THE EFFECT OF ACTIVE MUSICAL TRAINING ON LEARNING...

75
PHD PROJECT Universitat Pompeu Fabra THE EFFECT OF ACTIVE MUSICAL TRAINING ON LEARNING THE PRONUNCIATION OF A FOREIGN LANGUAGE BY SCHOOL CHILDREN FLORENCE BAILLS Project supervisor: Dr. PILAR PRIETO VIVES

Transcript of THE EFFECT OF ACTIVE MUSICAL TRAINING ON LEARNING...

PHD PROJECT

Universitat Pompeu Fabra

THE EFFECT OF ACTIVE MUSICAL

TRAINING ON LEARNING THE

PRONUNCIATION OF A FOREIGN

LANGUAGE BY SCHOOL CHILDREN

FLORENCE BAILLS

Project supervisor: Dr. PILAR PRIETO VIVES

ii

ABSTRACT

Previous research has reported that musical training leads to modification in brain

plasticity and enhanced speech abilities in a first language (see Besson, Chobert,

Astésano, & Marie, 2015, for a review), for instance, stronger phonological awareness

and reading abilities (Tierney & Kraus, 2013). Positive associations between musical

ability and second language pronunciation have been reported in children and adults

(see Milovanov & Tervaniemi, 2011 for a review). Moreover, in different speech-impaired

populations, rhythmic entrainment has shown beneficial effects on speech production

(François, Grau-Sánchez, Duarte, & Rodriguez-Fornells, 2015). However, little is known

about the potential effects of including musical elements to language instruction on the

pronunciation of a second language. The present thesis proposes three experimental

studies and a correlational study with second graders (7-8 year-olds) to explore the

effects of active musical training on second language pronunciation.

The first study (running) is a short between-subject training with a pretest and posttest

design to test the effects of hand-clapping the rhythmic structure of words on L2

pronunciation, using an elicited imitation task. Theoretically, it relies on the similarities

between musical and speech rhythm and it follows up on recent results showing that

using rhythmic primes matched to the prosodic features of speech enhances the

phonological processing of spoken words (Nia Cason, Astésano, & Schön, 2015). Thirty-

five 7-8 year-old children from Girona, Catalonia, were trained to learn and repeat 20

words whose meanings were transparent for Catalan speakers, in one of two conditions,

namely with or without hand-clapping. The pronunciation of those targets will be rated

for accentedness before and after training by 2 French native speakers and mean

pronunciation improvement will be compared across conditions.

Following up on findings that singers outperform instrumentalists in foreign speech

imitation (Christiner & Reiterer, 2015), a second experiment with a similar design as the

first study will test the gains of learning words and simple sentences in a novel second

language by listening to songs in this language. Second-graders will be tested when

learning short songs with well-known melodies such as “Happy Birthday”, and their

pronunciation on related and unrelated speech fragments will be tested.

The third study of this thesis will consist of a classroom intervention study which will

assess the effects of one-month classroom training using productive and receptive

musical activities and their effects on pronunciation in a second language. Three second

grade class groups (approximately fifty 7-year-olds) will participate in 8 training sessions

iii

in one of the three conditions, namely a non-musical condition (e.g. learning target words

and simple sentences in French with games), an integrated musical condition (e.g.

learning words and simple sentences with musical activities integrated to the games) or

a consecutive musical condition (e.g. learning words and simple sentences with games

after a musical session). The pronunciation of French words and simple sentences will

again be rated before and after training by native speakers of French.

For each of the three studies, a set of linguistic and cognitive measures will be taken for

each participant. A last study will consist of a correlational analysis of the individual data

collected in the experiments and assess whether speech skills in a second language

(phonological perception, foreign speech imitation) are correlated with musical skills

(musical experience, musical aptitude, musical production), linguistic skills (narrative

task) and cognitive skills (working memory).

iv

RESUM

Algunes investigacions prèvies han demostrat que la formació musical modifica la

plasticitat del cervell i provoca beneficis en la parla de la primera llengua (vegeu Besson,

Chobert, Astésano, & Marie, 2015, per a una revisió), com, per exemple, en la

consciència fonològica i en les habilitats de lectura (Tierney & Kraus, 2013). A més,

s'han mostrat correlacions positives entre les habilitats musicals i la pronúncia d’una

segona llengua tant en nens com en adults (vegeu Milovanov & Tervaniemi, 2011 per

a una revisió). D'altra banda, també s’ha observat que els entrenaments rítmics amb

diferents poblacions amb impediments de la parla tenen efectes positius en les habilitats

lingüístiques dels subjectes (François, Grau-Sánchez, Duarte, & Rodríguez-Fornells,

2015). No obstant això, en l’àmbit de l’aprenentatge de la pronúncia, poc se sap dels

efectes potencials dels elements musicals inclosos en l'ensenyament d'una segona

llengua. Per explorar aquesta qüestió, a la present tesi es proposen tres estudis

experimentals i un quart estudi correlacional amb alumnes de segon de primària (7-8

anys d'edat).

El primer estudi (actualment en curs) consisteix en un entrenament curt amb un disseny

pre- i post-test que pretén estudiar els efectes del marcatge de l'estructura rítmica de les

paraules. Aquest estudi es fonamenta en les similituds entre els ritmes musicals i el ritme

de la parla i està basat en un experiment que ha demostrat que els marcadors rítmics

coherents amb les característiques prosòdiques de paraules milloren el processament

fonològic d’aquestes paraules (Nia Cason et al., 2015). Trenta-cinc nens d’una escola

de Girona van aprendre 20 paraules en francès. Sempre imitant l’instructor, en una

condició repetien les paraules a la vegada que picaven de mans, i en l’altre repetien les

paraules sense picar de mans. Està previst avaluar la pronúncia dels participants abans

i després de l’entrenament per tal de valorar si s’ha produït una millora en funció de les

condicions.

El segon experiment de la tesi tindrà un disseny similar al precedent i estudiarà els

efectes de les cançons en l’aprenentatge de la pronúncia de paraules i frases senzilles

en una segona llengua. Els alumnes de segon de primària aprendran la lletra de cançons

infantils (com “Moltes felicitats”) en dues condicions : (1) només repetint el text en

francès, i (2) repetint el text amb la melodia (és a dir, cantant).

El tercer experiment d'aquesta tesi consistirà en un estudi d'intervenció a l'aula que

avaluarà els efectes d’activitats musicals productives i receptives sobre la pronúncia

d’una segona llengua. Tres grups de classe de segon de primària (aproximadament

v

cinquanta nens) participaran en 8 sessions d'entrenament en una de les tres condicions

següents: (1) condició “no musical” (per exemple, aprendre paraules i frases senzilles

en francès amb jocs); (2) condició musical integrada (per exemple, l'aprenentatge de

paraules i frases senzilles amb activitats musicals integrades als jocs); o (3) condició

musical prèvia a l’exposició de la llengua (per exemple, l'aprenentatge de paraules i

frases senzilles amb jocs després d'una sessió musical). La pronúncia serà avaluada

abans i després de l'entrenament per parlants nadius de francès.

Dels tres primers estudis es recolliran tota una sèrie de mesures lingüístiques i

cognitives per cada participant, les quals seran emprades per realitzar un últim estudi,

que consistirà en un anàlisis de correlació d’aquestes dades individuals recollides en els

experiments, és a dir, entre les habilitats de la parla en una segona llengua (percepció

fonològica, imitació en llengües desconegudes), les habilitats musicals (experiència

musical, aptitud musical, producció musical), les habilitats lingüístiques (tasca narrativa)

i les habilitats cognitives (memòria de treball).

vi

RESUMEN

Algunas investigaciones previas han demostrado que la formación musical modifica la

plasticidad del cerebro y provoca beneficios en el habla de la primera lengua (véase

Besson, Chobert, Astesano, & Marie, 2015, para una revisión), como, por ejemplo, en la

conciencia fonológica y en las habilidades de lectura (Tierney & Kraus, 2013). Además,

se han mostrado correlaciones positivas entre las habilidades musicales y la

pronunciación de una segunda lengua tanto en niños, como en adultos (véase Milovanov

& Tervaniemi, 2011 para una revisión). Por otra parte, también se ha observado que los

entrenamientos rítmicos con diferentes poblaciones con impedimentos del habla tienen

efectos positivos en las habilidades lingüísticas de los sujetos (François, Grado-

Sánchez, Duarte, & Rodríguez-Fornells, 2015). Sin embargo, en el ámbito del

aprendizaje de la pronunciación, poco se sabe de los efectos potenciales de los

elementos musicales incluidos en la enseñanza de una segunda lengua. Para explorar

esta cuestión, en la presente tesis se proponen tres estudios experimentales y un cuarto

estudio correlacional con alumnos de segundo de primaria (7-8 años de edad).

El primer estudio (actualmente en curso) consiste en un entrenamiento corto con un

diseño pre- y post-test que pretende estudiar los efectos del marcaje de la estructura

rítmica de las palabras. Este estudio se fundamenta en las similitudes entre los ritmos

musicales y el ritmo del habla y está basado en un experimento que ha demostrado que

los marcadores rítmicos coherentes con las características prosódicas de palabras

mejoran el procesamiento fonológico de estas palabras (Cason, Astesano, & Schön,

2015). Treinta y cinco niños de una escuela de Girona aprendieron 20 palabras en

francés. Siempre imitando el instructor, en una condición repetían las palabras a la vez

que daban palmas, y en la otra repetían las palabras sin más. Está previsto evaluar la

pronunciación de los participantes antes y después del entrenamiento para valorar si se

ha producido una mejora en función de las condiciones.

El segundo experimento de la tesis tendrá un diseño similar al precedente y estudiará

los efectos de las canciones en el aprendizaje de la pronunciación de palabras y frases

sencillas en una segunda lengua. Los alumnos de segundo de primaria aprenderán la

letra de canciones infantiles (como "Cumpleaños feliz") en dos condiciones: (1) solo

repitiendo el texto en francés, y (2) repitiendo el texto con la melodía (es decir,

cantando).

El tercer experimento de esta tesis consistirá en un estudio de intervención en el aula

que evaluará los efectos de actividades musicales productivas y receptivas sobre la

vii

pronunciación de una segunda lengua. Tres grupos de clase de segundo de primaria

(aproximadamente cincuenta niños) participarán en 8 sesiones de entrenamiento en una

de las tres condiciones siguientes: (1) condición "no musical" (por ejemplo, aprender

palabras y frases sencillas en francés con juegos); (2) condición musical integrada (por

ejemplo, el aprendizaje de palabras y frases sencillas con actividades musicales

integradas en los juegos); o (3) condición musical previa a la exposición de la lengua

(por ejemplo, el aprendizaje de palabras y frases sencillas con juegos después de una

sesión musical). La pronunciación será evaluada antes y después del entrenamiento

para hablantes nativos de francés.

De los tres primeros estudios se recogerán toda una serie de medidas lingüísticas y

cognitivas por cada participante, las cuales serán utilizadas para realizar un último

estudio, que consistirá en un análisis de correlación de estos datos individuales

recogidos en los experimentos, es decir, entre las habilidades del habla en una segunda

lengua (percepción fonológica, imitación en lenguas desconocidas), las habilidades

musicales (experiencia musical, aptitud musical, producción musical), las habilidades

lingüísticas (tarea narrativa) y las habilidades cognitivas (memoria de trabajo).

viii

TABLE OF CONTENTS

ABSTRACT ......................................................................................................................................... II

RESUM ............................................................................................................................................... IV

RESUMEN ......................................................................................................................................... VI

TABLE OF CONTENTS ................................................................................................................. VIII

1. INTRODUCTION OF THE RESEARCH TOPIC ....................................................................... 1

1.1 OBJECT OF ANALYSIS ................................................................................................................. 1 1.2 STATE OF THE ART ...................................................................................................................... 1

1.2.1 Music, cognition, and language............................................................................................. 1 1.2.1.1 The effects of musical expertise .............................................................................................. 2

1.2.1.1.1 Effects of musical expertise on sound perception ........................................................... 2 1.2.1.1.2 Effects of musical expertise on speech perception ......................................................... 3 1.2.1.1.3 Effects of musical expertise and phonological skills in a second language ................ 4

1.2.1.2 The effects of musical aptitude ...................................................................................................... 5 1.2.1.3 The effects of musical training ....................................................................................................... 6

1.2.2 The acquisition of L2 pronunciation ...................................................................................... 8 1.2.2.1 The role of suprasegmental features in learner’s comprehensibility, intelligibility and

accentedness ................................................................................................................................................ 9 1.2.2.2 Teaching L2 suprasegmental features ......................................................................................... 9

1.2.3 Hypotheses of the studies: Enhancing L2 pronunciation ................................................ 11 1.2.3.1 Rhythm ............................................................................................................................................ 11 1.2.3.2 Songs .............................................................................................................................................. 12

1.3 OBJECTIVES .............................................................................................................................. 14 1.4 THEORETICAL FRAMEWORK(S) ................................................................................................. 15

1.4.1 Music and language: two faces of the same coin? .......................................................... 15 1.4.2 Cross-domain auditory plasticity ......................................................................................... 16

1.4.2.1 Musical rhythm and speech rhythm ............................................................................................ 17 1.4.2.2 Songs and cross-domain plasticity .............................................................................................. 18

1.4.3 Embodied cognition .............................................................................................................. 18 1.5 HYPOTHESES, ASSUMPTIONS, OR BACKGROUND IDEAS ................................................................. 20

1.5.1 Phonological differences between Catalan and French .................................................. 20 1.5.2 Musical activities in the second language classroom as part of pronunciation training

........................................................................................................................................................... 20 1.5.2.1 Hand-clapping as the body externalization of word prosody ................................................... 21 1.5.2.2 Singing as a facilitator to memorize and produce foreign words and sentences.................. 21

2. RESEARCH METHODOLOGY .................................................................................................... 22

2.1 PARTICIPANTS ................................................................................................................................. 22 2.2 CONTROL TESTS .............................................................................................................................. 22

2.2.1 Memory span test .................................................................................................................. 23 2.2.2 Narrative ability: story retelling task .................................................................................... 23 2.2.3. Speech imitation task .......................................................................................................... 24 2.2.4 Perceptual discrimination task ............................................................................................ 24 2.2.5 Musical expertise .................................................................................................................. 25 2.2.6 Musical aptitude .................................................................................................................... 25

2.2.6.1 Evaluation of musical perception ................................................................................................. 25 2.2.6.2 Evaluation of musical production ................................................................................................. 26

ix

2.3 STUDY 1 ........................................................................................................................................... 26 2.3.1 Research question ................................................................................................................ 26 2.3.2 Methodology .......................................................................................................................... 26

2.3.1.1. Participants .................................................................................................................................... 26 2.3.1.2. Materials ........................................................................................................................................ 27

2.3.1.2.1 Selection of French words .................................................................................................... 27 2.3.1.2.2 Audiovisual recordings of the training materials ................................................................ 28 2.3.1.2.3 Audio material for the pre and post-tests ........................................................................... 29

2.3.1.3. Procedure ...................................................................................................................................... 29 2.3.1.3.1 Procedure for the pre- and post-test ................................................................................... 29 2.3.1.3.2 Procedure for the training phase ......................................................................................... 30

2.3.1.4 Pronunciation ratings of the pre-test and post-test items ......................................................... 31 2.4 STUDY 2 ........................................................................................................................................... 31

2.4.1 Research Question ............................................................................................................... 32 2.4.2 Methodology .......................................................................................................................... 32 2.4.3 Materials ................................................................................................................................. 32

2.5 STUDY 3 ........................................................................................................................................... 33 2.5.1 Research Question ............................................................................................................... 33 2.5.2 Methodology .......................................................................................................................... 33 2.5.3. Materials ................................................................................................................................ 34

2.5.3.1Training sessions ............................................................................................................................ 34 2.5.3.2 Pre and posttest items .................................................................................................................. 34

2.6 STUDY 4 ........................................................................................................................................... 35 2.6.1 Research Questions ............................................................................................................. 36 2.6.2 Methodology .......................................................................................................................... 36 2.6.3 Tools and resources ............................................................................................................. 36

3. WORKING SCHEDULE ............................................................................................................... 38

4. SELECTED REFERENCES ......................................................................................................... 40

5. GENERAL REFERENCES ........................................................................................................... 44

5. APPENDIX ..................................................................................................................................... 60

APPENDIX A – LANGUAGE AND MUSICAL EXPERIENCE QUESTIONNAIRE .............................................. 60 APPENDIX B – LISTS OF CATALAN WORDS FOR THE MEMORY SPAN TEST ........................................... 61 APPENDIX C – NARRATIVE SCORING ..................................................................................................... 62 APPENDIX D – PHONOLOGICAL DISCRIMINATION TEST FOR FRENCH PHONEMES ................................ 63 APPENDIX E – WORDS SELECTED FOR THE TRAINING, THE PRE-TEST AND THE POST-TEST OF STUDY

1 ............................................................................................................................................................. 64 APPENDIX F – TRAINING PROCEDURE OF THE FIRST SESSION, STUDY 3 ............................................. 65

1

1. INTRODUCTION OF THE RESEARCH TOPIC

In the present section, we explain the object of this PhD project and a brief overview of

prior work in two complementary fields, e.g., first in the field of musical expertise, aptitude

and training and how they relate to speech and language learning, and second in the

field of second language acquisition of pronunciation.

