Social-Emotional Well-being and Resilience of Children in Early Childhood

14
This article was downloaded by: [Universiti Pendidikan Sultan Idris] On: 26 November 2012, At: 05:49 Publisher: Routledge Informa Ltd Registered in England and Wales Registered Number: 1072954 Registered office: Mortimer House, 37-41 Mortimer Street, London W1T 3JH, UK Early Years: An International Journal of Research and Development Publication details, including instructions for authors and subscription information: http://www.tandfonline.com/loi/ceye20 Socialemotional wellbeing and resilience of children in early childhood settings – PERIK: an empirically based observation scale for practitioners Toni Mayr a & Michaela Ulich a a State Institute of Early Childhood Research, Munich, Germany Version of record first published: 11 Mar 2009. To cite this article: Toni Mayr & Michaela Ulich (2009): Socialemotional wellbeing and resilience of children in early childhood settings – PERIK: an empirically based observation scale for practitioners, Early Years: An International Journal of Research and Development, 29:1, 45-57 To link to this article: http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/09575140802636290 PLEASE SCROLL DOWN FOR ARTICLE Full terms and conditions of use: http://www.tandfonline.com/page/terms-and- conditions This article may be used for research, teaching, and private study purposes. Any substantial or systematic reproduction, redistribution, reselling, loan, sub-licensing, systematic supply, or distribution in any form to anyone is expressly forbidden. The publisher does not give any warranty express or implied or make any representation that the contents will be complete or accurate or up to date. The accuracy of any instructions, formulae, and drug doses should be independently verified with primary sources. The publisher shall not be liable for any loss, actions, claims, proceedings, demand, or costs or damages whatsoever or howsoever caused arising directly or indirectly in connection with or arising out of the use of this material.

Transcript of Social-Emotional Well-being and Resilience of Children in Early Childhood

Page 1: Social-Emotional Well-being and Resilience of Children in Early Childhood

This article was downloaded by: [Universiti Pendidikan Sultan Idris]On: 26 November 2012, At: 05:49Publisher: RoutledgeInforma Ltd Registered in England and Wales Registered Number: 1072954 Registeredoffice: Mortimer House, 37-41 Mortimer Street, London W1T 3JH, UK

Early Years: An International Journal ofResearch and DevelopmentPublication details, including instructions for authors andsubscription information:http://www.tandfonline.com/loi/ceye20

Social‐emotional well‐being andresilience of children in earlychildhood settings – PERIK: anempirically based observation scale forpractitionersToni Mayr a & Michaela Ulich aa State Institute of Early Childhood Research, Munich, GermanyVersion of record first published: 11 Mar 2009.

To cite this article: Toni Mayr & Michaela Ulich (2009): Social‐emotional well‐being and resilienceof children in early childhood settings – PERIK: an empirically based observation scale forpractitioners, Early Years: An International Journal of Research and Development, 29:1, 45-57

To link to this article: http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/09575140802636290

PLEASE SCROLL DOWN FOR ARTICLE

Full terms and conditions of use: http://www.tandfonline.com/page/terms-and-conditions

This article may be used for research, teaching, and private study purposes. Anysubstantial or systematic reproduction, redistribution, reselling, loan, sub-licensing,systematic supply, or distribution in any form to anyone is expressly forbidden.

The publisher does not give any warranty express or implied or make any representationthat the contents will be complete or accurate or up to date. The accuracy of anyinstructions, formulae, and drug doses should be independently verified with primarysources. The publisher shall not be liable for any loss, actions, claims, proceedings,demand, or costs or damages whatsoever or howsoever caused arising directly orindirectly in connection with or arising out of the use of this material.

Page 2: Social-Emotional Well-being and Resilience of Children in Early Childhood

Social-emotional well-being and resilience of children in early childhoodsettings – PERIK: an empirically based observation scale forpractitioners

Toni Mayr* and Michaela Ulich

State Institute of Early Childhood Research, Munich, Germany

Compared with the traditional focus on developmental problems, research onpositive development is relatively new. Empirical research in children’s well-beinghas been scarce. The aim of this study was to develop a theoretically andempirically based instrument for practitioners to observe and assess preschoolchildren’s well-being in early childhood settings. The analysis of preschoolteachers’ ratings yields six dimensions of social-emotional well-being: (1) makingcontact/social performance, (2) self control/thoughtfulness, (3) self-assertiveness,(4) emotional stability/coping with stress, (5) task orientation, (6) pleasure inexploration. Composite scales were constructed. PERIK consists of six scales ofsix items each. The scales differentiate in both the upper and lower range anddespite their brevity have good psychometric qualities. The instrument waspublished together with a booklet containing examples of how PERIKobservations can be employed in practical work with children.

Keywords: observation scale; social-emotional well-being; resilience; early child-hood settings

Introduction

Well-being is a topic that concerns us every day in our professional and private lives

and it is quite natural for adults to be particularly concerned about the well-being of

children. All parents wish that their children will develop positively. Even for pre-

school and other teachers who deal with children professionally, the well-being of the

children in their care is of paramount importance beyond all pedagogical methods

and trends. They know that learning and developmental processes succeed best when

the children are healthy and happy. Thus, well-being of children is a central indicator

of the quality of educational institutions and processes (Laevers 2003; Ministerium

fur Kultus, Jugend und Sport Baden-Wurttemberg 2006; New Zealand Ministry of

Education 1996; Van Sanden and Joly 2003).

