The Effect of Goal Conflict_Journal of Leadership & Organizational Studies-2002-Slocum-77-89

13
7/21/2019 The Effect of Goal Conflict_Journal of Leadership & Organizational Studies-2002-Slocum-77-89 http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/the-effect-of-goal-conflictjournal-of-leadership-organizational-studies-2002-slocum-77-89 1/13 The Effect of Goal Conflict on Performance* John W. Slocum, Jr., Southern Methodist University, Dallas, TX William L. Cron, Texas Christian University, Fort Worth, TX Steven P. Brown, University of Houston, Houston, TX The authors investigated the motivational effects of goal conflict in a complex goal performance setting. Goal conflict was found to have an indirect influence on performance through its relationship with goal commitment. Goal conflict was negatively associated with goal commitment when controlling for the other antecedents, including expectancy, self-efficacy, and need for achievement.  As predicted, goal commitment and self-efficacy were related to goal-directed behaviors and goal-directed behaviors were related to performance. Performance was positively related to positive outcome emotions and negatively related to negative outcome emotions. The authors discuss the implications for the professional practitioner, theory development, and future research. * The authors wish to acknowledge the constructive comments made by Bill Dillon, Larry Peters, Don Vande and Charles Williams. Portions of this paper were presented at the  Academy of Management meetings in This research was funded by the Cox School of Business, the participating company, and OxyChem Corporati THE EFFECT OF GOAL CONFLICT ON PERFORMANCE Motivation continues to be an important issue to managers and researchers. In the motivation literature, goal theory has emerged as one of the most valid approaches to understanding the process of motivation (Austin and Vancouver, 1996). The core finding of goal-setting research is that under certain conditions, specific, difficult goals lead to higher levels of performance than easy goals or vague goals (Locke and Latham, 1990). One of the most frequently cited conditions necessary for the goal-performance relationship is that individuals must possess requisite commitment to achieving the goal. short, no motivational effects will occur f goal-setting, if there is no commitment to the g Goal commitment represents a pers attachment to or determination to reach a (Locke, Latham, and Erez, 1988), embodying the strength of one’s intention to reach a goal the unwillingness to abandon or lower a goal time. Two reviews (Hollenbeck and Klein, 1 Locke, Latham, and Erez, 1989) highlight central importance of goal commitment in goal-setting process. Locke and Latham (1 point out that goal commitment’s impact on goal-setting process is reduced when goal conf is present. However, the few research stu dealing with goal conflict have eviden consistent results. In addition only one s (Locke, Smith, Erez, Chah, and Schaffer, 19 directly measured goal conflict. The purpose of this research is to exp how goal conflict is related to goal commitm and performance. We explore this issue in context of a longitudinal, field study.  As common in many organizational setti participants in the study are asked to juggle t time to meet multiple, competing performa goals, including family-work tradeoffs. I complex situation, goal commitment hypothesized to influence performance indire through the mediation of goal-directed behavi Lastly, we also examine the emotio consequences of performance.  at University of Lincoln on April 11, 2015  jlo.sagepub.com Downloaded from 

Transcript of The Effect of Goal Conflict_Journal of Leadership & Organizational Studies-2002-Slocum-77-89

Page 1: The Effect of Goal Conflict_Journal of Leadership & Organizational Studies-2002-Slocum-77-89

7/21/2019 The Effect of Goal Conflict_Journal of Leadership & Organizational Studies-2002-Slocum-77-89

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/the-effect-of-goal-conflictjournal-of-leadership-organizational-studies-2002-slocum-77-89 1/13

The Effect of Goal Conflict

on Performance*

John W. Slocum, Jr., Southern Methodist University, Dallas, TXWilliam L. Cron, Texas Christian University, Fort Worth, TX

Steven P. Brown, University ofHouston, Houston, TX

The authors investigated the motivational effectsof goal conflict in a complex goal performance

setting. Goal conflictwas

found to have an

indirect influence on performance through its

relationship with goal commitment. Goal conflictwas negatively associated with goal commitment

when controlling for the other antecedents,

including expectancy, self-efficacy, and need forachievement.  As predicted, goal commitment and

self-efficacy were related to goal-directedbehaviors and goal-directed behaviors were

related to performance. Performance was

positively related to positive outcome emotions

and negatively related to negative outcome

emotions.The authors

discussthe

implications forthe professional practitioner, theory development,and future research.

* The authors wish to acknowledge the constructive comments made by Bill Dillon, Larry Peters, Don Vandeand Charles Williams. Portions of this paper were presented at the Academy of Management meetings in

This research was funded by the Cox School of Business, the participating company, and OxyChem Corporati

THE EFFECT OF GOAL CONFLICT ON

PERFORMANCE

Motivation continues to be an important issue

to managers and researchers. In the motivation

literature, goal theory has emerged as one of the

most valid approaches to understanding the

process of motivation (Austin and Vancouver,

1996). The core finding of goal-setting research isthat under certain conditions, specific, difficult

goals lead to higher levels of performance than

easy goals or vague goals (Locke and Latham,

1990). One of the most frequently cited

conditions necessary for the goal-performance

relationship is that individuals must possess

requisite commitment to achieving the goal.short, no motivational effects will occur f

goal-setting, if there is no commitment to the gGoal commitment represents a pers

attachment to or determination to reach a

(Locke, Latham, and Erez, 1988), embodyingthe strength of one’s intention to reach a goalthe unwillingness to abandon or lower a goaltime. Two reviews (Hollenbeck and Klein, 1Locke, Latham, and Erez, 1989) highlightcentral importance of goal commitment in

goal-setting process. Locke and Latham (1point out that goal commitment’s impact on

goal-setting process is reduced when goal conf

is present. However, the few research studealing with goal conflict have eviden

consistent results. In addition only one s(Locke, Smith, Erez, Chah, and Schaffer, 19directly measured goal conflict.

