The mismatch negativity (MMN) – A unique window to ... et al (2011... · Invited Review The...

35
Invited Review The mismatch negativity (MMN) – A unique window to disturbed central auditory processing in ageing and different clinical conditions R. Näätänen a,b,c,, T. Kujala c,d , C. Escera e,f , T. Baldeweg g,h , K. Kreegipuu a , S. Carlson i,j,k , C. Ponton l a Institute of Psychology, University of Tartu, Tartu, Estonia b Center of Integrative Neuroscience (CFIN), University of Aarhus, Aarhus, Denmark c Cognitive Brain Research Unit, Cognitive Science, Institute of Behavioural Sciences, University of Helsinki, Helsinki, Finland d Cicero Learning, University of Helsinki, Helsinki, Finland e Institute for Brain, Cognition and Behavior (IR3C), University of Barcelona, Barcelona, Catalonia, Spain f Cognitive Neuroscience Research Group, Department of Psychiatry and Clinical Psychobiology, University of Barcelona, Barcelona, Catalonia, Spain g Institute of Child Health, University College London, London, UK h Great Ormond Street Hospital for Sick Children NHS Trust, London, UK i Neuroscience Unit, Institute of Biomedicine/Physiology, University of Helsinki, Helsinki, Finland j Brain Research Unit, Low Temperature Laboratory, Aalto University School of Science and Technology, Espoo, Finland k Medical School, University of Tampere, Tampere, Finland l Compumedics NeuroScan, Santa Fe, NM, USA article info Article history: Accepted 20 September 2011 Available online 13 December 2011 Keywords: The mismatch negativity (MMN) Central auditory processing Event-related potentials (ERP) Magnetoencephalography Auditory discrimination Neuropsychiatric disorders Neurological disorders Neurodevelopmental disorders The NMDA-receptor system highlights The mismatch negativity (MMN) indexes different types of central auditory abnormalities in different neuropsychiatric, neurological, and neurodevelopmental disorders. The diminished amplitude/prolonged peak latency observed in patients usually indexes decreased audi- tory discrimination. An MMN deficit may also index cognitive and functional decline shared by different disorders irrespec- tive of their specific aetiology and symptomatology. MMN deficits index deficient N-methyl-D-aspartate (NMDA) receptor function affecting memory-trace formation and hence cognition in different disorders. abstract In this article, we review clinical research using the mismatch negativity (MMN), a change-detection response of the brain elicited even in the absence of attention or behavioural task. In these studies, the MMN was usually elicited by employing occasional frequency, duration or speech-sound changes in repetitive background stimulation while the patient was reading or watching videos. It was found that in a large number of different neuropsychiatric, neurological and neurodevelopmental disorders, as well as in normal ageing, the MMN amplitude was attenuated and peak latency prolonged. Besides indexing decreased discrimination accuracy, these effects may also reflect, depending on the specific stimulus paradigm used, decreased sensory-memory duration, abnormal perception or attention control or, most importantly, cognitive decline. In fact, MMN deficiency appears to index cognitive decline irrespective of the specific symptomatologies and aetiologies of the different disorders involved. Ó 2011 International Federation of Clinical Neurophysiology. Published by Elsevier Ireland Ltd. All rights reserved. Contents 1. Introduction ......................................................................................................... 425 2. The MMN as an index of decreased auditory discrimination accuracy (Table 1) ................................................... 431 3. The MMN as an index of shortened sensory-memory duration (Table 2) ........................................................ 433 1388-2457/$36.00 Ó 2011 International Federation of Clinical Neurophysiology. Published by Elsevier Ireland Ltd. All rights reserved. doi:10.1016/j.clinph.2011.09.020 Corresponding author at: Cognitive Brain Research Unit, Cognitive Science, Institute of Behavioural Sciences, University of Helsinki, Helsinki, Finland. Tel.: +358 40 501 5740; fax: +358 9 191 29450. E-mail address: risto.naatanen@helsinki.fi (R. Näätänen). Clinical Neurophysiology 123 (2012) 424–458 Contents lists available at SciVerse ScienceDirect Clinical Neurophysiology journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/clinph

Transcript of The mismatch negativity (MMN) – A unique window to ... et al (2011... · Invited Review The...

Page 1: The mismatch negativity (MMN) – A unique window to ... et al (2011... · Invited Review The mismatch negativity (MMN) – A unique window to disturbed central auditory processing

Clinical Neurophysiology 123 (2012) 424–458

Contents lists available at SciVerse ScienceDirect

Clinical Neurophysiology

journal homepage: www.elsevier .com/locate /c l inph

Invited Review

The mismatch negativity (MMN) – A unique window to disturbed centralauditory processing in ageing and different clinical conditions

R. Näätänen a,b,c,⇑, T. Kujala c,d, C. Escera e,f, T. Baldeweg g,h, K. Kreegipuu a, S. Carlson i,j,k, C. Ponton l

a Institute of Psychology, University of Tartu, Tartu, Estoniab Center of Integrative Neuroscience (CFIN), University of Aarhus, Aarhus, Denmarkc Cognitive Brain Research Unit, Cognitive Science, Institute of Behavioural Sciences, University of Helsinki, Helsinki, Finlandd Cicero Learning, University of Helsinki, Helsinki, Finlande Institute for Brain, Cognition and Behavior (IR3C), University of Barcelona, Barcelona, Catalonia, Spainf Cognitive Neuroscience Research Group, Department of Psychiatry and Clinical Psychobiology, University of Barcelona, Barcelona, Catalonia, Spaing Institute of Child Health, University College London, London, UKh Great Ormond Street Hospital for Sick Children NHS Trust, London, UKi Neuroscience Unit, Institute of Biomedicine/Physiology, University of Helsinki, Helsinki, Finlandj Brain Research Unit, Low Temperature Laboratory, Aalto University School of Science and Technology, Espoo, Finlandk Medical School, University of Tampere, Tampere, Finlandl Compumedics NeuroScan, Santa Fe, NM, USA

a r t i c l e i n f o

Article history:Accepted 20 September 2011Available online 13 December 2011

Keywords:The mismatch negativity (MMN)Central auditory processingEvent-related potentials (ERP)MagnetoencephalographyAuditory discriminationNeuropsychiatric disordersNeurological disordersNeurodevelopmental disordersThe NMDA-receptor system

1388-2457/$36.00 � 2011 International Federation odoi:10.1016/j.clinph.2011.09.020

⇑ Corresponding author at: Cognitive Brain ReseaInstitute of Behavioural Sciences, University of Helsink40 501 5740; fax: +358 9 191 29450.

E-mail address: [email protected] (R. Näät

h i g h l i g h t s

� The mismatch negativity (MMN) indexes different types of central auditory abnormalities in differentneuropsychiatric, neurological, and neurodevelopmental disorders.� The diminished amplitude/prolonged peak latency observed in patients usually indexes decreased audi-tory discrimination.� An MMN deficit may also index cognitive and functional decline shared by different disorders irrespec-tive of their specific aetiology and symptomatology.� MMN deficits index deficient N-methyl-D-aspartate (NMDA) receptor function affecting memory-traceformation and hence cognition in different disorders.

a b s t r a c t

In this article, we review clinical research using the mismatch negativity (MMN), a change-detectionresponse of the brain elicited even in the absence of attention or behavioural task. In these studies, theMMN was usually elicited by employing occasional frequency, duration or speech-sound changes inrepetitive background stimulation while the patient was reading or watching videos. It was found thatin a large number of different neuropsychiatric, neurological and neurodevelopmental disorders, as wellas in normal ageing, the MMN amplitude was attenuated and peak latency prolonged.

Besides indexing decreased discrimination accuracy, these effects may also reflect, depending on thespecific stimulus paradigm used, decreased sensory-memory duration, abnormal perception or attentioncontrol or, most importantly, cognitive decline. In fact, MMN deficiency appears to index cognitivedecline irrespective of the specific symptomatologies and aetiologies of the different disorders involved.� 2011 International Federation of Clinical Neurophysiology. Published by Elsevier Ireland Ltd. All rights

reserved.

Contents

1. Introduction . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 4252. The MMN as an index of decreased auditory discrimination accuracy (Table 1). . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 4313. The MMN as an index of shortened sensory-memory duration (Table 2) . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 433

f Clinical Neurophysiology. Published by Elsevier Ireland Ltd. All rights reserved.

rch Unit, Cognitive Science,i, Helsinki, Finland. Tel.: +358

änen).

Page 2: The mismatch negativity (MMN) – A unique window to ... et al (2011... · Invited Review The mismatch negativity (MMN) – A unique window to disturbed central auditory processing

Felm

R. Näätänen et al. / Clinical Neurophysiology 123 (2012) 424–458 425

4. The MMN as an index of abnormal auditory perception . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 4345. The MMN as an index of increased backward masking (Table 3) . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 4346. The MMN as an index of abnormal involuntary attention switching: either too weak (Table 4) or too strong (Table 5). . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 4347. The MMN attenuation caused by cerebral grey-matter loss or other structural change (Table 6) . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 4358. The MMN as an index of pathological brain excitation/excitability (Table 7) . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 4379. The MMN as an index of cognitive and functional deterioration (Table 8) . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 43710. The MMN as an index of the level of consciousness (Table 9) . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 43811. The MMN as an index of the progression/severity of the illness (Table 10) . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 43812. The MMN in predicting illness course/drug response (prognosis) (Table 11) . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 43913. The MMN as an index of genetic disposition to different pathologies (Table 12) . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 44014. The MMN as an index of central auditory processing improvement or recovery (Table 13) . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 44115. Concluding discussion . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 443

Acknowledgements . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 444References . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 444

1. Introduction

Interestingly, central auditory processing is affected in a largenumber of different clinical conditions with very different aetiolo-gies and symptoms such as schizophrenia, dyslexia, stroke, specificlanguage impairment (SLI), multiple sclerosis (MS), amyotrophiclateral sclerosis (ALS), epilepsy and autism, suggesting someshared aetiology in these different abnormalities (see also Gottes-man and Gould, 2003; Bishop, 2009; Dolan et al., 1993; Hugdahland Calhoun, 2010; Reite et al., 2009). These effects on centralauditory processing in different clinical conditions and in ageingcan now be objectively evaluated, and hence their degree of simi-larity determined, by using the electrophysiological change-detec-

ig. 1. Grand average difference waveforms (9 subjects) for changes in duration, frequenectrode Fz and referenced to the mean of the two mastoid electrodes (upper panel). Theastoid (RM); lower panel. Sound onset is always at 0 ms (From Pakarinen et al., 2007)

tion (or regularity-violation) response, the mismatch negativity(MMN; Näätänen et al., 1978; Näätänen and Michie, 1979) andits magnetoencephalographic (MEG) equivalent, the MMNm (Hariet al., 1984; Sams et al., 1985b). The MMN can be reliably (Pekko-nen et al., 1995a,b; Escera and Grau, 1996; Tervaniemi et al., 1999;Escera et al., 2000b) recorded even in the absence of the subject orpatient’s attention or behavioural task, e.g., in sleeping infants(Ruusuvirta et al., 2009), stroke patients even at a very early poststroke-onset period (Ilvonen et al., 2001, 2004) and in comatose(Kane et al., 1993, 1996; Fischer et al., 1999; Fischer and Luauté,2005) and persistent-vegetative-state (PVS) patients (Wijnenet al., 2007).

cy, and perceived sound-source location for six different magnitudes of deviance atsame waveforms referenced to the nose electrode and shown from Fz and the right

.

Page 3: The mismatch negativity (MMN) – A unique window to ... et al (2011... · Invited Review The mismatch negativity (MMN) – A unique window to disturbed central auditory processing

Fig. 2. The differential effects of ethanol on MMN at the frontal (Fz) and left andright mastoid (LM and RM, respectively) electrode positions. MMN was reduced atthe frontal electrode but the polarity-inverted MMN recorded at the mastoids,suggesting the supratemporal MMN subcomponent, was not affected by ethanol(0.55 g/kg) whereas the frontal subcomponent was affected by ethanol. Adaptedfrom Jääskeläinen et al. (1996b).

426 R. Näätänen et al. / Clinical Neurophysiology 123 (2012) 424–458

The auditory MMN response is mainly generated by a change-detection process occurring bilaterally in auditory cortices, inwhich the current auditory input is found to differ from the repre-sentation of the preceding auditory events, including regularitiesgoverning consecutive stimulus events (for reviews, see Näätänenet al., 2001, 2007, 2010, 2011b; Duncan et al., 2009; Winkler,2007). For an illustration, see Fig. 1. In subjects performing someprimary, e.g., visual, task, this preconscious auditory-cortexchange-detection process reaches conscious perception by activat-ing, with a brief delay (Rinne et al., 2000; see also Rinne et al., 2005,2006), a right frontal-cortex process which, in turn, initiates furthercerebral processes which may lead to conscious change detection(Näätänen, 1990, 1992; Näätänen et al., 2011b; Alain et al., 1998;Berti and Schröger, 2001; Deouell et al., 2007; Giard et al., 1990;Schröger, 1996; Escera et al., 2000a). Hence, the MMN is composedof overlapping contributions from auditory- and frontal-cortex pro-cesses (the supratemporal and frontal MMN subcomponents,respectively) (Giard et al., 1990). These two subcomponents of theMMN can be inferred from the data presented in Fig. 2 showinghow ethanol selectively attenuates the MMN amplitude recordedover the frontal cortex, whereas the polarity-reversed MMN re-corded at the mastoids (with a nose reference) is unaffected. Thisdata pattern suggests that the frontally recorded MMN is composedof contributions from both the auditory and frontal cortices,whereas the mastoid ‘MMN’ gets a contribution from the audi-tory-cortex MMN generator only. Intracranial recordings in humansalso support both auditory- and frontal-cortex MMN generators(Halgren et al., 1995a,b, 1998; Baudena et al., 1995; Kropotovet al., 1995, 2000; Liasis et al., 1999, 2000a,b; Rosburg et al., 2005,2007).

The MMNm, in turn, selectively reflects the temporal-lobe com-ponent of the MMN because the MEG is insensitive to the radiallyoriented frontal generators (Hämäläinen et al., 1993) of the MMN.Therefore, the MMNm is particularly well suited for detecting cen-tral auditory processing deficits specific for the temporal lobes.Consequently, the combined use of the electroencephalography(EEG) and MEG recordings helps one to separately determine towhat extent the temporal and frontal MMN components are im-paired (Hämäläinen et al., 1993; Näätänen, 1992). Further, thesetwo components have their functional magnetic resonance imag-ing (fMRI) (Molholm et al., 2005; Celsis et al., 1999; Opitz et al.,2002; Schall et al., 2003), positron emission tomography (PET)(Tervaniemi et al., 2000; Dittmann-Balcar et al., 2001; Mülleret al., 2002) and optical-imaging (OI) (Rinne et al., 1999; Tse andPenney, 2008) equivalents as well. Moreover, there is event-relatedpotential (ERP; Paavilainen et al., 1991; Doeller et al., 2003; Frodl-Bauch et al., 1997; Escera et al., 2002), MEG (Levänen et al., 1993,1996; Rosburg, 2003) and fMRI evidence (Molholm et al., 2005) forthe attribute-specific organisation of the supratemporal MMNgenerator.

Moreover, the MMN has its equivalents in other sensory modal-ities, too [the visual MMN; vMMN (Alho et al., 1992; Amenedoet al., 2007; Astikainen and Hietanen, 2009; Tales et al., 1999,2002a,b, 2008; Tales and Butler, 2006; Maekawa et al., 2005,2009; Berti and Schröger, 2004, 2006; Stagg et al., 2004; Stefanicset al., 2011; Heslenfeld, 2003; Pazo-Alvarez et al., 2003, 2004; Fro-yen et al., 2010; Iijima et al., 1996; Kenemans et al., 2003; Kimuraet al., 2009; Czigler, 2007; Czigler and Csibra, 1990, 1992; Czigleret al., 2002, 2004, 2006a,b; Czigler and Pató, 2009; Nordby et al.,1996; Wei et al., 2002; Woods et al., 1992); the somatosensoryMMN; sMMN (Kekoni et al., 1997; Shinozaki et al., 1998; Akatsukaet al., 2005; Kida et al., 2001; Spackman et al., 2007, 2010; Astikai-nen et al., 2001; Näätänen, 2009); and the olfactory MMN; oMMN(Krauel et al., 1999; Pause and Krauel, 2000)]. The vMMN has aparieto-occipital scalp distribution but there is no convincing evi-dence for a frontal component analogous to that in the auditory

modality. By contrast, the sMMN appears to have, similar to theauditory MMN, sensory-specific (somatosensory cortex) and fron-tal subcomponents (Restuccia et al., 2009; Spackman et al.,2010), consistent with its fronto-central, contralaterally predomi-nant scalp distribution (Wei et al., 2002). Currently, it is not clearwhether the auditory and somatosensory modalities share thefrontal activation (generating the frontal MMN component) proba-bly serving attention switch to stimulus change (Näätänen, 1992;Näätänen et al., 2002; Schröger, 1997). In addition, there is alsoan oMMN, with a long peak latency (about 500–600 ms) and parie-tally predominant scalp distribution (Krauel et al., 1999).

The MMN can also be recorded in different animals [in monkey(Javitt et al., 1992); cat (Csépe et al., 1987, 1988, 1989; Pincze et al.,2001, 2002); rabbit (Astikainen et al., 2001); rat (Astikainen et al.,2006; Ruusuvirta et al., 1998, 2007; Roger et al., 2009; Tikhonravovet al., 2008, 2010); guinea pig (Kraus et al., 1994); and mouse(Umbricht et al., 2005; Ehrlichman et al., 2008, 2009)].

The MMN enables one to determine discrimination accuracy,usually with a good correspondence with behavioural discrimina-tion (Sams et al., 1985a; Amenedo and Escera, 2000; Gottseliget al., 2004; Lang et al., 1990; Näätänen et al., 1993, 2007; Leitmanet al., 2010; Kujala and Näätänen, 2010; Tremblay et al., 1998), sep-arately for each auditory dimension (such as frequency, intensityand duration) as well as for the different speech sounds (Krauset al., 1996), with the MMN vanishing at about the behavioural dis-crimination threshold (Sams et al., 1985a; Winkler and Näätänen,1994; Winkler et al., 1993). Therefore, it permits one to form objec-tive deterioration profiles covering all important auditory dimen-sions in different patient groups (Näätänen et al., 2004; Kujalaet al., 2005a, 2006, 2007, 2010; Pakarinen et al., 2007, 2009, 2010).In addition, these MMN-based objective tests can be extended toall aspects of short-term auditory sensory memory, too, such as its

Page 4: The mismatch negativity (MMN) – A unique window to ... et al (2011... · Invited Review The mismatch negativity (MMN) – A unique window to disturbed central auditory processing

Table 1The auditory MMN (MMNm) provides an objective index of affected auditory discrimination in:

– ADHD (Alexandrov et al., 2003; Barry et al., 2003; Gumenyuk et al., 2005; Huttunen et al., 2007; Huttunen-Scott et al., 2008; Kemner et al., 1996; Kilpeläinen et al., 1999; Oades et al., 1996; Rothenberger, 1995;Rothenberger et al., 2000; Satterfield et al., 1988; Sawada et al., 2010; Wild-Wall et al., 2005; Winsberg et al., 1993)

– Ageing (Alain and Woods, 1999; Alain et al., 2004; Amenedo and Diaz, 1998; Bellis et al., 2000; Bertoli et al., 2005; Cooper et al., 2006; Czigler et al., 1992; Fabiani et al., 2006; Gaeta et al., 1998, 1999, 2001, 2002; Gunteret al., 1996; Horváth et al., 2007; Iijima et al., 1996; Ikeda et al., 2004; Jääskeläinen et al., 1999a; Karayanidis et al., 1995; Kiang et al., 2007, 2009; Kisley et al., 2004a,b, 2005; Kok, 2000; Mager et al., 2005; Osawa et al.,1996; Pekkonen, 2000; Pekkonen et al., 1995a, 1996a, 2005; Schiff et al., 2008; Schroeder et al., 1995; Verleger et al., 1991; Woods, 1992; Woods and Clayworth, 1986)

– Alcohol intoxication, acute (Ahveninen et al., 2000a; Jääskeläinen et al., 1995, 1996a,b, 1998, 1999a,b; Kähkönen et al., 2001, 2004)Alcoholism (Ahveninen et al., 2000a,b; Fein et al., 2004; Grau et al., 2001; Holguin et al., 1998; Kathmann et al., 1995; Marco-Pallares et al., 2007; Pekkonen et al., 1998; Polo et al., 2003; Realmuto et al., 1993; Rodriguez et al.,

1998; van der Stelt and Belger, 2007; Zhang et al., 2001)

– Alzheimer’s disease and dementia (Engeland et al., 2002; Gaeta et al., 1999; Katada et al., 2004; Kazmerski et al., 1997; Missioner et al., 1999; Pekkonen, 2000; Pekkonen et al., 1994, 1996b, 1999, 2001a,b; Riekkinen et al.,1997; Schroeder et al., 1995; Yokoyama et al., 1995)

– Amusia(Congenital) (Moreau et al., 2009; Peretz et al., 2009)

– Amusia (Caused by Stroke) (Kohlmetz et al., 2001; Särkämö et al., 2010b)Amyotrophic Lateral Schlerosis (ALS) (Gil et al., 1993; Hanagasi et al., 2002; Pekkonen et al., 2004; Raggi et al., 2008)

Anaesthesis and sedation (Csepe et al., 1989; Heinke and Koelsch, 2005; Heinke et al., 2004; Koelsch et al., 2006; Astikainen et al., 2006; Yppärilä et al., 2002; Simpson et al., 2002; van Hooff et al., 1995, 1997)

– Anhedonia (Giese-Davis et al., 1993)– Aphasia and stroke (Aaltonen et al., 1993; Alain et al., 1998; Auther et al., 2000; Becker and Reinvang, 2007; Csépe et al., 2001; Ilvonen et al., 2003, 2004; Jacobs and Schneider, 2003; Kohlmetz et al., 2001; Peach et al., 1992,

1993, 1994; Pettigrew et al., 2004a, 2005; Särkämö et al. 2010a,b; Wertz et al., 1998)

– Asperger syndrome (Jansson-Verkasalo et al., 2003a, 2005; Korpilahti, 1995; Korpilahti et al., 2007; Kujala et al., 2005a, 2007, 2010; Lepistö et al., 2006, 2007)

– Autism (Bomba and Pang, 2004; Ceponiene et al., 2003; Dunn et al., 2008; Ferri et al., 2003; Gomot et al., 2002; Kasai et al., 2005; Kemner et al., 1995; Kuhl et al., 2005; Lepistö et al., 2005, 2008; Näätänen and Kujala, 2011;Oram Cardy et al., 2005a,b; Roberts et al., 2008, 2011; Seri et al., 1999, 2007; Siegal and Blades, 2003)

– Bipolar disorder (Hall et al., 2007, 2009; Takei et al., 2010)

– Bipolar II disorder (Andersson et al., 2008)– Blindness (early) (Alho et al., 1993; Kujala et al., 1995a,b, 1997, 2000b)– Brain lesions (Alain et al., 1998; Alho et al., 1994; Auther et al., 2000; Deouell et al., 2000a,b; Hämäläinen et al., 2007; Jacobs and Schneider, 2003; Kaipio et al., 1999, 2000, 2001; Kohlmetz et al., 2001; Kotchoubey, 2007;

Kotchoubey et al. 2003, 2005; Mäkelä et al., 1998b; Neumann and Kotchoubey, 2004; Polo et al., 2002; Reinvang et al., 2000; Tarkka et al., 2011; Woods and Knight, 1986); (see also Aphasia and Stroke)

– Brainstem auditory processing syndrome (Kraus et al., 1993a)

– CATCH-22 syndrome (Baker et al., 2005; Cheour et al., 1998a; Ceponiene et al., 2002)

– Central auditory processing disorder (CAPD) (Bamiou et al., 2000)

– Cerebellar degeneration (Moberget et al., 2008)

– Childhood cancer and brain radiation (Järvelä et al., 2011; Lähteenmäki, 1999)

– Chronic pain (Dick et al., 2003)

– Chronic primary insomnia (Wang et al., 2001)

– Cleft palate (different forms of) (Ceponiene et al., 2000, 2002; Cheour et al., 1998a, 1999)

– Closed-head injury (Kaipio et al., 1999, 2000, 2001; Polo et al., 2002)

– Cochlear-implant (CI) users (Groenen et al., 1996; Kelly et al., 2005; Kileny et al., 1997; Koelsch et al., 2004; Kraus et al., 1993b; Lonka et al., 2004; Nager et al., 2007; Ponton et al., 1996, 2000, 2009; Ponton and Don, 1995,2003; Ponton and Eggermont, 2007; Roman et al., 2005; Salo et al., 2002; Sandmann et al., 2010; Wable et al., 2000)

– Coma (Daltrozzo et al., 2007; Fischer et al., 1999, 2000, 2004, 2006a,b, 2008; Fischer and Luauté, 2005; Guérit et al., 1999; Kane et al., 1993, 1996, 2000; Kotchoubey et al., 2003; Laureys et al., 2005; Luauté et al., 2005;Morlet et al., 2000; Naccache et al., 2005; van der Stelt and van Boxtel, 2008; Vanhaudenhuyse et al., 2008)

– Conduct disorder (Rothenberger et al., 2000)

– Craniosynosthosis (Huotilainen et al., 2008)

– Deafness to musical dissonance ( Acquired) (Brattico et al., 2003)

– Dementia (Schroeder et al., 1995; Yokoyama et al., 1995; Osawa et al., 1996)

– Depression (He et al., 2010; Iv et al., 2010; Kähkönen et al., 2007; Lepistö et al., 2004; Ogura et al., 1991, 1993; Takei et al., 2009)

(continued on next page)

R.N

äätänenet

al./ClinicalN

europhysiology123

(2012)424–

458427

Page 5: The mismatch negativity (MMN) – A unique window to ... et al (2011... · Invited Review The mismatch negativity (MMN) – A unique window to disturbed central auditory processing

Table 1 (continued)

– Developmental dysphasia (Holopainen et al., 1997, 1998; Korpilahti and Lang, 1994)– Diabetes mellitus (late phase) (Vanhanen et al., 1996)

– Distractibility (Escera et al., 2000a; Gumenyuk et al., 2004; Kaipio et al., 2000, 2001; Kilpeläinen et al., 1999; Mager et al., 2005)

– Down’s syndrome (Diaz and Zurron, 1995; Lalo et al., 2005)

– Drugs

–Antipsychotics/Neuroleptics:

– Clozapine (Horton et al., 2010; Schall et al., 1999; Umbricht et al., 1998)

– Haloperidol (Kähkönen and Ahveninen, 2002; Kähkönen et al., 2001, 2002; Pekkonen et al., 2002)– Olanzapine (Korostenskaja et al., 2005)– Risperidone (Umbricht et al., 1999)

– Alcohol:– (Hirvonen et al., 2000; Jääskeläinen et al., 1995, 1996a,b, 1998, 1999a; Kähkönen et al., 2001,2004,2005b; Kenemans et al., 2010)

– Acetylcholine:Tetrahydroaminoacridine (THA, Tacrine) (Engeland et al., 2002; Riekkinen et al., 1997)– (a) Muscarinic receptor antagonist:

– Glycopyrrolate (Pekkonen et al., 2001a)– Scopolamine (Pekkonen et al., 2001a)

– (b) Nicotinic receptor agonists:– Nicotine: (Baldeweg et al., 2006; Dulude et al., 2010; Engeland et al., 2002; Fisher et al., 2010; Harkrider and Hedrick, 2005; Inami et al., 2005, 2007; Knott et al., 2009; Martin et al., 2009; Pritchard et al., 2004)– AZD3480 (Dunbar et al., 2007)

– Benzodiazepines:– (Kivisaari et al., 2007; Murakami et al., 2002)– Triatzolam (Nakagome et al., 1998)– Lorazepam (Rosburg et al., 2004)

– GABA agonists/antagonists:– Bicuculline (Javitt et al., 1996)– Flumazenil (Smolnik et al., 1998)– Propofol (Heinke et al., 2004; Koelsch et al., 2006; Simpson et al., 2002)

– Monoamines– Review: (Kähkönen and Ahveninen, 2002)

– Adrenergic drugs:– Clonidine (Duncan and Kaye, 1987; Hansenne et al., 2003)– Atipamezole (Mervaala et al., 1993)– Noradrenergic agonist S 12024-4 (Missioner et al., 1999)

– Histamine:– Antihistamines: Chlorpheniramine (Serra et al., 1996)

– Dopamine:– Apomorphine (Hansenne et al., 2003)– Methamphetamine (Hosák et al., 2008)– Methylphenidate (Winsberg et al., 1993, 1997; Sawada et al., 2010; Verbaten et al., 1994)– Bromocriptine (Leung et al., 2007)– Domperidone (Leung et al., 2007)

– Serotonin:– Serotonin (Kähkönen et al., 2005a; Oranje et al., 2008)– Acute tryptophan depletion (Kähkönen and Ahveninen, 2002)– Dimethyltryptamine (Heekeren et al., 2008)– Escitalopram (Oranje et al., 2008; Wienberg et al., 2010)– Psilocybin (Umbricht et al., 2003)

– Neuropeptides/Hormones:– Vasopressin (Born et al., 1986, 1987a, 1998)– Oxytocin (Born et al., 1987a)– Cholecystokinin (Schreiber et al., 1995)– Desacetyl-alpha-MSH (Smolnik et al., 1999)

428R

.Näätänen

etal./Clinical

Neurophysiology

123(2012)

424–458

Page 6: The mismatch negativity (MMN) – A unique window to ... et al (2011... · Invited Review The mismatch negativity (MMN) – A unique window to disturbed central auditory processing

– Hydrocortisone (Born et al., 1987a)– ACTH-related neuropeptides (Born et al., 1987b; Smolnik et al., 1999; Pietrowsky et al., 1990)

– NMDA/Glutamate:– Ketamine (Ehrlichman et al., 2008; Heekeren et al., 2008; Javitt et al., 2000; Kreitschmann-Andermahr et al., 2001; Umbricht et al., 2002)– MK-801 (Javitt et al., 1996; Tikhonravov et al., 2010)– Phencyclidine (PCP) (Javitt et al., 1996)– Memantine (Korostenskaja et al., 2007)– Nitrous oxide (Pang and Fowler, 1999)– Glycine (Leung et al., 2008)– N-acetyl-cysteine (NAC; Lavoie et al., 2007)

– Opiods:– Naltrexone (Jääskeläinen et al., 1998)– Opioid Dependence (Kivisaari et al., 2007; Näätänen, 2001)

– Other/alternative therapies:– Hiba odour (Hiruma et al., 2002)

– Purine nucleotides: – Adenosine (Hirvonen et al., 2000)– Caffeine (Rosburg et al., 2004)

– Dyslexia (Ahissar, 2007; Ahissar et al., 2006; Alonso-Búa et al., 2006; Baldeweg et al., 1999; Bishop, 2007a,b; Blitz et al., 2007; Bonte et al., 2005, 2007; Bradlow et al., 1999; Bruder et al., 2011a,b; Corbera et al., 2006; Csépe,2002; Czamara et al., 2011; Fisher et al., 2006; Froyen et al., 2008, 2009, 2010; Guttorm et al., 2005, 2001; Hämäläinen et al., 2008; Heim et al., 2000; Huttunen et al., 2007; Huttunen-Scott et al., 2008; Jansson-Verkasaloet al., 2010; Kujala, 2007; Kujala and Näätänen, 2001; Kujala et al., 2000a, 2001, 2003, 2006; Kurtzberg et al., 1995; Lachmann et al., 2005; Leppänen and Lyytinen, 1997; Leppänen et al., 1997, 1999, 2002, 2004, 2010;Lovio et al., 2010; Lyytinen et al., 2001, 2004, 2006, 2009; Maurer et al., 2003, 2009; Paul et al., 2006a,b; Pihko et al., 1999, 2008; Renvall and Hari, 2003; Richardson et al., 2003; Roeske et al., 2011; Schulte-Körne andBruder, 2010; Schulte-Körne et al., 1998, 1999, 2001; Sebastian and Yasin, 2008; Shankarnarayan and Maruthy, 2007; Sharma et al., 2005, 2006, 2007, 2009; Stoodley et al., 2006; van Leeuwen et al., 2006)

– Dysthymia (Giese-Davis et al., 1993)– Electromagnetic fields (Kwon et al., 2009, 2010)

– Epilepsy (Boatman et al., 2008; Borghetti et al., 2007; Duman et al., 2008; Gene-Cos et al., 2005; Korostenskaja et al., 2010b; Liasis et al., 2000; Liasis et al., 2001; Liasis et al., 2006; Lin et al., 2007; Metz-Lutz and Philippini,2006; Miyajima et al., 2011; Rosburg et al., 2005)

– Frontal-lobe injury (Alho et al., 1994; Woods and Knight, 1986)– Herpes Simplex Encephalitis (Mäkelä et al., 1998a)– Hippocampus-Amygdala Partial Temporal-Lobe Resection (Hämäläinen et al., 2007)– HIV (Schroeder et al., 1994)– Huntington’s disease (pathological enhancement of discrimination) (Beste et al., 2008)– Hyperkinetic disorder (Rothenberger, 1995)– Hypnosis (Jamieson et al., 2005; Kallio et al., 1999)– Insomnia (Wang et al., 2001)– Intellectual disability (Ikeda et al., 2004; Nakagawa et al., 2002; Kaga et al., 1999)– Introvert/extrovert personality (Sasaki et al., 2000)– Landau–Kleffner syndrome (Honbolygó et al., 2006; Metz-Lutz and Philippini, 2006)– Language-learning impairment (LLI) (Benasich and Tallal, 2002; Benasich et al., 2006; Bradlow et al., 1999; Choudhury and Benasich, 2011; Kraus et al., 1996; Kujala, 2007; Marler et al., 2002; Pihko et al., 2007; Shaferet al., 2005; Uwer et al., 2002; Weber et al., 2005)

– Late talkers (Grossheinrich et al., 2010)– Learning-disabled children (Banai and Ahissar, 2005; Bradlow et al., 1999; Kraus et al., 1996)– Left sylvian infarct in neonate (Dehaene-Lambertz et al., 2004)– Locked-in patients (Ragazzoni et al., 2000)– Meditation (Srinivasan and Baijal, 2007)– Mental work load (Kramer et al., 1995)– Mental retardation (Holopainen et al., 1998; Ikeda et al., 2004; Kaga et al., 1999; Nakagawa et al., 2002; Yokoyama et al., 1995; Zurrón and Díaz, 1997)– Migraine (de Tommaso et al., 2004; Korostenskaja et al., 2010a; Valeriani et al., 2008)– Multiple schlerosis (MS) (Gil et al., 1993; Jung et al., 2006; Santos et al., 2006)– Narcolepsy (Naumann et al., 2001)– Neglect (Hemifield inattention) and extinction (Deouell et al., 2000a,b; Tarkka et al., 2011)– Noise and distraction (Bertoli et al., 2005; Gumenyuk et al., 2004; Mikkola et al., 2010; Shtyrov et al., 1998, 1999; Simoens et al., 2007)– Noise, different types of (Kozou et al., 2005)– Noise, occupational (Mäntysalo and Salmi, 1989)– Noise, occupational, long-term exposure (Brattico et al., 2005; Kujala and Brattico, 2009; Kujala et al., 2004)– Obsessive–compulsive disorder (Towey et al., 1994; Oades et al., 1996)– Obstructive sleep apnea syndrome (OSAS) (Gosselin et al., 2006)– Opioid dependence (Kivisaari et al., 2007)– Parkinson’s disease (Brønnick et al., 2010; Karayanidis et al., 1995; Pekkonen et al., 1995c; Vieregger et al., 1994)– Perinatal asphyxia (Leipälä et al., 2011)

(continued on next page)

R.N

äätänenet

al./ClinicalN

europhysiology123

(2012)424–

458429

Page 7: The mismatch negativity (MMN) – A unique window to ... et al (2011... · Invited Review The mismatch negativity (MMN) – A unique window to disturbed central auditory processing

Table 1 (continued)

– Perinatal intracerebral hemorrhage (Leipälä et al., 2011)– Persistent vegetative state (Boly et al., 2011; Fischer and Luauté, 2005; Kotchoubey, 2007; Kotchoubey et al., 2003, 2007, 2008; Laureys et al., 2005; Wijnen et al., 2007; Zarza-Lucianez et al., 2007)– Personality differences (Matsubayashi et al., 2008; Franken et al., 2005)– Phonological deficit (Blitz et al., 2007)– Plagiocephaly (Huotilainen et al., 2008)– Post-traumatic stress syndrome (PTSS) (Cornwell et al., 2007; Ge et al., 2011; Menning et al., 2008; Morgan and Grillon, 1999)– Psychosocial stress (Simoens et al., 2007)– Prematurely born (with very low birth weight, VLBW) infants (Bisiachi et al., 2009; Cheour-Luhtanen et al., 1996; Fellman and Huotilainen, 2006; Fellman et al., 2004; Gomot et al., 2007; Holst et al., 2005; Jansson-Verkasaloet al., 2004, 2010; Leipälä et al., 2011; Mento et al., 2010; Mikkola et al., 2007, 2010)

– REM-sleep deprivation (Zerouali et al., 2010)– Schizophrenia (Ahveninen et al., 2006; Baldeweg et al., 2002, 2004; Banati and Hickie, 2009; Bodatsch et al., 2011; Braff and Light, 2004; Brockhaus-Dumke et al., 2005; Catts et al., 1995; Davalos et al., 2003, 2005;Devrim-Ücok et al., 2008; Ehrlichman et al., 2008, 2009; Fisher et al., 2008, 2011; Grzella et al., 2001; Hermens et al., 2010; Hirayasu et al., 1998, 2000, 2001; Inami et al., 2007; Jahshan et al., 2011; Javitt, 1993, 2000,2009a,b; Javitt et al., 1996, 1998, 1999, 2000, 2008; Jessen et al., 2001; Kasai et al., 2001, 2002a,b,c, 2003a,b; Kathmann et al., 1995; Kaur et al., 2011; Kawakubo et al., 2006, 2007; Kiang et al., 2007, 2009; Kircher et al.,2004; Korostenskaja et al., 2005; Kreitschmann-Andermahr et al., 1999, 2001; Lavoie et al., 2007; Leitman et al., 2010; Light and Braff, 2005a,b; Luck et al., 2011; Magno et al., 2008; Matthews et al., 2007; Michie, 2001;Michie et al., 2002; Minami and Kirino, 2005; Morlet et al., 2000; Näätänen and Kähkönen, 2009; Niznikiewicz et al., 2009; Oades et al., 2006; Oknina et al., 2005; Park et al., 2002; Potts et al., 1998; Price et al., 2006;Rasser et al., 2011; Rissling et al., 2010; Salisbury et al., 2002, 2007; Sato et al., 1998, 2002; Schall et al., 1998, 1999, 2003; Schreiber et al., 1992; Shelley et al., 1991; Shin et al., 2009; Shinozaki et al., 2002; Stone et al.,2010; Thönnesen et al., 2008; Todd et al., 2008; Todd et al., in press; Todd et al., 2003, 2008; Turetsky et al., 2007, 2009; Umbricht et al., 1998, 1999, 2000, 2002, 2003, 2006; van der Stelt and Belger, 2007; Verbaten andvan Engeland, 1995; Wible et al., 2001; Wynn et al., 2010; Yamasue et al., 2004; Youn et al., 2003; for a meta-analysis of 32 studies, see Umbricht and Krljes, 2005)

– Serotonin transporter gene variants, individuals with (Sysoeva et al., 2009)– Sensorineural hearing loss (Oates et al., 2002; Stapells, 2002)– Sleep (Atienza et al., 1997, 2000, 2002; Atienza and Cantero, 2001; Campbell et al., 1991; Cheour et al. 2000,2002; Martynova et al., 2003; Nashida et al., 2000; Nittono et al., 2001; Ruby et al., 2008; Sallinen et al., 1994,1996; Sculthorpe et al., 2009)

– Sleep deprivation (Gumenyuk et al., 2010; Raz et al., 2001; Sallinen and Lyytinen, 1997; Salmi et al., 2005; Wang et al., 2001; Zerouali et al., 2010)– Socially withdrawn children (Bar-Haim et al., 2003)– Somatisation disorder (James et al., 1989)– Specific language impairment (SLI) (Barry et al., 2008; Bishop and McArthur, 2004; Friedrich et al., 2004; Korpilahti, 1995; Marler et al., 2002; Pihko et al., 2008; Rinker et al., 2007; Shafer et al., 2007; Uwer and vonSuchodoletz, 2000; Uwer et al., 2002; Weber et al., 2005)

– Stroke (see Aphasia)– Stuttering (Corbera et al., 2005; Wu et al., 1997)– Temporal-parietal brain lesions (Alain et al., 1998)– Thalamic infarction (Mäkelä et al., 1998b)– Tourette syndrome (TS) (Oades et al., 1996; Rothenberger et al., 2000; van Woerkom et al., 1988, 1994)– Tune deafness (Braun et al., 2008)– Velo-cardio-facial (DiGeorge) syndrome (Baker et al., 2005)

430R

.Näätänen

etal./Clinical

Neurophysiology

123(2012)

424–458

Page 8: The mismatch negativity (MMN) – A unique window to ... et al (2011... · Invited Review The mismatch negativity (MMN) – A unique window to disturbed central auditory processing

Table 2THE MMN shows abnormally short auditory sensory-memory duration in:

– Alzheimer’s disease (Pekkonen et al., 1994)– Infants and children with cleft palate and CATCH-22 syndrome (Ceponiene et al., 2002; Cheour et al., 1997, 1998a, 1999)– Late talkers (Grossheinrich et al., 2010)– Chronic alcoholism (Grau et al., 2001; Marco-Pallares et al., 2007; Polo et al., 1999)– Normal ageing (Cooper et al., 2006; Grau et al., 2001; Jääskeläinen et al., 1999a; Pekkonen et al., 1996a)– Parents of children with specific language impairment (SLI) (Barry et al., 2008)

Table 3The MMN provides an index of increased backward masking (affecting speechperception) in:

– Chronic alcoholism (Ahveninen et al., 1999)– Dyslexia (Kujala et al., 2003)– Specific language impairment (SLI) (Marler et al., 2002)

3-5 days 10 days3 months

Lefthemisphere

Righthemisphere

Time

Am

plitu

de

MMNMMN

Fig. 3. MMN shows how frequency discrimination recovered to normal levelswithin 3 months from a stroke onset (averaged data of 8 patients). This MMNrecovery was slower for right-ear stimulation (with the main part of the auditoryinput going to the damaged left hemisphere) (curves on the left) than with left-earstimulation (curves on the right). During the MMN recording, the patient waswatching video-films. Adapted from Ilvonen et al. (2003).