But what are the consequences for pedagogical work in early childhood settings

and schools? Here, it very quickly becomes clear that it is one thing to use terms like

‘well-being’ and ‘positive development’ in everyday discussions and educational

programs and quite another thing to make such concepts concrete, because terms

like ‘well-being’ and ‘positive development’ refer to complex physical and

psychological states and dispositions. In order to observe and keep track of

each child’s individual well-being, professional educators need to have (a) a well-

defined concept of well-being and (b) reliable and valid instruments for its

observation and assessment. There is a vast need for development here, which is

*Corresponding author. Email: [email protected]

Early Years

Vol. 29, No. 1, March 2009, 45–57

ISSN 0957-5146 print/ISSN 1472-4421 online

# 2009 TACTYC

DOI: 10.1080/09575140802636290

http://www.informaworld.com

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Uni

vers

iti P

endi

dika

n Su

ltan

Idri

s] a

t 05:

49 2

6 N

ovem

ber

2012

Page 3: Social-Emotional Well-being and Resilience of Children in Early Childhood

certainly surprising considering how often the topic of well-being is highlighted in

preschool education.

The lack of theoretical concepts and instruments for describing and recording

‘well-being’ in children is due mainly to the fact that ‘positive development’ has long

been neglected by research. Developmental risks were prominent; the instruments for

observing children are still strongly focused on developmental problems – with a few

exceptions, e.g. the innovative and widely known work of Laevers on involvement

(Laevers 2000, 2003; Ulich and Mayr 2003a) or, with a different perspective, the

scales of Ulich and Mayr (2003b, 2006) for the observation and assessment of

language and literacy competencies.

It is only in the past few years that this ‘deficit perspective’ has been questioned.

Currently, there is an ever-growing interest in competence and positive development.

The following gives a report on a newly developed instrument for observing and

assessing the well-being of children in early childhood centres (Mayr and Ulich

2006). The instrument is called PERIK (Positive development and resilience

in kindergarten; in German: Positive Entwicklung und Resilienz im

Kindergartenalltag).

Theoretical background

The development of PERIK was based primarily on three different sources: research

on ‘mental health’, ‘resilience’ and ‘school readiness’.

Mental health

About 30 years ago, research on mental health started to look systematically at

positive development and physical and mental well-being (salutogenesis). The aim

was to achieve a discrete concept of ‘positive’ development (e.g. Antonovsky

1979).

Different concepts were developed with regard to individual mental health (e.g.

Compton 1998; Goppel 1997). A differentiation made by Ryan and Deci (2000) is

important for the understanding and theoretical classification of these currently very

heterogeneous concepts: the focus is either on subjective well-being (‘hedonic

approach’) or on optimum development of the individual’s intrinsic potential

(‘eudaimonic approach’); the latter can but does not have to be linked with subjective

well-being, and vice versa.

In Germany, it was above all Becker who concentrated on research on mental

health (e.g. 1982, 1986). He developed a complex concept of seven bipolar

dimensions (Becker 1986), e.g. ‘physical well-being’ (frequently positive feelings,

seldom negative feelings), ‘high level of energy’ (vitality, activity, initiative, interest)

and ‘expansivity’ (spontaneity, self-assertiveness, self-actualization).

The concept of ‘life skills’ (World Health Organisation 1994; Asshauer and

Hanewinkel 2000) is influential in the educational sector. What is meant here are

skills that enable appropriate interaction with fellow human beings and also

facilitate coping with everyday problems and stress situations: (1) self-awareness and

empathy, (2) effective handling of stress and difficult situations, (3) communication

and social competence, (4) critical, creative, independent thinking, (5) ability to solve

problems.

46 T. Mayr and M. Ulich

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Uni

vers

iti P

endi

dika

n Su

ltan

Idri

s] a

t 05:

49 2

6 N

ovem

ber

2012

Page 4: Social-Emotional Well-being and Resilience of Children in Early Childhood

Resilience

The concept of resilience came to the fore in the field of so-called risk research. This

concentrates on children who grow up in particularly difficult conditions; these

children often suffer from different problems – in childhood or later.

Longitudinal studies of these children from birth or early childhood toadolescence and adulthood (e.g. Block and Block 1988; Garmezy 1981; Murphy

and Moriarty 1976; Rutter 1997; Werner and Smith 1982; see also review: Luthar,

Cicchetti, and Becker 2000; Werner 2000) show that even in such high risk samples

there are children who do not fail in their ‘developmental tasks’ or show deviant

behaviours, but develop positively in kindergarten and school and later become

successful and happy. These children are described as ‘resilient’.

What characterises these children? The results of longitudinal studies have

provided perspectives on critical developmental personality factors of resilient

children (Mayr and Ulich 2003):

N easy temperament and friendliness

N ability to elicit positive attention from family members and strangers

N positive self concept, self-esteem

N autonomy and independence

N proactive approach to problem-solving

N persistence and concentration

N pleasure in novel experiences, curiosity and exploratory drive, alertness

N empathy and prosocial orientation

N positive social relationships

N ability to delay gratification

N positive processing (and restructuring) of negative experiences

N control of affect

N adequate expression of feelings and demands

N optimism, vitality and energy

N having hobbies and interests

N ability to recover after distressful experiences

N being calm and relaxed

Readiness for school

Transition to school is a complex phenomenon involving an organised system ofinteractions and transactions among persons and institutions (Pianta and Rimm-

Kaufmann 2006). As far as the competencies of children are concerned, mostly we

associate ‘school readiness’ with intellectual, linguistic and numeracy competencies.

However, more recent studies show that also early social and emotional

competencies have a significant influence on how a child copes with school later

on (Becker and Luthar 2002; Ladd, Birch, and Buhs 1999; Lane et al. 2007; Meisels

1999; McClelland and Morrison 2003; McClelland, Morrison, and Holmes 2000;

Normandeau and Guay 1998; Raver and Zigler 1997; Rimm-Kaufmann, Pianta, andCox 2000; Shonkoff and Phillips 2000; Zins et al. 2004).