The purpose of this research is to exphow goal conflict is related to goal commitm

and performance. We explore this issue in

context of a longitudinal, field study.  As

common in many organizational setti

participants in the study are asked to juggle t

time to meet multiple, competing performa

goals, including family-worktradeoffs. I

complex situation, goal commitment

hypothesized to influence performance indirethrough the mediation of goal-directed behavi

Lastly, we also examine the emotio

consequences of performance.

 at University of Lincoln on April 11, 2015 jlo.sagepub.comDownloaded from 

Page 2: The Effect of Goal Conflict_Journal of Leadership & Organizational Studies-2002-Slocum-77-89

7/21/2019 The Effect of Goal Conflict_Journal of Leadership & Organizational Studies-2002-Slocum-77-89

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/the-effect-of-goal-conflictjournal-of-leadership-organizational-studies-2002-slocum-77-89 2/13

78

Goal Conflict

Goal conflict is the degree to which

individuals feel that their multiple goals are

incompatible (Locke, Smith, Erez, Chah, Schaffer,

1994).  As such, it constitutes an important facet

of the more general role conflict construct, which

has played a prominent role in organizationalbehavior studies (Jackson and Schuler 1985).Several types of goal conflict have been identified.

One type occurs when an externally imposed goalconflicts with one’s personal goal. Erez, Earleyand Hulin (1985) found that when subjects were

assigned a goal that is significantly higher than

their previously chosen personal goal level, the

commitment to the assigned goal and performancewere lower than when personal goals were set

after the goal was assigned. Latham, Erez, and

Locke (1988) replicated this finding. Vance and

Colella (1990) observeda

similar decrease incommitment to an assigned goal as the level of the

assigned goal was steadily increased over time,which made it more and more discrepant from

one’s personal goal. Unfortunately, none of these

studies actually measured goal conflict.

 A second type of goal conflict occurs when

people are asked to achieve multiple outcomes

(e.g., meeting a quantity quota vs. not making any

mistakes) when performing a single task. In this

case, the conflict is over which of the multipleperformance dimensions an individual should

focus on or emphasize. Erez (1990) found a

significant negative correlation between

performance quantity and quality on a

computerized task involving arithmetic problems.Once again, experienced goal conflict was not

explicitly manipulated or measured, but was

recognized as a cause of the goal performanceresults.

 A third type of goal conflict involves trade-

offs between several types of tasks or outcomes

when multiple goals or tasks exist (e.g., devotingtime to selling product A and product B, givenlimited available time). In a laboratory setting,Erez, Gopher, and Arizi (1990) found that when

subjects were instructed to improve their

performance on one of two assigned tasks, their

performance suffered on the other task. Subjectshandled the conflict by prioritizing one goal at the

expense of the other. In another laboratoryexperiment, Keman and Lord (1990) found that

when subjects were required to set goals for two

different tasks, they often gave one task priority by

setting higher goals for it.  As was the case with

the other studies described, experienced conflict

was not actually measured in either of these

studies, but inferred.

In a critical advancement of goal conflict

research, Locke, et al. (1994) explicitly measured

goal conflict both in a laboratory study and a

follow-up field study. The laboratory studyfocused on goal conflict that arose when qualityand quantity objectives were assigned for a singletask. The results indicate that goal conflict was

related to goal commitment. High experiencedgoal conflict was related to moderate levels of goalcommitment and was negatively related to

quantity, but not quality of performance. This

study also differed from previous goal conflict

studies in that groups of subjects worked togetherto perform the assigned task.  All measures and

analyseswere

performed at the group level.To test the generalizability of their results,

they conducted a second study. The second studywas a correlational field study in which conflict

existed between two different tasks. The subjectsin this study were college professors and the typeof goal conflict was research versus teachingperformance over a 14 year period of time. The

results indicated a negative relationship between

goal conflict and research performance, as

measured by number of academic publicationsover the 14 year time period. Goal conflict was

unrelated to teaching performance. Goalcommitment was not measured in this second

study. It is also important to note the post-hocnature of the goal conflict and goal level measures

in this study. In other words, performance had

already occurred prior to people indicating their

performance goal level and experienced goalconflict. Notwithstanding this limitation, we

concur with Locke et al., in concluding that goalconflict is an important concept worthy of

attention and further studyThis study examines the goal conflict that

arises from pressure to perform multiple goalswithin a limited time. Goal conflict is likely to

arise in organizations when a manager assigns a

performance goal for a new task or deliverable in

addition to one’s regular tasks. - When the

additional tasks are short-term and of limited

temporal duration, goal conflict is especially likelyto be experienced. This type of goal conflict is

likely in organizational settings in which

individual may participate in special projects or

Page 3: The Effect of Goal Conflict_Journal of Leadership & Organizational Studies-2002-Slocum-77-89

7/21/2019 The Effect of Goal Conflict_Journal of Leadership & Organizational Studies-2002-Slocum-77-89

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/the-effect-of-goal-conflictjournal-of-leadership-organizational-studies-2002-slocum-77-89 3/13

temporary project teams in additional to their

regular duties for which a financial reward is

offered.

The Model

We believe that there is little consensus as to

how goal conflict influences performance. The

early studies suggest that goal conflict willinfluence performance through its negativerelationship with goal commitment, while later

studies propose a direct relationship of goalconflict on performance. Even in research that

actually measured goal conflict (Locke, et al.,

1994), one study hypothesized that goal conflict

would be related to goal commitment, while the

second study did not include goal commitment and

hypothesized a direct relationship between goalconflict and performance. The post-hoc nature of

the measures in the second study, however, may

have precluded the inclusion of goal commitmentin the study.

