Solomon y Corbit - 1978 - An Opponent-process Theory of Motivation

14
An Opponent-Process Theory of Motivation Richard L. Solomon; John D. Corbit The American Economic Review, Vol. 68, No. 6. (Dec., 1978), pp. 12-24. Stable URL: http://links.jstor.org/sici?sici=0002-8282%28197812%2968%3A6%3C12%3AAOTOM%3E2.0.CO%3B2-3 The American Economic Review is currently published by American Economic Association. Your use of the JSTOR archive indicates your acceptance of JSTOR's Terms and Conditions of Use, available at http://www.jstor.org/about/terms.html. JSTOR's Terms and Conditions of Use provides, in part, that unless you have obtained prior permission, you may not download an entire issue of a journal or multiple copies of articles, and you may use content in the JSTOR archive only for your personal, non-commercial use. Please contact the publisher regarding any further use of this work. Publisher contact information may be obtained at http://www.jstor.org/journals/aea.html. Each copy of any part of a JSTOR transmission must contain the same copyright notice that appears on the screen or printed page of such transmission. The JSTOR Archive is a trusted digital repository providing for long-term preservation and access to leading academic journals and scholarly literature from around the world. The Archive is supported by libraries, scholarly societies, publishers, and foundations. It is an initiative of JSTOR, a not-for-profit organization with a mission to help the scholarly community take advantage of advances in technology. For more information regarding JSTOR, please contact [email protected]. http://www.jstor.org Tue Feb 19 14:08:32 2008

Transcript of Solomon y Corbit - 1978 - An Opponent-process Theory of Motivation

Page 1: Solomon y Corbit - 1978 - An Opponent-process Theory of Motivation

An Opponent-Process Theory of Motivation

Richard L. Solomon; John D. Corbit

The American Economic Review, Vol. 68, No. 6. (Dec., 1978), pp. 12-24.

Stable URL:

http://links.jstor.org/sici?sici=0002-8282%28197812%2968%3A6%3C12%3AAOTOM%3E2.0.CO%3B2-3

The American Economic Review is currently published by American Economic Association.

Your use of the JSTOR archive indicates your acceptance of JSTOR's Terms and Conditions of Use, available athttp://www.jstor.org/about/terms.html. JSTOR's Terms and Conditions of Use provides, in part, that unless you have obtainedprior permission, you may not download an entire issue of a journal or multiple copies of articles, and you may use content inthe JSTOR archive only for your personal, non-commercial use.

Please contact the publisher regarding any further use of this work. Publisher contact information may be obtained athttp://www.jstor.org/journals/aea.html.

Each copy of any part of a JSTOR transmission must contain the same copyright notice that appears on the screen or printedpage of such transmission.

The JSTOR Archive is a trusted digital repository providing for long-term preservation and access to leading academicjournals and scholarly literature from around the world. The Archive is supported by libraries, scholarly societies, publishers,and foundations. It is an initiative of JSTOR, a not-for-profit organization with a mission to help the scholarly community takeadvantage of advances in technology. For more information regarding JSTOR, please contact [email protected].

http://www.jstor.orgTue Feb 19 14:08:32 2008

Page 2: Solomon y Corbit - 1978 - An Opponent-process Theory of Motivation

An Opponent-Process Theory of M o t i ~ a t i o n ~ ~ * ~ ~

RICHARD L. SOLOMON JOHN D. CORBIT University of Pennsylvania Brown University

The paper by Solomon and Corbit is regarded by many psychologists as the most successful attempt so far of providing a general theory capable of explaining both psychological addiction and some people's acquired taste for exposing themselves to extreme danger. Apart from the omission of two somewhat technical sections, in which the authors relate their hypothesis to Pavlovian conditioning, the paper is reprinted in full. It is the first of a series of articles: the one which presents the hypothesis and gives an overview of its applications. The subse- quent articles contain accounts of experiments designed to test the hypothesis.*

The behavioral hypothesis is clearly one the postulate of consumer rationality cannot very well explain. Addiction, like externalities and non-convexities, is one of those awkward exceptions that do not fit into the economist's standard models. We might, perhaps, be excused for ignoring it, if addiction were confined to a few chemical substances and due to physiological causes; but there is a growing suspicion of its being a much more general phenomenon. The psychologists' motivation theory has a large overlap with our theory of consumer preference; the Solomon and Corbit article has been chosen as a good example of their approach to a problem we have not yet considered though are likely, sooner or later, to feel compelled to consider. The approach of the arti- cle differs from and should supplement that of the more general theory of motivation which is built around the concept of arousal and formed the core of Scitovsky's account of the psychologists' view of individual behavior.**

T. Scitovsky

First, we describe the kind of phenomenon which has in her breast and immediately is terrified. She sits still, caught our attention. Two fictitious examples will intermittently weeping, or she paces the floor. After a suffice. In the first, a woman at work discovers a lump few hours, she slowly regains her composure, stops cry-

ing and begins to work. At this point, she is still tense *R. L. Solomon and J . D. Corbit, Journal of Abnormal Psy- and disturbed, but no longer terrified and distracted.

chology. Vol. 81, pp. 158-71, 1973. She manifests the symptoms usually associated with Howard S. Hoffman and R. L. Solomon, Learning and Motiva- intense anxiety. While in this state she calls her doctor

tion. Vol. 5, pp. 149-64, 1974. for an appointment. A few hours later she is in his R. L. Solomon in Psychopathology: Experimental Methods, edit-ed by J . D. Maser and M. E. P. Seligman, 1977, pp. 66-103. office, still tense, still frightened: She is obviously a

R. L. Solomon in Learning Mechanisms in Food Selection, edited very unhappy woman. The doctor makes his examina- by L. M. Barker, M. R. Best, and M. Domjan, 1977. tion. He then informs her that there is no possibility of

M. D. Starr, Journalof ExperimentalPsychology, 1978 (in press). cancer, that there is nothing to worry about, and that **Cf. T. Scitovsky, The Joyless Economy (O.U.P., 1976, New her problem is just a clogged sebaceous gland requir- York) chapters 2 through 4. 'This research was supported by U. S. Public HealthserviceGrant ing no medical attention.

MH-04202 to the first author and Grant MH-16608 to the second A few minutes later, the woman leaves the doctor's author. We are grateful to Burton S . Rosner, Francis W. Irwin, and office, smiling, greeting strangers, and walking with an Martin E. P. Seligman for their painstaking and helpful editing of an unusually buoyant stride. Her euphoric mood per-earlier draft of this paper. Finally, we are indebted to Dorothea meates all her activities as she resumes her normal du- Jameson Huwich and Leo M. Hurvich, whose development of the Hering theory into their coherent, opponent-process color vision ties. She exudes joy, which is not in character for her. theory first suggested to us a new way of thinking about affect and A few hours later, however, she is working in her nor- hedonic process. mal, perfunctory way. Her emotional expression is

'A subtitle, "I. Temporal Dynamics of Affect," and an abstract back to normal. She once more has the personality im- were omitted in this reprinting.

'Reprinted by permission from Pschological Review, Vol. 81, No. mediately recognizable by all of her friends. Gone is 2, pp. 119-133, 142-145, 1974. Copyright 1974 by the American the euphoria, and there is no hint of the earlier terrify- Psychological Association. ing experience of that day.

