The Power of Numbers: Base-Ten Threshold Effects in Reported … · 2019. 1. 10. · 1 The Power of...

56
The Power of Numbers: Base-Ten Threshold Effects in Reported Revenue Derrald Stice* School of Business and Management Hong Kong University of Science and Technology [email protected] Earl K. Stice Marriott School of Business Brigham Young University [email protected] Han Stice CUHK Business School Chinese University of Hong Kong [email protected] Lorien Stice-Lawrence Leventhal School of Accounting University of Southern California [email protected] Draft: July 2018 We thank Dan Amiram, Erv Black, Zahn Bozanic, Mark Bradshaw (discussant), Ted Christensen, Jenny Chu, Mark DeFond, Ed deHaan, Greg Miller, Linda Myers (discussant), Kyle Peterson, Ethan Rouen (discussant), Roy Schmardebeck, and Tim Seidel for helpful comments and suggestions. We appreciate comments and suggestions from participants at the 2016 AAA Annual Meeting, BYU Accounting Research Symposium, 2016 HKUST Accounting Research Symposium, 2017 EAA Annual Meeting, the 2018 Multi-School University Conference at USC, and the 2018 CUHK Accounting Research Conference, as well as workshop participants at Chinese University of Hong Kong, University of Georgia, Georgia State University, Nazarbayev University, and University of Southern California. Professor E.K. Stice acknowledges financial support from the PricewaterhouseCoopers Professorship at Brigham Young University. *Address for correspondence: Department of Accounting, School of Business and Management, Hong Kong University of Science and Technology, Clear Water Bay, Kowloon, Hong Kong. Phone: 852-2358-7556. Email: [email protected].

Transcript of The Power of Numbers: Base-Ten Threshold Effects in Reported … · 2019. 1. 10. · 1 The Power of...

Page 1: The Power of Numbers: Base-Ten Threshold Effects in Reported … · 2019. 1. 10. · 1 The Power of Numbers: Base-Ten Threshold Effects in Reported Revenue ABSTRACT We provide evidence

0

The Power of Numbers: Base-Ten Threshold Effects in Reported Revenue

Derrald Stice*

School of Business and Management

Hong Kong University of Science and Technology

[email protected]

Earl K. Stice

Marriott School of Business

Brigham Young University

[email protected]

Han Stice

CUHK Business School

Chinese University of Hong Kong

[email protected]

Lorien Stice-Lawrence

Leventhal School of Accounting

University of Southern California

[email protected]

Draft: July 2018

We thank Dan Amiram, Erv Black, Zahn Bozanic, Mark Bradshaw (discussant), Ted

Christensen, Jenny Chu, Mark DeFond, Ed deHaan, Greg Miller, Linda Myers (discussant), Kyle

Peterson, Ethan Rouen (discussant), Roy Schmardebeck, and Tim Seidel for helpful comments

and suggestions. We appreciate comments and suggestions from participants at the 2016 AAA

Annual Meeting, BYU Accounting Research Symposium, 2016 HKUST Accounting Research

Symposium, 2017 EAA Annual Meeting, the 2018 Multi-School University Conference at USC,

and the 2018 CUHK Accounting Research Conference, as well as workshop participants at

Chinese University of Hong Kong, University of Georgia, Georgia State University, Nazarbayev

University, and University of Southern California. Professor E.K. Stice acknowledges financial

support from the PricewaterhouseCoopers Professorship at Brigham Young University.

*Address for correspondence: Department of Accounting, School of Business and

Management, Hong Kong University of Science and Technology, Clear Water Bay, Kowloon,

Hong Kong. Phone: 852-2358-7556. Email: [email protected].

Page 2: The Power of Numbers: Base-Ten Threshold Effects in Reported … · 2019. 1. 10. · 1 The Power of Numbers: Base-Ten Threshold Effects in Reported Revenue ABSTRACT We provide evidence

1

The Power of Numbers: Base-Ten Threshold Effects in Reported Revenue

ABSTRACT

We provide evidence that managers have a revealed preference for reporting total revenue numbers

just above base-ten thresholds (i.e., “round” numbers) of the form N × 10K. We are the first to

document several explanations for base-ten threshold-beating on the part of managers. First, we

find that formal and informal contracts which set revenue targets for firms—such as executive

compensation plan targets, management forecasts, and analyst forecasts—are subject to this base-

ten phenomenon, and firms appear to beat base-ten thresholds as a byproduct of beating these other

explicit targets. We also provide novel evidence that firms have incentives to engage in base-ten

beating behavior even in the absence of explicit targets; one specific motivator that we document

is an expected increase in press coverage. Lastly, we find that firms experience bursts in revenue

growth leading up to a base-ten revenue threshold and the new level of revenue attained appears

to be sustainable and the result of increased effort for firms on average, although firms with

particularly strong incentives to beat base-ten thresholds appear to engage in revenue management.

JEL Classification: G01, M4, M41

Key words: Base-Ten Thresholds, Revenue Quality, Analyst Forecast Heuristics, News

Coverage

Page 3: The Power of Numbers: Base-Ten Threshold Effects in Reported … · 2019. 1. 10. · 1 The Power of Numbers: Base-Ten Threshold Effects in Reported Revenue ABSTRACT We provide evidence

2

I. Introduction

We examine salient numbers, or cognitive reference points, at base-ten thresholds (i.e.,

“round” numbers) and provide evidence that firms take strategic actions in order to report revenue

just above these thresholds. These base-ten thresholds take the general form of N × 10K, where N

and K are integers. Examples are $1 million (1 × 106), $300 million (3 × 108), $7 billion (7 × 109),

and so forth. Most importantly, we provide novel evidence relating to two major research

questions: why do reported revenues exhibit this regularity? and what actions do firms take in order

to pass these thresholds (how do they beat them)?

Substantial research in fields outside of accounting has documented the salience of base-

ten numbers. For example, Rosch (1975) concludes that humans use numbers that are factors of

ten as reference points when evaluating all other numbers, and Schindler and Wiman (1989) find

that round numbers are easier to remember and come to mind more readily. This round-number

preference may influence managers to desire to report revenue above a base-ten threshold for

several reasons. For example, a processing constraint among investors, analysts, and business

reporters associated with base-ten numbers may lead to an increase in visibility for firms reaching

a base-ten threshold point in revenue. This increase in attention may then lead to a net increase in

buying by individual investors and a net increase in coverage by the media (Odean, 1998; Barber

and Odean, 2008). These human perception explanations imply that there could be a

disproportionate number of firms reporting revenue just above rather than just below base-ten

thresholds if managers take actions to exploit this processing constraint of financial market

participants or if managers are subject to this constraint themselves. If investors, analysts, and

other market participants have a preference for base-ten thresholds, then it would be rational for a

Page 4: The Power of Numbers: Base-Ten Threshold Effects in Reported … · 2019. 1. 10. · 1 The Power of Numbers: Base-Ten Threshold Effects in Reported Revenue ABSTRACT We provide evidence

3

manager to expend costly effort to reach a base-ten revenue threshold in order to attract more

market attention and potentially enhance firm valuation.

Empirically, we use approximate randomization techniques to show that firms are

significantly more likely to report revenue just above base-ten thresholds ($10 million, $200

million, $7.0 billion, and so forth) than just below. We also demonstrate that this base-ten revenue

threshold effect is the result of intentional actions and not a byproduct of the data generation

process or innocuous rounding of all financial numbers.

Critically, we also provide novel evidence exploring several explanations for this revenue

management behavior on the part of managers. We find that revenue targets specified in formal

and informal contracts, including executive compensation plans, management forecasts, and

analyst forecasts, also exhibit this base-ten effect, with significantly more revenue targets falling

just above than just below base-ten thresholds. External pressure from boards and shareholders to

meet these targets drives reported revenues to also exhibit a discontinuity around base-ten

thresholds. Thus, one explanation for the observed base-ten threshold effect in reported revenues

is the base-ten preference of the parties who set external performance targets.

In addition, we also provide evidence that this base-ten threshold beating behavior exists

in the absence of explicit targets or pressures such as executive compensation contracts,

management forecasts, or analyst forecasts. This evidence further underscores the novelty of our

results because we document an instance of threshold-beating not previously explored in the

accounting literature. Given the great prevalence of base-ten thresholds and the potential for the

existence of other implicit targets (for example, beating the maximum revenue ever reached by

other firms in the same industry), we find these initial results to be both exciting and important.

Page 5: The Power of Numbers: Base-Ten Threshold Effects in Reported … · 2019. 1. 10. · 1 The Power of Numbers: Base-Ten Threshold Effects in Reported Revenue ABSTRACT We provide evidence

4

We explore in more depth one specific incentive for firms to beat base-ten revenue

thresholds even in the absence of explicit targets, namely visibility, and find that firms which

exceed a base-ten threshold for the first time experience increases in press coverage. Most of these

increases are due to an increase in coverage by third parties, but firms also increase their own

coverage of their performance through press releases, potentially as a way of drawing further

attention to their base-ten threshold beating revenue. Given that press coverage can affect other

market outcomes with ties to firm value such as liquidity and price efficiency (Fang and Peress,

2009; Bushee et al., 2010; Drake et al., 2014), expected increases in visibility provide a powerful

incentive for firms to beat base-ten thresholds.

Lastly, we show that firms experience short-term bursts in revenue growth that enable them

to cross a base-ten threshold for the first time, and on average this leads to sustainable increases in

the level of revenue after crossing a base-ten threshold. This suggests that for the average firm, the

incremental revenue required to push firms past a base-ten threshold for the first time is the result

of additional effort motivated by the salience of base-ten targets. However, we also provide

evidence that firms which have particularly strong incentives to beat a base-ten threshold in a given

period have positive discretionary revenues, consistent with them engaging in less sustainable

actions commonly studied in the earnings management literature.

Our study has important implications for practitioners and academics. First, our study

provides novel evidence on the motives for and means by which firms meet base-ten revenue

thresholds. Although prior evidence has documented unusual patterns in earnings (Carslaw, 1988;

Thomas, 1989), no prior studies have tied these patterns to economic incentives or actions on the

part of firms. We show that base-ten threshold beating is driven both by a base-ten effect in

performance targets specified in formal and informal contracts and also by managers’ expected

Page 6: The Power of Numbers: Base-Ten Threshold Effects in Reported … · 2019. 1. 10. · 1 The Power of Numbers: Base-Ten Threshold Effects in Reported Revenue ABSTRACT We provide evidence

5

benefits of beating base-ten thresholds, in particular increased press coverage. We show that these

incentives lead to real actions taken by firms’ managers. Far from being merely an interesting

empirical regularity, the base-ten threshold effect appears to have real economic implications.

Our results are also important in understanding the role of goal-setting in motivation

(Locke and Latham, 2002). Our main results on revenue growth and persistence imply that, for

many firms, base-ten thresholds motivate employees throughout an organization to exert extra

effort that leads to a new level of operations that is persistent in future periods. In that sense, our

study presents evidence consistent with Allen et al. (2016) that base-ten numbers provide an

unusually powerful and salient benchmark for individuals to work towards and that this motivation

effect can be achieved at the organization level.

Although base-ten thresholds can arise in the context of a variety of financial metrics, there

are several key advantages to focusing on revenues. First, we expect the power of our tests to be

particularly strong for revenues compared to other financial metrics such as total assets, sales

growth, or ROA because reported revenues are an especially prominent performance target. We

present empirical evidence that not only are revenues extremely prevalent performance metrics

throughout our sample period, but that revenue targets are even more common than EPS targets in

the most recent years of our sample. This is consistent with prior evidence that shows that revenue

is a key target for financial performance management (i.e., real earnings management)

(Roychowdhury, 2006; Zang, 2012) and that it contains incremental information content beyond

earnings (Davis, 2002; Jegadeesh and Livnat, 2006; Swaminathan and Weintrop, 1991). Similarly,

the base-ten threshold effect is a number line phenomenon. In other words, base-ten thresholds

occur systematically throughout the number line meaning that the only way to reach another one

is to increase the metric of interest. Revenues, unlike earnings which net revenues and expenses,

Page 7: The Power of Numbers: Base-Ten Threshold Effects in Reported … · 2019. 1. 10. · 1 The Power of Numbers: Base-Ten Threshold Effects in Reported Revenue ABSTRACT We provide evidence

6

are generally monotonic because of natural growth processes and the presence of inflation.

Because of this monotonicity, much more emphasis is placed on growth in revenue and reaching

additional new benchmarks, and therefore we would expect base-ten thresholds to be especially

important in the context of revenue, again lending power to our tests.

There also methodological advantages to focusing on reported revenue. Revenue does not

suffer from the scaling problems to which aggregated measures of performance such as earnings

are subject and which cause difficulty in statistically identifying threshold-beating behavior

(Durtschi and Easton, 2005; 2009). Additionally, to the extent that we wish to identify accounting

manipulation, previous work has demonstrated that focusing on one accounting component

provides greater power than examining earnings as a whole (Stubben, 2010). In fact, our revenue

results help to extend the literature on manipulation by providing evidence on the magnitude of

the flexibility that firms have to manage reported revenue and, by extension, earnings, which can

be used in further studies to calibrate tests trying to detect revenue and earnings management.

Lastly, our revenue results as a whole provide evidence that the “one number” mentality

(Dichev et al., 2013) in which managers only care about bottom-line income for both internal

decision-making and external reporting may not be as dominant as once assumed. Indeed, our

results indicate that the level of reported revenue itself is an important reporting metric for

managers, and that the level of the reported revenue number is an object of strategic management.

II. Background and Hypothesis Development

Numbers that are factors of ten tend to be easier for humans to process. Schindler and

Wiman (1989) find that round numbers are easier to remember and come to mind more readily,

and Tversky and Kahneman (1973) show that when making decisions individuals give excess

weight to information that is easily retrieved from memory. Consistent with this, research has

Page 8: The Power of Numbers: Base-Ten Threshold Effects in Reported … · 2019. 1. 10. · 1 The Power of Numbers: Base-Ten Threshold Effects in Reported Revenue ABSTRACT We provide evidence

7

shown that individuals have a strong tendency to disproportionately produce round numbers when

required to generate random numbers (Huttenlocher et al., 1990; Turner, 1958; Whynes et al.,

2005) and that consumers perceive numbers and prices just below round numbers (e.g., $29.99) to

be disproportionately smaller than the round number above (e.g., $30.00) (Brenner and Brenner,

1982; Gabor and Granger, 1964).

