Sound Structure

36
Sound Structure Part II: Phonology 1-28-2009

description

 

Transcript of Sound Structure

Page 1: Sound Structure

Sound Structure

Part II: Phonology

1-28-2009

Page 2: Sound Structure

Review of Phonetics

• Speech sounds are decomposable into articulatory primitives (also known as features)

• Consonants and Vowels• Feature differences (e.g., voiced vs.

voiceless, nasal vs. not nasal, labial vs. alveolar vs. velar) lead to the diversity of sounds across languages

• We seem to perceive speech sounds as discrete units rather than continuous acoustic signals

Page 3: Sound Structure

Seeing Speech

Page 4: Sound Structure

Further aspects of sound: Cognitive• How important is speech to language?

• The phoneme: the basic, distinctive sounds of a language– What it means to be distinctive

• How words are organized into subparts (syllables and other units) for the determination of stress

• Knowledge of language--the internal “grammar” in your mind--will get more and more abstract than the physiological aspects of language

Page 5: Sound Structure

Cross-Linguistic Differences

• Everyone has encountered a language that contains a speech sound that is not in their native language

• As a general point, languages differ greatly both in terms of how many phonemes they have and in terms of which phonemes they have– Number of phonemes:

•Many: Some Khoisan languages, around 140 phonemes

•Few: E.g. Hawaiian, 13 phonemes

Page 6: Sound Structure

Being Distinctive

• We refer to the phonemes above as distinctive because they make contrasts between different words

• This can be illustrated for stops by using minimal pairs: a pair of words that differ in only one phoneme: pill bill [p] vs. [b]

till dill [t] vs. [d]

kill gill [k] vs. [g]

Page 7: Sound Structure

Lost in Translation

Page 8: Sound Structure

R vs. L

• Many Korean and Japanese speakers have trouble learning the contrast between R and L in English

• It is NOT because these sounds are absent in the native language– Seoul vs. Korea

– Notice that they are NOT in contrastive positions (l at the end of a syllable but r at the front)

– But English uses R and L contrastively: minimal pairs

– bLue~bRew, Light~Right, maLt~maRt

– Phonology is not just about the sound inventory, it’s also about how sounds are put in use

Page 9: Sound Structure

Differences that are not distinctive

• Some aspects of pronunciation are not distinctive.– Example: aspiration (puffing air)

pit vs. spit– The former [p] is aspirated, but the [p] in the latter is

not– But: the distinction between aspirated and non-

aspirated [p] is not distinctive in English (although it is in other languages). That is, in English there are no pairs like

[phIt] ‘hole in the ground, etc.’

[pIt] (whatever this might be)

Rule of thumb: come up with minimal pairs as a test for phonemes

Page 10: Sound Structure

Transcribing differences

• When we transcribe speech sounds using the IPA notation, we may do so in different ways.– If we are interested in every phonetic detail,

we would indicate effects like aspiration in English, even if it is not distinctive ([phIt])

– If we are interested more in the phonological inventory, we would omit the aspiration, as it is not distinctive ([pIt])

• For our purposes we will be concentrating on the latter type

• Sometimes when we focus on phonology, an abstract representation, we use slashes, e.g. /p/

Page 11: Sound Structure

Phonemes and Allophones

• Sometimes the same phoneme is pronounced in different ways depending on its context

• The variants of a phoneme are called allophones of that phoneme

• When we are talking about such distinctions, the phoneme is in slashes /…/ and the allophones are in square brackets […]

• The aspiration of e.g. /p/ is a case of this type; we say that /p/ in English has the allophones [p] and [ph]

Page 12: Sound Structure

Phonemes and Allophones, cont.• So, for instance, the phoneme /p/ appears in

each of the following words:

pit

spit• How, the first contains the allophone [ph],

while the second contains [p]• In fact, the rule for aspiration in English is

more general & complex:1. English voiceless stops (e.g., p, k, etc.) are

• Aspirated if word initial, or syllable-initial preceding a stressed vowel:

Compare récord vs. recórd

b. Otherwise unaspirated.

Page 13: Sound Structure

Phonemes: Nasalized vowels

• e.g. English speakers have not memorized any nasal vowels

• but English speakers do make nasal vowels: mat [mӕt] vs man [mǣn]

• We have one phoneme that can be realized phonetically as nasal or oral

• One phoneme /ӕ/ with two allophones [ӕ] and [ǣ]

Page 14: Sound Structure

Implications for learning words

• When we learn words, we don’t memorize their pronunciations directly

• We memorize the abstract phoneme representations (e.g., /pit/ and /spit/)

• The aspiration rule will turn /p/ in the former to an aspirated [ph]

• This saves a tremendous amount of memory but involves online computation

Page 15: Sound Structure

Phonemes and Allophones

allophone

allophone

Page 16: Sound Structure

Finding Phonemes: More in recitation

• The phonemes differ from language to language.

• How do we figure out what the phonemes of a language are?

• One trick is to look for minimal pairs (pӕt bӕt)

Page 17: Sound Structure

Finding Phonemes

• Minimal pairs are two words that have different meanings, but differ in only one sound sip/zip, day/bay, ram/ran/rang

• Since the difference between the sounds is meaningful, it must be stored in memory.

• Our minimal pairs above let us conclude that: – s/z are distinct phonemes, – d/b are distinct phonemes, – m/n/ŋ are distinct phonemes

Page 18: Sound Structure

Finding Phonemes

• Sometimes it isn’t possible to find minimal pairs for all sounds, but speakers can tell whether a contrast would yield a distinct possible word, even if it’s not a real word.