Of primary significance here is the capability of children to regulate themselves at

different levels (e.g. Blaire 2002; Bronson 2000; Kopp 1982, 1989; Rothbart and

Bates 1998). In detail, this concerns the following:

Early Years 47

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Uni

vers

iti P

endi

dika

n Su

ltan

Idri

s] a

t 05:

49 2

6 N

ovem

ber

2012

Page 5: Social-Emotional Well-being and Resilience of Children in Early Childhood

N Regulative competencies at the cognitive level (control of attentiveness,

planning and target orientation in activities, independent working, persistence).

N ‘Impulse control’ or ‘effortful control’ (ability to resist to temptation, tolerate

frustration, comply with requests, wait patiently, listen to others).

N Emotion regulation (appropriate expression of emotions, modulation of

emotional arousal, managing aversive emotions like sadness, frustration, or

anger, remaining behaviorally organized in face of distressing circumstances).

N Regulation of exploratory behavior (level of interest, curiosity, initiative,

persistence and motivation to explore).

‘Social competence’ is also a multilayered construct (e.g. Eisenberg, Fabes, and

Spinrad 2006; Kienbaum 2003, forthcoming; Petermann 2002; Saarni 1999). The

following competencies are often emphasised as being particularly relevant in

relation to ‘school readiness’:

N Assertive behaviours (ability to say what he/she wants, to defend own viewsunder group pressure, to question unfair rules).

N Prosocial behaviours (initiating interactions with peers, cooperating withchildren and adults, sharing, showing concern for others, resolving conflicts

without aggression, cooperating with adults).

N Social integration/social performance (friendships, appreciation by other

children, good relationships with preschool teacher).

Early regulative and early social competencies are linked with each other (e.g. Fabes

et al. 1999) and establish a framework for later development. They provide the

requirements for good social relationships in school – with peers and the teachingstaff – and lay the essential foundations for learning and performance. These

competencies are important not only at the transition to school, but also at the ages

of three and four and can thus be observed and encouraged early on (McClelland

and Morrison 2003).

From the theory to the observation instrument

A basic task of research is – beyond personal wishes and educational aims – to define

the content of positive development more precisely and to define the ways of

achieving this aim. Here all three theories are relevant. The traits and conditions of

positive development are differentiated and researched empirically, e.g. on the basis

of large-scale longitudinal studies. Important for our work was the fact that various

approaches reveal perhaps not quite identical, but nevertheless very similar,

dimensions of positive development. This is remarkable in that we are consideringdifferent scientific ‘schools’ that hardly take any notice of each other. However,

apparently there is a common core of socioemotional competencies that build the

foundations for positive development – regardless of the scientific contexts.

This common core was the basis for developing PERIK. PERIK focuses on a

concept of well-being emphasising the ‘eudaimonic approach’ and socioemotional

competencies. The aim was to develop a practical, not too complex but nevertheless

reliable instrument for preschool teachers enabling them to observe and record

systematically the well-being of children in their everyday educational settings.

PERIK is the result of various phases of development. The first step was to

mould the results and concepts of research into a construct that is suitable for

48 T. Mayr and M. Ulich

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Uni

vers

iti P

endi

dika

n Su

ltan

Idri

s] a

t 05:

49 2

6 N

ovem

ber

2012

Page 6: Social-Emotional Well-being and Resilience of Children in Early Childhood

observing children in preschool. Some concepts (e.g. self-actualization) were

developed in research with youths and adults and so cannot be transferred directly

to the preschool age group; others, for example ‘proactive coping’, are very difficult

to observe externally.

On the basis of theoretical considerations and working sessions with

practitioners from eight day care centres, we first defined nine areas and designed

items for each area (a total of 78 items with a five-point rating scale). The work of

the Ferre Laevers research group (e.g. Vandenbussche et al. 1999) with their more

phenomenological descriptions of well-being was also helpful in this process.

The first experimental version of the questionnaire was tested in two empirical

studies: the first with 171 children, the second with 309 children (Mayr and Ulich

1999, 2003). The results were interesting and encouraging. Basically, children’s well-

being in preschool emerged as a multidimensional construct. A total of 11

independent dimensions were identified by principal component analysis: (1)

empathic, prosocial behavior, (2) social initiative and vitality, (3) self-assertiveness,

openness, (4) pleasure in exploring, (5) coping with stress, (6) positive self-defence,

(7) pleasure in sensory experiences (food, smells), (8) persistence/robustness, (9) sense

of humour, (10) positive attitude toward warmth and closeness, (11) ability to rest

and relax. The first six dimensions could be interpreted relatively clearly, whereas the

remaining five factors were more hypothetical. This was a first exploratory step

toward developing a theory of well-being in early childhood and constructing a scale

to be used in early childhood settings.

The experience gained from this first empirical phase was channelled into a

thorough overhaul of the observation instrument. On the conceptual level, we had

not paid enough attention to the topic of self-regulation and self-organisation as a

central development task in preschool age (e.g. Aksan and Kochanska 2004;

Bronson 2000; Kochanska, Murray, and Harlan 2000; Kopp 1982, 1989; Rothbart

and Rueda 2005). The studies of Hightower et al. (1986), Kendall and Wilcox (1979),

Olson and Kashiwagi (2000) and Shields and Cichetti (1997) were the main points of

reference at the observation procedure level.

The second experimental version of the observation scale comprised a total of 85

items. This instrument was used to observe a random group of 351 children in 30

kindergartens1 (159 boys, 161 girls; 116 four-year-olds, 128 five-year-olds, 76 six-

year-olds). Teachers were asked to rate the children on a six-point Likert scale

ranging from 1 (always applies) to 6 (does not apply).