Page 3: Solomon y Corbit - 1978 - An Opponent-process Theory of Motivation

13 SOLOMON AND CORBIT: THEORY OF MOTIVATION

In the second example, a couple have just begun sex- ual foreplay, and it is quite pleasurable. After a few moments of a constant level of mutual stimulation, the pleasure decreases somewhat. Normally, this decline would elicit behavior calculated to increase the intensi- ty of mutual stimulation and to maintain the high level of pleasure. Unfortunately, at that moment a tele-phone rings. One partner leaves and goes into another room to answer it, and the other partner lies alone the in bed. The abandoned partner experiences a quick de- cline of the pleasure, then becomes tense and irritated, and strongly craves a resumption of the sexual stimula- tion. Time goes by, however, and the other partner does not return. Finally, the abandoned partner gets out of bed, absentmindedly turns on the television set, and becomes absorbed in a news broadcast. Gone is the irritability and intense craving. There is no hint, in overt behavior, of the pleasurable sexual experience of a few minutes ago. A type of dispassionate normality now pervades.

We can distill from these two examples some impor- tant empirical features common to many hedonic, \

emotional, or affective experiences. First, following the sudden introduction of either a pleasurable or aversive stimulus, an affective or hedonic reaction begins and quickly rises to a peak. It then slowly declines to a steady level where it remains if the stimulus quality and intensity is maintained. Then, at the sudden ter- mination of the stimulus, the affective reaction quickly disappears and gives way to a qualitatively very differ- ent type of affective reaction which reaches its own peak of intensity and then slowly disappears with time.

Figure 1 diagrams these changes in hedonic or affec- tive state and illustrates what we call the standardpat- tern of afective dynamics. The pattern has five distinc-

PEAK OF PRIMARY

PRIMARY ADAPTATON PHASE -al AFFECT 0U m U

'5 INTENSITY OF 73al AFFECTIVE PEAK OF AFFECTIVE I AFTER -REACTION AFTER- REACTION

ON +loo OFF F

TIME Standard Pattern of Affective

Dynamics

FIGURE1. The standard pattern of affective dynamics, showing the five distinctive features: the peak of the primary affective reaction, the adaptation phase, the steady level, the peak of the affective after-reaction, and, finally, the decay of the after-reaction. (The heavy black bar represents the time during which the affect-arousing stimulus is present. The or- dinate represents two hedonic scales, each departing from neutrality, one for the primary affect, the other for the affec- tive after-reaction.)

tive features: (a) the peak of the primary hedonic process or state, precipitated by stimulus onset; (b) a period of hedonic or affective adaptation during which the intensity of the hedonic state declines, even though stimulus intensity is maintained; (c) a steady level of the hedonic process which continues as long as stimu- lus intensity is maintained; (d) a peak of afective after- reaction, which quickly follows stimulus termination, and whose quality is hedonically very different from that of the primary hedonic state; and (e) finally, the afterstate decays and subsequently disappears.

This standard pattern describes both fictitious ex- amples. In the first, the initial, primary hedonic or affective process was unpleasant and the after-reaction was pleasant. In the second, the primary process was pleasant and the after-reaction was unpleasant. The data of psychology contain literally dozens of examples of this kind. For brevity we will first describe some of the more interesting or important cases chosen from very different areas of psychological research. We can therefore demonstrate the great generality of the stan- dard pattern of affective dynamics. Then we will describe a theoretical model for the underlying mechanism.

Examples of Empirical Phenomena To Be Explained

Table 1 presents seven examples of emotional, affec- tive, hedonic, or motivational phenomena. Some are "behavioral," some "experiential." Some are exper- imental, others are observational, derived from com- mon everyday experiences. Finally, some are precipi- tated by pleasant, and some by unpleasant stimuli.

Example 1 describes behavior changes seen in a dog subjected to intense aversive stimulation. The example draws on selected parts of studies reported by Katcher, Solomon, Turner, LoLordo, Overmier, and Rescorla (1969) and Church, LoLordo, Overmier, Solomon, and Turner (1966). A dog in a Pavlov harness was stimulated by several 10-second shocks. The dog ap- peared to be terrified during the first few shocks. It screeched and thrashed about, its pupils dilated, its eyes bulged, its hair stood on end, its ears lay back, its tail curled between its legs. Expulsive defecation and urination, along with many other symptoms of intense autonomic nervous system activity, were seen. At this point, the dog was freed from the harness, it moved slowly about the room, appeared to be stealthy, hesi- tant, and unfriendly. Its "state" had suddenly changed from terror to stealthiness.

We now arbitrarily label the state during shock (the terror state or the peak of the primary reaction to shock) State "A." The stealthy state, right after shocks were terminated (the after-reaction), will be called State "B." In that way, we can temporarily ignore whether we have correctly labeled the states. We know that State A was not State B. Indeed, State A was very

Page 4: Solomon y Corbit - 1978 - An Opponent-process Theory of Motivation

AMERICAN ECONOMIC ASSOCIATION

TABLE 1

Examvle

Dogs in Pavlov harness, 10-second shocks, gross behavior

Dogs in Pavlov harness, 10-second shocks, electrocardiograph responses

Epstein's parachutists, free fall, gross behavior, physiology

Opiate users, intravenous injection, moods and feelings

Dogs and M & Ms, gross behavior

Love, interpersonal stimulation, moods, feelings

Imprinting, the attachment of creatures to their "mothers"

First few stimulations After many stimulations

State A (input present) State B (input gone) State A' (input present) State B' (input gone)

terror, panic

large cardiac acceleration

terror, autonomic nervous system arousal

euphoria, rush, pleasure

pleasure, tail wagging, chewing

ecstasy, excitement, happiness

pleasure, cessation of fear, no distress

stealth (subdued, unhappy (annoyed, cautious, inactive, anxious, afraid) hesitant)

slow deceleration, small acceleration or small overshoot none

1 stunned, stony-faced I tense, eager, I I expectant

craving, aversive loss of euphoria, withdrawal signs, normal feeling, short duration relief

-tenseness, motionless

loneliness normal, comfortable, content

loneliness, distress pleasure, no cries cries, short duration

joy (euphoric, active, social), happy

quick decleration, large overshoot

I exhilaration,I jubilation

intense craving, abstinence agony, long duration

grief, separation syndrome, long duration

loneliness, intense cries, long duration

different from State B, judging by the many behavioral changes observed when the dog was suddenly released. Furthermore, State B gradually disappeared. In a few minutes, the dog appeared to be normal, like its previous, preshock, natural, self: active, alert, and socially responsive. When this happened, it was impos- sible to tell by looking at the dog that either State A or B had just transpired. The evidence was gone. The dog had progressed from "normalcy" to A to B, and back to "normalcy." This sequential pattern will be seen in all of the examples given in Table 1. It is, we believe, the basic pattern for the dynamics of afect.

However, Example 1 has not yet been fully de- scribed. When the same dog was brought back for the same treatment day after day, its behavior gradually changed. During shocks, the signs of terror disap-peared. Instead, the dog appeared pained, annoyed, or anxious, but not terrified. For example, it whined rather than shrieked, and showed no further urination, defecation, or struggling. Then, when released sudden- ly at the end of the session, the dog rushed about, jumped up on people, wagged its tail, in what we called at the time "a fit of joy." Finally, several minutes later,

the dog was its normal self: friendly, but not racing about. Here again, the sequence was normal- State A-State B+normal, where State A accompanied the arousing input, and State B directly followed the sud- den termination of that input, and then slowly died out.

We wish to emphasize that the qualitative and quan- titative features of States A and B during later shocks were not the same as those for States A and B during the first few shocks. This pattern of changes, occurring as a consequence of repeated exposures to the stimulus input which causes A, also is, we believe, typical of the basic pattern of affective dynamics. Because the later States A and B were not identical to the earlier states, we label the later ones A' and B', respectively.

