The effects of bedding materials on learning and memory ...

9
Corresponding author: Razieh Kooshki Tell/Fax: +98 9168568557 Email: [email protected] Iranian Journal of Veterinary Science and Technology Received: 2020-May- 19 Accepted aſter revision: 2020- Jun- 26 Published online: 2020- Sep- 09 DOI: 10.22067/veterinary.v12i1.86943 RESEARCH ARTICLE ABSTRACT Keywords Abbreviations www.IJVST.um.ac.ir e effects of bedding materials on learning and memory performance and texture preference in rats Mehdi Abbasnejad, Razieh Kooshki, Saeed Esmaeili-Mahani, Abbas Tajabadi, Reyhaneh Naderi a a a Department of Biology, Faculty of Sciences, Shahid Bahonar University of Kerman, Kerman, Iran. b Department of Biology, Faculty of Sciences, Lorestan University, Khorramabad, Iran Bedding materials, Learning and memory, Texture preference, Rats MWM: Morris water maze STL: Step-through latency TDC: Time spent in the dark compartment LTP: Long term potentiation e present study was designed to investigate the effect of different available bedding materials on learning and memory performance, bedding texture preference as well as intra-cage ammonia concentration in rats. e animals were housed on different bedding types for two weeks. Bedding materials were produced in the same sizes from poplar, walnut, pistachio, apricot, almond woods and alfalfa steam and live. Spatial and pas- sive avoidance learning and memory were assessed by Morris water maze (MWM) and shuttle box tasks. A modifying six-arm radial maze was used to assess bedding texture preference by rats. For each bedding groups, average ammonia level (ppm) over a week was calculated. e data indicated that the rats that had walnut and almond chips show better learning and memory performance in both MWM and shuttle box tests than other groups. e weakest learning and memory performances were observed in rats exposed to alfalfa bedding. In texture preference test, the rats spent more time in walnut and almond arms, and less time in alfalfa. Besides, the total water and food intake as well as the number of visit to alfalfa arm were decreased as compared to other arms. Moreover, in alfalfa bedding cage, average intra-cage ammonia level was utmost. Overall, current bedding materials may contain diverse biochemically effective compounds or individual micro edges which alter learning and memory performances of rats. b a a

Transcript of The effects of bedding materials on learning and memory ...

Corresponding author:Razieh KooshkiTell/Fax: +98 9168568557

Email: [email protected]

Iranian Journal of VeterinaryScience and Technology

Received: 2020-May- 19Accepted after revision: 2020- Jun- 26Published online: 2020- Sep- 09

DOI: 10.22067/veterinary.v12i1.86943RESEARCH ARTICLE

ABSTRACT

Keywords Abbreviations

www.IJVST.um.ac.ir

The effects of bedding materials on learning and memory performance and texture preference in rats

Mehdi Abbasnejad, Razieh Kooshki, Saeed Esmaeili-Mahani, Abbas Tajabadi,

Reyhaneh Naderi

a a

a Department of Biology, Faculty of Sciences, Shahid Bahonar University of Kerman, Kerman, Iran. b Department of Biology, Faculty of Sciences, Lorestan University, Khorramabad, Iran

Bedding materials, Learning and memory, Texture preference, Rats

MWM: Morris water mazeSTL: Step-through latency TDC: Time spent in the dark compartment LTP: Long term potentiation

The present study was designed to investigate the effect of different available bedding materials on learning and memory performance, bedding texture preference as well as intra-cage ammonia concentration in rats. The animals were housed on different bedding types for two weeks. Bedding materials were produced in the same sizes from poplar, walnut, pistachio, apricot, almond woods and alfalfa steam and live. Spatial and pas-sive avoidance learning and memory were assessed by Morris water maze (MWM) and shuttle box tasks. A modifying six-arm radial maze was used to assess bedding texture preference by rats. For each bedding groups, average ammonia level (ppm) over a week was calculated. The data indicated that the rats that had walnut and almond chips show better learning and memory performance in both MWM and shuttle box tests than other groups. The weakest learning and memory performances were observed in rats exposed to alfalfa bedding. In texture preference test, the rats spent more time in walnut and almond arms, and less time in alfalfa. Besides, the total water and food intake as well as the number of visit to alfalfa arm were decreased as compared to other arms. Moreover, in alfalfa bedding cage, average intra-cage ammonia level was utmost. Overall, current bedding materials may contain diverse biochemically effective compounds or individual micro edges which alter learning and memory performances of rats.