Empirical findings

Six dimensions of well-being

Teachers’ ratings were subjected to principal component analysis with orthogonal

varimax rotation2. Six central dimensions could be interpreted as follows (percentage

of the variance, number of markers and factor loadings in brackets).3

Factor 1: ‘Self-control, thoughtfulness’ (13.2% of variance; 18 items; 7 items with

loading >.70, 5 items >.60, 3 items >.50)

This first factor is composed of items that refer to self-control and to empathic-

thoughtful behaviour; both aspects are highly interrelated. Children’s ability to

Early Years 49

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Uni

vers

iti P

endi

dika

n Su

ltan

Idri

s] a

t 05:

49 2

6 N

ovem

ber

2012

Page 7: Social-Emotional Well-being and Resilience of Children in Early Childhood

control their own actions consciously and deliberately grows rapidly in the three to

six-year-old age group. The observation is focused on the one hand on the aspect:

can children defer needs and put aside their own wishes? Can they keep to rules and

regulations? On the other hand, it focuses on perspective taking and empathy. Canchildren put themselves in the place of others? Do they show compassion, do they

have respect for others? The ability to control oneself consciously and the ability to

respect and care for others are stages in development that are of long-term

significance.

Factor 2: ‘Making contact, social performance’ (11.4% of variance; 16 items; 4 items

with loading >.70, 6 Items >.60, 3 Items >.50; 2 Items >.40)

The items for this area primarily target the four different social competencies

involved when interacting with other children: the ability to make positive contact

easily; the ability to apply appropriate means to join in the games of other children;the ability and readiness to communicate with other children verbally; the ability to

initiate games that are attractive to other children.

All these competencies – when used by children – affect the development of early

behavioural patterns in relationships with peers. Two major aspects of thedevelopment of relationships in early childhood are addressed; each has to be

observed separately also with respect to their different significance for the

development of the child:

N The position of a child in the group: how important is the child in the eyes of

the other children, what influence does the child have?

N Friendships: to what extent does the child have intensive, friendly relation-

ships with other children?

Factor 3: ‘Task orientation’ (8.5% of variance; 14 items; 2 items with loading >.70, 4

Items >.60, 5 Items >.50; 3 Items >.40)

Factor three refers to activities that have to be planned and executed purposefully

and where the children have to act independently. These tasks can be set by the

preschool teacher or chosen by the children. The following aspects play a role in

responding to the question ‘How does the child behave with respect to such tasks?’.

It is a matter of concentration and persistence in executing the tasks, and also a

matter of independence. Does the child try to master the tasks independently or does

he/she always need praise and encouragement? Does the child start quickly? Is he/she

careful and accurate?

Factor 4: ‘Self-assertiveness’ (7% of variance; 11 items; 1 item with loading >.70, 3

Items >.60, 2 Items >.50; 5 Items >.40)

Factor four describes a child who has a self-confident attitude, who will assert

himself/herself and take a stand (hold his/her ground), who is ready to defend

himself/herself when necessary. More specifically it refers to children’s ability or

readiness to communicate their feelings, needs and wishes to others (also to adults),

to make claims, to tell other people what is on their minds. Self-assertiveness should

be distinguished from aggressive behaviour.

50 T. Mayr and M. Ulich

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Uni

vers

iti P

endi

dika

n Su

ltan

Idri

s] a

t 05:

49 2

6 N

ovem

ber

2012

Page 8: Social-Emotional Well-being and Resilience of Children in Early Childhood

Factor 5: ‘Pleasure in exploring’ (5.2% of variance; 11 items; 1 item >.60, 4 items

>.50; 4 items >.40)

This factor has a very clear focus. It includes items like ‘asks questions, wants toknow about things’, ‘likes to explore new things’, ‘is optimistic and positive when

beginning something new’. Even exploratory ‘courage’ comes under this factor: ‘will

try things that seem difficult’. All in all, this factor seems to describe a very basic

tendency of children to be curious and optimistic rather than defensive toward novel

situations, to show a positive and constructive attitude toward challenges.

Factor 6: ‘Emotional stability, coping with stress’ (4.6% of variance; 8 items; 2 items

>.60, 2 items >.50; 4 items >.40)

Factor six describes two highly interrelated aspects: ‘reactivity’ or ‘emotionalstability’ and ‘coping with stress’. Reactivity refers to habitual emotional

responsivity, coping with stress means the ability of children to modulate this

reactivity (‘is able to calm down on his/her own’). Children with high scores in this

dimension remain accessible and open to their social environment even when under

stress: they do not tend to withdraw, and they will let themselves be comforted when

they are sad – which means that they can accept and use emotional support from

peers and adults.

Scale development

With reference to the factor pattern, six composite scales were constructed with sixitems each. Items are chosen for inclusion on the basis of factor patterns, item-total

correlations, internal consistency and a thorough examination of the contents of

individual items. PERIK consists of 36 items (Table 1); some statistics of the

instrument are presented in Table 2. Reliability coefficients (Cronbachs alpha)

range from .81 (‘emotional stability/coping with stress’) to .88 (‘Making contact/

social performance’) demonstrating a relatively high internal consistency of the

scales, considering the measure’s brevity. The scales are approximately normally

distributed; they differentiate in both the upper and lower range.To evaluate age and gender effects, children were divided into three different age

groups (four, five, and six years). Next, 2 (child gender) 63 (child age) ANOVAS

were conducted separately for each scale. Analyses revealed that children’s scores on

all six scales varied significantly with child’s age, in the expected direction of older

children manifesting more advanced competencies. Significant main effects were

obtained for child’s gender too: univariate tests revealed that girls had higher scores

than boys on five scales. Boys and girls did not differ significantly on the scale ‘self-

assertiveness’.For all six scales percentile norms were calculated (percentile 25, percentile 75).

