School of Education University of Tampere, Finland Issues in Study Design Petri Nokelainen...

98
School of Education University of Tampere, Finland Issues in Study Design Petri Nokelainen [email protected]

Transcript of School of Education University of Tampere, Finland Issues in Study Design Petri Nokelainen...

Page 1: School of Education University of Tampere, Finland Issues in Study Design Petri Nokelainen petri.nokelainen@uta.fi.

School of Education University of Tampere, Finland

Issues in Study Design

Petri [email protected]

Page 2: School of Education University of Tampere, Finland Issues in Study Design Petri Nokelainen petri.nokelainen@uta.fi.

CONTENTS

Issues in Study DesignWriting Scientific Reports

Ten Questions About YOUR Research

Page 3: School of Education University of Tampere, Finland Issues in Study Design Petri Nokelainen petri.nokelainen@uta.fi.

AND OR

AND

Scientific research

Theoretical

Empirical

T1: Research body T2: Innovation E1: Numerical data E2: Textual / nominal data

ANDOR

Issues in Study Design

Page 4: School of Education University of Tampere, Finland Issues in Study Design Petri Nokelainen petri.nokelainen@uta.fi.

Qual: {T1,T2,E2} {T1,T2}

Quan: {E1} !

Theoretical research

Empirical research

A: {T1,T2}

B: {T1,T2,E1}C: {T1,T2,E2}D: {T1,T2,E1,E2}

Issues in Study Design

Page 5: School of Education University of Tampere, Finland Issues in Study Design Petri Nokelainen petri.nokelainen@uta.fi.

B: {T1,T2,E2}

C: {T1,T2,E1}

Research question

Description of person’s attitudes, feelings, meanings, knowledge, etc. about S.

How many cases in the data D have certain attribution x?

How many cases in the data D have certain attribution y?

Is there a relationship between x and y?

Is that relationship causal?

Issues in Study Design

Page 6: School of Education University of Tampere, Finland Issues in Study Design Petri Nokelainen petri.nokelainen@uta.fi.

Issues in Study Design

(Nokelainen, 2008, p. 119)

Page 7: School of Education University of Tampere, Finland Issues in Study Design Petri Nokelainen petri.nokelainen@uta.fi.

Issues in Study Design

Page 8: School of Education University of Tampere, Finland Issues in Study Design Petri Nokelainen petri.nokelainen@uta.fi.

• The left-hand side of the figure shows two main categories of data collection:– Probability sample (PS) and – Non-probability sample (NPS).

• Both methods aim to produce a scientific, representative sample from the target population.

Issues in Study Design

Page 9: School of Education University of Tampere, Finland Issues in Study Design Petri Nokelainen petri.nokelainen@uta.fi.

• According to Jackson (2006), a representative sample is “like” the population. – Thus, we can be confident that the results we find based on

the sample also hold for the population. • This is not a problem with PS, which is based on

random, stratified or cluster sampling. – In random sampling each member of the population has an

equal likelihood of being selected into the sample. – Stratified random sampling allows taking into account

different subgroups in the population. – If the population is too large for random sampling of any sort,

cluster sampling is applied.

Issues in Study Design

Page 10: School of Education University of Tampere, Finland Issues in Study Design Petri Nokelainen petri.nokelainen@uta.fi.

• Problems arise with NPS as the individual members of the population do not have an equal likelihood of being selected to be a member of the sample.

• The most commonly applied NPS technique is convenience sampling (CS) in which participants are obtained wherever they can be found and wherever is convenient for the researcher (Hair, Anderson, Tatham & Black, 1998).

Issues in Study Design

Page 11: School of Education University of Tampere, Finland Issues in Study Design Petri Nokelainen petri.nokelainen@uta.fi.

• Why, then, educational scientists use NPS, typically CS? – Simply because it “tends to be less expensive [than RS] and it

is easier to generate samples using this technique” (Jackson, 2006, p. 84).

Issues in Study Design

Page 12: School of Education University of Tampere, Finland Issues in Study Design Petri Nokelainen petri.nokelainen@uta.fi.

• However, on the lower left-hand part of the figure, it is shown that when researcher ensures that the CS is like the population on certain characteristics (location and dispersion descriptive statistics about, for example, age and job title), it becomes a quota sample (QS). – A quota sample is better than a

CS as it allows us to ensure that the results we find based on the sample also hold for the population.

Issues in Study Design

Page 13: School of Education University of Tampere, Finland Issues in Study Design Petri Nokelainen petri.nokelainen@uta.fi.