b a

a

RESEARCH ARTICLE

IJVST 2020-1 (22) DOI:10.22067/veterinary.v12i1.86943

56

Introduction

The normal physiological functions of labora-tory animals were strongly affected by their

housing and husbandry conditions. In particular, bed-ding is one of the most important housing elements, which has influence on various neurobiological func-tions of laboratory animals [1-3]. Ethogram of mice behaviors including agonistic interaction, feeding, drinking, locomotion, nest-building and resting show bedding material-related changes [4]. On behalf of ro-dents, various kind of wood shavings such as paper, corncobs and hill rice are known as common bedding substances. There is no definition for ideal bedding, however, ideal bedding must have a low ability of in-fection, high absorbency, low allergenic activity, min-imal chemical toxicity, low cost and high accessibility and compatibility [4-7].

It has been indicated that variations of bedding materials affects the stress and immune system [8], somatosensory signaling [9], neuropathic pain[10], vocalizations [11], body mass [12], temperature reg-ulation, metabolism [13, 14], as well as liver enzyme levels in laboratory rodents [15, 16]. In particular, ro-dent’s neurocognitive development has been affected by bedding materials and housing conditions. It has been indicated that corncob bedding suppresses es-trogen-dependent aggressive behavior in rats [17]. In another study, mice housed in cages containing pulp chips, for 8 week, show better water maze per-formance than wood flakes group [18]. It has been indicated that exposure to an enriched environment induces dendritic branching and synaptogenesis in cortex and hippocampus of rats. Besides, housing enrichment can improve learning, memory, and syn-aptic plasticity in rodents[19]. Housing rats in cages with limited nesting/bedding materials impaired spa-tial learning and memory and hippocampal long term potentiation (LTP). Providing enriched environment can overcome the memory impairments through the recovery of LTP [1].

Preference testing has indicated that rodents have different preferences for bedding substances. Addi-tionally, another study on rats and mice indicated that animals had strong preference for large fibrous bed-ding particles than relatively small particles [20]. It has been also reported that mice prefer cloth bedding type in comparison to wood shavings, paper and poly-carbonate bedding substances [21]. Bedding materi-al preferences for animals are determined by various chemical and physical features including color, odor, chemical compounds, and texture properties such as condition of surfaces, edges and coarseness.

In the present study, bedding texture preference was evaluated by housing the rats on six different bed-

ding types including standard hard bed poplar chip, dried alfalfa as typical soft bedding material; walnut, apricot and almond as unusual bedding, and pistachio chip as very exclusive hard bed available in Iran and a few countries. Regarding climate change in Iran many large quantities of pistachio, walnut, apricot, and al-mond at low cost are available and can be used as bedding surface. We also examined bedding material mediated possible alterations in learning and memory performance of rats. In each bedding cage, ammonia concentration was also assessed.

Results

MWM testThere was a significant difference in the escape la-

tency time among experimental groups during acqui-sition blocks [F (3,480) = 44.52, p = 0.001]. In the first day, the latency time to find the hidden platform was significantly decreased in the rats housed on walnut and almond as compared to other groups (p < 0.05). In the second day, there was a significant decrease in the escape latency of rats housed on almond as compared to apricot and poplar groups (p < 0.05) (Fig. 1, upper graph). In addition, the main escape latency was sig-nificantly different among groups [F (5,165) = 15.93, p = 0.001]. The rats housed on walnut and almond bed-ding showed the lowest latency time to find the hid-den platform (Fig. 1, lower graph).