Discussion

Because interest in children’s competencies is relatively new, compared with the

traditional focus on developmental problems, less is known about empirically

derived dimensions of competencies/dispositions and about instruments to observe

and assess competencies. The present research revealed six dimensions of socio-

emotional competency in preschool age: ‘making contact/social performance’,

Early Years 51

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Uni

vers

iti P

endi

dika

n Su

ltan

Idri

s] a

t 05:

49 2

6 N

ovem

ber

2012

Page 9: Social-Emotional Well-being and Resilience of Children in Early Childhood

Table 1. PERIK: Dimensions and items.

Making contact/social performance

N the child makes (positive) contact easily with peers

N initiates games which are attractive for other children

N tells other children about his/her experiences

N if he/she wants to join other children in play, he/she can express this adequately, e.g.

using entrance rituals like ‘may I play with you?’

N his/her opinion is important among peers

N has close relationships (friendships) with other children

Self-control/thoughtfulness

N the child can wait for his/her turn, e.g. in group discussions, when food or materials are

handed out

N respects the boundaries and needs of other children

N isworriedwhenhe/shehashurtanotherchildordamagedsomething;apologises, tries tomakeup

N has respect and empathy for feelings and mood of adults, e.g. when I ask the children to

be a little quiet, because I am not feeling well

N respects ‘do’s’ and ‘don’ts’, e.g. concerning the use of certain rooms or objects

N can be glad for other children, shares their joy and success, e.g. when a child gets a present

Self-assertiveness

N the child enjoys relating tells his/her experiences, e.g. about the weekend

N when an adult does not treat him/her justly, the child will speak up for himself/herself

N is able to make justified demands on adults, e.g. reminding them of a promise

N when something is wrong/disagreeable or something bad happens among children, he/she

will speak up, e.g. will say ‘stop it’, ‘no I don’t want to do that’

N can defend himself/herself verbally or physically when attacked by other children

N does not allow himself/herself to be put under pressure, e.g. holds an opinion that others

do not share

Emotional stability/coping with stress

N the child remains reachable when in distress, e.g. when he/she is cross, disappointed, sad

N he/she calms down on his/her own following excitement or stress

N appears well-balanced

N doesn’t mind too much when he/she makes a mistake, loses at a game…

N takes relatively long to recover after stress and excitement (2)

N quickly loses his balance, feels stressed easily (2)

Task orientation

N the child quickly begins a task

N works on a task independently

N works quickly

N works carefully and precisely, e.g. when cutting, gluing, building a bridge…

N can remain concentrated on one thing for relatively long

N needs praise and encouragement to finish a task (2)

Pleasure in exploring

N the child likes to explore new things

N is optimistic and positive when beginning something new

N asks questions, wants to know about things

N explores new things independently

N gives himself/herself time to get acquainted with new situations and things

N will try things that seem difficult or might not succeed

52 T. Mayr and M. Ulich

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Uni

vers

iti P

endi

dika

n Su

ltan

Idri

s] a

t 05:

49 2

6 N

ovem

ber

2012

Page 10: Social-Emotional Well-being and Resilience of Children in Early Childhood

‘self-control/thoughtfulness’, ‘self-assertiveness’, ‘emotional stability/coping with

stress’, ‘Task orientation’, ‘Pleasure in exploring’.

At the scientific level, the dimensions found here match well the research referred

to at the beginning on mental health, resilience and readiness for school. There is

also a high degree of agreement with temperament research (e.g. Rothbart and Bates

1998), research on prosocial development – e.g. the link between impulse control and

the development of empathy (e.g. Eisenberg, Fabes, and Spinrad 2006), and research

on self-regulation (e.g. Bronson 2000).

With reference to observation procedures, all the dimensions of PERIK could be

identified as independent factors in other empirical studies (Fingerle 2000; Gresham

and Elliot 1990; Hightower et al. 1986; Janus, Walsh, and Duku 2005; Kendall and

Wilcox 1979; McDermott, Leigh, and Perry 2002; Olson and Kashiwagi 2000;

Putnam and Rothbart 2006; Shields and Cichetti 1997).

At the educational level, PERIK can be used both quantitatively and

qualitatively. Quantitatively, a summary score can be formed for each area of

competence. This provides the option of making systematic comparisons, e.g. where

does a child stand in a specific area compared with other children in the same age

group? Such scores can be used also in educational projects and interventions.

Existing findings indicate that the PERIK scales, despite their brevity, have good

psychometric qualities. The scales demonstrate a relatively high internal consistency.

There is evidence of validity as well: (a) factorial structure of the instrument was

compatible with existing theoretical models and with other empirical results. (b)

Associations between PERIK scores with age and gender replicated age and gender

differences demonstrated in other studies (Janus and Offord 2007; Kochanska,

Murray, and Coy 1997; Mayr 2000). (c) The relationships between children’s PERIK

scores and the quality of teacher–child relationships, measured via the Student–

Teacher Relationship Scale (Pianta 2001), were consistently in the expected direction

(Mayr n.d.): there was a high negative correlation between children’s self-regulation

(PERIK: ‘self-control/thoughtfulness’ and ‘emotional stability/coping with stress’)

and ‘conflict’ (STRS). As can be predicted from the attachment theory (e.g.

Scholmerich and Legning 2004), emotional ‘closeness’ (STRS) was strongly

(positively) associated with ‘pleasure in exploration’ (PERIK).

At the qualitative level there should be a close connection between assessment in

pedagogical settings on the one hand and curricular goals and pedagogical action on

the other hand (Meisels and Atkins-Burnett 2006). The items in PERIK describe

competencies that are designated as concrete learning goals in many German

Table 2. PERIK: selected statistical parameters.

