supp.apa.orgsupp.apa.org/.../pag0000167/MWST_OA_supplemental.docx · Web viewDifferent mechanisms...

22

Click here to load reader

Transcript of supp.apa.orgsupp.apa.org/.../pag0000167/MWST_OA_supplemental.docx · Web viewDifferent mechanisms...

Page 1: supp.apa.orgsupp.apa.org/.../pag0000167/MWST_OA_supplemental.docx · Web viewDifferent mechanisms have been proposed to explain how stereotype threat affects task performance. In

Supplemental Materials

Stereotype Threat as a Trigger of Mind-Wandering in Older Adults

By Megan L. Jordano and Dayna R. Touron, 2015, Psychology and Aging

Additional measures. Several single-item, Likert-scale post-task questions were included to

measure factors that might influence mind-wandering and Operation Span (OSPAN) letter recall

performance. We found that perceived overall task difficulty (F (3,110) = 1.992, p = .119) and

perceived difficulty of the letter recall portion of the OSPAN (F (3,110) = 1.106, p = .350) did

not differ by condition. Although older adults (OAs) did worse on average than younger adults

(YAs) on the OSPAN, they did not report finding the OSPAN or its subcomponents more

difficult than did YAs. Self-rated effort in completing the OSPAN likewise did not differ by

condition (F (3,110) = 2.231, p = .089). As shown in Table 2, greater recall and overall difficulty

were related to higher proportions of task-related interference (TRI), whereas greater effort was

related to lower proportions of task-unrelated thoughts (TUTs).

We also included a single-item Likert-scale question asking participants if they believed

there was a stereotype that OAs have worse memory ability than do YAs, and found that there

was an effect of condition of this response, F (3,110) = 5.186, p = .002. ST activated OAs had

higher scores for this question than did YAs and OA controls (all p values < .05 for t-test focused

comparisons), meaning that they were more likely to believe that the negative stereotype existed.

ST relief OAs were also more likely to believe the negative stereotype existed than did YAs and

OA controls (all p values < .05 for t-test focused comparisons). As shown in Table 2, greater

stereotype awareness was related to lower proportions of TUTs and to higher proportions of TRI.

Page 2: supp.apa.orgsupp.apa.org/.../pag0000167/MWST_OA_supplemental.docx · Web viewDifferent mechanisms have been proposed to explain how stereotype threat affects task performance. In

Because the effect of stereotype threat (ST) on task performance has been proposed to be

mediated by the importance of good performance in the stereotype domain (Hess et al., 2013)

and anxiety during completion of the stereotype task (Chasteen, 2005; Abrams et al., 2005), we

included the Anxiety and Achievement subscales of the Metamemory in Adulthood (MIA)

Questionnaire (Dixon & Hultsch, 1984). There was no effect of condition on MIA Achievement

scores (F (3,113) = .212, p =.888) but there was on effect of condition on MIA Anxiety scores, F

(3,113) = 3.716, p = .014. As expected, OAs activated for stereotype threat had higher scores on

the anxiety scale of the MIA than did OA controls and OAs relieved of stereotype threat (all p

values < .05 for t-test focused comparisons), indicating that activated OAs reported experiencing

greater anxiety while completing everyday memory tasks. YAs also reported significantly higher

scores on the anxiety scale of the MIA than did OA controls and ST relief OAs relieved

stereotype threat (all p values < .05 for t-test focused comparisons). As shown in Table 2, higher

MIA achievement scores were related to lower proportions of TUTs and to higher proportions of

TRI, and higher MIA Anxiety scores were related to lower proportions of TUTs.

The Influence of Age, Task difficulty, and Importance of Task Performance on TRI.

Different mechanisms have been proposed to explain how stereotype threat affects task

performance. In the current study, we conducted exploratory mediational analyses to gain a

better understanding of which factors influence proportion of probe-caught TRI. We also wished

to gain a better understanding of how mind-wandering (particularly TRI) may mediate the

relationship between factors such as task engagement, perceived task difficulty, and mood, and

performance on the stereotyped task. To do this, we conducted a regression predicting letter

recall accuracy, as well as an additional mediational analyses using bootstrapping.