Our model ofhow goal conflict impacts goalperformance is presented in Figure 1. The model is

consistent with goal-setting theory. Goal conflict

is expected to indirectly influence performancethrough goal commitment. As stated by Locke, et

al. (1968), &dquo;It is virtually axiomatic that if there is

no commitment to goals, then goal setting will not

work&dquo; (p. 23). Commitment to difficult goals is a

necessary condition for the goal difficulty effect to

occur. If one feels that a particular goal is in

conflict with other goals, then they are less likelyto feel committed to the goal:

Goal Commitment

One of the most frequently stated conditions

necessary for goals to lead to higher performance

is commitment to a specific, difficult goal.has been a central concept in goal-setting tsince its inception (Locke and Latham, 1Two recent meta-analytic of the accumul

evidence on the antecedents of goal commit

indicate that individual factors affect

commitment (Klein, Wesson, Hollenbeck,

 Alge, 1999; Wofford, Goodwin, and Prem1992). Our model likewise hypothesizesconflict will influence goal commitment: the

conflict they experience with respect to this

the less committed a person is to a goal. The

of conflict introduced here is one that arises

being asked to achieve multiple goals, glimited resources and time.

In an effort to demonstrate the increme

contribution of goal conflict as an anteceden

goal commitment, our model also incl

expectancy, self-efficacy, and need

achievement as three important persantecedents of goal commitment. Two m

analytic studies of the antecedents of

commitment (Klein, et al., 1999; Wofford, e

1992) conclude that expectancy of goal attain

is an important antecedent of goal commitm

Self-efficacy refers to one’s beliefs about o

capacity to achieve a particular goal or

(Bandura, 1986). Bandura claims that hiefficacious people actively pursue increasidifficult challenges, while inefficac

individuals attempt to avoid challenges that

result in negative self-evaluations. Thoughincluded in the Klein, et al study, the meta-analresults of Wofford indicate that self-efficacyimportant antecedent of goal commitment.  A

result, self-efficacy is expected to be positirelated to goal commitment.

Figure 1: Hypothesized Model

 at University of Lincoln on April 11, 2015 jlo.sagepub.comDownloaded from 

Page 4: The Effect of Goal Conflict_Journal of Leadership & Organizational Studies-2002-Slocum-77-89

7/21/2019 The Effect of Goal Conflict_Journal of Leadership & Organizational Studies-2002-Slocum-77-89

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/the-effect-of-goal-conflictjournal-of-leadership-organizational-studies-2002-slocum-77-89 4/13

80

Our model also proposes a positiverelationship between need for achievement and

goal commitment. Need for achievement is a

personality variable in which an individual aspiresto accomplish difficult tasks and to maintain highstandards over time. Keman and Lord (1990)indicate that individuals high in need for

achievement are more goal-committed and

outperform those low in need for achievement.

Hollenbeck, Klein, O’Leary, and Wright (1989),Hollenbeck, Williams and Klein (1989), and

Johnson and Perlow (1992) also found significantcorrelations between need for achievement and

goal commitment.

Goal-directed Behaviors

In complex task environments, managing and

executing behaviors that are instrumental to

performance are likely to be importantconsequences of goal commitment (Latham and

Locke, 1991; Early and Shalley, 1991; Wright and

Kacmar, 1994;  Austin and Vancouver, 1996).Development of goal-directed behaviors involves

making choices regarding the intensity, duration,and direction of work-related effort (Kanfer, 1991;

Naylor, Pritchard, and Ilgen, 1980).  According to

Locke and Latham (1990), the ultimate success of

goal-directed behaviors in producing desired

outcomes depends on maintaining a high level of

focused effort. When goal commitment is high

(comparedto

low),individuals are more

likelyto

direct their cognitive and behavioral resources

toward planning and executing goal-directedbehaviors that facilitate performance. Individuals

lacking a commitment to a goal are not likely to

sustain sufficient effort to achieve their

performance expectations. We expect that

commitment to a goal will lead to more extensive

and persistent engagement in goal directed

behaviors that are instrumental for performance.Our model also shows a direct effect of self-

efficacy on goal directed behaviors. Locke and

Latham

(1990)have

arguedthat

self-efficacyhas a

direct effect on performance. We also posit that

self-efficacy will influence goal-directedbehaviors. People tend to avoid tasks they believe

exceed their capabilities, but they perform with

confidence activities they judge themselves

capable of completing successfully. Individuals

with strong self-efficacy beliefs, for example, have

been found to maintain task focus and high levels

of effort when confronted with challenging

demands (Martin and Manning, 1995). To be

successful, individuals must translate goal-directedbehaviors into achieved goals. Our model also

posits a direct positive relationship between goal-directed behaviors and performance.

Outcome Emotions

We also posit that performance hasemotional consequences that may subsequentlyinfluence motivation and future performance. Anumber of theories hypothesize that feedback

gives rise to emotional responses (e.g., Carver and

Scheier, 1996, 1998; Lazarus, 1991; Oatley and

Johnson-Laird, 1996).  A fundamental tenet of

Lazarus’s cognitive theory of emotions (1991) is

that emotions arise from situations that affect a

person’s well-being. Therefore, emotions are likelyto result from performance feedback. Goal

success, assuming that the goal is of personalimportance and one to which the person has

expended some amount of effort, will triggerpositive emotional responses, while goaldisconfirmation will trigger negative emotional

responses. Goal-directed emotions (e.g., anger,

shame, pride, or happiness) are potentiallyimportant because they arise as a consequence of a

person’s performance (Ortony, Clore and Collins,

1998) and may affect subsequent attitudes and

motivation. Emotions function to direct attention,

cognitive processing and behavioral intentions

(Bagozzi, Baumgartnerand

Pieters, 1998).The relationships based on theory are shown

in Figure 1. We propose the following hypothesesto test our model:

Hl. Goal conflict will be negativelyrelated to goal commitment

H2. Self-efficacy, expectancy and need

for achievement will be positivelyrelated to goal commitment.

H3: Goal commitment and self-efficacywill be positively related to goal-directed behaviors.

H4: Goal-directed behaviors will be

positively related to performance.H5: Performance will be

negatively related to negativeemotions and positively related to

positive emotions.

 at University of Lincoln on April 11, 2015 jlo.sagepub.comDownloaded from 

Page 5: The Effect of Goal Conflict_Journal of Leadership & Organizational Studies-2002-Slocum-77-89

7/21/2019 The Effect of Goal Conflict_Journal of Leadership & Organizational Studies-2002-Slocum-77-89

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/the-effect-of-goal-conflictjournal-of-leadership-organizational-studies-2002-slocum-77-89 5/13

METHODS

Sample and Goal Situation

The sample for this study consisted of

salespeople working for a distributor of medical

supplies. Customers consisted of physicianpractices in the western part of the United States.