Scale M S Kurtosis Skewness Alpha

Making contact/social performance 21.23 4.85 .08 2.51 .88

Self-control/thoughtfulness 21.85 4.40 2.46 2.36 .86

Self-assertiveness 20.97 4.61 2.19 2.35 .81

Emotional stability/coping with stress 22.35 4.11 .13 2.37 .82

Task orientation 22.50 4.62 2.33 2.21 .85

Pleasure in exploration 19.84 4.52 2.28 2.08 .86

Early Years 53

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Uni

vers

iti P

endi

dika

n Su

ltan

Idri

s] a

t 05:

49 2

6 N

ovem

ber

2012

Page 11: Social-Emotional Well-being and Resilience of Children in Early Childhood

preschool curricula. The questionnaire helps professionals to recognise and

encourage such competencies in everyday kindergarten life.

The questionnaire covers emotions, social processes and interactions. This means

that we are moving in a sensitive and complex field and must be careful with quick-

fix solutions (patent recipes). The relationship between observation and pedagogical

action is at different levels in PERIK:

N Working with the questionnaire leads to a differentiated perspective of the

individual child. Thus, for example, within a specific area of development

different individual competencies become clear: a child might have a lot of

contact with other children, but only rarely takes the initiative and has the feeling

that he/she does not have many friends. In the synopsis of areas and separate

components there are indications of where support can be practically applied.

N In addition to providing information about children, observation with PERIK

also raises questions that concern the preschool teachers themselves: what are

my pedagogical aspirations? How should I attune to a child? Where should I

change my expectations? How do I react to the self-assertiveness of a six-year-old boy if I am always ready to make compromises and have a need for

harmony, for example? How do I differentiate between aggression and self-

assertiveness in such a case?

N Based on the observations, specific competencies can be deliberately

encouraged in everyday life. For example in the case of Lena, a girl who

has little self-confidence, the preschool teacher observed that Lena is reticent

when something new is introduced and hardly tries anything that she is not

sure to succeed in. When making a self-portrait, she refuses to paint her nose.

She is afraid of messing up her picture. The preschool teacher suggests trying

to draw her nose first on a separate piece of paper and thus with this little

stimulus helps her over the ‘I can’t do that’ threshold.

The accompanying booklet to PERIK contains practical suggestions and examples

of how preschool teachers can support and reinforce specific areas and competencies.

The examples reflect different levels of support – from spontaneous assistance in

everyday activities to the systematic structuring of the learning environment.

Notes

1. Traditionally in Germany ‘kindergarten’ is for children from three to six.

2. Exploratory factor analysis revealed 14 factors with eigenvalues greater than 1. An eight-

factor solution was selected because it best satisfied standard criteria for retention and

because this solution was psychologically meaningful and compatible with existing

theoretical models. With reference to Guadagnoli and Velicer’s (1988) criteria, the first

six factors could be substantially interpreted.

3. For reasons of space we did not include the whole matrix of factor loadings; the complete

matrix will be sent upon request.

References

Antonovsky, A. 1979. Health, stress, and coping. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.

Asshauer, M., and R. Hanewinkel. 2000. Lebenskompetenztraining fur Erst- Zweitklassler:

Ergebnisse einer Interventionsstudie. Kindheit und Entwicklung 9: 251–63.

54 T. Mayr and M. Ulich

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Uni

vers

iti P

endi

dika

n Su

ltan

Idri

s] a

t 05:

49 2

6 N

ovem

ber

2012

Page 12: Social-Emotional Well-being and Resilience of Children in Early Childhood

Becker, B.E., and S.S. Luthar. 2002. Social-emotional factors affecting achievement outcomes

among disadvantaged students: Closing the achievement gap. Educational Psychologist

37: 197–214.

Becker, P. 1982. Psychologie der seelischen Gesundheit, Band 1. Gottingen: Hogrefe.

Becker, P. 1986. Erste Uberprufung der Theorie der Seelischen Gesundheit. In Psychologie der

Seelischen Gesundheit. Band 2, ed. P. Becker and B. Minsel, 91–119. Gottingen: Hogrefe.

Blair, C. 2002. Integrating cognition and emotion in a neurobiological conceptualization of

children’s functioning at school entry. American Psychologist 57: 111–27.

Block, J., and J.H. Block. 1988. Longitudinally foretelling drug usage in adolescence: Early

childhood personality and environmental precursors. Child Development 59: 336–355.

Bronson, M. 2000. Self-regulation in early childhood. New York: Guilford Press.

Compton, W.C. 1998. Measures of mental health and a five factor theory of personality.

Psychological Reports 83: 371–81.

Eisenberg, N., R.A. Fabes, and T.L. Spinrad. 2006. Prosocial development. In Handbook of

child psychology Vol. 3, ed. W. Damon, N. Eisenberg, and R.M. Lerner, 646–718.

Hoboken, NJ: John Wiley.

Fabes, R.A., N. Eisenberg, S. Jones, M. Smith, I. Guthrie, R. Poulin, St. Shepard, and J.

Friedman. 1999. Regulation, emotionality, and preschoolers’ socially competent peer

interactions. Child Development 70: 432–42.

Fingerle, M. 2000. Lehrerratingskalen fur adaptive psychische Ressourcen von

Grundschulkindern – Faktorenstruktur und Reliabilitaten. Heilpadagogische

Forschung 26: 192–8.

Garmezy, N. 1981. Children under stress: Perspectives on antecedents and correlates of

vulnerability and resistance to psychopathology. In Further explorations in personality,

ed. A.I. Rabin, A.M. Barclay, and R.A. Zucker, 196–289. New York: Wiley

Interscience.