R. Näätänen et al. / Clinical Neurophysiology 123 (2012) 424–458 431

duration, capacity and accuracy (Pekkonen et al., 1996a; Cooperet al., 2006; Polo et al., 1999; Grau et al., 2001). Furthermore, theauditory MMN can even index auditory long-term memory tracessuch as the language-specific memory traces, enabling one to cor-rectly perceive the speech sounds of the mother tongue and otherfamiliar languages (Dehaene-Lambertz, 1997; Näätänen, 2001;Näätänen et al., 1997; Pulvermuller et al., 2004; Shtyrov and Pulver-muller, 2007; Winkler et al., 1999c; Shtyrov and Pulvermüller,2007). This linguistic MMN subcomponent is usually left-hemi-spherically predominant and generated posteriorly to the bilateralMMN subcomponent for mere acoustic change (Näätänen et al.,1997; Shestakova et al., 2002).

The extensive MMN literature of clinical studies can be classi-fied according to the kind of clinically useful information thatcan be obtained by using the MMN. The MMN can index:

(1) Auditory discrimination accuracy (which is decreased in anumber of clinical groups and affected by different drugs)(Table 1);

(2) shortened sensory-memory duration (and hence possiblydecreased general brain plasticity) (Table 2);

(3) abnormal auditory perception;(4) increased backward masking (Table 3);(5) abnormal involuntary attention switching, either too weak

(Table 4) or too strong (Table 5);(6) cerebral grey-matter loss and other structural changes

(Table 6);(7) pathological brain excitation/excitability state (Table 7);(8) cognitive and functional decline (Table 8);(9) the level of consciousness (Table 9);

(10) the progression of illness (Table 10);(11) future clinical condition (prognosis) (Table 11);(12) genetic disposition to certain disorders (Table 12); and(13) recovery/improvement as a function of time or treatment

(Table 13).Moreover, recent studies using the vMMN (Iijima et al.,1996; Lorenzo-Lopez et al., 2004; Tales and Butler, 2006;Tales et al., 2002a,b; Kenemans et al., 2010; Tales and Butler,2006; Tales et al., 2002a, 2008; Maekawa et al., 2011; Froyenet al., 2010; Chang et al., 2010; Qiu et al., 2011; Horimotoet al., 2002; Hosák et al., 2008; Kremlácek et al., 2008, Ver-baten et al., 1994; Tales and Butler, 2006; Tales et al.,2008; Fisher et al., 2010; Urban et al., 2008; for a recentreview, see Kimura et al., 2011) and sMMN (Restucciaet al., 2007; Akatsuka et al., 2005, 2007; Näätänen, 2009)have also provided clinically important results.

The present review aims at covering all clinical MMN studies inthe auditory modality published in refereed international English-

language journals. Almost all these studies report, with only a fewexceptions, group-level results.

2. The MMN as an index of decreased auditory discriminationaccuracy (Table 1)

In several clinical conditions, the MMN amplitude is attenuated,usually indexing decreased behavioural discrimination accuracy(Javitt et al., 1998; Rabinowicz et al., 2000; Matthews et al.,2007). This amplitude reduction was usually found by recordingthe MMN to simple frequency or duration changes in sinusoidaltones, for instance, in schizophrenia (Todd et al., 2003; Javittet al., 2000; Michie et al., 2000; see also Javitt et al., 1998). Inschizophrenia, the duration MMN, in particular that to durationincrement, was even more affected than that to frequency change(Michie, 2001; Michie et al., 2000; for a meta-analysis, see Umb-richt and Krljes, 2005). The frequency MMN is attenuated in ampli-tude in several other clinical groups, too, such as children withdevelopmental dysphasia (Holopainen et al., 1997, 1998; Kor-pilahti and Lang, 1994), dyslexia (Baldeweg et al., 1999; Renvalland Hari, 2003; Kujala et al., 2003, 2006; Sebastian and Yasin,2008) and SLI (Mengler et al., 2005; Rinker et al., 2007).

In children and adults with dyslexia, the MMN to frequencychange but not that to duration change was considerably attenu-ated in amplitude (Baldeweg et al., 1999; Kujala et al., 2006). Fur-thermore, in keeping with this profile of MMN-amplitudereduction, the behavioural frequency but not duration discrimina-tion was affected (Baldeweg et al., 1999). In addition, thisfrequency-MMN deficit strongly correlated with the severity ofthe reading problem. Subsequently, Lachmann et al. (2005)

Page 9: The mismatch negativity (MMN) – A unique window to ... et al (2011... · Invited Review The mismatch negativity (MMN) – A unique window to disturbed central auditory processing

Table 4The MMN shows decreased involuntary attention switching in:

– Alcohol intoxication (Jääskeläinen et al., 1995, 1996a,b, 1999b)– Schizophrenia (Baldeweg et al., 2002; Catts et al., 1995; Matthews et al., 2007; Oades et al., 2006; Sato et al., 2003)– Closed-head injury (Polo et al., 2002)– Frontal-lesion patients (Alho et al., 1994)– Right-hemisphere damage (unilateral neglect) (Deouell et al., 2000a,b)– Children with major depression (Lepistö et al., 2004)

432 R. Näätänen et al. / Clinical Neurophysiology 123 (2012) 424–458

showed that the MMN-attenuation profile involving frequency andconsonant–vowel (CV) syllable changes could disentangle differentdiagnostic subgroups of developmental dyslexia from each other.Moreover, Shafer et al. (2005) and Uwer et al. (2002) found thatSLI children had attenuated MMN amplitudes for syllable change.

Using the new multi-feature MMN ‘optimum’ paradigm (Näätä-nen et al., 2004), permitting one to obtain five different MMNs in15 min, Lovio et al. (2010) found a widespread auditory discrimi-nation deficit in children at risk for dyslexia. Their MMNs for con-sonant, vowel, vowel duration and intensity changes of syllableswere diminished in amplitude. By contrast, Kujala et al. (2006),also using the multi-feature paradigm, found that in adult dyslex-ics, the frequency MMN was attenuated, whereas their locationMMN was in fact enhanced in amplitude. This shows that not allchanges in central auditory processing in dyslexia signify deterio-rated discrimination. However, the MMN data may also suggestthe presence of cognitive deficiencies associated with dyslexia, atleast deficient phonotactic processing (Bonte et al., 2007).

Furthermore, in children with autism, the MMN to durationchange (Lepistö et al., 2005) and the MMNm to speech-soundchange (Kasai et al., 2005; for recent corroborating results, see Rob-erts et al., 2011) were attenuated in amplitude. In addition, theMMN to CV-syllable change was prolonged in peak latency butonly in those children who preferred non-speech analogy signalsto speech (Kuhl et al., 2005). By contrast, the MMN to frequencychange was enhanced in amplitude (Lepistö et al., 2005; Ferriet al., 2003) and shortened in peak latency (Gomot et al., 2002)in autistic children, consistent with their better-than-average pitchdiscrimination and musical abilities (Bonnel et al., 2003; Heatonet al., 2001). Very recently, it was found by Roberts et al. (2011;

Fig. 4. Scatterplot of MMN amplitude at Fz versus age, for schizophrenia patients (operegression line) (From Kiang et al., 2009).

see also Näätänen and Kujala, 2011) that in autistic children of7–9 years, the MMNm peak latency for tone-frequency and vowelchanges was delayed, in comparison with that of normally devel-oping children of the same age, and, further, that this delay wasconsiderably increased if the child also suffered from a delay in lin-guistic development. This might contribute to their speech-under-standing difficulties and hence to their social isolation. In addition,in the Asperger syndrome, a condition belonging to the autismspectrum, the duration-increment MMN of children of about9 years of age was attenuated in amplitude over the left hemi-sphere, whereas it was enhanced over the right hemisphere(Lepistö et al., 2006). Moreover, in this syndrome, the MMN ampli-tude was also attenuated for changes in prosody (Kujala et al.,2005a). Recently, a shortened MMN peak latency for frequencychange and increased MMN amplitudes for duration and gapchanges were found in Asperger adults by Kujala et al. (2007).

In addition, the MMNs for frequency and duration changes wereattenuated in amplitude in patients with a left-hemispheric stroke(Csépe et al., 2001; Aaltonen et al., 1993; Auther et al., 2000; Ilvo-nen et al., 2001, 2003; Pettigrew et al., 2005). These two MMNsshowed somewhat different time courses of post-stroke recovery,however (Ilvonen et al., 2003) (Fig. 3). Importantly, the MMNrecovery indexed that of speech perception, there being a correla-tion between the duration-MMN amplitude and the Boston Diag-nostic Aphasia Examination Speech-Comprehension Test from10 days to 3 months post-stroke onset (Ilvonen et al., 2003). Subse-quently, Särkämö et al. (2010b) found that the MMNm recoveryand behavioural discrimination could be expedited with musicand speech stimulation. In addition, the MMNm recovery corre-

n circles; dashed regression line) and normal control subjects (solid circles; solid

Page 10: The mismatch negativity (MMN) – A unique window to ... et al (2011... · Invited Review The mismatch negativity (MMN) – A unique window to disturbed central auditory processing

Table 5The MMN shows increased involuntary attention switching in:

– Closed head injury (Kaipio et al., 2000)– Ageing (Gaeta et al., 2001)– Alcoholism (Ahveninen et al., 2000b; Polo et al., 2003)– Children with major depression (Lepistö et al., 2004)– Sleep disorder (Gumenyuk et al., 2010)

Younger Males (mean 22 yrs)

Older Males (mean 59 yrs)

0.5 s

4.5 s

MMN

Fz F3 F 4

Fig. 5. MMN to frequency change (700 ? 600 Hz) was normal in older males withan inter-stimulus interval (ISI) of 0.5 s but was considerably attenuated, relative tothat of younger males, with an ISI of 4.5 s, indexing faster auditory sensory-memorytrace decay with aging (From Pekkonen et al., 2001a).

R. Näätänen et al. / Clinical Neurophysiology 123 (2012) 424–458 433

lated with the recovery of behavioural speech-sounddiscrimination.

In epilepsy, the MMN is also affected. The MMN peak latencieswere abnormally long or no clear MMN response could be foundfor speech (but could for tones) in patients with benign childhoodepilepsy with contra-temporal spikes (Boatman et al., 2008; Du-man et al., 2008). Patients with such spikes with atypical featuresand learning difficulties also exhibit attenuated MMN amplitudes(Metz-Lutz and Philippini, 2006). In addition, in the Landau–Kleff-ner syndrome, an MMN was obtained for phoneme change but notfor prosodic change (Honbolygó et al., 2006). By contrast, Miyajimaet al. (2011), studying adult patients with temporal-lobe epilepsy,found that at fronto-central sites, their MMN amplitude for tone-frequency change was enhanced, interpreted by the authors asreflecting frontal-lobe hyperexcitability to compensate for tempo-ral-lobe dysfunction indexed, according to the authors, by pro-longed MMN peak latencies in patients relative to controls atboth fronto-central and mastoid sites, consistent with previous re-ports (Lin et al., 2007; Borghetti et al., 2007). Furthermore, in theMiyajima et al. (2011) temporal-lobe epilepsy patients, the fron-to-centrally recorded MMN persisted longer than that in controls,consistent with the results of Gene-Cos et al. (2005) who suggestedthat a prolonged MMN duration might point to difficulty in ‘theclosure mechanism of the MMN process’.

The MMN can also be used to evaluate the possible central-auditory-processing damage associated with a very prematurebirth. Studying very prematurely born children (mean gestationalage 29 weeks) with a very low birth weight (mean 1115 g), Jans-son-Verkasalo et al. (2003b, 2004) found that their MMN ampli-tude for consonant change at 4 years (from conception) wassmaller than that of controls and, further, that the absence of theMMN at 4–5 years of age predicted naming difficulties at 6 years.However, if the MMN amplitude nevertheless was normal at4 years, then there were no naming difficulties at 6 years.

Moreover, the MMN enables one to also determine central-auditory-processing damage caused by long-term exposure to loudoccupational noise. In workers exposed to such a noise for 2–17 years, the MMN to syllable change was attenuated in amplitude(even though their peripheral hearing was not affected), in keepingwith their increased speech-perception problems (Kujala et al.,2004). In addition, the Brattico et al. (2005) MMN data showed thatlong-term exposure to occupational noise altered the cortical orga-nisation of speech-sound processing in these workers (in the ab-sence of peripheral hearing loss) but did not alter that of non-speech processing. In addition, the discrimination of auditory stim-uli was generally slowed down, as indexed by prolonged MMN-peak latencies.

Furthermore, by using the MMN, one can also monitor age-re-lated changes in central auditory processing (Fabiani et al., 2006;Woods, 1992; Woods and Clayworth, 1986; Jääskeläinen et al.,1999a; Kiang et al., 2009; Cooper et al., 2006; Gaeta et al., 1998,2001, 2002; Kisley et al., 2005; Pekkonen, 2000; Pekkonen et al.,1995a, 1996a). A particularly convincing characterisation of theMMN–age relationship was provided by Kiang et al. (2009) whocarefully plotted the MMN and P3a amplitudes for tone-durationincrement across adulthood for 147 normal subjects and 253schizophrenic patients (Fig. 4). Along with an increasing age, the

level of performance in different cognitive tasks is gradually de-creased (Cansino, 2009). There is also evidence linking theseMMN changes to cognitive deterioration (Kisley et al., 2005). Evenin healthy elderly persons, the MMN to a short gap in the middle ofa tone (Desjardin et al., 1999) was attenuated in amplitude, index-ing their increased difficulties in correctly perceiving rapid speech-sound patterns such as consonants (Bertoli et al., 2002, 2005).

The MMN and MMNm are also affected by different drugs(Table 1).

3. The MMN as an index of shortened sensory-memory duration(Table 2)

The MMN elicitation depends on the presence of the sensory-memory trace representing the preceding stimuli and their regu-larities at the moment of the delivery of a deviant stimulus (for areview, see Näätänen et al., 2007). Hence, by gradually prolongingthe inter-stimulus interval (ISI), the MMN eventually vanishes,which enables one to assess sensory-memory duration in audition(a potential general index of brain plasticity; Näätänen and Kreegi-puu, 2011).

In young healthy adults, the trace duration, as estimated in thismanner, approximates 10 s (Böttcher-Gandor and Ullsperger,1992; Sams et al., 1993), but is gradually shortened with ageing.With short ISIs such as 0.5 s, the MMN amplitude for frequencychange was very similar in elderly (mean 59 years) and young(mean 25 years) male subjects, suggesting that the memory-traceformation for sounds and, thus, perception (see Näätänen and Win-kler, 1999) were not affected by ageing. By contrast, with a stimu-lus-onset asynchrony (SOA) of 4.5 s, the MMN amplitude of theelderly was much more attenuated than that of the young (Pekko-nen et al., 1996a) (Fig. 5), indicating that in ageing, the auditorysensory-memory duration is shortened. Moreover, correspondingMMN data for frequency change in chronic alcoholics suggestaccelerated age-related shortening of the memory-trace durationin these patients (Polo et al., 1999; see also Grau et al., 2001).

The memory-trace duration is particularly short in patientswith Alzheimer’s disease, of course. Their MMN was, however, nor-mal with short ISIs (0.5 s), suggesting normal memory-trace for-mation/perception (see Näätänen and Winkler, 1999) even inthese patients, but absent at long ISIs (3 s) (Pekkonen et al.,1994), indexing their extremely short sensory-memory duration.Interestingly, this data pattern is orthogonal to that observed in pa-tients with schizophrenia in whom no normal-amplitude MMN canbe obtained in any condition, not even with very short ISIs andwide frequency deviations (Javitt et al., 1998). This suggests thattheir memory-trace formation, and hence auditory perception

Page 11: The mismatch negativity (MMN) – A unique window to ... et al (2011... · Invited Review The mismatch negativity (MMN) – A unique window to disturbed central auditory processing

Table 6The MMN correlates with structural changes in:

– Schizophrenia (Hirayasu et al., 2000, 2001; Kasai et al., 2003a,b; Park et al.,2002; Rasser et al., 2011; Salisbury et al., 2002, 2007; Yamasue et al., 2004;Youn et al., 2003; for a review, see van der Stelt and Belger, 2007)

– Cleft palate (children with) (Ceponiene et al., 1999)

350-ms ISI700-ms ISI1400-ms ISI

-4 µV

+1 µV 350 ms

F4

Fig. 6. MMN for tone-frequency change was of normal amplitude in 7–9 year oldchildren with oral clefts when a short ISI (350 ms) was used but was considerablysmaller than that of controls when the ISI was prolonged to 700 ms and to 1400 ms,suggesting an accelerated sensory-memory trace in patients (From Ceponiene et al.,1999).

434 R. Näätänen et al. / Clinical Neurophysiology 123 (2012) 424–458

(Näätänen and Winkler, 1999), is abnormal, consistent with clini-cal observations (for a review, see Näätänen and Kähkönen,2009). By contrast, their sensory-memory duration, as judged fromthe ISI–MMN relationship in these patients, is unaffected (Javittet al., 1998; see also March et al., 1999).

The auditory memory-trace duration shows developmentalchanges also in the beginning of the life, but to the opposite direc-tion, of course, being only 0.5 s in newborns, as judged from MMNsrecorded during sleep (Cheour et al., 2002b), but is thereafter grad-ually prolonged during infancy and childhood. By using the Grauet al. (1998) MMN paradigm for effectively determining the sen-sory-memory duration, Glass et al., 2008a,b) found that this dura-tion was about 1–2 s at the age of 2–3 years but was prolonged to3–5 s by the age of 6 years (see also Gomes et al., 1999).

This memory-trace duration is affected in some child patientgroups, too. In children of 7–8 years with cleft palate, the MMNfor tone-frequency change was of normal amplitude with shortSOAs (350 and 700 ms), whereas the prolongation of the SOA to1400 ms attenuated this amplitude much more than it attenuatedthat of control children (Ceponiene et al., 1999) (Fig. 6). Further-more, an analogous data pattern was obtained in small infants withcleft palate, which was even more abnormal in children with theCatch-22 syndrome (Cheour et al., 1997, 1998a, 1999).

In addition, a short auditory sensory-memory duration even innormal healthy children is associated with delayed linguisticdevelopment. Late talkers had a shorter sensory-memory durationthan that of controls, as judged from MMN data for tone-frequencychange as a function of the ISI recorded at the age of 4 years7 months, even though the language development was normal bythat age (Grossheinrich et al., 2010).

Furthermore, the Barry et al. (2008) recent MMN data for tone-frequency change as a function of the ISI suggested a shorter audi-tory sensory-memory duration in parents of children with SLI thanthat in parents of normally developing children.

4. The MMN as an index of abnormal auditory perception

In the foregoing, it was already mentioned that in patients withschizophrenia, no normal-size MMN can be obtained irrespectiveof experimental manipulations, not even with very short SOAs (Ja-vitt et al., 1998). This suggests a fundamental abnormality in mem-ory-trace formation, and thus in auditory perception (see Näätänenand Winkler, 1999), in these patients (Näätänen and Kähkönen,2009). Moreover, a number of further studies in patients withschizophrenia (Oades et al., 1996; Hirayasu et al., 1998; Schallet al., 1999; Youn et al., 2003; Kasai et al., 2002a,b) showed thatthe frequency-MMN amplitude deficit tends to correlate with theseverity of auditory hallucinations. In addition, Youn et al. (2003)found that the abnormality of the left–right MMN-amplitudeasymmetry for frequency change of a tone correlated with the po-sitive scores of the positive-and-negative syndrome scale (PANSS),in particular with its hallucination subscale. For corroboratingMMNm data, see Thönnesen et al. (2008). Moreover, Fisher et al.(2008) found a smaller duration-MMN amplitude in patients withauditory hallucinations than in those with no hallucinations. Inaddition, the MMN amplitude for intensity change was also smallerin hallucinating than non-hallucinating patients. Furthermore,both patient groups showed an attenuated MMN amplitude for fre-quency change. This MMN deficiency however was differently dis-tributed in frontal areas in the two patient groups.

5. The MMN as an index of increased backward masking(Table 3)

Auditory masking refers to any observation such that informa-tion in a test auditory stimulus is reduced by the presentation ofanother (masking) auditory stimulus (Massaro, 1973). The percep-tion of sequentially presented auditory stimulation may be hin-dered by backward masking, with a sound preventing theperception of an immediately preceding sound, apparently byaffecting its memory-trace formation (Massaro, 1975), in particularwhen the later sound is stronger in intensity than the precedingsound (see Hawkins and Presson, 1986). Massaro (1975) concludesthat backward masking occurs because the masking stimulusessentially overwrites the auditory image of the test stimulusand, therefore, terminates the perceptual processing of the teststimulus.

Backward masking can be assessed both behaviourally and byusing the MMN: a masker presented shortly after each stimulusof the oddball paradigm causes the MMN to deviants and the sub-ject’s ability to discriminate them behaviourally to disappear inparallel (Winkler and Näätänen, 1994; Winkler et al., 1993). Byusing this type of paradigm, it was found by Kujala et al. (2003)that backward masking is enhanced in dyslexic individuals. In-creased backward masking, as reflected by MMN-data pattern,was also reported in chronic alcoholism (Ahveninen et al., 1999).Importantly, this MMN deficit in chronic alcoholism correlatedwith the amount of their alcohol consumption and predicted theirworking-memory performance deficit (Ahveninen et al., 1999).

6. The MMN as an index of abnormal involuntary attentionswitching: either too weak (Table 4) or too strong (Table 5)

As already mentioned, in addition to its auditory-cortex gener-ators, the MMN also has a frontal (usually right-hemisphericallypredominant (Giard et al., 1990)) generator contributing, togetherwith the auditory-cortex MMN generator (see Fig. 2), to the fron-tally and centrally recorded MMN amplitudes (Deouell, 2007;Giard et al., 1990; Rinne et al., 2000). The frontally recordedMMN amplitude is considerably attenuated, whereas the

Page 12: The mismatch negativity (MMN) – A unique window to ... et al (2011... · Invited Review The mismatch negativity (MMN) – A unique window to disturbed central auditory processing

Table 7The MMN indexes pathological brain excitation/excitability in:

– Alcoholism (Ahveninen et al., 2000a; Pekkonen et al., 1998)– ALS (with bulbar signs) (Pekkonen et al., 2004)– Chronic primary insomniacs (Wang et al., 2001)– Epilepsy (Miyajima et al., 2011)– Huntington’s disease, Syndromatic Phase (Beste et al., 2008)– Major depression, children with (Lepistö et al., 2004)– Pediatric migraine (Valeriani et al., 2008; see, however, de Tommaso et al.,

2004)– Schizophrenia (pathological microglia activation, indexing local disease

process and neuronal death) (Banati and Hickie, 2009)– Stuttering (Corbera et al., 2005; see also Wu et al., 1997)

Table 8The MMN indexes cognitive and functional decline in:

– Ageing (Gaeta et al., 2002; Kisley et al., 2005; Schroeder et al., 1995)– Alzheimer’s disease and dementia (Engeland et al., 2002; Schroeder et al.,

1995)– Amyotropic lateral schlerosis (ALS) (Raggi et al., 2008)– Autism (Seri et al., 2007; Roberts et al., 2008, 2011; Näätänen and Kujala,

2011)– CATCH-22 syndrome: children (for a review, see Ceponiene et al., 2002),

adults (Baker et al., 2005)– Children with cleft palate (Ceponiene et al., 2002)– Chronic alcoholism (Polo et al., 1999)– Coma, survivors of (Kane et al., 2000)– Developmental dysphasia (Holopainen et al., 1997, 1998; Korpilahti and

Lang, 1994)– Diabetes mellitus (advanced phase) (Vanhanen et al., 1996)– Down’s syndrome (Lalo et al., 2005)– Dyslexia (Bonte et al., 2007)– Epilepsy (Boatman et al., 2008; Liasis et al., 2000, 2001, 2006; Metz-Lutz and

Philippini, 2006)– HIV (Schroeder et al., 1994)– Intellectual disability (Ikeda et al., 2004; Nakagawa et al., 2002)– Multiple schlerosis (MS) (Gil et al., 1993; Jung et al., 2006; Santos et al., 2006)– Parkinson’s disease (Brønnick et al., 2010; Karayanidis et al., 1995; Pekkonen

et al., 1995c)– Persistent-vegetative-state (PVS), survivors of (Kotchoubey, 2007; Wijnen

et al., 2007; van der Stelt and van Boxtel, 2008; Zarza-Lucianez et al., 2007)– Preterm children with very low birth weight (Jansson-Verkasalo et al., 2004,

2010)– REM-sleep deprivation (Zerouali et al., 2010)– Schizophrenia (e.g., Baldeweg et al., 2004; Hermens et al., 2010; Kawakubo

et al., 2007; Light and Braff, 2005a,b; Rasser et al., 2011; Toyomaki et al.,2008)

– Sleep deprivation (Raz et al., 2001)– Stroke and aphasia (Auther et al., 2000; Ilvonen et al., 2003, 2004; Pettigrew

et al., 2005; Särkämö et al., 2009, 2010a,b; Wertz et al., 1998)– Velo-cardio-facial syndrome (Baker et al., 2005)

R. Näätänen et al. / Clinical Neurophysiology 123 (2012) 424–458 435

mastoid-recorded MMN (with nose reference) is unaffected in pa-tients with schizophrenia (Baldeweg et al., 2002, 2004; Sato et al.,1998, 2002), suggesting dampened involuntary attention switch-ing to auditory change and hence probably accounting forsome of their withdrawal (negative) symptoms, as proposed byNäätänen and Kähkönen (2009) (cf. Fig. 2). Consistent with this,Matthews et al. (2007) found that the MMN at fronto-central elec-trodes for change in the interaural time difference, one of the twomain cues for sound lateralisation, was attenuated in amplitude inpatients with schizophrenia. This result was attributed by theauthors to patients’ impaired ability to use temporal cues to soundlateralisation in natural settings, a deficit probably affecting,according to the authors, their ability to control the focus of atten-tion effectively. The dampened bottom-up attention-switchingfunction in schizophrenia patients might also contribute to theirgradual cognitive decline because of the resulting reduction of cog-nitive and social stimulation (cf. sensory deprivation) (Näätänenand Kähkönen, 2009). For further supportive MMN evidence, seeHermens et al. (2010).

A similar (but transient) dampening of involuntary attentionswitching can also be observed in acute alcohol intoxication(Jääskeläinen et al., 1995, 1996a,b, 1998, 1999b). Even a moderatedose of ethanol selectively weakened the frontal-MMN process,whereas the supratemporal MMN process (with most of it beingseparately observable in nose-referenced mastoid recordings)was unaffected (Jääskeläinen et al., 1996b) (Fig. 2). Furthermore,ethanol abolished the performance decrement caused by auditorydistraction in a visual task (Jääskeläinen et al., 1999b), i.e., damp-ened the effects of auditory distracting stimuli on brain mecha-nisms controlling for environment-driven involuntary attentionswitches in audition (Escera et al., 2000a; Escera and Corral,2007; Jääskeläinen et al., 1999b). This explains, in part, why alco-hol increases accident risk in traffic (cf. Näätänen and Summala,1976). In addition, the Alho et al. (1994) MMN data suggested de-creased involuntary attention switching to auditory changes as aconsequence of a frontal-lobe lesion (see Table 4).

Conversely, the frontal involuntary attention-switching mecha-nism may also become too sensitive, in this way disturbing pri-mary-task performance (Table 5). In patients with closed headinjury suffering from dramatically increased distractibility, thismechanism is abnormally sensitive, judging from their very largefrontally recorded MMN amplitudes at the presence of normalmastoid-recorded (nose-referenced) MMN amplitudes (Kaipioet al., 1999, 2000) (cf. Fig. 2). A subsequent study (Polo et al.,2002) did not confirm this finding, however, which might resultfrom the abnormally fast vigilance decrement of these patients(Kaipio et al., 2001).

Moreover, in children suffering from major depression, short-ened MMN latencies and increased P3a amplitudes (an index ofthe occurrence of an attention switch; Squires et al., 1975; Esceraet al., 2000a) might be associated with these patients’ increaseddistractibility and difficulties in concentrating on task perfor-mance, as shown by the Lepistö et al. (2004) MMN data for CV-syl-lable change. Analogous MMN evidence for increaseddistractibility was also obtained in chronic alcoholism (Ahveninenet al., 2000a,b) and ageing (Gaeta et al., 2001).

7. The MMN attenuation caused by cerebral grey-matter loss orother structural change (Table 6)

The MMN is also attenuated by structural damage in the centralauditory system and even elsewhere in the brain. In schizophreniapatients, a gradual loss of the left-hemisphere temporal grey-mat-ter volume has been observed, which was reflected by attenuatedMMN amplitudes for frequency change (Hirayasu et al., 1998).More recently, Salisbury et al. (2007) found, by using magnetic res-onance imaging (MRI), that at first hospitalisation, schizophreniapatients’ left Heschl-gyrus volume did not differ from that of bipo-lar-disorder patients and normal controls and, further, that thiswas reflected by similar MMN amplitudes for tone-frequencychange. However, in an 18-month follow-up, this volume was re-duced in schizophrenia patients only, and the magnitude of theHeschl’s gyrus volume reduction in these patients strongly corre-lated with the parallel attenuation of their MMN amplitude. Also,Yamasue et al. (2004) found that in schizophrenia patients, MMNmdeficiency for phoneme change in the left hemisphere correlatedwith the magnitude of the left planum-temporale grey-matter loss,suggesting that these structural abnormalities may underlie thefunctional abnormalities of fundamental language-related process-ing in these patients. In addition, very recently, Rasser et al. (2011)found that MMN-amplitude attenuation for frequency change inpatients with schizophrenia (reflecting their impaired day-to-dayfunctioning level) correlated with the magnitude of grey-matterloss in the left Heschl’s gyrus as well as in the motor and executive

Page 13: The mismatch negativity (MMN) – A unique window to ... et al (2011... · Invited Review The mismatch negativity (MMN) – A unique window to disturbed central auditory processing

Fig. 7. Post Hoc correlation analyses of reduced gray matter measures – averaged over specific gyri defined on the Deformed ‘‘Montreal Neurological Institute’’ brain template– with reduced frequency mismatch negativity (MMN) peak amplitudes recorded at fronto-central electrodes in control subjects (blue) and schizophrenia patients (Red).Solid regression lines indicate significant correlation (P < .05, uncorrected) (From Rasser et al., 2011).

436 R. Näätänen et al. / Clinical Neurophysiology 123 (2012) 424–458

Page 14: The mismatch negativity (MMN) – A unique window to ... et al (2011... · Invited Review The mismatch negativity (MMN) – A unique window to disturbed central auditory processing

Table 9The MMN can be used to on-line monitor the patient’s level-of-consciousness in:

– Anaesthesis: depth and recovery (Heinke et al., 2004; Heinke and Koelsch,2005; Koelsch et al., 2006; Yppärilä et al., 2002)

– Coma (Kane et al., 1993, 1996, 2000; Fischer and Luauté, 2005; Luauté et al.,2005; Morlet et al., 2000; Naccache et al., 2005)

– Persistent vegetative state (Kotchoubey, 2007; Kotchoubey, 2007; Wijnenet al., 2007)

R. Näätänen et al. / Clinical Neurophysiology 123 (2012) 424–458 437

regions of the frontal cortex while MMN reduction for durationdeviants also correlated with grey-matter loss in the right Heschl’sgyrus (Fig. 7).

It is possible that the MMN deficiency observed in schizophre-nia can be linked to grey-matter loss occurring in these patients.Olney and Farber (1995) suggested that N-methyl-D-aspartate(NMDA)-receptor (NMDAR) hypofunction accounts for structuralbrain changes in these patients. The data reviewed by the authorsshowed, among other things, that a repeated treatment with anNMDA antagonist during a 3- to 4-day period induces a patternof neurodegenerative changes distributed over several corticolim-bic brain regions. The authors concluded that these and other find-ings signify that the persistent suppression of the NMDAR functionlasting for a few days only can lead to relatively subtle but perma-nent structural changes that are distributed over the neocorticaland limbic brain regions in a pattern roughly similar to the patternof structural changes observed in schizophrenia (Bogerts, 1993).The fact that the MMN is an index of NMDAR dysfunction wouldthen explain the relationship between MMN deficiency and grey-matter loss (Salisbury et al., 2007; Rasser et al., 2011).