The majority of salespeople’s time was spentcalling on existing customers and taking orders to

replenish inventories of medical supplies regularlyused in the practice (e.g., paper products, gloves,syringes, etc.). The sales force was also

responsible for identifying opportunities to sell

medical equipment (e.g., examination tables,examination lights, wireless patient monitors,

etc.), usually under $10,000. Approximately 90%of total sales were in medical supplies, thoughequipment sales accounted for as much as 20% of

total sales for some salespeople.The

present studywas conducted in a field

setting in which the distributor’s salespeople were

assigned a performance goal for a promotion on a

piece of equipment with an average selling priceof $5,400. The promotion lasted 90 days.Salespeople received extra monetary

compensation ($300) for each piece of equipmentthey sold during the promotion period. In supportof this promotion, physicians were offered a

special reduced price for the promoted product.This type of promotion is standard within the

industry, and this particular product line is

annually promoted at some time. To consummate

a power table sale, however, salespeople had to

deviate from their normal calling patterns. The

piece of equipment represented a significantcompensation opportunity for the salespeople, but

they were not likely to sell more than two or

possibly three during the promotion because of the

product’s overall demand level, the length of the

selling process, and the need to sustain their

primary medical supply selling responsibilities.It is important to note that the selling process

for equipment is quite different from that for

medical supplies. A typical sales call to pick up

orders for supplies usually took less than 15minutes to complete. It consists of checkingstorage cabinets for inventory levels, checkingwith nurses and/or an office manager for

additional order items, and ensuring that all needs

were being met for the regular flow of supplies.Selling a piece of equipment was quite different.

 A sales opportunity needed to be identified (i.e.,

was the practice in need of a new examinat

table?). One of the key decision-makers is

doctor.  A formal sales presentation to expproduct features and benefits would be necessaIn short, a different call routine was necessary;

requiring a different set of competencies f

those typically needed for medical sup

replenishment. As a result, therewas

the potenfor goal conflict. The degree of felt conflict mivary as a result of a salesperson’s competencand past success in selling equipment.

The average salesperson in the sample was

years of age, earned $41,000 annually, 75 perwere college educated, and 85 percent were ma

This is largely a veteran sales force with

average organizational tenure of just over

years.

Consistent with the distributor’s stan

practices, product promotions were introduce

the

company’s quarterlysales

meetManagement encouraged salespeople to set gfor this promotion but did not formally assisalesperson-specific goal. Instead, manageminformed the sales force that each salespeshould sell at least one power table duringpromotional period. It should be acknowledghowever, that personal goal-setting may have b

stimulated by our asking salespeople to complequestionnaire following a briefing on the prodand promotion by sales management.

The time 1 questionnaire included measu

ofself-efficacy, expectancy, need for achieveme

goal conflict, and goal commitment. Immediatfollowing the conclusion of this promotion t

months later, we recorded the number ofunits

from company records and sent all salespeopquestionnaire to measure the goal-direbehaviors they employed during this promotand their emotions related to achieving or faito attain their goal. One hundred fifty-nine ou

167 salespeople returned the first questionnwhile 153 completed the second questionnaOne questionnaire was not fully completed at

2, leaving a total of 152 usable sets

questionnaires, representing a 91 percent resprate.

Measures

Goal conflict. A focus group was conduc

with salespeople from the organization to deveitems for this scale. Our aim was to unders

the personal goal conflicts that this promot

 at University of Lincoln on April 11, 2015 jlo.sagepub.comDownloaded from 

Page 6: The Effect of Goal Conflict_Journal of Leadership & Organizational Studies-2002-Slocum-77-89

7/21/2019 The Effect of Goal Conflict_Journal of Leadership & Organizational Studies-2002-Slocum-77-89

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/the-effect-of-goal-conflictjournal-of-leadership-organizational-studies-2002-slocum-77-89 6/13

82

would cause for them. These discussions yieldeditems that were consistent with prior research

fmdings suggesting that salespeople often perceivesome degree of goal conflict between various

expectations during a special product promotionalcampaign (e.g., Singh, 1993). On the basis of this

feedback, we developed a 4-item scale for use in

conjunction with the promotion: &dquo;Time spent on

this promotion will significantly limit my time for

selling other products,&dquo; &dquo;My present workload

does not allow me to spend much time on this

promotion,&dquo; &dquo;I call on too many customers to

spend much time on this promotion,&dquo; &dquo;If I spendmuch time on this promotion, it would hurt myoverall sales.&dquo; The response format was a 5-pointLikert scale ranging from strongly agree to

strongly disagree. Coefficient alpha was .70.

Self efficacy. This measure assesses a

person’s confidence in achieving various

performance levels. We asked study participantsto indicate whether or not they expected to achieve

each of ten levels of output sales performance(from 1-10 units). For each level, studyparticipants indicated whether they believed theycould reach that level of performance (yes or no)and how confident they were in attaining it

(ranging from 100% certain to 0%). Consistent

with Lee and Bobko (1994) and Mathieu and

Button (1992), self-efficacy scores consisted of the

summation of confidence ratings across levels of

performance the salespeople felt confident that

they could achieve (i.e., indicated a yes response).Expectancy. A single item asked salespeople

to estimate the probability of reaching their self-

set goal for the promotion.Need for  Achievement.  Achievement

motivation was adapted from Steers and

Braunstein’s (1975) measure. This five-item

achievement motivation scale had an internal

consistency reliability of .75.

Goal Commitment. Goal commitment was

measured using the 9-item measure developed byHollenbeck, Klein, O’Leary, and Wright (1989).The internal consistency reliability was .85.