• The upper part of the figure contains two sections, namely “parametric” and “non-parametric” divided into eight sub-sections (“DNIMMOCS OLD”).

• Parametric approach is viable only if – 1) Both the phenomenon modeled

and the sample follow normal distribution.

– 2) Sample size is large enough (at least 30 observations).

– 3) Continuous indicators are used.– 4) Dependencies between the

observed variables are linear. • Otherwise non-parametric

techniques should be applied.

Issues in Study Design

Page 14: School of Education University of Tampere, Finland Issues in Study Design Petri Nokelainen petri.nokelainen@uta.fi.

• First, study design (D) is made on the basis of the research question and major goal.

• According to de Vaus (2004, p. 9), “… research design is to ensure that the evidence obtained enables us to answer the initial question as unambiguously as possible.”

Issues in Study Design

Page 15: School of Education University of Tampere, Finland Issues in Study Design Petri Nokelainen petri.nokelainen@uta.fi.

• In order to obtain relevant evidence, we need to specify the type of evidence needed to answer the research question.

• More specifically, we need to ask: Given this research question, what type of evidence (data) is needed to answer the question in a convincing way?

Issues in Study Design

Page 16: School of Education University of Tampere, Finland Issues in Study Design Petri Nokelainen petri.nokelainen@uta.fi.

• Sometimes we proceed with the so-called qualitative designs, sometimes a quantitative orientation is more appropriate, and sometimes we work both qualitatively and quantitatively (mixed-methods research, for a thorough discussion, see Brannen, 2004).

• Methodological, conceptual etc. triangulation.• Design research is quite new approach, see Bannan-

Ritland (2003).

Issues in Study Design

Page 17: School of Education University of Tampere, Finland Issues in Study Design Petri Nokelainen petri.nokelainen@uta.fi.

• Experimental design (a.k.a. ‘pretest post-test randomized experiment’) is the most recommended approach, but only possible with a random sample (a.k.a ‘probability sample’) and random assignment (participants are randomly selected for the experimental and control groups).

• Research is conducted in a controlled environment (e.g., laboratory) with experiment and control groups (threat to external validity due to artificial environment).

• Using experimental design, both reliability and validity are maximized via random sampling and control in the given experiment (de Vaus, 2004).

Issues in Study Design

Page 18: School of Education University of Tampere, Finland Issues in Study Design Petri Nokelainen petri.nokelainen@uta.fi.

Random sample

Exp.

Contr.

Pre

Pre

I

-

Post

Post

Random assignment

Issues in Study Design

Page 19: School of Education University of Tampere, Finland Issues in Study Design Petri Nokelainen petri.nokelainen@uta.fi.

Random assignment to

groupsPretest Intervention Post-test

Experimentalgroup

Measurement (X) Treatment Measurement (Y)

Control group

Measurement (X) No treatment Measurement (Y)

Issues in Study Design

Page 20: School of Education University of Tampere, Finland Issues in Study Design Petri Nokelainen petri.nokelainen@uta.fi.

• Quasi-experimental design (a.k.a. non-equivalent groups design) resembles experimental design but lacks random assignment (sometimes also random sampling) and controlled research environment.

• This type of design is sometimes the only way to do research in certain populations as it minimizes the threats to external validity (natural environments instead of artificial ones).

Random / convenience

sample

Exp.

Contr.

Pre

Pre

I

-

Post

Post

Issues in Study Design

Page 21: School of Education University of Tampere, Finland Issues in Study Design Petri Nokelainen petri.nokelainen@uta.fi.

• The most popular quantitative approach in educational research, correlational design (a.k.a. ‘descriptive study’ or ‘observational study’), allows the use of non-probability sample (a.k.a ‘convenience sample’).

• Most correlational designs are missing control, and thus loose some of their scientific power (Jackson, 2006). – Some research journals accept factorial analysis (main and interaction

effects, e.g., MANOVA) based on quasi-experimental design.

Convenience

sample Exp.

Pre

IPos

t

Issues in Study Design

Page 22: School of Education University of Tampere, Finland Issues in Study Design Petri Nokelainen petri.nokelainen@uta.fi.

– Observational studies can further be classified into cross-sectional and longitudinal studies (see Caskie & Willis, 2006). • Longitudinal design includes series of measurements over

time.– Change over time, age effect.

• Cross-sectional study involves usually one measurement and is thus considerably cheaper and faster to conduct (although producing less controllable and less powerful results).

– If there are several measurements, individual participants answers are not connected over time (e.g., due to anonymity).

– Causal conclusions are usually out of scope of this research type (ibid.).