Look at Example 2 in Table 1. A dog was in an ex- periment in which its heartbeat was measured by an electrocardiograph during repeated shocks to its hind feet. The observations are from Church et al. (1966). During the first few shocks, there was a large cardiac acceleration; in some dogs it was an increase of 150 beats per minute. At shock termination, the rate sud- denly decreased, and within 5 seconds it descended below the baseline rate. It often fell as low as 20 to 30

Page 5: Solomon y Corbit - 1978 - An Opponent-process Theory of Motivation

15 SOLOMON AND CORBIT: THEORY OF MOTIVATION

0 2.5 5:0 2'5 ld.0

Time After Shock Onset (~ec)

TerminationTime After FIGURE2. Heart rate changes as a function of shock onset,

maintenance, and termination. (There is a decline following the initial peak reaction to shock onset. There is a decelera- tory "overshoot" following shock termination, and then the heart rate slowly returns to baseline rate. Note that the eight-milliampere shock produces a bigger heart rate in- crease and a bigger deceleration than does the four-milli- ampere shock.) (Adapted from an article by Russell M. Church, Vincent LoLordo, J. Bruce Overmier, Richard L. Solomon, and Lucille H. Turner appearing in the August 1966 Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psy- chology. Copyrighted by the American Psychological As- sociation, Inc., 1966.)

beats per minute below baseline rate. Then, it slowly "recovered" to baseline rate, over a period of as long as 30 to 60 seconds. The below-baseline excursion has been called "vagal overshoot." It is a well-studied phenomenon. Figure 2 is taken from Church et al. (1966) and demonstrates these dynamic events. The figure also suggests that there may be a relationship between intensity of the A state and the magnitude and duration of the B state. Now we can define baseline, State A, State B, and the return to baseline using an electrodardiograph measurement instead of gross ob- servations of emotional behavior.

States A and B changed markedly after several ses- sions. Shock onset now caused little, if any, increase in heart rate. Any increase was momentary. The rate of- ten decreased even while the shock was still on. How-

ever, when shock was suddenly terminated, the "over- shoot" was much larger than it was on early shock trials. Heart rate in some individual cases descended to 50 to 60 beats per minute (sometimes more than 50 beats per minute below baseline rate), and recovery to baseline took as long as two to five minutes. As Katcher et al. (1969) put it, "The deceleratory heart- rate overshoot produced by stimulus termination shows shortened latencies . . . and greater magnitude over trials [p. 1721." Thus, States A and B have changed: A' seems to be weaker than A, B' stronger than B, and B' longer lasting than B. But, as in the previous examples, we can still identify the sequence: baseline+A+B+baseline.

Example 3 comes from Epstein's (1967) report of physiological, emotional-expressive, and social reac-tions of parachutists. When parachutists make their first jump, they are often terrified, judging by tele- metered autonomic responses and photographed facial expressions. When they land safely, they look stony- faced or stunned for several minutes, then gradually resume normal composure. After the parachutists have made several jumps and are experts, their responses are different. When jumping, they are no longer ter- rified. They may be anxious, tense, or even eager. After they land safely, they feel exuberant, exhilarated, and good. They like the feeling, and the mood lasts some- times for hours. Such parachutists love to jump because of this after-feeling. Again, we see that the qualitative and quantitative attributes of States A and B have changed with the repetition of eliciting condi- tions. However, two very different states are still ob- servable, and with each stimulation input, we can iden- tify the sequence: baseline+A+B+baseline.

Example 4 represents states in opiate use (Jaffe, 1965; Maurer & Vogel, 1967). Early in a history of opiate use, the user experiences the "rush" (an intense- ly pleasurable feeling) directly after the opiate injec- tion, followed by a period of less intense euphoria. Then, with further passage of time, the user suffers aversive, painful, and frightening somatic withdrawal symptoms, together with a feeling of craving. Here again, we see a baseline+A+B+baseline sequence. However, with opiates, B may last a long time, some- times for several days.

After repeated dosages of opiates over several weeks, State A begins to weaken, and a t the same time State B begins to intensify and takes longer to return to baseline. State A' is called "normal" rather than eu- phoric. The rush is no longer experienced. Yet, State B' is more physiologically disturbing than B was, and lasts much longer than did State B. The craving aspect of State B' is now extremely intense, aversive, and en- during. It is called abstinence agony. It can be months before B' returns to baseline. Perhaps it never really does. It is a ghastly experience.

The repeated use of some drugs results in the behavioral phenomenon of addiction. People find

Page 6: Solomon y Corbit - 1978 - An Opponent-process Theory of Motivation

16 AMERICAN ECONOMIC ASSOCIATION

themselves craving a substance in which they previous- ly had little interest. It is the most vivid instance of ac- quired motivation, because of its intensity and dura- tion. It also provides a fruitful, empirical model for analyzing many kinds of acquired motivation. Indeed, we later show, with examples taken from love and affection, social attachment, and imprinting in birds, that addiction does not differ in principle from any ac-quired motivational system. We can easily describe opiate, alcohol, barbiturate, amphetamine, or cigarette addiction (see Solomon & Corbit, 1973) within the empirical framework of the analysis we have proposed. They all have four attributes: (a) The B' state lasts a long time; (b) the B' state is intensely aversive; (c) the elicitation of State A or A' is effective in causing im- mediate removal of State B or B'; and ( d ) the user learns to employ the drug which elicits States A and A' in order to get rid of State B or B'.

A lasting cycle of addiction will not arise, even though A and B are repeatedly experienced, if the properties of affective response to a drug are such that B fades out to baseline very quickly. This is true because another dosage of the drug is never needed in order to get rid of the aversive B state. It quickly gets rid of itself. This is the case in Example 5, a common type of pleasurable situation. A laboratory dog is sit- ting dejectedly in a cage. It is suddenly handed one M & M candy. It wags its tail, moves about, chews, and swallows. This reflects State A. It smacks its lips a few times, curls its tongue across its lips, then becomes mo- tionless and tense. It orients toward the experimenter and focuses its eyes on the experimenter's hand (the one which held the single M & M). Assume this to reflect State B. Then, if another M & M is not forth- coming, the B state dies out after about 15 to 30 seconds. The dog moves away, walks around the cage, sniffs here and there, begins to "ignore" the exper- imenter, then sits again. It has returned to its original state. Here again, termination of one hedonic event has precipitated another state not present prior to the onset of stimulation, and the second state disappeared by it- self merely with the passage of time. In this case, A is pleasant, so we infer B to be qualitatively different, and probably craving is the best term for it. The so-called "peanut phenomenon" in humans is comparable. Once you start eating peanuts, it is hard to stop unless the cy- cle is interrupted for a period longer than the time required for the peanut craving, or B state, to die out. This has been called a mini-addiction.' Indeed, the case of the couple interrupted during mutual sexual stimulation, described in the Introduction, is certainly similar.

In Example 6, the pleasurable input generates a con- dition in which the B state typically lasts a lot longer than that for an M &M or a peanut, and so the favor-

'This term was first suggested by Eliot Stellar at a cocktail party.

able conditions for addiction are present. A boy and girl "fall in love." This State A is characterized by pleasurable excitement, frequent sexual feelings, a prevailing mood of ecstasy, happiness, and good feel- ings. When the lovers, whose multimodal mutual stimulation will cause State A, are separated from each other, they will feel lonely, sad, and depressed (State B). Even with anticipations of reunion (sym- bolic, conditioned arousers of State A) loneliness may prevail. Actual reunion will simultaneously erase B and reinstate A just as described in Attribute c of ad- diction to opiates.