At the same time, a large body of work in finance has documented that processing

constraints and psychological biases may affect financial decision making.1 If preparers and users

of financial information are susceptible to processing constraints associated with base-ten

numbers, then this may affect the generation and response to reported accounting numbers.

Consistent with this intuition, two studies in accounting demonstrate that firms tend to

disproportionately report earnings just above base-ten thresholds (Carslaw, 1988; Thomas, 1989).

Additionally, a vast literature provides evidence of earnings management employed to reach a

variety of other (non-base-ten) targets (see e.g., Abarbanell and Lehavy, 2003; Bartov et al., 2002;

Burgstahler and Dichev, 1997; Das and Zhang, 2003; Dichev and Skinner, 2002; Healy 1985). As

a necessary preliminary step to examine the main research questions of this paper, we first establish

that base-ten thresholds are a salient threshold for reported revenues. Formally, we predict:

H1: Firms are more likely to report revenue just above a base-ten threshold than just below.

While observing a base-ten threshold effect in reported revenue is consistent with a variety

of explanations tied to the salience of base-ten numbers in general, observing evidence consistent

with our first hypothesis does not help us to distinguish which of these possible explanations is

actually driving the observed results or what actions managers take in order to report revenues

1 Models in behavioral finance have examined overconfidence (Daniel et al., 1998; Kyle and Wang, 1997; Odean,

1998), limited attention (Hirshleifer and Teoh, 2003; Peng and Xiang, 2006), and cumulative prospect theory (Barberis

and Huang, 2008).

Page 9: The Power of Numbers: Base-Ten Threshold Effects in Reported … · 2019. 1. 10. · 1 The Power of Numbers: Base-Ten Threshold Effects in Reported Revenue ABSTRACT We provide evidence

8

above these thresholds. Indeed, while Carslaw (1988) and Thomas (1989) document evidence of

the existence of this effect in earnings, neither attempts to disentangle the economic forces driving

this regularity. Therefore, we explore several potential drivers of this phenomenon in more detail.

First, managers may engage in base-ten threshold beating behavior because they are

incentivized to do so by explicit formal or informal contracts. One formal contract, executive

compensation plans, has long been examined in the accounting literature. These contracts

incentivize managers to exert costly effort to attain predetermined performance targets. If board

members are subject to a base-ten threshold bias, then the revenue targets in compensation

contracts will be disproportionately set at or above base-ten thresholds. If this is the case, managers

will have an incentive to meet these base-ten revenue targets in order to earn bonus compensation,

leading to a corresponding base-ten threshold effect in actual reported revenue.

Similarly, management forecasts and analyst forecasts represent informal contracts with

market participants, setting a performance expectation for both managers and investors. Therefore,

managers have an incentive to report accounting numbers that meet or beat analyst and

management forecasts. However, both managers and analysts are subject to processing constraints

and may fixate to some extent on base-ten thresholds when generating their forecasts (Bamber et

al., 2010; Herrmann and Thomas, 2005). If this is the case, then the presence of a base-ten threshold

effect in analyst and management revenue forecasts may lead firms, which have an incentive to

meet or beat these biased thresholds, to report revenue which also exhibits this threshold effect.

Given the reasoning above, we make our first prediction about the drivers of the base-ten

threshold effect in revenue:

H2a: The presence of a base-ten threshold effect in formal and informal contract revenue

TARGETS increases the probability that firms will report revenue that is just above a

base-ten threshold.

Page 10: The Power of Numbers: Base-Ten Threshold Effects in Reported … · 2019. 1. 10. · 1 The Power of Numbers: Base-Ten Threshold Effects in Reported Revenue ABSTRACT We provide evidence

9

However, even in the absence of explicit targets from formal and informal contracts, firms

may still be more likely to report revenues just above base-ten thresholds if the managers or

employees of those firms are subject to a base-ten bias which leads them to fixate on base-ten

thresholds or if firm managers expect that a base-ten bias on the part of market participants will

lead to disproportionate benefits to firms whose revenues exceed base-ten thresholds. Evidence in

support of either of these conjectures would be especially important because it would document

target-beating behavior on the part of firms even in the absence of explicit targets, which has not

previously been documented in the accounting literature.2 As a result, our next prediction is:

H2b: Even in the absence of formal and informal contract revenue targets, firms are more

likely to report revenue that is just above a base-ten threshold than just below.

To further explore the incentives of firms to beat base-ten thresholds, we focus on one potential

expected benefit of beating a base-ten threshold: increased visibility in the form of press coverage.

As mentioned earlier, round numbers stand out more to individuals and are more likely to be

remembered (Schindler and Wiman, 1989). This additional attention can influence journalists

when they are choosing which firms to feature in their articles.3 In addition, research such as

Huston and Kamdar (1996) argues that consumers perceive 99-cent prices to be disproportionately

lower than whole dollar prices just one cent above. In our setting, this phenomenon could lead

journalists to view firms with revenues just above a base-ten threshold as disproportionately larger

than firms with revenues just below. Given the prior research documenting a link between firm

size and visibility (Miller, 2006), we would expect disproportionate increases in the perceived size

of a firm to lead to disproportionate increases in firm visibility. Last, anecdotally, mutual funds,

2 Our setting is different from targets at zero earnings and last year’s earnings because base-ten thresholds have no

economic meaning, whereas last year’s and zero earnings have implications for future performance by providing

information on earnings sustainability and whether a firm has met its breakeven point, respectively. 3 Appendix 1 contains five examples illustrating the emphasis base-ten thresholds are given in the press.

Page 11: The Power of Numbers: Base-Ten Threshold Effects in Reported … · 2019. 1. 10. · 1 The Power of Numbers: Base-Ten Threshold Effects in Reported Revenue ABSTRACT We provide evidence

10

exchanges, and investors use size cutoffs to determine whether they will consider following or

investing in a company.4 To the extent that heuristics lead these cutoffs to be set at base-ten

thresholds, we expect passing base-ten thresholds to lead to disproportionate increases in visibility,

either because of the journalist’s own cutoff, or because a firm has passed a cutoff for a relevant

exchange, index, fund, etc. We predict that these increases in visibility will manifest in the form

of increased press coverage when a firm beats a base-ten threshold (e.g., $50 million, or $200

million) for the first time.5 Because increased public visibility through press coverage can lead to

other benefits such as a greater ability to detect fraud (Miller, 2006), higher price efficiency (Fang

and Peress, 2009), and increased stock liquidity (Bushee et al., 2010), which have implications for

firm value (Diamond and Verrecchia, 1991), expected increases in press coverage provide a strong

incentive for managers to beat base-ten thresholds.

Formally, our last hypothesis relating to the drivers of the base-ten threshold effect in reported

revenue is as follows:

H2c: Firms that exceed a base-ten revenue threshold for the first time will experience

increases in press coverage.

Although there are many potential consequences to a firm of beating a base-ten threshold, we focus

on press coverage because we see this expected outcome as the most direct driver of firms’

4 For example, the minimum market capitalization requirements are $6.1 billion to be included in the S&P 500, $30

million to be included in the Russell 2000 index, and $40 million to be listed on the New York Stock Exchange. 5 We only expect increases in visibility the first time that a firm’s revenues exceed a particular base-ten threshold (e.g.,

$50 million). Intuitively, we would not expect the attention-grabbing nature of a firm’s level of revenue to continue

increasing if the firm is repeatedly beating the same base-ten threshold. That is, if a firm has exactly $50 million in

revenue every period, we do not expect that firm to continue experiencing increases in visibility every year when it

hits the same level of revenues. We would expect a firm’s visibility to increase when it hits a particular base-ten

threshold for the first time and then to stay at that level of visibility (all else equal) until it beats another base-ten

threshold (in this case, the next closest threshold is $60 million). Similarly, once a company has experienced a

discontinuous increase in its perceived size by beating a base-ten revenue threshold (say, $50 million), beating that

same revenue threshold again will not change the market’s perception of the firm’s size. Only when the firm beats

another larger base-ten threshold (for example $60 million or $80 million) will its perceived size experience another

discontinuous increase. Consistent with this reasoning, in our tests we focus only on periods when firms beat a new

base-ten revenue threshold for the first time.

Page 12: The Power of Numbers: Base-Ten Threshold Effects in Reported … · 2019. 1. 10. · 1 The Power of Numbers: Base-Ten Threshold Effects in Reported Revenue ABSTRACT We provide evidence

11

incentives and a likely antecedent to many other potential outcomes, in particular capital market

consequences. Further, as we will demonstrate in our empirical tests of this hypothesis, firms can

also try to directly affect this outcome through their use of press releases.

Lastly, while the second major research question in this paper relates to the mechanisms

by which firms are able to disproportionately report revenues above base-ten thresholds, we make

no formal hypotheses about the methods they may use to do so. We see this as an empirical

question and examine it in our last set of analyses.

III. Research Design and Sample

In order to rigorously document the presence, drivers, and outcomes of base-ten revenue

thresholds, we use a variety of samples and analyses, including regression and approximate

randomization tests. Below we describe the data and tests used in this paper. Further detail is

given in the discussion of each analysis and in the Appendix.

Approximate Randomization

We use approximate randomization techniques to document statistically significant

discontinuities around base-ten revenue thresholds. These techniques rely on the properties of

randomly-generated distributions as discussed in Noreen (1989) to detect significant deviations

from the null hypothesis of no threshold effect. Other papers in the accounting literature, including

Dichev and Skinner (2002), have used similar methods.

In order to determine whether there is an unexpectedly large number of firms with total

revenue just above base-ten thresholds, we calculate the ratio of the number of observations in the

interval (or “bin”) just above base-ten thresholds to the number of observations in the bin just

below and then compare this above/below ratio to the corresponding ratios for a set of randomly-

generated pseudo-thresholds. An above/below ratio around base-ten thresholds that is significantly

Page 13: The Power of Numbers: Base-Ten Threshold Effects in Reported … · 2019. 1. 10. · 1 The Power of Numbers: Base-Ten Threshold Effects in Reported Revenue ABSTRACT We provide evidence

12

larger than the ratios around other points along the number line is consistent with firms managing

their reported revenue to be just above these thresholds. We use a similar procedure to compare

the magnitude of the base-ten threshold effect across different subsamples and financial metrics

by comparing the ratio of observations above and below base-ten thresholds between the two

samples of interest. For further details see the Appendix.

We identify 53 potential thresholds that we expect to be salient to managers, investors, and

other market participants. The potential thresholds range from $100,000 to $80 billion.6 As shown

in Table A.1, each order of magnitude (105 through 109) includes nine thresholds such as $100,000,

$200,000, $300,000, up through $900,000; the final order of magnitude, 1010, includes 8 potential

thresholds up to $80 billion. We define the width of each “bin” around potential thresholds in

percentage terms by calculating the percentage increase in revenues necessary to move from the

bottom to the top of the bin. Initially, our smallest bin width is 0.25% and our largest bin width is

20%, although subsequent to Table 3 the largest width that we examine is 2.5%.

In their work on base-ten thresholds in net income, Carslaw (1988) and Thomas (1989) use

a methodology of looking at deviations of second digits from their expected frequencies based on

Benford’s Law.7 The benefit of our approximate randomization technique, which controls for the

empirical distribution of reported numbers, is that we are not required to make any assumptions

about the statistical properties of the reported revenue distribution. Also, our technique allows us

to examine the magnitude of the flexibility firms have to reach a revenue threshold because we

can precisely vary the width of the interval (bin) above and below the thresholds that we examine.

The second-digits methodology employed by Carslaw and Thomas is constrained to examining

6 We do not examine thresholds outside this range because very few firms covered by Compustat report revenue values

larger or smaller than this. 7 Benford’s Law is an empirical regularity whereby certain digits are more likely to occur in the leading positions of

numbers in naturally-generated data (Newcomb, 1881).

Page 14: The Power of Numbers: Base-Ten Threshold Effects in Reported … · 2019. 1. 10. · 1 The Power of Numbers: Base-Ten Threshold Effects in Reported Revenue ABSTRACT We provide evidence

13

non-adjustable bin widths of varying relative sizes.8 Finally, and critically, our approach allows us

to statistically compare the threshold effect across two sets of firms or reported numbers.

Revenue Threshold and Growth Test Samples

We focus on U.S. firms and use all Compustat firm-years over the period 1950 through

2017 for which both total revenue and total assets are non-missing and positive; this yields 367,233

firm-years which we use in our basic approximate randomization tests.9 Our tests focus on revenue

observations just below and just above base-ten thresholds, but we use all of the available data to

generate the approximate randomization test statistics based on the empirical distribution of

revenue observations just below and just above randomly-chosen revenue thresholds.10 The

number of firm-years, by decade and by data source, is given in Table 1 Panel A.

Regression Analyses Sample

For the regression analyses in the paper, we use a sample of firm-years with accounting

data from Compustat, market data from CRSP, analyst forecast data from I/B/E/S, and press

coverage data from RavenPack over the fiscal years 1999 to 2014.11 All variable definitions are

given in the Variable Appendix. Table 1 Panel B presents descriptive statistics for the variables

used in the regression analyses in Tables 7 and 8. All variables are winsorized at the 1% level.

[Insert Table 1 Here]

8 For example, the relative size of any revenue management activity to move a firm from $990,000 in revenue to

$1,000,000 in revenue is much smaller (1.01%) than to move a firm from $1,900,000 in revenue to $2,000,000 in

revenue (5.26%), and yet both these two cases are treated the same with the second-digits methodology. 9 Although our tests include data from as early as the 1950s, this base-ten revenue threshold phenomenon persists

today and can be observed in the data even when restricting the sample to include only those firm-years occurring

after the year 2000. We use the widest sample possible to maximize power, particularly in our subsample tests. 10 In any given test using a subsample of these data, we discard the bottom and top 1% of revenue observations. 11 Because our press coverage variables are measured over the subsequent year, our regression sample must end in

2014 because our last year of RavenPack data is 2015. Additionally, although we use IBES data up until the beginning

of 2018 in our approximate randomization tests, we restrict the sample period to be the same for all variables used in

the regression analyses. Results are similar if we impose no restrictions on the sample periods used in our regression

analyses or if we restrict the approximate randomization tests to end in 2014.