Page 3: supp.apa.orgsupp.apa.org/.../pag0000167/MWST_OA_supplemental.docx · Web viewDifferent mechanisms have been proposed to explain how stereotype threat affects task performance. In

When looking at the entire sample of participants, the following factors were

significantly correlated with TRI and with letter recall accuracy during the OSPAN:

chronological age, perceived overall OSPAN difficulty, perceived letter recall difficulty, self-

rated task effort, self-rated stress, self-rated focus on the letter recall portion of the OSPAN,

importance of good memory performance (as measured by the Achievement scale of the MIA),

memory anxiety (as measured by the Anxiety scale of the MIA), and belief in the existence of a

negative aging and memory stereotype (see Table 2). When excluding YA participants and only

looking at OA participants, the only factor significantly correlated with TRI variables and with

letter recall accuracy during the OSPAN was perceived letter recall difficulty (see Table 3).

To better understand which factors are most important in predicting proportion of probe-

caught TRI, we conducted three regression analyses using our entire sample of participants in

which chronological age, OSPAN difficulty, letter recall difficulty, task effort, stress, focus on

the letter recall portion of the OSPAN, MIA Achievement scores, and MIA anxiety scores were

simultaneously entered to predict proportion of probe-caught TRI, proportion of probe-caught

proactive TRI, and proportion of probe-caught reactive TRI (see Table 4). Chronological age (

= .033, p = .012) and participants’ self-rated importance of good memory performance as

measured by MIA Achievement scores ( = .009, p = .031) both emerged as significant

predictors of overall proportion of probe-caught TRI. Chronological age also emerged as a

significant predictor of reactive TRI about task performance evaluation ( = .001, p = .002).

Conversely, for proportion of proactive TRI about task-strategy or task approach, participants’

self-rated letter recall difficulty was the only factor that emerged as a significant predictor (

= .002, p = .011). These regression analyses indicate that different participant characteristics

predict different types of TRI experiences. Wihtin our older adult sample, self-rated letter recall

Page 4: supp.apa.orgsupp.apa.org/.../pag0000167/MWST_OA_supplemental.docx · Web viewDifferent mechanisms have been proposed to explain how stereotype threat affects task performance. In

difficulty was a significant predictor of mean proportion of overall probe-caught TRI ( = .051,

p = .002).

TRI as a Mediator in the Relationships between Stress, Task Difficulty, and Task

Performance. In addition to the regression analyses, we conducted exploratory mediational

analyses to determine whether probe-caught TRI mediates the relationship between the

aforementioned factors and letter recall accuracy. Using our entire sample of participants, we

examined overall TRI as a mediator for the stress, OSPAN difficulty, and recall difficulty

factors, while the fourth examined proactive TRI as a mediator for recall difficulty. The results

of these analyses are displayed in Figure 1.

First, using 6000 resamples, we tested for indirect effects of overall probe-caught TRI on

letter recall accuracy. Bootstrapping was used to calculate the confidence interval around the

indirect effect. The 95% confidence interval (CI) was [-2.236, -.167] with zero falling outside of

this confidence interval, indicating that probe-caught TRI mediates the relationship between

stress and letter recall accuracy, p = .03. Probe-caught TRI was also found to mediate the

relationship between perceived OSPAN task difficulty and letter recall accuracy and between

perceived letter recall difficulty and letter recall accuracy. The 95% CIs were [-3.20, -.233] and

[-3.212, -.326], respectively, with zero falling outside of these intervals (p = .021 and p = .047,

respectively). Finally, we tested for an indirect effect of proactive TRI on recall accuracy,

finding that proactive TRI likewise mediated the relationship between perceived letter recall

difficulty and recall accuracy (95% CI [-2.367, -.071], p = .025).