The MMN can also reflect structural changes (craniofacial mal-formations) associated with cleft palate (for a review, see Ceponi-ene et al., 2002). In children with cleft palate of 7–9 years of age, itwas found by Ceponiene et al. (1999) that the more posteriorlydelimited the cleft was, the smaller was the MMN amplitude fortone-frequency change and the more severe was the cognitivedefect.

8. The MMN as an index of pathological brain excitation/excitability (Table 7)

In some conditions, an increased MMN amplitude appears to in-dex pathologically increased central nervous system (CNS) or cen-tral-auditory-system excitation/excitability. For example, inabstinent chronic alcoholics, the MMN for frequency change wasabnormally enhanced in amplitude, which might be associatedwith their increased distractibility (Ahveninen et al., 2000a,b). Inaddition, in patients with persistent developmental stuttering,the MMN to phonetic contrasts was strongly enhanced in ampli-tude, whereas that to simple tone contrasts (both frequency andduration) was unaffected (Corbera et al., 2005). This might indexabnormal excitability located specifically in the speech-soundMMN generation region (Näätänen et al., 1997). This MMN findingcorrelated with the patients’ subjective appraisal of their speech-production problems. These results might indicate that properspeech production requires the presence of stable sensory refer-ence provided by the speech-sound-specific memory traces ofthe left temporal cortex (Näätänen, 2001; Näätänen et al., 1997;Shestakova et al., 2003).

In the same vein, an MMNm enhancement to tone-durationdecrement found by Pekkonen et al. (2004) in patients with ALSwith bulbar signs might result from cortical overactivity of excit-atory neurotransmitter glutamate observed in ALS (Rowland andShneider, 2001). Raggi et al. (2008), however, found MMN-ampli-tude decrement for frequency change in non-demented patientswith sporadic ALS.

Consistent with the Pekkonen et al. (2004) above-reviewed ALSresults, at late, symptomatic, stages of Huntington’s disease, theMMN to frequency change was considerably enhanced in ampli-tude, along with behavioural improvement in an auditory signal-detection task, compared to the control group and Huntingtonpatients in the early, presymptomatic, stage (Beste et al., 2008).This paradoxical effect, as contrasted to effects of neurodegenera-tive diseases in general, was attributed by the authors to toxic-le-vel overexcitability of the NMDAR system (due to increasedglutamate concentrations).

In addition, very importantly, Banati and Hickie (2009); (seealso Banati, 2003) found in patients in schizophrenic psychosis thatMMN deficiency for frequency and duration deviants indexedpathological microglia (the main constituent of the brain’s innateimmune system) activation, reflecting local ‘disease activity’ andneuronal death in these patients. The authors emphasised that thismicroglia activation is not a diagnostically specific pathologicalprocess but rather that this activation can serve as a generic mar-ker that relates more directly to disease progression and may becorrelated with other meaningful illness measures such as theMMN.

Pathological brain excitation, as suggested by increased MMNamplitudes, can also be found in temporal-lobe epilepsy (Miyajimaet al., 2011; Gene-Cos et al., 2005) even though MMN peak laten-cies may be prolonged, a possible sign of central-auditory-process-ing disturbance in these patients (Miyajima et al., 2011).

Pathological excitation might also explain some results in pa-tients suffering from post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD), whoshowed considerably elevated MMN amplitudes for frequencychange (Morgan and Grillon, 1999; Ge et al., 2011), whereas Men-ning et al. (2008) could not confirm these results.

9. The MMN as an index of cognitive and functionaldeterioration (Table 8)

Baldeweg et al. (2004) found a relationship between the fron-tally recorded duration-increment MMN deficit and impairmentsin memory functions of patients with schizophrenia, which are evi-dent in these patients (Sullivan et al., 1995; Jahshan et al., 2010).Subsequently, Light and Braff (2005a) obtained a strong correlationbetween the global-assessment-of-functioning (GAF) ratings andthe fronto-central MMN amplitude for tone-duration prolongationin these patients. Furthermore, the MMN amplitude was highlypredictive of the patients’ level of independence in their domesticlife. This correlation was replicated in longitudinal studies 1–2 years later, indicating a stable relationship (Light and Braff,2005b). Subsequently, the relationship between the duration-increment MMN amplitude and patients’ functional status was alsofound by Kawakubo et al., 2007; social skills acquisition) and byToyomaki et al. (2008; executive functioning). (For corroboratingresults with the frequency-MMN amplitude, see Rasser et al.(2011).) Interestingly, this relationship for duration-incrementMMN amplitude is also present in normal healthy subjects (Lightet al., 2007). For previous analogous results with the frequency-MMN amplitude in normal healthy subjects, see Bazana and Stel-mack (2002) and Beauchamp and Stelmack (2006), and with theMMN amplitude for syllable change in children, Liu et al. (2007).For recent corroborating results, see Troche et al. (2009, 2010).

In a similar vein, Jung et al. (2006) found that the MMN-ampli-tude attenuation for duration decrement in patients with MS in-dexed cognitive decline occurring in part of these patients, beinglarger in magnitude for greater cognitive loss. For corroborating re-sults, see Gil et al., 1993) and Santos et al. (2006). Furthermore,MMN (MMNm) deficit indexes cognitive decline also in chronicalcoholism (Polo et al., 1999, 2003), stroke (Ilvonen et al., 2003;

Page 15: The mismatch negativity (MMN) – A unique window to ... et al (2011... · Invited Review The mismatch negativity (MMN) – A unique window to disturbed central auditory processing

Table 10The MMN can be used to monitor the patient’s state in:

– Stroke: recovery (Ilvonen et al., 2003; Särkämö et al., 2010a,)– Schizophrenia: illness progression (Banati and Hickie, 2009; Bodatsch et al.,

2011; Javitt et al., 2000; Oades et al., 2006; Oknina et al., 2005; Salisburyet al., 2007; Umbricht and Krljes, 2005; Umbricht et al., 2003, 2006) andtemporary recovery (Shinozaki et al., 2002)

438 R. Näätänen et al. / Clinical Neurophysiology 123 (2012) 424–458

Särkämö et al., 2009, 2010a,; Auther et al., 1998, 2000; Pettigrewet al., 2005; Wertz et al., 1998), epilepsy (Boatman et al., 2008; Lia-sis et al., 2006), velo-cardio-facial (DiGeorge) syndrome (Bakeret al., 2005), Down’s syndrome (Lalo et al., 2005), children withcertain forms of cleft palate and those with the Catch-22 syndrome(Ceponiene et al., 2002) and, of course, in Alzheimer’s disease(Pekkonen et al., 1994; vMMN: Tales and Butler, 2006; Taleset al., 2008), dementia (Schroeder et al., 1995), intellectual disabil-ity (Nakagawa et al., 2002; Ikeda et al., 2004) and mild cognitiveimpairment (MCI) (vMMN: Tales and Butler, 2006; Tales et al.,2008).

MMN-amplitude attenuation (at least for frequency change) canalso index cognitive and functional degeneration in patients withan advanced stage of diabetes mellitus (Vanhanen et al., 1996),Parkinson’s disease (Karayanidis et al., 1995; Pekkonen et al.,1995b; Brønnick et al., 2010), HIV (Schroeder et al., 1994) and alsoin normal ageing (Gaeta et al., 2001; Kisley et al., 2005; Cooperet al., 2006). In addition, the MMN amplitude indexes decreasedcognition in different subnormal states such as sleep deprivation(Raz et al., 2001), rapid eye movement (REM)-sleep deprivation(Zerouali et al., 2010) and in patients in the comatose (Fischeret al., 1999, 2006b) or PVS (Wijnen et al., 2007) state.

Recently, Bisiachi et al. (2009) found smaller-amplitude MMNresponses to tone-frequency change in newborns of less than 30gestational weeks than in those with a longer gestational period.This amplitude strongly correlated with several maturational fac-tors (gestational age, weight, length and cranial circumference).These results suggest, according to the authors, the role of the ges-tational age as the main factor explaining cortical functioning atthe early age.

10. The MMN as an index of the level of consciousness (Table 9)

In patients in the comatose state, no MMN can usually be re-corded unless a latent recovery process has started, leading tothe return of consciousness and cognitive capacities in the near fu-ture. Therefore, the MMN can be used as a tool in coma-outcomeprediction (Kane et al., 1993, 1996; Fischer et al., 1999, 2004,2006a,b; Luauté et al., 2005; Morlet et al., 2000; Fischer andLuauté, 2005; Daltrozzo et al., 2007; Vanhaudenhuyse et al.,2008). Moreover, the MMN peak latency measured at hospitalisa-tion of survivors of coma caused by a severe head injury predicted,to some extent, their expressive language ability and visuo-spatialperformance 1 year after the injury (Kane et al., 2000). Kotchoubey(2007) stressed that in using the MMN for the objective assessmentof the patient’s remaining cognitive capacity, it is better to usecomplex rather than simple sound stimuli which may result in se-vere underestimation of the remaining capacity. For a meta-analy-sis of studies on the MMN in coma, see Daltrozzo et al. (2007).Recently, it was concluded by Vanhaudenhuyse et al. (2008) thatMMN results in comatose patients converge to the conclusion thatMMN is a very good predictor of recovery from coma, in particularin anoxic coma.

In patients in the PVS, the frequency-MMN recovery occurred inparallel with the recovery of consciousness (Wijnen et al., 2007;Kotchoubey, 2007; Kotchoubey, 2007), showing a step-wise ampli-

tude increase when the patient crossed the boundary from uncon-sciousness to consciousness. In addition, in locked-in patients, thefrequency MMN can give evidence for the presence of at least somelevel of consciousness (Ragazzoni et al., 2000). Moreover, the MMNcan also index anaesthesia depth (for a review, see Heinke andKoelsch, 2005): a small-amplitude MMN was recorded to fre-quency change during deep sedation, whereas in complete uncon-sciousness, this MMN was absent. It, however, returned while thepatient was recovering from anaesthesia (Heinke et al., 2004).Interestingly, an MMN type of response was also elicited by mu-sic-syntactic violations in deep sedation (Heinke et al., 2004; Koe-lsch et al., 2006), indicating that this kind of complex analysisfollowing the rules of Western music can occur even in deepsedation.

Very recently, it was found by Boly et al. (2011) that in the veg-etative state, feed-forward processes are preserved whereas top-down processes are impaired. The effective connectivity wasmeasured by using the MMN for tones presented in a roving para-digm (Baldeweg et al., 2004). The authors found that the only dif-ference between patients in the vegetative state and controls wasan impairment of backward connectivity from frontal to temporalcortices, suggesting that a selective disruption of top-down pro-cesses from high levels of a cortical hierarchy can lead to loss ofconsciousness in brain-damaged patients, and can clearly differen-tiate the vegetative state from minimally conscious state. In addi-tion to its neuroscientific relevance, the present approach could,according to Boly et al. (2011), constitute a new diagnostic toolto quantify the level of consciousness electrophysiologically atthe patients’ bedside.

The MMN can also reflect effects of hypnosis (Jamieson et al.,2005; Kallio et al., 1999) and concentrative meditation (Srinivasanand Baijal, 2007).

11. The MMN as an index of the progression/severity of theillness (Table 10)

In patients with schizophrenia, the MMN amplitude in particu-lar to frequency change is attenuated concomitantly with diseaseprogression (for a meta-analysis, see Umbricht and Krljes, 2005).Consistent with this, several studies (Salisbury et al., 2002, 2007;Umbricht et al., 2006; Devrim-Ücok et al., 2008; Todd et al.,2008) showed that the frequency MMN deficit was more robustin chronic patients than in first-episode patients in whom the ef-fect was smaller in size or had not yet emerged. In addition, Shino-zaki et al. (2002) found that a temporary recovery of acutesymptoms of schizophrenia patients was accompanied by anMMN amplitude increase for tone-frequency change recorded atmastoids as polarity-reversed MMN (whereas the frontally re-corded MMN did not correlate with this temporary recovery). Inaddition, Urban et al. (2008), using the vMMN, found that thereduction of its amplitude for motion-direction deviants was largerfor a more severe state and longer duration of the illness of thepatient.

Moreover, MMN data also reflected clinical severity in patientswith persistent developmental stuttering (Corbera et al., 2005),dyslexia (Baldeweg et al., 1999; Stoodley et al., 2006), PTSD (Men-ning et al., 2008) and in newborns and 6-month-old infants withcleft palate (with clinical severity being determined as the ante-rior–posterior extent of the cleft; Ceponiene et al., 1999, 2000a,b,2002). In a similar vein, Moberget et al. (2008) found that patientsexhibiting the most severe symptoms of ataxia, a sign of cerebellarcortical atrophy, produced smaller-amplitude MMN responses totone-duration change than other patients.

Studying stroke patients, Särkämö et al. (2008, 2009, 2010a, ad-dressed amusia caused by a left or right middle cerebral artery

Page 16: The mismatch negativity (MMN) – A unique window to ... et al (2011... · Invited Review The mismatch negativity (MMN) – A unique window to disturbed central auditory processing

Table 11The MMN predicts:

– Recovery from coma (Daltrozzo et al., 2007; Fischer and Luauté, 2005;Fischer et al., 1999, 2002, 2004, 2006a,b; Morlet et al., 2000; Naccacheet al., 2005; Vanhaudenhuyse et al., 2008 (with the MMN being the bestpredictor of positive outcome, i.e., recovery without getting into thepersistent vegetative state: Fischer and Luauté, 2005; Luauté et al., 2005);for a meta analysis, see Daltrozzo et al., 2007)

– Recovery from the persistent vegetative state (Kotchoubey, 2007; Kotchoubeyet al., 2007; Wijnen et al., 2007 (with the MMN recorded at hospilizationalso predicting the level of consciousness at discharge and even the extentof cognitive recovery during the next 2 years; Wijnen et al., 2007))

– Recovery from locked-in state (Ragazzoni et al., 2000)– Recovery from alcoholism (Kathmann et al., 1995; Pekkonen et al., 1998)– Social-skills learning in schizophrenia Kawakubo et al., 2007)– Social cognition in schizophrenia (Wynn et al., 2010)– Schizophrenia (first-episode onset) risk (Bodatsch et al., 2011; Stone et al.,

2010)– Progression of schizophrenia (Bodatsch et al., 2011; Devrim-Ücok et al., 2008;

Kawakubo et al., 2007; Kiang et al., 2009; Light and Braff, 2005b; Salisburyet al., 2007; Umbricht and Krljes, 2005; Umbricht et al., 2006)

– Dyslexia (Blitz et al., 2007; Leppänen et al., 2010; van Leeuwen et al., 2006;Stoodley et al., 2006)

– Language development (Choudhury and Benasich, 2011; for a review, seeStix, 2011)

– Language development in prematurely born children (Jansson-Verkasalo et al.,2003b, 2004, 2010)

– Drug-therapy outcome in schizophrenia (Horton et al., 2010; Javitt et al.,2008; Lavoie et al., 2007; Schall et al., 1999)

– Working-memory performance decrement in chronic alcoholism (Ahveninenet al., 1999)

Fig. 8. Grand-average difference waves showing the duration mismatch negativityin the frontal (upper row) and central electrodes (lower row) for converting (dashedline) and nonconverting subjects (solid line). AR-C = at risk – coinversion, AR-NC = at risk – no conversion (From Bodatsch et al., 2011).

Table 12The MMN shows increased genetic risk of:

– Alcoholism (Rodriguez et al., 1998; Zhang et al., 2001)– Asperger syndrome (Jansson-Verkasalo et al., 2005; Korpilahti et al., 2007)– Dyslexia (Blitz et al., 2007; Friedrich et al., 2004; Leppänen et al., 2002,

2010; Leppänen and Lyytinen, 1997; Lyytinen et al., 2001, 2004, 2006;Maurer et al., 2009; Pihko et al., 1999; Richardson et al., 2003; vanLeeuwen et al., 2006)

– Language learning impairment (LLI) (Benasich et al., 2006; Choudhury andBenasich, 2011; Friedrich et al., 2004; Weber et al., 2005)

– Schizophrenia in children (Schreiber et al., 1992), adults (Ahveninen et al.,2006; Bodatsch et al., 2011; Brockhaus-Dumke et al., 2005; Hall et al.,2007, 2009; Jessen et al., 2001; Magno et al., 2008; Michie et al., 2002;Shin et al., 2009), and in Individuals with the CATCH syndrome (with ahigher risk in individuals carrying the COMT108/158Val than COMT108/

158Met allele) (Baker et al., 2005)– Specific language impairment (SLI) (Barry et al., 2008; Benasich and Tallal,

2002; Benasich et al., 2006; Friedrich et al., 2004; Weber et al., 2005)

R. Näätänen et al. / Clinical Neurophysiology 123 (2012) 424–458 439

stroke. They found that amusia caused by a right-hemisphere dam-age, especially that to temporal and frontal areas, was more severethan that caused by a left-hemisphere damage. Furthermore, inamusic right-hemisphere patients, the severity of amusia corre-lated with weaker frequency-MMNm responses. Additionally,within the right-hemisphere-damaged group, the amusic patientswho had damage in the auditory cortex showed worse recoveryfrom amusia as well as weaker MMNm responses throughout the6-month follow-up than did the non-amusic patients or the amusicpatients with no auditory-cortex damage. Also, the amusic patients(both with and without auditory-cortex damage) performed worsethan did the non-amusic patients on a number of tests of differentaspects of cognitive performance (including working memory,attention and cognitive flexibility). These findings suggested,according to the authors, domain-general cognitive deficits as theprimary mechanism underlying amusia without auditory-cortexdamage, whereas amusia with auditory-cortex damage is associ-ated with both auditory and cognitive deficits.

In conclusion, because of the large number of different disor-ders affecting the MMN, it appears that the MMN provides a gen-eric illness measure, useful for assessing illness severity, assuggested by Banati and Hickie (2009) for both the MMN andmicroglia activation.

12. The MMN in predicting illness course/drug response(prognosis) (Table 11)

A striking finding was that the recovery of the MMN in a coma-tose patient strongly predicted, as already mentioned, the recoveryof consciousness in the near future (Kane et al., 1993, 1996; Fischeret al., 2000, 2006a,b; see also Fischer et al., 2008; Luauté et al.,2005; Vanhaudenhuyse et al., 2008; for a meta-analysis, see Dal-trozzo et al., 2007). In addition, the MMN in patients in the PVS re-corded at hospitalisation predicted the magnitude of cognitiverecovery even 2 years ahead (Wijnen et al., 2007; see also Kotchou-bey, 2007; Kotchoubey et al., 2007).

Furthermore, in patients with schizophrenia, MMN data canpredict future developments of the patient’s state and the effi-ciency of different treatments. For example, Umbricht et al.(2006) found that MMN deficiency at illness onset may index thepresence of a more pervasive brain pathology and be observed onlyin a subgroup of patients, possibly in those with more precursors ofschizophrenia or with an elevated risk of developing chronicschizophrenia. In addition, Kawakubo et al. (2007) found that theMMN in patients with schizophrenia was predictive of the acquisi-tion of social skills during a 3-month social skills trainingprogramme.

Importantly, it was recently found by Lavoie et al. (2007) thatthe deficient MMN amplitude for frequency change in patientswith schizophrenia was corrected towards normal levels by a glu-tathione precursor, N-acetyl-cysteine (NAC), which improved theirNMDAR function, supporting the role of the NMDARs in MMNgeneration.

The MMN improvement was, however, found in the absence ofany robust changes in clinical-severity indices, even though theywere observed in a larger and more prolonged clinical study (La-voie et al., 2007; Berk et al., 2008a,b; for supporting evidence,

Page 17: The mismatch negativity (MMN) – A unique window to ... et al (2011... · Invited Review The mismatch negativity (MMN) – A unique window to disturbed central auditory processing

Table 13MMN indexes central-auditory-processing improvement in:

– Autistic patients (with frequency processing better than in Controls; Gomot et al., 2002)– Children with specific language impairment (SLI) receiving language and speech training (Pihko et al., 2007)– Cochlear-implant (CI) patients as a function of time since CI installation (Kraus et al., 1993b; Lonka et al., 2004; Ponton et al., 2000; Ponton and Eggermont, 2001) and

training (Ponton et al., 2009)– Dyslexic children presented with consonant–vowel syllables with lengthened formant-transition duration (Bradlow et al., 1999)– Dyslexic children receiving audio-visual rehabilitation (Kujala et al., 2001)– Early Child development, mainly speech-sound learning (Alho and Cheour-Luhtanen, 1997; Alho et al., 1990; Carral et al., 2005; Cheour et al., 1998b, 2002a,b; Cheour-

Luhtanen et al., 1996; Choudhury and Benasich, 2011; Draganova et al., 2005, 2007; Gomes et al., 2000; Gomot et al., 2000; Fellman and Huotilainen, 2006;Huotilainen et al. 2005, 2007; Kraus and Cheour, 2000; Kraus et al., 1992, 1993a, 1999; Kuhl, 2004; Kujala et al., 2004; Kushnerenko et al., 2002, 2007; Leppänenet al., 2004; Lovio et al., 2009; Lyytinen et al., 2001, 2006; Molholm et al., 2004; Novitski et al., 2007; Oades et al., 1996; Pang et al., 1998; Pihko et al., 2005; Tanakaet al., 2001; Trainor et al., 2001, 2003; Uwer and von Suchodoletz, 2000)

– Early-onset blindness (Kujala et al., 1995a,b, 2000b, 2005b; Alho et al., 1993)– Epileptic patients with vagus-nerve stimulation (Borghetti et al., 2007)– Infants operated for craniosynosthosis (Huotilainen et al., 2008)– Patients with hearing aids (Rudner et al., 2008)– Patients with ultrasound hearing aids (Hosoi et al., 1998)– Schizophrenic patients progressing from the Acute to Post-Acute Phase (Shinozaki et al., 2002)– Sleeping infants exposed to auditory discrimination training (Cheour et al., 2002a)– Stroke patients as a function of time since stroke onset (Ilvonen et al., 2003; Särkämö et al., 2010a,)– Stroke patients receiving music or audio-book stimulation (Särkämö et al., 2010a,)

440 R. Näätänen et al. / Clinical Neurophysiology 123 (2012) 424–458

see Coyle, 2006; Schwieler et al., 2008). Hence, an MMN enhance-ment may precede improvement in clinical severity, which,according to the authors, highlights the possible utility of theMMN as a biomarker of treatment efficacy, and, therefore, also asa tool in drug development (see also Javitt, 2007, 2009; Javittet al., 2008). Previously, in the Schall et al. (1999) study, theMMN predicted the outcome of clozapine therapy in patients withschizophrenia. Moreover, more recently, Horton et al. (2011) foundthat clozapine increased the MMN amplitude for tone-frequencychange, with increasing doses being associated with increasing fre-quency-MMN amplitudes (but attenuated that for tone-durationincrements and decrements).

Very recently, it was found by Bodatsch et al. (2011) that inindividuals with a high schizophrenia risk, the MMN for tone-dura-tion decrement (but not for tone-frequency change) was lower inamplitude (resembling that of first-episode patients) in those pa-tients who subsequently, during an observation period of 2 years,converted to the first-episode psychosis than that in those whodid not convert to psychosis during that period (Fig. 8). This resultwas remarkable as these groups were thought to share a similarclinical risk of psychosis at baseline. The authors proposed thatthe duration-MMN reduction in their converters might reflect bothstructural and neurochemical alterations associated with the con-version to psychosis. Furthermore, such results do not, according tothe authors, only contribute to the prediction of conversion andlead time to it but also contribute to a more individualised riskassessment and hence also to risk-adapted prevention. In addition,recently, Stone et al. (2010) found in subjects at risk of psychosis,an attenuated frontal-MMN amplitude for tone-duration incre-ment and, further, that this MMN deficit was related to reducedthalamic glutamate plus glutamine and NMDA levels.

Further, a phonological deficit in young elementary-school chil-dren, reflected by a reduced MMN amplitude to syllable change(but not that to simple frequency change), appeared to predicttheir dyslexia risk (Blitz et al., 2007). Also, in very prematurelyborn children, the MMN amplitude for consonant change at 4 yearsof age predicted, as already mentioned, naming performance at theage of 6 years (Jansson-Verkasalo et al., 2003b, 2004). Furthermore,Jansson-Verkasalo et al. (2010) found that the perceptual narrow-ing for non-native phoneme contrasts occurring in normal healthyinfants from 6 to 12 months of age (Kuhl et al., 1991; Kuhl, 2004),reflected by attenuated MMNs for non-native phoneme contrastsin these infants (Cheour et al., 1998b), does not occur in very pre-maturely born infants who showed large MMNs even for non-na-tive phoneme contrasts. This indicates that the ability to

discriminate non-native phonemes was preserved in prematurelyborn infants. Moreover, it was also found that the better this abilitywas preserved (reflected by larger MMN-amplitudes for non-na-tive contrasts) at the age of 1 year, the worse was the performancein native-language tests at the age of 2 years. Thus, decline in sen-sitivity to non-native phonemes, as reflected by MMN attenuationfor non-native phoneme contrasts, served as a positive predictorfor further age-appropriate language development.

Moreover, the Weber et al. (2005) MMN data showing a deficitof prosodic perception in 5-month-old infants suggested that theMMN might be taken as an early marker of SLI risk.

13. The MMN as an index of genetic disposition to differentpathologies (Table 12)

Some studies suggest that in schizophrenia there is a geneticcontribution to its aetiology. An MMN-amplitude attenuation forduration increment was found by Michie et al. (2002) and thatfor frequency change by Jessen et al. (2001) in symptom-freefirst-order relatives of patients with schizophrenia but studies withlarger samples have either not confirmed these findings (Bramonet al., 2004) or showed a trend only (Price et al., 2006). However,Hall et al. (2006) recently found a significant, although modest, ge-netic correlation between the duration-increment MMN-ampli-tude deficit and schizophrenia, and therefore suggested that theMMN is a potentially valid endophenotype in schizophrenia. Bycontrast, Ahveninen et al. (2006), using the MMN to tone-fre-quency changes in their study of twin pairs discordant for schizo-phrenia, obtained results suggesting, according to the authors, thatMMN abnormalities in patients with schizophrenia might reflectpredominantly state-dependent neurodegeneration. A similar con-clusion was made by Magno et al. (2008) on the basis of their dura-tion- and frequency-MMN amplitude results.

In patients with the 22q11 deletion syndrome, the considerablyincreased rates of schizophrenia are attributed to the loss of one al-lele containing the catechol-O-methyl transferase (COMT) gene.Symptom-free carriers showed attenuated MMN amplitudes forboth simple auditory (duration and frequency) and syllabicchanges as well as corresponding decrements in behavioural sounddiscrimination (Baker et al., 2005). Moreover, these MMN andbehavioural effects were stronger in subjects carrying the COMT108/158Met allele on their single intact chromosome (associatedwith a greater risk of schizophrenia) than in those carrying theCOMT 108/158Val allele (a smaller schizophrenia risk). The authors

Page 18: The mismatch negativity (MMN) – A unique window to ... et al (2011... · Invited Review The mismatch negativity (MMN) – A unique window to disturbed central auditory processing

R. Näätänen et al. / Clinical Neurophysiology 123 (2012) 424–458 441

concluded that because the MMN depends on the NMDAR function(Javitt et al., 1996), the MMN modification by the COMT Val 108/158Met polymorphism points towards abnormal dopamine–gluta-mate interactions in the MMN-generator system.

Furthermore, children with a parent or sibling with dyslexiahave an increased risk of dyslexia, and this is reflected by theirattenuated MMN amplitudes to simple frequency (Maurer et al.,2003) or duration changes (Friedrich et al., 2004; Leppänen et al.,2002) or to phoneme-category change (van Leeuwen et al.,2006). Similarly predictive were the lack of a left-hemisphereMMN response to speech-sound change (Maurer et al., 2003) andthe bilateral distribution of this MMN response (Maurer et al.,2009).

Also, studying 6-month-old infants with a family history of lan-guage-related learning difficulties, Benasich et al. (2006) foundthat these infants had smaller mismatch responses (MMRs; the po-sitive-polarity analogue of MMN; Maurer et al., 2003) than those ofcontrol infants with no such family history. In addition, as alreadymentioned, Barry et al. (2008) obtained MMN data suggesting thatparents of children with SLI have a shorter auditory sensory-mem-ory duration than that of control parents.

Very recently, a major breakthrough in the research of the ge-netic background of dyslexia was made by Roeske et al. (2011)who found that a certain marker (rs4234898), located on chromo-some 4q32.1, was associated with the MMN deficit for a CV-sylla-ble change. Moreover, this MMN was also associated with atwo-marker haplotype of rs4234898 and rs11100040, one of itsneighbouring single-nucleotide polymorphisms. The authors con-cluded that their results suggest a possible transregulation effecton SLC2A3, the predominant facilitative glucose transporter inneurons, which might lead to glucose deficits in dyslexic childrenand could explain their attenuated MMN in passive listening tasks,as observed in this study. Moreover, in a very recent study of thesame group, Czamara et al. (2011) obtained results pointing toan association between the late MMN for speech-sound changeand rare variants in a candidate region for dyslexia.

14. The MMN as an index of central auditory processingimprovement or recovery (Table 13)

Spontaneous and training-induced improvement. A number ofstudies in normal healthy subjects (Atienza and Cantero, 2001;Kraus et al., 1995; Gottselig et al., 2004; Menning et al., 2000;Tremblay et al., 1997, 1998; Näätänen et al., 1993; for a review,see Kujala and Näätänen, 2010) demonstrated that the MMN re-flects plastic neural changes associated with discrimination learn-ing. Therefore, it is a very attractive tool for following up recoveryand intervention effects in various patient groups, for instance, instroke, coma, PVS or cochlear-implant (CI) patients or in childrenwith developmental disorders.

In stroke and other brain-injured patients, it would be impor-tant to determine the changes in perceptual abilities very soonafter the damage. However, such patients are often unable to coop-erate or to understand instructions, particularly if the neural net-work involved in speech perception is affected. Aaltonen et al.(1993), in an early study, found that the MMN to speech-soundchange was abolished in posterior left-hemispheric stroke patients,whereas an anterior left-hemispheric stroke had no such effect. Bycontrast, the tone-frequency MMN was normal in both patientgroups. Subsequently, as already mentioned, Ilvonen et al. (2003)followed up the recovery of left-hemisphere stroke patients,recording the MMN at 4 and 10 days, and then at 3 and 6 monthsafter stroke onset. Language-comprehension tests (Boston Diag-nostic Aphasia Examination and Token tests) were also adminis-tered in all but the first session in which the patients could not

cooperate. The MMNs in the first two sessions were quite smallin amplitude for sound-duration and -frequency changes, but be-came larger thereafter, disclosing normal-like amplitudes at3 months post-stroke (Fig. 3). Furthermore, the duration-MMNamplitude increase correlated with the improvement in the BostonDiagnostic Aphasia Examination test scores from 10 days to3 months.

Subsequently, as already mentioned, it was found by Särkämöet al. (2010b) that the recovery of the MMNm and behavioural dis-crimination can be expedited with music and audio-book stimula-tion. Dividing their 60 middle cerebral artery stroke patients intomusic and audio-book listening and control groups, these authorsrecorded patients’ MMNm to frequency and duration changes ina binaurally delivered complex tone. (In addition, all patients re-ceived the normal hospital treatment and rehabilitation of strokepatients, of course.) It was found, as expected, that the MMNmamplitudes were, in general, smaller in the lesioned than oppositehemisphere. Importantly, the frequency-MMNm amplitude in-creased more in both music and audio-book groups than in thecontrol group during the 6-month post-stroke period. In addition,the duration-MMNm amplitude increased more in the audio-bookgroup than in the other groups. Moreover, the frequency-MMNmamplitude changes correlated with the behavioural improvementof verbal memory and focussed attention induced by music listen-ing. The authors interpreted their results as demonstrating that themere listening to music or speech starting soon post-stroke onsetcan induce long-term plastic changes in early sensory processing,as reflected by the MMNm enhancement, which, in turn, may facil-itate the recovery of higher cognitive functions. These remarkableresults encourage the use of music and speech stimulation in therehabilitation of stroke patients in particular in the early post-stroke period.

The deficit of the auditory/language system may be inborn, as isthe case in developmental dyslexia, SLI or in the autism spectrum.Because of the high prevalence of these disorders and their devas-tating effects on the individual’s academic success and therebyself-esteem, early identification and remediation methods shouldbe developed. Recent evidence fortunately shows that the aberrantneural substrate of language deficits can be ameliorated withappropriate intervention. For instance, Pihko et al. (2007) foundthat language-impaired children at the age of 5–6 years benefitedfrom language and speech exercises. Furthermore, this trainingalso changed the neural processing of speech sounds so that theMMNm for syllable changes became stronger in the lefthemisphere.

In dyslexic first-grade children, intervention, judging from anMMN-amplitude enhancement, induced neural plastic changesand ameliorated reading difficulties (Kujala et al., 2001). These 7-year-old school children were exposed to an audiovisual trainingprogramme with no linguistic items (Audilex Program; Karma,1999) for 7 weeks. Their reading skills as well as the MMN elicitedby tone-order reversals were assessed before and after this trainingperiod. After this period, the training group made fewer reading er-rors and tended to read faster than did the age-matched controlgroup of dyslexic children. Moreover, the MMN amplitude fortone-order reversals was increased in the training group but notin the control group. These results consequently showed that read-ing deficits can be ameliorated with audiovisual training and, fur-ther, that the improvement of reading skills is associated withplastic neural changes of the central auditory system as reflectedby the MMN enhancement. Furthermore, these positive trainingresults were obtained by using no linguistic stimuli, which sug-gests that impaired auditory-perceptual processes other than defi-cient phonology may also contribute to reading impairments.

Similar effects were also obtained with extremely low-birth-weight children of 6 years of age (Huotilainen et al., 2011). These

Page 19: The mismatch negativity (MMN) – A unique window to ... et al (2011... · Invited Review The mismatch negativity (MMN) – A unique window to disturbed central auditory processing

442 R. Näätänen et al. / Clinical Neurophysiology 123 (2012) 424–458

children were allocated to a 5-week training programme withAudilex (training group) or to a control group playing computergames. Before and after the intervention, auditory ERPs to soundchanges were recorded and reading-related skills were assessed.The MMN responses to frequency and duration deviants increasedin amplitude in the Audilex training group but not in the control-game group. However, the reading skills were similar in the twogroups after invention and 2 years later.

As already mentioned, the shortening of the abnormally longMMN-peak latency and the MMN-amplitude recovery in drug-resistant adult epilepsy patients after vagus-nerve stimulatorimplantation was recently found by Borghetti et al. (2007), sug-gesting a positive effect of vagus-nerve stimulation on centralauditory processing in these patients.

Drug-induced improvement. In Table 1, short-term effects of dif-ferent drugs are reviewed. In addition, in the foregoing, somefavourable drug effects in schizophrenia patients were alreadymentioned (NAC, Lavoie et al., 2007; clozapine, Schall et al.,1999; Horton et al., 2011). Moreover, very recently, Dulude et al.(2010) found that nicotine, relative to placebo, not only prolongedthe peak latency of the duration MMN but also increased its ampli-tude in patients to a level comparable with that seen in controls. Bycontrast, the frequency MMN was not affected. The authors con-cluded that these preliminary findings demonstrate that acute nic-otine can normalise temporal aspects of central auditoryprocessing in patients with schizophrenia, an effect that might bemediated by the activation of a7 nicotinic acetylcholine receptors,the functioning of which is diminished in schizophrenia. Theauthors further concluded that these ameliorating effects of nico-tine may have implications for understanding the increased tobac-co smoking in schizophrenic patients and for developing nicotinicpharmacotherapeutics to alleviate sensory-memory impairmentsin schizophrenia. In addition, a positive effect of nicotine in bothsmoking and non-smoking normal healthy subjects on temporal-interval processing (indexed by an MMN-amplitude enhancementto ISI deviants) was recently found by Martin et al. (2009).

Furthermore, very recently, it was found by Sawada et al. (2010)that in children of 7–13 years of age with attention deficit hyperac-tivity disorder (ADHD), the intake of osmotic-release methylpheni-date (MPH) increased the MMN amplitude for tone-frequencychange and simultaneously alleviated the severity of their ADHDsymptoms.

Central auditory discrimination and training in CI users. For pro-foundly deaf individuals, a sensation of hearing can be restoredby a CI. CIs are currently the most successful and widely used sen-sory neuroprotheses. In a large percentage of those individuals fit-ted with a CI, a remarkable degree of language communication viathe auditory domain is restored. Nevertheless, objective, non-behavioural measures of auditory discrimination such as theMMN remain a potentially powerful, but as yet under-utilised,application for this population.

The first MMN recordings from CI users were reported by Krauset al. (1993b) who employed speech-syllable contrasts. For thegroup of individuals investigated in that study, Kraus et al. foundthat the MMN elicited from CI users was remarkably similar to thatobtained from a group of normal-hearing listeners. These resultssuggested that despite the differences in peripheral input, the brainwas processing basic speech units in a very similar fashion.