Page 15: The Power of Numbers: Base-Ten Threshold Effects in Reported … · 2019. 1. 10. · 1 The Power of Numbers: Base-Ten Threshold Effects in Reported Revenue ABSTRACT We provide evidence

14

Other Datasets

In further analyses, we use several databases to document the existence of base-ten

revenue thresholds in a variety of contexts. We use executive compensation plan revenue targets

from 1998 to 2016 obtained from Incentive Lab, management revenue forecasts from 2003 to

2018 obtained from IBES, and analyst revenue forecasts from 2000 to 2018 also from IBES.

These data are aggregated and used at the firm-year level in Tables 2 and 6 and used in their

disaggregated form in Table 5 (so we can observe the distribution of multiple targets issued in

the same firm-year). See the relevant tables for additional information, including sample sizes.

IV. Empirical Results

Importance of Revenue Targets

Earlier we offered several arguments for why threshold-beating behavior is particularly

important with respect to revenues. In Table 2, we provide empirical support for these arguments

by documenting the prevalence of revenue in three performance metrics – executive compensation

plans, management forecasts, and analyst forecasts. Panel A reports the five most common

accounting metrics on which executive compensation plans are contracted. During our sample

period, 29% of all firm-years had at least one compensation contract that included a revenue target.

Revenue was present in executive compensation contracts second only to EPS (30.1%).

Panel B reports the five most commonly-forecasted accounting numbers in management

forecasts. During our sample period, 52.0% of firm-years had at least one management revenue

forecast. Again, this proportion was second only to EPS (53.3%). Finally, Panel C reports the five

most commonly-forecasted accounting numbers present in analyst forecasts. In our sample, 86.4%

of firm-years had at least one analyst revenue forecast and 88.7% had at least one EPS forecast.12

12 Inferences are unchanged when we restrict the sample periods to be consistent across the performance metrics.

Page 16: The Power of Numbers: Base-Ten Threshold Effects in Reported … · 2019. 1. 10. · 1 The Power of Numbers: Base-Ten Threshold Effects in Reported Revenue ABSTRACT We provide evidence

15

Figures 1A, 1B, and 1C, display the frequency of revenue and EPS as performance metrics over

time. Interestingly, we observe that revenue has become more prevalent in all three samples over

time, and it appears more often than EPS in recent years for all three samples.

[Insert Table 2 and Figures 1A, 1B, and 1C here]

Overall, the descriptive statistics presented in Table 2 and Figures 1A, 1B, and 1C

demonstrate the great prevalence of revenue as a benchmark and speak to its importance in

performance evaluation. This combined with the monotonically increasing nature of revenue

strongly suggests that revenue is an ideally powerful setting to examine base-ten thresholds.

Basic Threshold Effect

In Table 3, we test our first hypothesis and provide evidence that firms are significantly

more likely to report revenue numbers just above than just below base-ten thresholds. In Panel A

we report values of the ratio of the number of revenue observations in the bin just above a threshold

point to the number of observations in the bin just below that same threshold point for bin widths

ranging from 0.25% to 20%. The number of ABOVE observations exceeds the number of BELOW

observations for all bin widths, and the approximate randomization p-values are statistically

significant at the 1% level for bin widths from 0.25% of revenue through 10% of revenue. These

results have an extremely low probability of occurring by random chance and support our

hypothesis that managers manage reported revenue to reach these base-ten benchmarks.13,14

13 Prior studies examining base-ten thresholds for earnings identified these irregularities by examining the distribution

of the digits in reported numbers. A digits-based methodology must take into account Benford’s Law, which

demonstrates that digits in naturally-occurring numbers are not uniformly distributed and numbers with a second digit

of 0 (e.g., 500) are more likely to occur than numbers with a second digit of 9 (e.g., 69,000). However, Benford’s Law

does not explain our evidence of discontinuities around base-ten thresholds because Benford’s Law explains the

distribution of digits in naturally occurring data and not the distribution of observations at adjacent points on the

number line (i.e., Benford’s Law does not imply any sort of discontinuity in the distribution of numbers) (Newcomb,

1881; Benford, 1938). Similarly, Amiram et al. (2015) uses deviations in the distributions of digits from Benford’s

Law to identify earnings management, but our evidence of discontinuities cannot be inferred from their results. 14 We performed several alternative untabulated versions of this analysis. First, we restricted our sample to firms with

revenues greater than $100 million and still find a significant effect, ruling out the possibility that our results are driven

Page 17: The Power of Numbers: Base-Ten Threshold Effects in Reported … · 2019. 1. 10. · 1 The Power of Numbers: Base-Ten Threshold Effects in Reported Revenue ABSTRACT We provide evidence

16

[Insert Table 3 here]

One issue to consider with respect to the test statistics in Panel A is that the observation

counts in the ABOVE and BELOW bins for the different bin widths are cumulative, meaning that

if an observation is in the ABOVE bin for the 0.25% bin width, that same observation is also in

the ABOVE bin for the 20% bin width. Part, or all, of the significant difference in the

ABOVE/BELOW number of observations for the larger bin widths could therefore be the result

of the differences for the smaller bin widths. This is analogous to a significant five-day abnormal

market return being the result of four days of zero abnormal returns and one day with a large

abnormal return. Intuitively, it seems unrealistic that firms would be able to increase reported

revenue by 10% or 15% (two of the significant bin widths) in order to reach a base-ten threshold.

To explore this issue, in Table 3 Panel B we tabulate observations in incremental revenue

bands. For example, we observe in Panel A that there are 8,220 observations in the ABOVE bins

within 0.5% of a base-ten threshold. However, only 3,606 of these observations are in the band

close to the upper limit of 0.5% in the band between 0.25% and 0.5%. We use these incremental

observation counts (i.e., observations in the current bin width that were not included in the next

smallest width) to generate new ratios and associated approximate randomization p-values. The

incremental p-values of three of the four smallest bins are significant at the 1% level.15 These

incremental results suggest that firms are desirous and able to manage reported revenue within

by the smallest thresholds such as $1 million. Additionally, while our main tests use all 53 base-ten thresholds listed

in Table A.1, we examined only the 6 thresholds of the form 1 x 10k (i.e., $10 million, $1 billion, etc.) and find that

the magnitude of the threshold effect (the above/below ratio) is larger than for the full set of thresholds, although the

statistical significance is slightly smaller (undoubtedly as a result of a loss in power from examining fewer thresholds

and including significant thresholds in the “control” group). We also examined the remaining 47 thresholds and still

find a strongly significant threshold effect. These tests support our use of all 53 thresholds, and we include the full set

in all tests in this paper. 15 Note that the incremental p-value in Panel B for 0.25% is the same as in Panel A because it is the smallest bin width

and therefore incremental to a bin width of 0. Also, there are some minor discrepancies between the cumulative

number of observations in Panels A and B because we truncate the distribution of bins at the 1% level for each analysis.

Page 18: The Power of Numbers: Base-Ten Threshold Effects in Reported … · 2019. 1. 10. · 1 The Power of Numbers: Base-Ten Threshold Effects in Reported Revenue ABSTRACT We provide evidence

17

about a 2.5% range to reach base-ten threshold points. The statistical significance for wider bins

results from the holdover effect from the ABOVE/BELOW difference for the narrower bins. We

use this finding to narrow the set of bins used in subsequent tests and from now on report results

only for the four smallest bin widths. We point out that these results are interesting in their own

right because they allow us to quantify the degree of flexibility managers have to manage reported

revenue and, by extension, reported earnings. This information can allow future researchers to

more accurately calibrate tests of revenue or earnings management.16

To further validate that these results are driven by intentional actions to beat base-ten

revenue thresholds, we conducted additional analyses described in Appendix 2. Specifically, to

rule out the possibility that our results are driven by rounding of accounting numbers introduced

by the data-provider or by managers subconsciously rounding up all reported accounting numbers,

we use approximate randomization to compare the magnitude of the base-ten threshold effect of

revenue with that of cost of goods sold, total assets, and market value of equity. These other metrics

are of a similar magnitude as revenue and serve as reasonable benchmarks. We show that the base-

16 Although we focus on these four percentage bin widths, we recognize that there are a variety of ways to define and

choose bin widths, each with its own set of advantages and disadvantages. While one benefit of bin widths defined in

percentage terms is that they increase in size with the magnitude of revenues to take into account greater dollar

amounts of flexibility, this results in overlapping bins for some adjacent thresholds when the bin width is large (for

example, the 20% bin above $500 million completely overlaps with the 20% bin below $600 million). This can be

seen in the last two rows of Table 3 Panel A where the sum of observations in the top and bottom bins exceeds the

total number of observations in our sample because some observations fall in the top bin for one threshold and the

bottom bin for another threshold. However, this does not bias our p-values because it also occurs in the random

“pseudo” thresholds used in the randomization procedure. Further, it only occurs when bin widths are wide (7.5% and

greater) where firms are unlikely to feasibly be managing their revenues in the overlapping regions. However, to allay

any concerns about inferences drawn from our percentage bin width tests, we ran our approximate randomization tests

using absolute dollar bin widths (i.e., $10,000, $50,000) and flexible bin widths defined as the minimum percentage

bin width around each potential threshold required to capture the nearest 30, 100, and 200 observations. All methods

of defining bin widths strongly indicate the presence of a base-ten threshold effect in revenue. We use the percentage

bin width method because we think it is the most intuitive and theoretically sound.

Page 19: The Power of Numbers: Base-Ten Threshold Effects in Reported … · 2019. 1. 10. · 1 The Power of Numbers: Base-Ten Threshold Effects in Reported Revenue ABSTRACT We provide evidence

18

ten effect in revenue is significantly stronger than in any of these other metrics, further supporting

our prediction that managers manage revenue to exceed base-ten thresholds.17

In untabulated results, we also extend our revenue analysis to an international setting using

data from the Osiris database and show that there is a significant base-ten threshold effect in the

revenue of firms reporting in a variety of countries in a variety of currencies, further ruling out the

possibility that we are capturing a rounding-related artifact specific to Compustat.

To provide additional evidence supporting our interpretation of Table 3, we next examine

systematic cross-sectional variation in the strength of the base-ten threshold effect in reported

revenue. If the base-ten effect in revenue is the result of intentional actions by firms, as opposed

to the data-generating process or innocuous rounding, then we would expect that the base-ten

threshold effect would be stronger for firms which have high incentives to beat revenue thresholds.

In particular, we expect that firms with high market expectations of growth will be particularly

sensitive to revenue news and will exhibit a stronger base-ten revenue threshold effect. In Table 4

Panels A and B, we report results examining the magnitude of the base-ten threshold effect for

firms with high versus low lagged revenue growth and price-to-sales ratios, respectively.18

[Insert Table 4 here]

Table 4 Panel A reports the results of approximate randomization tests comparing

magnitude of the base-ten revenue threshold effect of high past revenue growth firm-years (the

highest revenue growth decile) with low past revenue growth (the lowest revenue growth decile)

17 These results also serve the additional purpose of further justifying our decision to examine revenues as the focus

of our study of base-ten thresholds, as opposed to one of these alternative metrics. While the preference for base-ten

threshold beating is not unique to revenues, we focus on this metric because we predict (and find) that this phenomenon

is particularly important in this setting compared to several other common metrics. 18 We define revenue growth as the percentage change in revenue from year t-1 to t (where t is the current year) and

the price-to-sales ratio as a firm’s total market capitalization divided by total revenue. Because revenue growth is

correlated with current revenue, we use lagged values to sort firms into revenue growth deciles. Before constructing

deciles, we exclude the extreme high/low 2% of past revenue growth (price-to-sales ratio) firm-years for Panel A (B)

to avoid our results being skewed by extreme growth firms.

Page 20: The Power of Numbers: Base-Ten Threshold Effects in Reported … · 2019. 1. 10. · 1 The Power of Numbers: Base-Ten Threshold Effects in Reported Revenue ABSTRACT We provide evidence

19

firms. These results indicate that past revenue growth appears to have a significant effect on the

tendency of a firm to reach a base-ten threshold. For three of the four bin widths, the ratio of firms

with revenues just above relative to just below a base-ten threshold is significantly greater for high-

growth firms relative to low growth firms. Similarly, in Panel B we find that the base-ten threshold

effect in revenues is stronger for high (top decile) relative to low (bottom decile) price-to-sales

ratio firms for all four bin widths. These results are consistent with the idea that managers in firms

where revenues are subject to more attention are more motivated to seek the positive attention

stemming from reaching base-ten reference points. Interestingly, in untabulated results we find no

significant difference in the magnitude of the base-ten revenue threshold effect between groups of

firms with high and low price-to-earnings ratios, suggesting that incentives tied to revenue

performance are not necessarily the same as those tied to earnings performance.

In summary, Tables 3 and 4 provide support for our prediction that firms are more likely

to report revenue that is just above a base-ten threshold than just below, and this is not an artefact

of the data-gathering process or rounding.

Formal and Informal Contracts

Table 5 studies the base-ten threshold effect in both formal and informal revenue contracts.

In Panel A we investigate formal executive compensation contracts and document that revenue

targets in these contracts are significantly more likely to be set at or just above a base-ten threshold

than just below. This effect is most pronounced within the smallest bin (0.25%) around a base-ten

threshold where contracts are more than 11 times more likely to have a target at or just above a

threshold than just below the threshold. Even in wider bins (0.50% and 1.00%), contracts are 4.9

and 2.6 times more likely to have a revenue target at or just above a threshold than just below. In

other words, the numeric revenue targets themselves which are specified in executive

Page 21: The Power of Numbers: Base-Ten Threshold Effects in Reported … · 2019. 1. 10. · 1 The Power of Numbers: Base-Ten Threshold Effects in Reported Revenue ABSTRACT We provide evidence

20

compensation plans exhibit a discontinuity around base-ten thresholds. To give a more intuitive

sense of this behavior on the part of boards setting targets for managers, in our sample we find that

1,062 out of 20,446 revenue targets are set exactly at a base-ten threshold (i.e., exactly $200

million, exactly $50 million, etc.), which is strongly consistent with the targets that firms face

being a driver for a base-ten threshold effect in actual revenues.