After several years of repeated mutual stimulation, the qualitative and quantitative changes in A and B are a matter of public lore. State A' is characterized (if all has gone well) as contentment, normalcy, and comfort. But State B' is now potentially of high intensity and long duration. If it should occur, it is often called grief or, as Bowlby (1952) has described it in children, the "separation syndrome." It requires a lot of time for this B' state to decay. The partners have become ad- dicted to one another, and when separated they expe- rience withdrawal symptoms. As in all the previous five examples, note that the sudden termination of the stimulus that arouses A or A' leads to the occurrence of B or B' before the eventual return to emotional base- line or normalcy. In this case, the termination of A', even though A' does not manifest itself as intensely as did A, is followed by a more powerful and much more protracted B' state. This is the same pattern seen in all the previous examples, whether the A state is pleasur- able or aversive.

Example 7 is one of imprinting. If we take the duck- ling as our subject, it shows the same patterns of affect revealed in opiate addiction. First, right after hatching there are very few distress cries. Indeed, the duckling may appear to be quite happy with its new environ- ment. Then, if the duckling is exposed to a white, mov- ing object, it looks intently at it. Also, if there were any distress vocalizations, they tend to disappear. How- ever, if the moving object is then removed from view (Hoffman, 1968; Hoffman, Stratton, Newby, & Bar-rett, 1970), there is a burst of distress crying which may last for several minutesand then disappear. With successive presentations and removals of the imprint- ing stimulus, the frequency and intensity of distress crying will increase.

The efficacy of the presentation of the moving stimulus in eliminating the distress cries appears to be optimal at the outset. Then the duckling can be shaped to push a pole in order to present itself with the im- printing stimulus. At that point, of course, the duck- ling is exhibiting all criteria for addictive behavior. It is "hooked" on the imprinting object, the presence of which is a positive reinforcer and the absence of which is an aversive event.

Note that the ethological description of imprinting as the sudden establishment of "following behavior,"

Mario
Resaltado
Page 7: Solomon y Corbit - 1978 - An Opponent-process Theory of Motivation

17 SOLOMON AND CBRBIT: THEORY OF MOTIVATION

released by an adequate imprinting stimulus, is utterly inappropriate in the light of our analysis. The moving object releases some affective State A, presumably an unconditioned, pleasant emotional reaction to the moving stimulus. The removal of the stimulus then precipitates State B, which is an aversive event. State B intensifies with repeated stimulations. Ducklings will then work on an avoidance schedule to prevent the dis- appearance of the imprinting object, just as the opiate addict will develop anticipatory behavior which prevents the occurrence of a t least the more intense withdrawal symptoms.

Empirical Generalizations

First, in all seven empirical examples, as well as in the fictitious ones in our Introduction, the sudden onset of some new stimulus aroused an affect or hedonic state not present prior to onset. The state terminated when the stimulus terminated. Then, a new state ap- peared, qualitatively unlike either the prestimulation state or the state produced by the onset and mainte- nance of the stimulus. Finally, this new poststimulus state persisted for a while and died out. The baseline state eventually returned. In none of the examples did the subject's affective state return directly to baseline upon cessation of stimulation. Baseline was regained via some new state which became manifest a t stimulus termination, and then slowly died away.

Second, in some cases the states changed in their quality and intensity with successive, repeated stimu- lations. Whenever this occurred, the A states became weaker and the B states stronger and longer lasting.

These two phenomena, the dynamic hedonic re-sponse pattern, and its modification with repeated ex- perience, were seen whether the A state was pleasur- able or aversive.

The Explanatory Model

In our opinion, the simplest theoretical model that organizes these typical motivational phenomena (and countless other phenomena of innate and acquired mo- tivation) is an opponent-process system. It was bor- rowed from previously developed accounts of sensory dynamics (Hurvich & Jameson, 1957). The primary a process for a given hedonic state is aroused by its adequate stimulus. W e then imagine a single opponent loop generating the secondary b process and having an hedonic sign opposite to that of the state aroused by the input. The loop generating the b process is activated whenever any input evokes a sufficient hedonic con- sequence. The b process is sluggish, so it has a relative- ly long latency, recruits slowly, and dies out slowly. Finally, the b process is strengthened by use and weak- ened by disuse.

Because many formal properties of the patterns of both sensory and affective phenomena seem so similar, we must show at the outset that they are not identical and that the standard pattern of affective dynamics is not a direct consequence of the pattern of sensory dy- namics. Otherwise, one could explain all hedonic or affective dynamics in terms of sensory events. For example, in the case of the woman who discovered the lump in her breast, one might argue that her steady- level anxiety state was less intense than her initial peak of terror a t the discovery of the lump because of sen- sory adaptation; that is, the perceived presence of and magnitude of the lump decreased. We can refute such an interpretation, leaning heavily on the fact that the time course of typical sensory dynamics is of a com- pletely different order of magnitude from that of the standard pattern of affective dynamics reflected in Figure 1 and Table 1. Sensory changes usually occur in a matter of milliseconds, seconds, and minutes, where- as the emotional changes usually occur in minutes, hours, days, weeks, and months. There is, therefore, a theoretical necessity for a distinct opponent-process mechanism for affect and motivation independent of mechanisms for sensory dynamics.

Figure 3 illustrates the type of system that we have in mind. Panel A shows two stages of information processing, a cognitive-perceptual stage that converts the stimulus to an informational signal, and an affec- tive or hedonic stage that converts the informational signal to an affective signal. The affective system in Figure 3 receives a square wave input, and follows it with a dynamic affective response of the standard pat- tern shown in Figure 1.

Perhaps an example will clarify the point of Figure 3. The sight of a dog is a fear-arousing stimulus for a cat. The dog represents a complex, multidimensional display, and the perception of the dog is categorical, all-or-none. The stimulus sequence is as follows: The dog enters the cat's environment, remains for a while, and then leaves. The cognitive-perceptual sequence is: The dog is detected, continues to be detected as long as it remains, and then ceases to be detected when it leaves. There is no adaptation (i.e., the dog does not become less doglike), nor is there any appreciable sen- sory after-reaction a t stimulus removal (i.e., no nega- tive [antidog?] afterimage). The affective sequence, in contrast, will show the primary reaction and after- reaction components: intense fear a t first, subsiding to a steady level, and then, when the dog is gone, the ap- pearance of another state, very different from the first. The after-reaction (relief?) then gradually dies out.