1.1 Object of analysis

The use of musical activities in the second language classroom1 has been promoted for

some years now. Language teachers frequently integrate songs in their lesson plan.

Directed to children or to adults, listening to songs and singing are associated with a

feeling of joy and are considered to amplify the motivation for learning (Abbott, 2002).

Typically, musical activities in the classroom are used to introduce new vocabulary and

new syntactic forms, and some authors in the field of second language acquisition claim

that such activities increase learning retention and effectiveness (Fonseca Mora & Gant,

2016). However, more empirical evidence is needed in both laboratory and educational

settings about the benefits of musical training for second language learning, especially

in the domain of pronunciation acquisition, where little research has been carried out.

Since musicians have been proven to perform better than non-musicians in many

aspects of speech perception (see Besson, Chobert, Astésano, & Marie, 2015, for a

review) we believe that there are reasons to think that musical expertise and musical

training may positively influence the acquisition of second language pronunciation

abilities.

1.2 State of the art

1.2.1 Music, cognition, and language

A whole line of investigation has explored the role of music on different cognitive

1 In this thesis plan, the term ‘second language learning’ is used as a cover term that refers to the process of learning another language after the native or dominant one. This is a common strategy in the field, which also uses this term to refer to the learning of a third or a fourth language (Gass & Selinker, 2001).

2

functions and has shown that musical experience has enhancing effects on a variety of

cognitive functions (see Benz, Sellaro, Hommel, & Colzato, 2016, for a review).

Improvements have been found on executive control (Bialystok & DePape, 2009),

auditory and visual working memory (George & Coch, 2011; D. L. Strait, Parbery-Clark,

Hittner, & Kraus, 2012), verbal memory (Brandler & Rammsayer, 2003; Chan, Ho, &

Cheung, 1998; Franklin et al., 2008), auditory and visual attention (Roden, Grube,

Bongard, & Kreutz, 2014; Rodrigues et al., 2013; D. L. Strait et al., 2012), processing

speed in both visual and auditory information (Bugos & Mostafa, 2011; Roden et al.,

2014), reasoning ability (Bergman Nutley, Darki, & Klingberg, 2014; Bilhartz, Bruhn, &

Olson, 1999; Forgeard et al., 2008), as well as on higher IQ measures (Schellenberg,

2006; Schellenberg, Burnham, Peretz, & Pliner, 1999) and creativity in general

(Woodward & Sikes, 2015).

There is also growing evidence about the tight relationship between music and language

(see section 1.4.1), notably in relation to language prosody (Heffner & Slevc, 2015). A

large range of studies have suggested that music perception and language perception

processes share structural and auditory mechanisms and can influence each other (see

Asaridou & McQueen, 2013; and Jäncke, 2012, for a review). One of the strongest

theories related to music and language has been proposed by Patel (2011, 2012, 2014)

who hypothesized that the brain plasticity induced by music affects speech abilities

under a series of conditions (see section 1.4.1.2 for further details).

In the following sections we will review the work on the how a set of linguistic abilities are

influenced by individual factors such as musical expertise (generally understood as the

expertise in regularly playing an instrument or singing for more than two years; see

Chobert & Besson, 2013), musical aptitude (e.g., innate musical talent which is

independent of playing an instrument or singing; see Milovanov, Huotilainen, Välimäki,

Esquef, & Tervaniemi, 2008), and musical training (e.g., relatively short trainings up to

two years of duration; see Chobert, François, Velay, & Besson, 2014).

1.2.1.1 The effects of musical expertise

1.2.1.1.1 Effects of musical expertise on sound perception

Many studies in the neuroscience of music have shown that musicians outperform non-

3

musicians in various aspects of sound and melodic perception. Musicians have been

shown to have more fine-grained perceptual abilities for pitch discrimination of pure and

harmonic tones (e.g. Bidelman, Krishnan, & Gandour, 2010; Kishon-Rabin, Amir, Vexler,

& Zaltz, 2001; Micheyl, Delhommeau, Perrot, & Oxenham, 2006; Spiegel & Watson,

1984, among others), as well as better accuracy scores in discrimination tasks focusing

on sound pitch and duration (Koelsch, Schröger, & Tervaniemi, 1999; Micheyl et al.,

2006; Tervaniemi et al., 2006). Musical expertise increases the ability to detect pitch

changes in melodic contours (e.g. Fujioka, Trainor, Ross, Kakigi, & Pantev, 2004;

Trainor, Desjardins, & Rockel, 1999) and the faster and better detection of subtle

variations of pitch, rhythm, and harmony within musical phrases (e.g. Besson & Faïta,

1995; Bidelman, Krishnan, & Gandour, 2010; Koelsch et al., 2002). At the same time,

studies using MRI, fMRI, or MEG methods have demonstrated that professional

musicians with years of intensive musical practice present modified brain anatomy

related to white matter organization and thicker gray matter in motor and auditory cortices

(Bengtsson et al., 2005; Imfeld, Oechslin, Meyer, Loenneker, & Jancke, 2009;

Schmithorst, Vincent J. Wilke, 2002).

1.2.1.1.2 Effects of musical expertise on speech perception

Research on the effect of musical expertise on language features is based on the overlap

of functional brain processing between music and speech, on the similarities between

their acoustic features (see 1.3.1 and 1.3.2). In a variety of experiments, Besson and

colleagues (Besson, Schön, Moreno, Santos, & Magne, 2007; Magne, Schön, & Besson,

2006; Marques, Moreno, Castro, & Besson, 2007; Moreno & Besson, 2006; Schön,

Magne, & Besson, 2004) demonstrated the musicians’ increased sensitivity to sound

perception. For example, in two studies, they manipulated the pitch of sentence-final

words to different degrees (subtle or easy to detect) and measured pitch variation

detection in the native language of the listener (Schön et al., 2004) and in an unknown

language (Marques et al., 2007). In both studies, results showed that musicians

outperformed non-musicians only when pitch variations that were difficult to detect. The

ERP analysis confirmed these results by showing increased activity of the P3 component

during subtle pitch variations only in musicians.

A handful of studies have used Mandarin Chinese to show that musicians detect tone

and segmental variations better than non-musicians (Delogu, Lampis, & Belardinelli,

4

2010; Gottfried, Staby, & Ziemer, 2004; Lee & Hung, 2008; Marie, Delogu, Lampis,

Berlardinelli, & Besson, 2011). In Marie et al. (2011), twenty-four French participants (12

musicians and 12 non musicians) listened to two sequences of four monosyllabic

Mandarin words that were either the same or different. When different, one word in the

sequence varied in tone (e.g., fēi vs. féi) or in segmental cues (e.g., dié vs. tié). Analysis

of the ERPs showed a larger latency of the N2/N3 component when exposed to tone

variations and shorter amplitude and latency of the P3b component when exposed to

tone and segmental variations only in musicians.

The influence of musical expertise on vowel duration and metrical processing in natural

speech has also been investigated (e.g., Magne et al., 2007; Marie, Kujala, & Besson,

2011). In these two studies, the authors created an unexpected lengthening of the

penultimate syllable that disrupted the metrical structure of words without modifying

timber or frequency. Sentence-final words were also semantically congruous or

incongruous within the context. Participants performed two tasks. In a metric task, they

were asked to decide whether the sentence was correctly pronounced or not. In the

semantic task, they had to decide whether the final word was expected within the context

or not. In both tasks, musicians outperformed non-musicians (as measured by the

percentage of errors). As expected, the ERP data differed between tasks: the P2

component (perceptual processing) was larger in musicians than in non-musicians. By

contrast, the N400 effect (semantic processing) was not different in both groups.

Additionally, François and Schön (2011) used training in a sung artificial language to

study speech segmentation abilities. Sixteen professional musicians and twenty non-

musicians were asked to listen carefully to a stream of sung syllables for several minutes.

Consecutively, participants had to choose which of the two presented items was present

in the stream. In a first, linguistic test, items had a flat contour (‘‘spoken’’ version) while

in a second, musical test, they were accompanied with a piano sound. While behavioral

measures didn’t show any significant difference between musicians and non-musicians,

electrophysiological data revealed that French musicians had larger amplitude at the

N400-like component during the tests, which suggest that musicians have a more

“robust” representation of both musical and linguistic structures.

1.2.1.1.3 Effects of musical expertise and phonological skills in a second

language

5

Studies have shown that musical expertise improve children’s skills for second language

listening comprehension and vocabulary (Swaminathan & Gopinath, 2013; Yang, Ma,

Gong, Hu, & Yao, 2014), vocabulary learning (Coyle & Gómez Gracia, 2014) and reading

(Fonseca-Mora, Jara-Jiménez, & Gómez-Domínguez, 2015; Lowe, 1997). Interestingly,

Swaminathan and Gopinath (2013) undertook a study with 76 primary-school children

who were native speakers of various Indian languages and found that those who were

learning either Western or Indian music performed better in English L2 comprehension

and vocabulary than non-musicians, therefore showing that the type of musicianship did

not influence the results.

If musical expertise seems to facilitate perceptive phonological skills, less is known about

its potential effects on productive skills in a second language. Gottfried et al. (2004)

tested 43 American English participants with or without musical expertise at imitating

Mandarin Chinese syllables with the four tones. Results showed that participants with

musical expertise were rated significantly better that non-musicians. Christiner and

Reiterer (2015) investigated the relationship between singing/instrument performers and

speech imitation skills. Their results showed that professional singers and

instrumentalists outperformed non-singers and non-instrumentalists in the speech

imitation of foreign languages. Additionally, they found that singers performed

significantly better than instrumentalists. They propose that vocal motor training such as

the kind of training singers undergo may induce vocal flexibility and facilitate speech

imitation abilities.

The abovementioned studies suggest that a clear transfer of abilities from music-to

language related elements and specifically speech-related elements is taking place. This

constitutes the main motivation for some of the studies presented for this PhD thesis. In

parallel, other studies have focused on the effect of musical aptitude on language

acquisition, which we will review in the following section.

1.2.1.2 The effects of musical aptitude

Musical aptitude2 and its effect on language learning has been much less studied than

2 Musical aptitude is generally assessed by taking a battery of tests measuring the ability of the participant in a series of musical domains, most frequently rhythm, pitch, and melody. In the literature mentioned in this research plan, authors have generally been using the Seashore Measures of Musical Talent (Seashore, Lewis, & Saetveit, 1960) or the Advanced Measure of Musical Audition (Gordon, 1989).

6

musical expertise. Nevertheless, this section presents a handful of studies that have

shown a positive relationship between musical aptitude and a variety of linguistic skills.

Anvari, Trainor, Woodside, & Levy, (2002) found a significant relationship between music

perception skills and reading skills when testing 100 children between 4 and 5 years of

age, while controlling for phonological awareness and other cognitive abilities. In a study

by Posedel, Emery, Souza, and Fountain (2012), pitch perception (and not musical

expertise), was the only significant predictor of Spanish pronunciation quality. However,

this study did not control for intelligence and L2 vocabulary knowledge. Moreover, they

considered the length of formal Spanish learning as a single measure of L2 competency,

without taking into account any other individual difference that could influence

pronunciation. Milovanov et al. (2008) investigated the relationship between musical

aptitude and second language pronunciation skills. They assessed a set of forty 10 to

12-year-old Finnish children in their English pronunciation skills. Children with better

performance in the language tests were those who possessed higher musical abilities.

Along these lines, Milovanov, Pietilä, Tervaniemi, & Esquef, (2010) examined the

pronunciation of English as a second language by 3 groups of 46 Finnish adults, divided

into three groups, namely, non-musician, non-English speaking participants with a

musical aptitude score of 38%, non-musician English philology students with a musical

aptitude score of 69%, and non-English-speaking choir singers with a musical aptitude

score of 78%. While all the participants performed equally in a phonemic discrimination

task, those with higher music aptitude obtained higher scores in ratings of a set of difficult

phonemes.

Musical aptitude has been regarded as an individual difference in L2 learning mainly due

to its impact on phonological skills, but in light of a growing body of evidence from

neurolinguistics studies, further investigations have tried to pinpoint the transfer of

competence between music and language through training and intervention

methodologies.

1.2.1.3 The effects of musical training

Experiments including musical training periods are varied in terms of the object of study

(e.g., speech segmentation, phonological skills, memory skills, reading skills etc.)

training length (from short priming treatments to two years of implementation), and also

in terms of the training materials used in the study (rhythm training, melodic training,

7

etc.). In this section we will review a handful of the effects of musical training on a variety

of language skills.

Bhide, Power and Goswami (2013) compared 2 months of rhythm-based training against

more classical phoneme-grapheme training with nineteen children between 6 and 7

years of age considered to be poor readers. They found that both interventions had

positive effects on the standardized tests for auditory, phonological and literacy skills.

François, Chobert, Besson and Schön (2013) carried out a 2-year longitudinal study with

a set of thirty-seven 8-year-old French children to examine the role of musical training

on speech segmentation. Children were assigned randomly either to music training or

painting training groups. The authors tested the children three times (before training,

after one year and after two years) at a speech segmentation task and found that children

in the music training group outperformed children in the painting training group during

post-tests.

Moreno et al. (2009) conducted a longitudinal study with thirty-two 8-year-old Portuguese

children over 9 months. The training lasted 6 months and consisted of either musical

activities or painting activities. Musical training was based on a combination of Kodály3,

Orff4 (Goodkin, 2004), and Wuytack (Wuytack & Palheiros, 1995) methodologies, and

included training on rhythm, melody, harmony, and timbre. Before and after training,

event-related brain potentials were recorded during reading and pitch processing tasks.

Only after musical training, children showed enhanced reading abilities and subtle pitch

discrimination abilities in speech, as detected by the ERP.

In an electrophysiological (EEG) experiment, Cason & Schön (2012) found evidence that

musical rhythmic priming enhanced the phonological processing of bi- or trisyllabic

pseudo-words with a final stressed syllable when the speech metrical structure matched

the prime meter. In a follow-up study with thirty-four French adults, Cason et al. (2015)

aimed to investigate if this effect could also be found with real sentences in French.

Participants heard a musical rhythmic prime followed by a sentence either with a

matching or mismatching prosodic structure and immediately performed a phoneme

detection task. To check for the possible effect of active production, half the participants

had received audio–motor training with the musical rhythms presented in the

experiment5. Analysis of reaction times showed that phoneme detection was faster with

3 http://www. kodaly.org 4 See also : http://www.aosa.org 5 The audio-motor training is only vaguely described in the article: “[…] the ‘audio-motor’ group (AM group) underwent an additional period of audio-motor training with the rhythmic primes.” (p.45)

8

a matching metrical prime. In addition, authors compared results between the group that

benefited the audio-motor training with the prime against results of the group that only

heard them and found that the priming effect was enhanced in the first group.

In a production experiment, Ludke, Ferreira and Overy (2014) found that a singing

learning method can facilitate short-term memory for phrases in an unknown language

(Hungarian) after a short learning period. Sixty adult participants were randomly

assigned to one of three "listen-and-repeat" learning conditions: speaking, rhythmic

speaking, or singing. Participants in the singing condition showed significantly superior

overall performance on recalling and producing sentences after a 15-min learning period,

as compared with participants in the speaking and rhythmic speaking conditions. Other

possible interfering factors like age, gender, mood6, phonological working memory

ability, or musical ability7 and expertise were controlled for.

In a longitudinal study, Herrera, Lorenzo, Defior, Fernández-Smith, & Costa-Giomi

(2011) tested whether phonological and music training can improve reading readiness in

42 native speakers of Spanish and 52 Tamazight learners of Spanish. They examined

pre-school children during 2 years of either phonological training with music (based on

children’s rhymes and songs) or without music. During post-testing, the first group

significantly outperformed the second group in naming speed and phonological

awareness, which are two strong predictors of reading readiness. Tamazight children

who received training with music developed naming speed skills and phonological

awareness of word endings in Spanish more rapidly than Spanish children in the control

group.