Salespeople were asked to use their self-set goalfor the promotion as their reference point for

answering this questionnaire.Goal-directed behaviors. We also asked

focus group participants what behaviors they could

engage in to enhance their performance during the

promotion. Their responses could generally be

grouped into three categories: 1) behaviors

associated with general sales territorymanagement, 2) behaviors for specific account

management, and 3) overall effort directed toward

attainment of one’s goal for the promotion. We

constructed items to tap each of these three

dimensions. Principal components analysis with

oblique rotation produced three factors with eigen

values greater than 1.0, explaining 77 percent ofthe variance. These factors corresponded to the

three intended dimensions previously noted.

Territory management (5 Likert-type items, a =

.80) focused on the amount of time salespeoplededicated to planning territorial strategies for this

power table promotion.  Account management (3

Likert-type items, a = .77) measured the extent of

specific account planning (calling on particulardoctors) for the promotion. Effort items (3 Likert-

type items, a = .92) asked salespeople to rate how

much time, intensity, and overall effort theyinvested in the promotion. Summated compositeindices were created for each first-order factor,and the three composite indices were then used as

manifest indicators of goal-directed behaviors.

Performance. Company records were

consulted following the 90-day promotion to

measure the number of units each salesperson sold

during the promotion. Management was very

pleased with the results of the promotion, which

exceeded their expectations based on previousexperience with similar promotions. The average

number of units sold

byeach

salesperson duringthe promotion was 2.75 units. Management’sassigned goal was one unit per salesperson.  An

assigned goal of selling one unit was still

challenging, however, as 21 % of the salespeoplein the study failed to sell a single unit. Twenty-two (14%) salespeople sold 6 units during the

promotion.Emotions. Positive and negative emotions

were measured using a scale developed byBagozzi, et al., (1998) and validated in other goal-setting research (Pergugini and Bagozzi, 2001;

Perguginiand

Conner, 2001).The instrument was

administered after the promotion was completed.The instructions asked: &dquo;As a result of your

performance relative to the goal you set for the

promotion, how intensely did you feel each of the

following emotions.&dquo; Eleven-point scales

anchored by &dquo;not at all&dquo; and &dquo;very much&dquo; were

used to measure seven positive emotions --

excited, delighted, etc. -- (a = .94) and 10 negative

 at University of Lincoln on April 11, 2015 jlo.sagepub.comDownloaded from 

Page 7: The Effect of Goal Conflict_Journal of Leadership & Organizational Studies-2002-Slocum-77-89

7/21/2019 The Effect of Goal Conflict_Journal of Leadership & Organizational Studies-2002-Slocum-77-89

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/the-effect-of-goal-conflictjournal-of-leadership-organizational-studies-2002-slocum-77-89 7/13

emotions --

angry, frustrated, etc. -- (a= .95). The

correlation between positive and negativeemotions was -.45 {p < .05), indicating that the

more positive emotions people experienced as a

result of their performance, the less negativeemotions they experienced. In the covariance

structure model, we allowed the residuals of

positiveand

negativeemotions to correlate to

reflect this rather strong negative correlation.

Results

Descriptive statistics and intercorrelations are

presented in Table 1. The model presented in

Figure 1 was estimated with LISREL 8.51 usingthe sample covariance matrix as input. The

adequacy of the model to represent structural

relationships among the constructs was assessed

by estimating a series of hierarchically nested

models, beginning with the most parsimonious and

proceeding to the more complex. Estimation of the

hypothesized (i.e., basic) model resulted in a fit of

chi square (36 d.f.) = 146.11, RMSEA = .14, GFI= .85, CFI = .86. These indices suggest a

generally poor fit of the basic model to the data.

 All hypothesized paths, however, were statisticallysignificant.

LISREL modification indices suggested that

adding two interpretable structural paths to the

basic model would substantially improve model fit

and provide a more accurate overall representationof the data. First, a direct path linking self-

efficacy to performance was added. A direct self-

efficacy - performance path is consistent withsocial cognitive theory (Bandura, 1986; 1997) and

suggests that the effects of self-efficacy on

performance are not completely mediated by goalcommitment and goal-directed behaviors.

 Addition of this path to the model dramaticallyimproved the overall fit (chi square change [1 d.f.]= 56.97).

Second, a direct path linking goal-directedbehaviors to positive emotions was added. This

path indicates that higher levels of goal-directedbehaviors were positively and directly related to

positive outcome emotions. The effect of goal-directed behaviors was not completely mediated

by performance outcomes. This suggests that

greater involvement in goal-directed behaviors

was intrinsically rewarding in and of itself, over

and above the effect of performance outcomes.

 Addition of this path further improved the model

fit to chi square (34 d.f.) = 68.18, RMSEA = .082,

RMR = .058, GFI = .93, CFI = .96. These ind

all suggest a good fit to the data. To control

possible method variance, we allowed

residuals of goal-directed behaviors and posiemotions to correlate. The standardized pcoefficient for the correlated error term was

(ns). The addition of this correlated error term

not affect the structural

path linking goal-direbehaviors to positive outcome emotions. T

indicates that the direct path from goal-direbehavior to positive emotions is not primarattributable to common method variance.

Standardized path coefficients for the rev

model are presented in Figure 2. Table 2 repstandardized estimates and t-values for

hypothesized and revised models. All of

hypothesized antecedent relationships with gcommitment were statistically significant.  

predicted in hypothesis 1, goal conflict

negatively related to goal commitment. Hypothe2 was also supported in that self-efficexpectancy, and need for achievement were

positively related to goal commitment. Th

antecedents collectively accounted for 47 percof the variance in goal commitment.

significant negative path between goal conflict

goal commitment indicates the inevitable trade

that employees in organizations must manage

attain high performance. Some of these tasks mbe perceived as unpleasant (e.g., spending m

time on the road and away from family; c

calling on new physicians) and detract from o

commitment to attaining their goal.Goal commitment and self-efficacy w

positively related to goal-directed behaviosupporting hypothesis 3. Consistent with previresearch by Locke and Kristof (1996), salespeowith high, as opposed to low, self-efficacy w

were committed to their self-set goal spent m

time planning general territorial and specaccount strategies.  Additionally, they inve

more effort during the promotion than did

self-efficacy employees.  As predicted in H3,model accounted for 47 percent of the varianc

goal-directed behaviors. As predicted in hypothesis 4, goal-direbehaviors were positively related to performan Although the path from self-efficacyperformance was not hypothesized a-priorisubstantially improved the model’s fit when ad

to the basic model. Explained variance

 at University of Lincoln on April 11, 2015 jlo.sagepub.comDownloaded from 

Page 8: The Effect of Goal Conflict_Journal of Leadership & Organizational Studies-2002-Slocum-77-89