We were interested in whether these patterns would hold for older adults. Accordingly,

excluding the YA sample and only looking at our OA sample, we conducted an additional

Page 5: supp.apa.orgsupp.apa.org/.../pag0000167/MWST_OA_supplemental.docx · Web viewDifferent mechanisms have been proposed to explain how stereotype threat affects task performance. In

mediational analysis to determine whether probe-caught TRI mediates the relationship between

older adults’ self-rated letter recall difficulty and letter recall accuracy. We did not test the

mediation models for the relationships between stress and overall difficulty ratings with TRI and

recall accuracy, due to having obtained nonsignificant correlations between these variables in the

OA sample. The result of our analysis is displayed in Figure 2. Using 6000 resamples, we tested

for indirect effects of overall probe-caught TRI on letter recall accuracy. Bootstrapping was used

to calculate the confidence interval around the indirect effect. The 95% confidence interval (CI)

was [-.177, .002] with zero falling inside of this confidence interval, indicating that probe-caught

TRI did not mediate the relationship between self-rated letter recall difficulty and letter recall

accuracy, p = .06 in older adults.

The difference in correlation and mediation model patterns when we restrict analysis to

the OA sample likely reflects the reduced statistical power of these tests. However, it is also

possible that the effects obtained for the full sample are inflated by the low levels of TRI and

higher recall accuracy by the young adult group. Given the exploratory nature of these models,

the outcomes should be considered tentative and should be further investigated in future

research.

Page 6: supp.apa.orgsupp.apa.org/.../pag0000167/MWST_OA_supplemental.docx · Web viewDifferent mechanisms have been proposed to explain how stereotype threat affects task performance. In

Table 1. Experimental task verbal instructions.

Instructions for Younger Adult and Older Adult Control Groups:

“You will soon complete a variety of tasks presented on our lab computers. For the first task, you will be required to alternate between verifying mathematical equations and remembering letters in a specified order. You will have opportunity to practice the task before data collection actually begins. Feedback will be provided. Occasionally, questions will come up during the task asking you to report whether or not you were completely focused on the task. Having some off-task thoughts is normal, so please answer these questions as honestly and accurately as possible. Following this task, you will complete a few additional questionnaires. A more detailed set of instructions will be presented on the computer screen as you move through the experiment.”

Instructions for the Older Adult Stereotype Threat Relief Group:

“You will soon complete a variety of tasks presented on our lab computers. For the first task, you will complete a task that is specifically designed to measure mathematical ability. For this task, you will be required to alternate between verifying mathematical equations and remembering letters in a specified order. You will have opportunity to practice the mathematical task before data collection actually begins. Feedback will be provided. Occasionally, questions will come up during the mathematical task asking you to report whether or not you were completely focused on the task. Having some off-task thoughts is normal, so please answer these questions as honestly and accurately as possible. Following this mathematical task, you will complete a few additional questionnaires. A more detailed set of instructions will be presented on the computer screen as you move through the experiment.”

Instructions for the Older Adult Stereotype Threat Activated Group:

“You will soon complete a variety of tasks presented on our lab computers. For the first task, you will complete a task that is specifically designed to measure memory ability. For this task, you will be required to alternate between verifying mathematical equations and remembering letters in a specified order. You will have opportunity to practice the memory task before data

Page 7: supp.apa.orgsupp.apa.org/.../pag0000167/MWST_OA_supplemental.docx · Web viewDifferent mechanisms have been proposed to explain how stereotype threat affects task performance. In

collection actually begins. Feedback will be provided. Occasionally, questions will come up during the memory task asking you to report whether or not you were completely focused on the task. Having some off-task thoughts is normal, so please answer these questions as honestly and accurately as possible. Following this memory task, you will complete a few additional questionnaires. A more detailed set of instructions will be presented on the computer screen as you move through the experiment.”