All in all, these studies show evidence that music training may facilitate phonological

awareness and verbal comprehension skills in a second language. Still, very little work

has been done on the potential beneficial effects of music training on L2 pronunciation

abilities.

1.2.2 The acquisition of L2 pronunciation

In this second subsection, we broadly review the literature on the important role of

6 Participants answered a 20-item self-report Positive and Negative Affect Scale (PANAS) mood questionnaire (Watson, Clark, & Tellegen, 1988) at the beginning and end of the experimental session. 7 Participants were administered a brief musical ability test (adapted from the musical ability tests developed by Overy, Nicolson, Fawcett, & Clarke (2003).

9

suprasegmental features (rhythm, durational patterns, and pitch patterns) in the

perception of accentedness in a foreign accent. We finally suggest that training with

musical activities could improve those suprasegmental features, as well as general

pronunciation abilities in a second language.

1.2.2.1 The role of suprasegmental features in learner’s comprehensibility,

intelligibility and accentedness

In the classroom, instruction of pronunciation is typically centered around segmental

aspects of speech, that is, on the pronunciation of individual phonemes (see Derwing &

Munro, 2015 for a review). However, when respectively comparing the potential effects

of segmental and suprasegmental deviances on oral language competence in a L2,

suprasegmental deviances tend to influence accentedness, comprehensibility and

intelligibility ratings more than segmental deviances (e.g. Anderson-Hsieh, Johnson, &

Koehler, 1992; Edmunds, 2010; Field, 2005). Specifically, Kang (2010) showed that

suprasegmental features explain a good amount of the variability in accentedness ratings

of foreign speech by native speakers: in his experiment, 41% of the variance in

accentedness ratings was due to pitch range, word stress, and mean length of pauses

while 35% of the variance in comprehensibility ratings was due to speech rate alone.

Along this line, White and Mattys (2007) found that rhythm significantly correlated with

native speakers’ ratings of foreign language in L2 speech.

1.2.2.2 Teaching L2 suprasegmental features

A series of classroom studies in second language acquisition has demonstrated that

teaching the suprasegmental components of speech is important to improving overall

fluency and comprehensibility of language learners. A seminal study by Derwin, Munro

and Wiebe (1998) showed that global pronunciation instruction in the ESL classroom

addressing elements such as speaking rate, intonation, rhythm, and stress at the word

and sentence level during 11 weeks allowed learners to improve significantly in

spontaneous speech, whereas learners who took only segmental or no pronunciation-

specific instruction did not. In a follow-up experiment with a similar design, Derwin and

Rossiter (2003) found that improvement in the global instruction group improved

learner’s comprehensibility and fluency, but not their accentedness. Gordon, Darcy and

10

Ewert (2013) also found that overall comprehensibility improved only for learners in the

suprasegmental group (whose instruction was focused on stress, rhythm, linking and

reductions) after 3 weeks’ training.

These intervention studies provide strong evidence for the important role of

suprasegmental instruction in the L2 classroom for improving learner’s pronunciation

abilities. However, teachers encounter incertitude when the time comes to decide

teaching content, when to teach pronunciation and the method that can be used (see

Foote, Holtby, & Derwing, 2011), despite researchers’ advocating to create a curriculum

dedicated to pronunciation for ESL classes (Darcy, Ewert, & Lidster, 2012). For example,

there is no agreement in the field about how to implement rhythm instruction. A couple

of methods used in the ESL classroom are Graham’s (1978) Jazz chants or Nakata

(2002)’s BossaNova KenMC, where learners are asked to finger-tap out the beats of

short, poem-like structures in order to notice the target rhythm and reproduce it. Still,

there isn’t any assessment showing the improvements based on this method. Even less

has been done to test experimentally concrete aspects of pronunciation instruction, i.e.

intonation or rhythm. Recently, Gluhareva, and Prieto (in press) have proposed that beat

gestures, that is, hand gestures that mark the rhythm of speech, are an effective aid for

L2 acquisition of pronunciation. In a brief within-subjects training study, participants were

asked to watch an English instructor producing a set of target sentences in English within

a discourse situation either accompanied by rhythmic beat gestures or not. Twenty

Catalan participants improved their accentedness significantly on the most difficult

trained items only in the beat perception condition. A follow-up study (Kushch,

Gluhareva, & Prieto, in preparation) has been carried out which investigated whether

participants show higher gains in accent improvement if they are instructed to imitate the

experimenter and produce beat gestures themselves, rather than only observe them. In

the experiment 30 participants were randomly assigned to two groups. In the first group

participants watched the videos in which an instructor gave spontaneous responses to

English discourse prompts with accompanying beat gestures and had to orally repeat

the utterances after the instructor (no-gesture production group). In the second group,

participants watched the same videos and had to repeat the utterances, accompanying

them with beat gestures. The results of this experiment showed that producing beat

gestures had a significant beneficial effect in comparison to gesture observation alone.

While there is a growing body of evidence which shows that learning the prosodic

aspects of a second language can benefit from musical expertise and musical aptitude,

few studies have tried to demonstrate how musical training could enhance pronunciation

11

acquisition. Music has been suggested to appeal to learners’ emotions (Arnold, 1999),

to favor concentration and to stimulate learning in a positive and relaxed atmosphere in

the second language classroom (see Fonseca-Mora, Toscano-Fuentes, & Wermke,

2011). We review this work in the following section.

1.2.3 Hypotheses of the studies: Enhancing L2 pronunciation

In this section, we review a set of studies that are closely related to our project which

deal with rhythmic and singing training and pronunciation in a second language.

1.2.3.1 Rhythm

Training studies with musical rhythm applied to second language pronunciation

acquisition are scarce. To our knowledge, only two studies have been conducted. In a

qualitative study with a set of six advanced learners of English from various language

backgrounds, Fischler (2009) explored the effects of an intensive 4-week training on the

learning of English words and stress patterns through rap music and related activities.

The author reports improvement in stress placement. Unfortunately, without any control

group, it is not possible to ascertain that improvement was due to the musical training

itself.

In a recent book edited by Fonseca-Mora, Pacheco-Costa (2016) explained the

possibilities of the Orff-Schulwerk methodology to improve English pronunciation in

primary school children. The Orff-Schulwerk methodology aims at developing musical

abilities through rhythm, speech, and movement and it is now a widely accepted musical

education method for children (Goodkin, 2004). One part of the method highlights the

principles of musical rhythm and meter through language, in practice, the syllabic

structure of words is translated into musical notation and become rhythmic blocks that

can be combined. Pacheco-Costa suggests applying the Orff-Schulwerk method with

English in the second language classroom by creating such rhythmic blocks and

repeating them while at the same time as marking the beat with a musical instrument.

The next step would be to practice sequences of rhythmic blocks as sayings and rhymes

partnered with their rhythmic notation. The reading should be accompanied by body

percussion, a percussion instrument, and movement. The author also recommends

12

letting students create their own texts based on rhythmic patterns. Finally, she advocates

completing the practice by singing traditional songs and playing around with the lyrics for

these songs.

In the growing field of computer-aided language learning, Wang, Mok, & Meng, (2016)

proposed a very interesting tool based on musical rhythm to support prosody acquisition.

The authors developed a software that automatically generated musical rhythm for a

given text in English. Evaluation of the software showed that sentences produced after

training with the rhythmic cue were rated significantly higher by native English speakers

in terms of native-likeness than the same sentences produced without rhythm training.

Again, however, no control group was tested, and it is also difficult to see how to

implement this very promising tool in a classroom setting, let alone with children.

One of the first hypotheses of this PhD thesis is that rhythmic entrainment with the

prosodic structure of the target L2 language can produce beneficial effects in L2

pronunciation. Moreover, we have empirical evidence that rhythmic entrainment

programs show beneficial effects on speech production in different speech-impaired

populations (see François et al., 2015, for a review), for instance, in dyslexic children

(e.g. Overy, 2000, 2003) or deaf children (e.g. Cason, Hidalgo, Isoard, Roman, & Schön,

2015).

1.2.3.2 Songs

In the language classroom, songs have been used for many decades as a way to learn

new vocabulary and enhance student’s motivation. However, in a vast majority of

studies, the effects of instrumental expertise are examined, excluding singing. Songs

combine elements from speech (phonemes, syllables) and music (melodies and

rhythms). They possess a series of features which draw attention to the sound structure

of their speech elements, and thus could implicitly facilitate speech processing. For

example, speech in songs can sometimes be slower than in normal speech and ease

the comprehension of words and syllables. The repetition of some sequences together

with their rhythmic structure makes the content of the song more predictable and helps

process further details of the song, such as the sound structure of the lyrics. Still, very

few empirical studies have investigated whether songs may facilitate pronunciation

during second language learning.

Toscano-Fuentes and Fonseca (2012) implemented a classroom training with forty-nine

13

Spanish 6th-graders. They added musical activities to the classic ESL curriculum for the

academic year (9 months). Songs were used actively to teach listening comprehension,

vocabulary, grammar and focus on pronunciation whereas instrumental music was used

passively as ambient music. The authors claim that students with high auditory capacity

(measured by the Seashore test) had better results in English in general and in reading

and listening comprehension in particular. In this study, pronunciation abilities were

evaluated by teachers during regular speaking activities. Most importantly, the absence

of a control group does not allow us to assess if the improvements in the five skills under

scrutiny (speaking, reading, listening, writing and grammar) are due to a normal

development after nine month instruction or are increased thanks to the musical

activities, in other words, there isn’t any control group.

Tizian (2016) studied the pronunciation of English words before and after a training with

songs in English. Five Italian learners of English (intermediate/high level) in the

experimental group had to listen and sing along with the songs every two days during

three weeks. Their pronunciation of target words was compared to the production of the

control group’s five participants, who had to read the same target words and a brief

explanation on their pronunciation, with no acoustic input at all. The author comments

that slight improvements on the pronunciation of single words were observed in the

experimental group whereas no improvement were observed in the control group.

Additionally, results showed positive effects for both trainings for participants who had a

low-intermediate level of English and no improvement for participants with higher

proficiency. Importantly, the fact that the control group did not receive any acoustic input

means that the results of both groups are hardly comparable.

Nemoto, Wilson, and Perkins (2016) investigated whether English pronunciation by

Japanese speakers improved more after training with spoken or sung sentences. 30

participants trained their pronunciation individually during 10 minutes either by listening

to an English song and singing themselves or by listening to the lyrics of the song spoken

by a native speaker. Results showed that participants in the sung condition performed

significantly worse at post-test than participants in the spoken condition. The authors

suggest that the fact that participants in the experimental group only heard the song’s

melody, and not the English language melody, negatively influenced the production of

the sentences’ prosody at posttest. This could constitute a reasonable explanation, since

the participants did not hear the spoken sentence and therefore never had the

opportunity to listen to the prosody of the target sentences.

All in all, given the scarcity of studies on the topic, further experiments, both in the

14

laboratory and in a classroom setting, are still needed to assess the influence of songs

on pronunciation. The promising findings by Christiner and Reiterer (2015) on the

superior skills in language imitation by singers constitute potential evidence on the

benefits of singing for boosting second language pronunciation abilities.

1.3 Objectives

Based on extensive research on the commonalities between music and language (see

Theoretical framework, 1.3.1), authors in the field of second language acquisition have

shown interest in the possible benefits of musical activities in the foreign language

classroom. However, as shown in section 1.2.3, very little work has been carried out on

the potential positive effects of songs and rhythmic activities on L2 pronunciation

specifically.

The objectives of this thesis derive from the findings of the review of the literature outlined

above. The idea behind this thesis is that, given that musicians have clear advantages

for speech processing tasks, it is not unrealistic to think that that training with melodies

and rhythm should benefit all types of students. In general, this thesis has a triple aim,

separated in three experimental studies. First, to assess whether musical activities

related to rhythm and pitch (like hand-clapping or singing) can improve pronunciation in

a second language, both with two short training experiments and a longer intervention

experiment. A final second objective of the thesis is to explore the relationship between

children’s musical aptitudes/expertise and other cognitive and linguistic abilities and their

pronunciation in the second language. Finally, we would like to prepare an educational

interface to provide language teachers with experimentally tested teaching tools that can

be easily integrated in the second language classroom at the primary school level.

The PhD thesis will be comprised of three studies. In the first study, I would like to show

that a short rhythmic training highlighting the rhythmic structure of words in a second

language (French) can improve the pronunciation of these words. A second study will

assess the effects of a short training with songs on the acquisition of a second language

both at the word and sentence levels. The third intervention study will consist in testing

whether a long training with rhythmic and melodic activities in the classroom (8 sessions

during 4 weeks) where the children will learn basic French words and sentences.

Children will be assessed for the following individual skills, which will be assessed in the

final correlational study: speech skills in a second language (phonological perception,

15

foreign speech imitation) are correlated with musical skills (musical experience, musical

aptitude, and musical production), linguistic skills (narrative task) and cognitive skills

(working memory).

1.4 Theoretical framework(s)

1.4.1 Music and language: two faces of the same coin?

The commonalities and differences between language and music have been examined

from different perspectives. For example, the anthropologist Steven Mithen (2005)

argues that the human beings are musical in nature, under the hypothesis that a musical

and physical expressive protolanguage preceded modern human linguistic abilities.

Functions of music and language in society have been and still are fundamental,

especially during the first years of life (Trehub, 2003) although their functions seem to

differ. Put simply, we consider that the functions of language are typically communicative

- express our thoughts about the present, the past or the future; and music is a channel

for expressing emotions and feelings. Nevertheless, research on common structural

properties between music and syntax or prosody is growing (see Heffner & Slevc, 2015,

for a review). Neuroscientific studies have successfully proved that music and language

use common networks in the brain (see Jäncke, 2012, for a review). In a seminal study,

Besson and Schön (2001) conducted a series of ERPs studies and found that syntactic

violations in language and harmonic violations in music elicited similar effects at the

P600 component. Neuroimaging studies have shown that the brain regions that play a

crucial role in language (Broca and Wernicke’s areas) are also activated during music

processing (e.g. Abrams et al., 2010; Levitin & Menon, 2003; Maess, Koelsch, Gunter,

& Friederici, 2001; Vuust, Ostergaard, Pallesen, Bailey, & Roepstorff, 2009). Kotilahti et

al., (2010) also showed evidence that neural activity in infants overlapped for infant-

directed speech and instrumental music.

All these findings may not come as a surprise when considering the common tangible

properties between speech and music. Both are sequential auditory signals that unfold

in time, according to determined rules (syntax/harmony) and displaying the same

acoustic parameters (frequency, duration, intensity, and timber). Similar to music, the

central elements in speech prosody are rhythm (stress and timing) and melody

(intonation) (Nooteboom, 1997). Hence in the last decades the development of a wide

and dynamic field of research has started to successfully explore the benefit of musical

16

expertise, musical aptitude, and musical training on a variety of language-specific

abilities (see section 1.2.1 above).

1.4.2 Cross-domain auditory plasticity

The present thesis relies on the notion of cross-domain plasticity. This term refers to the

changes in neural processing in one domain driven by experience or training in another

domain, here music and speech. Strait and Kraus (2011) and Besson, Chobert, & Marie

(2011) provide exhaustive reviews of studies showing that musicians present enhanced

auditory attention and working memory and studies investigating the overlap in the brain

networks involved in music and speech for auditory attention and working memory. Patel

(2014) formulates the hypothesis that the demands placed on sensory and cognitive

processing by long musical practice provoke a change in plasticity in the brain networks

involved in speech processing. More precisely, certain conditions need to be fulfilled for

this change to take place. First, the transfer can only concern musical and speech

features (e.g. waveform periodicity, amplitude envelope) that are dealt in brain networks

shared by the two domains. Second, the practice of music augments in complexity so

that it places higher demands on the process in question than speech does. Third, the

musical activities that activate this process also induce positive emotion, focused

attention and repetition of the features.

Longitudinal experiments assessing the effect of musical training on non-musician adults

or children have shown that training has an effect and that the improved abilities are not

due to genetic predispositions for music (Hyde et al., 2009; Moreno et al., 2009).

Supporting this hypothesis, Musacchia, Strait, and Kraus (2008) reported positive

correlations between the number of years of musical practice and the strength of

subcortical pitch encoding as well as with the amplitude of ERPs cortical components.