7/21/2019 The Effect of Goal Conflict_Journal of Leadership & Organizational Studies-2002-Slocum-77-89

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/the-effect-of-goal-conflictjournal-of-leadership-organizational-studies-2002-slocum-77-89 8/13

roi

00

rm cd

a

I bOto

wM4.1

0

 at University of Lincoln on April 11, 2015 jlo.sagepub.comDownloaded from 

Page 9: The Effect of Goal Conflict_Journal of Leadership & Organizational Studies-2002-Slocum-77-89

7/21/2019 The Effect of Goal Conflict_Journal of Leadership & Organizational Studies-2002-Slocum-77-89

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/the-effect-of-goal-conflictjournal-of-leadership-organizational-studies-2002-slocum-77-89 9/13

performance increased nine percent (from 59 to 68

percent) when the direct path from self-efficacywas added. The strength of this relationship is

particularly impressive given that measurement of

the self-efficacy and performance constructs was

separated temporally by three months and

accomplished using different types of measures.

The model accounted for 68 percent of thevariance in performance.

 As predicted in hypothesis 5, performancewas positively related to positive emotions and

negatively related to negative emotions. These

paths also indicate that high performance results in

mild positive emotions, whereas low performanceresults in strong negative emotions. As previouslynoted, the direct path between goal-directedbehaviors and positive emotions significantly

improved the model’s fit. Individuals w

engaged in high levels of account and territmanagement and expended effort experienmore positive emotions than individuals w

invested less management of their accounts a

territory and exerted less effort. It is notewortthat the direct effect of goal-directed behaviors

positive emotions was substantially stronger ththat of performance on positive emotions (evafter statistically controlling for method variancIt appears that people who extensively plan a

vigorously execute goal-directed behaviors f

good about themselves and their work. The

emotions result primarily from engagement in t

work itself and only secondarily fr

performance.

Figure 2: Standardized

Estimates for Final

 at University of Lincoln on April 11, 2015 jlo.sagepub.comDownloaded from 

Page 10: The Effect of Goal Conflict_Journal of Leadership & Organizational Studies-2002-Slocum-77-89

7/21/2019 The Effect of Goal Conflict_Journal of Leadership & Organizational Studies-2002-Slocum-77-89

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/the-effect-of-goal-conflictjournal-of-leadership-organizational-studies-2002-slocum-77-89 10/13

86

Table 2

Standardized Structural Parameter Estimates for Hypothesized and Revised Models

DISCUSSION

Goal commitment plays an important role in

the goal-setting and performance process. In his

original work, Locke (1968) hypothesized that

goal commitment was a necessary condition for

goal-setting to affect performance.  As a result,there has been some effort to establish the

antecedents of goal commitment. In meta-

analyses of the antecedents of goal commitment,Wofford, et al. (1992) and Klein, et al. (1999)identified several personal factors -- self-efficacy,expectancy, and need for achievement -- as

important antecedents of goal commitment. Theresults of this study demonstrate the impressiveability of personal factors to explain the variance

in goal commitment in a complex field-studyenvironment. The magnitude of the various

personal factors’ impact on goal commitment is

consistent with the meta-analytic results, even

though 78 percent of the studies included in the

meta-analysis were conducted in lab-settings.

 As expected in a complex task setting, goal-

directed behaviors mediated the relationshipbetween goal commitment and performance. Goal

commitment was positively related to goal-directed behaviors, which in turn, were positivelyrelated to performance. This is particularlynoteworthy because goal commitment was

measured only at the beginning of the promotionperiod, three months prior to its conclusion, while

goal-directed behaviors were measured at the end

of the promotion. It is important to remember that

in our study, cause and effect were not clearlyestablished. Data were collected at only two

points in time.  Adjustments in goal level, goalcommitment and goal-directed behaviors duringthe promotion were not measured. Control theorywould suggest that feedback on performanceduring the promotional period would influence

goal-directed behaviors (Klein, 1989). The

conflicting theoretical propositions between goaltheory and control theory can be resolved bydesigning a study that permits the researchers to

study the behavioral processes that people choose

at University of Lincoln on April 11 2015jlo sagepub comDownloaded from

Page 11: The Effect of Goal Conflict_Journal of Leadership & Organizational Studies-2002-Slocum-77-89

7/21/2019 The Effect of Goal Conflict_Journal of Leadership & Organizational Studies-2002-Slocum-77-89

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/the-effect-of-goal-conflictjournal-of-leadership-organizational-studies-2002-slocum-77-89 11/13

87

to enact after receiving feedback on each

performance event.

The effect of goal conflict on goalcommitment has received little attention, althoughit has been suggested that conflict can be

debilitating and may adversely influence

performance, as well as result in heightened levels

of stress. In our study, individuals were rewardedfor one type of activity (performance in the

promotion) while simultaneously being asked to

make other activities top priorities (e.g., achievingoverall sales volume goals, filling orders, etc.).Our results indicate that goal conflict is associated

with lower levels of commitment to a particulargoal. Interestingly, salespeople indicated only a

modest level of goal conflict (2.18 average on a

three-item, five-point scale) between competingsales objectives in this study. Six percent of the

salespeople noted that all-out pursuit ofthe special

promotion objectives would appreciably detractfrom their ability to meet their continuing sales

objectives for the quarter. Our results suggest that

even modest levels of goal conflict may be

detrimental to goal commitment and eventual

performance. An interesting issue for future research is to

examine the set of personal goals an individual

may establish within a particular role and the

causes of perceived conflict(s) between these

goals. It would likewise be interesting to obtain

commitment measures on each of an individual’s

conflicting goals.Relative to others, a

person maybe highly committed to one goal, but even more

committed to a second, conflicting goal. Without

measuring commitment to both goals, this

dynamic would go undetected.