[Insert Table 5 here]

In Panels B and C, we examine the threshold effect in management and analyst revenue

forecasts. Once issued by managers and analysts, these informal benchmarks become relevant

performance targets. Similar to the results in Panel A, managers are more likely to forecast

revenues at or just above a threshold than just below a threshold. This effect is strongest in the

smallest bin around a particular threshold, with management forecasts in the 0.25% bin-width

being 27.3 times more likely to be at or above a threshold than below, and even in a bin-width of

2.50%, managers are still 2.2 times more likely to forecast revenues at or just above a threshold

than just below. In Panel C, we examine the threshold effect in analyst revenue forecasts and find

that analysts are significantly more likely to issue a revenue forecast at or above a base-ten

threshold. Taken together, these results are consistent with a preference for setting both formal and

informal contracts at or just above base-ten thresholds.19

Explicit Targets and the Revenue Threshold Effect

Although Table 5 documents the presence of a base-ten threshold effect in the revenue

targets specified in formal and informal contracts, simply demonstrating the presence of this effect

19 It is possible that analyst revenue forecasts exhibit a base-ten threshold effect because analysts rationally expect that

managers will try to beat base-ten thresholds. However, in untabulated analyses we find that the magnitude of the

threshold effect in analyst revenue forecasts is significantly stronger for the first forecast an analyst makes for a given

period relative to the last forecast, consistent with analysts relying more on base-ten heuristics earlier in the period

when they have less information and less later in the period when their information is more precise (Herrmann and

Thomas, 2005). This systematic time-series variation in the base-ten threshold effect is inconsistent with it being solely

driven by analysts’ attempt to forecast managers’ base-ten beating behavior.

Page 22: The Power of Numbers: Base-Ten Threshold Effects in Reported … · 2019. 1. 10. · 1 The Power of Numbers: Base-Ten Threshold Effects in Reported Revenue ABSTRACT We provide evidence

21

in these targets does not necessarily indicate that they are driving the observed threshold effect in

actual revenue. In order to more convincingly draw this causal link, in Table 6 we investigate the

effect that beating explicit revenue targets has on the base-ten threshold effect of actual revenue,

and we also examine base-ten threshold beating behavior in the absence of explicit targets.

In Table 6 Panel A, we partition our sample into two groups by whether firms either did or

did not beat all of their explicit revenue targets. “Beaters” are firms which either met or beat all of

the revenue targets defined by their executive compensations plans, management revenue

forecasts, analyst revenue forecasts, and last year’s revenue. Although both revenue target beaters

and non-beaters are more likely to report revenue just above rather than just below base-ten

thresholds, the effect is significantly stronger for beaters, consistent with beating a base-ten

revenue threshold being a byproduct of beating other revenue targets.

It is possible that the results in Panel A are driven by the fact that firms which beat all of

their explicit revenue targets are just good at beating targets in general (including base-ten revenue

targets). To rule out this possibility, in Panel B of Table 6, we compare the revenue threshold effect

of firms which beat and fail to beat their earnings targets. If the results in Panel A are driven solely

by whether firms are good at beating targets, instead of showing a causal link between revenue

targets and the base-ten revenue threshold effect, then we would expect to find that firms which

beat their earnings targets have a stronger base-ten revenue threshold effect. In actuality, our one-

tailed test finds no significant evidence that earnings target beaters have a stronger revenue

threshold effect. Therefore, the strength of the base-ten revenue threshold effect is specifically tied

to beating base-ten denominated revenue targets, consistent with our prediction in H2a.

Interestingly, the results in Panel B suggest that the base-ten revenue threshold effect is actually

stronger for firms which fail to beat their earnings targets (although the one-tailed nature of our

Page 23: The Power of Numbers: Base-Ten Threshold Effects in Reported … · 2019. 1. 10. · 1 The Power of Numbers: Base-Ten Threshold Effects in Reported Revenue ABSTRACT We provide evidence

22

statistical tests does not allow us to document this rigorously). This suggests managers may place

more emphasis on beating revenue targets when they miss their earnings targets.

Lastly, in Table 6 Panel C we investigate our prediction in H2b that the revenue threshold

effect still exists for firms which have no explicit revenue targets available.20 We find a significant

base-ten threshold effect in revenue for the two smallest bin widths for firms in this sub-sample,

indicating that firms still exert effort to report revenues above base-ten thresholds even when doing

so will not help them beat an explicit target.21 We view this result as one of the most interesting

and novel of the paper. Although many previous accounting studies have identified actions to beat

external performance targets set in forecasts and formal contracts, we are the first to identify the

presence of threshold-beating behavior in the absence of explicit predetermined targets. Given the

great prevalence of base-ten thresholds (about one third of firm-years in our sample are beating a

new base-ten revenue threshold for the first time) and the potential for the existence of other

implicit targets (for example, the maximum revenue ever reached by other firms in the same

industry), we find these initial results to be both exciting and important.

Visibility

As predicted in H2c, one potential benefit of reaching a base-ten revenue threshold is that

it attracts the attention of the press. In Table 7 we present results for our analysis of press coverage

for firms crossing a threshold for the first time which show that threshold-beaters see an increase

in news coverage over the year following a base-ten beating earnings announcement. Specifically,

the coefficient on Threshold is positive and significant in Columns 1 and 2 which include industry

20 In order to alleviate concerns that firms are missing explicit revenue targets simply because of a lack of database

coverage, we restrict the sample period in this test to 2004-2016 when we have substantial coverage for our executive

compensation plan revenue target data and management and analyst revenue forecast data. 21 One concern with Panel C is that the firms we examine may have explicit targets not captured in our data. To allay

this concern, we examine the set of firms which have explicit revenue targets but fail to beat all of them (i.e., the “non-

beaters” from Panel A). These firms have a significant base-ten threshold effect; in other words, firms have incentives

to beat base-ten revenue thresholds even when doing so does not help them beat their explicit targets.

Page 24: The Power of Numbers: Base-Ten Threshold Effects in Reported … · 2019. 1. 10. · 1 The Power of Numbers: Base-Ten Threshold Effects in Reported Revenue ABSTRACT We provide evidence

23

and year and firm and year fixed effects, respectively. The magnitude of this increase appears to

be large, with firms experiencing an increase in press coverage of roughly 10 articles compared to

an average (median) total of 226 (151).

In addition to controlling for a variety of firm fundamentals (untabulated for parsimony),

these analyses control for whether the firm met or beat its revenue and earnings forecasts for the

current year in order to ensure that our results are not picking up increases in press coverage for

firms which meet or beat expectations in general. Nor are our results explained by the amount of

a firm’s revenue growth (Revenue Growth) in a given year or the magnitude of revenue alone

(ln(Revenue)). Lastly, our results cannot be explained by the fact that any time a firm reaches a

previously unattained level of revenue it may attract additional attention (for example by being

able to tout “record” sales) because we include an indicator for whether current revenues are

greater than those in any prior period (First_Ever). In short, our results appear to show a substantial

increase in media coverage for firms which beat a base-ten threshold for the first time. Given the

potential economic benefits of increased press coverage (Bushee et al., 2010; Drake et al., 2014;

Fang and Peress, 2009), we view this as evidence of another incentive for firms to beat base-ten

thresholds to the extent that firms rationally anticipate these increases in press coverage.

[Insert Table 7 here]

In addition to our main results presented in Columns 1 and 2, Columns 3 and 4 further

break up press coverage into firm-initiated and non-firm-initiated (or external) press coverage.22

To the extent that firms anticipate increases in visibility after beating a base-ten threshold for the

first time, we would expect that they would issue additional press releases in order to highlight

their threshold-beating revenues and potentially drum up further interest in the business press.

22 Results for Columns 3 and 4 using industry and year fixed effects are consistent but untabulated for parsimony.

Page 25: The Power of Numbers: Base-Ten Threshold Effects in Reported … · 2019. 1. 10. · 1 The Power of Numbers: Base-Ten Threshold Effects in Reported Revenue ABSTRACT We provide evidence

24

Consistent with this, in Column 3 we find a small but significant increase in firm-initiated press

coverage (i.e., press releases) following the announcement of base-ten threshold beating revenues

equivalent to roughly one additional press release, compared to an average (median) of 43 (33)

over the total period. Further, Column 4 shows that total press coverage after excluding firm-

initiated press releases (or external coverage) increases following a base-ten beating

announcement. In short, not only are firms more likely to draw attention to their performance

through the use of an additional press release after they beat a base-ten revenue threshold, but

journalists take notice and provide additional coverage of these firms.23 Overall, the results of this

table provide evidence consistent with H2c and suggest that expected increases in press coverage

provide another incentive for firms to beat base-ten revenue thresholds.24

IV. Actions Taken to Reach Base-Ten Thresholds

We now turn to our second, more exploratory, research question: what actions do firms

take to beat base-ten revenue thresholds? In order to answer this question, we document the

behavior of firms in the period surrounding the first time they reach a particular base-ten threshold.

We investigate the sustainability of revenue levels achieved in the run-up to beating a base-ten

threshold and whether firms engage in increased effort or real or accrual management practices.

We envision several possible scenarios. First, the salience of base-ten thresholds may serve

as a particularly effective motivation device for managers to use in encouraging employees

throughout the organization to exert more effort and push the organization to a higher level of

revenues than would otherwise have occurred (Allen et al, 2016). In this case, we would expect to

23 Inferences were the same when excluding firms with revenue less than $100 million or examining press coverage

over the 30 days following the earnings announcement (although the magnitude of coefficients was smaller). 24 Similar to the robustness test of the analysis in Table 3 (discussed in Footnote 14), we ran all of our tests of revenue

management and press coverage for “major” and “minor” thresholds separately and find consistent results for both

subsets, although the “major” threshold tests tend to have more marginally significant results, again probably because

of a lack of power and because significant minor thresholds are now included in the control group.

Page 26: The Power of Numbers: Base-Ten Threshold Effects in Reported … · 2019. 1. 10. · 1 The Power of Numbers: Base-Ten Threshold Effects in Reported Revenue ABSTRACT We provide evidence

25

see an abnormal jump in the level and growth of revenues leading up to a base-ten threshold that

is not driven by discretionary revenues, with a subsequent return of revenue growth to its previous

level (because indefinite abnormal growth is not sustainable for any organization) and no

subsequent reversal in the level of revenue. Alternatively, firms may be tempted to engage in

unsustainable business practices (such as channel stuffing) or revenue accruals management in

order to obtain the visibility benefits of base-ten revenue thresholds described above. An extensive

literature in accounting documents managers achieving earnings and revenue targets through

“real” and “accruals-based” earnings management (see e.g., Jones, 1991; Roychowdhury, 2006;

Stubben, 2010; Zang, 2012). If this is the case, we would expect to see positive discretionary

revenues in the year a base-ten threshold is first beaten, accompanied by an abnormally large jump

in the level and growth of revenues leading up to a base-ten threshold, with a subsequent reversion

in revenue growth and level. Lastly, it is possible that firms experience no abnormal changes in

the level or growth of revenues or discretionary revenues leading up to a base-ten threshold if firms

simply meet each new base-ten threshold as part of their natural growth process.

In order to explore these three alternative scenarios, we present the results of discretionary

revenue tests in Table 8 and plot the growth and level of quarterly revenues in Figure 2. As in

Table 7, for these analyses we focus on the first year that a firm beats a particular base-ten revenue

threshold in order to study firm behavior in the year when the benefits of beating a threshold are

expected to be the highest, thus increasing the power of our tests.

In Table 8 we rely on the Stubben (2010) measure of discretionary revenue with some

modifications. The Stubben measure is defined as the residual from a model regressing the change

in receivables on the change in revenues in quarters 1 through 3 and in quarter 4 in order to capture

deviations from the normal relation between revenues and collections. Stubben (2010) provides

Page 27: The Power of Numbers: Base-Ten Threshold Effects in Reported … · 2019. 1. 10. · 1 The Power of Numbers: Base-Ten Threshold Effects in Reported Revenue ABSTRACT We provide evidence

26

evidence that this model does a particularly good job of identifying revenue management relative

to other accruals models and argues that focusing on the relation between revenues and receivables

increases the power of the measure. More recently, Collins et al. (2017) and McNichols and

Stubben (2018) have pointed out the importance of accounting for growth in accruals models

because firms experiencing or anticipating high growth tend to increase accruals as they shift up

the scale of their operations. We follow the recommendation of Collins et al. (2017) and expand

the Stubben (2010) specification by including non-linear controls for past and expected growth

(market-to-book ratio) as well as current performance.25 This modification is especially important

in our setting because we expect base-ten threshold beating firms to experience abnormal growth.

Our main results, presented in Columns 1 and 2 of Table 8 provide mixed evidence on the

existence of discretionary accruals for base-ten revenue threshold beating firms. In Column 1 in

the presence of industry and year fixed effects, as well as a variety of control variables used in

Table 7, we find significantly positive discretionary revenues for first-time base-ten revenue

threshold beaters. However, Column 2 which includes firm and year fixed effects finds no

significant effect after controlling for time-invariant firm characteristics.

In order to try to further clarify and interpret the results in Table 8, in Figure 2 we present

results of tests comparing the seasonally adjusted quarterly growth26 (Figure 2A) and logged level

(Figure 2B) of revenues for firms which beat a base-ten revenue threshold for the first time with a

set of control firm-years. Treatment (first-time threshold beaters) and control firm-years are

selected from the set of firm-years used in our initial test in Table 3 and must have 8 consecutive

quarters of revenue growth (level) data starting 4 quarters before the threshold-beating fourth

quarter (t-4), as well as total asset data in t-4. Firm-years are included in the control sample if they

25 See the Variable Appendix for further details. 26 That is (Revenuet – Revenuet-4)/Revenuet-4, where t is fiscal quarter.

Page 28: The Power of Numbers: Base-Ten Threshold Effects in Reported … · 2019. 1. 10. · 1 The Power of Numbers: Base-Ten Threshold Effects in Reported Revenue ABSTRACT We provide evidence

27

are not current threshold beaters (Threshold=0), are in the same 3-digit SIC code and fiscal year

as a treatment firm, and have quarter t-4 revenue growth (level) within 10% of the treated firm.