If in each domain, rhythm can modulate attention time in such a way that processing

efficiency is influenced, and that this effect may well be possible across domains. For

example, a series of studies by Cason and collaborators (Cason et al., 2015; Cason et

al., 2015; Cason & Schön, 2012)’s priming studies showed that the phonological

processing of speech is enhanced by the temporal expectations generated by a musical

rhythmic prime. Another study giving support to the hypothesis that musical rhythm can

have a cross-domain impact on speech processing has been realized by Gordon,

Magne, & Large (2011), who demonstrated that temporal alignment between musical

17

meter in song and stressed syllables in the song lyrics facilitates their comprehension by

synchronizing neural activity with strong syllables.

Additionally, Boll-Avetisyan, Bhatara, and Hoele (2017) investigated the effect of musical

aptitudes on rhythmic grouping in speech and found that musical rhythm perception

abilities are associated with a more consistent grouping of speech.

1.4.2.1 Musical rhythm and speech rhythm

Musical rhythm and language rhythm represent the organization of acoustic events in a

time scale, with a regular succession of strong and weak elements and a regular

framework in which they appear. Beat, meter and tempo are the elements of rhythm.

Beat is the basic unit of time in music, often defined as the rhythm one instinctively

reproduces by tapping with a finger or foot while listening to music. When beats are

organized into recurring stress and unstressed patterns, a meter appears. Tempo

represents the speed of the beat (J. London, 2012). Infants can perceive meter and

classify musical rhythm from an early age (Hannon & Johnson, 2005). This preference

persists: Bolton (1894) showed that we automatically apply a metrical structure when

listening to an isosynchronous sequence of identical sounds, with the illusion of

perceiving a sequence of strong and weak elements.

In a similar way, salient and non-salient syllables in speech create the metrical

organization of utterances. Although speech is not as regular as music (Patel, 2008), it

is possible to find a certain degree of rhythmic regularity and predictability. Rhythmic

recurrences seem to be based on language-specific patterns, which are partially

dependent on syllable structure and vowel reduction characteristics (Dauer, 1983;

Roach, 1982). For example, some languages distinguish between different levels of

prosodic stress with different functions (e.g. lexical stress, phrasal stress, and emphatic

stress), as well as different degrees of stress (e.g. primary and secondary lexical stress;

and high, low, or complex pitch stress).

In both domains, rhythm allows a listener to form predictions about what will happen

next. For instance, upon hearing a ternary metrical sequence in music (123 123 123),

we expect that the same ‘123’ (weak–weak–strong) pattern will follow. A study by Pitt

and Samuel (1990) demonstrated that hearing a list of trisyllabic words with a final stress

induced expectations for a word with the same meter. As a result, auditory events

occurring at these predictable locations get more attention (Large & Jones, 1999; Justin

18

London, 2012; Snyder & Large, 2005) and are better processed (Ellis & Jones, 2010;

Jones, Boltz, & Kidd, 1982, among others).

1.4.2.2 Songs and cross-domain plasticity

In a review by Teixido, François, Bosch, and Männel (in press), the authors highlight that

songs are relevant for speech segmentation and word learning. Songs present

exaggerated language-specific stress patterns that may help infants acquire linguistic

traits of their target language (Brandt, Gebrian, & Slevc, 2012; Hannon, Lévêque, Nave,

& Trehub, 2016). Infants have been shown to present a clear preference for infant-

directed songs over adult-directed songs (Unyk, Trehub, Trainor, & Schellenberg, 1992).

Importantly, song melody seems to specifically benefit speech processing as well as

language acquisition (e.g. Lebedeva & Kuhl, 2010; D. Schön et al., 2008).

According to Patel (2014), any impact of singing-based training on speech abilities could

be partly or wholly due to within-domain plasticity, i.e., to the effects of the linguistic

components of song. Rate, repetition, rhythm, and rhyme act to highlight the sound

structure of language. Hence finding an impact of singing-based training on the brain’s

processing of speech could be largely due to within-domain plasticity.

1.4.3 Embodied cognition

The three training studies in this thesis also rely on a fundamental paradigm, e.g. the

embodied cognition paradigm. Broadly, embodied cognition underlines the importance

of event perception and body actions in cognitive processes. Under this approach,

cognitive processes are conditioned by perceptual and motor modalities (Borghi &

Caruana, 2015). In other words, any knowledge relies on the reactivation of external

states (perception) and internal states (proprioception, emotion, and introspection) as

well as bodily actions (simulation of the sensorimotor experience with the object or event

to which they refer). Much research on this domain, especially in neuroscience, has

shown brain activation of motor and perception networks when participants were

engaged in different tasks involving abilities such as memory, knowledge, language or

thought (see Barsalou, 2008, for a review). By highlighting the importance of appropriate

sensory and motor interactions during learning for the efficient development of human

cognition, embodied cognition has crucial implication for education (see Kiefer & Trumpp,

2012; Wellsby & Pexman, 2014, for reviews). In this filed, researchers have explored

19

embodiment related to reading and writing, memory for events and conceptual memory

for objects and for numbers (Kiefer & Trump, 2012). Several training studies in preschool

children and adults showed that writing new letters by hand improved subsequent letter

recognition and discrimination more than typing training (see Mangen & Velay, 2010, for

a review). The reactivation of sensory-motor experiences collected during an active

learning phase seems crucial for memory performance. For example, Engelkamp and

Jahn (2003) found that participants remembered a list of action verbs better when they

performed these actions during the training compared to only reading the list. Senkfor,

Van Petten, and Kutas (2002) also found an enhancing effect of observing actions on

memory for events, compared with reading the action words. Concerning the conceptual

memory for objects, Neuroimaging and electrophysiological studies have provided

evidence that sensory-motor brain areas are activated during the processing of words

and concepts (see Kiefer & Pulvermüller, 2012, for a review). As for counting, one study

by Fischer, Moeller, Bientzle, Cress, & Nuerk (2011) demonstrated that training children

in finger counting accelarated their learning of numbers and had positive effects on their

subsequent skills in mathematics.

Other aspects of learning need to be investigated in their relation to embodiment.For

example, Baills, Suárez-González, & Prieto (submitted) explored the effect of observing

and producing pitch gestures in the learning of Chinese tones in a training study with

respectively, 49 and 54 Catalan participants. Pitch gestures are hand gestures spacially

describing the melodic curves of the tones, therefore they can be considered as the

embodiment of the prosody of tone-words. Results show that a training with observation

of the pitch gestures enhanced tone discrimination and tone-word learning more than a

training with no gestures and that a training with the production of pitch gestures

enhanced tone discrimination and tone-word learning more than a training with

observation of gestures and repeating the words aloud. However no significant

differences were found between producing and observing the gestures. Yuan, González-

Fuente, Baills, & Prieto (under review) also looked at how pitch gestures can help the

learning of Spanish intonation by sixty-four Mandarin Chinese learners. Half of the

participants received intonation training without gestures while the other half received

the same training with pitch gestures representing nuclear intonation contours. Results

showed that observing pitch gestures during the learning phase improved prononciation

sikills significantly better than a training without gestures.

20

1.5 Hypotheses, assumptions, or background ideas

The ideas to be tested in this PhD thesis are based on a number of assumptions, which

have been explained above. An important assumption behind the three studies is that

there are important similarities between sentence prosody and musical phrases and that

we can use this potential for L2 pronunciation training. While the first two studies build

on the usefulness of hand-clapping and singing for L2 pronunciation, the third

intervention study builds on the potential effects of general musical training on L2

pronunciation improvement in an educational setting.

1.5.1 Phonological differences between Catalan and French

For the three studies, French will be used as the target L2 language for Catalan-dominant

children. This choice is due to the contrasting prosodic and segmental characteristics

between French and Catalan. Even though the two languages are syllable-timed

languages (Ramus, Dupoux, & Mehler, 2003; Ramus, Nespor, & Mehler, 1999), while

stress has a distinctive function in Catalan (e.g., fugir 'to flee' vs fugi '(s)he flees-subj'),

in French it is demarcative at the phrasal level (that is, stresses mark beginnings and

ends of prosodic phrases). Specifically, at the word/accentual phrase level, French

possesses (a) a fixed stress located at the final syllable of a prosodic phrase which

consists either of a rise in F0 and/or lengthening of the final syllable; and (b) an optional

secondary phrasal stress in the initial position which consists of an F0 rise with no

lengthening (Welby, 2006; Jun & Fougeron, 2000, 2002). In French, both demarcative

stresses can be used as cues for segmentation (Banel & Bacri, 1994; Spinelli, Grimault,

Meunier, & Welby, 2010).

1.5.2 Musical activities in the second language classroom as part of pronunciation

training

The principal motivations behind the experiments developed in this thesis are threefold.

First, Studies 1 and 2 aim at testing behaviorally and in a laboratory setting, the potential

benefits of music on speech, following convincing findings from neuroscientific studies

concerning the effect of music expertise, aptitude and training on a number of perceptual

21

and productive skills (see section 1.2.1). Second, Study 3 will try to apply the methods

independently tested in Studies 1 and 2 (e.g., hand-clapping and singing), together with

general musical training in a more natural setting, the second-language classroom, and

in a longer format of intervention in order to extend the possible findings and show their

applicability. Finally, Study 4 is to be a complementary correlational study on the

relationship between pronunciation abilities in a second language with musical aptitudes

and other cognitive and linguistic skills.

1.5.2.1 Hand-clapping as the body externalization of word prosody

The two assumptions behind the first study of the thesis is that (a) there is a clear link

between musical rhythmic perception and metrical groupings of speech; and (b) that

highlighting the rhythmic structure of words through hand-clapping could facilitate the

auditory perception of this word, increase phonological awareness, and eventually lead

to better pronunciation.

McNeill (1992) observed how children produced beat gestures on each syllable of a word

when searching for an unfamiliar word and suggested that beat gesture has a

metalinguistic function for children until they are 5 years of age. McCafferty (2006)

suggested that beat gestures accompanying speech in a second language helped

speakers to externalize prosodic features of the L2 in order to better control them. A

simple way to make the internal rhythm of words salient is to mark each syllable by

clapping one’s hand in the same way as one might clap the beat of a song.

Along these lines, the hypothesis for the first experiment proposes that hand-clapping

the rhythm of words may help children to raise their awareness on the pronunciation of

foreign words.

1.5.2.2 Singing as a facilitator to memorize and produce foreign words and

sentences

Empirical work comparing rhythmic and melodic structure across music and language

have shown that instrumental music composed by a French artist, for example, bears

the same durational and pitch characteristics as the corresponding French speech

(Patel, 2012; Patel & Daniele, 2003; Patel, Iversen, & Rosenberg, 2006). Additionally,

22

Christiner and Reiterer (2015) showed that the vocal flexibility of singers plays a

significant role in speech imitation. Therefore, I hypothesize that second language

training which includes singing (and not only speaking) can help fine-tune vocal-motor

training abilities that can translate into better pronunciation patterns of foreign words and

sentences.

2. RESEARCH METHODOLOGY

Although studies 1, 2, and 3 will have different methodologies, participants will have the

same profile in the three studies and I will apply the same control tasks. Therefore, I will

start with a common section that describes the two.

2.1 Participants

Participants of the three studies will be 7 or 8-year old children in their second grade

attending a public or semi-public Catalan school. They are all bilingual Spanish-Catalan

speakers with Catalan being their predominant language of daily use, as reported in a

language exposure questionnaire given to parents based on Bosch & Sebastián-Gallés

(2001). Second graders at school typically have two-hour weekly exposure to English as

a second language and also musical education for one hour. We also made sure that

participants had no previous knowledge of the French language. Moreover, the language

questionnaire was expanded with a set of questions related to language and music

expertise such as whether the child is learning to play an instrument, to sing, to read

music, or to dance, how long has he/she been learning, the amount of music the child is

generally exposed to during the week, etc. (see Appendix A for the language and musical

experience questionnaire).

2.2 Control tests

In order to take into account individual differences, a series of control tests will be applied

for all participants. These behavioral tests will be run in a separate session approximately

one week before the children go through the experimental pre-test/training/post-test

tasks.

23

2.2.1 Memory span test

A standardized way of measuring short term memory capacity, also called memory span,

is to present a sequence of numbers, letters, or words aloud to a person/child at the rate

of about 1 second per item. The participant is instructed to repeat the sequence verbally,

either in the order each item was presented or in a backward order (Bunting, Cowan, &

Saults, 2006). The length of the sequence is continually increased until the person is

correct only 50% of the time. Following Henry, Messer, Luger-Klein & Crane’s (2012)

procedure, memory span is measured in terms of the maximum number of words from

the list that the child can recall.

In our case, for children aged 7-8, the memory span test consisted of a list of target

Catalan words, translated from the Spanish version of MacArthur communicative

developmental test (López-Ornat et al, 2005). These words are considered as acquired

already at the age of 30 months. It started with a list of three items, which was followed

by a list of four, a list of five, and so on (see Appendix B). This procedure continued until

the child could no longer succeed in recalling all the words in the list. Once the maximum

list length seemed to have been reached, the child was told to remember four lists of the

same length but consisting of different words.

This number of words was regarded as the child’s memory span if all the items were

recalled in at least three out of the four lists.

2.2.2 Narrative ability: story retelling task

Narratives are typically used as a measure of a child’s language abilities and can be

used as a detector of possible language pathologies in children (Demir, Rowe, Heller,

Levine, & Goldin-Meadow, 2015, among others). Better narrative abilities at an early age

(accounted by a well-designed structure) could predict later literacy development,

reading comprehension, and academic performance (Demir & Küntay, 2014). As noted

by Demir and Küntay (2014), around 5 to 6 years of age children begin to create stories

that more reliably include the main components of a story line and start to be able to

refer to the story’s goals.

Materials for this control test consist of one animated cartoon without dialogue or

narration (approximate length 41-50 s) about a small mouse and an elephant, characters

with which the child are not familiar. The structure follows the narratives used in studies

such as Demir et al. (2015): first, some initiating event presents a goal or challenge to

24

the protagonist. This provokes an action on the character’s part, which leads to a

particular outcome. After watching the clip, the child is asked to retell the story to the

experimenter.

To obtain a measure of narrative proficiency, four main features will be considered

(Demir et al., 2015): the presence of an animate protagonist, temporal structure, causal

structure, and goal-directed action. Each feature is considered to be a prerequisite for

the next, and thus narratives with more of these features are rated more complex than

stories with fewer of them. The rubric used to score the narratives produced by

participants are detailed in Appendix C and replicate the grid used in Vilà-Giménez,

Igualada, and Prieto (submitted).

2.2.3. Speech imitation task

Speech imitation talent and pronunciation skills in a foreign language may be highly

interdependent, as suggested by Nardo & Reiterer (2009). To test the children’s ability

to spontaneously imitate non-native speech, we elaborated an imitation task with target

words of different lengths in seven different languages, all completely unknown to the

participants: German, Hebrew, Tagalog, Chinese, Russian, Turkish and Greek. A total

of fourteen words (2 in each language, between 2 and 6 syllables long) were recorded

in a soundproof room by native speakers of those languages.

Each child was asked to listen to each word twice and repeat it immediately. Words were

presented in a randomized order using the automatic shuffle option in Microsoft

PowerPoint.

The raters, expert phoneticians, will be instructed to use headphones and to rate the

imitation ability of the child production on a scale from 1 (very different) to 7 (no difference

at all) immediately after listening to a file which consists of the pair adult target word

followed by the child production.

2.2.4 Perceptual discrimination task

To ensure that participants correctly perceived the language input during the trainings of

studies 1, 2 and 3, or to detect possible problems, children's perceptual discrimination

abilities were assessed through a phonological discrimination task developed for French

25

children at the Department of Orthophony8 of Lille University, France.

The test consists of listening to pairs of nonsense words respecting French phonotactic

rules and telling if the words were the same or different (see Appendix D). The final score

is the sum of the correct answers.

2.2.5 Musical expertise

In a procedure similar to Boll-Avetisyan et al. (2017), musical expertise is measured

using a composite score that combines information from a music questionnaire filled out

by parents (see Appendix A) and some questions which are included in the PROMS test

(see section 2.2.6 below) about the number of learned musical instruments (including

voice for singing and body for dancing), the age of acquiring a first instrument, the total

number of years of musical training, and the frequency of listening to music.