Figure 3, Panel A, shows that the affective system follows its square wave input with an output having the complex dynamic features of Figure 1. How can we ac- count for this behavior? The affective system in Panel A is shown as a single stage. Now, we will open the "black box" for this stage and look inside. Our pro- posal for the mechanism responsible for affective dy-

Mario
Resaltado
Mario
Resaltado
Page 8: Solomon y Corbit - 1978 - An Opponent-process Theory of Motivation

18 AMERICAN ECONOMIC ASSOCIATION

Panel A. THE PROCESSING SYSTEM

no

Cognitive- Affective k c t w l or Hbdonic

Xow: Stage . b Envimmrntal catwicd, ' Cognitive- ' Affective stirnukn: signal ados

network YWmmdetection

Panel B. THE AFFECTIVE STAGE OF THE SYSTEM 1

FIRSTJCOMPONENT - COMPONENT -

Perceptual hedonic tone Reinforcer quality and intensity;

I Hedonic output + I standard

Process b:' opponent- b - signalof fect

opponent -emotion

opponent -hedonic tone opponent -

SECOND feelingCOMPONENT opponent-

FIGURE3. Panel A: The detection of an environmental stimulus by a cognitive-perceptual mechanism, and the arousal of an affective stage yielding an affective signal which shows the standard pattern of Figure 1. Panel B: Kn analysis of the three com- ponents of the affective mechanism. (In the first component the a process is aroused. The second component, the b process, is aroused via the arousal of a. Then the third component, a summing device, combines the a and b signals to generate the standard pattern of affective dynamics.)

namics is shown in Figure 3, Panel B. Here, the affec- the affective signal, which shows the sequence of the tive stage is analyzed into its three component parts. peak primary reaction A, adaptation, steady level, The cognitive-perceptual stage has acted as a categor- after-reaction B, and decay of B. Thus, we see that the ical detector. The informational signal enters the affec: input from the perceptual-cognitive stage has an affec- tive system as input to the first component, the a tive, hedonic side effect, Process a. When it does, the process, which has a short time constant. The signal opponent loop is activated, calling into play the oppo- from the a process activates the second component or b nent process (which has a hedonic quality in some way process, which responds with a slow rise and a slow opposite to, and very different from, that of Process a ) . decay. The third component is a summing device that The opponent process, which we call Process b, reduces adds the a and b signals, and it generates as its output the hedonic intensity of the state which the input ini-

Page 9: Solomon y Corbit - 1978 - An Opponent-process Theory of Motivation

SOLOMON AND CORBIT: THEORY OF MOTIVATION

1 Panel A .

FIRST FEW STIMULATIONS

Panel B.

AFTER M A N Y STIMULATK)NS 1

First Few Stimulations

a+loo l o . b PEAK OF A

................ RESPONSE

UNDERLYING IJF-L A OPPONENT .. b ,,"PROCESSES ....... ' b ,' ......... J

STIMULUS

FIGURE4. Panel A: The operation of the summing device for the first few stimulations. (The summation of the under- lying opponent processes, a and 6, yields the manifest affec- tive response.) Panel B: The operation of the summing device after many repeated stimulations.

tially aroused. When the perceptual-cognitive input ceases, the opponent process reveals itself as "pure" State B, because the b process takes a while to decay.

The opponent process is a slave process: It is activat- ed indirectly via the activation of the a process. Pre- sumably, the slave process has an evocation threshold, a latency, a recruitment or augmentation time, and a decay function, all characteristic of a given opponent- process system. We will also see later that the oppo- nent process can, under proper conditions, be activated by events in memory, as a consequence of Pavlovian conditioning procedures.

The block diagram of the affect-control system in Figure 3 yields the temporal dynamics of affect shown in Figure 4, Panel A. There we first see a baseline state. Then the affect-arousing stimulus is presented and it stays on for 10 seconds. Next, it is suddenly terminat- ed. This simple event sequence activates the underly- ing opponent processes. First, there is a quick rise of Process a to a peak intensity. Shortly afterward, there is a slow recruitment of Process b. When the stimulus is terminated, Process a quickly goes to zero, but Process b, having a sluggish decay property, perseverates and dies out slowly. The resultant manifest dynamics of affect are a consequence of subtracting the b process from the a process. The subtraction of the two quanti- ties yields Manifest State A when a > b and yields Manifest State B when b > a. The intensity of the manifest state is given by the quantity I a - b 1 .

When we subtract the underlying processes, a and b from each other, we obtain the manifest affective re- sponse pattern shown in Figure 5. Immediately after stimulus onset, a is large and b is zero, thus yielding a peak of State A. As b is slowly recruited, the quantity

/ a - b 1 decreases, yielding a decline in the magnitude of State A. This is the adaptation phase shown in Figure 1, the standard pattern of affective dynamics. When the b process reaches an asymptote, there will be a steady level of State A. Then, when the stimulus is

Neutral o - - - - - - - _ I - - - - ---- Baseline\ ,."'DECAY ,,-,OF B

Q) PEAK OF B

+loo

TIME

FIGURE5. The manifest temporal dynamics generated by the opponent-process system during the first few stimula- tions. (The five features of the affective response are labeled.)

terminated, the intensity of a goes quickly to zero, but the b process dies away slowly. At this moment, b > a, yielding the peak and subsequent decay of State B. At this point, State B is pure b.

The theory represented in Figures 3 and 4 gives a rough, qualitative account of all of the data of Table 1, for the first few stimulations only. First, it explains peaks of intensity of affect or hedonic quality a t stimulus onset: The opponent process has not yet had enough time to get into action. So we see phenomena like terror, the rush, ecstasy, etc. Or, rate the pain- fulness of a 30-second shock. The peak painfulness is a t onset. For example, see the peak of heart rate in Figure 2.

The other major event explained by the theory is the emergence of the after-reaction, postulated to be a function of the opponent process, which becomes man- ifest after the termination of stimulus input. This emergence is due to pure b perseverating in time after the a has quickly disappeared. The B state slowly decays, and baseline is eventually attained.

In each of the seven empirical examples of Table 1, the manifest B state was, in some unspecified way, related to A, but not the same as A. The model desig- nates the relationship as oppositeness. But in what way can we say, for example, that loneliness is opposite to the pleasure produced by the presence of a loved one? Surely, they are hedonically opposed. This concept is built into the theoretical model. If A is pleasant, then B must be unpleasant. If A is a positive reinforcer, then B is a negative reinforcer. Other affective attributes of A and B remain an empirical question. The model tells us how to identify the attributes of oppositeness. All one has to do, for any given A state, is observe the at- tributes of affect which are revealed at the peak of B. Therefore, the model puts a constraint on what we call oppositeness. For example, the opposite of love must be the attributes of grief. To say that "Hate is the oppo-

Mario
Nota adhesiva
Esta gráfica se explica mediante la comparación del proceso sensorial con el predictivo, tal y como predice la teoría.
Page 10: Solomon y Corbit - 1978 - An Opponent-process Theory of Motivation

20 AMERICAN ECONOMIC ASSOCIATION

vein, disuse should weaken a b process, and it should '

site of love" is simply wrong, when at the death of a loved one, one experiences grief, not hate. The kinds of oppositeness which are generated by each A state are still, in many cases, waiting to be studied and named. Here is a vast, neglected area of investigation.

We postulated that the b process is a slave process. That means that at first it cannot be aroused directly by ordinary sensory inputs, but instead can arise only indirectly via the arousal of an a process (see Figure 3) and the subsequent activation of the opponent loop. This fits common sense. Try to imagine being grief- stricken without having loved someone. Try to imagine craving and abstinence agony without drug use. Try to imagine the exhilaration of the parachutist without any jump. On the other hand, we will point out later that it may be possible to arouse a b process directly by electrical or chemical stimulation of the brain, or to eliminate it by surgery, or to condition it by Pavlovian procedures. Initially, however, it is a slave process, inaccessible to direct environmental inputs, but indi- rectly arousable via hedonic and affective processes elicited by environmental inputs.