Significant differences were observed in the trav-elled distance to find hidden platform among exper-imental groups during acquisition days [F (3,480) = 31.47, p = 0.001]. In the first day, the rats housed on walnut and almond travelled lower distance to find the hidden platform as compared to rats that were housed on poplar and pistachio (p < 0.05). In the sec-ond day, rats kept on almond travelled lower distance to find the hidden platform in comparison with pista-chio, apricot and alfalfa group (p < 0.05). In the third day, rats kept on almond and poplar travelled lower distance to find the hidden platform as compared to walnut, pistachio, apricot and alfalfa group (p < 0.05) (Fig. 2, upper graph). In the fourth day, however, rats subjected to walnut travelled lower distance to reach the hidden platform as compared to alfalfa and pista-chio groups (p < 0.05). Furthermore, the main trav-elled distance to find the platform was significantly decreased in rats reserved on almond (p < 0.05) and walnut bedding (p < 0.05) in comparison to pistachio, alfalfa, and apricot (Fig. 2, lower graph).

The results of probe test indicated that time spent in the target zone was significantly increased in rats kept on walnut and almond as compared to poplar, apricot and alfalfa groups (p < 0.01) (Fig. 3).

57

IJVST DOI: 10.22067/veterinary.v12i1.869432020-1 (22)

RESEARCH ARTICLE

Shuttle box testAs shown in Fig. 4A, the number of trials re-

quired to reach acquisition were significantly de-creased in walnut, apricot and almond groups in comparison with poplar group (p < 0.05). In the retention test, rats housed in alfalfa bedding sub-

stance showed significant decrease in STL (p < 0.05) and increase in TDC (p < 0.001) as compared to other groups (Fig. 4B and 4C).

Ammonia concentration Intra-cage ammonia level was recorded during

the first week of experiment. As shown in Fig. 5, the mean ammonia concentration in alfalfa cage was sig-nificantly increased in comparison with others groups (p < 0.001).

Bedding preference During the first week, the rats spent highest time

in walnut and almond (p < 0.001) and lowest time in the alfalfa comprised arms (p < 0.001) (Fig. 6A). In addition, the rats had more visit to arms containing al-mond than pistachio and apricot (p < 0.05) and alfalfa (p < 0.001). Besides, rats visit into the cage contain-ing alfalfa bedding was lowest (Fig. 6B). Moreover, As shown in Fig. 7A, the total water consumption by rats were significantly increased in walnut, almond as well as poplar cages in comparison with pistachio and alfalfa cages (p < 0.01). In addition, the total food con-sumption was significantly increased in almond cage as compared to pistachio and apricot (p <0.05) as well as alfalfa cage (p < 0.01). Moreover, food consumption

Figure 1.Evaluation of the escape latency in each acquisition block (upper graph) and the mean scape latency (lower graph) in rats subject-ed to different bedding surfaces. *: p < 0.05, **: p < 0.01 vs walnut group#: p < 0.05 , ##: p < 0.01 vs almond group

Figure 2. Evaluation of the travelled distance in each acquisition block (upper graph) and the mean travelled distance (lower graph) in rats subjected to different bedding surfaces. Data are presented as mean ± SEM. *: p < 0.05, **: p < 0.01 vs walnut##: p < 0.01, #: p < 0.05 vs almond&: p < 0.01 vs poplar.

Figure 3. Evaluation of the time spent in the target zone in probe test between the groups of rat subjected to the different bedding surfaces. Data are presented as mean ± SEM.**: p < 0.01 vs apricot and alfalfa#: p < 0.05 vs poplar

RESEARCH ARTICLE

IJVST 2020-1 (22) DOI:10.22067/veterinary.v12i1.86943

58

Figure 4. Evaluation of the number of acquisition trials to reach successful learning (A), the step through latency (STL) (B) and time spent in dark chamber (TDC) (C) in shuttle box test in the groups of rats subjected to different bedding types. Data are presented as mean ± SEM.*: p < 0.05 vs poplar group #: p < 0.05, ###: p < 0.001 vs alfalfa

was significantly increased in poplar and almond cag-es in comparison with alfalfa (p < 0.05) (Fig. 6B).