2.2.6 Musical aptitude

2.2.6.1 Evaluation of musical perception

Sometimes, relying on musical expertise and the information given by participants on

their musical background is not enough to grasp musical perceptive skills, because

musically untrained individuals may possess high musical skills, and conversely some

musicians may not be as skilled in perception tasks as expected by their extended

musical training. To test the musical perceptive aptitude of the participants, we chose to

use the PROMS test (Profile of Music Perception Skills). The test was developed by Law

and Zentner (2012) to measure perceptual musical skills across multiple domains:

melody, pitch, timbre, tuning, rhythm, rhythm-in-melody, metric accent, and tempo. The

PROMS has satisfactory psychometric properties for the composite score (internal

consistency and test-retest >.85) and fair to good coefficients for the individual subtests

(.56 to .85). Each subtest corresponding to a musical domain is composed of 18 items;

however, the test can be tailored for children with 8-10 items per subtest.

For reasons of time and for the children to remain concentrated, we chose to take

8 http://orthophonie.univ-lille2.fr/fr/stocks/stock-contents/epreuve-lilloise-de-discrimination-phonologique

26

measure of only the most important subtests in function of the focus of each experiment.

For Study 1 on the effects of hand-clapping the rhythm of words, the chosen subtests

were rhythm and metric accent. For Study 2 on exploring the effects of singing, the

chosen subtests were melody and rhythm. For study three, we plan to use the subtests

rhythm, melody, rhythm, and meter accent.

The results of the tests give a composite score that represent musical perceptive

aptitude.

2.2.6.2 Evaluation of musical production

To assess musical production abilities, we chose to use a series of rhythms; all on an 8-

beat measure to which children had to listen and try to reproduce immediately. The score

consists of the total number of rhythms each child could correctly reproduce.

2.3 Study 1

Study 1 is a between-subjects French word learning experiment with a pre-test and post-

test design aimed at testing the effects of hand-clapping the rhythmic structure of words

on the pronunciation of transparent French words. It relies on the results of experiments

by Cason and colleagues (Nia Cason et al., 2015; Nia Cason & Schön, 2012) showing

that rhythmic priming enhances the phonological processing of speech.

2.3.1 Research question

Does a short training with handclapping the rhythmic structure of words enhance

pronunciation acquisition?

2.3.2 Methodology

2.3.1.1. Participants

27

Thirty 7-8 year-old children from Girona, Catalonia, were trained to learn and repeat 20

target words in French whose meanings were transparent for Catalan speakers in one

of two conditions, namely with or without hand-clapping. They also underwent the control

tests described in 2.2. At this stage, I have collected the data from all the children.

2.3.1.2. Materials

The experiment has a pre/post-test design. One of the first tasks was to select the target

French words for the training phase, as well as the pre- and posttest target French words.

The training was composed of French words directed to Catalan native children.

2.3.1.2.1 Selection of French words

As mentioned before, there are many salient contrasting prosodic and segmental

characteristics between French and Catalan, most notably final stress and a variety of

sounds that are not present in Catalan. To select the target French words for the training,

pre-test and post-test phases, the following considerations were taken:

- Given that children do not have any knowledge of French, the target form of the selected

words should be transparent to Catalan (avió/avion, ordenador/ordinateur)

- The selected words should include the most frequent consonant and vowel sounds in

the target language, in different positions, in order to offer an ample range of possible

sound combinations in the target language

- The selected words includes monosyllabic, disyllabic, and trisyllabic structures

- The total number of words should not be excessive in order to avoid fatigue in the

subjects

- To ensure transparency, the selected words can be represented visually

Initially, a total of forty words were selected. These were rated for “word transparency”

(that is, their phonological proximity to the Catalan corresponding word) by a total of 38

Catalan native speakers through an online survey software. Raters were asked to listen

to the recording of the word in French and rate the degree of transparency between the

target words from 1 to 5 (1= very transparent, 2= quite transparent 3= somewhat

transparent 4= lightly transparent, and 5= Not transparent). For each target word, the

28

image illustrating the word was in the center.

The twenty words which were rated as most transparent (and ensuring a balance of

syllabic structures) were finally chosen as items for the training (see Appendix E). As for

the selection of the twenty items in the pre-test, 10 words from the training were selected

according to their syllable structure and their representativeness of segmental difficulties

in French. Ten additional unrelated words were also selected with the same requirement.

For the post-test, the 10 related items were the same as in the pre-test, 5 unrelated items

were selected from the pre-test and 5 new items were added (see Appendix E for the

total list of words in the training, pre-test, and post-test).

2.3.1.2.2 Audiovisual recordings of the training materials

The training consisted of a video where two French instructors presented the training

items in the two conditions (e.g., clapping vs non-clapping condition). The target words

were video-recorded at the professional broadcasting studio of Universitat Pompeu

Fabra with the help of a technician and subsequently edited in Premiere Pro to create

the training videos.

Each training video clip first contained a short introduction by a native speaker of Catalan

to explain the task to the participants. After this, the target words in the pre-test and then

two repetitions of the 20 items. Each trial was presented in an audiovisual format and

preceded by a picture representing the meaning of the word. After this, two occurrences

of the target word were followed by a 5-second black screen where participants had the

opportunity to repeat the word.

29

Figure 1. Procedure of item presentation in the clapping and the non-clapping conditions.

The twenty items were presented alternately by the two instructors and during the two

trials, participants had the chance to listen and repeat each word after both instructors.

Additionally, for each condition, six video clips were produced with different orders of

presentation of the items to avoid any recency or saliency effects.

2.3.1.2.3 Audio material for the pre and post-tests

The selected items for the pre and post-tests were recorded in audio only by a French

native speaker using an audio recorder.

2.3.1.3. Procedure

A summary of the procedure can be seen in Figure 2.

Figure 2. Experimental procedure of study 2.

2.3.1.3.1 Procedure for the pre- and post-test

Children were individually tested at a quiet room at escola Masmitjà, the participating

public school in Girona.

In both the pretest and the post-test, participants were tested with an adaptation of the

30

elicited imitation task (EIT). Also known as the sentence repetition task, it is a reliable

measure of L2 proficiency in L2 acquisition research, particularly for assessing the oral

modality. It reflects the ease with which spoken language is comprehended and

produced (Van Moere, 2012). In this experiment, sentences were discarded after

preliminary trials which showed that children were not able to repeat unknown sentences.

Therefore, we only used words.

Participants had to follow a user-friendly “words” path prepared on a Powerpoint slide

(see Figure 3), where arrows indicated in which order they needed to click on the sound

icon for each word. They pressed on the icon directly on the screen, listened to the word,

repeated the word immediately after hearing it and then pressed the next icon. The task

was self-paced but the experimenter was always present to monitor the task. The

children's productions were recorded through a high quality microphone.

Figure 3. Words “path” where children clicked on to listen to each item of the pre- and

posttests.

The post-test was composed of a word recall/phonological memory task and a EIT with

a similar procedure as in the pre-test. The word recall/phonological memory task

consisted of showing the pictures representing the words in the training to the children

to discover how many words they could remember. The target words were also recorded.

2.3.1.3.2 Procedure for the training phase

Children were randomly assigned either to the control condition, where they only had to

repeat the words after the instructors during the training, or to the experimental hand-

clapping condition, where they had to repeat the target word uttered by the instructor

31

while hand-clapping its rhythm. In the video clip, before the training started, a native

speaker of Catalan explained the task to the child. Then two trial words were used for

familiarization with the task. After this, participants were asked to learn and repeat a

series of French words in the following way (see also Figure XX for the experimental

procedure):

1) first, they see an image of the word they will learn,

2) second, they see an instructor pronounce the word in French twice (control

condition) or they see an instructor pronounce the word in French twice while

handclapping the rhythm of the word (experimental condition),

3) third, they repeat the word (control condition) or they repeat the word while they

mimic the rhythmic handclapping made by the instructor (experimental condition).

The training session lasted around 10 minutes and around 20 minutes with the pre-test

and post-test. The whole experiment (including control tasks) lasted around 60 minutes.

2.3.1.4 Pronunciation ratings of the pre-test and post-test items

This phase of the experiment has not been completed yet. Pre and post-tests

productions will be presented in pairs to four native speakers of French, who will rate

them simultaneously on an accentedness scale from 1 to 7. Raters will be unaware about

the fact that words will be produced before or after the training.

Mean pronunciation improvement will be compared across conditions. We expect that

the hand-clapping condition will be more effective than the control condition. Moreover,

a potential improvement on the pronunciation of unrelated items will mean that

knowledge was generalized during the training.

2.4 Study 2

Nemoto, Wilson, & Perkins (2016) investigated whether listening to an English song

could be more effective than listening to speech in improving the English pronunciation

of Japanese learners of English. They unexpectedly found that training with speech was

more effective. However, the training item consisted only in one sentence and

participants were not familiar with the melody. Moreover, it seems that participants only

trained their pronunciation with the song and never heard the sentence pronounced in

32

regular speech. Additionally, the authors do not provide much information on the training,

except that participants trained on their own with the material during 10 minutes. Despite

these negative results, there are arguments in favor of the positive effect of songs on

speech abilities (e.g. Moreno et al., 2009; Schön et al., 2004). Study 2 will investigate

whether children improve more on French pronunciation after training on sung or spoken

sentences extracted from children songs whose melody is widely known.

2.4.1 Research Question

Does singing in a second language enhance pronunciation acquisition?

2.4.2 Methodology

This experiment is a short training study with a pre- and post-test design. Participants

will be individually tested at participating schools. The singing/speech training will last

around 20 minutes. Participants will watch a video and learn the lyrics of three French

songs for children whose melody is widely known in European countries, for example

Brother John, Happy Birthday, or Twinkle Twinkle Little Star. The training will consist in

short prosodic/musical phrases that the children must hear and repeat in either the sung

condition (singing), or in the spoken condition, where the lyrics of the song are

pronounced in normal speech (speaking). As in Study 1, during pre and post-tests,

participants will have to repeat a number of French words that are either related or

unrelated to the training items.

After the experiment, children's pre and post-tests productions will be presented in pairs

to 4 native speakers of French who will rate them on an accentedness scale from 1 to 7

without knowing whether they were produced before or after the training. A comparison

between the rates of improvement of the two groups will allow us to test whether the

rhythmic training is more effective. Moreover, an improvement on unrelated items will

mean that knowledge was generalized during the training.

2.4.3 Materials

As in Study 1, two instructors will be recorded either in the singing or the non-singing

condition. The video of the training will feature the upper body of the instructor and

33

display the items in sequences. After each sequence, the participant will have to repeat

the item.

The word imitation task will include words learned during the training and also items that

are unrelated. Items should be of different length and cover the range of difficulties of

French pronunciation, especially the features that are known to be challenging for

Catalan speakers. The items for the tests will be recorded in a sound proof room by two

native speakers of French.

2.5 Study 3

This study aims to transfer our research interest on musical effects on pronunciation

learning to a more ecological setting, i.e. the language classroom. The classroom study

by Fonseca-Mora, Jara-Jiménez, and Gómez-Domíngez (2015) focused on reading in

English as a second language. The authors couldn’t show a significantly higher effect of

a phonological training program compared to phonological training without music.

Toscana-Fuentes and Fonseca Mora (2012) implemented a sound-music program of

English as a second language with six-graders. The authors suggested that this program

improved students’ listening comprehension, speaking, reading, and motivation.

However, they didn’t perform any quantitative analysis to confirm the findings of their

qualitative analysis. The aim of Study 3 will be to focus only on one aspect of language

learning – pronunciation – and to evaluate the effects of the musical training on this

specific linguistic aspect.

2.5.1 Research Question

Does (1) language training in the classroom with integrated active musical activities

related to the phonological structure of the language; and (2) language training in the

classroom preceded by active musical activities unrelated to the phonological structure

of the language enhance acquisition of the pronunciation of words and simple sentences

by children more than language training with games only?

2.5.2 Methodology

34

The third study of this thesis aims to analyze pronunciation achievements in a L2 after a

relatively short training with 7-8 year-old children, with a pre-posttest design. The training

will last 4 weeks. During each week, participants will attend two 45-minute sessions in

groups of 10. The experiment will take place at their school. The total length of the

experiment will be 8 weeks, counting data collection during pre- and posttests.

There will be three conditions: in the “game” group, participants will learn 25 French

words and sentences through games, based on a French language method9 designed

to develop children’s auditory and speaking skills primarily. There will be no writing or

reading activities. In the “integrated music group”, rhythmic and melodic activities will be

integrated into the teaching of the same 25 words and sentences following the same

method. Finally, in the “consecutive music group”, rhythmic and melodic activities will

precede the learning of 25 French words with games.

As in Studies 1 and 2, the pretest will consist of an Elicited Imitation task (EIT) with words

that will be learned during the training and words unrelated to the training. The post-test

will reproduce the same EIT and add to the words present in the pre-test some short

sentences learned during the training, as well as completely new words. Word recall and

phonological memory of the training items will also be assessed at post-test in a picture-

naming task.

2.5.3. Materials

2.5.3.1Training sessions

The eight training sessions will be planned carefully to make sure that the only difference

between interventions in the three conditions will be the musical activities. The lesson

plans will be largely inspired from the language method for children Ludo et ses Amis,

which is mostly oriented towards oral skills in French for young beginners. Appendix F is

an example of the lesson plan for the first session under the three conditions.

2.5.3.2 Pre and posttest items

Among the 25 items that will be part of the word and sentence imitation task, there will

be words and sentences learnt during the training and also items that are not covered at

9 Marchois, C. (2015). Ludo et ses amis – Méthode de Français. Paris: Editions Didier.

35

all. Items should be of different length and cover the range of difficulties of French

pronunciation, especially the features that are known to be challenging for Catalan

speakers. The items for the tests will be recorded in a sound proof room by two native

speakers of French.

For the rating, pre and post-tests productions will be presented in pairs to 4 native

speakers of French who will rate them on an accentedness scale from 1 to 7 without

knowing whether words were produced before or after the training. A comparison

between the rates of improvement of the two groups will allow us to assess whether the

non-integrated music group or the integrated music group is more effective than the

control group. Moreover, an improvement on unrelated items will mean that knowledge

has been generalized during the training.

2.6 Study 4

Following studies by Reiterer and colleagues (Reiterer et al. 2011, Christiner & Reiterer,

2013) on speech imitation talent and musical expertise and aptitude, this study aims at

replicating findings by these authors with children, focusing on perceptive and productive

differences. Retereir et al. (2011) showed a very clear connection between musicality in

general and an ability to pronounce and imitate in a second language. Christiner and

Reiterer (2013) found that singing performance is a better indicator of the ability to imitate

speech than the playing of a musical instrument. Moreover, their results revealed that

64% of the speech imitation score variance could be explained by working memory

together with educational background and singing performance and that 66% of the

speech imitation variance of completely unintelligible and unfamiliar language stimuli

(Hindi) could be explained by working memory together with a singer’s sense of rhythm

and quality of voice. A last study will consist of a correlational analysis of the individual

data collected in the experiments and assess whether speech skills in a second language

(phonological perception, foreign speech imitation) are correlated with musical skills

(musical experience, musical aptitude, musical production), linguistic skills (narrative

task) and cognitive skills (working memory).

This last study will therefore consists of a correlational analysis of the individual data

collected in the experiments and assess whether speech skills in a second language

(phonological perception, foreign speech imitation) are correlated with musical skills

36

(musical experience, musical aptitude, musical production), linguistic skills (narrative

task) and cognitive skills (working memory).

2.6.1 Research Questions

Are speech skills in a second language (phonological perception, foreign speech

imitation) correlated with musical skills (musical expertise, musical perceptive aptitude,

musical productive aptitude), linguistic skills (narrative task) and cognitive skills (working

memory)?

2.6.2 Methodology

The fourth study of this thesis is a correlational analysis between data on musical

aptitude and musical experience obtained from the children during the three preceding

experiments and their results at a phonological perception task, their imitation abilities in

French, calculated after the rating by French native speakers, and their ability to imitate

a series of words in different languages. The choice of productive and perceptive

variables is being motivated by Cohrdes, Grolig, and Schroeder (2016). In this

correlational study with 7 year-old children, the authors selected representative

language- and music-related competencies and found that language and music skills are

significantly interrelated.

2.6.3 Tools and resources

The data collected with the children during studies 1, 2, and 3 concern the following

specific abilities:

- Cognitive skills: working memory (see section 2.2.1 Memory span test),

- Language skills: narrative abilities (see section 2.2.2 Story retelling task),

- Speech skills: speech imitation talent (see section 2.2.3 Speech imitation task) and

phonological perception (see section 2.2.4 Perceptual discrimination task),

- Musical skills: musical expertise (see section 2.2.5), musical aptitude (see section

2.2.6.1 Evaluation of musical perception, and section 2.2.6.2 Evaluation of musical

production).