Strengthening of Opponent Processes by Repeated Stimulation

We have not yet explained the changes in hedonic dynamics brought about by repeated affect-arousing stimulations over a relatively long period of time. There are important differences between the A and B states on the left side of Table 1 as compared to the right side. Fortunately, one postulate brings order into the data: The opponent process is strengthened through use and weakened through disuse, but the primary affective process is not seriously affected by use. A b process will acquire more power if frequently elicited. It will show a shorter latency of response to a, a quicker rise, a higher asymptote, and a longer decay time. In contrast, an a process is a relatively stable, un- conditioned reaction. This seems reasonable for a sys- tem which is designed to minimize deviations from affective neutrality. Why should an opponent process not act like a defensive or immunization process, which produces antibodies more efficiently and in larger numbers in the face of repeated challenge? In the same

ing later stimulations the b process increases at a faster rate at stimulus onset and reaches a greater asymptotic intensity. In addition, the b process requires a much longer time to decay after the later stimulus termina- tions. As a consequence of these changes in the b process, the resultant pattern of the affective reaction changes so that the peak of A is considerably attenuat- ed, and the peak of B becomes much greater and longer lasting. Figure 4, Panel B, shows these simple alge- braic summations of the a and b processes after many repeated stimulations.

Figure 6 shows the pattern of emotional dynamics after many repeated stimulations and should be con- trasted with the pattern shown in Figure 5. Three phenomena are corollaries of the use postulate. First, the peak of A' will be less intense because the latency of the b process is decreased and its intensity is in- creased. Second, the steady level of A', I a - b 1 , during maintained stimulation will be close to baseline and perhaps even below it in some cases. Third, the peak of B' should be intense and long lasting, compared to what it was during early stimulation (Figure 5).

The events in Figure 6 do not follow from a simple aflective contrast model. If a principle of simple hedonic or affective contrast were operating, then the peak of B in Figure 5 should be greater in intensity than it is in Figure 6, because the intensity of A direct-ly prior to stimulus termination is greater in Figure 5 than it is in Figure 6. However, if we run down the de- scriptive adjectives for States A, B, A', and B' in Table 1 , even as loosely defined as they are, the opponent- process model works well, and a simple contrast model fails.

More convincingly, the objective data from the elec- trocardiograph experiments with dogs fit the oppo- nent-process model. In Table 1 we saw the following:

A, large acceleration; B, small overshoot, short duration;

I After Many Stimulations

+'O0

" LEVEL OF

Neutral 0 - - Baseline ;=-.. - -

slowly return to its original magnitude whenever its a process has not occurred for a long time.

Figure 4, Panel B, shows how the opponent processes will interact after the b process has been strengthened by repeated use. A comparison of Panels A and B ex-plains why the left-hand portion of Table 1, describing "early stimulation," differs from the right-hand por- tion of Table 1, describing "later stimulations."

The effect of repeated experiences on the hedonic re- sponse is confined to strengthening the b process. Dur-

! 1, ,' '-i PEAK OF B'

TIME FIGURE6. The manifest temporal dynamics generated by

the opponent-process system after many repeated stimula- tions. (The major features of the modified pattern are labeled.)

Mario
Resaltado
Page 11: Solomon y Corbit - 1978 - An Opponent-process Theory of Motivation

21 SOLOMON AND CORBIT: THEORY OF MOTIVATION

A', small or absent acceleration; B', large overshoot, longer lasting.

These findings fit the postulate that repetitions of affect-arousing stimulation, in this case shocks, will strengthen the opponent process which dampens the primary cardiac acceleration process. Furthermore, there are many signs of qualitative changes; e.g., signs of pleasure and euphoria appear in the period follow- ing termination of the shock session, although they were absent in earlier sessions. Our model cannot yet deal precisely with these qualitative changes which are produced by many repetitions of the same stimulus. A' is less intense than A, and B' is more intense and longer lasting than B. The model deduces these quantitative changes (Figure 4). In many cases, however, A' is qualitatively different from A, even though they have the same hedonic sign. Similarly B' and B are some- times qualitatively different, even though they have the same hedonic sign. When the model is fully developed, it must contain a rationale for these qualitative changes.

The observations on the right-hand side of Table 1 also support the assumption that many repetitions of pleasures will strengthen their aversive opponent processes. Aversive states, manifesting themselves after the sudden termination of pleasurable inputs, become more intense with repeated experiences. Mild loneliness later becomes grief. Mild craving later becomes abstinence agony and intense craving. In addition, after many repetitions the steady level of pleasure produced by the continued presence of the pleasurable stimulus input has decreased. The con- firmed opiate user experiences a "loss of euphoria," and the rush is gone. The pleasure-affective systems seem eventually to yield to opponent processes which keep departures from hedonic equilibrium relatively small. The aversive opponent process, when it is man- ifest, is more intense and longer lasting than it once was. So frequently repeated pleasure has its costs, psychologically, in an increased potentiality for dis- pleasure.

Similarly, for A' states aroused by aversive input, there is a "cost," but this cost is an increased poten- tiality for pleasure. Table 1 lists in the column under B' the opponent affects for the aversive A' states. Words like "joy," "exhilaration," and "good feeling" appear. These are the emotional costs of aversiveness. The model thus requires that any prolonged or repeated departures from hedonic or affective neutrality, regardless of hedonic sign, have a cost. Any significant departure from hedonic or affective neutrality should have correlates in increased autonomic and central nervous system activity aimed at reducing that depar- ture. The cost of this activity will not only be psy- chological, but also will be physiological (metabolic, hormonal, and neural).

We are assuming that prolonged exercise of an op- ponent-process system, whether it be pleasurable or aversive, might cause physiological stress in the same sense that Selye (1950) uses the term stress. That is, many physiological resources might be required in order to keep the opponent process strong. If we follow Selye's argument, we would expect that such a con- stant demand might lead to the exhaustion of a par- ticular overworked opponent-process system or to the debilitation of other defensive systems.

Thus we come to a major implication of our oppo- nent-process model: There probably are stresses caused by pleasurable stimulation just as there are stresses caused by aversive stimulation. Therefore, there should be adaptational costs as a consequence of both kinds of stresses. Furthermore, if we look for them, we should find diseases of adaptation for both, due to the correlated, physiological side effects of long- duration often-elicited intense b processes.

In the past, psychologists have identified psy-chological stress with aversion, pain, and unpleasant- ness. Theories of mental disease, psychosomatic dis- ease, and behavior disorders usually emphasize that aversiveness means stress, stress means aversiveness, and both cause emotional disorders, psychosomatic ill- nesses, and behavioral malfunctions. Our opponent- process theory of motivation puts all this to question. From our point of view, stresses caused by aversive stimulation are only one half of the story. There should be emotional disorders, psychosomatic diseases, and behavior disorders caused by long-lasting, repeated, and intense b processes in general, whether these op- ponent processes are pleasurable or aversive.

Take the case of the parachutists. Their operant behavior, when B' is strong, will be reinforced by the pleasurable experience of B'. But the cost of neutraliz- ing the innate aversiveness of a fall through space might be high, both physiologically and emotionally, just as Epstein (1967) has postulated. Even so, one might imagine, if there were no other pleasurable sen- sory inputs elsewhere available to the individual, that he might put himself through repeated aversive stimulation in order to experience the pleasurable B' state which would be both intense and lasting. From our point of view, this type of apparent masochistic behavior is not a mental disorder but is, rather, a reflection of the normal functioning of a healthy, au- tomatic, affect-control system. Thus, also, from this point of view, there is nothing abnormal or strange about addiction. It is only a socially vivid example of the normal hedonic and motivational functionings of an efficiently operating affect-control system.