Discussion

The present study investigated the learning and memory performances of rats subjected to different bedding chips including poplar, walnut, pistachio, apricot, almond, and alfalfa. According to the results, in comparison with other groups, rats housed on wal-

nut and almond chips showed better learning and memory performance in MWM and shuttle box tests. The learning and memory performances of rats sub-jected to alfalfa bedding were lesser than other bed-ding types. Besides, investigation preferences of rats for types of bedding by a six-arm radial maze showed that rats spent more time in chips of walnut in com-parison with pistachio, apricot and alfalfa bedding types. In addition, the rats had highest and lowest visit into almond and alfalfa-containing cage, respectively. Likewise, food and water intake by rats in alfalfa con-tain cage was lowest. There are only a few studies on the substance quality as bedding material. The pres-ent study, tried to fill the gap of data about pistachio, apricot and almond.

It has been well documented that bedding and husbandry have influence on rodent’s neurophysi-ologic responses. However, just a few studies have shown bedding texture property on cognitive-related behaviors. A study by Tanaka and colleagues showed that mice subjected to pulp chips bedding, over eight weeks period, had better water maze performance than those kept on wood flakes [18].

The results of this study also showed that the rats find walnut and almond sawdust more suitable as a resting surface than other four bedding types. It shows direct relationship between comfortable bedding tex-ture and learning and memory performances. The bedding materials used here due to their structure have diverse edge and surface which may result in changed stimulation of the plantar foot area [22]. In other words, edge of the bedding show different me-chanical effect on rat planter surface. It has been indi-cated that the daily behaviors of rats such as nesting and sleeping are affected by kind of bedding materi-als [10]. Besides, bedding material features may affect sensory processes even down to the molecular and cellular levels [10].

Here, the rats subjected to walnut and almond chips showed better learning and memory perfor-mance. Nuts, leaves, woods and hulls from walnut and almond are highly valued for their biological prop-erties. In particular, the compounds have potential health-promoting activities because of their pheno-lic-enriched contents. It has been indicated that wal-nut polyphenol improves learning and memory per-formances of hypercholesterolemia mice [23]. It has been indicated that flavonoid and other polyphenols material improve learning and memory performances mainly via their antioxidant capacity and cholinester-ase activities [24-26]. In the present study, it is like-ly that beddings different influences on learning and memory are partially mediated by differences in their polyphenolic capacity. However, we did not explore polyphenols capacity of different chips and to clarify

59

IJVST DOI: 10.22067/veterinary.v12i1.869432020-1 (22)

RESEARCH ARTICLE

Figure 5. The average of ammonia concentration in each bed-ding cage. Data are presented as mean ± SEM.***: p < 0.001, vs alfalfa

Figure 6. Assessment time spent and the number of visits by rats into each arm of radial maze containing different bedding substances during a week. Data are presented as mean ± SEM.**: p < 0.01 vs poplar and walnut ##: p < 0.05, ###: p < 0.001 vs walnut and almond+: p < 0.05 vs pistachio and apricot &: p < 0.05, &&: p < 0.01, &&&: p < 0.001 vs almond

Figure 7.Comparison of food and water consumption by rats in the maze containing different bedding substance in each arm. Data are present-ed as mean ± SEM.*: p < 0.05,**: p < 0.01, vs poplar ##: p < 0.05, ###: p < 0.001 vs walnut&: p < 0.05, &&: p < 0.01, &&&: p < 0.001 vs almond

RESEARCH ARTICLE

IJVST 2020-1 (22) DOI:10.22067/veterinary.v12i1.86943

60

such relationship further studies are still required. To the best of our knowledge the study was the first

to consider association between bedding preference and learning and memory related behaviors. Howev-er, some studies have indicated that improvement of nesting conditions positively modulate learning and memory -related functions. It has been demonstrated that environmental enrichment improves learning and memory in various laboratory tasks [2]. In addition, environmental enrichment can induce morphological changes in the cortex and hippocampus of rat such as enhancement of neuron numbers, synapses and den-dritic branches [3, 27]. Increasing bedding volume has been correlated with intracage ammonia decrease [4, 28]. Moreover, there is significant relationship be-tween the depth of bedding and animal preference as well as animal physiology [4].