Ponton et al. (2000; see also Ponton and Don, 1995, 2003; Pon-ton and Eggermont, 2007), recording the MMN evoked by a singlecontrast in stimulus duration from groups of normal-hearingadults and adult CI users, also found that MMNs of the two groupswere quite similar. In a more detailed study in which subjects wereseparated into groups based on benefit gained by using a CI, Groe-nen et al. (1996) reported that whereas the group classified as goodperformers showed an MMN, poor performers failed to generate a

comparable response. Furthermore, Kelly et al. (2005) reported asimilar failure to obtain the MMN from CI users defined as poorperformers. In addition, Lonka et al. (2004), in longitudinally track-ing the changes in the language function after a small group of deafadults received a CI, reported that while the MMN was absent forapproximately 12 months post-surgery, the response subsequentlyemerged, first for a large vowel contrast, and then later for moresubtle vowel distinctions. Converging behavioural improvementin a speech-discrimination test was also observed during the fol-low-up period.

Trautwein et al. (1998) and Ponton et al. (2000), who assessedthe CI users with non-speech stimuli, obtained results that relateback to both speech performance and training. First, Trautweinet al. (1998) found that while psychophysical thresholds weresuperior to MMN-estimated discrimination thresholds for durationcontrasts in the control group, the opposite was found for a groupof adult CI users. Thus, in several individuals, there was neuro-physiological evidence of discrimination based on the MMN, with-out the individual being able to behaviourally perceive thedifference. Based on the apparent separation of discrimination atthe level of sensation versus perception, Ponton et al. (2009) devel-oped a protocol based on pre-training MMN evaluation to deter-mine whether there was neurophysiological evidence ofdiscrimination in the absence of perceptual discrimination. Thisknowledge was then used to develop a training protocol which re-sulted in significant improvements in frequency discrimination aswell as combined consonant and vowel discrimination in a singlehighly trained adult implant user.

As noted by Roman et al. (2005), there is a clear application ofthe MMN in clinical testing and training of CI populations. How-ever, refinements are needed in methods and recording technique,to optimise the protocols for frequency discrimination and speechtesting.

Recently, music perception of CI users was also studied by usingthe MMN. Koelsch et al. (2004) found that CI users can perceivetimbre, judging from the MMN elicited by timbre deviants in them(the timbre MMN: Tervaniemi et al., 1997; Toiviainen et al., 1998).Moreover, music-syntactic violations elicited an early right ante-rior negativity which might be interpreted as a music-syntacticMMN in CI users, which again was considerably smaller in ampli-tude than that in controls but nevertheless showed that CI users’central neural mechanisms underlying music perception are acti-vated by musical stimulation (even though they reported that theyhad difficulties in discriminating musical information and thatthey were in general frustrated by their inability to accurately hearmusic). Consistent with this, subsequently, Sandmann et al. (2010),by using the multi-feature MMN paradigm with six different levelsof the magnitude of deviation for each feature (Pakarinen et al.,2007) (Fig. 1) found that CI users had difficulties in discriminatingsmall changes in the acoustic properties of musical sounds. It wasconcluded by Sandmann et al. (2010) that this type of paradigmcould be of substantial clinical value by providing a comprehensiveprofile of the extent of restored hearing in CI users. Moreover, aninverse relationship was found between the MMN amplitude andthe duration of profound deafness; however, there was also evi-dence for experience-related plastic changes as a function of theduration of CI use (Sandmann et al., 2010).

Ultrasonic hearing. Some profoundly deaf individuals can per-ceive ultrasound only, which occurs by bone conduction. Hosoiet al. (1998) found that in normal healthy subjects, ultrasoundmodulated by different speech sounds produces discriminablestimuli, which points to the possibility of developing ultrasonichearing aids as an alternative for CIs for those profoundly deaf indi-viduals who can sense ultrasound only. Hosoi and colleagues deliv-ered ultrasound to the right sterno-cleidomastoid through aceramic vibrator. Substantial N1m responses were elicited by both

Page 20: The mismatch negativity (MMN) – A unique window to ... et al (2011... · Invited Review The mismatch negativity (MMN) – A unique window to disturbed central auditory processing

R. Näätänen et al. / Clinical Neurophysiology 123 (2012) 424–458 443

audible air-conducted sound stimuli and bone-conducted ultra-sonic stimuli. Further, when the (40 kHz) ultrasound stimuli mod-ulated by vowels/i/ and/a/ were presented as standards anddeviants, respectively, substantial MMNm responses were ob-served for deviants, indicating that the two sounds were discrimi-nable. Moreover, the equivalent current dipole (ECD) for theMMNm was located within the auditory cortex, in the vicinity ofthe MMNm observed in a separate condition with the respectiveair-conducted stimuli. The authors also tested 53 profoundly deafindividuals behaviourally and found that 28 of them could detectultrasonic stimuli and, further, that 11 of them could discriminateultrasounds modulated by different speech signals. These resultsprovide, according to the authors, the rationale for developingultrasound hearing aids, which do not require surgery, for thoseprofoundly deaf who can sense ultrasound.

Improved discrimination in early blind individuals. A special caseof enhanced central auditory function of compensatory naturecan be encountered in early blinds. The MMN elicited by sound-location changes was larger in amplitude in early-blind individualsthan in sighted control subjects (Kujala et al., 1995a). (The MMNwas recorded while subjects’ attention was directed to the tactilemodality.) This suggests enhanced spatial auditory perceptionand discrimination in the blind. The result is consistent withbehavioural data showing that the blind are more accurate thanthe sighted in detecting loci of origin of sounds (Lessard et al.,1998; Röder et al., 1999). The scalp topography of MMN responseof Kujala et al., 1995a) did not differ between the two groups, how-ever. It, therefore, behaved differently from the N2b (Näätänenet al., 1982; for a review, see Näätänen and Gaillard, 1983) re-sponse elicited by auditory targets in an active discrimination con-dition, which had a scalp topography in the blind that wasposterior to that in the sighted (Kujala et al., 1992, 1995a,b). Thus,the MMN appears to be hard-wired in the central auditory systemof the blind, reflecting plastic changes within the auditory cortex,whereas the generators of the N2b, associated with attentionalprocesses in audition, may become part of the neural system nor-mally subserving vision. This was supported by imaging studiessuggesting occipital activity in the blind during auditory change-detection tasks (Kujala et al., 1995b, 2007, 2000b, 2005b).

15. Concluding discussion

This article has shown that central auditory processing, as in-dexed by the MMN and the MMNm, is affected in a wide rangeof different clinical conditions and in ageing. Most of these effectsare seen as indexing decreased auditory discrimination accuracy.In some cases, however, the duration of auditory short-term sen-sory memory, essential, for instance, in speech perception andunderstanding, is affected. This further decreases automatic dis-crimination when SOAs are prolonged. Moreover, these data alsoshow effects on mechanisms controlling for involuntary shifts ofattention (passive attention) or on those determining the charac-teristics of backward masking in different patient groups.

Consequently, this article has shown that the MMN provides aunique window to the neuropsychology of central auditory pro-cessing, and hence a possibility for the objective assessment ofauditory discrimination and sensory memory, in different patientgroups, which previously could be mainly inferred from behav-ioural performance only. This is a major improvement, in particularwhen considering the special communicational and motivationalproblems with patients, deducting from the reliability of the re-sults, and the fact that reliable behavioural measurements arenot at all possible in some clinical groups and often in small in-fants. These new possibilities in particular involve the improvedmonitoring of the patient’s state and the prediction of its future

development, for example, the prediction of coma or PVS outcomeand even that of the extent of the cognitive recovery of these pa-tients (during the next 2 years; Wijnen et al., 2007), or the moni-toring of, and predicting, the progress of schizophrenia.Moreover, importantly, very recent results of Bodatsch et al.(2011) showed that the duration MMN can predict, in individualsin schizophrenia-risk group, conversion to first-episode schizo-phrenia and even when this would occur in the period of 2 years.In addition, another recent study (Banati and Hickie, 2009) sug-gested that the duration and frequency MMNs might enable oneto online monitor local disease activity and neuronal death in pa-tients with schizophrenia.

Furthermore, in case of schizophrenia (see Lavoie et al., 2007;Horton et al., 2011; Javitt et al., 2008; Banati and Hickie, 2009;see also Berk et al., 2008a,b), and perhaps of other severe neuro-psychiatric, neurological and neurodevelopmental disorders, too,the MMN can be used as an online index of treatment efficacy.As far as dyslexia is concerned, such a use of the MMN in assessingthe effectiveness of different rehabilitation programmes is alreadywell established (Kujala et al., 2001). In addition, in the foregoing,we reviewed results of Särkämö et al. (2010a) demonstrating howthe MMNm can be used in evaluating the effectiveness of differentauditory stimulus material in an attempt at facilitating strokerecovery.

Some further clinically important uses of MMN data are, amongother things, the possibility to determine the dyslexia risk in new-borns and infants (Maurer et al., 2003; Leppänen et al., 2002; vanLeeuwen et al., 2006; Friedrich et al., 2004), the magnitude of cog-nitive and functional deficit associated with different clinical con-ditions (Näätänen et al., 2011a) and the degree of severity ofdifferent conditions (in Schizophrenia: Umbricht et al., 2006; Dev-rim-Ücok et al., 2008; Todd et al., 2008; Light and Braff, 2005a,b;Salisbury et al., 2002, 2007; Stuttering: Corbera et al., 2005; PTSD:Menning et al., 2008; Cleft Palate: Ceponiene et al., 1999, 2000,2002; Stroke and Aphasia: Ilvonen et al., 2003; Pettigrew et al.,2005; Särkämö et al., 2010b; Dyslexia: Baldeweg et al., 1999; Kuj-ala et al., 2001).

One might wonder why auditory discrimination is affected insuch a large number of different clinical conditions (Table 1),which indeed points to a possible common pathology shared bythese different conditions. A major part of this common pathologyappears to be found in the deficient functioning of the NMDAR sys-tem, as there is very strong evidence indicating that MMN defi-ciency reflects deficient NMDAR functioning (Javitt et al., 1996;Näätänen et al., 2011a). For example, sub-anaesthetic doses of ket-amine, an NMDAR antagonist, attenuated the MMN amplitude ofhealthy volunteers (Kreitschmann-Andermahr et al., 2001:MMNm; Umbricht et al., 2000, 2002), monkey (Javitt et al.,1996), rat (Tikhonravov et al., 2008, 2010) and mouse (Ehrlichmanet al., 2008), and caused schizophrenic kinds of psychotic experi-ences in normal subjects (Umbricht et al., 2000, 2002; see alsoSauer and Volz, 2000; Pang and Fowler, 1999). Furthermore, keta-mine disturbed the performance in a memory task in normal sub-jects in a manner similar to that normally observed inschizophrenia, as shown by Umbricht et al. (2000). Importantly,it was found by Umbricht et al. (2002) that the smaller was thesubject’s MMN amplitude for frequency and duration changes be-fore ketamine administration, the stronger were the psychoticexperiences caused by the drug. Therefore, the authors concludedthat the MMN in the normal state of consciousness indexes thevulnerability to disruption of the NMDAR system in normal healthysubjects. Consequently, the MMN appears to index the functionalstate of the NMDAR-mediated neurotransmission even in subjectsdemonstrating no psychopathology and may therefore ‘‘become auseful tool in studies on NMDAR functioning in humans’’ (Umb-richt et al., 2002).

Page 21: The mismatch negativity (MMN) – A unique window to ... et al (2011... · Invited Review The mismatch negativity (MMN) – A unique window to disturbed central auditory processing

444 R. Näätänen et al. / Clinical Neurophysiology 123 (2012) 424–458

Consequently, the MMN attenuation observed in a large num-ber of different clinical populations and conditions (Table 1) can,to a large extent, be accounted for by NMDAR dysfunction associ-ated with different clinical conditions. For instance, as already re-viewed, the dysfunction of the NMDAR system plays a centralrole in the brain pathology of schizophrenia and in particular inthe cognitive deficit in these patients (Javitt et al., 1996; Coyle,2006). Moreover, the cognitive decline occurring in several otherneurocognitive abnormalities might also be accounted for, at leastin part, by the dysfunctional NMDAR system (Näätänen et al.,2011a). Alzheimer’s disease, stroke, traumatic brain injury andeven normal ageing are accompanied by widespread dysfunctionsin the NMDAR system (Lipton and Rosenberg, 1994; Magnussonet al., 2010; Biegon et al., 2004; Dalkara et al., 1996; Izquidero,1991; McGeer and McGeer, 2010). For example, Dhawan et al.(2010) recently found that middle central artery occlusion in ratresulted in widespread decreases in NMDAR density in brain re-gions extending far beyond the infarct area, including even regionscontralateral to the lesion important to cognitive function. Dhawanet al. (2010), therefore, concluded that a persistent and widespreadloss of NMDARs may contribute to cognitive and other neurologicaldeficits in stroke patients, which cannot be localised to the side ofinfarction.

Importantly, this NMDAR functional deficiency may hence ac-count for, besides decreased discrimination accuracy, even the cog-nitive and functional decline occurring in a number of theseconditions (Table 8) (for a review, see Näätänen et al., 2011a). Itis, for instance, well established that the adequate functioning ofthe NMDAR system plays a crucial role in the initiation of long-term and working memories (Javitt et al., 1996; Newcomer et al.,1998). Therefore, the MMN and MMNm deficiency reflecting a defi-cient NMDAR function can also index cognition and its decrementin ageing and in different neurological and neuropsychiatric abnor-malities (Näätänen et al., 2011a). Consequently, as an easily acces-sible non-invasive measure of the functional condition of theNMDAR system, the MMN (MMNm) might play an important rolein future attempts at understanding what is common in the differ-ent neuropsychiatric diseases and abnormalities and what is spe-cific to each of them (Umbricht et al., 2003). These conclusionsare further supported by studies showing that pharmacologicalmanipulations enhancing cognition also enhance the MMN(MMNm) response (Baldeweg et al., 2006; Dunbar et al., 2007;Inami et al., 2005; Fisher et al., 2010; Harkrider and Hedrick,2005; Engeland et al., 2002; Knott et al., 2006, 2009) and vice versa(Pang and Fowler, 1999; Nakagome et al., 1998; Serra et al., 1996;Jääskeläinen et al., 1996a), and, further, that the MMN-cognitionrelationship holds even in normal adults (Light et al., 2007; Bazanaand Stelmack, 2002; Beauchamp and Stelmack, 2007; Troche et al.,2009, 2010) and children (Liu et al., 2007).

Optimising MMN protocols for clinical environments. Perhaps themost frequent issues associated with use of the MMN in clinicalenvironments are time required to record enough data to obtainan identifiable response and the expertise required to identifywhen a response is present or absent. By definition, the traditionalMMN protocol is very time consuming to obtain informationregarding even a single stimulus contrast. In a standard paradigm,the standard stimulus is presented 80–90% of the time, with devi-ant stimulus presentations, which evoke the MMN, randomlyinterspersed through the sequence. The introduction of the multi-ple-deviant paradigm for tones (Näätänen et al., 2004; Pakarinenet al., 2007, 2010; Fisher et al., 2011; Grimm et al., 2008; Jankowiakand Berti, 2007; see also Vaz Pato et al., 2002; Deacon et al., 1998)and for speech (Pettigrew et al., 2004b; Pakarinen et al., 2009;Sorokin et al., 2010; Lovio et al., 2009) and music (Vuust et al.,2011) provided a method for rapidly acquiring information on anumber of stimulus contrasts. This approach considerably in-

creases the amount of information that can be gathered within aspecific time period. Equally as important are methods that wouldfurther reduce the amount of time required to acquire sufficientdata that reach some objective criteria of identification.

One such approach has recently been described by Rahne et al.(2008). In this modification to standard averaging, individualepochs to stimulus presentation are sorted on the signal-to-noiseratio (SNR) to each individual epoch. This epoch with the highestSNR, that is, the lowest background noise, is added first to average.Individual epochs are then added to the average in rank orderbased on the background noise in each epoch. Thus, averaging iscontinued until the SNR is optimised. Objective or statistical detec-tion of these responses could then easily be achieved by using oneof a number of online algorithms involving randomisation or boot-strapping analyses such as those proposed by, for example, Pontonet al. (1997) or Fujioka et al. (2005). Moreover, further progress tothis end has been very recently made by Cong et al., 2009, 2010,2011); Burger et al. (2007); Kalyakin et al. (2007, 2008a,b, 2009)and Marco-Pallares et al. (2005).

Needless to say, a lot of work is still needed to bring the MMNmethodology to clinics as a tool of everyday patient work in whichreliable measurements have to be carried out at the level ofindividual patients (Bishop and McArthur, 2004; Boly et al.,2011; Taylor and Baldeweg, 2002; Lang et al., 1995), but studiesas those reviewed in the foregoing have shown that this goal isboth valuable and probably also attainable.

Acknowledgements

RN and KK were supported by Grant # 8332 from the EstonianScience Foundation. TK was supported by Grant # 128840 and RNby grant # 122745 from The Academy of Finland. CE was supportedby grants from ICREA Academia 2010 and CSD2007-00012,EUI2009-04806, PSI2009-08063, and SGR2009-11. We wish toacknowledge Ms. Piiu Lehmus’ skilful and patient text-editingwork.

References

Aaltonen O, Tuomainen J, Laine M, Niemi P. Cortical differences in tonal versusvowel processing as revealed by an ERP component called mismatch negativity.Brain Lang 1993;44:139–52.

Ahissar M. Dyslexia and the anchoring-deficit hypothesis. Trends Cogn Sci2007;11:458–65.

Ahissar M, Lubin Y, Putter-Katz H, Banai K. Dyslexia and the failure to form aperceptual anchor. Nat Neurosci 2006;9:1558–64.

Ahveninen J, Escera C, Polo MD, Grau C, Jääskeläinen IP. Acute and chronic effects ofalcohol on pre-attentive auditory processing as reflected by mismatchnegativity. Audiol Neuro-Otol 2000a;5:303–11.

Ahveninen J, Jääskeläinen IP, Osipova D, Huttunen MO, Ilmoniemi RJ, Kaprio J, et al.Inherited auditory-cortical dysfunction in twin pairs discordant forschizophrenia. Biol Psychiat 2006;60:612–20.

Ahveninen J, Jääskeläinen IP, Pekkonen E, Hallberg A, Hietanen M, Mäkelä R, et al.Suppression of mismatch negativity by backward masking predicts impairedworking-memory performance in alcoholics. Alcohol Clin Exp Res1999;23:1507–14.

Ahveninen J, Jääskeläinen IP, Pekkonen E, Hallberg A, Hietanen M, Näätänen R, et al.Increased distractibility by task-irrelevant sound changes in abstinentalcoholics. Alcohol Clin Exp Res 2000b;24:1850–4.

Akatsuka K, Wasaka T, Nakata H, Inui K, Hoshiyama M, Kakigi R. Mismatch responserelated to temporal discrimination of somatosensory stimulation. ClinNeuropshysiol 2005;116:1930–7.

Akatsuka K, Wasaka T, Nakata H, Kina T, Hoshiyama M, Tamura Y, et al. Objectiveexamination for two-point stimulation using a somatosensory oddballparadigm: an MEG study. Clin Neurophysiol 2007;118:403–11.

Alain C, Woods DL. Age-related changes in processing auditory stimuli during visualattention: evidence for deficits in inhibitory control and sensory memory.Psychol Aging 1999;14:507–19.

Alain C, Woods DL, Knight RT. A distributed cortical network for auditory sensorymemory in humans. Brain Res 1998;812:23–37.

Alain C, McDonald KL, Ostroff JM, Schneider B. Aging: a switch from automatic tocontrolled processing of sounds? Psychol Aging 2004;19:125–33.

Alexandrov AA, Karpina NV, Stankevich LN. Mismatch negativity in evoked brainpotentials in adolescents in normal conditions and attention deficit in response

Page 22: The mismatch negativity (MMN) – A unique window to ... et al (2011... · Invited Review The mismatch negativity (MMN) – A unique window to disturbed central auditory processing

R. Näätänen et al. / Clinical Neurophysiology 123 (2012) 424–458 445

to presentation of short-duration acoustic stimuli. Neurosci Behav Physiol2003;33:671–5.

Alho K, Cheour-Luhtanen M. Auditory discrimination in infants as revealed by themismatch negativity of the event-related brain potential. Dev Neuropsychol1997;13:157–65.

Alho K, Kujala T, Paavilainen P, Summala H, Näätänen R. Auditory processing in thevisual brain areas of the early blind: evidence from event-related potentials.Electroen Clin Neuro 1993;86:418–27.

Alho K, Sainio K, Sajaniemi N, Reinikainen K, Näätänen R. Event-related brainpotential of human newborns to pitch change of an acoustic stimulus. ElectroenClin Neuro 1990;77:151–5.

Alho K, Woods DL, Algazi A, Knight RT, Näätänen R. Lesions of frontal cortexdiminish the auditory mismatch negativity. Electroen Clin Neuro1994;91:353–62.

Alho K, Woods DL, Algazi A, Näätänen R. Intermodal selective attention II: effects ofattentional load on processing auditory and visual stimuli in central space.Electroen Clin Neuro 1992;82:356–68.

Alonso-Búa B, Dıaz F, Ferraces J. The contribution of AERPs (MMN and LDN) tostudying temporal vs. linguistic processing deficits in children with readingdifficulties. Int J Psychophysiol 2006;59:159–67.

Amenedo E, Diaz F. Automatic and effortful processes in auditory memory reflectedby event-related potentials. Age-related findings. Electroen Clin Neuro1998;108:361–9.

Amenedo E, Escera C. The accuracy of sound duration representation in the humanbrain determines the accuracy of behavioural perception. Eur J Neurosci2000;12:2570–4.

Amenedo E, Pazo-Alvarez P, Cadaveira F. Vertical asymmetries in pre-attentivedetection of changes in motion direction. Int J Psychophysiol 2007;64:184–9.

Andersson S, Barder HE, Hellvin T, Løvdahl H, Malt UF. Neuropsychological andelectrophysiological indices of neurocognitive dysfunction in bipolar II disorder.Bipol Disord 2008;10:888–99.

Astikainen P, Hietanen JK. Event-related potentials to task-irrelevant changes infacial expressions. Behav Brain Funct 2009;5:30.

Astikainen P, Ruusuvirta T, Korhonen T. Somatosensory event-related potentials inthe rabbit cerebral and cerebellar cortices: a correspondence with mismatchresponses in humans. Neurosci Lett 2001;298:222–4.

Astikainen P, Ruusuvirta T, Wikgren J, Penttonen M. Memory-based detection of raresound feature combinations in anaesthetized rats. NeuroReport 2006;17:1561–4.

Atienza M, Cantero JL. Complex sound processing during human REM sleep byrecovering information from long-term memory as revealed by the mismatchnegativity (MMN). Brain Res 2001;901:151–60.

Atienza M, Cantero JL, Gomez CM. The mismatch negativity component reveals thesensory memory during REM sleep in humans. Neurosci Lett 1997;237:21–4.

Atienza M, Cantero JL, Gomez CM. Decay time of the auditory sensory memory traceduring wakefulness and REM sleep. Psychophysiology 2000;37:485–93.

Atienza M, Cantero JL, Dominguez-Marin E. Mismatch negativity (MMN): anobjective measure of sensory memory and long-lasting memories during sleep.Int J Psychophysiol 2002;46:215–25.

Auther LL, Wertz RT, Miller TA, Kirshner HS. Relationships among the mismatchnegativity (MMN) response, auditory comprehension, and site of lesion inaphasic adults. Aphasiology 2000;14:461–70.

Baker K, Baldeweg T, Sivagnanasundaram S, Scambler P, Skuse D. COMTVal108/

1581653 Met polymorphism modifies mismatch negativity and cognitivefunction in 22q11 deletion syndrome. Biol Psychiat 2005;58:23–31.

Baldeweg T, Klugman A, Gruzelier JH, Hirsch SR. Impairment in frontal but nottemporal components of mismatch negativity in schizophrenia. Int JPsychophysiol 2002;43:111–22.

Baldeweg T, Klugman A, Gruzelier J, Hirsch SR. Mismatch negativity potentials andcognitive impairment in schizophrenia. Schizophr Res 2004;69:203–17.

Baldeweg T, Richardson A, Watkins S, Foale C, Gruzelier J. Impaired auditoryfrequency discrimination in dyslexia detected with mismatch evokedpotentials. Ann Neurol 1999;45:1–9.

Baldeweg T, Wong D, Stephen KE. Nicotinic modulation of human auditory sensorymemory: evidence from mismatch negativity potentials. Int J Psychophysiol2006;59:49–58.

Bamiou DE, Liasis A, Boyd S, Cohen M, Raglan E. Central auditory processingdisorder as the presenting manifestation of subtle brain pathology. J Audiol2000;39:168–72.

Banai K, Ahissar M. Psychoacoustics and working memory in dyslexia. In: Fourthconference on plasticity of the central auditory system and processing ofcomplex acoustic signals, July 07–10, 2003. Plasticity and Signal Representationin the Auditory System, Prague, Czech Republic; 2005. p. 233–42.

Banati RB. Neuropathological imaging: in vivo detection of glial activation as ameasure of disease and adaptive change in the brain. Br Med Bull2003;65:121–31.

Banati R, Hickie IB. Therapeutic signposts: using biomarkers to guide bettertreatment of schizophrenia and other psychotic disorders. Med J Aust2009;190:S26–32.

Bar-Haim Y, Marshall PJ, Fox NA, Schorr EA, Gordon-Salant S. Mismatch negativityin socially withdrawn children. Biol Psychiat 2003;54:17–24.

Barry JG, Hardiman MJ, Line E, White KB, Yasin I, Bishop DVM. Duration of auditorysensory memory in parents of children with SLI: a mismatch negativity study.Brain Lang 2008;104:75–88.

Barry RJ, Johnstone SJ, Clarke AR. A review of electrophysiology in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: II. Event-related potentials. Clin Neurophysiol2003;114:184–98.

Baudena P, Halgren E, Heit G, Clarke JM. Intracerebral potentials to rare target anddistractor auditory and visual stimuli. III. Frontal cortex. ElectroencephalogrClin Neurophysiol 1995;94:251–64.

Bazana PG, Stelmack RM. Intelligence and information processing during anauditory discrimination task with backward masking: an event-relatedpotential analysis. J Pers Soc Psychol 2002;83:998–1008.

Beauchamp CM, Stelmack RM. The chronometry of mental ability: an event-relatedpotential analysis of an auditory oddball discrimination task. Intelligence2006;34:571–86.

Becker F, Reinvang I. Mismatch negativity (MMN) to tone duration and speechsound changes in aphasics. Brain Lang 2007;100:69–78.

Bellis TJ, Nicol T, Kraus N. Aging affects hemispheric asymmetry in the neuralrepresentation of speech sounds. J Neurosci 2000;20:791–7.

Benasich AA, Choudhury N, Friedman JT, Realpe-Bonilla T, Chojnowska C, Gou Z. Theinfant as a prelinguistic model for language learning impairments: predictingfrom event-related potentials to behaviour. Neuropsychologia 2006;44:396–411.

Benasich AA, Tallal P. Infant discrimination of rapid auditory cues predicts laterlanguage impairment. Behav Brain Res 2002;136:31–49.

Berk M, Copolov DL, Dean O, Lu K, Jeavons S, Schapkaitz I, et al. N-Acetyl Cysteine fordepressive symptoms in bipolar disorder-a double-blind randomized placebo-control trial. Biol Psychiat 2008a;64:468–75.

Berk M, Copolov D, Dean O, Lu K, Jeavons S, Schapkaitz I, et al. N-acetyl cysteine as aglutathione precursor for schizophrenia – a double-blind, randomized, placebo-controlled trial. Biology 2008b;64:361–8.

Berti S, Schröger E. A comparison of auditory and visual distraction effects:behavioural and event-related indices. Cogn Brain Res 2001;10:265–73.

Berti S, Schröger E. Distraction effects in vision: behavioural and event-relatedpotentials indices. NeuroReport 2004;15:665–9.

Berti S, Schröger E. Visual distraction: a behavioural and event-related brainpotential study in humans. NeuroReport 2006;2:151–5.

Bertoli S, Smurzynski J, Probst R. Temporal resolution in young and elderly subjectsas measured by mismatch negativity and a psychoacoustic gap detection task.Clin Neurophysiol 2002;113:396–406.

Bertoli S, Smurzynski J, Probst R. Effects of age, age-related hearing loss, andcontralateral cafeteria noise on the discrimination of small frequency changes:psychoacoustic and electrophysiological measures. J Assoc Res Otolaryng2005;6:207–22.

Beste C, Saft C, Gunturkun O, Falkenstein M. Increased cognitive functioning insymptomatic Huntington’s Disease as revealed by behavioral and event-relatedpotential indices of auditory sensory memory and attention. J Neurosci2008;28:11695–702.

Biegon A, Fry PA, Paden CM, Alexandrowitch A, Tsenter J, Shohami E. Dynamicchanges in N-methyl-D-aspartate receptors after closed head injury in mice:implications for treatment of neurological and cognitive deficits. P Natl Acad SciUSA 2004;101:5117–22.

Bishop DVM. Curing dyslexia and attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder by trainingmotor co-ordination: miracle or myth? J Paediatr Child H 2007a;43:653–5.

Bishop DVM. Genes, cognition, and communication. Insights fromneurodevelopmental disorders. The Year in Cognitive Neuroscience 2009. AnnNY Acad Sci 2009;1156:1–18.

Bishop DVM. Using mismatch negativity to study central auditory processing indevelopmental language and literacy impairments: where are we, and whereshould we be going? Psychol Bull 2007b;133:651–72.

Bishop DVM, McArthur GM. Immature cortical responses to auditory stimuli inspecific language impairment: evidence from ERPs to rapid tone sequences.DevSci 2004;7:F11–8.

Bisiachi PS, Mento G, Suppiej A. Cortical auditory processing in preterm newborns:an ERP study. Biol Psychol 2009;82:176–85.

Blitz U, Gust K, Spitzer M, Kiefer M. Phonological deficit in school children isreflected in the mismatch negativity. NeuroReport 2007;18:911–5.

Boatman DF, Trescher WH, Smith C, Ewen J, Los J, Wied HM, et al. Cortical auditorydysfunction in benign rolandic epilepsy. Epilepsia 2008;49:1018–26.

Bodatsch M, Ruhrmann S, Wagner M, Müller R, Schultze-Lutter F, Frommann I, et al.Prediction of psychosis by mismatch negativity. Biol Psychiat 2011;69:959–66.

Bogerts B. Recent advances in the neuropathology of schizophrenia. Schizophr Bull1993;19:431–45.

Boly M, Garrido MI, Gosseries O, Bruno M-A, Boveroux P, Schnakers C, et al.Preserved feedforward but impaired top-down processes in the vegetativestate. Science 2011;332:858–62.

Bomba MD, Pang EW. Cortical auditory evoked potentials in autism: a review. Int JPsychophysiol 2004;53:161–9.

Bonnel AC, Mottron L, Peretz I, Trudel M, Gallun E, Bonnel AM. Enhanced pitchsensitivity in individuals with autism: a signal detection analysis. J CognitNeurosci 2003;15:1–10.

Bonte ML, Mitterer H, Zellagui N, Poelmans H, Blomert L. Auditory cortical tuning tostatistical regularities in phonology. Clin Neurophysiol 2005;116:2765–74.

Bonte ML, Poelmans H, Blomert L. Deviant neurophysiological responses tophonological regularities in speech in dyslexic children. Neuropsychologia2007;45:1427–37.

Borghetti D, Pizzanelli C, Maritato P, Fabbrini M, Jensen S, Iudice A, et al. Mismatchnegativity analysis in drug-resistant epileptic patients implanted with vagusnerve stimulator. Brain Res Bull 2007;73:81–5.

Born J, Bothor R, Pietrowsky R, Fehm-Wolfsdorf G, Pauschinger P, Fehm HL.Influences of vasopressin and oxytocin on human event-related potentials in anattention task. J Psychophysiol 1987a;4:351–60.

Page 23: The mismatch negativity (MMN) – A unique window to ... et al (2011... · Invited Review The mismatch negativity (MMN) – A unique window to disturbed central auditory processing

446 R. Näätänen et al. / Clinical Neurophysiology 123 (2012) 424–458

Born J, Brüninger W, Fehm-Wolsdorf G, Pauschinger P, Fehm HL. Dose-dependentinfluences on electrophysiological signs of attention in humans afterneuropeptide ACTH 4-10. Exp Brain Res 1987b;67:85–92.

Born J, Fehm-Wolsdorf G, Lutzenberger W, Voigt KH, Fehm HL. Vasopressin andelectrophysiological signs of attention in man. Peptides 1986;7:189–93.

Born J, Pietrowsky R, Fehm HL. Neuropsychological effects on vasopressin in healthyhumans. Prog Brain Res 1998;119:619–43.

Böttcher-Gandor C, Ullsperger P. Mismatch negativity in event-related potentials ofauditory stimuli as a function of varying interstimulus interval.Psychophysiology 1992;29:546–50.

Bradlow AR, Kraus N, Nicol TG, McGee TJ, Cunningham J, Zecker SG. Effects oflengthened formant transition duration on discrimination and neuralrepresentation of synthetic syllables by normal and learning-disabledchildren. J Acoust Soc Am 1999;106:2086–96.

Braff DL, Light GA. Preattentional and attentional cognitive deficits as targets fortreating schizophrenia. Psychopharmacology 2004;174:75–85.

Bramon E, Croft RJ, McDonald C, Virdi GK, Gruzelier JG, Baldeweg T, et al. Mismatchnegativity in schizophrenia: a family study. Schizophr Res 2004;67:1–10.

Brattico E, Kujala T, Tervaniemi M, Alku P, Ambrosi L, Monitillo V. Long-termexposure to occupational noise alters the cortical organization of soundprocessing. Clin Neurophysiol 2005;116:190–203.

Brattico E, Tervaniemi M, Välimäki V, van Zuijen T, Peretz I. Cortical correlates ofacquired deafness to dissonance. Ann NY Acad Sci 2003;999:158–60.

Braun A, McArdie J, Jones J, Nechaev V, Zalewski C, Brewer C, et al. Tune deafness:processing melodic errors outside of conscious awareness as reflected bycomponents of the auditory ERP. PloS ONE 2008;3:e2349.

Brockhaus-Dumke A, Tendolkar I, Pukrop R, Schultze-Lutter F, Klosterkötter J,Ruhrmann S. Impaired mismatch negativity generation in prodromal subjectsand patients with schizophrenia. Schizophr Res 2005;73:297–310.

Brønnick KS, Nordby H, Larsen JP, Aarsland D. Disturbance of automatic auditorychange detection in dementia associated with parkinson’s disease: a mismatchnegativity study. Neurobiol Aging 2010;31:104–13.

Bruder J, Leppänen PHT, Bartling J, Csépe V, Démonet J-F, Schulte-Körne G. Childrenwith dyslexia reveal abnormal native language representations: evidence froma study of mismatch negativity. Psychophysiology 2011a;48:1107–18.

Bruder J, Leppänen PHT, Bartling J, Csépe V, Démonet J-F, Schulte-Körne G. Aninvestigation of prototypical and atypical within-category vowels and non-speech analogues on cortical auditory evoked related potentials (AERPs) in 9year old children. Int J Psychophysiol 2011b;79:106–17.

Burger M, Hoppe U, Kummer P, Lohscheller J, Eysholdt U, Dollinger M. Wavelet-based analysis of MMN responses in children. Biomed Tech 2007;52:111–6.

Campbell K, Bell I, Bastien C. Evoked potential measures of information processingduring natural sleep. In: Broughton RJ, Ogilvie RD, editors. Sleep arousal andperformance. Cambridge, MA: Birkhauser, Boston; 1991. p. 88–116.

Cansino S. Episodic memory decay along the adult lifespan: a review of behavioraland neurophysiological evidence. Int J Psychophysiol 2009;71:64–9.

Carral V, Huotilainen M, Ruusuvirta T, Fellman V, Näätänen R, Escera C. A kind ofauditory ‘primitive intelligence’ already present at birth. Eur J Neurosci2005;21:3201–4.

Catts SV, Shelley A-M, Ward PB, Liebert B, McConaghy N, Andrews S, et al. Brainpotential evidence for an auditory sensory memory deficit in schizophrenia. AmJ Psychiat 1995;152:213–9.

Celsis P, Boulanouar K, Doyon B, Ranjeva JP, Berry I, Nespoulous JL, et al. DifferentialfMRI responses in the left posterior superior temporal gyrus and leftsupramarginal gyrus to habituation and change detection in syllables andtones. NeuroImage 1999;9:135–44.

Ceponiene R, Haapanen M-L, Ranta R, Näätänen R, Hukki J. Auditory sensoryimpairment in children with oral clefts as indexed by event-related potentials:a review. J Craniofac Surg 2002;13:554–66.

Ceponiene R, Hukki J, Cheour M, Haapanen M-L, Koskinen M, Alho K, et al.Dysfunction of the auditory cortex persists in infants with certain cleft types.Dev Med Child Neurol 2000;42:258–65.

Ceponiene R, Hukki J, Cheour M, Haapanen M-L, Ranta R, Näätänen R. Corticalauditory dysfunction in children with oral clefts: relation with cleft type. ClinNeurophysiol 1999;110:1921–6.

Ceponiene R, Lepistö T, Shestakova A, Vanhala R, Näätänen R, Yaguchi K. Speechsound-selective auditory impairment in infantile autism: can perceive but willnot attend. P Natl Acad Sci USA 2003;100:5567–72.