37

38

3. WORKING SCHEDULE

April - May 2017

Data collection for experiment 1 at school Masmitjà, Girona.

Submission of the article based on the Treball Final de Màster to the journal

Studies in Second Language Acquisition. Title: Observing and producing pitch

gestures facilitates the learning of mandarin Chinese tones and words.

Coauthors: Nerea Suárez González, Santiago González Fuente, Pilar Prieto.

Writing of the PhD research plan.

June 2017

Writing and submission of PhD research plan

Poster presentation at the conference Phonetics and Phonology in Europe

(PaPE), Köln Universität: Cologne (Germany), June 12-14. Title: Gestures

reproducing the F0 curve of lexical tones help learning Mandarin Chinese tones

and words. Coauthors: Nerea Suárez González, Santiago González Fuente, Pilar

Prieto.

Poster presentation at the conference Language as a form of Action, Institute of

Cognitive Sciences and Technologies (ISTC): Roma (Italy), June 21-23.

Title: Pitch gestures help learning Mandarin Chinese tones and words: a brief

training study on perception and production. Coauthors: Nerea Suárez González,

Santiago González Fuente, Pilar Prieto.

July 2017

Oral presentation of the PhD research plan: July 3rd Oral presentation at the Workshop sobre la Prosòdia del Català, University of

Barcelona. Preparation of the items for ratings and analysis of the results for Study 1

August 2017

Oral presentation at the European Language Association Conference (EUROSLA), Centre for Literacy and Multilingualism at the University of Reading: Reading (UK), August 30 - September 2.Title: Does a training with hand-clapping enhance the pronunciation of foreign words by second-graders?

September-October 2017

39

Methods design and testing design for Study 2

Contact with schools for studies 2 and 3

November- December 2017

Data collection for Study 2

January-March 2018

Data preparation and analysis for Study 2

Methods design and testing design for Study 3

April-June 2018

8 weeks’ training and data collection for Study 3

Possible attendance to Speech Prosody: Grenoble (France), May 15-18

Possible attendance to LabPhon16: Lisboa (Portugal), June 20-22

July 2018

Data preparation and analysis for Study 3

Writing of Study 3

September-October 2018

Proposal preparation of Study 4 in collaboration with Susanne Reiterer and Markus Christiner, from the Research center for Language Learning and Teaching at Vienna University, Austria

November-December 2018

Data preparation and analysis for Study 4

January- June 2019

Writing of the PHD thesis

July 2019

PHD defense

40

4. SELECTED REFERENCES

Christiner, M., & Reiterer, S. M. (2015). A Mozart is not a Pavarotti : singers outperform

instrumentalists on foreign accent imitation, Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 9,

1–8.

This experiment by Christiner and Reiterer (2015) follows up on previous findings that

there is a very clear connection between musicality in general and an ability to articulate

well (Reiterer et al., 2011) and that – together with auditory working memory – good

singing predicts good imitation skills in an unknown language (Christiner & Reiterer,

2013). Here, the authors test whether singing capacity and instrument playing contribute

to speech imitation to different extent. Ninety-seven participants (27 instrumentalists, 33

vocalists and 36 non-musicians/non-singers) were tested on their abilities to imitate

speech in an unknown language (Hindi) and in a second language (English), and on their

musical aptitude. Results show that vocalists and instrumentalists significantly

outperformed non-musicians/non-singers in imitating speech both unknown and foreign.

Moreover, vocalists significantly outperformed instrumentalists. The authors emphasize

the complementary role of the vocal motor system in the ability to learn new languages,

in addition to auditory processes.

This study is crucial for this thesis, as it supports the underlying effects of musical

expertise and aptitude on second language pronunciation. Most importantly, participants

are engaged in a productive task, which highlight a different aspect of the language-

music relationship compared to most investigations (neurobiological and behavioral),

which tend to examine speech perception.

Cason, N., Astésano, C., & Schön, D. (2015). Bridging music and speech rhythm:

Rhythmic priming and audio-motor training affect speech perception. Acta

Psychologica, 155, 43–50.

This experiment follows up on findings by Cason & Schön (2012), who found evidence

In an electrophysiological (EEG) experiment that musical rhythmic priming enhanced the

phonological processing of bi- or trisyllabic pseudo-words with a final stressed syllable

when the speech metrical structure matched the prime meter. Cason et al., (2015) aimed

to investigate if this effect could also be found with real sentences in French. Thirty-four

French adult participants heard a musical rhythmic prime followed by a sentence either

41

with a matching or mismatching prosodic structure and immediately performed a

phoneme detection task. To check for the possible effect of active production, half the

participants had received audio–motor training with the musical rhythms presented in the

experiment. Analysis of reaction times showed that phoneme detection was faster with

a matching metrical prime. In addition, authors compared results between the group that

benefited the audio-motor training with the prime against results of the group that only

heard them and found that the priming effect was enhanced in the first group.

This experiment is particularly important for the present thesis in two ways. First, it

underlines the link of musical training – the effect of the primes – to final sentence stress,

which is one of the objective of Study 1. Second, it demonstrate the enhancing role of

production over perception – the effect of training the primes actively, which goes in the

sense of an important effect of embodiment. Nevertheless, the terms of the audio-motor

training cannot be assessed as they are not described in the article.

Nemoto, S., Wilson, I., & Perkins, J. (2016). Analysis of the effects on pronunciation of

training by using song or native speech. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of

America, 140(4), 3343.

Nemoto, Wilson, and Perkins (2016) investigated whether English pronunciation by

Japanese speakers improved more after training with spoken or sung sentences. 30

participants trained their pronunciation individually during 10 minutes either by listening

to an English song and singing themselves or by listening to the lyrics of the song spoken

by a native speaker. Hundred-eight native speakers listened to the sentence produced

by the participants and rated isolated structures from the text on a scale from 1 to 4

against the following criteria: pronunciation, intonation and foreign accent. Results

showed that participants in the sung condition performed significantly worse at post-test

than participants in the spoken condition. The authors suggest that the fact that

participants in the experimental group only heard the song’s melody, and not the English

language melody, negatively influenced the production of the sentences’ prosody at

posttest. This could constitute a reasonable explanation, since the participants did not

hear the spoken sentence and therefore never had the opportunity to listen to the

prosody of the spoken target sentences. The negative results could also be due to the

rating system, through Amazon Mechanical Turk, or more probably the instructions given

to the raters, who could listened to the sentences only twice and had to rate various

42

isolated items from these sentences.

This experiment is closely related to Study 2, which will be an experiment with songs. It

will however differ in several aspects. First, the melodies will be well-known by the

participants. Second, they will hear the words both sung and spoken. Third, native

speakers will listen to the items in isolation for judgment, instead of full sentences, and

they will be able to rate in a more fine-grained scale from 1 to 7.

Toscano-Fuentes, M. C., & Fonseca Mora, M. C. (2012). La música como herramienta

facilitadora del aprendizaje del inglés como lengua extranjera. Teoría de La

Educación, 24 (2), 197–213.

Toscano-Fuentes and Fonseca (2012) implemented a classroom training with forty-nine

Spanish 6th-graders. They added musical activities to the classic ESL curriculum for the

academic year (9 months). Songs were used actively to teach listening comprehension,

vocabulary, grammar and focus on pronunciation whereas instrumental music was used

passively as ambient music. The authors claim that students with high auditory capacity

(measured by the Seashore test) had better results in English in general and in reading

and listening comprehension in particular. In this study, pronunciation abilities were

evaluated by teachers during regular speaking activities. Most importantly, the absence

of a control group does not allow us to assess if the improvements in the five skills under

scrutiny (speaking, reading, listening, writing and grammar) are due to a normal

development after nine month instruction or are increased thanks to the musical

activities, in other words, there isn’t any control group.

This longitudinal intervention study is very interesting for Study 3 of the present thesis,

as it is an implementation of musical activities in the English language classroom.

Reflecting on this experience, we aim at an intervention in the classroom that will be

more controlled, with a careful selection of the structure to be learned and very organized

lesson plans to avoid discrepancies between the control group and the experimental

groups. We will also focus on pronunciation and recall. Most importantly, children will

practice active rhythmic activities and songs.

Cohrdes, C., Grolig, L., & Schroeder, S. (2016). Relating language and music skills in

young children: A first approach to systemize and compare distinct competencies

on different levels. Frontiers in Psychology, 7, 1–11.

43

Cohrdes, Grolig, and Schroeder (2016) conducted a correlational study with forty-four 7

year-old children with a large number of representative language- and music-related

competencies from lower to higher levels. In this study, language and music skills are

hierarchized in 5 levels in a parallel manner a task is applied in each domain for each

level of competence.

- Level 1: small perceptual units such as vowels and phonemes (phonemic

discrimination, word discrimination) and sounds and short tonal phrases

(sound discrimination, tonal discrimination).

- Level 2: perception and production of syllables and words (phonological

awareness, prosodic repetition) and short rhythms and melodies

(rhythmic repetition, melodic repetition).

- Level 3: processing of syntactic (syntactic integration) or harmonic

sequences (harmonic progression).

- Level 4: recognition of emotions in spoken sentences (ERs) or tonal

phrases (ERt).

- Level 5: high-level units such as stories (narrative comprehension) or

songs (synchronization).

Results show that performance on lower levels was predictive for the performance on

higher levels within domains. Moreover, correlations between domains were stronger for

competencies reflecting a similar level of cognitive processing.

This article is extremely helpful for the design of our control tests and for Study 4, as it

gives an exhaustive description of the tests the authors used to obtain data for each of

the variables described above. For study one, we considered the phonemic

discrimination task and the rhythmic repetition task. For the remaining studies, we will

probably tap into melodic perception and production as well.

44

5. GENERAL REFERENCES

Abbott, M. (2002). Using music to promote L2 learning among adult learners. TESOL

Journal, 11(1), 10–17. http://doi.org/10.1002/j.1949-3533.2002.tb00061.x

Abrams, D. A., Bhatara, A., Ryali, S., Balaban, E., Levitin, D. J., & Menon, V. (2010).

Decoding temporal structure in music and speech relies on shared brain

resources but elicits different fine-scale spatial patterns. Cerebral Cortex, 21(7),

1507–1518.

Anderson-Hsieh, J., Johnson, R., & Koehler, K. (1992). The relationship between

native speaker judgments of nonnative pronunciation and deviance in segmentals,

prosody, and syllable structure. Language Learning, 42, 529–555.

Anvari, S. H., Trainor, L. J., Woodside, J., & Levy, B. A. (2002). Relations among

musical skills, phonological processing, and early reading ability in preschool

children. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 83(2), 111–130.

http://doi.org/10.1016/S0022-0965(02)00124-8

Arnold, J. (1999). Affect in Language Learning. New York: Cambridge University Press.

Asaridou, S. S., & McQueen, J. M. (2013). Speech and music shape the listening brain:

Evidence for shared domain-general mechanisms. Frontiers in Psychology, 4, 1–

14. http://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00321

Baills, F., Suárez-González, N. G.-F. S., González-Fuente, S., & Prieto, P. (n.d.).

Observing and Producing Pitch Gestures Facilitates the Learning of Mandarin

Chinese Tones and Words. Studies in Second Language Acquisition (submittted).

Studies in Second Language Acquisition.

Banel, M. H., & Bacri, N. (1994). On metrical patterns and lexical parsing in French.

Speech Communication, 15(1–2), 115–126.

Barsalou, L. W. (2008). Grounded cognition. Annual Review of Psychology, 59, 617–

645.

Bengtsson, S. L., Nagy, Z., Skare, S., Forsman, L., Forssberg, H., & Ullén, F. (2005).

Extensive piano practicing has regionally specific effects on white matter

development. Natural Neuroscience, 8, 1148–1150.

Benz, S., Sellaro, R., Hommel, B., & Colzato, L. S. (2016). Music makes the world go

45

round: The impact of musical training on non-musical cognitive functions - a

review. Frontiers in Psychology, 6, 1–5. http://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2015.02023

Bergman Nutley, S., Darki, F., & Klingberg, T. (2014). Music practice is associated with

development of working memory during childhood and adolescence. Frontiers in

Human Neuroscience, 7, 926. http://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2013.00926

Besson, M., Chobert, J., Astésano, C., & Marie, C. (2015). Influence of musical

expertise on the perception of pitch, duration and intensity variations in speech

and harmonic sounds. In C. Astésano & M. Jucla (Eds.), Neuropsycholinguistic

Perspectives on Language Cognition: Essays in Honour of Jean-Luc Nespoulous

(pp. 88–102). London, New York: Psychology Press.

Besson, M., Chobert, J., & Marie, C. (2011). Transfer of training between music and

speech: Common processing, attention, and memory. Frontiers in Psychology,

2(MAY), 1–12. http://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2011.00094

Besson, M., & Faïta, F. (1995). An event-related potential (ERP) study of musical

expectancy: Comparison of musicians with nonmusicians. Journal of Experimental

Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 21, 1278–1296.

Besson, M., & Schön, D. (2001). Comparison between language and music. In R. J.

Zatorre & I. Peretz (Eds.), The Biological Foundations of Music (p. 232−258). New

York: The New York Academy of Sciences.

Besson, M., Schön, D., Moreno, S., Santos, A., & Magne, C. (2007). Influence of

musical expertise and musical training on pitch processing in music and language.

Restorative Neurology and Neuroscience, 25(3–4), 399–410.

Bhide, A., Power, A., & Goswami, U. (2013). A rhythmic musical intervention for poor

readers: A comparison of efficacy with a letter-based intervention. Mind, Brain,

and Education, 7(2), 113–123. http://doi.org/10.1111/mbe.12016

Bialystok, E., & DePape, A.-M. (2009). Musical expertise, bilingualism, and executive

functioning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and

Performance, 35(2), 565–574. http://doi.org/10.1037/a0012735

Bidelman, G. M., Krishnan, A., & Gandour, J. T. (2010). Enhanced brainstem encoding

predicts musicians’ perceptual advantages with pitch. European Journal of

Neuroscience, 33, 530–538.

46

Bilhartz, T. D., Bruhn, R. A., & Olson, J. E. (1999). The effect of early music training on

child cognitive development. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 20,

615–636.

Boll-avetisyan, N., Bhatara, A., & Hoehle, B. (2017). Effects of Musicality on the

Perception of Rhythmic Structure in Speech. Laboratory Phonology: Journal of the

Association for Laboratory Phonology, 8(1), 1–16.

Boll-Avetisyan, N., Bhatara, A., & Höhle, B. (2017). Effects of musicality on the

perception of rhythmic structure in speech. Laboratory Phonology, 8(1), 1–16.

http://doi.org/10.5334/labphon.91

Bolton, T. L. (1894). Rhythm. American Journal of Psychology, 6, 145–238.

Borghi, A. M., & Caruana, F. (2015). Embodiment Theory. In International

Encyclopedia of the Social & Behavioral Sciences (Second Edition) (pp. 420–426).

Bosch, L., & Sebastián-Gallés, N. (2001). Evidence of Early Language Discrimination

Abilities in Infants From Bilingual Environments. Infancy, 2(1), 29–49.

Brandler, S., & Rammsayer, T. H. (2003). Differences in mental abilities between

musicians and non-musicians. Psychology of Music, 31(2), 123–138.

http://doi.org/10.1177/0305735603031002290

Brandt, A., Gebrian, M., & Slevc, L. R. (2012). Music and early language acquisition.

Frontiers in Psychology, 3(SEP), 327. http://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00327

Bugos, J., & Mostafa, W. (2011). Musical training enhances information processing

speed. Bulletin of the Council for Research in Music Education, 187, 7–18.

Bunting, M. F., Cowan, N., & Saults, J. S. (2006). How does running memory span

work? Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 59(10), 1691–1700.

Cason, N., Astésano, C., & Schön, D. (2015). Bridging music and speech rhythm:

Rhythmic priming and audio-motor training affect speech perception. Acta

Psychologica, 155, 43–50. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.actpsy.2014.12.002

Cason, N., Hidalgo, C., Isoard, F., Roman, S., & Schön, D. (2015). Rhythmic priming

enhances speech production abilities: evidence from prelingually deaf children.

