–221,223–224Videogames,271–271,273Violence

counselorsof,172determinantsof,168–169

Violenceagainstintimatepartners,167–179Violenceandostracism,205–206Violenceinthemedia,270–271ViolenceontheInternet,277–278Violence­proneindividuals,257–258

W

War,260–262WorldTradeCenter,285WorldWarI,6

y

YouTube,15