Welfare of laboratory animals are closely related to optimum intracage features like the level of ammonia, moisture, and absorbency and bacterial growth [29]. In the current study, assessment of ammonia concen-tration in each cage showed that there was the highest intracage ammonia in the cage containing alfalfa bed-ding. As shown in the results, the mean ammonia level was lower than adverse level 100 ppm or 130 ppm [22, 30]. Metabolized urea from urine and feces of the ani-mal is responsible for ammonia production [31].

Interestingly, the rats housed on alfalfa bedding surface showed lowest learning and memory perfor-mance. Various studies have been reported on the distractive effect of ammonia on brain functions. In-creased ammonia concentration in the brain as a re-sult of diseases could induce a range of neurobehav-ioral dysfunctions like learning and memory deficient, sleep-wake inversions, brain edema and seizures [32, 33]. It has also been reported that inhalation of am-monia was able to reduce cognitive performance for culture fair, digit symbol and vocabulary[34]. Besides, increased intra cage CO2 levels and fecal cortisol con-centrations have crucial role in animal function and behaviors [4], but were not included in this study.

ConclusionThis study provides some primary data support-

ing the relationship between bedding texture prefer-ence and learning and memory function of rats. The effect may be partially mediated by bedding differenc-es in ammonia absorption capacity. It also could be related to biochemical diversity and individual micro edges of distinct bedding materials.

Material and methods

Animal and housing conditions Adult male Wistar rats (230–270 gr) were used. The animals

were housed in conventional animal room (3× 8× 3 m) under a 12 h light/dark cycle in controlled condition with temperature of 22 ± 2 °C. The ventilation rate was 8-15 times per hour. During the experiment the same amount of food and water was available ad libitum. All experimental procedures were approved by the Ani-mal Research Ethics Committee of the Shahid Bahonar University of Kerman, Iran.

Experimental design

Experiment 1: Evaluation of the spatial and passive avoidance learning and memory perfor-mances

The rats were divided into six experimental groups and kept on the same amount of different bedding substances for 2 weeks. Bedding materials included were chip of poplar, walnut, pistachio, apricot, almond and dried alfalfa. The wood ship average particle sizes were 15 × 4 × 1 mm with a moisture of 8%, which was sieved (purchased from zist mehvar-pajoheshe Pars Company, Kerman, Iran). The number of animals per group and per cage were seven. The amounts of animal biomass per cage were the same in the be-ginning and during the study. The rats learning and memory per-formances were evaluated using Morris water maze (MWM) and shuttle box tests. Besides, during first week of the experiment daily changes in intracage ammonia concentration was measured using multigas transmitter (ModBus, TM-1280). To take a reading, the sampling tube was inserted in the middle of bedding. The monitor took continuous reading for 5 min, for measuring ammonia level according manufacturer’s recommendations. The experimenters were not blinded to the bedding types; they had no expectations of any group differences.

Experiment 2: bedding material preference Bedding texture preference by rats (n=7) was assessed using

a modified bedding preference test system introduced by Blom [20]. Briefly, the maze consisted of a central wire mesh circle area surrounded by six enclosed wooden cages (50 × 10 × 40 cm). The central zone was raised 2 cm above the floor than the cages. Equal amounts of six bedding materials, almost the same in size and shape, were situated on different cages of the maze. In each cage food and water was ad libitum. For beginning the experi-ment, each rat was placed on the central area of maze and during one-week period the rats’ behavior including the number of visit and time spent in each arm were monitored by a video camera system. Besides, rats’ food and water intakes in different cages of maze were measured. Before this test, rats were housed on pine shavings.

Assessment of learning and memory perfor-mance

Shuttle box testPA learning and memory was assessed by a shuttle box appa-

ratus as previously described. The test protocol was divided into a learning session on the first day and a test trial 24 h later. For the learning trial, each animal was placed in light chamber of shuttle box apparatus. Then, the door was opened and the animal was al-lowed to enter into the dark sector. The animal received an electri-cal shock (0.5 mA, 50 Hz, 2 s once) upon entrance to dark sector via the stainless steel floor. The learning trial was terminated when the rat remained in the light chamber for 5 consecutive minutes. After one day, the retention test for assessing memory, each rat was placed in the light compartment of shuttle box device. After 30

61

IJVST DOI: 10.22067/veterinary.v12i1.869432020-1 (22)

RESEARCH ARTICLEseconds, the door was opened and the step-through latency (STL), the time before the first entry of the rat to the dark sector, and total time spent in the dark compartment (TDC) were recorded.