Neuropsychology, 29, 102–107. http://doi.org/10.1037/neu0000115

Cason, N., & Schön, D. (2012). Rhythmic priming enhances the phonological

47

processing of speech. Neuropsychologia, 50(11), 2652–2658.

http://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2012.07.018

Chan, A. S., Ho, Y. C., & Cheung, M. C. (1998). Music training improves verbal

memory. Nature, 396(6707), 128. http://doi.org/10.1038/24075

Chobert, J., & Besson, M. (2013). Musical Expertise and Second Language Learning.

Brain Sciences, 3(2), 923–940. http://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci3020923

Chobert, J., François, C., Velay, J. L., & Besson, M. (2014). Twelve months of active

musical training in 8-to 10-year-old children enhances the preattentive processing

of syllabic duration and voice onset time. Cerebral Cortex, 24(4), 956–967.

http://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhs377

Christiner, M., & Reiterer, S. M. (2013). Song and speech : examining the link between

singing talent and speech imitation ability. Frontiers in Psychology, 4, 1–11.

http://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00874

Christiner, M., & Reiterer, S. M. (2015). A Mozart is not a Pavarotti : singers outperform

instrumentalists on foreign accent imitation, 9, 1–8.

http://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2015.00482

Cohrdes, C., Grolig, L., & Schroeder, S. (2016). Relating language and music skills in

young children: A first approach to systemize and compare distinct competencies

on different levels. Frontiers in Psychology, 7, 1–11.

http://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2016.01616

Coyle, Y., & Gómez Gracia, R. (2014). Using songs to enhance L2 vocabulary

acquisition in preschool children. ELT Journal, 68(3), 276–285.

http://doi.org/10.1093/elt/ccu015

Darcy, I., Ewert, D., & Lidster, R. (2012). Bringing pronunciation instruction back into

the classroom: An ESL teachers’ pronunciation “toolbox.” Proceedings of the 3rd

Pronunciation in Second Language Learning and Teaching Conference, 93–108.

Dauer, R. (1983). Stress-timing and syllable-timing re-analyzed. Journal of Phonetics,

11, 51–62.

Delogu, F., Lampis, G., & Belardinelli, M. O. (2010). From melody to lexical tone:

Musical ability enhances specific aspects of foreign language perception.

European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 22(1), 46–61.

48

http://doi.org/10.1080/09541440802708136

Demir, Ö. E., & Küntay, A. C. (2014). Cognitive and Neural Mechanisms Underlying

Socioeconomic Gradients in Language Development: New Answers to Old

Questions. Child Development Perspectives, 8, 113–118.

Demir, Ö. E., Rowe, M., Heller, G., Levine, S. C., & Goldin-Meadow, S. (2015).

Vocabulary, syntax and narrative development in children with and without early

unilateral brain injury: Early parental talk about the there-and-then matters.

Developmental Psychology, 51(2), 161–175.

Derwing, T. M., & Munro, M. J. (2015). Pronunciation fundamentals. evidence-based

Perspectives for L2 Teaching and Research. Amsterdam/Philadelphia: John

Benjamins Publishing Company.

Derwing, T. M., Munro, M. J., & Wiebe, G. (1998). Evidence in favor of a broad

framework for pronunciation instruction. Language Learning, 48(3), 393–410.

http://doi.org/10.1111/0023-8333.00047

Derwing, T. M., & Rossiter, M. J. (2003). The effects of pronunciation instruction on the

accuracy, fluency, and complexity of L2 accented speech. Applied Language

Learning, 13(1), 1–17.

Edmunds, P. (2010). ESL speakers’ production of English lexical stress: The effect of

variation in acoustic correlates on perceived intelligibility and nativeness. The

University of New Mexico, Albuquerque, NM, USA.

Ellis, R. J., & Jones, M. R. (2010). Rhythmic context modulates foreperiod effects.

Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 72(8), 2274–2288.

Engelkamp, J., & Jahn, P. (2003). Lexical, conceptual and motor information in

memory for action phrases: A multi-system account. Acta Psychologica.

http://doi.org/10.1016/S0001-6918(03)00030-1

Field, J. (2005). Intelligibility and the listener: The role of lexical stress. TESOL

Quarterly, 39, 399–423.

Fischer, U., Moeller, K., Bientzle, M., Cress, U., & Nuerk, H.-C. (2011). Sensori-motor

spatial training of number magnitude representation. Psychonomic Bulletin &

Review, 18(1), 177–183. http://doi.org/10.3758/s13423-010-0031-3

Fischler, J. (2009). The rap on stress: Teaching stress patterns to English language

49

learners through rap music. MinneWITESOL Journal, 26.

Fonseca-Mora, M. C., Jara-Jiménez, P., & Gómez-Domínguez, M. (2015). Musical plus

phonological input for young foreign language readers. Frontiers in Psychology, 6,

1–9. http://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00286

Fonseca Mora, C., & Gant, M. (2016). Melodies, Rhythm and Cognition in Foreign

Language Learning. Newcastle upon Tyne: Cambridge Scholars Publishing.

Fonseca Mora, M. C., Toscano-Fuentes, M. C., & Wermke, K. (2011). Melodies that

help: the relation between language aptitude and musical intelligence. Anglistik

International Journal of English Studies, 22(1), 101–118.

Foote, J. A., Holtby, A., & Derwing, T. M. (2011). Survey of pronunciation teaching in

adult ESL programs in Canada. TESL Canada Journal, 29, 1–22.

Forgeard, M., Schlaug, G., Norton, A., Rosam, C., Iyengar, U., & Winner, E. (2008).

The relation between music and phonological processing in normal-reading

children and children with dyslexia. Music Perception: An Interdisciplinary Journal,

25(4), 383–390. http://doi.org/10.1525/mp.2008.25.4.383

François, C., Chobert, J., Besson, M., & Schön, D. (2013). Music training for the

development of speech segmentation. Cerebral Cortex, 23(9), 2038–2043.

http://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhs180

François, C., Grau-Sánchez, J., Duarte, E., & Rodriguez-Fornells, A. (2015). Musical

training as an alternative and effective method for neuro-education and neuro-

rehabilitation. Frontiers in Psychology, 6, 1–15.

http://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00475

Francois, C., & Schön, D. (2011). Musical expertise boosts implicit learning of both

musical and linguistic structures. Cerebral Cortex, 21(10), 2357–2365.

http://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhr022

Franklin, M. S., Moore, K. S., Yip, C.-Y., Jonides, J., Rattray, K., & Moher, J. (2008).

The effects of musical training on verbal memory. Psychology of Music, 36(3),

353–365. http://doi.org/10.1177/0305735607086044

Fujioka, T., Trainor, L. J., Ross, B., Kakigi, R., & Pantev, C. (2004). Musical training

enhances automatic encoding ofmelodic contour and interval structure. Journal of

Cognitive Neuroscience, 16, 1010–1021.

50

Gass, S., & Selinker, L. (2001). Second language acquisition : an introductory course.

London: Routledge.

George, E. M., & Coch, D. (2011). Music training and working memory: an ERP study.

Neuropsychologia, 49(5), 1083–1094.

http://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2011.02.001

Gluhareva, D., & Prieto, P. (2017). Brief training with beat gestures favors L2

pronunciation in discourse-demanding situations. Language Teaching Research

(Vol. in press). http://doi.org/10.1177/1362168816651463

Goodkin, D. (2004). Play, sing and dance. An introduction to Orff Schulwerk. Mainz:

Schott.

Gordon, E. E. (1989). Advanced Measures of Music Audiation. Chicago, IL: GIA.

Gordon, J., Darcy, I., & Ewert, D. (2013). Pronunciation teaching and learning:

Phonetic instruction in the L2 classroom. In Proceedings of the 4th Pronunciation

in Second Language Learning and Teaching Conference (pp. 194–206). Ames,

IA: Iowa State University.

Gordon, R. L., Magne, C. L., & Large, E. W. (2011). EEG correlates of song prosody: A

new look at the relationship between linguistic and musical rhythm. Frontiers in

Psychology, 2.

Gottfried, T. L., Staby, A. M., & Ziemer, C. J. (2004). Musical experience and mandarin

tone discrimination and imitation. Journal of Acoustic Society of America, 115,

2545.

Graham, C. (1978). Jazz Chants: Rhythm of American English for Student of English

as a Second Language. New York: Oxford University Press.

Hannon, E. E., & Johnson, S. P. (2005). Infants use meter to categorize rhythms and

melodies: Implications for musical structure learning. Cognitive Psychology, 50(4),

354–377. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.cogpsych.2004.09.003

Hannon, E. E., Lévêque, Y., Nave, K. M., & Trehub, S. E. (2016). Exaggeration of

language-specific rhythms in English and French children’s songs. Frontiers in

Psychology, 7, 939. http://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00939

Heffner, C. C., & Slevc, L. R. (2015). Prosodic structure as a parallel to musical

structure. Frontiers in Psychology, 6, 1962.

51

http://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2015.01962

Henry, L. A., Messer, D., Luger-Klein, S., & Craine, L. (2012). Phonological, visual, and

semantic coding strategies and children´s short-term memory span. Quarterly

Journal of Experimental Psychology, 65(10), 2033–2053.

Herrera, L., Lorenzo, O., Defior, S., Fernández-Smith, G., & Costa-Giomi, E. (2011).

Effects of phonological and musical training on the reading readiness of native-

and foreign- Spanish-speaking children. Psychology of Music, 39(1), 68–81.

Hyde, K. L., Lerch, J., Norton, A., Forgeard, M., Winner, E., Evans, A. C., & Schlaug,

G. (2009). Musical training shapes structural brain development. Journal of

Neuroscience, 29, 3019–3025.

Imfeld, A., Oechslin, M. S., Meyer, M., Loenneker, T., & Jancke, L. (2009). White

matter plasticity in the corticospinal tract of musicians: a diffusion tensor imaging

study. NeuroImage, 46, 600–607.

Jäncke, L. (2012). The relationship between music and language. Frontiers in

Psychology, 3, 123. http://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00123

Jones, M. R., Boltz, M., & Kidd, G. (1982). Controlled attending as a function of melodic

and temporal context. Perception and Psychophysics, 32, 211–238.

Kang, O. (2010). Relative salience of suprasegmental features on judgments of L2

comprehensibility and accentedness. System, 38, 301–315.

Kiefer, M., & Pulvermüller, F. (2012). Conceptual representations in mind and brain:

Theoretical developments, current evidence and future directions. Cortex.

http://doi.org/10.1016/j.cortex.2011.04.006

Kiefer, M., & Trumpp, N. M. (2012). Embodiment theory and education: The

foundations of cognition in perception and action. Trends in Neuroscience and

Education, 1(1), 15–20. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.tine.2012.07.002

Kishon-Rabin, L., Amir, O., Vexler, Y., & Zaltz, Y. (2001). Pitch discrimination: are

professional musicians better than non-musicians? Journal of Basic and Clinical

Physiology and Pharmacology, 12, 125–143.

Koelsch, S., Gunter, T. C., van Cramon, D. Y., Zysset, S., Lohmann, G., & Friederici, A.

D. (2002). Bach speaks: a cortical “language- network” serves the processing of

music. Neuroimage, 17, 956–966.

52

Koelsch, S., Schröger, E., & Tervaniemi, M. (1999). Superior pre-attentive auditory

processing in musicians. Neuroreport, 10, 1309–1313.

Kotilahti, K., Nissilä, I., Näsi, T., Lipiäinen, L., Noponen, T., Meriläinen, P., & Al., E.

(2010). Hemodynamic responses to speech and music in newborn infants. Human

Brain Mapping, 31, 595–603.

Kushch, O., Gluhareva, D., & Prieto, P. (n.d.). Positive effects of beat gesture

production on pronunciation improvement. In Preparation.

Large, E. W., & Jones, M. R. (1999). The dynamics of attending: How people track

time- varying events. Psychological Review, 106, 119–159.

Law, L. N. C., & Zentner, M. (2012). Assessing Musical Abilities Objectively:

Construction and Validation of the Profile of Music Perception Skills. PLoS ONE,

7(12).

Lebedeva, G. C., & Kuhl, P. K. (2010). Sing that tune: Infants’ perception of melody

and lyrics and the facilitation of phonetic recognition in songs. Infant Behavior and

Development, 33(4), 419–430. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.infbeh.2010.04.006

Lee, C.-Y., & Hung, T.-H. (2008). Identification of Mandarin tones by English-speaking

musicians and nonmusicians. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America,

124, 3235–3248. http://doi.org/10.1121/1.2990713

Levitin, D. J., & Menon, V. (2003). Musical structure is pro- cessed in “language” areas

of the brain: a possible role for Brodmann area 47 in temporal coherence.

NeuroImage, 20, 2142–2152.

London, J. (2012). Hearing in time: psychological aspects of musical meter. New York:

Oxford University Press.

London, J. (2012). Hearing in Time: Psychological Aspects of Time. New York: Oxford

University Press.

Lowe, A. (1997). The effect of the incorporation of music learning into the second

language classroom on the mutual reinforcement of music and language. UMI.

University of Illinois at Urbana-Champaign.

Ludke, K. M., Ferreira, F., & Overy, K. (2014). Singing can facilitate foreign language

learning. Memory & Cognition, 42, 41–52. http://doi.org/10.3758/s13421-013-

0342-5

53

Maess, B., Koelsch, S., Gunter, T. C., & Friederici, A. D. (2001). Musical syntax is

processed in Broca’s area: an MEG study. Natural Neuroscience, 4(5), 540–545.

Magne, C., Astésano, C., Aramaki, M., Ystad, S., Kronland-Martinet, R., & Besson, M.

(2007). Influence of syllabic lengthening on semantic processing in spoken french:

behavioral and electrophysiological evidence. Cerebral Cortex, 17, 2659–2668.

Magne, C., Schön, D., & Besson, M. (2006). Musician Children Detect Pitch Violations

in Both Music and Language Better than Nonmusician Children: Behavioral and

Electrophysiological Approaches, 199–211.

Mangen, A., & Velay, J. (2010). Digitizing literacy: reflections on the haptics of writing.

Advances in Haptics, 385–403. http://doi.org/10.5772/8710

Marie, C., Delogu, F., Lampis, G., Berlardinelli, M. O., & Besson, M. (2011). Influence

of musical expertise on segmental and tonal processing in Mandarin Chinese.

Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 23(10), 2701–2715.

http://doi.org/10.1162/jocn.2010.21585

Marie, C., Kujala, T., & Besson, M. (2011). Musical and linguistic expertise influence

preattentive and attentive processing of non-speech sounds. Cortex, 48(4), 447–

457.

Marques, C., Moreno, S., Castro, S. L., & Besson, M. (2007). Musicians detect pitch

violation in a foreign language better than nonmusicians: behavioral and

electrophysiological evidence. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 19(9), 1453–

1463. http://doi.org/10.1162/jocn.2007.19.9.1453

McCafferty, S. G. (2006). Gesture and the materialization of second language prosody.

IRAL - International Review of Applied Linguistics in Language Teaching, 44, 197–

209. http://doi.org/10.1515/IRAL.2006.008

McNeill, D. (1992). Hand and Mind: What Gestures Reveal About Thought. Chicago:

University of Chicago Press.

Michelas, A., & D’Imperio, M. (2010). Durational cues and prosodic phrasing in French:

evidence for the intermediate phrase. In Speech Prosody 2010 (pp. 1–4).

Micheyl, C., Delhommeau, K., Perrot, X., & Oxenham, A. J. (2006). Influence of

musical learning and psychoacoustical training on pitch discrimination. Hearing

Research, 219, 36–47.

54

Milovanov, R., Huotilainen, M., Välimäki, V., Esquef, P. A. A., & Tervaniemi, M. (2008).

Musical aptitude and second language pronunciation skills in school-aged

children: Neural and behavioral evidence. Brain Research, 1194, 81–89.

http://doi.org/10.1016/j.brainres.2007.11.042

Milovanov, R., Pietilä, P., Tervaniemi, M., & Esquef, P. A. A. (2010). Foreign language

pronunciation skills and musical aptitude: A study of Finnish adults with higher

education. Learning and Individual Differences, 20(1), 56–60.

http://doi.org/10.1016/j.lindif.2009.11.003

Milovanov, R., & Tervaniemi, M. (2011). The interplay between musical and linguistic

aptitudes: A review. Frontiers in Psychology, 2, 1–6.

http://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2011.00321

Mithen, S. (2005). The Singing Neanderthals: The Origins of Music, Language, Mind

and Body. Cambridge: Harvard University Press.