Motivational systems involving pleasurable A states and aversive A states are similar. In both cases the onset, maintenance, and termination of the stimulus results in a certain amount of pleasure and a certain amount of displeasure. They differ mainly in whether

Page 12: Solomon y Corbit - 1978 - An Opponent-process Theory of Motivation

22 AMERICAN ECONOMIC ASSOCIATION

pleasure or displeasure comes first. In the case of the pleasurable A states, we can assume that the sub- sequent aversive B state functions as a drive that ener- gizes the performance of operants, and that the plea- surable A states may positively reinforce these oper- ants. Electrical self-stimulation of rewarding brain sites, chemical self-stimulation with opiates, and love relationships should work this way. In contrast, when the A state is aversive, and when the A state stimula- tion is absent, nothing functions as a drive to energize an operant upon which the A state is contingent. So we have the problem of how to get the behavior started. The B state for an aversive A state is not an energizer. Instead, it is a positive reinforcer. There is thus an im- portant asymmetry between motivational systems for pleasurable and aversive A states. Some outside ener- gizing influence is needed to get behavior started when the operant is followed by an aversive A state. Exam- ples of such behaviors include "thrill-seeking" be-haviors, such as parachute jumping, mountain climb- ing, automobile racing, etc., all of which involve an aversive component, followed by a pleasurable feeling of exhilaration. Why should one initiate an activity when its immediate effect is aversive, i.e., when a pun- ishment contingency exists? Some competing outside influence, such as social pressure from peers, is re- quired. However, after many repeated stimulations such outside influences may not be needed. Because the aversive A state is then weak and the subsequent, positively reinforcing B state is strong, the A state will function as a positive reinforcer. We know that this can happen when a weak shock signals the onset of food for cats (Masserman, 1943). Note that an outside influ- ence is not needed when the A state is pleasurable. These operants are energized by the aversive B state.

Relation of the Theory to Other Concepts

To the reader well versed in the history of theories of learning, two aspects of our model should now be ap- parent. First, the phenomena of acquired motivation produced merely' by the repetition of affect-arousing stimuli are nonassociative in nature. For example, the person repeatedly dosed with morphine does not have to know anything and is not required to be subjected to Pavlovian stimulus contingencies, nor to contingencies between operants and outcomes, in order to develop an increasing tendency to suffer when the morphine is withdrawn. The model is therefore very different from previous theories of acquired motivation, all of which have emphasized associational processes.

Second, the model, in relating its hypothetical mech- anism to the phenomena of operant conditioning, ex- plicitly assumes that operants are energized only by aversive states and that they can be reinforced either by the onset of pleasurable states or by the termination of aversive states. This is the case whether the pleasure

or aversiveness comes from A states or B states. For example, the reinforcing effects of shock termination will not only be due to the elimination of the aversive A state but also, and perhaps more importantly, it will be due to the subsequent pleasurable B state. Woodworth and Schlosberg's (1954) concept of "safety," Mowrer's (1960) concept of "relief," and Denny's (1971) concept of "relaxation" are all emphasizing this concept of reinforcement in aversive situations.

The assumption that operants are energized only by aversive states fits most comfortably with the theo- retical position taken by Hull (1943, 1952) and elab- orated by Mowrer (1947), Miller (1948), Spence (1956), and Brown (1961). The assumption that oper- ants can be reinforced by the onset of pleasurable states fits easily with the position taken by Young (1955) and by Pfaffman (1960). Of course, the as- sumption that operants are reinforced by the termina- tion of aversive states is most like the drive-reduction position maintained especially by Hull (1943) and by Mowrer (1947). These assumptions are really not a t is- sue for us. We take all three to be axiomatic, a point of departure, and we go on from there to spell out the dy- namics of pleasurable and aversive states.

Conditionability of A States and B States

(not included in this reprinting)

Analysis of Selected Motivational Phenomena in Light of the Opponent-Process Theory

(not included in this reprinting)

A States Which Have Little or No Opponent Process

A few hedonic disequilibriums may cause very little corrective, oppositional reaction in the nervous system. A nonopposed system would manifest no peak of the A state, no adaptation, and no appearance of a B state after stimulus termination. One possible example is the hedonic state engendered by marijuana. As a chemical stimulus it precipitates a mildly pleasurable A state. However, there is no reported peak or adaptation, nor are there aversive withdrawal symptoms or craving. Furthermore, tolerance often does not develop with repeated dosages. The concommitance of all these at- tributes would be exactly what the opponent-process model would predict for a pleasure without an oppo- nent. Nausea is an example of an aversive A state which way have no opponent B state. Perhaps some aesthetic pleasures have no opponent process.

We have already talked about the variations in the intensity and duration of B states as a function of the particular kind of A state elicited. For example, we

Page 13: Solomon y Corbit - 1978 - An Opponent-process Theory of Motivation

23 SOLOMON AND CORBIT: THEORY OF MOTIVATION

mentioned the intense and long-lasting aversive B states in the cases of grief and opiate withdrawal. In contrast, we mentioned the relatively short duration of B states associated with the taste of M & Ms and pea- nuts, and rewarding electrical stimulation of the brain. In addition to these inherent variations in intensity and duration of B states elicited by different types of stimuli, repeated elicitation causes large changes in the intensity and duration of B states in some cases and not in others. Therefore, differences in the initial strength of b processes must reflect a parameter of our model, and the effects of repeated elicitation of A states must operate on this parameter. In the case of A states hav- ing no opponent process, the value of the parameter is zero.

We have been constantly amazed by the huge varia- tions in the strengths and durations of different b processes after repeated elicitations. Some b processes, even when well exercised, last but a few minutes. Some examples are taste cravings, the average b process for electrical stimulation of the brain, cardiac deceleration after shock termination, the "fit of joy" when the dog is released from a shock box, etc. Some last a few hours. Examples are the exhilaration following dangerous or endurance-challenging exercise, or distress calling in precocial birds when the imprinting object is gone. Yet others last for months. Examples are loneliness and grief, craving following withdrawal from either opiates, alcohol, or barbiturates.

At first glance it may appear that we have complete- ly undercut our theory. We have speculated that some A states may not arouse B states, and we have provided no principles by which one could designate in advance whether or not a particular A state would be a part of an opponent-process pair. At present, we look on this as an empirical problem, and we use the following ar- gument. Opponent processes defend a hedonic equili- brium. They are part of the biological defense system mediated by the brain. In the realm of foreign body reactions to bacteria, viruses, and poisons we often can detect defense systems. Indeed, the actions of antigens and antibody formation bear many resemblances to the opponent-process systems we have described. How- ever, not all poisons and not all foreign bodies engender defense reactions. We can be defenseless. These sub- stances are, therefore, called deadly poisons. We think the same situation holds in the defense of hedonic equilibrium, and so we should not be surprised to dis- cover that a given A state goes relatively unopposed. Perhaps one day we will have a theoretical rationale for the parameter and operator involved here.

Discussion

We have argued that there are certain systems in the brain, the business of which is to suppress or reduce all excursions from hedonic neutrality, whether those ex-

cursions be appetitive or aversive, pleasant or unpleas- ant. The systems operate to decrease the intensity of subjective "hedonic quality," "affect," "emotion," "arousal," or the objective reinforcing properties of stimuli. The systems function independently of oper- ants or instrumental acts. They are fully automatic. Thus, whereas operants tend to maximize positive rein- forcement and to minimize negative reinforcement, the affect-controlling systems of the central nervous system minimize both. They are brought into play whenever significant departures from affective equi- librium occur as a consequence of stimulation on-set and maintenance. When such a system is effective, it will reduce the intensity of the affective experience even while the input is still there. This reduction will be manifest for both positive and negative reinforcers, and for pleasant and unpleasant stimuli. The theoret- ical model refers to both subjective and objective psy- chological phenomena.