The results of this study reinforce the need to

include individual difference factors, such as goalorientation in research conducted in complex task

settings (VandeWalle, Cron and Slocum, 2001).One must keep in mind, however, the particularsof the research setting when interpreting these

results. In this study, all salespeople had some and

most had extensive prior experience with

promotions of this type and with this specificproduct line.  As a result, they were aware of

effective behaviors for increasing their

performance. The organization set a uniform

performance goal for all people (e.g., sell at least

one) with few sanctions for non-achievement.

Finally, in a complex field task, it is possible that

unmeasured influences (e.g., past performance)

may exert influence on goal-directed behavior

that affect performance that are not completelycaptured by our measure of self-efficacy.

It is worth noting that the goal commitment

goal directed behavior and goal directed behavior

performance relationships, while statisticallsignificant, are not as strong as the self-efficacy

goal-directed behaviors and self-efficacyperformance relationships. While the direc

influence of self-efficacy is consistent with

previous laboratory results (Bandura and Jourden1991; Wood and Bandura, 1989; and WoodBandura and Bailey, 1990), the relatively weake

goal-directed behavior relationships may be due t

limitations in the measure of goal-directedbehavior utilized in this study. Goal-directe

behavior captured only certain aspects of tas

strategy. Goal-directed behaviors not fullcaptured here include novelty, creativity

complexity, and adaptability over the course of thepromotion. The appropriateness of our goaldirected behaviors was established throughconversations with company’s representatives. On

the other hand, there is no indication that peoplselected the best accounts on which to concentrate

their promotional efforts. A more comprehensivegoal-directed measure may have resulted i

stronger associations.

 As with almost all previous research on rol

conflict (e.g., King and King, 1990), the focus o

this study has been on the reactions of a foca

personwith

respectto

onlyone of many

goals. As

a result, very little prescriptive advice concerninghow to mitigate the effects of goal conflict has

been offered to management. The reciprocanature of the role sender-focal person relationshiphas not been adequately addressed. Particularlfruitful areas for further investigation include th

message transmission from role sender to the foca

person and the mechanisms, both personal and

organizational, by which external pressurebecome perceived as goal conflicts by the foca

person. Our approach of focusing on specific goaconflicts in a well defmed situation may be

particularly useful in future research.This is one of the few studies to examine

people’s emotional reactions to performance Although emotions may contribute to attitudessuch as job satisfaction, that are frequentlconsidered in organizational studies, emotions

have very rarely been explicitly considered

Emotions are likely to be more ephemeral but also

 at University of Lincoln on April 11, 2015 jlo.sagepub.comDownloaded from 

Page 12: The Effect of Goal Conflict_Journal of Leadership & Organizational Studies-2002-Slocum-77-89

7/21/2019 The Effect of Goal Conflict_Journal of Leadership & Organizational Studies-2002-Slocum-77-89

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/the-effect-of-goal-conflictjournal-of-leadership-organizational-studies-2002-slocum-77-89 12/13

88

more intense forms of affect than job attitudes and

are likely to have important effects on work

behavior. In our study, we considered emotions as

outcomes of goal-directed behaviors and

performance outcomes. Positive emotions resulted

not only from high performance but also from

extensive engagement in goal-directed behaviors.

In other words, working hard does produce its own

rewards independent of the outcome. Goal-

directed behaviors had both direct and indirect

effects on positive emotions (the indirect effect

mediated by performance). Of these, the direct

effect was considerably stronger. This suggeststhat one’s degree of planning and exertion of effort

in goal-directed activities generates positiveemotion over and above that which results from

performance. In other words, working hard is

emotionally rewarding. That is, the challenge of

the &dquo;chase&dquo; isexciting

in and of itself.

REFERENCES

 Austin, J. L., & Vancouver, J. B. (1996). Goal

constructs in psychology: Structure, processand content. Psychological Bulletin, 120(3),338-375.

Bagozzi, R. P., Baumgartner, H., & Pieters, R.

(1998). Goal-directed emotions. Cognitionand Emotion, 12(1), 1-26.

Bandura, A. (1977).Self-efficacy: The Exercise ofControl. San Francisco: Freeman.

Bandura, A. (1986). Social foundations of thoughtand action. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-

Hall.

Bandura  A., & Jourden, F. J. (1991). Self-

regulatory mechanisms governing the impactof social comparison on complex decision

making. Journal of Personality and Social

Psychology, 60, 941-951.

Carver, C., & Scheier, M. (1996).Perspectives on

Personality. Boston: Allyn and Bacon.

Early, P. C., & Shalley, C. E. (1991). New

perspectives on goals and performance:Merging motivation and cognition. In G.

Ferris & K. Rowland (Eds.), Research in

Personnel and Human Resources Management(Vol. 9, pp. 309-330). Greenwich, CT: JAI

Press.

Erez, M. (1990). Performance quality and wrk

motivation. In U. Kleinbeck, H. Quast, H.

Thierry, & H. Hacker (Eds.),Work motivation

(pp. 53-65). N.J.: L. Earlbaum.

Erez, M., Early, P. & Hulin, C. (1985). The impactof participation on goal acceptance and

performance: A two-step model. Academy ofManagement Journal, 28, 50-66.

Erez, M., Gopher, D., &  Arazi, N. (1990). Effects

of self-set goals and monetary rewards on dual

task performance. Organizational Behavior

and Human Decision Processes, 47, 247-269.Hollenbeck, J. R., & Klein, H. J. (1987). Goal

commitment and the goal setting process:

problems, prospects and proposals for future

research. Journal of Applied Psychology, 72,212-220.

Hollenbeck, J. R., Klein, H. J., O’Leary, A. M., &

Wright, P. M. (1989). Investigation of the

construct validity of a selfreport measure of

goal commitment. Journal of  AppliedPsychology, 74, 951-956.