Moreno, S., & Besson, M. (2006). Musical training and language-related brain electrical

activity in children. Psychophysiology, 43(3), 287–291.

http://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-8986.2006.00401.x

Moreno, S., Marques, C., Santos, A., Santos, M., Castro, S. L., & Besson, M. (2009).

Musical training influences linguistic abilities in 8-year-old children: More evidence

for brain plasticity. Cerebral Cortex, 19(3), 712–723.

http://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhn120

Musacchia, G., Strait, D., & Kraus, N. (2008). Relationships between behavior,

brainstem and cortical encoding of seen and heard speech in musicians and

nonmusicians. Hearing Research, 241, 34–42.

Nakata, K. (2002). Eigo No Atama Nikawaru Hon. Tokyo: Chukei Publishing Company.

Nardo, D., & Reiterer, S. M. (2009). Musicality and phonetic language aptitude. In G.

Dogil & S. M. Reiterer (Eds.), Language talent and brain activity. Trends in Applied

Linguistics (pp. 213– 255). Berlin, Germany: Mouton de Gruyter.

Nemoto, S., Wilson, I., & Perkins, J. (2016). Analysis of the effects on pronunciation of

training by using song or native speech. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of

America, 140(4), 3343. http://doi.org/10.1121/1.4970678

Nooteboom, S. (1997). The prosody of speech: melody and rhythm. In W. J. Hardcastle

55

& J. Laver (Eds.), The Handbook of Phonetic Sciences (pp. 640–673). Oxford, UK:

Blackwell Publishing Ltd.

Overy, K. (2000). Dyslexia, Temporal Processing and Music: The Potential of Music as

an Early Learning Aid for Dyslexic Children. Psychology of Music, 28(2), 218–229.

http://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1177/0305735600282010

Overy, K. (2003). Dyslexia and Music. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences,

999, 497–505. http://doi.org/10.1196/annals.1284.060

Overy, K., Nicolson, R. I., Fawcett, A. J., & Clarke, E. F. (2003). Dyslexia and music:

Measuring musical timing skills. Dyslexia, 9(1), 18–36.

http://doi.org/10.1002/dys.233

Pacheco-Costa, A. (2016). Shall We Sing? Orff-Schulwerk Tools for Rhythmic

Development in L2 Students. In M. C. Fonseca-Mora & M. Gant (Eds.), Melodies,

Rhythm and Cognition in Foreign Language Learning.

Patel, A. (2008). Music, language and the brain. New York: Oxford University Press.

Patel, A. D. (2011). Why would musical training benefit the neural encoding of speech?

The OPERA hypothesis. Frontiers in Psychology, 2, 1–14.

http://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2011.00142

Patel, A. D. (2012). The OPERA hypothesis: Assumptions and clarifications. Annals of

the New York Academy of Sciences, 1252(1), 124–128.

http://doi.org/10.1111/j.1749-6632.2011.06426.x

Patel, A. D. (2014). Can nonlinguistic musical training change the way the brain

processes speech? The expanded OPERA hypothesis. Hearing Research, 308,

98–108. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.heares.2013.08.011

Patel, A. D., & Daniele, J. R. (2003). An empirical comparison of rhythm in language

and music. Cognition, 87, B35–B45.

Patel, A. D., Iversen, J. R., & Rosenberg, J. C. (2006). Comparing the rhythm and

melody of speech and music: The case of British English and French. The Journal

of the Acoustical Society of America, 119(5), 3034–3047.

http://doi.org/10.1121/1.2179657

Pitt, M. A., & Samuel, A. G. (1990). The use of rhythm in attending to speech. Journal

of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 16, 564–573.

56

Posedel, J., Emery, L., Souza, B., & Fountain, C. (2012). Pitch perception, working

memory, and second-language phonological production. Psychology of Music,

40(4), 508–517. http://doi.org/10.1177/0305735611415145

Ramus, F., Dupoux, E., & Mehler, J. (2003). The psychological reality of rhythm

classes: Perceptual studies. 15th ICPhs Barcelona, 1–6. Retrieved from

http://cogprints.org/3079/

Ramus, F., Nespor, M., & Mehler, J. (1999). Correlates of linguistic rhythm in the

speech signal. Cognition, 73(3), 265–292. http://doi.org/10.1016/S0010-

0277(99)00058-X

Reiterer, S., Hu, X., Erb, M., Rota, G., Nardo, D., & Grodd, W. (2011). Individual

differences in audio-vocal speech imitation aptitude in late bilinguals: functional

neuro-imaging and brain morphology. Frontiers in Psychology, 2:271.

Roach, P. (1982). On the distinction between “stress-timed” and “syllable-timed”

languages. In D. Crystal (Ed.), Linguistic controversies (pp. 73–79). London:

Edward Arnold.

Roden, I., Grube, D., Bongard, S., & Kreutz, G. (2014). Does music training enhance

working memory performance? Findings from a quasi-experimental longitudinal

study. Psychology of Music, 42(2), 284–298.

http://doi.org/10.1177/0305735612471239

Rodrigues, A. C., Loureiro, M. A., Caramelli, P., Carolina, A., Alves, M., & Caramelli, P.

(2013). Brain and Cognition Long-term musical training may improve different

forms of visual attention ability. Brain and Cognition, 82(3), 229–235.

http://doi.org/10.1016/j.bandc.2013.04.009

Schellenberg, E. G. (2006). Long-term positive associations between music lessons

and IQ. Journal of Educational Psychology, 98(2), 457–468.

http://doi.org/10.1037/0022-0663.98.2.457

Schellenberg, E. G., Burnham, D., Peretz, I., & Pliner, P. (1999). Music lessons

enhance IQ. Psychological Science, 15, 1–5.

Schmithorst, Vincent J. Wilke, M. (2002). Differences in white matter architecture

between musicians and non-musicians: a diffusion tensor imaging study.

Neuroscience Letters, 321(1–2), 57–60.

57

Schön, D., Boyer, M., Moreno, S., Besson, M., Peretz, I., & Kolinsky, R. (2008). Songs

as an aid for language acquisition. Cognition, 106(2), 975–983.

http://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2007.03.005

Schön, D., Magne, C., & Besson, M. (2004). The music of speech: Music training

facilitates pitch processing in both music and language. Psychophysiology, 41(3),

341–349. http://doi.org/10.1111/1469-8986.00172.x

Seashore, C. E., Lewis, D., & Saetveit, J. G. (1960). Seashore measures of musical

talents manual, 2nd edn. New York: Psychological Corporation.

Senkfor, A. J., Petten, C. Van, & Kutas, M. (2002). Episodic action memory for real

objects: An ERP investigation with perform, watch, and imagine action encoding

tasks versus a non-action encoding task. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience,

14(3), 402–419. http://doi.org/10.1162/089892902317361921

Snyder, J. S., & Large, E. W. (2005). Gamma-band activity reflects the metric structure

of rhythmic tone sequences. Cognitive Brain Research, 24(1), 117–126.

Spiegel, M. F., & Watson, C. S. (1984). Performance on frequency discrimination tasks

by musicians and non-musicians. Journal of Acoustic Society of America, 76,

1690–1695.

Spinelli, E., Grimault, N., Meunier, F., & Welby, P. (2010). An intonational cue to word

segmentation in phonemically identical sequences. Attention, Perception &

Psychophysics, 72(3), 775–787.

Strait, D., & Kraus, N. (2011). Playing music for a smarter ear: Cognitive, perceptual

and neurobiological evidence. Music Perception: An Interdisciplinary Journal,

29(2), 133–146. http://doi.org/10.1525/mp.2011.29.2.133

Strait, D. L., Parbery-Clark, A., Hittner, E., & Kraus, N. (2012). Musical training during

early childhood enhances the neural encoding of speech in noise. Brain and

Language, 123(3), 191–201. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.bandl.2012.09.001

Swaminathan, S., & Gopinath, J. K. (2013). Music training and second-language

English comprehension and vocabulary skills in Indian children. Psychological

Studies, 58, 164–170.

Teixido, M., François, C., Bosch, L., & Männel, C. (n.d.). The role of prosody in early

speech segmentation and word learning: Electrophysiological evidence. In P.

58

Prieto & N. Esteve-Gibert (Eds.), Prosodic Development in First Language

Acquisition. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.

Tervaniemi, M., Jacobsen, T., Röttger, S., Kujala, T., Widmann, A., Vainioi, M., …

Schröger, E. (2006). Selective tuning of cortical sound-feature processing by

language experience. European Journal of Neuroscience, 23, 2538–2541.

Tierney, A., & Kraus, N. (2013). Music training for the development of reading skills.

Progress in Brain Research, 207, 209–241. http://doi.org/10.1016/B978-0-444-

63327-9.00008-4

Tizian, S. (2016). Listen and Learn : an experiment on the effectiveness of songs in

English L2 Pronunciation. Università degli Studi di Padova.

Toscano-Fuentes, M. C., & Fonseca Mora, M. C. (2012). La música como herramienta

facilitadora del aprendizaje del inglés como lengua extranjera. Teoría de La

Educación, 24 (2), 197–213.

Trainor, L. J., Desjardins, R. N., & Rockel, C. (1999). A comparison of contour and

interval processing in musicians and nonmusicians using event-related potentials.

Australian Journal of Psychology, 51, 147–153.

Trehub, S. E. (2003). The developmental origins of musicality. Natural Neuroscience,

669–673.

Unyk, A. M., Trehub, S. E., Trainor, L. J., & Schellenberg, E. G. (1992). Lullabies and

simplicity: A cross-cultural perspective. Psychology of Music, 20(1), 15–28.

http://doi.org/10.1177/0305735692201002

Vilà-Giménez, I., Igualada, A., & Prieto, P. (n.d.). Training with rhythmic beat gestures

improves children’s narrative discourse skills.

Vuust, P., Ostergaard, L., Pallesen, K. J., Bailey, C., & Roepstorff, A. (2009). Predictive

coding of music - Brain responses to rhythmic incongruity. Cortex, 45(1), 80–92.

http://doi.org/10.1016/j.cortex.2008.05.014

Wang, H., Mok, P., & Meng, H. (2016). Capitalizing on musical rhythm for prosodic

training in computer-aided language learning. Computer Speech and Language,

37, 67–81. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.csl.2015.10.002

Watson, D., Clark, L. A., & Tellegen, A. (1988). Development and validation of brief

measures of positive and negative affect: The PANAS scales. Journal of

59

Personality and Social Psychology, 54, 1063–1070.

Welby, P. (2006). French intonational structure: Evidence from tonal alignment. Journal

of Phonetics, 34(3), 343–371.

Wellsby, M., & Pexman, P. M. (2014). Developing embodied cognition: Insights from

children’s concepts and language processing. Frontiers in Psychology, 5, 1–10.

http://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00506

White, L., & Mattys, S. L. (2007). Calibrating rhythm: First language and second

language studies. Journal of Phonetics, 35, 501–522.

Woodward, J., & Sikes, P. L. (2015). The creative thinking ability of musicians and

nonmusicians. Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts, 9, 75.

Wuytack, J., & Palheiros, G. B. (1995). Audiçao musical activa [Active musical

audition]. Porto: Associaçao Wuytack de Pedagogia Musical.

Yang, H., Ma, W., Gong, D., Hu, J., & Yao, D. (2014). A Longitudinal Study on

Children’s Music Training Experience and Academic Development. Scientific

Reports, 4, 5854. http://doi.org/10.1038/srep05854

Yuan, C., González-Fuente, S., Baills, F., & Prieto, P. (n.d.). Observing Pitch Gestures

Favors the Learning of Spanish Intonation by Mandarin Speakers (under review).

Studies in Second Language Acquisition.

60

5. APPENDIX

Appendix A – Language and musical experience questionnaire10

10 Additionally, in the PROMs test, participants are asked if there is someone playing music in their family, as amateur or professional musician.

61

Appendix B – Lists of Catalan words for the memory span test

(3words) barba, suc, cosí

bolígraf, plàtan, porta

cadira, coll, paper

nuvi, pastís, ull

(4 words) casa, llengua, poma, telèfon

sabata, guitarra, sol, cuina

ungla, llapis, nebot, maduixa

abella, peu, pallasso, llibre

(5 words) clau, aigua, dent, nina, dutxa

lluna, bruixa, germà, cocodril, banyador

ratolí, guants, tren, vestit, mandarina

barret, grua, autobus, jardí, llum

(6 words) lloro, raqueta, cotxe, poma, sandalia, neu

tisores, dibuix, nit, groc, tovallola, cuina

aixeta, moneda, sabó, marieta, nas, llaminadura

got, sofà, crema, pantalons, ou, conill

62

Appendix C – Narrative scoring

63

Appendix D – Phonological discrimination test for French phonemes

64

Appendix E – Words selected for the training, the pre-test and the post-test of

Study 1

Training disyllabic trisyllabic more than 3 syllables

balcon ambulance télévision

tambour crocodile ordinateur

musique biberon hélicoptère

purée céréales aspirateur

oreille confiture

pizza spaguettis

avion éléphant

famille mandarine

Total number 8 8 4 20

Pretest disyllabic trisyllabic more than 3 syllables

RELATED

tambour crocodile aspirateur

balcon mandarine

musique biberon

purée confiture

ambulance

UNRELATED

sportive imprimante calendrier

rateau guarderie

cheveu dinosaure

tortue

serpent

princesse

Total number 10 8 2 20

Post test disyllabic trisyllabic more than 3 syllables

RELATED

tambour crocodile aspirateur

balcon mandarine

musique biberon

purée confiture

ambulance

UNRELATED

cheveu guarderie calendrier

rateau imprimante

NEW

sorcière cheminée

statue

fourchette

ceinture

Total number 10 8 2 20

65

Appendix F – Training procedure of the first session, Study 3

Language structures Écoute répète À toi Encore une fois! C’est très bien bravo!

Vocabulary (transparent words) tomate banane pizza sándwich moto taxi

musique guitare girafe princesse café coca

Grup 1: Game Group Grup 2: Integrated Rhythm Group

Grup 3. Consecutive Rhythm Group

Part 1 Listening to a poem (prerecorded) – 14 minutes

Part 1 Rhythmic activities - 20 minutes

Activity 1: During the listening, mimes are used by the instructor to help children get the meaning of the poem. Activity 2 : Children listen again and repeat the mimes together with the instructor

Activity 1: During the listening, mimes are used by the instructor to help children get the meaning of the poem. They can hear the tempo in the background while listening to the poem. Activity 2: Children listen again and spontaneously follow the rhythm of the poem. They can walk, run, jump, and swing, etc.

Activity 1: Children listen to and spontaneously follow different rhythms including the rhythm of the poem that will be introduced later. They can walk, run, jump, and swing, etc. Activity 2: Children listen again to the rhythm and use freely their hands and feet to follow the rhythm. They explore different ways of marking a rhythm.

Part 2: Games with flashcards – 26 minutes Part 2: Games with flashcards – 20

12 flashcards with drawings representing the transparent words are distributed to the children. Activity 1:

A/ Children listen to the poem once again and raise the card when they hear the corresponding word. B/ Children listen to the poem, stand up and form a line according to the sequence of the words in the poem. Then the instructor calls one of the items on the flashcard at a time and the child that has got the card must go back to her seat. Activity 2:

A/ Instructor picks up one flashcard and proposes a word. If it is not the corresponding word, children put their hand on their mouth to tell they do not agree. If it is the correct word, they repeat the word. B/Instructor asks children to remember and announce the words in the right order and hang them on the blackboard. They listen to the poem to

Activity 1:

A/ Children listen to the poem once again and raise the card when they hear the corresponding word. B/Instructor names and claps a series of words as in the poem (series of 4), and then asks children to move and invent rhythmic movement on their body with the instructor repeat the series and vary the tempo. Activity 2:

A/ Instructor picks up one flashcard and proposes a word. If it is not the corresponding word, children put their hand on their mouth to tell they do not agree. If it is the correct word, they repeat the word and clap. B/ Instructor asks children to remember and announce the words in the right order and hang them on the blackboard. They listen to the poem to check if they are

Activity 1: Listening to a poem

A/During the listening of the poem, mimes are used by the instructor to help children get the meaning of the poem. B/ Children listen again and repeat

the mimes together with the instructor Activity 2:

A/Children listen to the poem once again and raise the card when they hear the corresponding word

B/ Instructor picks up one flashcard and proposes a word. If it is not the corresponding word, children put their hand on their mouth to tell they do not agree. If it is the correct word, they repeat the word.

66

check if they are right and repeat after each series (series of 4)

right, they repeat and clap after a series is finish (series of 4)