Morris water maze (MWM) testSpatial learning and memory was assessed by MWM pool. It

consisted of a dark circular pool (136 cm in diameter and 60 cm high) filled with water (20 ± 1 °C) to a depth of 25 cm. The extra maze cues were placed in consistent locations on the walls which were visible to the rats. The pool was divided into four quadrants defined by the four cardinal directions. A circular platform was located 2 cm below the water surface in the middle of one of the quadrants. At the beginning of experiment, each rat was lightly placed in the water facing the wall of the pool from one of the directions. The location of each rat was tracked by a digital TV system and analyzed using the Ethovision video tracking system (Noldus Information Technology, the Netherlands). One day prior to the beginning of training; the rats were habituated to the pool by allowing them to swim for 60 s without the platform.

The test included acquisition and probe trials. The acquisi-tion test was performed on 4 consecutive days with four trials per day with a 5 min interval between trials. The rats were allowed to swim within 60 s to find the hidden platform at each trial. Once the platform was found, the animal would have to stay on the plat-form for 30 s. If unsuccessful within 60 s, it was gently guided to the platform for 30 s. The escape latency, traveled distance and swimming speed for each rat were evaluated. In probe test, one day after acquisition test, the hidden platform was removed from the pool and rats were placed in the quadrant opposite the target quadrant and allowed to swim freely for 60 s. The time spent in the target quadrant was recorded and analyzed as a measure of spatial memory retention.

Statistical analysisAll data were expressed as mean ± standard error of mean

(SEM). The acquisition test data related to four acquisition days of MWM test were analyzed using repeated measures ANOVA. The statistical significances for probe and preference tests were deter-mined by one way ANOVA followed by post-hoc Tukey’s test. The results of shuttle box test were analyzed by Friedman and Krus-kal-Wallis tests followed by Mann-Whitney U test.

Acknowledgments

This work was supported by Shahid Bahonar Uni-versity of Kerman, Kerman, Iran.

Author Contributions

M.A and S.EM designed the study; A.T and R.N collected data; R.K analyzed the data and wrote the paper.

Conflict of Interest

None .

References

1. Cui M, Yang Y, Yang J, Zhang J, Han H, Ma W, et al. Enriched environment experience overcomes the memory deficits and

depressive-like behavior induced by early life stress. Neuro-science letters. 2006;404(1-2):208-12.

2. Van Praag H, Kempermann G, Gage FH. Neural consequenc-es of enviromental enrichment. Nature Reviews Neurosci-ence. 2000;1(3):191.

3. Diamond MC, Ingham CA, Johnson RE, Bennett EL, Rosenz-weig MR. Effects of environment on morphology of rat ce-rebral cortex and hippocampus. Journal of neurobiology. 1976;7(1):75-85.

4. Freymann J, Tsai P-P, Stelzer H, Hackbarth H. The impact of bedding volumes on laboratory mice. Applied Animal Be-haviour Science. 2017;186:72-9.

5. Couto M. Laboratory guidelines for animal care. Vertebrate Embryogenesis: Springer; 2011. p. 579-99.

6. Council NR. Guide for the care and use of laboratory animals: National Academies Press; 2010.

7. Potgieter F, Wilke P. The dust content, dust generation, am-monia production, and absorption properties of three differ-ent rodent bedding types. Laboratory animals. 1996;30(1):79-87.

8. Vijayakumar R, Samanta R, Samanta A, Guria R, Joardar S. Influence of different types of bedding materials on immune response and serum biochemical profile of caged mice. Veter-inary World. 2010;3(9):417.

9. Moehring F, O’Hara CL, Stucky CL. Bedding material affects mechanical thresholds, heat thresholds, and texture prefer-ence. The Journal of Pain. 2016;17(1):50-64.

10. Robinson I, Dowdall T, Meert TF. Development of neu-ropathic pain is affected by bedding texture in two mod-els of peripheral nerve injury in rats. Neuroscience letters. 2004;368(1):107-11.