Page 8: supp.apa.orgsupp.apa.org/.../pag0000167/MWST_OA_supplemental.docx · Web viewDifferent mechanisms have been proposed to explain how stereotype threat affects task performance. In

Table 2

Overall Correlations for Mind-Wandering Variables and Letter Recall

Variable OT TUT TRI Proactive TRI

Reactive TRI

Recall Accuracy

Age .059 -.349** .210* .135 .195* -.109DSSQ TUT -.340** .416** .052 -.030 .096 -.027DSSQ TRI -.247** -.060 .347** .173 .324** -.119Recall Difficulty -.172 -.096 .291** .336** .128 -.279**Overall Difficulty -.067 -.164 .236* .212* .121 -.284**Recall Focus .209* -.238** -.050 .023 -.071 .367**Stress -162 -.005 .197* .152 .157 -.202*Fatigue -.068 .063 .018 .008 .033 -.206*Effort .175 -.400** .145 .134 .079 -.040Stereotype Awareness

-.074 -.240** .283** .226* .226* -.148

MIA Achievement -.039 -.215* .220* .185* .168 -.120MIA Anxiety -.129 -.214* .155 .021 .142 -.116Positive Mood .104 -.305** .128 .175 .052 -.045Negative Mood -.144 .048 .154 -.056 .101 -.055

Note. OT = proportion of probe-caught on-task thoughts. TUT = proportion of probe-caught task-unrelated thoughts. TRI =proportion of probe-

caught task-related interference. Proactive TRI = proportion of probe-caught TRI regarding task approach or strategy. Reactive TRI = proportion

of probe-caught TRI regarding evaluation of task performance. Recall accuracy = accuracy on the letter recall portion of the OSPAN. Age =

Chronological age in years. DSSQ TUT= score on the TUT subscale of the DSSQ (out of 40), with higher scores indicating more TUTs. DSSQ TRI=

score on the TRI subscale of the DSSQ (out of 40), with higher scores indicating more TRI. Recall difficulty = self-rated perceived difficulty of the

recall portion of the OSPAN (out of 5, where 1=not at all difficulty, 5= very difficult). Overall difficulty = self-rated perceived difficulty of the

Page 9: supp.apa.orgsupp.apa.org/.../pag0000167/MWST_OA_supplemental.docx · Web viewDifferent mechanisms have been proposed to explain how stereotype threat affects task performance. In

OSPAN task overall (out of 5, where 1= not at all difficult, 5 = very difficult. Stress = self-rated perceived stress or anxiety during the OSPAN (out

of 5, where 1= not at all stressed, 5 = very stressed). Fatigue = how fatiguing the OSPAN was perceived to be (out of 5, where 1= not at all

fatiguing, 5 = very fatiguing). Effort = self-rated effort during the OSPAN task (out of 5, where 1= no effort at all, 5 = a lot of effort). Stereotype

awareness = self-rated belief that a negative stereotype regarding aging and cognitive decline exists (out of 5, where 1= strongly disagree that

there is a stereotype that OAs are inferior to YAs in terms of cognitive ability, 5 = strongly agree that there is a stereotype that OAs are inferior to

YAs in terms of cognitive ability). MIA Achievement = Score on the memory achievement scale of the MIA (out of 75), with higher scores

indicating more self-rated importance of good memory performance. MIA Anxiety = Score on the memory anxiety scale of the MIA (out of 65) ,

with higher scores indicating more self-rated anxiety while completing daily task that have a memory component. Positive mood = score on the

positive affect scale of the PANAS (out of 50), with higher scores indicating more positive mood. Negative mood = score on the negative affect

scale of the PANAS (out of 50), with higher scores indicating more negative mood.

* indicates p < .05. ** indicates p < .001.