Hollenbeck, J. R., Williams, C. R., & Klein, J. J.

(1989).  An empirical examination of the

antecedents of commitment to difficult goals.Journal of Applied Psychology, 74, 18-23.

Jackson, S., & Schuler, R.  A. (1985).  A Meta

analysis and conceptual critique of research on

role ambiguity and role conflict in work

settings. Organizational Behavior and Human

Decision Processes, 36, 16-78.

Johnson, D. S., & Perlow, R. (1992). The impact of

need for achievement components on goalcommitment and performance. Journal of

 Applied Social Psychology, 22, 1711-1720.

Kanfer, R. (1991). Motivation theory and industrial

and organizational psychology. In M. D.

Dunnette & L. Hough (Eds.), Handbook ofIndustrial and Organizational Psychology(2nd ed., Vol. 1, pp. 75170).

Kernan, M. C., & Lord, R. G. (1990). Effects of

valence, expectancies, and goalperformancediscrepancies in single and multiple goalenvironments. Journal of Applied Psychology,75, 194-203.

King, L., & King, D. (1990). Role conflict and

ambiguity: a critical assessment of construct

validity. Psychological Bulletin, 107, 48-64.

Klein, H., Wesson, M., Hollenbeck, J., &  Alge, B.

(1999). Goal commitment and the goalsetting

process: conceptual clarification and empiricalsynthesis. Journal of Applied Psychology, 84,885-896.

Latham, G. P., & Locke, E.  A. (1991). Self

regulation through goal setting.

Organizational Behavior and Human Decision

Processes, 50, 212-247.

Lazarus, R. S. (1991). Emotion and Adaptation.Oxford, New York, NY.

 at University of Lincoln on April 11, 2015 jlo.sagepub.comDownloaded from 

Page 13: The Effect of Goal Conflict_Journal of Leadership & Organizational Studies-2002-Slocum-77-89

7/21/2019 The Effect of Goal Conflict_Journal of Leadership & Organizational Studies-2002-Slocum-77-89

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/the-effect-of-goal-conflictjournal-of-leadership-organizational-studies-2002-slocum-77-89 13/13

8

Lee, C., & Bobko, P. (1994). Selfefficacy beliefs:

Comparison of five measures. Journal of Applied Psychology, 79, 364-369.

Locke, E.  A. (1968). Toward a theory of task

motivation and incentives. OrganizationalBehavior and Human -Performance, 3, 157-

189.

Locke, E.  A., & Kristof, A. L. (1996). Volitional

choices in the goal achievement process. In P.M. Gollwitzer & J.  A. Bargh (Eds.), ThePsychology of Action: Linking Cognition and

Motivation to Behavior (pp. 365-384). New

York, NY: Guilford Publications, Inc.

Locke, E. A., & Latham, G. P. (1990).A Theory ofGoal Setting and Task Performance.Englewood Cliffs, NJ: PrenticeHall.

Locke, E.  A., Latham, G. P., & Erez, M. (1988)The determinants of goal commitment.

 Academy ofManagement Review, 13, 23-39.

Locke, E. A., Smith, K. G., Erez, M., Chuh, D., &

Schaffer,  A. (1994). The effect of goalconflict on

performance.Journal

ofManagement, 20, 67-91.

Martin, B. A., & Manning, D. J. (1995). Combined

effects of normative information and task

difficulty on the goal commitment

performance relationship. Journal ofManagement, 21, 65-80.

Mathieu, J. E., & Button, S. B. (1992).  An

examination of the relative impact of

normative information and selfefficacy on

personal goals and performance over time.

Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 22,1758-1775.

Naylor, J. C., Pritchard, R. D., & Ilgen, D. R.

(1980).  A Theory of Behavior in

Organizations. New York: Academic Press.

Oatley, K. & JohnsonLaird, P. (1996). The

communicative theory of emotions: Empiricaltests, mental models, and implications for

social interaction. In L. Martin &  A. Tesser

(Eds.), Striving and feeling: Interactions

among goals, affect, and self-regulation (pp.363-393). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.

Ortony,  A., Clore, G., & Collins,  A. (1988).Thecognitive structure of emotions Cambridge,UK: Cambridge University Press.

Pergugini, M. & Bagozzi, R. (2001). The role

desires and anticipated emotions in goadirected behaviors: broadening and deepeninthe theory ofplanned behavior. British Journa

of Social Psychology, 40, 79-98.

Pergugini, M., & Conner, M. (2000). Predicting a

understanding behavioral intentions: th

interplay between goals and behaviors

European Journal of Social Psychology, 30705-731.

Singh, J. (1993). Boundary role ambiguity: Facetdeterminants, and impacts. Journal oMarketing, 57, 11-31.

Steers, R. M., & Braunstein, D. N. (1975).  

behaviorally based measure of manifest need

in work settings. Journal of Vocational

Behavior, 9, 251-266.

Vance, R. J., & Colella, A. (1990). Effects of tw

types of feedback on goal acceptance an

personal goals. Journal of Applied Psychology,75, 68-76.

VandeWalle, D., Cron,W. &

Slocum,J.

W(2001).The role of goal orientation followinperformance feedback. Journal of AppliedPsychology, 86, 629-640.

Wofford, J. C., Goodwin, V. L., & Premack, S

(1992). Meta-analysis of the antecedents o

personal goal level and of the antecedents an

consequences of goal commitment.Journal ofManagement,18,595-615.

Wood, R. E., & Bandura, A. (1989). Impact o

conceptions of ability on self-regulatorymechanisms and complex decisionmaking.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,56, 407-415.

Wood, R. E., Bandura,  A., & Bailey, T. (1990)Mechanisms governing organizationalprformance in complex decisionmakingenvironments, Organizational Behavior and

Human Decision Processes, 46, 181-202.

Wright, P. M., & Kacmar, K. M. (1994). Goa

specificity as a determinant of goalcommitment goal change. OrganizationalBehavior and Human Decision Processes, 59,242-260.

at University of Lincoln on April 11 2015jlo sagepub comDownloaded from