We further restrict control firm-years to those with a value of First_Ever equal to one in order to

control for normal mean reversion. We discard treatment firms without at least one matched

control firm. This leaves us with 15,214 (12,035) treatment and 19,819 (9,818) control firms in the

revenue growth (level) tests. In addition, we use entropy balancing to ensure that the control

sample is similar to the treatment sample in the pre-period by weighting control firm observations

in the revenue growth (level) sample such that revenue growth (level) and total assets in t-4 are

insignificantly different between the control and treatment samples.27 The effects of these

procedures can be seen in time t-4 in Figures 2A and 2B where treatment and control firms begin

at nearly identical mean levels of revenue growth and total revenue.

If firms engage in unsustainable activities in order to beat a base-ten revenue threshold,

then we expect both revenue level and growth to revert in subsequent years. If firms engage in

sustainable activities to beat these thresholds, we expect a temporary increase in revenue growth

but a permanent increase in revenue level. If firms engage in no strategic actions to beat base-ten

thresholds, we expect no difference between control and treatment firms.

Figure 2A reports revenue growth for first-time threshold-beating and control firms for

four quarters before and after a base-ten threshold in annual revenues was first reached. We see

that revenue growth for threshold-beaters is higher leading up to the threshold-beating quarter

(difference of 0.39 with a t-statistic of 42.5 in time t) and then tapers off over the subsequent

quarters and becomes almost indistinguishable from the growth of control firms in economic

magnitude by quarter t+4 (difference of 0.05 and t-statistic of 8.5).

27 Entropy balancing causes the treatment and control firms to have insignificantly different first, second, and third

moments for both the revenue growth (level) and total asset distributions. See Hainmueller (2012) for more details.

Page 29: The Power of Numbers: Base-Ten Threshold Effects in Reported … · 2019. 1. 10. · 1 The Power of Numbers: Base-Ten Threshold Effects in Reported Revenue ABSTRACT We provide evidence

28

In Figure 2B we document the logged level of revenues for first-time threshold-beating

firms and control firms. Consistent with Figure 2A, we see that the logged level of revenues

diverges between threshold-beaters and control firms in the quarters leading up to time t (difference

of 0.20 and t-statistic of 8.4 in time t), with no subsequent reversal on the part of threshold-beating

firms (difference of 0.23 and t-statistic of 9.3 in t+4).28

Combined, the results in Table 8 Columns 1 and 2 and Figures 2A and 2B indicate that

firms increase their revenues in the quarters leading up to a base-ten threshold, and on average the

increased revenues appear to be sustainable. The tests fail to provide evidence that these firms

engage in revenue management on average and are instead consistent with firms experiencing a

“burst” in growth as a result of additional effort on the part of employees. In untabulated results,

we support this interpretation by documenting that, on average, threshold firms’ revenues are not

accompanied by subsequent increases in the proportion of doubtful accounts, higher levels of days

in accounts receivables or days in inventory, or decreases in gross margin which we would expect

if firms engaged in activities such as channel stuffing or decreasing credit requirements.

While the average threshold beater appears to beat base-ten revenue thresholds through

sustainable actions, there are likely subsets of firms that engage in opportunistic behavior.

Specifically, firms which have particularly strong incentives to beat a base-ten revenue threshold

may be more likely to engage in real or accruals-based revenue management. In order to explore

this possibility, in Columns 3 through 6 of Table 8 we examine the discretionary revenues of

several subsets of firms by interacting the Threshold indicator with different Split variables. In

these specifications we include firm and year fixed effects, as in Column 2 where we find no

significant main effect, although results are consistent when using industry and year fixed effects.

28 These results are not driven solely by the smallest thresholds; we find similar patterns when we focus on firms with

revenues greater than $100 million.

Page 30: The Power of Numbers: Base-Ten Threshold Effects in Reported … · 2019. 1. 10. · 1 The Power of Numbers: Base-Ten Threshold Effects in Reported Revenue ABSTRACT We provide evidence

29

First, we interact Threshold with High_Growth, an indicator variable which identifies firms

with high growth expectations, defined the same as in Table 4 Panel B as firms with lagged annual

revenue growth in the top decile.29 Table 4 showed that high growth expectation firms have a

particularly strong base-ten threshold effect, and it is possible that these firms may face

disproportionately negative consequences if they fail to beat a base-ten threshold in a year when

they are expected to do so. These increased incentives may lead firms to engage in more aggressive

techniques in order to ensure that their revenues exceed a base-ten threshold. Consistent with this,

Column 3 shows that the interaction between Threshold and High_Growth is significantly positive.

Thus while threshold-beating firms on average do not have significant positive discretionary

revenues, firms with high growth expectations appear to engage in revenue management.

Next, we interact Threshold with Small, an indicator variable identifying firms with current

revenues less than $100 million. Small firms tend to have lower levels of visibility (Lang and

Lundholm, 1996; Miller, 2009) and have greater difficulty meeting their financing needs (Hadlock

and Pierce, 2010) and therefore may have greater incentives to beat a base-ten revenue threshold

if they expect doing so will increase their visibility or potentially increase their available financing.

As a result, small firms may be more likely to engage in revenue management. Consistent with

this, the results in Column 4 show a positive and significant coefficient on the interaction between

Threshold and Small. An alternative explanation for this result is that by focusing on firms with

revenues less than $100 million, we are picking up a greater incentive to manage revenues for the

potentially more salient thresholds of $1 million and $10 million. However, in Column 5 we omit

firms with revenues less than or equal to $10 million and still find a significant effect.

29 Results are similar when using the price to sales ratio instead.

Page 31: The Power of Numbers: Base-Ten Threshold Effects in Reported … · 2019. 1. 10. · 1 The Power of Numbers: Base-Ten Threshold Effects in Reported Revenue ABSTRACT We provide evidence

30

Lastly, in Column 6 we examine the interaction between Threshold and Loss, an indicator

for whether a firm has negative earnings. Table 6 Panel B suggests that firms place more emphasis

on beating base-ten thresholds in years when they fail to beat their earnings targets, possibly

because embattled managers are desperate to report some type of good news when they have failed

to meet their earnings targets. This may lead them to engage in opportunistic revenue management.

Consistent with this, we find a significantly positive effect of Threshold on discretionary revenues

for firms reporting a loss, although the main effect is still insignificant.30,31

In untabulated tests, we find preliminary evidence that these subsample tests also lead to

systematic variation in the level and growth of sales and other quarterly outcomes. For example,

threshold-beating firms in all three high-incentive subsamples experience faster decreases in

revenue growth after beating a base-ten revenue threshold, and loss and high growth firms

experience increases in days receivable in the year after they beat a base-ten threshold. These

results are consistent with these firms engaging in unsustainable activities to reach a base-ten

threshold, specifically by either decreasing credit constraints or prematurely recognizing revenues.

Overall, the results of this section provide evidence that on average firms appear to beat a

base-ten threshold for the first time through increased effort which leads to a sustainable increase

in future revenues. However, some subsets of firms engage in more opportunistic and less

sustainable methods in order to achieve these increases. These results add both to the literature on

the use of salient thresholds as motivators within the organization (Allen et al., 2016; Locke and

Latham, 2002), as well as the extensive literature on earnings and revenue management.

30 We found similar results when splitting instead on whether a firm failed to meet the revenue target given in its

management forecast, although results were insignificant using analyst revenue forecast targets. There was insufficient

data to run a multivariate test using executive compensation plan targets. 31 Inferences from Table 8 were consistent when examining only “major” or “minor” thresholds (similar to Footnote

14) or excluding firms with revenues less than $100 million.

Page 32: The Power of Numbers: Base-Ten Threshold Effects in Reported … · 2019. 1. 10. · 1 The Power of Numbers: Base-Ten Threshold Effects in Reported Revenue ABSTRACT We provide evidence

31

V. Summary and Conclusions

Human fixation on base-ten thresholds is likely a consequence of an evolutionary path

entered 340 million years ago when the common ancestor of amphibians, reptiles, birds, and

mammals evolved to have five digits on each limb (Coates, 2005). Humans have two hands and

five fingers on each hand and, as a consequence, counting systems around the world are

overwhelmingly designed around the number 10. In this paper we show that this non-economic

base-ten legacy has a measurable effect on the actions of managers, financial analysts, the business

press, and, by implication, the broader set of market participants who respond to the numbers

produced by managers and analysts.

We find that firms tend to report revenue just meeting or beating base-ten thresholds, and

this phenomenon does not appear to be the result of innocuous data-gathering or rounding practices

which apply to all reported numbers. In addition, revenue targets given in formal and informal

contracts (e.g. executive compensation contracts, management forecasts, and analyst forecasts) are

subject to this base-ten bias, and this contracting phenomenon serves as one driver for the base-

ten bias in reported revenues. However, we also find evidence that the base-ten bias in revenues is

not entirely driven by external targets; firms without explicit targets still exhibit a strongly

significant base-ten bias in reported revenues. This second finding represents a new type of

threshold-beating behavior previously unexamined in the prior accounting literature and highlights

the novelty of our results. We document one incentive driving firms without explicit revenue

targets to beat base-ten thresholds by showing that firms experience subsequent increases in

visibility in the form of increased press coverage upon reaching a base-ten threshold for the first

time. Lastly, firms which beat a base-ten threshold for the first time experience a spike in revenue

growth relative to control firms, with growth leveling off after crossing a base-ten threshold. This

Page 33: The Power of Numbers: Base-Ten Threshold Effects in Reported … · 2019. 1. 10. · 1 The Power of Numbers: Base-Ten Threshold Effects in Reported Revenue ABSTRACT We provide evidence

32

new level of revenue attained appears to be sustainable for firms on average, suggesting increased

effort plays a role in these revenue changes, but we also document evidence consistent with firms

engaging in revenue management when they face particularly strong incentives to beat base-ten

thresholds. Combined, these results are the first to date which provide evidence on both the motives

for and the mechanisms by which firms meet base-ten thresholds.

Although unusual patterns in earnings have been documented in prior research, our paper

is the first to document specific economic drivers of and mechanisms for a base-ten threshold

phenomenon that link it with intentional actions by managers. We believe this phenomenon is

especially important with respect to revenues given its monotonically increasing nature and

because revenues are an important performance target. For example, in the last year of our sample,

a higher proportion of firms had revenue targets than had either raw earnings or EPS targets.

Our study provides important evidence on pervasive goal-setting behavior in corporate

reporting around base-ten thresholds. Our results tie into research demonstrating the importance

of goal-setting for motivation more generally (Locke and Latham, 2002) as well as research

examining the importance of salient thresholds specifically (Allen et al., 2016), but we are the first

to explore economic determinants and mechanisms of this phenomenon in the context of corporate

reporting. Further, we find some evidence consistent with revenue management around base-ten

thresholds, even in the context of audited financial statements, implying that revenue numbers that

just exceed a base-ten threshold (and their corresponding earnings numbers) should be cautiously

interpreted. Considering the large number of base-ten thresholds present in revenue numbers (53

in this study) and the potential for similar incentives to arise in unaudited revenue numbers used

in internal reports or for privately held businesses, we believe the existence of this base-ten

phenomenon has far-reaching implications for academics and practitioners alike.

Page 34: The Power of Numbers: Base-Ten Threshold Effects in Reported … · 2019. 1. 10. · 1 The Power of Numbers: Base-Ten Threshold Effects in Reported Revenue ABSTRACT We provide evidence

33

REFERENCES

Abarbanell, J., Lehavy, R., 2003. Biased forecasts or biased earnings? The role of reported

earnings in explaining apparent bias and over/underreaction in analysts’ earnings

forecasts. J. Account. Econ. 36, 105-146.

Allen, E. J., Dechow, P.M., Pope, D.G., Wu, G., 2016. Reference-dependent preferences:

Evidence from marathon runners. Manag. Sci. 63, 1657-1672.

Amiram, D., Bozanic, Z., Rouen, E., 2015. Financial statement errors: evidence from the

distributional properties of financial statement numbers. Rev. Account. Stud. 20, 1540-

1593.

Bamber, L., Hui, K.W., Yeung, E., 2010. Managers’ EPS forecasts: nickeling and diming the

market? Account. Rev. 85, 63-95.

Barber, B., Odean, T., 2008. All that glitters: the effect of attention and news on the buying

behavior of individual and institutional investors. Rev. Financ. Stud. 21, 785-818.

Barberis, N., Huang, M., 2008. Stocks as lotteries: the implications of probability weighting for

security prices. Am. Econ. Rev. 98, 2066-2100.

Bartov, E., Givoly, D., Hayn, C., 2002. The rewards to meeting or beating earnings expectations.

J. Account. Econ. 33, 173-204.

Benford, F., 1938. The law of anomalous numbers. Proc. Am. Phil. Soc. 78, 551-572.

Bradshaw, M., Christensen, T., Gee, K., Whipple, B., 2018. Analysts’ GAAP earnings forecasts

and their implications for accounting research. J. Account. Econ., Forthcoming.

Brenner, G.A., Brenner, R., 1982. Memory and markets, or what are you paying $2.99 for a

widget? J. Bus. 55, 147-158.

Burgstahler, D., Chuck, E., 2015. Do scaling and selection explain earnings discontinuities? J.

Account. Econ. 60, 168-186.

Burgstahler, D., Dichev, I., 1997. Earnings management to avoid earnings decreases and losses.

J. Account. Econ. 24, 99-126.

Bushee, B., Core, J., Guay, W., Hamm, S., 2010. The role of the business press as an information

intermediary. J. Account. Res. 48, 1-19.

Carslaw, C., 1988. Anomalies in income numbers: evidence of goal oriented behavior. Account.

Rev. 63, 321-327.

Coates, M., 2005. Why do most species have five digits on their hands and feet? Sci. Am. April

25, 2005.

Page 35: The Power of Numbers: Base-Ten Threshold Effects in Reported … · 2019. 1. 10. · 1 The Power of Numbers: Base-Ten Threshold Effects in Reported Revenue ABSTRACT We provide evidence

34

Collins, D., Pungaliya, R., Vijh, A., 2017. The effects of firm growth and model specification

choices on tests of earnings management in quarterly settings. Account. Rev. 92, 69-100.

Daniel, K., Hirshleifer, D., Subrahmanyam, A., 1998. Investor psychology and security market

under- and overreactions. J. Financ. 53, 1839-1885.

Davis, A.K., 2002. The value relevance of revenue for internet firms: does reporting grossed-up

or barter revenue make a difference? J. Account. Res. 40, 445-477.