Chang Y, Xu J, Shi N, Zhang B, Zhao L. Dysfunction of processing task-irrelevantemotional faces in major depressive disorder patients revealed by expression-related visual MMN. Neurosci Lett 2010;472:33–7.

Cheour M, Ceponiene R, Hukki J, Haapanen M-L, Näätänen R, Alho K. Braindysfunction in neonates with cleft palate revealed by the mismatch negativity(MMN). Electroen Clin Neuro 1999;110:324–8.

Cheour M, Ceponiene R, Lehtokoski A, Luuk A, Allik J, Alho K, et al. Development oflanguage-specific phoneme representations in the infant brain. Nature Neurosci1998b;1:351–3.

Cheour M, Haapanen M-L, Ceponiene R, Hukki J, Ranta S, Näätänen R. Mismatchnegativity (MMN) as an index of auditory sensory memory deficit in cleft-palateand catch-syndrome children. NeuroReport 1998a;9:2709–12.

Cheour M, Haapanen M-L, Hukki J, Ceponiene R, Kurjenluoma S, Alho K, et al. Thefirst neurophysiological evidence for cognitive brain dysfunctions in CATCHchildren. NeuroReport 1997;8:1785–7.

Cheour M, Leppänen P, Kraus N. Mismatch negativity (MMN) as a tool forinvestigating auditory discrimination and sensory memory in infants andchildren. Clin Neurophysiol 2000;111:4–16.

Cheour M, Martynova O, Näätänen R, Erkkola R, Sillanpää M, Kero P, et al. Speechsounds learned by sleeping newborns. Nature 2002a;415:599–600.

Cheour M, Ceponiene R, Leppänen P, Alho K, Kujala T, Renlund M, et al. Auditorysensory memory trace decays rapidly in newborns. Scand J Psychol2002b;43:33–9.

Cheour-Luhtanen M, Alho K, Sainio K, Rinne T, Reinikainen K, Pohjavuori M, et al.The ontogenetically earliest discriminative response of the human brain.Psychophysiol Special Rep 1996;33:478–81.

Choudhury N, Benasich AA. Maturation of auditory evoked potentials from 6 to 48months: prediction to 3 and 4 year language and cognitive abilities. ClinNeurophysiol 2011;122:320–38.

Cong F, Kalyakin I, Huttunen-Scott T, Li H, Lyytinen H, Ristaniemi T. Single-trialbased independent component analysis on mismatch negativity of children. IntJ Neural Syst 2010;20:279–92.

Cong F, Ristaniemi T, Lyytinen H. A potential real-time procedure to evaluatecorrelation of recordings among single trials (CoRaST) for mismatch negativity(MMN) with Fourier transformation. Clin Neurophysiol 2011;4:725–30.

Cong F, Sipola T, Huttunen-Scott T, Xu X, Ristaniemi T, Lyytinen H. Hilbert-Huangversus Morlet wavelet transformation on mismatch negativity of children inuninterrupted sound paradigm. Nonlinear Biomed Phys 2009;3:1.

Cooper RJ, Todd J, McGill K, Michie PT. Auditory sensory memory and the agingbrain: a mismatch negativity study. Neurobiol Aging 2006;27:752–62.

Corbera S, Corral M-J, Escera C, Idiazábal MA. Abnormal speech soundrepresentation in persistent developmental stuttering. Neurology 2005;65:1246–52.

Corbera S, Escera C, Artigas J. Impaired duration mismatch negativity indevelopmental dyslexia. NeuroReport 2006;17:1051–5.

Cornwell BR, Baas JMP, Johnson L, Holroyd T, Carver FW, Lissek S, et al. Neuralresponses to auditory stimulus deviance under threat of electric shock revealedby spatially-filtered magnetoencephalography. NeuroImage 2007;37:383–9.

Coyle JT. Glutamate and schizophrenia: beyond the dopamine hypothesis. Cell MolNeurobiol 2006;26:365–84.

Csépe V, Dyslexia. Different Brain, Different behaviour. In: Csépe V, editor.Neuropsychology and cognition. New York: Kluwer Academic Publisher;2002. p. 81–112.

Csépe V, Karmos G, Molnár M. Evoked potential correlates of stimulus devianceduring wakefulness and sleep in cat: Animal model of mismatch negativity.Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol 1987;66:571–8.

Csépe V, Karmos G, Molnár M. Subcortical evoked potential correlates of sensorymismatch process in cats. In: Bajic M, editor. Advances inbiosciences. Oxford: Pergamon Press; 1988. p. 43–6.

Csépe V, Karmos G, Molnár M, Winkler I. Cortical and intracerebral mismatchnegativity - animal-model of stimulus comparison. Int J Psychophysiol1989;7:166–8.

Csépe V, Osman-Sági J, Molnár M, Gósy M. Impaired speech perception in aphasicpatients: event-related potential and neurophysiological assessment.Neuropsychologia 2001;39:1194–208.

Czamara D, Bruder J, Becker J, Bartling J, Hoffman P, Ludwig KU, et al. Association ofa rare variant with mismatch negativity in a region between KIAA0319 andDCDC2 in dyslexia. Behav Genet 2011;41:110–9.

Czigler I. Visual mismatch negativity: violation of nonattended environmentalregularities. J Psychophysiol 2007;21:224–30.

Czigler I, Csibra G. Event-related potentials in a visual discrimination task: negativewaves related to detection and attention. Psychophysiology 1990;27:669–76.

Czigler I, Csibra G. Event-related potentials and the identification of deviant visualstimuli. Psychophysiology 1992;29:471–84.

Czigler I, Pató L. Unnoticed regularity violation elicits change-related brain activity.Biol Psychol 2009;80:339–47.

Czigler I, Balázs L, Pató LG. Visual change detection: event-related potentials aredependent on stimulus location in humans. Neurosci Lett 2004;364:149–53.

Czigler I, Balázs L, Winkler I. Memory-based detection of task-irrelevant visualchanges. Psychophysiology 2002;39:869–73.

Czigler I, Csibra G, Csontos A. Age and inter-stimulus interval effects on event-related potentials to frequent and infrequent auditory-stimuli. Biol Psychol1992;33:195–206.

Czigler I, Weisz J, Winkler I. ERPs and deviance detection: visual mismatchnegativity to repeated visual stimuli. Neurosci Lett 2006a;401:178–82.

Czigler I, Winkler I, Pató L, Várnagy A, Weisz J, Balázs L. Visual temporal window ofintegration as revealed by the mismatch negativity event-related potential tostimulus omission. Brain Res 2006b;1104:129–40.

Dalkara T, Ayata C, Demirci M, Erdemli G, Onur R. Effects of cerebral ischemia on N-methyl-D-aspartate and dihydropyridine-sensitive calcium currents: anelectrophysiological study in the rat hippocampus in situ. Stroke1996;27:127–33.

Daltrozzo J, Wioland N, Mutschler V, Kotchoubey B. Predicting coma and other lowresponsive patients’ outcome using event-related potentials: a meta-analysis.Clin Neurophysiol 2007;118:606–14.

Davalos DB, Kisley MA, Polk SD, Ross RG. Mismatch negativity in detection ofinterval duration deviation in schizophrenia. NeuroReport 2003;14:1283–6.

Deacon D, Nousak JM, Pilotti M, Ritter W, Yang CM. Automatic change detection:does the auditory system use representations of individual stimulus features orgestalts? Psychophysiology 1998;35:413–9.

Dehaene-Lambertz G. Electrophysiological correlates of categorical phonemeperception in adults. NeuroReport 1997;8:919–24.

Dehaene-Lambertz G, Pena M, Christphe A, Landrieu P. Phoneme perception in aneonate with left sylvian infarct. Brain Lang 2004;88:26–38.

Page 24: The mismatch negativity (MMN) – A unique window to ... et al (2011... · Invited Review The mismatch negativity (MMN) – A unique window to disturbed central auditory processing

R. Näätänen et al. / Clinical Neurophysiology 123 (2012) 424–458 447

Deouell LY. The frontal generator of the mismatch negativity revisited. JPsychophysiol 2007;27:188–203.

Deouell LY, Hämäläinen H, Bentin S. Unilateral neglect after right-hemispheredamage: contributions from event-related potentials. Audiol Neuro-Otol2000a;5:225–34.

Deouell LY, Bentin S, Soroker N. Electrophysiological evidence for an early (pre-attentive) information processing deficit in patients with right hemispheredamage and unilateral neglect. Brain 2000b;123:353–65.

Desjardin RN, Trainor LJ, Hevenor S, Polak CP. Using mismatch negativity tomeasure auditory temporal resolution thresholds. NeuroReport1999;10:2079–82.

de Tommaso M, Guido M, Libro G, Losito L, Difruscolo O, Sardaro M, et al. Interictallack of habituation of mismatch negativity in migraine. Cephalalgia2004;24:663–8.

Devrim-Ücok M, Keskin-Ergen HY, Ücok A. Mismatch negativity at acute and post-acute phases of first-episode schizophrenia. Eur Arch Psy Clin N2008;258:179–85.

Dhawan J, Benveniste H, Nawrocky M, Smith SD, Biegon A. Transient focal ischemiaresults in persistent and widespread neuroinflammation and loss of glutamateNMDA receptors. NeuroImage 2010;51:599–605.

Diaz F, Zurron M. Auditory evoked potentials in Down’s syndrome. Electroen ClinNeuro 1995;96:526–37.

Dick BD, Connolly JF, McGrath PJ, Finley GA, Stroink G, Houlihan ME, et al. Thedisruptive effect of chronic pain on mismatch negativity. Clin Neurophysiol2003;114:1497–506.

Dittmann-Balcar A, Juptner M, Jentzen W, Schall U. Dorsolateral prefrontal cortexactivation during automatic auditory duration mismatch processing in humans:a positron emission tomography study. Neurosci Lett 2001;308:119–212.

Doeller CF, Opitz B, Mecklinger A, Krick C, Reith W, Schröger E. Prefrontal cortexinvolvement in preattentive auditory deviance detection: neuroimaging andelectrophysiological evidence. NeuroImage 2003;20:1270–82.

Dolan RJ, Bench CJ, Liddle PF, Friston KJ, Frith CD, Grasby M, et al. Dorsolateralprefrontal cortex dysfunction in the major psychoses – symptom or diseasespecificity. J Neurol Neurosur Ps 1993;56:1290–4.

Draganova R, Eswaran H, Murphy P, Huotilainen M, Lowerey C, Preissl H. Soundfrequency change detection in fetuses and newborns, amagnetoencephalographic study. NeuroImage 2005;28:354–61.

Draganova R, Eswaran H, Murphy P, Lowery C, Preissl H. Serialmagnetoencephalographic study of fetal and newborn auditory discriminativeevoked responses. Early Hum Dev 2007;83:199–207.

Dulude L, Labelle A, Knott VJ. Acute nicotine alteration of sensory memoryimpairment in smokers with schizophrenia. J Clin Psychopharmacol2010;5:541–8.

Duman O, Kizilay F, Fettahoglu C, Ozkaynak S, Haspolat S. Electrophysiologic andneuropsychologic evaluation of patients with centrotemporal spikes. Int JNeurosci 2008;118:118–22.

Dunbar G, Boieijinga PH, Demazières A, Cisterni C, Kuchibhatla R, Wesnes K, et al.Effects of TC.1734 (AZD3480), a selective neuronal nicotinic receptorantagonist, on cognitive performance and the EEG of young healthy malevolunteers. Psychopharmacology 2007;191:919–29.

Duncan CC, Kaye WH. Effects of clonidine on event-related potential measures ofinformation processing. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophys 1987(Suppl. 40):527–31.

Duncan CC, Barry RJ, Connolly JF, Fischer C, Michie PT, Näätänen R, et al. Event-related potentials in clinical research: guidelines for eliciting, recording, andquantifying mismatch negativity, P300, and N400. Clin Neurophysiol2009;120:1883–908.

Dunn MA, Gomes H, Gravel J. Mismatch negativity in children with autism andtypical development. J Autism Dev Disord 2008;38:52–71.

Ehrlichman RS, Maxwell CR, Majumdar S, Siegel SJ. Deviance-elicited changes inevent-related potentials are attenuated by ketamine in mice. J Cognit Neurosci2008;20:1403–14.

Ehrlichman R, Luminais S, White S, Rudnick ND, Ma N, Dow HC, et al. Neuregulin 1transgenic mice display reduced mismatch negativity, contextual fearconditioning and social interactions. Brain Res 2009;1294:116–27.

Engeland C, Mahoney C, Mohr E, Ilivitsky V, Knott VJ. Acute nicotine effects onauditory sensory memory in tacrine-treated and nontreated patients withAlzheimer’s disease. An event-related potential study. Pharmacol Biochem Be2002;72:457–64.

Escera C, Alho K, Schröger E, Winkler I. Involuntary attention and distractibility asevaluated with event-related brain potentials. Audiol Neuro-Otol2000a;5:151–66.

Escera C, Corral MJ. Role of mismatch negativity and novelty-P3 in involuntaryauditory attention. J Psychophysiol 2007;21:251–64.

Escera C, Corral MJ, Yago E. An electrophysiological and behavioral investigation ofinvoluntary attention towards auditory frequency, duration and intensitychanges. Cogn Brain Res 2002;14:325–32.

Escera C, Grau C. Short-term replicability of the mismatch negativity. Electroen ClinNeuro 1996;100:549–54.

Escera C, Yago E, Polo MD, Grau C. The individual replicability of mismatchnegativity at short and long inter-stimulus intervals. Clin Neurophysiol2000b;111:546–51.

Fabiani M, Low KA, Wee E, Sable JJ, Gratton G. Reduced suppression or labilememory? Mechanisms of inefficient filtering of irrelevant information in olderadults. J Cognit Neurosci 2006;18:637–50.

Fein G, Whitlow B, Finn P. Mismatch negativity: no difference between controls andabstinent alcoholics. Alcohol Clin Exp Res 2004;28:137–42.

Fellman V, Huotilainen M. Cortical event-related potentials in newborn infants.Semin Fetal Neonat Med 2006;11:452–8.

Fellman V, Kushnerenko E, Mikkola K, Ceponiene R, Leipälä J, Näätänen R. Atypicalauditory event-related potentials in preterm infants – a possible sign ofcognitive dysfunction? Pediatr Res 2004;56:291–7.

Ferri R, Elia M, Agarwal N, Lanuzza B, Musumeci SA, Pennisi G. The mismatchnegativity and the P3a components of the auditory event-related potentials inautistic low-functioning subjects. Clin Neurophysiol 2003;114:1671–80.

Fischer C, Luauté J. Evoked potentials for the prediction of vegetative state in theacute stage of coma. Neuropsychol Rehab 2005;15:372–80.

Fischer C, Dailler F, Morlet D. Novelty P3 elicited by the subject’s own name incomatose patients. Clin Neurophysiol 2008;19:2224–30.

Fischer C, Luauté J, Adelaine P, Morlet D. Predictive value of sensory and cognitiveevoked potentials for awakening from coma. Neurology 2004;63:669–73.

Fischer C, Luauté J, Némoz C, Morlet D, Kirkorian G, Mauguière F. Improvedprediction of awakening or nonawakening from severe anoxic coma using tree-based classification analysis. Crit Care Med 2006a;34:1–5.

Fischer C, Luauté J, Némoz C, Morlet D, Kirkorian G, Mauguière F. Editorial response:evoked potentials can be used as prognosis factor for awakening. Crit Care Med2006b;34:2025.

Fischer C, Morlet D, Bouchet P, Luaute J, Jourdan C, Salord F. Mismatch negativityand late auditory evoked potentials in comatose patients. Clin Neurophysiol1999;110:1601–10.

Fischer C, Morlet D, Giard M-H. Mismatch negativity and N100 in comatosepatients. Audiol Neuro-Otol 2000;5:192–7.

Fisher AE, Barnes GR, Hillebrand A, Holliday IE, Witton C, Richards IL. Abnormalityof mismatch negativity in response to tone omission in dyslexic adults. BrainRes 2006;1077:90–8.

Fisher DJ, Grant B, Smith DM, Borracci G, Labelle A, Knott VJ. Effects of auditoryhallucinations on the mismatch negativity (MMN) in schizophrenia asmeasured by a modified ‘‘optimal’’ multi-feature paradigm. Int JPsychophysiol 2011;81:245–51.

Fisher DJ, Labelle A, Knott VJ. Auditory hallucinations and the mismatch negativity:processing speech and non-speech sounds in schizophrenia. Int J Psychophysiol2008;70:3–15.

Fisher DJ, Labelle A, Knott VJ. The right profile: mismatch negativity inschizophrenia with and without auditory hallucinations as measured by amulti-feature paradigm. Clin Neurophysiol 2008;119:909–21.

Fisher DJ, Lynne Scott T, Shah DK, Prise S, Thompson M, Knott VJ. Light up and see:enhancement of the visual mismatch negativity (vMMN) by nicotine. Brain Res2010;1313:162–71.

Fisher DJ, Grant B, Smith DM, Knott WJ. Effects of deviant probability on the‘‘optimal’’ multi-feature mismatch negativity (MMN) paradigm. Int JPsychophysiol 2011;79:311–5.

Franken IHA, Nijs I, Van Strien JW. Impulsivity affects mismatch negativity (MMN)measures. Biol Psychol 2005;70:161–7.

Friedrich M, Weber C, Friederici AD. Electrophysiological evidence for delayedmismatch response in infants at-risk for specific language impairment.Psychophysiology 2004;41:772–82.

Frodl-Bauch T, Kathmann N, Möller H-J, Hegerl U. Dipole localization and test–retest reliability of frequency and duration mismatch negativity generatorprocesses. Brain Topogr 1997;10:3–8.

Froyen D, van Atteveldt N, Blomert L. Exploring the role of low level visualprocessing in letter-speech sound integration: a visual MMN study. Front IntegrNeurosci 2010;4:9.

Froyen D, van Atteveldt N, Bonte M, Blomert L. Crossmodal enhancement of theMMN to speech sounds indicates early and automatic integration of letters andspeech sounds. Neurosci Lett 2008;430:23–8.

Froyen D, Bonte M, van Atteveldt N, Blomert L. The long road to automation:neurocognitive development of letter-speech sound processing. J CognitNeurosci 2009;21:567–80.

Froyen D, Willems G, Blomert L. Evidence for a specific cross-modal associationdeficit in dyslexia: an electrophysiological study of letter-speech soundprocessing. Dev Sci 2010;14:635–8.

Fujioka T, Trainor LJ, Ross B, Kakigi R, Pantev C. Automatic encoding of polyphonicmelodies in musicians and non-musicians. J Cognit Neurosci 2005;17:1578–92.

Ge Y, Wu J, Sun X, Zhang K. Enhanced mismatch negativity in adolescents withposttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD). Int J Psychophysiol 2011;79:231–5.

Gaeta H, Friedman D, Ritter W, Cheng J. An event-related potential study of age-related changes in sensitivity to stimulus deviance. Neurobiol Aging1998;19:447–59.

Gaeta H, Friedman D, Ritter W, Cheng J. Changes in sensitivity to stimulus deviancein Alzheimer’s disease: an ERP perspective. NeuroReport 1999;10:281–7.

Gaeta H, Friedman D, Ritter W, Cheng J. An event-related potential evaluation ofinvoluntary attentional shifts in young and older adults. Psychol Aging2001;16:55–68.

Gaeta H, Friedman D, Ritter W, Hunt G. Age-related changes in neural tracegeneration of rule-based auditory features. Neurobiol Aging 2002;23:443–55.

Ge Y, Wu J, Sun X, Zhang K. Enhanced mismatch negativity in adolescents withposttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD). Int J Psychophysiol 2011;79:231–5.

Gene-Cos N, Pottinger R, Barrett G, Trimble M, Ring HA. A comparative study ofmismatch negativity (MMN) in epilepsy and non-epilectic seizures. EpilepticDisord 2005;7:363–72.

Page 25: The mismatch negativity (MMN) – A unique window to ... et al (2011... · Invited Review The mismatch negativity (MMN) – A unique window to disturbed central auditory processing

448 R. Näätänen et al. / Clinical Neurophysiology 123 (2012) 424–458

Giard MH, Perrin F, Pernier J, Bouchet P. Brain generators implicated in processing ofauditory stimulus deviance: a topographic event-related potential study.Psychophysiology 1990;27:627–40.

Giese-Davis JE, Miller GA, Knight RA. Memory template comparison processes inanhedonia and dysthymia. Psychophysiology 1993;30:646–56.

Gil R, Zai L, Neau JP, Jonveaux T, Agbo C, Rosolacc T, et al. Event-related auditoryevoked potentials and multiple sclerosis. Electroen Clin Neuro 1993;88:182–7.

Glass E, Sachse S, von Suchodoletz W. Auditory sensory memory in 2-year-oldchildren: an event-related potential study. NeuroReport 2008a;19:569–73.

Glass E, Sachse S, von Suchodoletz W. Development of auditory sensory memoryfrom 2 to 6 years: an MMN study. J Neural Trans 2008b;115:1221–9.

Gomes H, Molholm S, Ritter W, Kurtzberg D, Cowan N, Vaughan Jr HG. Mismatchnegativity in children and adults, and effects of an attended task.Psychophysiology 2000;37:807–16.

Gomes H, Sussman E, Ritter W, Kurtzberg D, Cowan N, Vaughan Jr HG.Electrophysiological evidence of developmental changes in the duration ofauditory sensory memory. Dev Psychol 1999;35:294–302.

Gomot M, Bruneau N, Laurent J-P, Barthélémy C, Saliba E. Left temporal impairmentof auditory information processing in prematurely born 9-year-old children: anelectrophysiological study. Int J Psychophysiol 2007;64:123–9.

Gomot M, Giard M-H, Adrien J-L, Barthélémy C, Bruneau N. Hypersensitivity toacoustic change in children with autism: electrophysiological evidence of leftfrontal cortex dysfunctioning. Psychophysiology 2002;39:577–84.

Gomot M, Giard M-H, Roux S, Barthélémy C, Bruneau N. Maturation of frontal andtemporal components of mismatch negativity (MMN) in children. NeuroReport2000;11:3109–12.

Gosselin N, Mathieu A, Mazza S, Petit D, Malo J, Montplaisir J. Attentional deficits inpatients with obstructive sleep apnea syndrome: an event-related potentialstudy. Clin Neurophysiol 2006;117:2228–35.

Gottesman II, Gould TD. The endophenotype concept in psychiatry: etymology andstrategic intentions. Am J Psychiat 2003;160:636–45.

Gottselig JM, Brandeis D, Hofer-Tinguely G, Borbély AA, Achermann P. Humancentral auditory plasticity associated with tone sequence learning. LearnMemory 2004;11:162–71.

Grau C, Escera C, Yago E, Polo MD. Mismatch negativity and auditory sensorymemory evaluation: a new faster paradigm. NeuroReport 1998;9:2451–6.

Grau C, Polo MD, Yago E, Gual A, Escera C. Auditory sensory memory as indicated bymismatch negativity in chronic alcoholism. Clin Neurophysiol2001;112:728–31.

Grimm S, Schröger E, Bendixen A, Bäss P, Roye A, Deouell LY. Optimizing theauditory distraction paradigm: behavioural and event-related potential effectsin a laterized multi-deviant approach. Clin Neurophysol 2008;119:934–47.

Groenen P, Snik A, van der Broek P. On the clinical relevance of mismatchnegativity: results from subjects with normal hearing and cochlear implantusers. Audiol Neuro-Otology 1996;1:112–4.

Grossheinrich N, Kademan S, Bruder J, Bartling J, von Suchodoletz W. Auditorysensory memory and language abilities in former late talkers: a mismatchnegativity study. Psychophysiology 2010;47:822–30.

Grzella I, Müller BW, Oades RD, Bender S, Schall U, Zerbin D, et al. Novelty elicitedmismatch negativity in patients with schizophrenia on admission anddischarge. J Psychiatr Neurosci 2001;26:235–46.

Guérit J-M, Verougstraete D, de Tourtchaninoff M, Debatisse D, Witdoeckt C. ERPsobtained with the auditory oddball paradigm in coma and altered states ofconsciousness: clinical relationships, prognostic value, and origin ofcomponents. Clin Neurophysiol 1999;110:1260–9.

Gumenyuk V, Korzyukov O, Alho K, Escera C, Näätänen R. Effects of auditorydistraction on electrophysiological brain activity and performance in childrenaged 8–13 years. Psychophysiology 2004;41:30–6.

Gumenyuk V, Korzyukov O, Escera C, Hämäläinen M, Huotilainen M, Häyrinen T,et al. Electrophysiological evidence of enhanced distractibility in ADHDchildren. Neurosci Lett 2005;374:212–7.

Gumenyuk V, Roth T, Korzyukov O, Jefferson C, Kick A, Spear L, et al. Shift work sleepdisorder is associated with an attenuated brain response of sensory memoryand increased brain response to novelty: an ERP study. Sleep 2010;33:703–13.

Gunter TC, Jackson JL, Mulder G. Focussing on aging: an electrophysiologicalexploration of spatial and attentional processing during reading. Biol Psychol1996;43:103–45.

Guttorm TK, Leppänen PHT, Poikkeus A-M, Eklund KM, Lyytinen P, Lyytinen H. Brainevent-related potentials (ERPs) measured at birth predict later languagedevelopment in children with and without familial risk for dyslexia. Cortex2005;41:291–303.

Guttorm TK, Leppänen PHT, Richardson U, Lyytinen H. Event-related potentials andconsonant differentiation in newborns with familial risk for dyslexia. J LearnDisabil 2001;34:534–44.

Halgren E, Baudena P, Clarke JM, Heit G, Liegeois C, Chauvel P, et al. Intracerebralpotentials to rare target and distracter auditory and visual-stimuli .1. Superiortemporal plane and parietal lobe. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol1995a;94:191–220.

Halgren E, Baudena P, Clarke JM, Heit G, Marinkovic K, Devaux B, et al. Intracerebralpotentials to rare target and distracter auditory and visual-stimuli. 2. Medial,lateral and posterior temporal-lobe. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol1995b;94:229–50.

Halgren E, Marinkovic K, Chauvel P. Generators of the late cognitive potentials inauditory and visual oddball tasks. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol1998;106:156–64.

Hall M-H, Schulze K, Rijsdijk F, Kalitindi S, McDonald C, Bramon E, et al. Are auditoryP300 and duration MMN heritable and putative endophenotypes of psychoticbipolar disorder? A Maudsley bipolar twin and family study. Psychol Med2009;39:1277–87.

Hall M-H, Rijsdijk F, Kalitinidi S, Schulze K, Kravariti E, Kane F, et al. Genetic overlapbetween bipolar illness and event-related potentials. Psychol Med2007;37:667–78.

Hall MH, Schulze K, Rijsdijk F, Piccioni M, Ettinger U, Bramon E, et al. Heritabilityand reliability of P300, P50 and duration mismatch negativity. Behav Genet2006;36:845–57.

Hämäläinen H, Kujala T, Kekoni J, Hurskainen H, Pirilä J, Wikström H, et al. Effects ofunilateral hippocampus – amygdala – partial temporal lobe resection onauditory EEG/MEG responses: a case study. Scand J Psychol 2007;48:367–73.

Hämäläinen JA, Leppänen PHT, Guttorm TK, Lyyytinen H. Event-related potentials topitch and rise time change in children with reading disabilities and typicallyreading children. Clin Neurophysiol 2008;119:100–15.

Hämäläinen M, Hari R, Ilmoniemi RJ, Knuutila J, Lounasmaa OV.Magnetoencephalography – theory, instrumentation, and applications tononinvasive studies of the working human brain. Rev Mod Phys1993;65:413–97.

Hanagasi HA, Gurvit IH, Ermutlu N, Kaptanoglu G, Karamursel S, Idrisoglu HA, et al.Cognitive impairment in amyotropic lateral sclerosis: evidence fromneuropsychological investigation and event-related potentials. Cognit BrainRes 2002;14:234–44.

Hansenne M, Pinto E, Scantamburlo G, Couvreur A, Reggers J, Fuchs S, et al.Mismatch negativity is not correlated with neuroendocrine indicators ofcatecholaminergic activity in healthy subjects. Hum Psychopharm Clin2003;18:201–5.

Hari R, Hämäläinen M, Ilmoniemi R, Kaukoranta E, Reinikainen K, Salminen J, et al.Responses of the primary auditory cortex to pitch changes in a sequence of tonepips: neuromagnetic recordings in man. Neurosci Lett 1984;50:127–32.

Harkrider AW, Hedrick MS. Acute effect of nicotine on auditory gating in smokersand non-smokers. Hearing Res 2005;202:114–28.

Hawkins HL, Presson JC. Auditory information processing. In: Boff KR, Kaufman L,Thomas JP. (Eds.) Handbook of perception and human performance, vol 2. NewYork: Wiley; 1986. p. 261–4.

He W, Chai H, Zheng L, Yu W, Chen W, Li J, et al. Mismatch negativity in treatment-resistant depression and borderline personality disorder. Prog Neuro-Psychoph2010;34:366–71.

Heaton P, Pring L, Hermelin B. Musical processing in high functioning children withautism. Ann NY Acad Sci 2001;930:443–4.

Heekeren K, Daumann J, Neukirch A, Stock C, Kawohl W, Norra C, et al. Mismatchnegativity generation in the human 5HT2A agonist and NMDA antagonist modelof psychosis. Psychopharmacology 2008;199:77–88.

Heim S, Eulitz C, Kaufmann J, Füchter I, Pantev C, Lamprecht-Dinnesen A, et al.Atypical organisation of the auditory cortex in dyslexia as revealed by MEG.Neuropsychologia 2000;38:1749–59.

Heinke W, Kenntner R, Gunter TC, Sammler D, Olthoff D, Koelsch S. Sequentialeffects of increasing propofol sedation on frontal and temporal cortices asindexed by auditory event-related potentials. Anesthesiology2004;100:617–25.

Heinke W, Koelsch S. The effects of anesthetics on brain activity and cognitivefunction. Curr Opin Anaesthesiology 2005;18:625–31.

Hermens DF, Ward PB, Redoblado Hodge MA, Kaur M, Naismith SL, Hickie IB.Impaired MMN/P3a complex in first-episode psychosis: cognitive andpsychosocial associations. Prog Neuro-Psychoph 2010;34:822–9.

Heslenfeld DJ. Visual mismatch negativity. in: Polich J. (Ed.). Detection of change:event-related potential and fMRI findings. Boston: Kluwer Academic Publishers;2003. p. 41–60.

Hirayasu Y, McCarley RW, Salisbury DF, Tanaka S, Kwon JS, Frumin M, et al. Planumtemporale and Heschl gyrus volume reduction in schizophrenia: a magneticresonance imaging study of first-episode patients. Arch Gen Psychiat2000;57:692–9.

Hirayasu Y, Potts GF, O’Donnell BF, Kwon JS, Arakaki H, Akdag SJ, et al. Auditorymismatch negativity in schizophrenia: topographic evaluation with a high-density recording montage. Am J Psychiat 1998;155:1281–4.

Hirayasu Y, Tanaka S, Shenton ME, Salisbury DF, DeSantis MA, Levitt JJ, et al.Prefrontal gray matter volume reduction in first episode schizophrenia. CerebCortex 2001;11:374–81.

Hiruma T, Yabe H, Sato Y, Sutho T, Kaneko S. Differential effects of the hiba odor onCNV and MMN. Biol Psychol 2002;61:321–31.

Hirvonen J, Jääskeläinen IP, Näätänen R, Sillanaukee P. Adenosine A1/A2a receptorsmediate suppression of mismatch negativity by ethanol in humans. NeurosciLett 2000;278:57–60.

Holguin RS, Corral M, Cadaveira F. Mismatch negativity in young children ofalcoholics from high-density families. Alcohol Clin Exp Res 1998;22:1363–8.

Holopainen IE, Korpilahti P, Juottonen K, Lang H Sillanpää M. Attenuated auditoryevent-related potential (mismatch negativity) in children with developmentaldysphasia. Neuropediatrics 1997;28:253–6.

Holopainen IE, Korpilahti P, Juottonen K, Lang H, Sillanpää M. Abnormal frequencymismatch negativity in mentally retarded children and in children withdevelopmental dysphasia. J Child Neurol 1998;13:178–83.

Holst M, Eswaran H, Lowery C, Murphy P, Norton J, Preissl H. Development ofauditory evoked fields in human fetuses and newborns: a longitudinal MEGstudy. Clin Neurophysiol 2005;116:1949–55.

Page 26: The mismatch negativity (MMN) – A unique window to ... et al (2011... · Invited Review The mismatch negativity (MMN) – A unique window to disturbed central auditory processing

R. Näätänen et al. / Clinical Neurophysiology 123 (2012) 424–458 449

Honbolygó F, Csépe V, Fekésházy A, Emri M, Márián T, Sárközy G, et al. Coveringevidences on language impairment in Landau–Kleffner Syndrome revealed bybehavioural and brain activity measures: a case study. Clin Neurophysiol2006;117:295–305.

Horimoto R, Inagaki M, Yano T, Sata Y, Kaga M. Mismatch negativity of the colormodality during a selective attention task to auditory stimuli in children withmental retardation. Brain Dev JPN 2002;24:703–9.

Horton J, Millar A, Labelle A, Knott VJ. MMN responsivity to manipulations offrequency and duration deviants in chronic, clozapine-treated schizophreniapatients. Schizophr Res 2011;126:202–11.

Horváth J, Czigler I, Winkler I, Teder-Sälejärvi WA. The temporal window ofintegration in elderly and young adults. Neurobiol Aging 2007;28:964–75.

Hosák L, Kremlácek J, Kuba M, Libiger J, Cizek J. Mismatch negativity inmethamphetamine dependence. A pilot study. Acta Neurobiol Exp2008;68:7–102.

Hosoi H, Imaizumi S, Sakaguchi T, Tonoike M, Murata K. Activation of the auditorycortex by ultrasound. Lancet 1998;351:496–7.

Hugdahl K, Calhoun VD. An update on neurocognitive impairment in schizophreniaand depression. Front Hum Neurosci 2010;4:4.

Huotilainen M, Kujala A, Hotakainen M, Parkkonen L, Taulu S, Simola J, et al. Short-term memory functions of the human fetus recorded withmagnetoencephalography. NeuroReport 2005;16:81–4.

Huotilainen M, Lovio R, Kujala T, Tommiska V, Karma K, Fellman V. Couldaudiovisual training be used to improve cognition in extremely low birthweight children? Acta Paediatr 2011. doi: 10.1111/j.1651-2227.2011.02345.x.

Huotilainen M, Shestakova A, Hukki J. Using magnetoencephalography in assessingauditory skills in infants and children. Int J Psychophysiol 2008;68:123–9.

Huttunen T, Halonen A, Kaartinen J, Lyytinen H. Does mismatch negativity showdifferences in reading-disabled children compared to normal children andchildren with attention deficit? DevNeuropsychol 2007;31:453–70.

Huttunen-Scott T, Kaartinen J, Tolvanen A, Lyytinen H. Mismatch negativity (MMN)elicited by duration deviations in children with reading disorder, attentiondeficit or both. Int J Psychophysiol 2008;69:69–77.

Iijima, M., Osawa, M., Nageishi, Y., Ushijima, R., Iwata, M. Visual mismatchnegativity (MMN) in aging. in: Ogura C, Koga Y, Shimokochi M. (Eds.), RecentAdvances in Event-Related Brain Potentials Research. Amsterdam: Elsevier;1996. p. 804–9.

Ikeda K, Hayashi A, Hashimoto S, Kanno A. Distinctive MMN relative to sound typesin adults with intellectual disability. NeuroReport 2004;15:1053–6.

Ilvonen T-M, Kujala T, Kiesiläinen A, Salonen O, Kozou H, Pekkonen E, et al. Auditorydiscrimination after left hemisphere stroke: an MMN follow-up study. Stroke2003;34:1746–53.

Ilvonen T-M, Kujala T, Kozou H, Kiesiläinen A, Salonen O, Alku P, et al. Theprocessing of speech and non-speech sounds in aphasic patients as reflected bythe mismatch negativity (MMN). Neurosci Lett 2004;366:235–40.

Ilvonen T-M, Kujala T, Tervaniemi M, Salonen O, Näätänen R, Pekkonen E. Theprocessing of sound duration after left hemisphere stroke: event-relatedpotential and behavioral evidence. Psychophysiology 2001;38:622–8.

Inami R, Kirino E, Inoue R, Arai H. Transdermal nicotine administration enhancesautomatic auditory processing reflected by mismatch negativity. PharmacolBiochem Be 2005;80:453–61.

Inami R, Kirino E, Inoue E, Suzuki T, Arai H. Nicotine effects on mismatch negativityin nonsmoking schizophrenic patients. Neuropsychobiology 2007;56:64–72.

Iv J, Zhao L, Gong J, Chen C, Miao D. Event-related potential based evidence ofcognitive dysfunction in patients during the first episode of depression using anovelty oddball task. Psychiat Res-Neuroim 2010;182:58–66.

Izquidero L. The role of NMDA receptors in memory. Trends Pharmacol Sci1991;12:128–9.

Jääskeläinen IP, Hirvonen J, Kujala T, Alho K, Eriksson CFP, Lehtokoski A, et al. Effectsof naltrexone and ethanol on auditory event-related brain potentials. Alcohol1998;15:105–11.

Jääskeläinen IP, Lehtokoski A, Alho K, Kujala T, Pekkonen E, Sinclair JD, et al. Lowdose of ethanol suppresses mismatch negativity of auditory event-relatedpotentials. Alcohol Clin Exp Res 1995;19:607–10.