11. Natusch C, Schwarting R. Using bedding in a test environ-ment critically affects 50-kHz ultrasonic vocalizations in laboratory rats. Pharmacology Biochemistry and Behavior. 2010;96(3):251-9.

12. Burn CC, Peters A, Day MJ, Mason GJ. Long-term effects of cage-cleaning frequency and bedding type on laboratory rat health, welfare, and handleability: a cross-laboratory study. Laboratory animals. 2006;40(4):353-70.

13. Gordon CJ. Effect of cage bedding on temperature regulation and metabolism of group-housed female mice. Comparative medicine. 2004;54(1):63-8.

14. Gaskill BN, Gordon CJ, Pajor EA, Lucas JR, Davis JK, Garner JP. Impact of nesting material on mouse body temperature and physiology. Physiology & behavior. 2013;110:87-95.

15. Buddaraju AKV, Van Dyke RW. Effect of animal bedding on rat liver endosome acidification. Comparative medicine. 2003;53(6):616-21.

16. Armstrong KR, Clark TR, Peterson AR. Use of corn-husk nesting material to reduce aggression in caged mice. Journal of the American Association for Laboratory Animal Science. 1998;37(4):64-6.

17. Landeros RV, Morisseau C, Yoo HJ, Fu SH, Hammock BD, Trainor BC. Corncob bedding alters the effects of estrogens on aggressive behavior and reduces estrogen receptor-α ex-pression in the brain. Endocrinology. 2012;153(2):949-53.

18. Tanaka T, Ogata A, Inomata A, Nakae D. Effects of different types of bedding materials on behavioral development in lab-oratory CD1 mice (Mus musculus). Birth Defects Research

62

IJVST DOI:10.22067/veterinary.v12i1.869432020-1 (22)

RESEARCH ARTICLEPart B: Developmental and Repro-ductive Toxicology. 2014;101(5):393-401.

19. Hullinger R, O’riordan K, Burger C. Environmental enrichment improves learning and memory and long-term potentiation in young adult rats through a mechanism requiring mGluR5 signaling and sustained ac-tivation of p70s6k. Neurobiology of learning and memory. 2015;125:126-34.

20. Blom H, Van Tintelen G, Van Vor-stenbosch C, Baumans V, Beynen A. Preferences of mice and rats for types of bedding material. Laboratory ani-mals. 1996;30(3):234-44.

21. Kawakami K, Shimosaki S, Tongu M, Kobayashi Y, Nabika T, Nomura M, et al. Evaluation of bedding and nesting materials for laboratory mice by pref-erence tests. Experimental animals. 2007;56(5):363-8.

22. Broderson JR, Lindsey JR, Crawford JE. The role of environmental ammo-nia in respiratory mycoplasmosis of rats. The American journal of pathol-ogy. 1976;85(1):115.

23. Shi D, Chen C, Zhao S, Ge F, Liu D, Hao S. Effects of walnut polyphenol on learning and memory functions in hypercholesterolemia mice. J Food

Nutr Res. 2014;2(8):450-6.

24. Smach M, Hafsa J, Charfeddine B, Dridi H, Limem K, editors. Effects of sage extract on memory performance in mice and acetylcholinesterase ac-tivity. Annales pharmaceutiques fran-caises; 2015: Elsevier.

25. Bakoyiannis I, Daskalopoulou A, Per-gialiotis V, Perrea D. Phytochemicals and cognitive health: Are flavonoids doing the trick? Biomedicine & Phar-macotherapy. 2019;109:1488-97.

26. Khan H, Amin S, Kamal MA, Patel S. Flavonoids as acetylcholinesterase in-hibitors: Current therapeutic standing and future prospects. Biomedicine & Pharmacotherapy. 2018;101:860-70.

27. Rosenzweig MR, Bennett EL. Psy-chobiology of plasticity: effects of training and experience on brain and behavior. Behavioural brain research. 1996;78(1):57-65.

28. Rosenbaum MD, VandeWoude S, Johnson TE. Effects of cage-change frequency and bedding volume on mice and their microenvironment. Journal of the American Associa-tion for Laboratory Animal Science. 2009;48(6):763-73.