Das, S., Zhang, H., 2003. Rounding-up in reported EPS, behavioral thresholds, and earnings

management. J. Account. Econ. 35, 31-50.

Diamond, D.W., Verrecchia, R.E., 1991. Disclosure, Liquidity, and the Cost of Capital. J.

Financ. 46, 1325–1359.

Dichev, I., Skinner, D., 2002. Large-sample evidence on the debt covenant hypothesis. J.

Account. Res. 40, 1091-1123.

Dichev, I.D., Graham, J., Harvey, C., Rajgopal, S., 2013. Earnings quality: evidence from the

field. J. Account. Econ. 56, 1-33.

Drake, M., Guest, N., Twedt, B., 2014. The media and mispricing: the role of the business press

in the pricing of accounting information. Account. Rev. 89, 1673-1701.

Durtschi, C., Easton, P., 2005. Earnings Management? The shapes of the frequency distributions

of earnings metrics are not evidence ipso facto. J. Account. Res. 43, 557-592.

Durtschi, C., Easton, P., 2009. Earnings management? erroneous inferences based on earnings

frequency distributions. J. Account. Res. 47, 1249-1281.

Fang, L., Peress, J., 2009. Media coverage and the cross‐section of stock returns. J. Financ. 64,

2023-2052.

Gabor, A., Granger, C., 1964. Price sensitivity of the consumer. J. Advert. Res. 4, 40-44.

Hadlock, C., Pierce, J., 2010. New evidence on measuring financial constraints: moving beyond

the KZ index. Rev. Financ. Stud. 23, 1909-1940.

Hainmueller, J., 2012. Entropy balancing for causal effects: A multivariate reweighting method

to produce balanced samples in observational studies. Pol. Anal. 20, 25-46.

Healy, P. M., 1985. The effect of bonus schemes on accounting decisions. J. Account. Econ. 7,

85-107.

Herrmann, D., Thomas, W., 2005. Rounding of analyst forecasts. Account. Rev. 80, 805-823.

Page 36: The Power of Numbers: Base-Ten Threshold Effects in Reported … · 2019. 1. 10. · 1 The Power of Numbers: Base-Ten Threshold Effects in Reported Revenue ABSTRACT We provide evidence

35

Hirshleifer, D., Teoh, S., 2003. Limited attention, information disclosure, and financial reporting.

J. Account. Econ. 36, 337-386.

Huston, J., Kamdar, N., 1996. $9.99: Can "Just-Below" Pricing Be Reconciled with

Rationality? East. Econ. J. 22, 137-145.

Huttenlocher, J., Hedges, L., Bradburn, N., 1990. Reports of elapsed time: bounding and

rounding processes in estimation. J. Exp. Psychol.: Learn., Mem., Cogn. 16, 196-213.

Iliev, P., 2010. The Effect of SOX Section 404: Costs, earnings quality, and stock prices. J.

Financ. 65, 1163–1196.

Jegadeesh, N., Livnat, J., 2006. Revenues surprises and stock returns. J. Account. Econ. 41, 147-

171.

Jones, J.J., 1991. Earnings management during import relief investigations. J. Account. Res. 29,

193-228.

Kyle, A., Wang, F., 1997. Speculation duopoly with agreement to disagree: can overconfidence

survive the market test? J. Financ. 52, 2073-2090.

Lang, M.H., Lundholm, R.J., 1996. Corporate disclosure policy and analyst behavior. Acc. Rev.

467-492.

Locke, E. A., Latham, G.P., 2002. Building a practically useful theory of goal setting and task

motivation: A 35-year odyssey. Am. Psych. 57, 705.

McNichols, M., Stubben, S., 2018 Research design issues in studies using discretionary accruals.

Abacus 54, 227-246.

Miller, B., 2006. The press as a watchdog for accounting fraud. J. Account. Res. 44, 1001-1033.

Newcomb, S., 1881. Note on the frequency of use of the different digits in natural numbers. Am.

J. Math. 4, 39-40.

Noreen, E., 1989. Computer-intensive methods for testing hypotheses: an introduction. New

York City, NY: Wiley.

Odean, T., 1998. Are investors reluctant to realize their losses? J. Financ. 53, 1775-1798.

Peng, L., Xiang, W., 2006. Investor attention, overconfidence, and category learning. J. Financ.

Econ. 80, 563-602.

Rosch, E., 1975. Cognitive reference points. Cogn. Psychol. 7, 532-547.

Page 37: The Power of Numbers: Base-Ten Threshold Effects in Reported … · 2019. 1. 10. · 1 The Power of Numbers: Base-Ten Threshold Effects in Reported Revenue ABSTRACT We provide evidence

36

Roychowdhury, S., 2006. Earnings management through real activities manipulation. J. Account.

Econ. 42, 335-370.

Schindler, R., Wiman, A., 1989. Effects of odd pricing on price recall. J. Bus. Res. 19, 165-177.

Stubben, S., 2010. Discretionary revenues as a measure of earnings management. Account. Rev.

85, 695-717.

Swaminathan, S., Weintrop, J., 1991. The information content of earnings, revenues, and

expenses. J. Account. Res. 29, 418-427.

Thomas, J., 1989. Unusual patterns in reported earnings. Account. Rev. 64, 773-787.

Turner, S., 1958. Patterns of heaping in the reporting of numerical data. Proc. Soc. Stat. Sec.,

Am. Stat. Assoc. Washington, D.C., 248-251.

Tversky, A., Kahneman, D., 1973. Availability: a heuristic for judging frequency and

probability. Cogn. Psychol. 5, 207-232.

Whynes, D., Philips, Z., Frew, E., 2005. Think of a number…any number? Health Econ. 14,

1191-1195.

Zang, A., 2012. Evidence on the trade-off between real activities manipulation and accrual-based

earnings management. Account. Rev. 87, 675-703.

Page 38: The Power of Numbers: Base-Ten Threshold Effects in Reported … · 2019. 1. 10. · 1 The Power of Numbers: Base-Ten Threshold Effects in Reported Revenue ABSTRACT We provide evidence

37

VARIABLE APPENDIX

(For Regression Analyses)

Main Independent Variable of Interest

Threshold An indicator variable coded 1 if the firm has revenue

in the current period that has exceeded a particular

base-ten threshold of the form T = N x 10K (e.g.,

$50 million) for the first time, and 0 otherwise,

where the full set of potential values of T is given in

Table A.1. That is, Thresholdt=1 if there exists a T

(a base-ten threshold) where Revenuet >= T and

Revenuet-n < T for all n>0. All firm-years are

required to have at least two prior years of revenue

data to calculate this variable.

Main Dependent Variables of Interest

Discretionary_Rev The discretionary revenues for a firm in a given

year. Defined similar to Stubben (2010) but also

incorporating recommendations of Collins et al.

(2017) and McNichols and Stubben (2018).

Specifically, it is defined as the residual from the

following regression: ∆ARit = α + β1(1/ATit) +

β2∆R1_3it + β3∆R4it + ∑ βROA_Dumitk +

∑βSG_Dumitk + ∑βMB_Dumitk + εit, run by 2-digit

SIC industry-year. i and t denote firm i and year t.

Where ∆AR is the annual change in accounts

receivable, AT is total assets, ∆R1_3 is the change

in revenue for quarters 1-3, ∆R4 is the change in

revenue for quarter 4, ROA_Dumk is an indicator for

the kth quintile of return on assets, SG_Dumk is an

indicator for the kth quintile of current year sales

growth, MB_Dumk is an indicator for the kth

quintile of the current market to book ratio. The

third quintile indicators are omitted because of

collinearity.

Future Press Coverage The total number of articles written about a firm

over the 12-month period starting after the current

earnings announcement. An article is categorized as

being about a given firm if RavenPack has assigned

that article a relevancy score for that firm of greater

than or equal to 75.

Future External Press Coverage The total number of articles written about a firm

over the 12-month period starting after the current

Page 39: The Power of Numbers: Base-Ten Threshold Effects in Reported … · 2019. 1. 10. · 1 The Power of Numbers: Base-Ten Threshold Effects in Reported Revenue ABSTRACT We provide evidence

38

earnings announcement which were not issued by

the firm itself. Defined as (Future Press Coverage –

Future Firm-Initiated Press Coverage).

Future Firm-Initiated Press Coverage The total number of articles written about a firm

over the 12-month period starting after the current

earnings announcement that were issued by the firm

itself. Firm-initiated articles are identified as those

where the Article Type in Ravenpack is categorized

as Firm (i.e., Press Releases). An article is

categorized as being about a given firm if

RavenPack has assigned that article a relevancy

score for that firm of greater than or equal to 75.

Control Variables

12-Month Return The firm’s returns for the current period, calculated

over the 12-month period starting 3 months after the

prior fiscal year end.

Beat Analysts Inc An indicator variable coded 1 if the firm’s annual

earnings equal or exceed consensus (median) analyst

forecasts for the current period, using the last set of

estimates issued before the earnings announcement.

Beat Analysts Rev An indicator variable coded 1 if the firm’s annual

revenue equals or exceeds consensus (median)

analyst forecasts for the current period, using the last

set of estimates issued before the earnings

announcement.

Beat Last Year’s Inc An indicator variable coded 1 if the firm’s current

annual earnings equal or exceed annual earnings for

the prior year.

Beat Last Year’s Rev An indicator variable coded 1 if the firm’s current

annual revenue equals or exceeds annual revenue for

the prior year.

Big4 An indicator variable coded 1 if the firm has a Big-4

auditor.

Book-to-Market Book-to-market ratio, using book value of common

equity divided by market value of common equity,

both measured at the beginning of the year.

Equity Volatility The standard deviation of ln(1+rett), where rett is the

firm’s equity returns over the fiscal year. Requires at

least 9 months of data.

Firm Age Natural log of 1 plus the age of a firm in years. The

age is approximated using the earliest of the first

year a firm is covered in Compustat or CRSP or its

IPO year.

First_Ever Indicator variable coded 1 if current revenue is

greater than revenue in any prior year (i.e., this is the

Page 40: The Power of Numbers: Base-Ten Threshold Effects in Reported … · 2019. 1. 10. · 1 The Power of Numbers: Base-Ten Threshold Effects in Reported Revenue ABSTRACT We provide evidence

39

first year the company has ever reached the current

level of revenue). All firm-years are required to have

at least two prior years of revenue data to calculate

this variable.

High_Growth Indicator variable coded 1 if the firm has prior year

revenue growth in the top decile.

Leverage Beginning total long-term debt divided by total

assets.

ln(MVE) Natural log of the beginning of year market

capitalization (in millions).

ln(Revenue) Natural log of current revenue (in millions).

Loss An indicator variable coded 1 if the firm reports a

loss.

Merger An indicator variable coded 1 if the firm was

involved in a merger in the current year.

Revenue Growth Change in revenue, scaled by lagged revenue growth

(Revenuet – Revenuet-1/Revenuet-1)

ROA Return on assets. Net income before discontinued

operations and extraordinary items divided by

beginning of year total assets.

Small Indicator variable coded 1 if current revenues are

less than $100 million.

Page 41: The Power of Numbers: Base-Ten Threshold Effects in Reported … · 2019. 1. 10. · 1 The Power of Numbers: Base-Ten Threshold Effects in Reported Revenue ABSTRACT We provide evidence

40

APPENDIX 1

Examples of Organizations Reporting Salient Numbers Around Base-Ten Thresholds

1. A.O. Smith achieves $3 billion in sales (January 2018)

A.O. Smith, a manufacturer of water heaters, reported 2017 sales of $3 billion for the first time

in company history. The title of the 2017 earnings press release touts “A.O. Smith achieves

record sales of $3 billion in 2017” followed by a press release which only mentions the net

earnings number of $296.5 million after reiterating the record $3 billion in sales at the beginning

of the first paragraph.

2. U.S. Federal Government 2016 Budget Proposal (February 2015)

A “negative” threshold effect is evident in the fiscal 2016 federal budget submitted by U.S.

President Obama in February 2015. The president’s budget proposed fiscal 2016 spending of

$3.99 trillion. The Wall Street Journal editorial board found something strategic in this particular

number, writing: “The budget gnomes must have been told that, whatever you do, keep the top

line below $4 trillion.” (The Wall Street Journal. Editorial board. “Obama Unchained.” February

2, 2015.)

3. Facebook: First Reported Earnings of $1.000 billion (February 2012)

Facebook Inc. went public in May 2012. In February 2012 the company publicly released its full

financial results for the first time in its history. The most recent earnings reported in that first

public release were for the year ended December 31, 2011. The reported earnings amount was

exactly $1.000 billion. One conclusion is that Facebook wanted to make sure that in the run-up to

its long-anticipated IPO the company would enhance its reputation among investors by

financially elevating itself above the ranks of small, startup technology companies into the elite

set of “billion dollar companies.”

4. Heinz: Achieves Record Sales of Over $10 Billion (May 2008)

H.J. Heinz Company reported 12% sales growth to post record sales of $10.1 billion for fiscal

year 2007. Reaching this sales threshold led Business Wire to publish an article titled “Heinz

Achieves Record Sales of Over $10 Billion…” on May 29, 2008.

5. Callaway Golf: “Exceeded the billion-dollar mark” (April 2007)

The President and CEO of Callaway Golf George Fellows made a point to specifically highlight

reaching $1 billion in sales for the first time in the 2006 annual report. The firm reported annual

sales of $1.018 billion, and Fellows stated that for “the first time we have exceeded the billion-

dollar mark” in his 25th Anniversary of Callaway Golf Company letter to shareholders.

Page 42: The Power of Numbers: Base-Ten Threshold Effects in Reported … · 2019. 1. 10. · 1 The Power of Numbers: Base-Ten Threshold Effects in Reported Revenue ABSTRACT We provide evidence

41

APPENDIX 2

Approximate Randomization Procedure

In order to identify whether there are significantly more firms with total revenue just above

than just below base-ten thresholds, we assign observations into intervals or bins around each

potential threshold point according to the value of annual revenue. Bins above and below

thresholds are defined in percentage terms by calculating the percentage increase in revenues

necessary to move from the bottom to the top of the bin. For example, when the bin width is 1%,

the bin above $100,000 includes all numbers greater than or equal to $100,000 and less than

$101,000. Similarly, the bin below $100,000 includes all numbers greater than or equal to

$99,009.90 and less than $100,000 (because a 1% increase in $99,009.90 results in revenues of

$100,000). The bin widths used in our main tests are 0.25%, 0.5%, 1.0%, and 2.5%.32 The set of

base-ten thresholds that we study are all points of the form T = N x 10k, where $100,000 <= T <=

$80,000,000,000. This results in 53 base-ten thresholds, as shown in Table A.1.