Goppel, R. 1997. Ursprunge der seelischen Gesundheit. Wurzburg: Edition Bentheim.

Gresham, F.M., and N.S. Elliot. 1990. Social Skills Rating System. Circle Pines, MN:

American Guidance Service Inc.

Guadagnoli, E., and W.F. Velicer. 1988. Relation of sample size to the stability of component

patterns. Psychological Bulletin 103: 265–75.

Hightower, A., W. Work, E. Cowen, B. Lotyczewski, A. Spinell, J. Guare, and C. Rohrbeck.

1986. The teacher-child rating scale: A brief objective measure of elementary children’s

school problem behaviors and competencies. School Psychology Review 15: 393–409.

Janus, M., and D.R. Offord. 2007. Development and psychometric properties of the Early

Development Instrument (EDI): A measure of children’s school readiness. Canadian

Journal of Behavioural Science 39: 1–22.

Janus, M., C. Walsh, and E. Duku. 2005. Early Development Instrument: Factor structure,

sub-domains and Multiple Challenge Index. Hamilton, ON. Offord Centre of Child

Studies, McMasters University.

Kendall, Ph.C., and L.E. Wilcox. 1979. Self-control in children: Development of a rating scale.

Journal of Consulting Psychology 47: 1020–9.

Kienbaum, J. 2003. Entwicklungsbedingungen prosozialer Responsivitat in der Kindheit.

Lengerich: Pabst.

Kienbaum, J. Forthcoming. Entwicklungsbedingungen von Mitgefuhl in der Kindheit. In

Entwicklung und Forderung sozialer Kompetenz in Kindheit und Adoleszenz, ed. T. Malti

and S. Perren. Stuttgart: Kohlhammer.

Kochanska, G., K. Murray, and K. Coy. 1997. Inhibitory control as a contributor to conscience

in childhood: From toddler to early school age. Child Development 68: 5263–77.

Kochanska, G., K.T. Murray, and E.T. Harlan. 2000. Effortful control in early childhood:

Continuity and change, antecedents, and implications for social development.

Developmental Psychology 36: 220–32.

Early Years 55

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Uni

vers

iti P

endi

dika

n Su

ltan

Idri

s] a

t 05:

49 2

6 N

ovem

ber

2012

Page 13: Social-Emotional Well-being and Resilience of Children in Early Childhood

Kopp, C. 1982. Antecedents of self-regulation: A developmental perspective. Developmental

Psychology 18: 199–214.

Kopp, C. 1989. Regulation of distress and negative emotions: A developmental view.

Developmental Psychology 25: 343–54.

Ladd, G.W., S.H. Birch, and E. Buhs. 1999. Children’s social and scholastic lives in

kindergarten: Related spheres of influence? Child Development 70: 1337–400.

Laevers, F. 2000. Forward to basics! Deep-level-learning and the experiential approach. Early

Years: An International Journal of Research and Development 20: 20–9.

Laevers, F. 2003. Making care and education more effective through wellbeing and

involvement. In Involvement of children and teacher style, ed. F. Laevers and L.

Heylen, 13–24. Leuven: Leuven University Press.

Lane, K.L., T. Stanton-Chapman, K. Roorbach Jamison, and A. Phillips. 2007. Teacher and

parent expectations of preschoolers’ behavior: Social skills necessary for success. Topics

in Early Childhood Special Education 27: 86–97.

Luthar, S.S., D. Cicchetti, and B. Becker. 2000. The construct of resilience: A critical

evaluation and guidelines for future work. Child Development 71: 543–62.

Mayr, T. 2000. ‘Beobachtungsbogen fur Kinder in Vorschulalter’ (BBK) – ein Vorschlag zur

Skalenbildung. Psychologie in Erziehung und Unterricht 47: 281–95.

Mayr, T. n.d. Zur Erfassung von Erzieherin-Kind-Beziehungen – Ergebnisse einer empirischen

Studie. Unpublished manuscript, Staatsinstitut fur Fruhpadagogik, Munchen.

Mayr, T., and M. Ulich. 1999. Children’s well-being in day care centers. An exploratory

empirical study. International Journal of Early Years Education 7: 229–39.

Mayr, T., and M. Ulich. 2003. Seelische Gesundheit bei Kindergartenkindern. In

Elementarpadagogik nach Pisa, ed. W.E. Fthenakis, 190–205. Freiburg: Herder.

Mayr, T., and M. Ulich. 2006. Positive Entwicklung und Resilienz im Kindergartenalltag –

Perik. Freiburg: Herder.

Meisels, S.J. 1999. Assessing readiness. In The transition to kindergarten, ed. R.C. Pianta and

M.C. Cox, 39–66. Baltimore, MD: Paul H. Brookes publishing Co.

Meisels, S.J., and S. Atkins-Burnett. 2006. Evaluating early childhood assessments: A

differential analysis. In Blackwell handbook of early childhood development, ed. K.

McCartney and D. Phillips, 533–49. Oxford: Blackwell Publishing.

McClelland, M.M., and F.J. Morrison. 2003. The emergence of learning related social skills in

preschool children. Early Childhood Research Quarterly 18: 206–24.

McClelland, M.M., F.J. Morrison, and D.L. Holmes. 2000. Children at risk for early

academic problems: The role of learning-related social skills. Early Childhood Research

Quarterly 15: 307–29.

McDermott, P.A., N.M. Leigh, and M.A. Perry. 2002. Development and validation of the

Preschool Learning Behaviors Scale. Psychology in the Schools 39: 353–65.