Jääskeläinen IP, Näätänen R, Sillanaukee P. Effect of acute ethanol on auditory andvisual event-related potentials: a review and reinterpretation. BiolPsychiat1996a;40:284–91.

Jääskeläinen IP, Pekkonen E, Hirvonen J, Sillanaukee P, Näätänen R. Mismatchnegativity subcomponents and ethyl alcohol. Biol Psychol 1996b;43:13–25.

Jääskeläinen IP, Schröger E, Näätänen R. Electrophysiological indices of acute effectsof ethanol on involuntary attention shifting. Psychopharmacology1999b;141:16–21.

Jääskeläinen IP, Varonen R, Näätänen R, Pekkonen E. Decay of cortical pre-attentivesound discrimination in middle-age. NeuroReport 1999a;10:123–6.

Jacobs BJ, Schneider SL. Analysis of lexical-semantic processing and extensiveneurological, electrophysiological, speech perception, and language evaluationfollowing a unilateral left hemisphere lesion: pure word deafness? Aphasiology2003;17:123–41.

Jahshan C, Cadenhead KS, Rissling AJ, Kirihara K, Braff DL, Light GA. Automaticsensory information processing abnormalities across the illness course ofschizophrenia. Psychol Med 2011. p. 1–13 [Epub ahead of print].

Jahshan C, Heaton RK, Golshan S, Cadenhead KS. Course of neurocognitive deficits inthe prodrome and first episode of schizophrenia. Neuropsychology2010;24:109–20.

James L, Gordon E, Kraiuhin C, Meares R. Selective attention and auditory event-related potentials in somatisation disorder. Compr Psychiat 1989;30:84–9.

Jamieson GA, Dwivedi P, Gruzelier JH. Changes in mismatch negativity across pre-hypnosis, hypnosis and post-hypnosis conditions distinguish high from lowhypnotic susceptibility groups. Brain Res Bull 2005;67:298–303.

Jankowiak S, Berti S. Behavioral and event-related potential distraction effects withregularly occurring auditory deviants. Psychophysiology 2007;44:79–85.

Jansson-Verkasalo E, Ceponiene R, Kielinen M, Suominen K, Linna S-L, Moilanen I,et al. Deficient auditory processing in children with Asperger syndrome, asindexed by event-related potentials. Neurosci Lett 2003a;338:197–200.

Jansson-Verkasalo E, Ceponiene R, Valkama M, Vainionpää L, Laitakari K, Alku P,et al. Deficient speech-sound processing, as shown by the electrophysiologicbrain mismatch negativity response, and naming ability in prematurely bornchildren. Neurosci Lett 2003b;348:5–8.

Jansson-Verkasalo E, Korpilahti P, Jäntti V, Valkama M, Vainionpää L, Alku P, et al.Neurophysiologic correlates of deficient phonological representations andobject naming in prematurely born children. Clin Neurophysiol2004;115:179–87.

Jansson-Verkasalo E, Kujala T, Jussila K, Mattila ML, Moilanen I, Näätänen R, et al.Similarities in the phenotype of the auditory neural substrate in children withAsperger syndrome and their parents. Eur J Neurosci 2005;22:986–90.

Jansson-Verkasalo E, Ruusuvirta T, Huotilainen M, Alku P, Kushnerenko E, SuominenK, et al. Atypical perceptual narrowing in prematurely born infants is associatedwith compromised language acquisition at 2 years of age. BMC Neurosci2010;11:88.

Järvelä LS, Hurme S, Holopainen IE, Leino M, Hatanpää A-M, Mikola H, et al.Auditory event related potentials as tools to reveal cognitive late effects inchildhood cancer patients. Clin Neurophysiol 2011;122:62–72.

Javitt DC. Neurophysiological approaches to analyzing brain dysfunction inschizophrenia. Biol Treatment Schizophr 1993;23:144–50.

Javitt DC. Intracortical mechanisms of mismatch negativity dysfunction inschizophrenia. Audiol Neuro-Otol 2000;5:207–15.

Javitt DC. When doors of perception close: bottom-up models of disruptedcognition in schizophrenia. Annu Rev Clin Psychol 2009a;5:249–75.

Javitt DC. Glycine transport inhibitors for the treatment of schizophrenia: symptomand disease modification. Curr Opin Drug Discov Dev 2009b;12:468–78.

Javitt DC, Shelley A, Ritter W. Associated deficits in mismatch negativity generationand tone matching in schizophrenia. Clin Neurophysiol 2000;111:1733–7.

Javitt DC, Schroeder CE, Steinschneider M, Arezzo JC, Vaughan Jr HG. Electroen ClinNeuro 1992;83:87–90.

Javitt DC, Spencer KM, Thaker GK, Winterer G, Hajós M. Neurophysiologicalbiomarkers for drug development in schizophrenia. Nature Rev 2008;68:68–83.

Javitt DC, Liederman E, Cienfuegos A, Shelley A-M. Panmodal processingimprecision as a basis for dysfunction of transient memory storage systems inschizophrenia. Schizophrenia Bull 1999;25:763–75.

Javitt DC, Steinschneider M, Schroeder CE, Arezzo JC. Role of cortical N-methyl-D-aspartate receptors in auditory sensory memory and mismatch negativitygeneration: implications for schizophrenia. P Natl Acad Sci USA1996;93:11962–7.

Javitt DC, Grochowski S, Shelley AM, Ritter W. Impaired mismatch negativity(MMN) generation in schizophrenia as a function of stimulus deviance,probability, and interstimulus/interdeviant interval. Electroen Clin Neuro1998;108:143–53.

Jessen F, Fries T, Kucharski C, Nishimura T, Hoenig K, Maier W, et al. Amplitudereduction of the mismatch negativity in first-degree relatives of patients withschizophrenia. Neurosci Lett 2001;309:185–8.

Jung J, Morlet D, Mercier B, Confavreux C, Fischer C. Mismatch negativity (MMN) inmultiple sclerosis: an event-related potentials study in 46 patients. ClinNeurophysiol 2006;117:85–93.

Kaga M, Inagaki M, Uno A. Auditory verbal and non-verbal mismatch negativity(MMN) in patients with severe motor and intellectual disabilities. Electroen ClinNeuro 1999;49:194–8.

Kähkönen S, Ahveninen J. Combination of magneto- and electroencephalography instudies of monoamine modulation on attention. Method Find Exp Clin2002;24:27–34.

Kähkönen S, Kesäniemi M, Nikouline VV, Karhu J, Ollikainen M, Holi M, et al. Ethanolmodulates cortical activity: direct evidence with combined TMS and EEG.NeuroImage 2001;14:322–8.

Kähkönen S, Mäkinen V, Jääskeläinen I, Pennanen S, Liesivuori J, Ahveninen J.Serotonergic modulation of mismatch negativity. Psychiat Res-Neuroim2005a;138:61–74.

Kähkönen S, Marttinen-Rossi E, Yamashita H. Alcohol impairs auditory processingof frequency changes and novel sounds: a combined MEG and EEG study.Psychopharmacology 2005b;177:366–72.

Kähkönen S, Yamashita H, Rytsälä H, Suominen K, Ahveninen J, Isometsä E.Dysfunction in early auditory processing in major depressive disorder revealedby combined MEG and EEG. J Psychiat Neurosci 2007;32:316–22.

Kaipio M-L, Alho K, Winkler I, Escera C, Surma-aho O, Näätänen R. Event-relatedbrain potentials reveal covert distractibility in closed head injuries.NeuroReport 1999;10:2125–9.

Kaipio M-L, Cheour M, Ceponiene R, Öhman J, Alku P, Näätänen R. Increaseddistractibility in closed head injury as revealed by event-related potentials.NeuroReport 2000;11:1463–8.

Kaipio M-L, Novitski N, Tervaniemi M, Alho K, Öhman J, Salonen O, et al. Fastvigilance decrement in closed head injury patients as reflected by the mismatchnegativity (MMN). NeuroReport 2001;12:1517–22.

Kallio S, Revonsuo A, Lauerma H, Hämäläinen H, Lang H. The MMN amplitudeincreases in hypnosis: a case study. NeuroReport 1999;10:3579–82.

Page 27: The mismatch negativity (MMN) – A unique window to ... et al (2011... · Invited Review The mismatch negativity (MMN) – A unique window to disturbed central auditory processing

450 R. Näätänen et al. / Clinical Neurophysiology 123 (2012) 424–458

Kalyakin I, González N, Ivannikov A, Lyytinen H. Extraction of the mismatchnegativity elicited by sound duration decrements: a comparison of threeprocedures. Data Knowl Eng 2009;68:1411–26.

Kalyakin I, González N, Joutsensalo J, Huttunen T, Kaartinen J, Lyytinen H. Optimaldigital filtering versus difference waves on the mismatch negativity in anuninterrupted sound paradigm. Dev Neuropsychol 2007;31:429–52.

Kalyakin I, González N, Kärkkäinen T, Lyytinen H. Independent component analysison the mismatch negativity in an uninterrupted sound paradigm. J NeurosciMeth 2008b;174:301–12.

Kalyakin I, González N, Lyytinen H. Extraction of the mismatch negativity on twoparadigms using independent component analysis. Proceedings of the CBMS2008. In: Puuronen S, Pechenizkiy M, Tsymbal A, Lee DJ. (Eds.), 21st IEEEinternational symposium on computer-based medical systems, CBMS. LosAlamitos, CA: IEEE Computer Society Conference Publishing Services; 2008a. p.59–64.

Kane NM, Butler SR, Simpson T. Coma outcome prediction using event-relatedpotentials: P3 and mismatch negativity. Audiol Neuro-Otol 2000;5:186–91.

Kane NM, Curry SH, Butler SR, Cummings BH. Electrophysiological indicator ofawakening from coma. Lancet 1993;341:688.

Kane NM, Curry SH, Rowlands CA, Manara AR, Lewis T, Moss T, et al. Event-relatedpotentials – neurophysiological tools for predicting emergence and earlyoutcome from traumatic coma. Intens Care Med 1996;22:39–46.

Karayanidis F, Andrews S, Ward PB, Michie PT. ERP indexes of auditory selectiveattention in aging and Parkinson’s disease. Psychophysiology 1995;32:335–50.

Karma K. Auditory structuring in explaining dyslexia. In: McKevitt P, Nuallain SO,Mulvihill C. (Eds.), Advances in consciousness research, language, vision, andmusic, vol. 35. Amsterdam/Philadelphia: John Benjamins Publ. CO; 1999. p.221–9.

Kasai K, Hashimoto O, Kawakubo Y, Yumoto M, Kamio S, Itoh K, et al. Delayedautomatic detection of change in speech sounds in adults with autism: amagnetoencephalographic study. Clin Neurophysiol 2005;116:1655–64.

Kasai K, Nakagome K, Itoh K, Koshida I, Hata A, Iwanami A, et al. Impaired corticalnetwork for preattentive detection of change in speech sounds inschizophrenia: a high-resolution event-related potential study. Am JPsychiatry 2002a;159:546–53.

Kasai K, Shenton ME, Salisbury DF, Hirayasu Y, Lee C-U, Ciszewski AA, et al.Progressive decrease of left superior temporal gyrus gray matter volume inpatients with first-episode schizophrenia. Am J Psychiat 2003b;160:156–64.

Kasai K, Yamada H, Kamio S, Nakagome K, Iwanami A, Fukuda M, et al. Do high orlow doses of anxiolytics and hypnotics affect mismatch negativity inschizophrenic subjects? An EEG and MEG study. Clin Neurophysiol2002c;113:141–50.

Kasai K, Shenton ME, Salisbury DF, Hirayasu Y, Onitsuka T, Spencer MH, et al.Progressive decrease of left Heschl gyrus and planum temporale gray mattervolume in first-episode schizophrenia: a longitudinal magnetic resonanceimaging study. Arch Gen Psychiat 2003a;60:766–75.

Kasai K, Yamada H, Kamio S, Nakagome K, Iwanami A, Fukuda M, et al. Brainlaterization for mismatch response to across- and within-category change ofvowels. NeuroReport 2001;12:2467–71.

Kasai K, Yamada H, Kamio S, Nakagome K, Iwanami A, Fukuda M, et al.Neuromagnetic correlates of impaired automatic categorical perception ofspeech sounds in schizophrenia. Schizophr Res 2002b;59:159–72.

Katada E, Sato K, Ojika K, Ueda R. Cognitive event-related potentials: useful clinicalinformation in Alzheimer’s disease. Curr Alzheimer Res 2004;1:63–9.

Kathmann N, Wagner M, Rendtorff N, Engel RR. Delayed peak latency of themismatch negativity in schizophrenics and alcoholics. Biol Psychiat1995;37:754–7.

Kaur M, Battisti RA, Ward PB, Ahmed A, Hickie IB, Hermens DF. MMN/P3a deficits infirst episode psychosis: comparing schizophrenia-spectrum and affective-spectrum subgroups. Schizoph Res 2011;130:203–9.

Kawakubo Y, Kamio S, Nose T, Iwanami A, Nakagome K, Fukuda M, et al. Phoneticmismatch negativity predicts social skills acquisition in schizophrenia. PsychiatRes 2007;152:261–5.

Kawakubo Y, Kasai K, Kudo N, Rogers MA, Nakagome K, Itoh K, et al. Phoneticmismatch negativity predicts verbal memory deficit in schizophrenia.NeuroReport 2006;17:1043–6.

Kazmerski VA, Friedman D, Ritter W. Mismatch negativity during attend and ignoreconditions in Alzheimer’s disease. Biol Psychiat 1997;42:382–402.

Kekoni J, Hämäläinen H, Saarinen M, Gröhn J, Reinikainen K, Lehtokoski A, et al. Rateeffect and mismatch responses in the somatosensory system: ERP-recordings inhumans. Biol Psychol 1997;46:125–42.

Kelly AS, Purdy SC, Thorne PR. Electrophysiological and speech perception measuresof auditory processing in experimental adult cochlear implant users. ClinNeurophysiol 2005;116:1235–46.

Kemner C, Verbaten M, Cuperus J, Camfferman G, van Engeland H. Auditory event-related brain potentials in autistic children and three different control groups.Biol Psychiat 1995;38:150–65.

Kemner C, Verbaten MN, Koelega HS, Buitelaar JK, van der Gaag RJ, Camfferman G,et al. Event-related potentials in children with attention-deficit andhyperactivity disorder: effects of stimulus deviancy and task relevance in thevisual and auditory modality. Biol Psychiat 1996;40:522–34.

Kenemans JL, Gren-’t jong T, Verbaten MN. Detection of visual change: mismatch orrareness? NeuroReport 2003;14:1239–42.

Kenemans JL, Helby W, van den Heuvel EHM, Grent-’T-Jong T. Moderate alcoholdisrupts a mechanism for detection of rare events in human visual cortex. JPsychopharmacol 2010;24:839–45.

Kiang M, Braff DL, Sprock J, Light GA. The relationship between preattentive sensoryprocessing deficits and age in schizophrenia patients. Clin Neurophysiol2009;120:1949–57.

Kiang M, Light GA, Prugh J, Coulson S, Braff DL, Kutas M. Cognitive,neurophysiological, and functional correlates of proverb interpretationabnormalities in schizophrenia. J Int Neuropsych Soc 2007;13:653–63.

Kida T, Nishihira Y, Hatta A, Fumuto M, Wasaka T. Automatic mismatch negativitydetection in somatosensory modality and the effect of stimulus probability. JpnClin Neurophysiol (Tokyo) 2001;29:417–24 [in Japanese].

Kileny PR, Boerst A, Zwolan T. Cognitive evoked potentials to speech and tonalstimuli in children with implants. Otolaryng Head Neck 1997;117:161–9.

Kilpeläinen R, Partanen J, Karhu J. Reduced mismatch negativity (MMN) suggestsdeficits in preattentive auditory processing in distractible children.NeuroReport 1999;10:3341–5.

Kimura M, Katayama J, Ohira H, Schröger E. Visual mismatch negativity: newevidence from the equiprobable paradigm. Psychophysiology 2009;46:402–9.

Kimura M, Schröger E, Czigler I. Visual mismatch negativity and its importance invisual cognitive sciences. NeuroReport 2011;22:669–73.

Kircher TT, Rapp A, Grodd W, Buchkremer G, Weiskopf N, Lutzenberger W, et al.Mismatch negativity responses in schizophrenia: a combined fMRI and whole-head MEG study. Am J Psychiat 2004;161:294–304.

Kisley MA, Davalos DB, Engleman LL, Guinther PM, Davis HP. Age-related change inneural processing of time-dependent stimulus features. Cognit Brain Res2005;25:913–25.

Kisley MA, Davalos DB, Layton HS, Pratt D, Ellis JK, Seger CA. Small changes intemporal deviance modulate mismatch negativity in humans. Neurosci Lett2004a;358:197–200.

Kisley MA, Noecker TL, Guinther PM. Comparison of sensory gating to mismatchnegativity and self-reported perceptual phenomena in healthy adults.Psychophysiology 2004b;41:604–12.

Kivisaari R, Lehtinen R, Autti T, Puuskari V, Jokela O, Ahveninen J, et al. Impairedpre-attentive auditory processing in opioid dependence with and withoutbenzodiazepine co-dependence revealed by combinedmagnetoencephalography and electroencephalography. Prog Neuro-Psychoph2007;31:1378–86.

Knott V, Blais C, Scherling C, Camarda J, Millar A, Fisher D, et al. Neural effects ofnicotine during auditory selective attention in smokers: an event-relatedpotential study. Neuropsychobiology 2006;53:115–26.

Knott VJ, Bolton K, Heenan A, Shah D, Fisher DJ, Villeneuve C. Effects of acutenicotine on event-related potential and performance indices of auditorydistraction in nonsmokers. Nicotine Tobacco Res 2009;11:519–30.

Koelsch S, Wittfoth M, Wolf A, Müller J, Hahne A. Music perception in cochlearimplant users: an event-related potential study. Clin Neurophysiol2004;115:966–72.

Koelsch S, Heinke W, Sammler D, Olthoff D. Auditory processing during deeppropofol sedation and recovery from unconsciousness. Clin Neurophysiol2006;117:1746–59.

Kohlmetz C, Altenmüller E, Schuppert M, Wieringa BM, Münte T. Deficit inautomatic sound-change detection may underlie some music perceptiondeficits after acute hemispheric stroke. Neuropsychologia 2001;39:1121–4.

Kok A. Age-related changes in involuntary and voluntary attention as reflected incomponents of the event-related potential (ERP). Biol Psychol 2000;54:107–43.

Korostenskaja M, Dapsys K, Siurkute A, Maciulis V, Ruksenas O, Kähkönen S. Effectsof olanzapine on auditory P300 and mismatch negativity (MMN) inschizophrenia spectrum disorders. Prog Neuro-Psychoph 2005;29:543–8.

Korostenskaja M, Nikulin VV, Kicic D, Nikulina AV, Kähkönen S. Effects of NMDAreceptor antagonist memantine on mismatch negativity in healthy subjects.Brain Res Bull 2007;72:275–83.

Korostenskaja M, Pardos M, Fujiwara H, Kujala TM, Horn P, Rose D, et al.Neuromagnetic evidence of impaired cortical auditory processing in pediatricintractable epilepsy. Epilepsy Res 2010a;92:63–73.

Korostenskaja M, Pardos M, Kujala T, Rose DF, Xiang J, Horn P, et al. Impaired auditoryinformation processing during acute migraine: a magnetoencephalographystudy. Int J Neurosci 2010b;121:355–65.

Korpilahti P. Auditory discrimination and memory functions in SLI children: acomprehensive study with neurophysiological and behavioural methods. ScandJ Logoped Phoniatr 1995;20:131–9.

Korpilahti P, Lang HA. Auditory ERP components and mismatch negativity indysphasic children. Electroen Clin Neuro 1994;91:256–64.

Korpilahti P, Jansson-Verkasalo E, Mattila M-L, Kuusikko S, Suominen K, Rytky S,et al. Processing of affective speech prosody is impaired in Asperger syndrome. JAutism Dev Disord 2007;37:1539–49.

Kotchoubey B. Event-related potentials predict the outcome of the vegetative state.Clin Neurophysiol 2007;118:477–9.

Kotchoubey B, Lang S, Herb E, Maurer P, Schmalohr D, Bostanov V, et al. Stimuluscomplexity enhances auditory discrimination in patients with extremely severebrain injuries. Neurosci Lett 2003;352:129–32.

Kotchoubey B, Lang S, Mezger G, Schmalohr D, Schneck M, Semmler A, et al.Information processing in severe disorders of consciousness: vegetative stateand minimally conscious state. Clin Neurophysiol 2007;116:2441–53.

Kozou H, Kujala T, Shtyrov Y, Toppila E, Stark J, Alku P, et al. The effect of differentnoise types on the speech and non-speech elicited mismatch negativity.Hearing Res 2005;199:31–9.

Kramer AF, Trejo LJ, Humphrey D. Assessment of mental workload with task-irrelevant auditory probes. Biol Psychol 1995;40:83–100.

Page 28: The mismatch negativity (MMN) – A unique window to ... et al (2011... · Invited Review The mismatch negativity (MMN) – A unique window to disturbed central auditory processing

R. Näätänen et al. / Clinical Neurophysiology 123 (2012) 424–458 451

Krauel K, Schott P, Sojka B, Pause BM, Ferstl R. Is there a mismatch negativityanalogue in the olfactory event-related potential? J Psychophysiol1999;13:49–55.

Kraus N, Koch D, McGee T, Nicol T, Cunningham J. Speech-sound discrimination inschool-age children: psychophysical and neurophysiologic measures. J SpeechLang Hear Res 1999;42:1042–60.

Kraus N, Cheour M. Speech-sound representation in the brain: studies usingmismatch negativity. Audiol Neuro-Otol 2000;5:140–50.

Kraus N, McGee T, Carrell TD, King C, Tremblay K, Nicol N. Central auditory plasticityassociated with speech discrimination training. J Cognit Neurosci1995;7:25–32.

Kraus N, McGee TJ, Carrell TD, Zecker SG, Nicol TG, Koch DB. Auditoryneurophysiologic responses and discrimination deficits in children withlearning problems. Science 1996;273:971–3.

Kraus N, McGee TJ, Littman T, Nicol TG, King C. Nonprimary auditory thalamicrepresentation of acoustic change. J Neurophysiol 1994;72:1270–7.

Kraus N, McGee T, Micco A, Sharma A, Carrell T, Nicol T. Mismatch negativity inschool-age children to speech stimuli which are just perceptibly different.Electroen Clin Neuro 1993a;88:123–30.

Kraus N, McGee T, Sharma A, Carrell T, Nicol T. Mismatch negativity event-relatedpotential elicited by speech stimuli. Ear Hearing 1992;13:158–64.

Kraus N, Micco A, Koch D, McGee T, Carrell T, Wiet R, et al. The mismatch negativitycortical evoked potential elicited by speech in cochlear-implant users. HearingRes 1993b;65:118–24.

Kreitschmann-Andermahr I, Rosburg T, Demme U, Gaser E, Nowak H, Sauer H. Effectof ketamine on the neuromagnetic mismatch field in healthy humans. CognitBrain Res 2001;12:109–16.

Kreitschmann-Andermahr I, Rosburg T, Meier T, Volz HP, Nowak H, Sauer H.Impaired sensory processing in male patients with schizophrenia: amagnetoencephalographic study of auditory mismatch detection. SchizophrRes 1999;35:121–9.

Kremlácek J, Hosák L, Kuba M, Libiger J, Cizek J. Visual information processing inrecently abstaining methamphetamine-dependent individuals: evokedpotentials study. Doc Ophthalmol 2008;117:245–55.

Kropotov JD, Alho K, Näätänen R, Ponomarev VA, Kropotova OV, Anichkov AD, et al.Human auditory-cortex mechanisms of preattentive sound discrimination.Neurosci Lett 2000;280:87–90.

Kuhl P. Early language acquisition: cracking the speech code. Nature Rev2004;5:831–43.

Kuhl P, Coffey-Corina S, Padden D, Dawson G. Links between social and linguisticprocessing of speech in preschool children with autism: behavioral andelectrophysiological measures. Dev Sci 2005;8:F1–F12.

Kuhl PK, Williams KA, Lacerda F, Stevens KN, Lindblom B. Linguistic experiencealters phonetic perception in infants by 6 months of age. Science1991;225:606–8.

Kujala A, Huotilainen M, Hotakainen M, Lennes M, Parkkonen L, Fellman V, et al.Speech-sound discrimination in neonates as measured with MEG. NeuroReport2004;15:2089–92.

Kujala A, Huotilainen M, Uther M, Shtyrov Y, Monto S, Ilmoniemi RJ, et al. Plasticcortical changes induced by learning to communicate with non-speech sounds.NeuroReport 2003;14:1683–7.

Kujala T. The role of early discrimination deficits in language disorders. JPsychophysiol 2007;21:239–50.

Kujala T, Brattico E. Detrimental noise effects on brain’s speech functions. BiolPsychol 2009;81:135–43.

Kujala T, Näätänen R. The mismatch negativity in evaluating central auditorydysfunctions in dyslexia. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 2001;25:535–43.

Kujala T, Näätänen R. The adaptive brain: a neurophysiological perspective. ProgrNeurobiol 2010;91:55–67.

Kujala T, Aho E, Lepistö T, Jansson-Verkasalo E, Nieminen-von Wendt T, von WendtL, et al. Atypical pattern of discriminating sound features in adults withAsperger syndrome as reflected by the mismatch negativity. Biol Psychol2007;75:109–14.

Kujala T, Alho K, Huotilainen M, Ilmoniemi RJ, Lehtokoski A, Leinonen A, et al.Electrophysiological evidence for cross-modal plasticity in humans with early-and late-onset blindness. Psychophysiology 1997;34:213–6.

Kujala T, Alho K, Kekoni J, Hämäläinen H, Reinikainen K, Salonen O, et al. Auditoryand somatosensory event-related brain potentials in early blind humans. ExpBrain Res 1995a;104:519–26.

Kujala T, Alho K, Näätänen R. Cross-modal reorganization of human corticalfunctions. Trends Neurosci 2000b;3:115–20.

Kujala T, Belitz S, Tervaniemi M, Näätänen R. Auditory sensory memory disorder indyslexic adults as indexed by the mismatch negativity. Eur J Neurosci2003;17:1323–7.

Kujala T, Huotilainen M, Sinkkonen J, Ahonen AI, Alho K, Hämäläinen MS, et al.Visual cortex activation in blind humans during sound discrimination. NeurosciLett 1995b;183:143–6.

Kujala T, Karma K, Ceponiene R, Belitz S, Turkkila P, Tervaniemi M, et al. Plasticneural changes and reading improvement caused by audio-visual training inreading-impaired children. P Natl Acad Sci USA 2001;98:10509–14.

Kujala T, Lepistö T, Nieminen-von Wendt T, Näätänen P, Näätänen R.Neurophysiologial evidence for cortical discrimination impairment of prosodyin Asperger syndrome. Neurosci Lett 2005a;383:260–5.

Kujala T, Lovio R, Lepistö T, Laasonen M, Näätänen R. Evaluation of multi-attributeauditory discrimination in dyslexia with the mismatch negativity. ClinNeurophysiol 2006;117:885–93.

Kujala T, Myllyviita K, Tervaniemi M, Alho K, Kallio J, Näätänen R. Basic auditorydysfunction in dyslexia as demonstrated by brain-activity measurements.Psychophysiol Special Rep 2000a;37:262–6.

Kujala T, Palva JM, Salonen O, Alku P, Huotilainen M, Järvinen A, et al. The role ofblind humans’ visual cortex in auditory processing. Neurosci Lett2005b;379:127–31.

Kujala T, Shtyrov Y, Winkler I, Saher M, Tervaniemi M, Sallinen M, et al. Detrimentaleffects of long-term noise exposure on the central auditory processing.Psychophysiology 2004;41:875–81.

Kujala T, Alho K, Paavilainen P, Summala H, Näätänen R. Neural plasticity inprocessing of sound location by the early blind: an event-related potentialstudy. Electroen Clin Neuro 1992;84:469–72.

Kujala T, Kuuluvainen S, Saalasti S, Jansson-Verkasalo E, von Wendt L, Lepistö T.Speech-feature discrimination in children with Asperger syndrome asdetermined with the multi-feature mismatch negativity paradigm. ClinNeurophysol 2010;121:1410–9.

Kurtzberg D, Vaughan Jr HG, Kreuzer JA, Fliegler KZ. Developmental studies andclinical application of mismatch negativity: problems and prospects. EarHearing 1995;16:105–17.

Kushnerenko E, Ceponiene R, Balan P, Fellman V, Näätänen R. Maturation of theauditory change detection response in infants: a longitudinal ERP study.NeuroReport 2002;13:1843–8.

Kushnerenko E, Winkler I, Horváth J, Näätänen R, Pavlov I, Fellman V, et al.Processing acoustic change and novelty in newborn infants. Eur J Neurosci2007;26:265–74.

Kwon M, Huotilainen M, Shestakova A, Kujala T, Näätänen R, Hämäläinen H. Noeffects of mobile phone use on cortical auditory change-detection in children:an ERP study. Bioelectromagnetics 2010;31:191–9.

Kwon M, Kujala T, Huotilainen M, Shestakova A, Näätänen R, Hämäläinen H.Preattentive auditory information processing under exposure to the 902 MHzGSM mobile phone electromagnetic field: a mismatch negativity (MMN) study.Bioelectromagnetics 2009;30:241–8.

Lachmann T, Berti S, Kujala T, Schröger E. Diagnostic subgroups of developmentaldyslexia have different deficits in neural processing of tones and phonemes. IntJ Psychophysiol 2005;56:105–20.

Lähteenmäki P. Effects of childhood cancer on patients and their families – physicaland physiological aspects. Annales Universitatis Turkuensis, Ser D Tom 338,Medica-Odontologia; 1999.

Lalo E, Vercueil L, Bougerol T, Jouk P-S, Debû B. Late event-related potentials andmovement complexity in young adults with Down syndrome. NeurophysiolClin 2005;35:81–91.

Lang H, Eerola O, Korpilahti P, Holopainen IE, Salo SK, Aaltonen O. Practical issues inthe clinical application of mismatch negativity. Ear Hearing 1995;16:118–30.

Lang HA, Nyrke T, Ek M, Aaltonen O, Raimo I, Näätänen R. Pitch discriminationperformance and auditory event-related potentials. In. Brunia CHM, GaillardAWK, Kok A, Mulder G, Verbaten MN. (Eds.), Psychophysiological brainresearch, vol. 1. Tilburg, The Netherlands: Tilburg University Press; 1990. p.294–8.

Laureys S, Perrin F, Schnakers C, Boly M, Majerus S. Residual cognitive function incomatose, vegetative and minimally conscious states. Curr Opin Neurol2005;18:726–33.

Lavoie S, Murray MM, Deppen P, Knyazeva MG, Berk M, Boulat O, et al. Glutathioneprecursor, N-acetyl-cysteine, improves mismatch negativity in schizophreniapatients. Neuropsychopharmacol 2007;33:2187–99.

Leipälä JA, Partanen E, Kushnerenko E, Huotilainen M, Fellman V. Perinatal cerebralinsults alter auditory event-related potentials. Early Hum Dev 2011;87:89–95.

Leitman DI, Sehatpour P, Higgins BA, Foxe JJ, Silipo G, Javitt DC. Sensory deficits anddistributed hierarchical dysfunction in schizophrenia. Am J Psychiat2010;167:818–27.

Lepistö T, Kajander M, Vanhala R, Alku P, Huotilainen M, Näätänen R. The perceptionof invariant speech features in children with autism. Biol Psychol2008;77:25–31.

Lepistö T, Kujala T, Vanhala R, Alku P, Huotilainen M, Näätänen R. Thediscrimination of and orienting to speech and non-speech sounds in childrenwith autism. Brain Res 2005;1066:147–57.

Lepistö T, Nieminen-von Wendt T, von Wendt L, Näätänen R, Kujala T. Auditorycortical change detection in adults with Asperger syndrome. Neurosci Lett2007;414:136–40.

Lepistö T, Silokallio S, Nieminen-von Wendt P, Alku T, Näätänen R, Kujala T.Auditory perception and attention as reflected by the brain event-relatedpotentials in children with Asperger syndrome. Clin Neurophysiol2006;117:2161–71.

Lepistö T, Soininen M, Ceponiene R, Almqvist F, Näätänen R, Aronen E. Auditoryevent-related potential indices of increased distractibility in children withmajor depression. Clin Neurophysiol 2004;115:620–7.

Leppänen PHT, Guttorm TK, Pihko E, Takkinen S, Eklund KE, Lyytinen H, et al.Maturational effects on newborn ERPs measured in the mismatch negativityparadigm. Exp Neurol 2004;190:S91–S101.

Leppänen PHT, Lyytinen H. Auditory event-related potentials in the study ofdevelopmental language related disorders. Audiol Neuro-Otol 1997;2:308–40.

Leppänen PHT, Eklund KM, Lyytinen H. Event-related brain potentials to change inrapidly presented acoustic stimuli in newborns. Dev Neuropsychol1997;13:175–204.

Leppänen PHT, Pihko E, Eklund KM, Lyytinen H. Cortical responses of infants with orwithout a genetic risk for dyslexia: II. Group effects. NeuroReport1999;10:969–9673.

Page 29: The mismatch negativity (MMN) – A unique window to ... et al (2011... · Invited Review The mismatch negativity (MMN) – A unique window to disturbed central auditory processing

452 R. Näätänen et al. / Clinical Neurophysiology 123 (2012) 424–458

Leppänen PHT, Richardson U, Pihko E, Eklund KM, Guttorm TK, Aro M, et al. Brainresponses to changes in speech sound durations differ between infants with andwithout familial risk for dyslexia. Dev Neuropsychol 2002;22:407–22.

Leppänen PHT, Hämäläinen JA, Salminen HK, Eklund KM, Guttorm TK, Lohvansuu K,et al. Newborn brain event-related potentials revealing atypical processing ofsound frequency and the subsequent association with later literacy skills inchildren with familial dyslexia. Cortex 2010;46:1362–76.

Lessard N, Pare M, Lepore F, Lassonde M. Early-blind human subjects localize soundsources better than sighted subjects. Nature 1998;395:278–80.

Leung S, Croft RJ, Baldeweg T, Nathan PJ. Acute dopamine D1 and D2 receptorstimulation does not modulate mismatch negativity (MMN) in healthy humansubjects. Psychopharmacology 2007;194:443–51.

Leung S, Croft RJ, O’Neill BV, Nathan PJ. Acute high-dose glycine attenuatesmismatch negativity (MMN) in healthy human controls. Psychopharmacology2008;196:451–60.

Levänen S, Ahonen A, Hari R, McEvoy L, Sams M. Deviant auditory stimuli activatehuman left and right auditory cortex differently. Cerebr Cortex 1996;6:288–96.

Levänen S, Hari R, McEvoy L, Sams M. Responses of the human auditory cortex tochanges in one versus two stimulus features. Exp Brain Res 1993;97:177–83.

Liasis A, Towell A, Alho K, Boyd S. Intracranial identification of an electric frontal-cortex response to auditory stimulus change: a case study. Cognitive Brain Res2001;11:227–33.

Liasis A, Towell A, Boyd S. Intracranial auditory detection and discrimination potentialsas substrates of echoic memory in children. Cognit Brain Res 1999;7:503–6.

Liasis A, Towell A, Boyd S. Intracranial evidence for differential encoding offrequency and duration discrimination responses. Ear Hear 2000;21:252–6.

Liasis A, Bamiou DE, Boyd S, Towell A. Evidence for a neurophysiologic auditorydeficit in children with benign epilepsy with centro-temporal spikes. J NeuralTrans 2006;113:939–49.

Light GA, Braff DL. Mismatch negativity deficits are associated with poorfunctioning in schizophrenia patients. Arch Gen Psychiat 2005a;62:127–36.

Light GA, Braff DL. Stability of mismatch negativity deficits and their relationship tofunctional impairment in chronic schizophrenia. Am J Psychiat2005b;162:1741–3.

Light GA, Swerdlow NR, Braff DL. Preattentive sensory processing as indexed by theMMN and P3a brain responses is associated with cognitive and psychosocialfunctioning in healthy adults. J Cognit Neurosci 2007;19:1624–32.

Lin Y-Y, Hsiao F-J, Shih YH, Yiu C-H, Yen D-J, Kwan S-Y, et al. Plastic phase-lockingand magnetic mismatch response to auditory deviants in temporal lobeepilepsy. Cereb Cortex 2007;17:2516–25.

Lipton SA, Rosenberg PA. Excitatory amino acids as a final common pathway forneurologic disorders. New Engl J Med 1994;330:613–22.

Liu T, Shi J, Zhang Q, Zhao D, Yang J. Neural mechanism of auditory sensoryprocessing in children with high intelligence. NeuroReport 2007;18:1571–5.

Lonka E, Lehtokoski A, Kujala T, Johansson R, Rimmanen S, Alho K, et al. Themismatch negativity (MMN) brain response as an index of speech perceptionrecovery in cochlear-implant recipients. Audiol Neuro-otol 2004;9:160–2.

Lorenzo-Lopez L, Amenedo E, Pazo-Alvarez P, Cadaveira F. Pre-attentive detection ofmotion direction changes in normal aging. NeuroReport 2004;15:2633–6.