Issues in Study Design

Page 23: School of Education University of Tampere, Finland Issues in Study Design Petri Nokelainen petri.nokelainen@uta.fi.

• Longitudinal design– One sample that remains the same throughout the study.– Longitudinal study produces more convincing results as it allows the

understanding of change in a construct over time and variability and predictors of such change over time (ibid.).

– However, it takes naturally more time to carry out and suffers from participant drop-out.

Sample Pretest Intervention Post-test

Random sample

Measurement (X) Treatment Measurement (Y)

Issues in Study Design

Page 24: School of Education University of Tampere, Finland Issues in Study Design Petri Nokelainen petri.nokelainen@uta.fi.

• Cross-sectional design– Measurement is conducted once (or several times) and the sample varies

throughout the study.

Sample Pretest Intervention Post-test

Convenience or random sample

Treatment Measurement (Y)

Convenience orrandom sample

No treatment Measurement (Y)

Issues in Study Design

Page 25: School of Education University of Tampere, Finland Issues in Study Design Petri Nokelainen petri.nokelainen@uta.fi.

RS

RS

TEST

TEST Pre

CONTROL

Pre

Pre

I

I

-

Post

Post

Post

Pre

-Pos

t

CS TEST Pre

IPos

t

CONTROL

RANDOMSAMPLING

RANDOMSELECTION

PRETEST-POSTTEST RANDOMIZED EXPERIMENT

NON-EQUIVALENT GROUPS DESIGN

CORRELATIONAL DESIGN

Issues in Study Design

Page 26: School of Education University of Tampere, Finland Issues in Study Design Petri Nokelainen petri.nokelainen@uta.fi.
Page 27: School of Education University of Tampere, Finland Issues in Study Design Petri Nokelainen petri.nokelainen@uta.fi.
Page 28: School of Education University of Tampere, Finland Issues in Study Design Petri Nokelainen petri.nokelainen@uta.fi.
Page 29: School of Education University of Tampere, Finland Issues in Study Design Petri Nokelainen petri.nokelainen@uta.fi.
Page 30: School of Education University of Tampere, Finland Issues in Study Design Petri Nokelainen petri.nokelainen@uta.fi.
Page 31: School of Education University of Tampere, Finland Issues in Study Design Petri Nokelainen petri.nokelainen@uta.fi.
Page 32: School of Education University of Tampere, Finland Issues in Study Design Petri Nokelainen petri.nokelainen@uta.fi.
Page 33: School of Education University of Tampere, Finland Issues in Study Design Petri Nokelainen petri.nokelainen@uta.fi.
Page 34: School of Education University of Tampere, Finland Issues in Study Design Petri Nokelainen petri.nokelainen@uta.fi.

• Why do, then, educational scholars use correlational designs over controlled experiments?

• The first answer is simple: Correlational designs are far easier, faster and inexpensive to conduct than experimental designs.

• The second answer is more complex as we need to ask if the controlled experiment approach is at all viable method to study educational research questions.

Issues in Study Design

Page 35: School of Education University of Tampere, Finland Issues in Study Design Petri Nokelainen petri.nokelainen@uta.fi.

• In science and psychology, most areas of interest are quite easily quantifiable and replicable (like, for example, freezing point of chocolate or systolic blood pressure).

• However, in educational research we study, for example, topics like ‘pedagogical aspects of digital learning material’ (Nokelainen, 2006) or compare pre-existing characteristics of interest (e.g., gender, age, educational level). – In such situations researchers do apply correlational designs,

but still aim to employ different types of data in the analysis with a complementary way (quasi-experimental study).

Issues in Study Design

Page 36: School of Education University of Tampere, Finland Issues in Study Design Petri Nokelainen petri.nokelainen@uta.fi.

• Case study design is applied in qualitative research. – The aim is to collect information from one or more cases and stydy,

describe and explain them through how and why questions.– Cases are represented, for example, by individuals, their communication

and experiences. (For thorough discussion, see Flyvbjerg, 2004.)

Issues in Study Design

Page 37: School of Education University of Tampere, Finland Issues in Study Design Petri Nokelainen petri.nokelainen@uta.fi.

– As a conclusion, Abelson’s (1995) concept of statistics as principled argument becomes useful: • Data analysis should not be pointlessly formal, but instead

“ ... it should make an interesting claim; it should tell a story that an informed audience will care about and it should do so by intelligent interpretation of appropriate evidence from empirical measurements or observations” (p. 2).