We compute the test statistic, Sj, by summing the number of observations in the bins just

above all base-ten thresholds, the ABOVE bins, and dividing by the sum of the number of

observations in the bins just below all base-ten thresholds, the BELOW bins. The subscript j

denotes the various bin widths. If managers increase revenue to be above base-ten thresholds, we

would expect the Sj ratio to be greater than 1.

(1) 𝑆𝑗 =∑ 𝐴𝑖

𝑊𝑗𝑖=1

∑ 𝐵𝑖𝑊𝑗𝑖=1

where i=1,…,Wj are the base-ten threshold points, Wj is the number of threshold points for a

32 Defining the bin widths to be equal in percentage terms implies that the upper bin for a given revenue number is

always larger in absolute terms. In untabulated results, we perform our analysis with ABOVE and BELOW bins for

each threshold point constrained to be equal in absolute terms; there is no change in our inferences. The empirical

distributions generated in our approximate randomization tests naturally control for this difference in absolute size.

Additionally, this procedure essentially results in a logged distribution, where the bin widths above and below each

point are equal in log terms.

Page 43: The Power of Numbers: Base-Ten Threshold Effects in Reported … · 2019. 1. 10. · 1 The Power of Numbers: Base-Ten Threshold Effects in Reported Revenue ABSTRACT We provide evidence

42

given bin width j, up to a maximum of 53, and Ai and Bi represent the number of observations in

the ABOVE and BELOW bins within width j of threshold i, respectively.

We use an approximate randomization test to determine whether the Sj ratio is significantly

greater than one. We generate pseudo values of Sj, �̂�𝑗, by randomly selecting Wj pseudo threshold

points without replacement from the entire set of reported revenue numbers and then counting the

number of observations in the ABOVE and BELOW bins surrounding these pseudo threshold

points. We repeat this process 9,999 times and count the number of times, r, that the randomly-

generated pseudo-statistic �̂�𝑗 is greater than or equal to the originally-computed value of Sj.

Following Noreen (1989), the approximate randomization one-tailed p-value is computed as

follows:

(2) 𝑃(𝑆𝑗) = 𝑟+1

9,999+1

where 𝑟 = ∑ 𝐼(9,999𝑗=1 �̂�𝑗 ≥ 𝑆𝑗), I is the indicator function, and j is the bin width. We repeat this

procedure for each different bin width, j, from 0.25% of revenue to 2.5% of revenue. For

example, assume that the actual value of Sj is 1.20 and that of the 9,999 randomly-selected sets

of bins, none resulted in a pseudo-statistic �̂�𝑗 value as large as 1.20. In this case, the value of

P(Sj) is 0.0001 = ((0+1)/(9,999+1)).

Tests Comparing the Base-Ten Threshold Effect Between Metrics

To rule out the possibility that database providers introduce rounding or that managers

prefer round values for all numbers, Table A.2 compares the base-ten threshold effect of revenue

with that of cost of goods sold, total assets, and market capitalization. In order to do this, we

perform additional approximate randomization tests where we compare the Sj statistics (aggregate

Page 44: The Power of Numbers: Base-Ten Threshold Effects in Reported … · 2019. 1. 10. · 1 The Power of Numbers: Base-Ten Threshold Effects in Reported Revenue ABSTRACT We provide evidence

43

threshold ABOVE/BELOW ratio) for revenue with the Sj statistics of the other metrics.33 Finding

elevated ABOVE/BELOW ratios in revenue data beyond those in these other data items would

suggest that managers are motivated to take actions to increase revenue to reach these base-ten

thresholds above and beyond any intentional or unintentional behavior on the part of managers to

manage other metrics. We choose these data items as controls because they are the same order of

magnitude as revenue. Cost of goods sold is a particularly apt benchmark because it is generated

by similar processes that lead to revenue and is much less likely to be strategically managed to

exceed base-ten thresholds. Total assets are another reasonable benchmark because they are of a

similar magnitude as revenues; assets are a particularly conservative benchmark because, as a

measure of size, firms are also likely to have incentives to beat base-ten asset thresholds, although

we expect these incentives to be relatively weaker because total assets are not frequently used as

performance targets to the extent that revenues are, as demonstrated in Table 2. Market value of

equity is not subject to direct manipulation by firms, but managers could have incentives to

manipulate it upward through indirect actions, making it another conservative benchmark when

demonstrating the existence of the base-ten threshold effect in revenue.34

Using cost of goods sold as an illustration, the test statistic, Vj, for these tests is the ratio of

the separate Sj statistics for revenue and cost of goods sold.

(3) 𝑉𝑗 = 𝑆𝑗,𝑅𝑒𝑣𝑒𝑛𝑢𝑒

𝑆𝑗,𝐶𝑂𝐺𝑆

where Sj,Revenue is the ratio of the number of ABOVE and BELOW observations in bin width j

around all base-ten thresholds for revenue and Sj,COGS is the same ratio for cost of goods sold. If

33 We use this same approach to compare the base-ten threshold effect in revenue across different subsamples. 34 Although firms have more difficulty in directly managing ending market capitalization for an accounting reporting

period than in managing reported revenue, evidence such as in Iliev (2010) suggests that firms can exercise some

influence on the level of end-of-period market values. Specifically, Iliev examines public float.

Page 45: The Power of Numbers: Base-Ten Threshold Effects in Reported … · 2019. 1. 10. · 1 The Power of Numbers: Base-Ten Threshold Effects in Reported Revenue ABSTRACT We provide evidence

44

managers have incentives to manage reported revenue to exceed base-ten thresholds above and

beyond any threshold effect that may exist for cost of goods sold, then this Vj ratio should be

greater than one. Similar test statistics are computed using assets and market capitalization.

We then use a similar approximate randomization test as described earlier to determine

whether the Vj ratio is significantly greater than one, now randomly selecting points from both the

revenue and comparison metric distributions. Data comparing the magnitude of the base-ten

threshold effect in reported revenue to the corresponding effect in cost of goods sold, total assets,

and market value of equity are presented in Table A.2. For each of the bin widths reported, there

is a significantly greater proportion of revenue observations just above a base-ten threshold relative

to all three comparison metrics, with an approximate randomization p-value reported of less than

0.01 in each case. These results support our prediction that managers take strategic actions to report

revenue just above base-ten thresholds, and this base-ten revenue threshold effect is not caused by

a general preference for rounding or because of rounding introduced by the data provider.

1. $100,000 15. $6,000,000 29. $200,000,000 43. $7,000,000,000

2. $200,000 16. $7,000,000 30. $300,000,000 44. $8,000,000,000

3. $300,000 17. $8,000,000 31. $400,000,000 45. $9,000,000,000

4. $400,000 18. $9,000,000 32. $500,000,000 46. $10,000,000,000

5. $500,000 19. $10,000,000 33. $600,000,000 47. $20,000,000,000

6. $600,000 20. $20,000,000 34. $700,000,000 48. $30,000,000,000

7. $700,000 21. $30,000,000 35. $800,000,000 49. $40,000,000,000

8. $800,000 22. $40,000,000 36. $900,000,000 50. $50,000,000,000

9. $900,000 23. $50,000,000 37. $1,000,000,000 51. $60,000,000,000

10. $1,000,000 24. $60,000,000 38. $2,000,000,000 52. $70,000,000,000

11. $2,000,000 25. $70,000,000 39. $3,000,000,000 53. $80,000,000,000

12. $3,000,000 26. $80,000,000 40. $4,000,000,000

13. $4,000,000 27. $90,000,000 41. $5,000,000,000

14. $5,000,000 28. $100,000,000 42. $6,000,000,000

All points of the form T = N x 10k, where $100,000 <= T <= $80,000,000,000.

Table A.1 List of Base-Ten Thresholds Studied in the Paper

Page 46: The Power of Numbers: Base-Ten Threshold Effects in Reported … · 2019. 1. 10. · 1 The Power of Numbers: Base-Ten Threshold Effects in Reported Revenue ABSTRACT We provide evidence

45

0.25% 1.588 1.111 1.429 0.0001***

0.50% 1.344 1.077 1.248 0.0001***

1.00% 1.163 1.027 1.133 0.0001***

2.50% 1.079 1.006 1.072 0.0001***

Total Observations: 367,233 363,907

0.25% 1.588 1.273 1.248 0.0001***

0.50% 1.344 1.152 1.166 0.0001***

1.00% 1.163 1.080 1.077 0.0001***

2.50% 1.079 1.052 1.026 0.0062***

Total Observations: 367,233 367,233

0.25% 1.588 1.085 1.463 0.0001***

0.50% 1.344 1.031 1.304 0.0001***

1.00% 1.163 1.026 1.134 0.0001***

2.50% 1.079 1.015 1.063 0.0001***

Total Observations: 367,233 291,526

Panel B: Revenue and Total Assets Comparison

Bin Width

(%)

Revenue

ABOVE/BELOW

(1)

Assets

ABOVE/BELOW

(2)

Ratio of Ratios

(1)/(2)

Approximate

Randomization

p-value

Table A.2 Comparison of the Base-Ten Threshold Effect in Revenues to Other Metrics

Panel A: Revenue and Cost of Goods Sold Comparison

Bin Width

(%)

COGS

ABOVE/BELOW

(2)

For all Thresholds of the form T = N x 10K, for integers N (1 through 9) and K (5 through 10)

Ratio of Ratios

(1)/(2)

Revenue

ABOVE/BELOW

(1)

Approximate

Randomization

p-value

This table presents the results of one-tailed approximate randomization tests estimating the significance of the

Threshold Effect for reported revenues relative to that of cost of goods sold, total assets, and market value of

equity. Rows present the ratio of the number of observations in the bins just above and below threshold points

for each financial metric, as well as the ratio of these ratios (the test statistic used to generate the approximate

randomization p-value). ***,**,* Significant at the 0.01, 0.05, 0.1 level for one-tailed test.

Ratio of Ratios

(1)/(2)

Panel C: Revenue and Market Value of Equity Comparison

Bin Width

(%)

Revenue

ABOVE/BELOW

(1)

MVE

ABOVE/BELOW

(2)

Approximate

Randomization

p-value

Page 47: The Power of Numbers: Base-Ten Threshold Effects in Reported … · 2019. 1. 10. · 1 The Power of Numbers: Base-Ten Threshold Effects in Reported Revenue ABSTRACT We provide evidence

46

Page 48: The Power of Numbers: Base-Ten Threshold Effects in Reported … · 2019. 1. 10. · 1 The Power of Numbers: Base-Ten Threshold Effects in Reported Revenue ABSTRACT We provide evidence

47

Page 49: The Power of Numbers: Base-Ten Threshold Effects in Reported … · 2019. 1. 10. · 1 The Power of Numbers: Base-Ten Threshold Effects in Reported Revenue ABSTRACT We provide evidence

48

N

50s and 60s 32,651

70s 52,257

80s 67,233

90s 89,136

2000s 79,423

2010s* 46,533

367,233

*2010-2017

Firm-Year Revenue Observations by Decade

Panel A: Revenue Approximate Randomization Analysis Sample

Table 1. Sample Descriptive Statistics

N Mean Median Std P25 P75

Threshold 40,070 0.29 0.00 0.45 0.00 1.00

Future Press Coverage 34,831 225.78 151.00 258.20 95.00 243.00

Future Firm-Initiated Press Coverage 34,831 43.24 33.00 40.69 21.00 50.00

Future External Press Coverage 34,831 182.17 117.00 224.98 69.00 194.00

Discretionary Revenue 40,070 0.00 0.00 0.03 -0.01 0.01

First-Ever 40,070 0.56 1.00 0.50 0.00 1.00

ln(Revenue) 40,070 6.36 6.35 2.03 5.05 7.70

Revenue Growth 40,070 0.17 0.09 0.54 -0.01 0.23

Beat Last Year's Rev 40,070 0.73 1.00 0.45 0.00 1.00

Beat Analysts Rev 40,070 0.57 1.00 0.49 0.00 1.00

Loss 40,070 0.28 0.00 0.45 0.00 1.00

Beat Last Year's Inc 40,070 0.58 1.00 0.49 0.00 1.00

Beat Analysts Inc 40,070 0.64 1.00 0.48 0.00 1.00

High_Growth 40,070 0.09 0.00 0.29 0.00 0.00

Small 40,070 0.19 0.00 0.39 0.00 0.00

ROA 40,070 0.00 0.04 0.23 -0.01 0.09

12-Month Return 40,070 0.18 0.07 0.77 -0.23 0.38

Equity Volatility 40,070 0.14 0.11 0.08 0.08 0.17

ln(MVE) 40,070 6.69 6.58 1.81 5.43 7.85

Big4 40,070 0.87 1.00 0.34 1.00 1.00

Book-to-Market 40,070 0.55 0.44 0.60 0.26 0.71

Leverage 40,070 0.20 0.13 0.25 0.00 0.31

Firm Age 40,070 2.78 2.71 0.73 2.20 3.30

Merger 40,070 0.19 0.00 0.39 0.00 0.00

Panel B: Regression Analysis Sample

In Panel A, observations are required to have non-missing values of current total assets and revenues and correspond to the

full revenue approximate randomization sample in Table 3. Descriptive statistics of the variables used in the regression

analyses are given in Panel B. For the control variables, the statistics given correspond to the sample used for the

discretionary revenue regression in Table 8.