29. Jaasma L. A Review of the Housing Conditions for Laboratory Animals

2014.

30. Coon R, Jones R, Jenkins Jr L, Siegel J. Animal inhalation studies on ammo-nia, ethylene glycol, formaldehyde, dimethylamine, and ethanol. Toxi-cology and applied pharmacology. 1970;16(3):646-55.

31. Ferrecchia CE, Jensen K, Van Andel R. Intracage ammonia levels in stat-ic and individually ventilated cages housing C57BL/6 mice on 4 bedding substrates. Journal of the American Association for Laboratory Animal Science. 2014;53(2):146-51.

32. Bosoi CR, Rose CF. Identifying the direct effects of ammonia on the brain. Metabolic brain disease. 2009;24(1):95-102.

33. Niknahad H, Jamshidzadeh A, Heidari R, Zarei M, Ommati MM. Ammonia-induced mitochondrial dysfunction and energy metabolism disturbances in isolated brain and liver mitochondria, and the effect of taurine administration: relevance to hepatic encephalopathy treatment. Clinical and experimental hepatolo-gy. 2017;3(3):141.

34. Kilburn KH. Is inhaled ammonia neurotoxic? Environmental Manage-ment and Health. 2000;11(3):239-50.

Abstracts (in Persian)

Iranian Journal of VeterinaryScience and Technology

Received: Accepted after revision: Published online:

IJVST 2020-1 (22) DOI:10.22067/veterinary.v12i1.86943

85

چکیده

واژگان کلیدی

اثر مواد بسترساز بر عملکرد حافظه و یادگیری و ترجیح جنس در موش های صحرایی

مواد بستر ساز، حافظه و یادگیری، ترجیح جنس، موش صحرایی

مهدی عباس نژاد1، راضیه کوشکی*2، سعید اسماعیلی ماهانی1، عباس تاج آبادی1، ریحانه نادری1

2020- May- 19

2020- Sep- 09

2020- Jun- 26

1بخش زیست شناسی، دانشکده علوم، دانشگاه شهید با هنر کرمان، کرمان، ایران2بخش زیست شناسی، دانشکده علوم، دانشگاه لرستان، خرم آباد، ایران

* نویسنده مسئول

امروزه انواع مواد بستر ساز براساس درجه سلامت، در دسترس بودن و هزینه برای دام و حیوانات آزمایشگاهی مورد استفاده است. در مطالعه حاضر، عملکرد حافظه و یادگیری و ترجیح به مواد بستر ساز در موش های صحرایی نگهداری شده تحت مواد بستر ساز مختلف بررسی شد. حیوانات در مواد بستر ساز در اندازه مشابه از چوب خاک اره ، گردو، پسته، سمندر، بادام و ساقه و برگ یونجه برای دو هفته نگهداری شدند. حافظه و یادگیری با استفاده از تست ماز آبی موریس)MWM( و شاتل باکس بررسی شد. یک ماز شعاعی تغییر یافته برای سنجش ترجیح جنس مواد بسترساز استفاده شد. برای هر یک از مواد بسترساز سطح آمونیاک درون قفس طی یک هفته محاسبه و نتایج نشان داد حیواناتی که در چوب بادام و گردو نگهداری شدند توانایی حافظه و یادگیری بهتری در تست MWM و شاتل باکس دارند. ضعیف ترین حافظه و یادگیری در موش های صحرایی قرار گرفته در یونجه به عنوان ماده بستر ساز مشاهده شد. در تست ترجیح جنس، حیوانات زمان بیشتری را در بازوهای حاوی گردو و بادام و زمان کمتری را در ناحیه حاوی یونجه گذراندند. علاوه براین، مصرف آب و غذا و همچنین تعداد ورود به ناحیه حاوی ماده بستر ساز یونجه در مقایسه با دیگر مواد بستر ساز کاهش یافت. همچنین، در قفس حاوی یونجه بیشترین سطح آمونیاک مشاهده شد. در مجموع نتایج نشان داد، مواد بسترساز با جنس متفاوت و خواص شیمیایی معین

اثرات متفاوتی بر عملکرد حافظه و یادگیری موش های صحرایی دارند.