Lovio R, Näätänen R, Kujala T. Abnormal pattern of cortical speech featurediscrimination in 6-year-old children at risk for dyslexia. Brain Res2010;1335:53–62.

Lovio R, Pakarinen S, Huotilainen M, Alku P, Silvennoinen S, Näätänen R. Auditorydiscrimination profiles of speech sound changes in 6-year-old children asdetermined with the multi-feature MMN paradigm. Clin Neurophysiol2009;120:916–21.

Luauté J, Fischer C, Adelaine P, Morlet D, Tell L, Boisson D. Late auditory event-related potentials can be useful to predict good functional outcome after coma.Arch Physic Med Rehab 2005;86:917–23.

Luck SJ, Mathalon DH, O’Donnell BF, Hämäläinen MS, Spencer KM, Javitt DC, et al. Aroadmap for the development and validation of event-related potentialbiomarkers in schizophrenia. Biol Psychiat 2011;70:28–34.

Lyytinen H, Ahonen T, Eklund K, Guttorm TK, Laakso M-L, Leinonen S, et al.Developmental pathways of children with and without familial risk for dyslexiaduring the first years of life. Dev Neuropsychol 2001;20:535–54.

Lyytinen H, Aro M, Eklund K, Erskinel J, Guttorm T, Laakso M-L, et al. Thedevelopment of children at familial risk for dyslexia: birth to early school age.Ann Dyslexia 2004;54:184–220.

Lyytinen H, Erskine J, Kujala J, Ojanen E, Richardson U. In search of a science-basedapplication: a learning tool for reading acquisition. Scand J Psychol2009;50:668–75.

Lyytinen H, Erskine J, Tolvanen A, Torppa M. Trajectories of reading development: afollow-up from birth to school age of children with and without risk fordyslexia. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly 2006;52:514–46.

Maekawa T, Goto Y, Kinukawa N, Taniwaki T, Kanba S, Tobimatsu S. Functionalcharacterization of mismatch negativity to a visual stimulus. Clin Neurophysiol2005;116:2392–402.

Maekawa T, Tobimatsu S, Inada N, Oribe N, Onitsuka T, Kanba S, et al. Top-down andbottom-up visual information processing of non-social stimuli in high-functioning autism spectrum disorder. Res Autism Spectr Disord 2011;5:201–9.

Maekawa T, Tobimatsu S, Ogata K, Onitsuka T, Kanba S. Preattentive visual changedetection as reflected by the mismatch negativity (MMN) – evidence for amemory-based process. Neurosci Res 2009;65:107–12.

Mager R, Falkenstein M, Störmer R, Brand S, Müller-Spahn F, Bullinger AH. Auditorydistraction in young and middle-aged adults: a behavioural and event-relatedpotential study. J Neural Trans 2005;112:1165–76.

Magno E, Yeap S, Thakore JH, Garavan H, De Sanctis P, Foxe JJ. Are auditory-evokedfrequency and duration mismatch negativity deficits endophenotypic forschizophrenia? High-density electrical mapping in clinically unaffected first-degree relatives and first-episode and chronic schizophrenia. Biol Psychiat2008;64:385–91.

Magnusson KR, Brim BL, Das SR. Selective vulnerabilities of N-methyl-D-aspartate(NMDA) receptors during brain aging. Front Aging Neurosci 2010;2:11.

Mäkelä JP, Salmelin R, Hokkanen L, Launes J, Hari R. Neuromagnetic sequelae ofherpes simplex encephalitis. Electroen Clin Neuro 1998a;106:251–8.

Mäkelä JP, Salmelin R, Kotila M, Salonen O, Laaksonen R, Hokkanen L, et al.Modification of neuromagnetic cortical signals by thalamic infarctions.Electroen Clin Neuro 1998b;106:433–43.

Mäntysalo S, Salmi T. Effect of noise on the mismatch negativity of the event-relatedpotential of the human brain. In: Bond NW, Siddle DAT. (Eds.) Psychobiology:issues and applications. North-Holland: Elsevier Science Publishers B.V.; 1989.p. 277–88.

March L, Cienfuegos A, Goldbloom L, Ritter W, Cowan N, Javitt DC, et al. Normal timecourse of auditory recognition in schizophrenia, despite impaired precision ofthe auditory sensory (‘‘echoic’’) memory code. J Abnorm Psychol1999;108:69–75.

Marco-Pallares J, Grau C, Gual A, Escera C, Grau C. Mismatch negativity impairmentassociated with alcohol consumption in chronic alcoholics: a scalp currentdensity study. Int J Psychophysiol 2007;1:51–7.

Marco-Pallares J, Grau C, Ruffini G. Combined ICA-LORETA analysis of mismatchnegativity. NeuroImage 2005;25:471–7.

Marler JA, Chamolin CA, Gillam RB. Auditory memory for backward masking signalsin children with language impairment. Psychophysiology 2002;39:767–80.

Martin LF, Davalos DB, Kisley MA. Nicotine enhances automatic temporalprocessing as measured by the mismatch negativity waveform. NicotineTobacco Res 2009;11:698–706.

Martynova O, Kirjavainen J, Cheour M. Mismatch negativity and late discriminativenegativity in sleeping human newborns. Neurosci Lett 2003;340:75–8.

Massaro DW. A comparison of forward versus backward recognition masking. J ExpPsychol 1973;100:434–6.

Massaro DW. Backward recognition masking. J Acoust Soc Am 1975;58:1059–65.Matsubayashi J, Kawakubo Y, Suga M, Takei Y, Kumano S, Fukuda M, et al. The

influence of gender and personality traits on individual difference in auditorymismatch: a magnetoencephalographic (MMNm) study. Brain Res2008;1236:159–65.

Matthews N, Todd J, Budd TW, Cooper G, Michie PT. Auditory lateralization inschizophrenia – mismatch negativity and behavioral evidence of a selectiveimpairment in encoding interaural time cues. Clin Neurophysiol2007;118:833–44.

Maurer U, Bucher K, Brem S, Brandeis D. Altered responses to tone and phonememismatch in kindergartners at familial dyslexia risk. NeuroReport2003;14:2245–50.

Maurer U, Bucher K, Brem S, Benz R, Kranz F, Schulz E, et al. Neurophysiology inpreschool improves behavioral prediction of reading ability throughout primaryschool. Biol Psychiat 2009;66:341–8.

McGeer EG, McGeer PL. Neuroinflammation in Alzheimer’s disease and mildcognitive impairment: a field in its infancy. J Alzheimer’s Disease2010;19:355–61.

Mengler ED, Hogben JH, Michie PT, Bishop DVM. Poor frequency discrimination isrelated to oral language disorder in children: a psychoacoustic study. Dyslexia2005;11:155–73.

Menning H, Renz A, Seifert J, Maercker A. Reduced mismatch negativity inposttraumatic stress disorder: a compensatory mechanism for chronichyperarousal? Int J Psychophysiol 2008;68:27–34.

Menning H, Roberts LE, Pantev C, et al. Plastic changes in the auditory cortexinduced by intensive frequency discrimination training. NeuroReport2000;11:817–22.

Mento G, Suppiej A, Altoè G, Bisiacchi PS. Functional hemispheric asymmetries inhumans: electrophysiological evidence from preterm infants. Eur J Neurosci2010;31:565–74.

Mervaala E, Alhainen K, Helkala EL, Partanen J, Jousmäki V, Väyrynen M, et al.Electrophysiological and neuropsychological effects of a central alpha2-antagonist atipamezole in healthy volunteers. Behav Brain Res 1993;55:85–91.

Metz-Lutz M-N, Philippini M. Neuropsychological findings in rolandic epilepsy andLandau–Kleffner syndrome. Epilepsia 2006;47:S71–5.

Michie PT. What has MMN revealed about the auditory system in schizophrenia?International J Psychophysiol 2001;42:177–94.

Michie PT, Budd TW, Todd J, Rock D, Wichmann H, Box J, et al. Duration andfrequency mismatch negativity in schizophrenia. Clin Neurophysiol2000;111:1054–65.

Michie PT, Innes-Brown H, Todd J, Jablensky AV. Duration mismatch negativity inbiological relatives of patients with schizophrenia spectrum disorders. BiolPsychiat 2002;52:749–58.

Mikkola K, Kushnerenko E, Partanen E, Serenius-Sirve S, Leipälä J, Huotilainen M,et al. Auditory event-related potentials and cognitive function of pretermchildren at five years of age. Clin Neurophysiol 2007;118:1494–502.

Mikkola K, Wetzel N, Leipälä J, Serenius-Sirve S, Schröger E, Huotilainen M, et al.Behavioral and evoked potential measures of distraction in 5-year-old childrenborn preterm. Int J Psychophysiol 2010;77:8–12.

Minami Y, Kirino E. Schizophrenic patients are impaired in memory reinstatementunderlying mismatch negativity system. Clin Neurophysiol 2005;116:120–8.

Page 30: The mismatch negativity (MMN) – A unique window to ... et al (2011... · Invited Review The mismatch negativity (MMN) – A unique window to disturbed central auditory processing

R. Näätänen et al. / Clinical Neurophysiology 123 (2012) 424–458 453

Missioner P, Ragot R, Derouesné C, Guez D, Renault B. Automatic attentional shiftsinduced by a noradrenergic drug in Alzheimer’s disease: evidence from evokedpotentials. Int J Psychophysiol 1999;33:243–51.

Miyajima M, Ohta K, Hara K, Iino H, Maehara T, Hara M, et al. Abnormal mismatchnegativity for pure-tone sounds in temporal lobe epilepsy. Epilepsy Res2011;94:149–57.

Moberget T, Karns CM, Deouell LY, Lindgren M, Knight RT, Ivry RB. Detectingviolations of sensory expectations following cerebellar degeneration: amismatch negativity study. Neuropsychologia 2008;46:2569–79.

Molholm S, Gomes H, Lobosco J, Deacon D, Ritter W. Feature versus gestaltrepresentation of stimuli in the mismatch negativity system of 7- to 9-year-oldchildren. Psychophysiology 2004;41:385–93.

Molholm S, Martinez A, Ritter W, Javitt DC, Foxe JJ. The neural circuitry of pre-attentive auditory change-detection: an fMRI study of pitch and durationmismatch negativity generators. Cerebr Cortex 2005;15:545–51.

Moreau P, Jolicoeur P, Peretz I. Automatic brain responses to pitch changes incongenital amusia. Ann NY Acad Sci 2009;1169:191–4.

Morgan III CA, Grillon C. Abnormal mismatch negativity in women with sexualassault-related posttraumatic stress disorder. Biol Psychiat 1999;45:827–32.

Morlet D, Bouchet P, Fischer C. Mismatch negativity and N100 monitoring: potentialclinical value and methodological advances. Audiol Neuro-Otol 2000;5:198–206.

Murakami T, Nakagome K, Kamio S, Kasai K, Iwanami A, Hiramatsu K-I, et al. Theeffects of benzodiazepines on event-related potential indices of automatic andcontrolled processing in schizophrenia. A preliminary report. Prog Neuro-Psychoph 2002;26:651–61.

Müller BW, Jüptner M, Jentzen W, Müller SP. Cortical activation to auditorymismatch elicited by frequency deviant and complex novel sounds: a PET study.NeuroImage 2002;17:231–9.

Näätänen R. The role of attention in auditory information processing as revealed byevent-related potentials and other brain measures of cognitive function. BehavBrain Sci 1990;13:201–88.

Näätänen R. Attention and brain function. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum; 1992.Näätänen R. The perception of speech sounds by the human brain as reflected by the

mismatch negativity (MMN) and its magnetic equivalent MMNm.Psychophysiology (Presidential Address, 1999) 2001;38:1–21.

Näätänen R. Somatosensory mismatch negativity: a new clinical tool fordevelopmental neurological research? Dev Med Child Neurol 2009;51:930–1.

Näätänen R, Gaillard AWK. The orienting reflex and the N2 deflection of the event-related potential (ERP). In: Gaillard AWK, Ritter W. (Eds.), Tutorials in eventrelated potential research: endogenous components. North Holland:Amsterdam; 1983. p. 119–41.

Näätänen R, Kähkönen S. Central auditory dysfunction in schizophrenia as revealedby the mismatch negativity (MMN) and its magnetic equivalent MMNm – areview. Int J Neuropsychopharmacol 2009;12:125–35.

Näätänen R, Kreegipuu K. The mismatch negativity (MMN) as an index of differentforms of memory in audition. In: Bäckman L, Nyberg L, editors. Memory, aging,and brain. a festschrift in hounour of Lars-Göarn Nilsson. Psychology Press;2011. p. 287–99.

Näätänen R, Kujala T. The mismatch negativity and its magnetic equivalent: anindex of language impairment or more general cognitive decline in autism? BiolPsychiat 2011;70:212–3.

Näätänen R, Michie PT. Early selective attention effects on the evoked potential. Acritical review and reinterpretation. Biol Psychol 1979;8:81–136.

Näätänen R, Summala H. Road-user behavior and traffic accidents. North-Holland,New York: Amsterdam and American Elsevier; 1976. 270 p.

Näätänen R, Winkler I. The concept of auditory stimulus representation in cognitiveneuroscience. Psychol Bull 1999;6:826–59.

Näätänen R, Gaillard AWK, Mäntysalo S. Early selective-attention effect on evokedpotential reinterpreted. Acta Psychol 1978;42:313–29.

Näätänen R, Kujala T, Kreegipuu K, Carlson S, Escera C, Baldeweg T, et al. Themismatch negativity: an index of cognitive decline in neuropsychiatric andneurological diseases and in aging. Brain 2011. doi:10.1093/brain/awr064.

Näätänen R, Kujala T, Winkler I. Auditory processing that leads to consciousperception: a unique window to central auditory processing opened by themismatch negativity and related responses. Psychophysiology 2011b;48:4–22.

Näätänen R, Lehtokoski A, Lennes M, Cheour M, Huotilainen M, Iivonen A, et al.Language-specific phoneme representations revealed by electric and magneticbrain responses. Nature 1997;385:432–4.

Näätänen R, Tervaniemi M, Sussman E, Paavilainen P, Winkler I. Primitiveintelligence in the auditory cortex. Trends Neurosci 2001;24:283–8.

Näätänen R, Paavilainen P, Rinne T, Alho K. The mismatch negativity (MMN) in basicresearch of central auditory processing: a review. Clin Neurophysiol2007;118:2544–90.

Näätänen R, Schröger E, Alho K. Electrophysiology of Attention. In Wixted J. (Ed.),Stevens’ handbook of experimental psychology, 3rd ed. John Wiley & Sons, Inc.;2002. p. 601–53.

Näätänen R, Schröger E, Karakas S, Tervaniemi M, Paavilainen P. Development of amemory trace for a complex sound in the human brain. NeuroReport1993;4:503–6.

Näätänen R, Pakarinen S, Rinne T, Takegata R. The mismatch negativity (MMN) –towards the optimal paradigm. Clin Neurophysiol 2004;115:140–4.

Näätänen R, Simpson M, Loveless NE. Stimulus deviance and evoked potentials. BiolPsychol 1982;14:53–98.

Naccache L, Puybasset L, Gaillard R, Serve E, Willer J. Auditory mismatch negativityis a good predictor of awakening in comatose patients: a fast and reliableprocedure. Clin Neurophysiol 2005;116:988–9.

Nager W, Münte TF, Bohrer I, Lenarz T, Dengler R, Möbes J, et al. Automatic andattentive processing of sounds in cochlear implant patients –electrophysiological evidence. Restor Neurol Neurosci 2007;25:391–6.

Nakagawa K, Shoji H, Katada A, Ozaki H. How regularities in stimulus series and inISI affect on MMN in persons with intellectual disabilities. Int Congr Ser2002;1232:363–6.

Nakagome K, Ichikawa I, Kanno O, Akaho R, Suzuki M, Takazawa S, et al. Overnighteffects of triazolam on cognitive function: an event-related potentials study.Neuropsychobiology 1998;38:232–40.

Nashida T, Yabe H, Sato T, Sutoh T, Shinosaki N, Kaneko S. Automatic auditoryprocessing in sleep. Sleep 2000;23:1–8.

Naumann A, Bierbrauer J, Przuntek H, Daum I. Attentive and preattentive processingin narcolepsy as revealed by event-related potentials (ERPs). NeuroReport2001;12:2807–11.

Neumann N, Kotchoubey B. Assessment of cognitive functions in severely paralysedand severely brain-damaged patients: neuropsychological andelectrophysiological methods. Brain Res Protoc 2004;14:25–36.

Newcomer JW, Farber NB, Jevtovic-Todorovic V, Selke G, Melson AK, Hersey T, et al.Ketamine-induced NMDA-receptor hypofunction as a model of memoryimpairment and psychosis. Neuropsychopharmacol 1998;20:106–18.

Nittono H, Momose D, Hori T. The vanishing point of the mismatch negativity atsleep onset. Clin Neurophysiol 2001;112:732–9.

Niznikiewicz MA, Spencer KM, Dickey C, Volgmaier M, Seidman LJ, Shenton ME,et al. Abnormal pitch mismatch negativity in individuals with schizotypalpersonality disorder. Schizophr Res 2009;110:188–93.

Nordby H, Brønnick KS, Hugdahl K. Processing of deviant visual events reflected byevent-related brain potentials. In: Ogura C, Koga Y, Shimokochi M. Recentadvances in event-related brain potential research. Amsterdam: Elsevier; 1996.p. 99–104.

Novitski N, Huotilainen M, Tervaniemi M, Näätänen R, Fellman V. Neonatalfrequency discrimination in 250–4000 Hz frequency range:electrophysiological evidence. Clin Neurophysiol 2007;118:417–9.

Oades RD, Dittman-Balcar A, Schepker R, Eggers C, Zerbin D. Auditory event-relatedpotentials (ERPs) and mismatch negativity (MMN) in healthy children and thosewith attention-deficit or Tourette/tic symptoms. Biol Psychol 1996;43:163–85.

Oades RD, Zerbin DA, Dittmann-Balcar A, Eggers C. Auditory event-related potential(ERP) and difference-wave topography in schizophrenic patients with/withoutactive hallucinations and delusions: a comparison with young obsessive-compulsive disorder (OCD) and healthy subjects. Int J Psychophysiol1996;22:185–214.

Oades RD, Wild-Wall N, Juran SA, Sachsse J, Oknina LB, Röpcke B. Auditory changedetection in schizophrenia: sources of activity, related neuropsychologicalfunction and symptoms in patients with a first episode in adolescence, andpatients 14 years after an adolescent illness-onset. BMC Psychiatry 2006;6:7.

Oates PA, Kurtzberg D, Stapells DR. Effects of sensorineural hearing loss on corticalevent-related potential and behavioural measures of speech-sound processing.Ear Hearing 2002;23:399–415.

Ogura C, Nageishi Y, Fukao K, Shimoji Y, Hirano K, Hokama H, et al. Deviate N200component of event-related potentials in Shuchaku–Seikaku, a premorbidpersonality of depression. Jpn J Psychiat Neurol 1991;45:641–51.

Ogura C, Nageishi Y, Omura F, Fukao K, Ohta H, Kishimoto A, et al. N200 componentof event-related potentials in depression. Biol Psychiat 1993;33:720–6.

Oknina LB, Wild-Wall N, Oades RD, Juran SA, Röpcke B, Pfueller U, et al. Frontal andtemporal sources of mismatch negativity in healthy controls, patients at onsetof schizophrenia in adolescence and others at 15 years after onset. Scizophr Res2005;76:25–41.

Olney JW, Farber NB. Glutamate receptor dysfunction and schizophrenia. Arch GenPsychiat 1995;52:998–1007.

Opitz B, Rinne T, Mecklinger A, von Cramon DY, Schröger E. Differential contributionof frontal and temporal cortices to auditory change detection: fMRI and ERPresults. NeuroImage 2002;15:165–74.

Oram Cardy JE, Flagg EJ, Roberts W, Brian J, Roberts TPL. Magnetoencephalographyidentifies rapid temporal processing deficit in autism and language impairment.NeuroReport 2005a;16:329–32.

Oram Cardy JE, Flagg EJ, Roberts W, Roberts TPL. Delayed mismatch field for speechand non-speech sounds in children with autism. NeuroReport 2005b;16:521–5.

Oranje B, Jensen K, Wienberg M, Glenthøj BY. Divergent effects of increasedserotonergic activity on psychophysiological parameters of human attention.Int J Neuropsychopharmacol 2008;11:453–63.

Osawa M, Iijima M, Nageishi Y, Ushijima R, Iwata M. Automatic and controlledprocessing in normal aging and dementia. In: Ogura C, Koga Y, Shimokochi M,editors. Recent advances in event-related brain potential research 1996;Vol.1099. p. 743–50.

Paavilainen P, Alho K, Reinikainen K, Sams M, Näätänen R. Right-hemispheredominance of different mismatch negativities. Electroencephalogr ClinNeurophysio 1991;78:466–79.

Pakarinen S, Huotilainen M, Näätänen R. The mismatch negativity (MMN) with nostandard stimulus. Clin Neurophysiol 2010;121:1043–50.

Pakarinen S, Lovio R, Huotilainen M, Alku P, Näätänen R, Kujala T. Fast multi-featureparadigm for recording several mismatch negativities (MMNs) to phonetic andacoustic changes in speech sounds. Biol Psychol 2009;82:219–26.

Pakarinen S, Takegata R, Rinne T, Huotilainen M, Näätänen R. Measurement ofexistensive auditory discrimination profiles using the mismatch negativity(MMN) of the auditory event-related potential (ERP). Clin Neurophysiol2007;118:177–85.

Page 31: The mismatch negativity (MMN) – A unique window to ... et al (2011... · Invited Review The mismatch negativity (MMN) – A unique window to disturbed central auditory processing

454 R. Näätänen et al. / Clinical Neurophysiology 123 (2012) 424–458

Pang EW, Fowler B. Dissociation of the mismatch negativity and processingnegativity attentional waveforms with nitrous oxide. Psychophysiology1999;36:552–8.

Pang EW, Edmonds GE, Desjardins R, Khan SC, Trainor LJ, Taylor MJ. Mismatchnegativity to speech stimuli in 8-month-old infants and adults. Int JPsychophysiol 1998;29:227–36.

Park H-J, Kwon JS, Youn T, Pae JS, Kim J-J, Kim M-S, et al. Statistical parametricmapping of LORETA using high density EEG and individual MRI: application tomismatch negativities in schizophrenia. Hum Brain Mapp 2002;17:168–78.

Paul I, Bott C, Heim S, Eulitz C, Elbert T. Reduced hemispheric asymmetry of theauditory N260m in dyslexia. Neuropsychologia 2006a;44:785–94.

Paul I, Bott C, Heim S, Wienbruch C, Elbert T. Phonological but not auditorydiscrimination is impaired in dyslexia. Eur J Neurosci 2006b;24:2945–53.

Pause BM, Krauel K. Chemosensory event-related potentials (CSERP) as a key to thepsychology of odors. Int J Psychophysiol 2000;36:105–22.

Pazo-Alvarez P, Amenedo E, Cadaveira F. Automatic detection of motion directionchanges in the human brain. Eur J Neurosci 2004;19:1978–86.

Pazo-Alvarez P, Cadaveira F, Amenedo E. MMN in the visual modality: a review. BiolPsychol 2003;63:199–236.

Peach RK, Newhoff M, Rubin SS. Attention in aphasia as revealed by event-relatedpotentials: a preliminary investigation. In: Lemmer ML. (Ed.) Clinicalaphasiology, vol. 21. Austin, TX: Pro-Ed; 1992. p. 323–33.

Peach RK, Newhoff M, Rubin SS. Attention in aphasia as revealed by event-relatedpotentials: a preliminary investigation. Clin Aphasiol 1993;21:323–33.

Peach RK, Rubin SS, Newhoff M. A topographic event-related potential analysis ofthe attention deficit for auditory processing in Aphasia. Clin Aphasiol1994;22:81–96.

Pekkonen E. Mismatch negativity in aging and in Alzheimer’s and Parkinson’sdiseases. Audiol Neuro-Otol 2000;5:216–24.

Pekkonen E, Ahveninen J, Jääskeläinen IP, Seppä K, Näätänen R, Sillanaukee P.Selective acceleration of auditory processing in chronic alcoholics. Alcohol ClinExp Res 1998;22:605–9.

Pekkonen E, Hirvonen J, Ahveninen J, Kähkönen S, Kaakkola S, Huttunen J, et al.Memory-based comparison process not attenuated by haloperiodol: acombined MEG and EEG study. NeuroReport 2002;13:177–81.

Pekkonen E, Hirvonen J, Jääskeläinen IP, Kaakkola S, Huttunen J. Auditory sensorymemory and the cholinergic system: implication for Alzheimer’s disease.NeuroImage 2001a;14:376–82.

Pekkonen E, Huotilainen M, Virtanen J, Näätänen R, Ilmoniemi RJ, Erkinjuntti T.Alzheimer’s disease affects parallel processing between the auditory cortices.NeuroReport 1996b;7:1363–8.

Pekkonen E, Huotilainen M, Virtanen J, Sinkkonen J, Rinne T, Ilmoniemi R, et al. Age-related functional differences between auditory cortices: a whole-head MEG-study. NeuroReport 1995a;6:1803–6.

Pekkonen E, Jääskeläinen IP, Erkinjuntti T, Hietanen M, Huotilainen M, Ilmoniemi RJ,et al. Preserved stimulus deviance detection in Alzheimer’s disease.NeuroReport 2001b;12:1649–52.

Pekkonen E, Jääskeläinen IP, Hietanen M, Huotilainen M, Näätänen R, Ilmoniemi RJ,et al. Impaired preconscious auditory processing and cognitive function inAlzheimer’s disease. Clin Neurophysiol 1999;110:1942–7.

Pekkonen E, Jääskeläinen IP, Kaakkola S, Ahveninen J. Cholinergic modulation ofpreattentive auditory processing in aging. NeuroImage 2005;27:387–92.

Pekkonen E, Jousmäki V, Könönen M, Reinikainen K, Partanen J. Auditory sensorymemory impairment in Alzheimer’s disease: an event-related potential study.NeuroReport 1994;5:2537–40.

Pekkonen E, Jousmäki V, Reinikainen K, Partanen J. Automatic auditorydiscrimination is impaired in Parkinson’s disease. Electroen Clin Neuro1995c;95:47–52.

Pekkonen E, Osipova D, Laaksovirta H. Magnetoencephalographic evidence ofabnormal auditory processing in amyotrophic lateral sclerosis with bulbarsigns. Clin Neurophysiol 2004;115:309–15.

Pekkonen E, Rinne T, Näätänen R. Variability and replicability of the mismatchnegativity. Electroenceph Clin Neurophysiol 1995b;96:546–54.

Pekkonen E, Rinne T, Reinikainen K, Kujala T, Alho K, Näätänen R. Aging effects onauditory processing: an event-related potential study. Exp Aging Res1996a;22:171–84.

Peretz I, Brattico E, Järvenpää M, Tervaniemi M. The amusic brain: in tune, out ofkey, and unaware. Brain 2009;132:1277–86.

Pettigrew CM, Murdoch BE, Chenery HJ, Kei J. The relationship between themismatch negativity (MMN) and psycholinguistic models of spoken wordprocessing. Aphasiology 2004a;18:3–28.

Pettigrew CM, Murdoch BE, Ponton CW, Finnigan S, Alku P, Sockalingam R, et al.Automatic auditory processing of English words as indexed by the mismatchnegativity, using a multiple deviant paradigm. Ear Hearing 2004b;25:284–301.

Pettigrew CM, Murdoch BE, Kei J, Ponton CW, Alku P, Chenery HJ. The mismatchnegativity (MMN) response to complex tones and spoken words in individualswith aphasia. Aphasiology 2005;19:131–63.

Pietrowsky R, Fehm HL, Born J. An analysis of the effects of corticotropin-releasinghormone (h-CRH), adenocorticotropin (ACTH) and b-endorphin on ERPs inhumans. In: Brunia CHM, Gaillard AWK, Kok A. (Eds.), Psychol Brain Res1990;2:106–10 [Tilbury University Press].

Pihko E, Kujala T, Mickos A, Antell H, Alku P, Byring R, et al. Magnetic fields evokedby speech sounds in pre-school children. Clin Neurophysiol 2005;116:112–9.

Pihko E, Leppänen PHT, Eklund KM, Cheour M, Guttorm TK, Lyytinen H. Corticalresponses of infants with and without a genetic risk for dyslexia: I. Age effects.NeuroReport 1999;10:901–5.

Pihko E, Kujala T, Mickos A, Alku P, Byring R, Korkman M. Language impairment isreflected in auditory evoked fields. Int J Psychophysiol 2008;68:161–9.

Pihko E, Mickos A, Kujala T, Pihlgren A, Westman M, Alku P, et al. Groupintervention changes brain activity in bilingual language-impaired children.Cereb Cortex 2007;17:849–58.

Pincze Z, Lakatos P, Rajkai C, Ulbert I, Karmos G. Separation of mismatch negativityand the N1 wave in the auditory cortex of the cat: a topographic study. ClinNeurophysiol 2001;112:778–84.

Pincze Z, Lakatos P, Rajkai C, Ulbert I, Karmos G. Effect of deviant probability andinterstimulus/interdeviant interval on the auditory N1 and mismatch negativityin the cat auditory cortex. Brain Res Cognitive Brain Res 2002;13:249–53.

Polo MD, Escera C, Gual A, Grau C. Mismatch negativity and auditory sensorymemory in chronic alcoholics. Alcohol Clin Exp Res 1999;23:1744–50.

Polo MD, Escera C, Yago E, Alho K, Gual A, Grau C. Electrophysiological evidence ofabnormal activation of the cerebral network of involuntary attention inalcoholism. Clin Neurophysiol 2003;114:134–46.

Polo MD, Newton P, Rogers D, Escera C, Butler S. ERPs and behavioural indices oflong-term preattentive and attentive deficits after closed head injury.Neuropsychologia 2002;40:2350–9.

Ponton CW, Don M. The mismatch negativity in cochlear implant users. Ear Hearing1995;16:131–46.

Ponton CW, Don M. Cortically-evoked activity recorded from cochlear implantusers. Methods and Applications. In: Cullington H. (Ed.), Cochlear implantsobjective measures. London: Whurr Publishers Ltd; 2003. p. 187–230.

Ponton CW, Eggermont JJ. Of kittens and kids: altered cortical maturation followingprofound deafness and cochlear implant use. Audiol Neuro-Otol2001;6:363–3680.

Ponton CW, Eggermont JJ. Electrophysiological measures of human auditory systemmaturation: Relationship with neuroanatomy and behavior. in: Burkhard RF,Don M, Eggermont JJ. (Eds.), Auditory evoked potentials: basic principles andclinical application. Philadelphia: Lippincott Williams & Wilkins; 2007. p. 358–402.

Ponton CW, Bernstein LE, Auer ET. Mismatch negativity with visual-only andaudiovisual speech. Brain Topogr 2009;21:207–15.

Ponton CW, Don M, Eggermont JJ, Kwong B. The integrated mismatch negativity(MMNi): a noise-free representation of evoked responses allowing single-pointdistribution-free statistical tests. Electroen Clin Neuro 1997;104:143–50.

Ponton CW, Eggermont JJ, Don M, Waring MD, Kwong B, Cunningham J, et al.Maturation of the mismatch negativity: effects of profound deafness andcochlear implant use. Audiol Neuro-Otol 2000;5:167–85.

Potts GF, Hirayasu Y, O’Donnell BF, Shenton ME, McCarley RW. High-densityrecording and topographic analysis of the auditory oddball event-relatedpotential in patients with schizophrenia. Biol Psychiat 1998;44:982–9.

Price GW, Michie PT, Johnston J, Innes-Brown H, Kent A, Clissa P, et al. Amultivariate electrophysiological endophenotype, from a unitary cohort, showsgreater research utility than any single feature in the Western Australian familystudy of schizophrenia. Biol Psychiat 2006;60:1–10.

Pritchard W, Sokhadze E, Houliman M. Effects of nicotine and smoking on event-related potentials: a review. Nicotine Tobacco Res 2004;6:961–84.

Pulvermuller F, Shtyrov Y, Kujala T, Naatanen R. Word-specific cortical activity asrevealed by the mismatch negativity. Psychophysiol 2004;41:106–12.

Qiu X, Yang X, Qiao Z, Wang L, Ning N, Shi J, et al. Impairment in processing visualinformation at the pre-attentive stage in patients with a major depressivedisorder: a visual mismatch negativity study. Neurosci Lett 2011;491:53–7.

Rabinowicz EF, Silipo GMA, Goldman R, Javitt DC. Auditory sensory dysfunction inschizophrenia: imprecision or distractibility. Arch Gen Psychiat2000;57:1149–55.

Ragazzoni A, Grippo A, Tozzi F, Zaccara G. Event-related potentials in patients withtotal locked-in state due to fulminant Guillain–Barré syndrome. Int JPsychophysiol 2000;37:99–109.

Raggi A, Consonni M, Iannaccone S, Perani D, Zamboni M, Sferrazza B, et al. Auditoryevent-related potentials in non-demented patients with sporadic amyotrophiclateral sclerosis. Clin Neurophysiol 2008;119:342–50.

Rahne T, von Specht H, Mühler R. Sorted averaging-application to auditory event-related responses. J Neurosci Meth 2008;172:74–8.

Rasser PE, Schall U, Todd J, Michie PT, Ward PB, Johnston P, et al. Gray matterdeficits, mismatch negativity, and outcomes in Schizophrenia. SchizophreniaBull 2011;37:131–40.

Raz A, Deouell LY, Bentin S. Is pre-attentive processing compromised by prolongedwakefulness? Effects of total sleep deprivation on the mismatch negativity.Psychophysiology 2001;38:787–95.

Realmuto G, Begleiter H, Odencrantz J, Porjesz B. Event-related potential evidence ofdysfunction in automatic processing in abstinent alcoholics. Bioll Psychiat1993;33:594–601.

Reinvang I, Nordby H, Nielsen CS. Information processing deficits in head injuryassessed with ERPs reflecting early and late processing stages.Neuropsychologia 2000;38:995–1005.

Reite M, Teale P, Rojas DC, Asherin R, Hernandez O. MEG auditory evoked fieldssuggest altered structural/functional asymmetry in primary but not secondaryauditory cortex in bipolar disorder. Bipol Disord 2009;11:371–81.

Renvall H, Hari R. Diminished auditory mismatch fields in dyslexic adults. AnnNeurol 2003;53:551–7.

Restuccia D, Della Marca C, Valeriani M, Leggio MG, Molinari M. Cerebellar damageimpairs detection of somatosensory input changes. A somatosensory mismatch-negativity study. Brain 2007;130:276–87.

Page 32: The mismatch negativity (MMN) – A unique window to ... et al (2011... · Invited Review The mismatch negativity (MMN) – A unique window to disturbed central auditory processing

R. Näätänen et al. / Clinical Neurophysiology 123 (2012) 424–458 455

Restuccia D, Zanini S, Cazzagon M, Del Piero I, Martucci L, Della Marca G.Somatosensory mismatch negativity in healthy children. Dev Med ChildNeurol 2009;51:991–8.

Richardson U, Leppänen PHT, Leiwo M, Lyytinen H. Speech perception of infantswith high familial risk for dyslexia differs at the age of 6 months. DevNeuropsychol 2003;23:387–97.

Riekkinen Jr P, Pääkkönen A, Karhu J, Partanen J, Soininen H, Laakso M, et al. THAdisrupts mismatch negativity in Alzheimer disease. Psychopharmacology1997;133:203–6.

Rinker T, Kohls G, Richter C, Maas V, Schulz E, Schecker M. Abnormal frequencydiscrimination in children with SLI as indexed by mismatch negativity (MMN).Neurosci Lett 2007;413:99–104.

Rinne T, Alho K, Ilmoniemi RJ, Virtanen J, Näätänen R. Separate time behaviors of thetemporal and frontal mismatch negativity sources. NeuroImage 2000;12:14–9.

Rinne T, Degerman A, Alho K. Superior temporal and inferior frontal cortices areactivated by infrequent sound duration decrements: an fMRI study.NeuroImage 2005;26:66–72.

Rinne T, Gratton G, Fabiani M, Cowan N, Maclin E, Stinard A, et al. Scalp-recordedoptical signals make sound processing in the auditory cortex visible.NeuroImage 1999;10:620–4.

Rinne T, Särkkä A, Degerman A, Schröger E, Alho K. Two separate mechanismsunderlie auditory change detection and involuntary control of attention. BrainRes 2006;1077:135–43.

Rissling AJ, Makeig S, Braff DL, Light GA. Neurophysiologic markers of abnormalbrain activity in schizophrenia. Curr Psychiat Reports 2010;12:572–8.

Roberts TPL, Cannon KM, Tavabi K, Blaskey L, Khan SY, Monroe JF, et al. Auditorymagnetic mismatch field latency: a biomarker for language impairment inautism. Biol Psychiat 2011;70:263–9.

Roberts TPL, Schmidt GL, Egeth M, Blaskey L, Rey MM, Edgar C, et al.Electrophysiological signature: magnetoencephalographic studies of theneural correlates of language impairment in autism spectrum disorders. Int JPsychophysiol 2008;68:149–60.

Röder B, Teder-Sälejärvi W, Sterr A, Rösler F, Hillyard SA, Neville HJ. Improvedauditory spatial tuning in blind humans. Nature 1999;400:162–6.