Untabulated Control Variables

Page 50: The Power of Numbers: Base-Ten Threshold Effects in Reported … · 2019. 1. 10. · 1 The Power of Numbers: Base-Ten Threshold Effects in Reported Revenue ABSTRACT We provide evidence

49

Panel A. Prevalence of Performance Metrics in Executive Compensation Plans

Performance Metric Proportion

EPS 0.301

Revenue 0.288

Operating Income 0.236

Net Income 0.205

Operating Cash Flows 0.198

Performance Metric Proportion

EPS 0.533

Revenue 0.520

Capital Expenditures 0.440

EBITDA 0.130

Net Income 0.090

Panel C. Prevalence of Performance Metrics in Analyst Forecasts

Performance Metric Proportion

EPS 0.887

Revenue 0.864

Net Income 0.699

Pre-Tax Income 0.671

EBITDA 0.596

Panel B. Prevalence of Performance Metrics in Management Forecasts

This table demonstrates the prevalence of revenue targets for U.S. firms. Each panel lists the top five

most common accounting metric targets, by the proportion of firm-years which use each metric for a

given type of target. Panel A demonstrates the prevalence of various accounting metrics as targets in

executive compensation contracts for grants granted for 16,247 firm-years over 1998 to 2016 with at

least one accounting metric. Panel B demonstrates the prevalence of various accounting metrics in

management forecasts for 36,992 firm-years over 2003 to 2018. Panel C demonstrates the prevalence of

various accounting metrics in analyst forecasts for 111,556 firm-years over 2000 to 2018, using the

IBES Detail files and including stopped forecasts (the Summary files are restricted to only firms which

have at least one EPS forecast). The sample period for each of the three metrics starts in the year when

revenue targets are tracked in the data.

Table 2. Importance of Revenue Targets

Page 51: The Power of Numbers: Base-Ten Threshold Effects in Reported … · 2019. 1. 10. · 1 The Power of Numbers: Base-Ten Threshold Effects in Reported Revenue ABSTRACT We provide evidence

50

Page 52: The Power of Numbers: Base-Ten Threshold Effects in Reported … · 2019. 1. 10. · 1 The Power of Numbers: Base-Ten Threshold Effects in Reported Revenue ABSTRACT We provide evidence

51

0.25% 2.010 1.679 1.197

0.50% 1.563 1.446 1.081

1.00% 1.262 1.223 1.032

2.50% 1.147 1.079 1.063

Total Observations: 29,068 29,068

0.25% 2.188 1.267 1.727

0.50% 1.564 1.156 1.353

1.00% 1.248 1.043 1.196

2.50% 1.172 1.051 1.115

Total Observations: 26,358 26,358

Panel A: Firms with High Compared to Low Past Revenue Growth

Table 4. Revenue Threshold Effect for High Expected Revenue Growth Firms

For all Thresholds of the form T = N x 10K, for integers N (1 through 9) and K (5 through 10)

Panel B: Firms with a High Compared to Low Price-to-Sales Ratio

0.0760*

0.1978

0.0187**

Ratio of Ratios

(1)/(2)

Bin Width

(%)

High Growth Firms

ABOVE/BELOW

(1)

Low Growth Firms

ABOVE/BELOW

(2)

The results of one-tailed approximate randomization tests estimating the significance of the difference in the

Threshold Effect for high and low past revenue growth firms and price-to-sales ratios, where high (low) growth

(P/S ratio) is defined as the top (bottom) percentage revenue growth (P/S ratio) decile. Rows present the ratio of

the number of observations in the bins just above and below threshold points for each comparison group, as well

as the ratio of these ratios (the test statistic used to generate the approximate randomization p-value). ***,**,*

Significant at the 0.01, 0.05, 0.1 level for one-tailed test.

0.0838*

0.0312**

0.0735*

0.0947*

Bin Width

(%)

High Growth Firms

ABOVE/BELOW

(1)

Approximate

Randomization

p-value

0.0124**

Low Growth Firms

ABOVE/BELOW

(2)

Ratio of Ratios

(1)/(2)

Page 53: The Power of Numbers: Base-Ten Threshold Effects in Reported … · 2019. 1. 10. · 1 The Power of Numbers: Base-Ten Threshold Effects in Reported Revenue ABSTRACT We provide evidence

52

Panel A. Executive Compensation Plan Revenue Targets

0.25% 91 1,017 11.176

0.50% 242 1,186 4.901

1.00% 559 1,436 2.569

2.50% 1,814 2,348 1.294

Total Observations: 20,446

0.25% 52 1,419 27.288

0.50% 126 1,512 12.000

1.00% 309 1,723 5.576

2.50% 1,176 2,603 2.213

Total Observations: 18,001

Panel C: Analyst Revenue Forecasts

0.25% 3,458 5,970 1.726

0.50% 6,965 9,802 1.407

1.00% 14,213 17,168 1.208

2.50% 37,081 40,765 1.099

Total Observations: 404,008

This table presents the results of one-tailed approximate randomization tests estimating the significance of the

Threshold Effect for three revenue targets specified in formal and informal contracts: executive compensation

plan revenue targets, management revenue forecasts, and analyst revenue forecasts. The data used are not

actual reported revenues but the target numbers themselves. In Panel B (C), the last forecast issued by that

firm (analyst) preceding the earnings announcements for the period is used. Panel B uses all management

revenue point forecasts as well as the bottom value of management revenue range forecasts. ***,**,*

Significant at the 0.01, 0.05, 0.1 level for one-tailed test.

0.0001***

0.0001***

0.0001***

0.0001***

0.0001***

Bin Width

(%)

Table 5. Threshold Effect in Formal and Informal Revenue Contracts

For all Thresholds of the form T = N x 10K, for integers N (1 through 9) and K (5 through 10)

Bin Width

(%)

Bin BELOW

Threshold Points

Bin ABOVE

Threshold Points

0.0001***

0.0001***

0.0001***

0.0133**

Panel B: Management Revenue Forecasts

Ratio

ABOVE/BELOW

Number of Observations in Approximate

Randomization

p-value

Bin Width

(%)

Bin BELOW

Threshold Points

Bin ABOVE

Threshold Points

Ratio

ABOVE/BELOW

Number of Observations in Approximate

Randomization

p-value

Bin BELOW

Threshold Points

Bin ABOVE

Threshold Points

Approximate

Randomization

p-value

Ratio

ABOVE/BELOW

Number of Observations in

0.0001***

0.0001***

0.0001***

Page 54: The Power of Numbers: Base-Ten Threshold Effects in Reported … · 2019. 1. 10. · 1 The Power of Numbers: Base-Ten Threshold Effects in Reported Revenue ABSTRACT We provide evidence

53

Panel A: Revenue Threshold Effect for Firms which Beat vs. Fail to Beat Revenue Targets

0.25% 1.343 1.192 1.126 0.1469

0.50% 1.253 1.119 1.120 0.0604*

1.00% 1.176 1.015 1.158 0.0015***

2.50% 1.104 1.018 1.084 0.0046***

Total Observations: 28,929 40,421

Panel B: Revenue Threshold Effect for Firms which Beat vs. Fail to Beat Earnings Targets

0.25% 1.238 1.549 0.799 0.9991

0.50% 1.220 1.310 0.931 0.9282

1.00% 1.114 1.165 0.956 0.9091

2.50% 1.082 1.068 1.013 0.2137

Total Observations: 80,907 142,659

Panel C. Revenue Threshold Effect for Firms Without Other Available Revenue Targets

0.25% 341 472 1.384 0.0051***

0.50% 720 840 1.167 0.0435**

1.00% 1,456 1,551 1.065 0.2139

2.50% 3,690 3,759 1.019 0.4586

Total Observations: 40,391

Bin Width

(%)

Revenue Target Beaters

ABOVE/BELOW

(1)

Revenue Target

Non-Beaters

ABOVE/BELOW

(2)

Ratio of Ratios

(1)/(2)

Bin Width

(%)

Earnings Target

Beaters

ABOVE/BELOW

(1)

Earnings Target

Non-Beaters

ABOVE/BELOW

(2)

Ratio of Ratios

(1)/(2)

Bin Width

(%)

Bin BELOW

Threshold Points

Bin ABOVE

Threshold Points

Ratio

ABOVE/BELOW

This table presents the results of one-tailed approximate randomization tests which investigate the extent to which

the base-ten threshold effect in revenue is driven by a base-ten effect in explicit targets. In Panel A (B), the

magnitude of the revenue threshold effect is compared between firms which beat all and fail to beat at least one

revenue (earnings) target. The targets used in Panel A are: compensation plan revenue targets, management revenue

forecasts, analyst revenue forecasts, and last year's reported revenue. Panel B uses the following targets:

compensation plan EPS targets, management EPS forecasts, analyst EPS forecasts, last year's reported earnings, and

positive income. To be included in Panel A or B a firm must have at least two non-missing target values. Panel C

demonstrates the significance of the revenue threshold effect for firms which have no available compensation plan

revenue targets, management revenue forecasts, or analyst revenue forecasts.***,**,* Significant at the 0.01, 0.05,

0.1 level for one-tailed test.

Table 6. The Effect of Explicit Targets on the Revenue Threshold Effect

For all Thresholds of the form T = N x 10K, for integers N (1 through 9) and K (5 through 10)

Approximate

Randomization

p-value

Approximate

Randomization

p-value

Number of Observations in Approximate

Randomization

p-value

Page 55: The Power of Numbers: Base-Ten Threshold Effects in Reported … · 2019. 1. 10. · 1 The Power of Numbers: Base-Ten Threshold Effects in Reported Revenue ABSTRACT We provide evidence

54

(1) (2) (3) (4)

VARIABLES

Future

Press Coverage

Future

Press Coverage

Future

Firm-Initiated

Press Coverage

Future

External

Press Coverage

Threshold 10.59*** 8.892*** 0.889** 8.304***

(3.501) (4.180) (2.275) (4.396)

First_Ever -15.84*** 2.286 0.625 1.635

(-3.462) (0.681) (0.994) (0.555)

ln(Revenue) 43.54*** 18.02*** 3.026*** 14.76***

(11.68) (5.714) (5.123) (5.366)

Revenue Growth 9.768*** 4.835** 0.780 4.241**

(4.067) (2.377) (1.573) (2.525)

Beat Last Year's Rev -4.204 -4.377 -1.250** -3.192

(-1.219) (-1.433) (-2.218) (-1.218)

Beat Analysts Rev -0.576 -0.0438 -0.120 0.123

(-0.264) (-0.0259) (-0.387) (0.0831)

Loss 29.77*** 5.698* -0.133 5.820**

(7.375) (1.850) (-0.233) (2.182)

Beat Last Year's Inc 13.37*** 3.377* 0.187 2.994**

(5.712) (1.945) (0.567) (1.993)

Beat Analysts Inc -3.246 -1.528 -0.544 -0.951

(-0.0721) (2.601) (2.099) (2.455)

Controls Y Y Y Y

Industry & Year FE Y N N N

Firm & Year FE N Y Y Y

Observations 34,831 34,831 34,831 34,831

Adjusted R-squared 0.468 0.756 0.673 0.758

Table 7. Outcomes of Beating a Base-Ten Revenue Threshold for the First Time

Regression results estimating the effect of beating a base-ten revenue threshold for the first time on press

coverage for fiscal years 1999-2014. 3-digit SIC code insutry. Controls untabulated for parsimony comprise:

ROA, 12-Month Return, Equity Volatility, ln(MVE), Big4, Book-to-Market, Leverage, Firm Age, Merger .

Robust t-statistics in parentheses. ***,**,* Significant at the 0.01, 0.05, 0.1 level for two-tailed test.

Page 56: The Power of Numbers: Base-Ten Threshold Effects in Reported … · 2019. 1. 10. · 1 The Power of Numbers: Base-Ten Threshold Effects in Reported Revenue ABSTRACT We provide evidence

55

(1) (2) (3) (4) (5) (6)

VARIABLES Split:High_Growth Split:Small Split:Small Split:LossRev > $10 million

Threshold 0.000867** 0.000341 -4.32e-06 -0.000237 -0.000285 -8.50e-05

(2.221) (0.768) (-0.00984) (-0.536) (-0.641) (-0.189)

Threshold x Split 0.00396** 0.00424*** 0.00422*** 0.00271**

(2.373) (3.198) (3.061) (2.398)

Split 0.000103 -0.000958 -0.00111 -0.000826

(0.0805) (-0.682) (-0.781) (-1.283)

First_Ever -2.33e-05 -5.91e-05 -2.08e-05 -0.000179 -0.000126 -0.000114

(-0.0509) (-0.102) (-0.0358) (-0.308) (-0.215) (-0.197)

ln(Revenue) -0.00102*** -0.00145*** -0.00140*** -0.00160*** -0.00212*** -0.00150***

(-4.425) (-2.691) (-2.584) (-2.964) (-2.919) (-2.786)

Revenue Growth -0.00132*** -0.00178*** -0.00202*** -0.00197*** -0.00172** -0.00195***

(-2.822) (-2.719) (-3.036) (-2.972) (-2.158) (-2.947)

Beat Last Year's Rev 0.000163 -0.000348 -0.000365 -0.000360 -0.000565 -0.000429

(0.320) (-0.596) (-0.625) (-0.616) (-0.956) (-0.734)

Beat Analysts Rev 0.000600* 0.000712** 0.000726** 0.000730** 0.000615* 0.000730**

(1.957) (2.041) (2.081) (2.093) (1.756) (2.092)

Beat Last Year's Inc -6.07e-05 0.000208 0.000202 0.000250 0.000275 0.000284

(-0.186) (0.567) (0.553) (0.684) (0.740) (0.777)

Beat Analysts Inc -0.000422 8.12e-05 8.93e-05 9.71e-05 0.000116 8.22e-05

(-1.305) (0.215) (0.236) (0.257) (0.304) (0.218)

Controls Y Y Y Y Y Y

Industry & Year FE Y N N N N N

Firm & Year FE N Y Y Y Y Y

Observations 40,070 40,070 40,070 40,070 39,132 40,070

Adjusted R-squared 0.007 0.093 0.093 0.093 0.099 0.093

Table 8. Discretionary Revenue for First Time Base-Ten Threshold Beaters

Regression results estimating the magnitude of discretionary revenues of firms beating a base-ten revenue threshold for the first time for fiscal years

1999-2014. 3-digit SIC code industry. Controls untabulated for parsimony comprise: ROA, 12-Month Return, Equity Volatility, ln(MVE), Big4,

Book-to-Market, Leverage, Firm Age, Merger , and the main effect of all three Split variables in specifications where they are not part of an

interaction. Robust t-statistics in parentheses. ***,**,* Significant at the 0.01, 0.05, 0.1 level for two-tailed test.

Dependent Variable: Discretionary_Rev