Ministerium fur Kultus, Jugend und Sport Baden-Wurttemberg. 2006. Orientierungsplan fur

Bildung und Erziehung fur die baden-wurttembergischen Kindergarten – Pilotphase.

Weinheim: Beltz.

Murphy, L.B., and A.E. Moriarty. 1976. Vulnerability, coping, and growth from infancy to

adolescence. New Haven: Yale University Press.

New Zealand Ministry of Education. 1996. Te Whariki – Early Childhood Curriculum.

Wellington: Learning Media Limited.

Normendeau, S., and F. Guay. 1998. Preschool behaviour and first-grade school achievement: The

mediational role of cognitive self-control. Journal of Educational Psychology 90: 111–21.

Olson, S., and K. Kashiwagi. 2000. Teacher ratings of behavioral self-regulation in preschool

children: A Japanese/U.S. comparison. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology 21:

609–17.

Petermann, F. 2002. Das Konzept der sozialen Kompetenz. Zeitschrift fur Psychologie 210:

175–85.

56 T. Mayr and M. Ulich

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Uni

vers

iti P

endi

dika

n Su

ltan

Idri

s] a

t 05:

49 2

6 N

ovem

ber

2012

Page 14: Social-Emotional Well-being and Resilience of Children in Early Childhood

Pianta, R.C. 2001. STRS. Student-Teacher Relationship Scale. Lutz, FL: Psychological

Assessment Resources.

Pianta, R.C., and S. Rimm-Kaufmann. 2006. The social ecology of the transition to school:

Classrooms, families, and children. In Blackwell handbook of early childhood

development, ed. K. McCartney and D. Phillips, 490–507. Oxford: Blackwell Publishing.

Putnam, S.P., and M.K. Rothbart. 2006. Development of short and very short forms of the

Children’s Behavior Questionnaire. Journal of Personality Assessment 87: 103–13.

Raver, C.C., and E.F. Zigler. 1997. Social competence: An untapped dimension in evaluating

Head Start’s success. Early Childhood Research Quarterly 12: 363–85.

Rimm-Kaufmann, S., R.C. Pianta, and M. Cox. 2000. Teachers’ judgements of problems in

the transition to kindergarten. Early Childhood Research Quarterly 15: 147–66.

Rothbart, M.K., and J.E. Bates. 1998. Temperament. In Handbook of child psychology, vol. 3:

Social, emotional, and personality development, ed. W. Damon and N. Eisenberg, 105–76.

New York: John Wiley.

Rothbart, M.K., and M.R. Rueda. 2005. The development of effortful control. In Developing

individuality in the human brain: A tribute to Michael I. Posner, ed. U. Mayr, E. Awh, and

S. Keele, 167–88. Washington, D.C.: American Psychological Association.

Rutter, M. 1997. Psychosocial adversity: Risk, resilience, and recovery. Keynote address at the

7th EECERA Conference in Munich.

Ryan, R.M., and E.L. Deci. 2000. On happiness and human potentials: A review of research

on hedonic and eudaimonic well-being. Annual Review of Psychology 52: 141–66.

Saarni, C. 1999. The development of emotional competence. New York: The Guilford Press.

Scholmerich, A., and A. Lengning. 2004. Neugier, Exploration und Bindungsentwicklung. In

Fruhe Bindung, ed. L. Ahnert, 198–210. Munchen, Basel: Reinhardt.

Shields, A., and D. Cicchetti. 1997. Emotion regulation among school-aged children: The

development and validation of a new criterion Q-sort scale. Developmental Psychology

33: 906–16.

Shonkoff, J.P., and D.A. Phillips, eds. 2000. From neurons to neighborhoods: The science of

early childhood development. A report of the National Research Council. Washington,

DC: National Academy Press.

Ulich, M., and T. Mayr. 2003a. Implementing the Involvement Scales German day care

centres. In Involvement Scales of children and teacher style, ed. F. Laevers and L. Heylen,

25–41. Leuven: Leuven University Press.

Ulich, M., and T. Mayr. 2003b. Sismik. Sprachverhalten und Interesse an Sprache bei

Migrantenkindern in Kindertageseinrichtungen. Freiburg: Herder.

Ulich, M., and T. Mayr. 2006. SELDAK – Sprachentwicklung und Literacy bei deutschsprachig

aufwachsenden Kindern (Beobachtungsbogen und 2 Begleithefte). Freiburg: Herder.

Vandenbussche, E., M. Kog, L. Depondt, and F. Laevers. 1999. Beobachtung und Begleitung

von Kindern. Leuven: Centrum voor ErvaringsGericht Onderwijs.

Van Sanden, P., and A. Joly. 2003. Well-being and involvement as a guide in realizing good

conditions for inclusive education in Nicaragua. In Involvement of children and teacher

style, ed. F. Laevers and L. Heylen, 143–71. Leuven: Leuven University Press.

Werner, E.E. 2000. Protective factors and individual resilience. In Handbook of early childhood

intervention, ed. J.P. Shonkoff and S.J. Meisels, 115–32. Cambridge: Cambridge

University Press.

Werner, E.E., and R.S. Smith. 1982. Vulnerable but invincible. A longitudinal study of resilient

children and youth. New York: McGraw-Hill.

World Health Organisation. 1994. Life skills education in schools, parts 1 and 2. Geneva:

WHO, Division of Mental Health.

Zins, J.E., M.R. Bloodworth, R.P. Weissberg, and H.J. Walberg. 2004. In Building academic

success on social and emotional learning, ed. J.E. Zins, P.R. Weissberg, M.C. Wang, and

H.J. Walberg, 3–22. New York: Teachers College Press.

Early Years 57

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Uni

vers

iti P

endi

dika

n Su

ltan

Idri

s] a

t 05:

49 2

6 N

ovem

ber

2012