Page 10: supp.apa.orgsupp.apa.org/.../pag0000167/MWST_OA_supplemental.docx · Web viewDifferent mechanisms have been proposed to explain how stereotype threat affects task performance. In

Table 3

Correlations for Mind-Wandering Variables and Letter Recall for the Older Adult Sample

Variable OT TUT TRI Proactive TRI

Reactive TRI

Recall Accuracy

Age .035 .114 -.115 -.051 .035 .238*DSSQ TUT -.393** .286** .052 .074 .293** -.076DSSQ TRI -.391** .015 .431** .167 .433** -.117Recall Difficulty -.222* -.042 .284** .326** .120 -.271*Overall Difficulty -.113 -.081 .203 .170 .107 -.308**Recall Focus .216* -.111 -.177 -.068 -.162 .521**Stress -182 -.008 .210 .143 .182 -.200Fatigue -.101 -.007 .105 .035 .139 -.302**Effort .131 -.282** .055 .056 .012 -.009Stereotype Awareness

-.164 -.172 .290** .193 .255* -.129

MIA Achievement -.107 -.175 .222* .153 .201 -.094MIA Anxiety -.175 -.029 .251* .057 .226* -.170Positive Mood .123 -.135 -.061 .087 -.134 .115Negative Mood -.174 .109 .130 -.019 .183 -.109

Note. OT = proportion of probe-caught on-task thoughts. TUT = proportion of probe-caught task-unrelated thoughts. TRI =proportion of probe-

caught task-related interference. Proactive TRI = proportion of probe-caught TRI regarding task approach or strategy. Reactive TRI = proportion

of probe-caught TRI regarding evaluation of task performance. Recall accuracy = accuracy on the letter recall portion of the OSPAN. Age =

Chronological age in years. DSSQ TUT= score on the TUT subscale of the DSSQ (out of 40), with higher scores indicating more TUTs. DSSQ TRI=

score on the TRI subscale of the DSSQ (out of 40), with higher scores indicating more TRI. Recall difficulty = self-rated perceived difficulty of the

recall portion of the OSPAN (out of 5, where 1=not at all difficulty, 5= very difficult). Overall difficulty = self-rated perceived difficulty of the

Page 11: supp.apa.orgsupp.apa.org/.../pag0000167/MWST_OA_supplemental.docx · Web viewDifferent mechanisms have been proposed to explain how stereotype threat affects task performance. In

OSPAN task overall (out of 5, where 1= not at all difficult, 5 = very difficult. Stress = self-rated perceived stress or anxiety during the OSPAN (out

of 5, where 1= not at all stressed, 5 = very stressed). Fatigue = how fatiguing the OSPAN was perceived to be (out of 5, where 1= not at all

fatiguing, 5 = very fatiguing). Effort = self-rated effort during the OSPAN task (out of 5, where 1= no effort at all, 5 = a lot of effort). Stereotype

awareness = self-rated belief that a negative stereotype regarding aging and cognitive decline exists (out of 5, where 1= strongly disagree that

there is a stereotype that OAs are inferior to YAs in terms of cognitive ability, 5 = strongly agree that there is a stereotype that OAs are inferior to

YAs in terms of cognitive ability). MIA Achievement = Score on the memory achievement scale of the MIA (out of 75), with higher scores

indicating more self-rated importance of good memory performance. MIA Anxiety = Score on the memory anxiety scale of the MIA (out of 65) ,

with higher scores indicating more self-rated anxiety while completing daily task that have a memory component. Positive mood = score on the

positive affect scale of the PANAS (out of 50), with higher scores indicating more positive mood. Negative mood = score on the negative affect

scale of the PANAS (out of 50), with higher scores indicating more negative mood.

* indicates p < .05. ** indicates p < .001.