Reduction in affective or hedonic intensity is postu- lated to be brought about by the activation of an op- ponent loop, precipitated into action whenever affec- tive, hedonic, or emotional states are aroused. The opponent loop opposes the stimulus-aroused affective state. Furthermore, the opponent process is postulated to be sluggish in its latency, recruitment, and decay (a heavily damped circuit, an inertia-laden system). The opponent loop itself is postulated to generate an hedon- ic process which is, in some abstract sense, the opposite to that precipitated by the stimulus input which has initially aroused affect. The opponent process will manifest its quality and intensity when the stimulus is suddenly terminated. The persistence of the opponent process will be seen for some time because of its slug- gish decay property.

Furthermore, we have postulated that the opponent process is strengthened through use and weakened through disuse. These changes are nonassociative in nature. This makes the affect-control systems similar to some immunological mechanisms in their properties.

Even though the model is not yet as precise as some mathematical models, from it, nevertheless, one can unambiguously deduce many of the known phenomena of acquired motivation. As examples of this, we have illustrated in detail how the model organizes infor- mation on aversive behavior control and the drug addictions.

The theory postulates no conditioning or learning mechanisms responsible for the occurrence of the ac- quired motivations we have discussed. The acquisition is automatic, merely by virtue of the repeated occur- rence of affect-producing stimuli. However, the inter- relationships between the affect-control mechanisms and Pavlovian conditioning processes, and the inter- relations of both with operant behavior, are dis~ussed in the sections not included in this reprinting.

The novel feature of the opponent-process theory is that it sees the behavioral phenomenon of addiction as

Page 14: Solomon y Corbit - 1978 - An Opponent-process Theory of Motivation

24 AMERICAN ECONOMIC ASSOCIATION

an empirical model for all acquired motivation. Addic- tion is not viewed as an abnormality. Instead, it is the inevitable consequence of a normally functioning sys- tem which opposes affective or hedonic states. We as- sume, for example, that love is an addiction phenome- non characterized by habituation to the presence of the loved one and intensified aversion in the absence of the loved one. In the same vein, we assume that imprinting in precocial birds is an addiction phenomenon. In the case of aversive stimulation, we assume that masochis- tic phenomena are the consequence of a normally func- tioning system which opposes affect. These phenomena are characterized by habituation to the presence of the feared or unpleasant event and intensified pleasure after the termination of that event.

Finally, we have pointed out some new lines of em- pirical research suggested by the opponent-process theory of motivation.

References

Bowlby, J. Maternal care and mental health. Geneva: World Health Organization, 1952.

Brown, J. S. The motivation of behavior. New York: McGraw-Hill, 1961.

Church, R. M., LoLordo, V. M., Overmier, J. B., Solomon, R. L., & Turner, L. H. Cardiac responses to shock in curarized dogs. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology. 1966, 62, 1-7.

Denny, M. R. Relaxation theory and experiments. In F. R. Brush (Ed.), Aversive conditioning and learning. New York: Academic Press, 197 1.

Epstein, S. M. Toward a unified theory of anxiety. In B. A. Maher (Ed.), Progress in experimental personality research. Vol. 4. New York: Academic Press, 1967.

Hoffman, H. S. The control of distress vocalization by an imprinted stimulus. Behaviour, 1968, 30, 175-191.

Hoffman, H. S., & Solomon, R. L. An opponent-process theory of motivation: 111. Some affective dynamics in imprinting. Learn-ing and Motivation, 1974, in press.

Hoffman, H. S., Stratton, J. W., Newby, V., & Barrett, J. E. Develop-ment of behavioral control by an imprinting stimulus. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 1970, 7 1, 229-236.

Hull, C. L. Principles of behavior. New York: Appleton-Century, 1943.

Hull, C. L. A behavior system. New Haven: Yale University Press, 1952.

Hurvich, L. M., & Jameson, D. An opponent-process theory of color vision. Psychological Review, 1957, 64, 384-404.

Jaffee, J. H. Drug addiction and drug abuse. In L. Goodman & A. Gilman (Eds.), The pharmacological basis of therapeutics. (3rd ed.) New York: Macmillan, 1965.

Katcher, A. H?, Solomon, R. L.,Turner, L. H., LoLordo, V. M.,Overm- ier, J. B., & Rescorla, R. A. Heart-rate and blood pressure re-

sponses to signaled and unsignaled shocks: Effects of cardiac sympathectomy. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 1969, 68, 163-174.

Kimmel, H. D. Pathological inhibition of emotional behavior. In H. D. Kimmel (Ed.), Experimental psychopathology. New York: Academic Press, 197 1.

Masserman, J. H. Behavior and neurosis. Chicago: University of Chicago Press, 1943.

Maurer, D. W., & Vogel, M. P. H. Narcotics and narcotic addiction. Springfield, Ill.: Charles C Thomas, 1967.

Miller, N. E. Studies of fear as an acquirable drive: I. Fear as motiva- tion and fear-reduction as reinforcement in the learning of new responses. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 1948, 38, 89-101.

Moscovitch, A., & LoLordo, V. M. The role of safety in the Pavlovian backward conditioning procedure. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 1968, 66, 673-678.

Mowrer, 0 . H. On the dual nature of learning-A reinterpretation of "conditioning" and "problem-solving." Harvard Education- al Review, 1947, 17, 102-148.

Mowrer, 0 . H. Learning theory and behavior. New York: Wiley, 1960.

Pfaffman, C. The pleasures of sensation. Psychological Review, 1960, 67, 253-268.

Rescorla, R. A,, & LoLordo, V. M. Inhibition of avoidance behavior. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 1965, 69,406-41 2.

Selye, H. The physiology and pathology of exposure to stress. Mon-treal: Acta, 1950.

Solomon, R. L., & Brush, E. S. Experimentally derived conceptions of anxiety and aversion. In M. R. Jones (Ed.), Nebraska Sym- posium on Motivation: 1956. Lincoln: University of Nebraska Press, 1956.

Solomon, R. L., & Corbit, J. D. An opponent-process theory of mo- tivation: 11. Cigarette addiction. Journal oj' Abnormal Psy-chology, 1973, 81, 158-171.

Solomon, R. L., & Wynne, L. C. Traumatic avoidance learning: Ac- quisition in normal dogs. Psychological Monographs, 1953, 67(4, Whole No. 354).

Spence, K. W. Behavior theory and conditioning. New Haven: Yale University Press, 1956.

Turner, L. H., & Solomon, R. L. Human traumatic avoidance learn- ing: Theory and experiments on the operant-respondent distinc- tion and failures to learn. Psychological Monographs, 1962, 76(40, Whole No. 559).

Weisman, R. G., & Litner, J. C. The course of Pavlovian excitation and inhibition of fear in rats. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 1969, 69, 667-672. (a)

Weisman, R. G., & Litner, J. C. Positive conditioned reinforcement of Sidman avoidance behavior.. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 1969; 68, 597-603. (b)

Wikler, A. Opiate addiction. Springfield, Ill.: Charles C Thomas, 1953.

Wikler, A. Some implications of conditioning theory for problems of drug abuse. Behavioral Science, 197 1, 16, 92-97.

Woodworth, R. S., & Schlosberg, H. Experimentalpsychology. New York: Holt, 1954.

Young, P. T. The role of hedonic processes in motivation. In M. R. Jones (Ed.), Nebraska Symposium on Motivation: 1955. Lin-coln: University of Nebraska Press, 1955.