Rodriguez HS, Corral M, Cadaveira F. Mismatch negativity in young children ofalcoholics from high-density families. Alcohol Clin Exp Res 1998;22:1363–8.

Roeske D, Ludwig KU, Neuhoff N, Becker J, Bartling J, Bruder J, et al. First-genome-wide association scan on neurophysiological endophenotypes points to trans-regulation effects on SLC2A3 in dyslexic children. Mol Psychiatr2011;16:97–107.

Roger C, Hasbroucq T, Rabat A, Vidal F. Neurophysics of temporal discrimination inthe rat: a mismatch negativity study. Psychophysiol 2009;46:1028–32.

Roman S, Canévet G, Marquis P, Triglia J-M, Liégeois-Chauvel C. Relationshipbetween auditory perception skills and mismatch negativity recorded in freefield in cochlear-implant users. Hearing Res 2005;201:10–20.

Rosburg T. Left hemispheric dipole locations of the neuromagnetic mismatchnegativity to frequency, intensity and duration deviants. Cogn Brain Res2003;16:83–90.

Rosburg T, Marinou V, Haueisen J, Smesny S, Sauer H. Effects of lorazepam on theneuromagnetic mismatch negativity (MMNm) and auditory evoked fieldcomponent N100m. Neuropsychopharmacol 2004;29:1723–33.

Rosburg T, Trautner P, Dietl T, Korzyukov OA, Boutros NN, Schaller C, et al. Subduralrecordings of the mismatch negativity (MMN) in patients with focal epilepsy.Brain 2005;128:819–28.

Rosburg T, Trautner P, Ludowig E, Schaller C, Kurthen M, Elger CE, et al.Hippocampal event-related potentials to tone duration deviance in a passiveoddball paradigm in humans. NeuroImage 2007;37:274–81.

Rothenberger, A. Electrical brain activity in children with hyperkinetic syndrome:evidence of frontal cortical dysfunction. In: Sergeant J. (Ed.), Europeanapproaches to hyperkinetic disorder. Zürich: Trümpi; 1995. p. 255–270.

Rothenberger A, Banaschewski T, Heinrich H, Moll GH, Schmidt MH, van’t KloosterB. Comorbidity in ADHD-children: effects of coexisting conduct disorder or ticdisorder on event-related brain potentials in an auditory selective-attentiontask. Eur Arch Psychiat Clin Neurosci 2000;250:101–10.

Rowland LP, Shneider NA. Medical progress: amyotrophic lateral sclerosis. NewEngl J Med 2001;344:1688–700.

Ruby P, Caclin A, Boulet S, Delpuech C, Morlet D. Odd sound processing in thesleeping brain. J Cognitive Neurosci 2008;20:296–311.

Rudner M, Foo C, Sundewall-Thorén E, Lunner T, Rönnberg J. Phonological mismatchand explicit cognitive processing in a sample of 102 hearing-aid users. Int JAudiol 2008;47:S163–70.

Ruusuvirta T, Huotilainen M, Fellman V, Näätänen R. Numerical discrimination innewborn infants as revealed by event-related potentials to tone sequences. Eur JNeurosci 2009;30:620–4.

Ruusuvirta T, Koivisto K, Wikgren J, Astikainen P. Processing of melodic contours inurethane-anaesthetized rats. Eur J Neurosci 2007;26:701–3.

Ruusuvirta T, Penttonen M, Korhonen T. Auditory cortical event-related potentialsto pitch deviances in rats. Neurosci Lett 1998;248:45–8.

Salisbury DF, Kuroki N, Kasai K, Shenton ME, McCarley RW. Progressive andinterrelated functional and structural evidence of post-onset brain reduction inschizophrenia. Arch Gen Psychiat 2007;64:521–9.

Salisbury DF, Shenton ME, Griggs CB, Bonner-Jackson A, McCarley RW. Mismatchnegativity in chronic schizophrenia and first-episode schizophrenia. Arch GenPsychiat 2002;59:686–94.

Sallinen M, Kaartinen J, Lyytinen H. Is the appearance of mismatch negativity duringstage 2 sleep related to the elicitation of K-complex. Electroen Clin Neuro1994;91:140–8.

Sallinen M, Kaartinen J, Lyytinen H. Processing of auditory stimuli during tonic andphasic periods of REM sleep as revealed by event-related brain potentials. JSleep Res 1996;5:220–8.

Sallinen M, Lyytinen H. Mismatch negativity during objective and subjectivesleepiness. Psychophysiology 1997;34:694–702.

Salmi J, Huotilainen M, Pakarinen S, Siren T, Alho K, Aronen ET. Does sleep qualifyaffect involuntary attention switching system? Neurosci Lett 2005;390:150–5.

Salo S, Peltola MS, Aaltonen O, Johansson R, Lang AH, Laurikainen E. Stability ofmemory traces for speech sounds in cochlear implant patients. LogopedPhoniatr Vocol 2002;27:132–8.

Sams M, Hämäläinen M, Antervo A, Kaukoranta E, Reinikainen K, Hari R. Cerebralneuromagnetic responses evoked by short auditory stimuli. Electroen ClinNeuro 1985b;61:254–66.

Sams M, Paavilainen P, Alho K, Näätänen R. Auditory frequency discrimination andevent-related potentials. Electroen Clin Neuro 1985a;62:437–48.

Sams M, Hari R, Rif J, Knuuttila J. The human auditory sensory memory tracepersists about 10 s: neuromagnetic evidence. J Cognitive Neurosci1993;5:363–70.

Sandmann P, Kegel A, Eichele T, Dillier N, Lai W, Bendixen A, et al.Neurophysiological evidence of impaired musical sound perception incochlear-implant users. Clin Neurophysiol 2010;121:2070–82.

Santos MAR, Munhoz MSL, Peixoto MAL, Haase VG, Rodrigues JL, Resende LM.Mismatch negativity contribution in multiple sclerosis patients. Revi BrasOtorrinolaringol 2006;72:800–7.

Särkämö T, Pihko E, Laitinen S, Forsblom A, Soinila S, Mikkonen M, et al. Music andspeech listening enhance the recovery of early sensory processing after stroke. JCognitive Neurosci 2010a;22:2716–27.

Särkämö T, Tervaniemi M, Laitinen S, Forsblom A, Soinila S, Mikkonen M, et al.Music listening enhances cognitive recovery and mood after middle cerebralartery stroke. Brain 2008;131:866–76.

Särkämö T, Tervaniemi M, Soinila S, Autti T, Silvennoinen HM, Laine M, et al.Cognitive deficits associated with acquired amusia after stroke: aneuropsychological follow-up study. Neuropsychologia 2009;47:2642–51.

Särkämö T, Tervaniemi M, Soinila S, Autti T, Silvennoinen HM, Laine M, et al.Auditory and cognitive deficits associated with acquired amusia in stroke: amagnetoencephalography and neuropsychological follow-up study. PLoS ONE2010b;5:e15157.

Sasaki T, Campbell KB, Bazana PG, Stelmack RM. Individual differences in mismatchnegativity measures of involuntary attention shift. Clin Neurophysiolo2000;111:1553–60.

Sato Y, Yabe H, Todd J, Michie P, Shinozaki N, Sutoh T, et al. Impairment in activationof a frontal attention-switch mechanism in schizophrenic patients. Biol Psychol2002;62:49–63.

Sato Y, Yabe H, Hiruma T, Sutoh T, Shinozaki N, Nashida T, et al. Do changes inprobability on the MMN have a different effect in schizophrenic patients. In:Hashimoto I, Kakigi R. (Eds.), Recent advances in human neurophysiology.Amsterdam: Elsevier, 1998. p. 803–8.

Satterfield JH, Schell AM, Nicholas T, Backs RW. Topographic study of auditoryevent-related potentials in normal boys and boys with attention deficit disorderwith hyperactivity. Psychophysiology 1988;25:591–606.

Sauer H, Volz HP. Functional magnetic resonance imaging and magnetic resonancespectroscopy in schizophrenia. Curr Opin Psychiat 2000;13:21–6.

Sawada M, Iida J, Ota T, Negoro H, Tanaka S, Sadamatsu M, Kishimoto T. Effects ofosmotic-release methylphenidate in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder asmeasured by event-related potentials. Psychiat Clin Neuros 2010;64:491–8.

Schall U, Catts SV, Chaturvedi S, Liebert B, Redenbach J, Karayanidis F, et al. Theeffect of clozapine therapy on frontal lobe dysfunction in schizophrenia:neuropsychology and event-related potential measures. Int JNeuropsychopharmacol 1998;1:19–29.

Schall U, Catts SV, Karayanidis F, Ward PB. Auditory event-related potential indicesof fronto-temporal information processing in schizophrenia syndromes: validoutcome prediction of clozapine therapy in a three-year follow-up. Int JNeuropsychopharmacol 1999;2:83–93.

Schall U, Johnston P, Todd J, Ward PB, Michie PT. Functional neuroanatomy ofauditory mismatch processing: an event-related fMRI study of duration-deviantoddballs. NeuroImage 2003;20:729–36.

Schiff S, Valenti P, Andrea P, Lot M, Bisiacchi P, Gatta A, et al. The effect of aging onauditory components of event-related brain potentials. Clin Neurophysiol2008;119:1795–802.

Schreiber H, Stolz-Born G, Kornhuber H, Born J. Event-related potential correlates ofimpaired selective attention in children at high risk for schizophrenia. BiolPsychiat 1992;32:634–51.

Schreiber H, Stolz-Born G, Pietrowsky R, Kornhuber HH, Fehm HL, Born J. Improvedevent-related potential signs of selective attention after the administration ofthe cholecystokinin analog ceruletide in healthy persons. Biol Psychiat1995;37:702–12.

Schroeder MM, Ritter W, Vaughan Jr HG. The mismatch negativity to novel stimulireflects cognitive decline. Ann NY Acad Sci 1995;769:399–401.

Schroeder MM, Handelsman L, Torres L, Dorfman D, Rinaldi P, Jacobsen J, et al. Earlyand late cognitive event-related potentials mark stages of HIV-1 infection in thedrug-user risk group. Biol Psychiat 1994;35:54–69.

Page 33: The mismatch negativity (MMN) – A unique window to ... et al (2011... · Invited Review The mismatch negativity (MMN) – A unique window to disturbed central auditory processing

456 R. Näätänen et al. / Clinical Neurophysiology 123 (2012) 424–458

Schröger E. On the detection of auditory deviants: a pre-attentive activationmodel. Psychophysiol 1997;34:245–2457.

Schröger E, Tervaniemi M, Wolff C, Näätänen R. Preattentive periodicity detection inauditory patterns as governed by time and intensity information. Cognit BrainRes 1996;4:145–8.

Schulte-Körne G, Bruder J. Clinical neurophysiology of visual and auditoryprocessing in dyslexia: a review. Clinl Neurophysiol 2010;121:1794–809.

Schulte-Körne G, Deimel W, Bartling J, Remschmidt H. Auditory processing anddyslexia: evidence for a specific speech processing deficit. NeuroReport1998;9:337–40.

Schulte-Körne G, Deimel W, Bartling J, Remschmidt H. Pre-attentive processing ofauditory patterns in dyslexic human subjects. Neurosci Lett 1999;276:41–4.

Schulte-Körne G, Deimel W, Bartling J, Remschmidt H. Speech perception deficit indyslexic adults as measured by mismatch negativity (MMN). Int J Psychophysiol2001;40:77–87.

Sculthorpe LD, Ouellet DR, Campbell KB. MMN elicitation during natural sleep toviolations of an auditory pattern. Brain Res 2009;1290:52–62.

Schwieler L, Linderholm KR, Nilsson-Todd LK, Erhardt S, Engberg G. Clozapineinteracts with the glycine site of the NMDA receptor: electrophysiological studiesof dopamine neurons in the rat ventral tegmental area. Life Sci 2008;83:170–5.

Sebastian C, Yasin I. Speech versus tone processing in compensated dyslexia:discrimination and lateralization with a dichotic mismatch negativity (MMN)paradigm. Int J Psychophysiol 2008;70:115–26.

Seri S, Cerquiglini A, Pisani F, Curatolo P. Autism in tuberous sclerosis: evokedpotential evidence for a deficit in auditory sensory processing. ClinNeurophysiol 1999;110:1825–30.

Seri S, Pisani F, Thai JN, Cerquiglini A. Pre-attentive sensory processing in autisticspectrum disorder. Are electromagnetic measurements telling us a coherentstory. Int J Psychophysiol 2007;63:159–63.

Serra JM, Escera C, Sánchez-Turet M, Sánchez-Sastre J, Grau C. The H1-receptorantagonist chlorpheniramine decreases the ending phase of the mismatchnegativity of the human auditory event-related potentials. Neurosci Lett1996;203:77–80.

Shafer VL, Morr ML, Datta H, Kurzberg D, Schwartz RG. Neurophysiological indexesof speech processing deficits in children with specific language impairment. JCognitive Neurosci 2005;17:1168–80.

Shafer VL, Ponton C, Datta H, Morr ML, Schwartz RG. Neurophysiological indices ofattention to speech in children with specific language impairment. ClinNeurophysiol 2007;118:1230–43.

Shankarnarayan VC, Maruthy S. Mismatch negativity in children with dyslexiaspeaking Indian languages. Behav Brain Funct 2007;3:36.

Sharma M, Purdy SC, Newall P, Wheldall K, Beaman R, Dillon H. Electrophysiologicaland behavioral evidence of auditory processing deficits in children with readingdisorder. Clin Neurophysiol 2005;117:1130–44.

Sharma M, Purdy SC, Newall P, Wheldall K, Beaman R. Refractory effects onauditory-evoked responses in children with reading disorders. NeuroReport2007;18:133–6.

Sharma A, Purdy SC, Newall P, Wheldall K, Beaman R, Dillon H. Electrophysiologicaland behavioral evidence of auditory processing deficits in children with readingdisorder. Clin Neurophysiol 2006;117:1130–44.

Sharma M, Purdy AS, Kelly SC. Comorbidity of auditory processing, language, andreading disorders. J Speech Lang Hear R 2009;52:706–22.

Shelley AM, Ward PB, Catts SV, Michie PT, Andrews S, McConaghy N. Mismatchnegativity: an index of a preattentive processing deficit in schizophrenia. BiolPsychiat 1991;203:77–80.

Shestakova A, Brattico E, Huotilainen M, Galunov V, Soloviev A, Sams M, et al.Abstract phoneme representations in the left temporal cortex: magneticmismatch negativity study. NeuroReport 2002;13:1813–6.

Shestakova A, Huotilainen M, Ceponiene R, Cheour M. Event-related potentialsassociated with second language learning in children. Clin Neurophysiol2003;114:1507–12.

Shin KS, Kim JS, Kang DH, Koh Y, Choi JS, O’Donnell BF, et al. Pre-attentive auditoryprocessing in ultra-high-risk for schizophrenia with magnetoencephalography.Biol Psychiat 2009;65:1071–8.

Shinozaki N, Yabe H, Sato Y, Hiruma T, Sutoh T, Nashida T, et al. The difference inmismatch negativity between the acute and post-acute phase of schizophrenia.Biol Psychol 2002;59:105–19.

Shinozaki N, Yabe H, Sutoh T, Hiruma T, Kaneko S. Somatosensory automaticresponses to deviant stimuli. Cognit Brain Res 1998;7:165–71.

Shtyrov Y, Pulvermüller F. Language in the mismatch negativity design:motivations, benefits, and prospects. J Psychophysiol 2007;21:176–87.

Shtyrov Y, Kujala T, Ahveninen J, Tervaniemi M, Alku P, Ilmoniemi RJ, et al.Background acoustic noise and the hemispheric lateralization of speechprocessing in the human brain: magnetic mismatch negativity study.Neurosci Lett 1998;251:141–4.

Shtyrov Y, Kujala T, Ilmoniemi RJ, Näätänen R. Noise affects speech-signalprocessing differently in the cerebral hemispheres. NeuroReport1999;10:2189–92.

Siegal M, Blades M. Language and auditory processing in autism. Trends Cogn Sci2003;7:378–80.

Simoens VL, Istók E, Hyttinen S, Hirvonen A, Näätänen R, Tervaniemi M.Psychosocial stress attenuates general sound processing and duration changedetection. Psychophysiology 2007;44:30–8.

Simpson TP, Manara AR, Kane NM, Barton RL, Rowlands CA, Butler SR. Effect ofpropofol anaesthesia on the event-related potential mismatch negativity andthe auditory-evoked potential N1. Britt J Anaesthesia 2002;89:382–8.

Smolnik R, Molle M, Fehm HL, Born J. Brain potentials and attention after acute andsubchronic intranasal administration of ACTH 4-10 and desacetyl-alpha-MSH inhumans. Neuroendocrinology 1999;70:63–72.

Smolnik R, Pietrowsky R, Fehm HL, Born J. Enhanced selective attention after low-dose administration of the benzodiazepine antagonist flumazenil. J ClinPsychopharm 1998;18:241–7.

Sorokin A, Alku P, Kujala T. Change and novelty detection in speech and non-speechsound streams. Brain Res 2010;1327:77–90.

Spackman LA, Towell A, Boyd SG. Somatosensory discrimination: an intracranialevent-related potential study of children with refractory epilepsy. Brain Res2010;1310:68–76.

Spackman LA, Boyd SG, Towell A. Effects of stimulus frequency and duration onsomatosensory discrimination responses. Exp Brain Res 2007;117:21–30.

Squires KC, Squires NK, Hillyard SA. Decision-related cortical potentials during anauditory signal detection task with cued observation intervals. J Exp PsycholHuman 1975;1:268–79.

Srinivasan N, Baijal S. Concentrative meditation enhances preattentive processing: amismatch negativity study. NeuroReport 2007;18:1709–12.

Stagg C, Hindley P, Tales A, Butler S. Visual mismatch negativity: the detection ofstimulus change. NeuroReport 2004;15:659–63.

Stapells DR. Cortical event related potentials to auditory stimuli. In: Katz J. (Ed.),Handbook of clinical audiology, 5th ed. Baltimore, MD: Lippincott & Wilkins;2002. p. 378–406.

Stefanics G, Kimura M, Czigler I. Visual mismatch negativity reveals automaticdetection of sequential regularity violation. Front Hum Neurosci 2011;5:46.

Stix G. How to build a better learner. Sci Am 2011;305:50–7.Stone JM, Bramon E, Pauls A, Sumich A, McGuire PK. Thalamic neurochemical

abnormalities in individuals with prodromal symptoms of schizophrenia –relationship to auditory event-related potentials. Psychiat Res-Neuroim2010;183:174–6.

Stoodley CJ, Hill R, Stein JF, Bishop DVM. Auditory event-related potentials differ indyslexic even when auditory psychophysical performance is normal. Brain Res2006;1121:190–9.

Sullivan EV, Shear PK, Zipursky RB, Sagar HJ, Pfefferbaum A. A deficit profile ofexecutive, memory, and motor functions in schizophrenia. Biol Psychiat1995;36:641–53.

Sysoeva OV, Maluchenko NV, Smirnov KS, Shleptsova VA, Ivanitsky AM, TonevitskyAG. Pecularities of brain information processing in persons with differentserotonin transporter gene variants. B Exp Biol Med 2009;148:731–4.

Takei Y, Kumano S, Hattori S, Uehara T, Kawakubo Y, Kasai K, et al. Preattentivedysfunction in major depression: a magnetoencephalography study usingauditory mismatch negativity. Psychophysiology 2009;46:52–61.

Takei Y, Kumano S, Maki Y, Hattori S, Kawakubo Y, Kasai K, et al. Preattentivedysfunction in bipolar disorder: a MEG study using mismatch negativity. ProgNeuro-Psychoph 2010;34:903–12.

Tales A, Butler S. Visual mismatch negativity highlights abnormal preattentivevisual processing in Alzheimer’s disease. NeuroReport 2006;17:887–90.

Tales A, Haworth J, Wilcock G, Newton P, Butler S. Visual mismatch negativityhighlights abnormal pre-attentive visual processing in mild cognitiveimpairment and Alzheimer’s disease. Neuropsychologia 2008;46:1224–32.

Tales A, Muir J L, Bayer A, Jones R, Snowden RJ. Phasic visual alertness in Alzheimer’sdisease and ageing. NeuroReport 2002a;13:2557–60.

Tales A, Newton P, Troscianko T, Butler S. Mismatch negativity in the visualmodality. NeuroReport 1999;110:3363–7.

Tales A, Troscianko T, Wilcock GK, Newton P, Butler SR. Age-related changes in thepreattentional detection of visual change. NeuroReport 2002b;13:969–72.

Tanaka M, Okubo O, Fuchigami T, Harada K. A study of mismatch negativity innewborns. Pediatr Int 2001;43:281–6.

Tarkka IM, Luukainen-Markkula R, Pitkänen K, Hämäläinen H. Alterations in visualand auditory processing in hemispatial neglect: an evoked potential follow-upstudy. Int J Psychophysiol 2011;79:272–9.

Taylor MJ, Baldeweg T. Application of EEG, ERP and intracranial recordings to theinvestigation of cognitive functions in children. Dev Sci 2002;5:318–34.

Tervaniemi M, Winkler I, Näätänen R. Pre-attentive categorization of sounds bytimbre as revealed by event-related potentials. NeuroReport 1997;8:2571–4.

Tervaniemi M, Kujala A, Alho K, Virtanen J, Ilmoniemi RJ, Näätänen R. Functionalspecialization of the human auditory cortex in processing phonetic andmusical sounds: a magnetoencephalographic (MEG) study. NeuroImage 1999;9:330–6.

Tervaniemi M, Medvedev SV, Alho K, Pakhomov SV, Roudas MS, van Zuijen TL, et al.Lateralized automatic auditory processing of phonetic versus musicalinformation: a PET study. Hum Brain Mapp 2000;10:74–9.

Thönnesen H, Zvyagintsev M, Harke KC, Boers F, Dammers J, Norra C, et al.Optimized mismatch negativity paradigm reflects deficits in schizophreniapatients. A combined EEG and MEG study. Biol Psychol 2008;77:205–16.

Tikhonravov D, Neuvonen T, Pertovaara A, Savioja K, Ruusuvirta T, Näätänen R, et al.Effects of an NMDA receptor antagonist MK-801 on an MMN-like responserecorded in anesthetized rats. Brain Res 2008;1203:97–102.

Tikhonravov D, Neuvonen T, Pertovaara A, Savioja K, Ruusuvirta T, Näätänen R, et al.Dose-related effects of memantine on a mismatch negativity-like response inanesthetized rats. Neuroscience 2010;167:1175–82.

Todd J, Michie PT, Jablensky AV. Association between reduced duration mismatchnegativity (MMN) and raised temporal discrimination thresholds inschizophrenia. Clin Neurophysiol 2003;114:2061–70.

Todd J, Michie PT, Schall U, Karayanidis F, Yabe H, Näätänen R. Deviant matters:duration, frequency, and intensity deviants reveal different patterns of

Page 34: The mismatch negativity (MMN) – A unique window to ... et al (2011... · Invited Review The mismatch negativity (MMN) – A unique window to disturbed central auditory processing

R. Näätänen et al. / Clinical Neurophysiology 123 (2012) 424–458 457

mismatch negativity reduction in early and late schizophrenia. Biol Psychiat2008;63:58–64.

Todd, J, Michie PT, Schall U, Ward P, Catts S. Mismatch negativity (MMN) reductionin schizophrenia -impaired prediction-error generation, estimation or salience.Int J Psychophysiol, in press.

Toiviainen P, Tervaniemi M, Louhivuori J, Saher M, Huotilainen M, Näätänen R.Timbre similarity: convergence of neural, behavioral, and computationalapproaches. Music Percept 1998;16:223–41.

Towey JP, Tenke CE, Bruder GE, Leite P, Friedman D, Liebowitz M, et al. Brain event-related potential correlates of overfocused attention in obsessive-compulsivedisorder. Psychophysiology 1994;31:535–43.

Toyomaki A, Kusumi I, Matsuyama T, Kako Y, Ito K, Koyama T. Tone durationmismatch negativity deficits predict impairment of executive function inschizophrenia. Prog Neuro-Psychoph 2008;32:95–9.

Trainor L, McFadden M, Hodgson L, Darragh L, Barlow J, Matsos L, et al. Changes inauditory cortex and the development of mismatch negativity between 2 and 6months of age. Int J Psychophysiol 2003;51:5–15.

Trainor L, Samuel SS, Desjardins RN, Sonnadara RR. Measuring temporal resolutionin infants using mismatch negativity. NeuroReport 2001;12:2443–8.

Trautwein PG, Ponton CW, Kwong B, Waring MD. Neuropsysiological andpsychophysical measures of duration discrimination in normal-hearing adultsand adults with cochlear implants. In: Proceedings 16th international congresson acoustics and 135th meeting Acoustic Society of America, 1998. p. 877-8.

Tremblay K, Kraus N, Carrel TD, McGee T. Central auditory system plasticity:Generalization to novel stimuli following listening training. J Acoust Soc Am1997;102:3762–73.

Tremblay K, Kraus N, McGee T. The time course of auditory perceptual learning:neurophysiological changes during speech-sound training. NeuroReport1998;9:3557–60.

Troche SJ, Houlihan ME, Stelmack RM, Rammsayer TH. Mental ability and thediscrimination of auditory frequency and duration change without focusedattention: an analysis of mismatch negativity. Pers Individ Differ2010;49:228–33.

Troche SJ, Houlihan ME, Stelmack RM, Rammsayer TH. Mental ability, P300, andmismatch negativity: analysis of frequency and duration discrimination.Intelligence 2009;37:365–73.

Tse C-Y, Penney TB. On the functional role of temporal and frontal cortex activationin passive detection of auditory deviance. NeuroImage 2008;41:1462–70.

Turetsky BI, Calkins ME, Light GA, Olincy A, Radant AD, Swerdlow NR.Neurophysiological endophenotypes of schizophrenia: the viability of selectedcandidate measures. Schizophrenia Bull 2007;33:69–94.

Turetsky BI, Bilker WE, Siegel SJ, Kohler CG, Gur RE. Profile of auditory information-processing deficits in schizophrenia. Psychiat Res 2009;165:27–37.

Umbricht D, Krljes S. Mismatch negativity in schizophrenia: a meta-analysis.Schizophr Res 2005;76:1–23.

Umbricht DS, Bates JA, Lieberman JA, Kane JM, Javitt DC. Electrophysiological indicesof automatic and controlled auditory information processing in first-episode,recent-onset and chronic schizophrenia. Biol Psychiat 2006;59:762–72.

Umbricht D, Javitt D, Novak G, Bates J, Pollack S, Lieberman J, et al. Effects ofclozapine on auditory event-related potentials in schizophrenia. Biol Psychiat1998;44:716–25.

Umbricht D, Javitt D, Novak G, Bates J, Pollack S, Lieberman J, et al. Effects ofrisperidone on auditory event-related potentials in schizophrenia. Int JNeuropsychopharmacol 1999;2:299–304.

Umbricht D, Koller R, Schmid L, Skrabo A, Grübel C, Huber T, et al. How specific aredeficits in mismatch negativity generation to schizophrenia. Biol Psychiat2003;53:1120–31.

Umbricht D, Koller R, Vollenweider FX, Schmid L. Mismatch negativity predictspsychotic experiences induced by NMDA receptor antagonist in healthyvolunteers. Biol Psychiat 2002;51:400–6.

Umbricht D, Schmid L, Koller R, Vollenweider FX, Hell D, Javitt DC. Ketamine-induced deficits in auditory and visual context-dependent processing in healthyvolunteers: implications for models of cognitive deficits in schizophrenia. ArchGen Psychiat 2000;57:1139–47.

Umbricht D, Vyssotki D, Latanov A, Nitsch R, Lipp HP. Deviant-relatedelectrophysiological activity in mice: is there mismatch negativity in mice.Clin Neurophysiol 2005;116:353–63.

Urban A, Kremlácek J, Masopust J, Libiger J. Visual mismatch negativity amongpatients with schizophrenia. Schizophr Res 2008;102:320–8.

Uwer R, Albrecht R, von Suchodoletz W. Automatic processing of tones and speechstimuli in children with specific language impairment. Dev Med Child Neurol2002;44:527–32.

Uwer R, von Suchodoletz W. Stability of mismatch negativities in children. ClinNeurophysiol 2000;111:45–52.

Valeriani M, Galli F, Tarantino S, Graceffa D, Pignata E, Miliucci R, et al. Correlationbetween normal brain excitability and emotional symptomatology in paediatricmigraine. Cephalalgia 2008;29:204–13.

van der Stelt O, Belger A. Application of electroencephalography to the study ofcognitive and brain functions in schizophrenia. Schizophr Bull 2007;33:955–70.

van der Stelt O, van Boxtel GJM. Auditory P300 and mismatch negativity incomatose states. Clin Neurophysiol 2008;119:2172–4.

van Hooff JC, de Beer NAM, Brunia CHM, Cluitmans PJM, Kortsen HHM. Event-related potential measures of information processing during general anesthesia.Electroen Clin Neuro 1997;103:268–81.

van Hooff JC, de Beer NAM, Brunia CHM, Cluitmans PJM, Kortsen HHM, Tavilla G,et al. Information processing during cardiac surgery: an event related potentialstudy. Electroen Clin Neuro 1995;96:433–52.

Vanhanen M, Karhu J, Koivisto K, Pääkkönen A, Partanen J, Laakso M, et al. ERPsreveal deficits in automatic cerebral stimulus processing in patients withNIDDM. NeuroReport 1996;7:2767–71.

Vanhaudenhuyse A, Laureys S, Perrin F. Cognitive event-related potentials incomatose and post-comatose states. Neurocrit Care 2008;8:262–70.

van Leeuwen T, Been P, Kuijpers C, Zwarts F, Maassen B, van der Leij A. Mismatchresponse is absent in 2-month-old infants at risk for dyslexia. NeuroReport2006;17:351–5.

van Woerkom TC, Fortgens C, Rompel-Martens CM, van de Wetering BJ. Auditoryevent-related potentials in adult patients with Gilles de la Tourette’s syndromein the oddball paradigm. Electroen Clin Neuro 1988;71:443–9.

van Woerkom TCAM, Roos RAC, van Dijk JG. Altered attentional processing ofbackground stimuli in Gilles de la Tourette syndrome: a study in auditoryevent-related potentials evoked in an oddball paradigm. Acta Neurol Scand1994;90:116–23.

Vaz Pato M, Jones SJ, Perez N, Sprague LC. Mismatch negativity to single andmultiple pitch-deviant tones in regular and pseudo-random complex tonesequences. Neurophysiology 2002;113:519–27.

Verbaten MN, van Engeland H. (Neuro) physiologic similarities between childhooddevelopmental disorders and schizophrenia. In: Den Boer JA, Westenberg HGM,Praag HM. (Eds.), Advances in the neurobiology of schizophrenia. John Wiley &Sons Ltd; 1995. p. 369–400.

Verbaten MN, Overtoom CCE, Koelega HS, Swaab-Barneveld H, van der Gaag RJ,Buitelaar J, et al. Methylphenidate influences on both early and late ERP wavesof ADHD children in a continuous performance test. J Abnorm Child Psychol1994;22:561–78.

Verleger R, Neukäter W, Kömpf D, Viereggem P. On the reasons for the delay of P3latency in healthy elderly subjects. Electroen Clin Neuro 1991;79:488–502.

Vieregger P, Verleger R, Wascher E, Stüven F, Kömpf D. Auditory selective attentionis impaired in Parkinson’s disease – event-related evidence from EEG potentials.Cognit Brain Res 1994;2:117–29.

Vuust P, Brattico E, Glerean E, Seppänen M, Pakarinen S, Tervaniemi M, et al. Newfast mismatch negativity paradigm for determining the neural prerequisites formusical ability. Cortex 2011;47:1091–9.

Wable J, van der Abbeele T, Gallégo S, Frachet B. Mismatch negativity: a tool for theassessment of stimuli discrimination in cochlear implant subjects. ClinNeurophysiol 2000;111:743–51.

Wang W, Zhu SZ, Pan LC, Hu AH, Wang YH. Mismatch negativity and personalitytraits in chronic primary insomniacs. Funct Neurol 2001;16:3–10.

Weber C, Hahne A, Friedrich M, Friederici AD. Reduced stress pattern discriminationin 5-month-olds as a marker of risk for later language impairment:neurophysiological evidence. Cognit Brain Res 2005;25:180–7.

Wei J-H, Chan T-C, Luo Y-J. A modified oddball paradigm ‘‘cross-modal delayedresponse’’ and the research on mismatch negativity. Brain Res Bull2002;57:221–30.

Wertz RT, Auther LL, Burch-Sims GP, Abou-Khalil R, Kirshner HS, Duncan GW. Acomparison of the mismatch negativity (MMN) event-related potential to toneand speech stimuli in normal and aphasic adults. Aphasiology 1998;12:499–507.

Wible CG, Kubicki M, Yoo S-S, Kacher DF, Salisbury DF, Anderson MC, et al. Afunctional magnetic resonance imaging study of auditory mismatch inschizophrenia. Am J Psychiat 2001;158:938–43.

Wienberg M, Glenthoj BY, Jensen KS, Oranje B. A single high dose of escitalopramincreases mismatch negativity without affecting processing negativity or P300amplitude in healthy volunteers. J Psychopharmacol 2010;24:1183–92.

Wijnen VJM, van Boxtel GJM, Eilander HJ, de Gelder B. Mismatch negativity predictsrecovery from the vegetative state. Clin Neurophysiol 2007;118:597–605.

Wild-Wall N, Oades RD, Juran SA. Maturation processes in automatic changedetection as revealed by event-related brain potentials and dipole sourcelocalization: significance for adult AD/HD. Int J Psychophysiol 2005;58:34–46.

Winkler I. Interpreting the mismatch negativity. J Psychophysiol 2007;21:147–63.Winkler I, Näätänen R. The effects of auditory backward masking on event-related

brain potentials. Electroen Clin Neuro 1994;44:185–9.Winkler I, Reinikainen K, Näätänen R. Event-related brain potentials reflect echoic

memory in humans. Percept Psychophys 1993;53:443–9.Winkler I, Kujala T, Tiitinen H, Sivonen P, Alku P, Lehtokoski A, et al. Brain responses

reveal the learning of foreign language phonemes. Psychophysiology1999c;36:638–42.

Winsberg BG, Javitt DC, Silippo GS. Electrophysiological indices of informationprocessing in methylphenidate responders. Biol Psychiat 1997;42:434–45.

Winsberg BG, Javitt DC, Silippo GS, Doneshka P. Mismatch negativity in hyperactivechildren: effects of methylphenidate. Psychopharmacol Bull 1993;29:229–33.

Woods DL. Auditory selective attention in middle-aged and elderly subjects: anevent-related brain potential study. Electroen Clin Neuro 1992;84:456–68.

Woods DL, Clayworth CC. Age-related changes in human middle latency auditoryevoked potentials. Electroen Clin Neuro 1986;65:297–303.

Woods DL, Knight RT. Electrophysiologic evidence of increased distractibility afterdorsolateral prefrontal lesions. Neurology 1986;36:212–6.

Woods DL, Alho K, Algazi A. Intermodal selective attention I: effects on event-related potentials to lateralized auditory and visual stimuli. Electroen ClinNeuro 1992;82:341–55.

Page 35: The mismatch negativity (MMN) – A unique window to ... et al (2011... · Invited Review The mismatch negativity (MMN) – A unique window to disturbed central auditory processing

458 R. Näätänen et al. / Clinical Neurophysiology 123 (2012) 424–458

Wu JC, Maguire G, Riley G, Lee A, Keator D, Tang C, et al. Increased dopamineactivity associated with stuttering. NeuroReport 1997;8:767–70.

Wynn JK, Sugar C, Horan WP, Kern R, Green MF. Mismatch negativity, socialcognition, and functioning in schizophrenia patients. Biol Psychiat2010;67:940–7.

Yamasue H, Yamada H, Yumoto M, Kamio S, Kudo N, Uetsuki M, et al. Abnormalassociation between reduced magnetic mismatch field to speech sounds andsmaller left planum temporale volume in schizophrenia. NeuroImage2004;22:720–7.

Yokoyama Y, Nakashima K, Shimoyama R, Urakami K, Takahashi K. Distribution ofevent-related potentials in patients with dementia. Electroen Clin Neuro1995;35:431–7.

Youn T, Park H-J, Kim J-J, Kim MS, Kwon JS. Altered hemispheric asymmetry andpositive symptoms in schizophrenia: equivalent current dipole of auditorymismatch negativity. Schizophr Res 2003;59:253–60.

Yppärilä H, Karhu J, Westerén-Punnonen S, Musialowicz T, Partanen J. Evidence ofauditory processing during postoperative propofol sedation. Clini Neurophysiol2002;113:1357–64.

Zarza-Lucianez A, Arce-Arce S, Bhathal H, Sanjuan-Martin F. Mismatch negativityand conscience level in severe traumatic brain injury. Rev Neurologia2007;44:465–8.

Zerouali Y, Jemel B, Godbout R. The effects of early and late night partial sleepdeprivation on automatic and selective attention: an ERP study. Brain Res2010;1308:87–99.

Zhang XL, Cohen HL, Porjesz B, Begleiter H. Mismatch negativity in subjects at highrisk for alcoholism. Alcohol Clin Exp Res 2001;25:330–7.

Zurrón M, Díaz F. Auditory event-related potentials in mentally retarded subjectsduring active and passive oddball experiments. Biol Psychiat 1997;41:201–8.