Page 12: supp.apa.orgsupp.apa.org/.../pag0000167/MWST_OA_supplemental.docx · Web viewDifferent mechanisms have been proposed to explain how stereotype threat affects task performance. In

Table 4

Regression models for TRI, Proactive TRI, and Reactive TRI

TRI Proactive TRI Reactive TRIEstimate SE p Estimate SE p Estimate SE p

Intercept -.677 .246 <.001 -.306 .157 .053 -.357 .185 .055Age .003 .001 .012 .001 .001 .320 .002 .001 .011Recall Difficulty .044 .027 .108 .050 .017 .005 <.001 .020 .987Overall Difficulty .006 .028 .084 .001 .018 .954 -.003 .021 .869Recall Focus -.044 .027 .107 -.006 .017 .730 -.034 .020 .094Stress -.003 .025 .897 -.007 .016 .668 .001 .019 .615Effort .004 .026 .891 .001 .017 .978 -.002 .020 .886Stereotype Awareness

.024 .025 .339 .009 .016 .586 .019 .018 .311

MIA Achievement .009 .004 .031 .005 .003 .076 .005 .003 .104MIA Anxiety .005 .003 .132 -.001 .002 .686 .004 .002 .164

Note. Age = Chronological age in years. Recall difficulty = self-rated perceived difficulty of the recall portion of the OSPAN (out of 5, where 1=not

at all difficulty, 5= very difficult). Overall difficulty = self-rated perceived difficulty of the OSPAN task overall (out of 5, where 1=not at all

difficulty, 5= very difficult). Recall focus = self-rated focus on the letter recall portion of the OSPAN (out of 5, where 1= not at all focused, 5 = very

much focused). Stress = self-rated perceived stress or anxiety during the OSPAN (out of 5, where 1= not at all stressed, 5 = very stressed). Effort =

self-rated effort during the OSPAN task (out of 5, where 1= no effort at all, 5 = a lot of effort). Stereotype awareness = self-rated belief that a

negative stereotype regarding aging and cognitive decline exists (out of 5, where 1= strongly disagree that there is a stereotype that OAs are

inferior to YAs in terms of cognitive ability, 5 = strongly agree that there is a stereotype that OAs are inferior to YAs in terms of cognitive ability ).

MIA Achievement = Score on the memory achievement scale of the MIA (out of 75), with higher scores indicating more self-rated importance of

Page 13: supp.apa.orgsupp.apa.org/.../pag0000167/MWST_OA_supplemental.docx · Web viewDifferent mechanisms have been proposed to explain how stereotype threat affects task performance. In

good memory performance. MIA Anxiety = Score on the memory anxiety scale of the MIA (out of 65), with higher scores indicating more self-

rated anxiety while completing daily task that have a memory component.

Page 14: supp.apa.orgsupp.apa.org/.../pag0000167/MWST_OA_supplemental.docx · Web viewDifferent mechanisms have been proposed to explain how stereotype threat affects task performance. In

Figure 1. Analysis 1 examined overall TRI as a mediator of the relationship between stress and recall accuracy. Analysis 2 examined overall TRI as

a mediator of the relationship between overall OSPAN difficulty and recall accuracy. Analysis 3 examined overall TRI as a mediator of the

relationship between recall difficulty and recall accuracy. Analysis 4 examined proactive TRI as a mediator of the relationship between recall

difficulty and recall accuracy. Overall probe-caught TRI mediated the relationship between stress and recall accuracy, perceived task difficulty

and recall accuracy, and perceived recall difficulty and recall accuracy. Proactive probe-caught TRI mediated the relationship between perceived

recall difficulty and recall accuracy. a paths indicate the effects of the IVs on the mediators, b paths indicate the direct effects of mediators on

the DVs, c paths indicate the total effect of the IVs on the DVs, and c’ paths indicate the direct effect of the IVs on the DVs. * indicates p < .05. **

indicates p < .001.

Page 15: supp.apa.orgsupp.apa.org/.../pag0000167/MWST_OA_supplemental.docx · Web viewDifferent mechanisms have been proposed to explain how stereotype threat affects task performance. In

Figure 2. Mediation analyses excluding younger adults. The analysis examined overall TRI as a mediator of the relationship between recall

difficulty and recall accuracy. a paths indicate the effects of the IVs on the mediators, b paths indicate the direct effects of mediators on the DVs,

c paths indicate the total effect of the IVs on the DVs, and c’ paths indicate the direct effect of the IVs on the DVs. * indicates p < .05. ** indicates

p < .001.