• e.g. “bat” vs “bap”: I know that “bat” is a word and that “bap” isn’t, and that “bap” is a possible word. So /t/ /p/ are distinct phonemes.

Page 19: Sound Structure

Finding Phonemes

• Same procedure with vowels

• e.g. beat/bit/bait/bet/bat/but/boot/boat/bought

• When working on phonological problems, first look for minimal pairs. Yes = phonemes

Page 20: Sound Structure

Rules of Pronunciation

• [vowel] [nasal] / __ [nasal consonant]

• the kind of sound that changes

• in this rule, it’s vowels

• Note: man but not mat

• Note: man and can and tan and san(k)

Page 21: Sound Structure

Rules of Pronunciation

• [vowel] [nasal] / __ [nasal consonant]

• “becomes”

Page 22: Sound Structure

Rules of Pronunciation

• [vowel] [nasal] / __ [nasal consonant]

• the change

• here, nasal

• Notice: I didn’t put [nasal vowel]--I don’t have to put vowel b/c that doesn’t change

• Only put what changes = simpler, less to remember

Page 23: Sound Structure

Rules of Pronunciation

• [vowel] [nasal] / __ [nasal consonant]

• “when”

Page 24: Sound Structure

Rules of Pronunciation

• [vowel] [nasal] / __ [nasal consonant]

• This is the environment that causes the change

• The underlining shows the position of the sound that’s changing

• Here: before a nasal consonant

Page 25: Sound Structure

Rules of Pronunciation

• To show “after a nasal consonant”, we could have done this:

• [nasal consonant] ___

• To show “between a nasal consonant and a nasal consonant”, we would have done this:

• [nasal consonant] ___ [nasal consonant]

Page 26: Sound Structure

Rules of Pronunciation

• So, English speakers unconsciously know the following rule:

• [vowel] [nasal] / __ [nasal, consonant]

• “vowels become nasal when before a nasal consonant”

• This is a rule of assimilation, making sounds more similar.

• Rules of dissimilation (making sounds less similar) also exist, but are less common

Page 27: Sound Structure

Phonemes and their distributions• Other cases of distinctive features lead to

some interesting observations

• Consider the nasals:– rum run rung

• These phonemes are distinct at the end of the word; but, [ng], unlike the other two, has the property that it never occurs word-initially in English– map nap *ngap

• In order to understand these patterns, we have to move from the phonemes to the principles by which phonemes are organized into words and other units.

Page 28: Sound Structure

The Syllable

• A familiar notion is that of the syllable: as in, ‘Philadelphia’ has five syllables– Newborns perceive speech in terms of

syllables– Basically, each vowel corresponds to a

syllable

• A refined set of hypothesis about the syllable is important for many linguistic generalizations

• Definitions (initial):– Onset: the beginning of the syllable– Nucleus: vowel in the middle of the syllable– Coda: consonant(s) at the end of a syllable

Page 29: Sound Structure

Syllable Structure

Legislator: le-gi-sla-tor; four syllables (σ)

σ

Onset Rhyme k

Nucleus Coda æ t

Monosyllabic cat:

Page 30: Sound Structure

Onsets and Speech Errors

Spoonerisms (Rev. Dr. W. A. Spooner, 1844-1930)

Target: dear old queenOutput: queer old dean

Target: You have wasted the whole termOutput: You have tasted the whole worm.

Target: You missed my history lectures.Output: You hissed my mystery lectures.

Page 31: Sound Structure

Further aspects of the syllable• Onset:

– English normally allows two consonants.

– [s] can be added initially in many cases as well, resulting in onsets with three consonants (e.g. splash)

– All sounds can occur in this position with the exception of [ng]. Thus the subdivision of the syllable is crucial for stating this generalization.

• Coda:– English normally allows two consonants,

although again there are cases where more stack up (e.g. belts)

Page 32: Sound Structure

Syllables and well-formedness• Conditions on syllable structure define a

set of (phonologically) possible words in a language; for instance– Actual words: brick, true, free, crab, etc.

– Non-words that are possible words of English: blick, clee, flork

– Impossible words: *bnick, *fnee, *dmay

– Words in which historical change has made an initial consonant silent: knee, knight, gnat

– Another reason that we don’t just memorize words but form generalizations over them

Page 33: Sound Structure

Differences across languages• Languages differ in terms of the constraints

they impose on syllable structure:– E.g. Hawaiian:

• No coda consonants• Maximum of one consonant per onset•Examples: ink > 'înika

Norman > Nolemana

– E.g. Polish: many consonant clusters at the beginning of words that are impossible in English:

bzdura "nonsense"

babsk "witch"

grzbiet [gzhbyet] "back"

marnotrawstw [mar-no-trafstf] "of wastes"

Page 34: Sound Structure

Infixation: more on this next week

Suffix: Attached to the end of a word (work-ed)Prefix: Beginning (un-important)Infix: Inside a word

What is an example of an infix in English? There is at least one phenomenon with the relevant properties.

this illustrates the basic principle that larger linguistic units are built out of smaller ones

Page 35: Sound Structure

Expletive infixation

Expletive Infixation is not something that ourEnglish teachers instruct us in; yet we know a great deal about it – what’s the rule?

Go home and try with your friends & TasIt has to do with stress patterns of language

inde-f*cking-pendent unrea-f*cking-listic

*indepen-f*cking-dent *unreali-f*cking-stic

Page 36: Sound Structure

Summary

• Articulatory features

• Phonemes

• Syllables

• Feet

• Words

• Sentences