Issues in Study Design

Page 38: School of Education University of Tampere, Finland Issues in Study Design Petri Nokelainen petri.nokelainen@uta.fi.

• Second, optimal sample size (N) is divided into two sections in the figure: – Samples that operate in the

optimal area (n 30 – 250) for traditional parametric frequentistic techniques (Black, 1993; Tabachnick & Fidell, 1996), such as t-test or exploratory factor analysis, and the samples that fail to do so (n < 30 or n > 250).

Issues in Study Design

Page 39: School of Education University of Tampere, Finland Issues in Study Design Petri Nokelainen petri.nokelainen@uta.fi.

Estimation of sample size

• N– Population size.

• n– Estimated sample size.

• Sampling error (e)– Difference between the true

(unknown) value and observed values, if the survey were repeated (=sample collected) numerous times.

• Confidence interval– Spread of the observed values that

would be seen if the survey were repeated numerous times.

• Confidence level– How often the observed values

would be within sampling error of the true value if the survey were repeated numerous times.

(Murphy & Myors, 1998)

Page 40: School of Education University of Tampere, Finland Issues in Study Design Petri Nokelainen petri.nokelainen@uta.fi.

• Traditional non-parametric techniques, such as Mann-Whitney U-test, are considered to operate robustly, also with small samples (-> lack of power?). – Bayesian approach, however, is free of such restrictions.

Issues in Study Design

Page 41: School of Education University of Tampere, Finland Issues in Study Design Petri Nokelainen petri.nokelainen@uta.fi.

• Third, independent observations (IO) are always expected, also in time series analysis.

Issues in Study Design

Page 42: School of Education University of Tampere, Finland Issues in Study Design Petri Nokelainen petri.nokelainen@uta.fi.

• Controlled experiment designs, when conducted properly, rule out IO violations quite effectively (Martin, 2004), but correlational designs usually lack such control (e.g., to rule out employee’s co-operation when they respond to the survey questions). – On the other hand, some

qualitative techniques, like focus group analysis (Macnaghten & Myers, 2004), are heavily based on non-independent observations as informants are asked to talk to each other as an important part of the data collection.

Issues in Study Design

Page 43: School of Education University of Tampere, Finland Issues in Study Design Petri Nokelainen petri.nokelainen@uta.fi.

• Fourth, parametric techniques assume continuous (c) measurement level (ML) of indicators (i.e., so called ‘quantitative’ variables).

Issues in Study Design

Page 44: School of Education University of Tampere, Finland Issues in Study Design Petri Nokelainen petri.nokelainen@uta.fi.

Discrete

0 1 2, ..

PHENOMENON

OBSERVATION

Discrete

0 1 2, ..

Issues in Study Design

Page 45: School of Education University of Tampere, Finland Issues in Study Design Petri Nokelainen petri.nokelainen@uta.fi.

Discrete

0 1 2, ..

PHENOMENON

OBSERVATION

Continuous

0 ∞

Continuous

0 ∞

Issues in Study Design

Page 46: School of Education University of Tampere, Finland Issues in Study Design Petri Nokelainen petri.nokelainen@uta.fi.

Measurements

QuantitativeQualitative

Discrete

Nominal

Continuous

Ordinal Ordinal Interval Ratio

Issues in Study Design

Page 47: School of Education University of Tampere, Finland Issues in Study Design Petri Nokelainen petri.nokelainen@uta.fi.

• Non-parametric analysis is based on ordering of values and thus discrete (d) or, when applicable, nominal (n) values are expected (i.e., so called ‘qualitative’ variables). – A respondent’s income level

(euros) or age (years or months) is a representative example of the first indicator type.

– A Likert scale from 1 to 5 is an example of the second indicator type (ordered discrete values).

– Respondent’s gender is an example of the third indicator type (nominal discrete values).

Issues in Study Design

Page 48: School of Education University of Tampere, Finland Issues in Study Design Petri Nokelainen petri.nokelainen@uta.fi.

– It is important to note that the central limit theorem, discovered by Pierre-Simon Laplace (1749 - 1827), assures an approximate normal distribution for practically all sums of independent random variables. • For example, it allows the use of parametric t-test with

binomial or ordinal indicators (as the sample of normally distributed group means are compared, not the indicator values themselves).

– Bayesian analysis is based on discrete values, and thus, continuous values must be disceticized (automatically or manually) before the analysis.

Issues in Study Design

Page 49: School of Education University of Tampere, Finland Issues in Study Design Petri Nokelainen petri.nokelainen@uta.fi.

• Fifth, parametric techniques are technically based on the assumption of the multivariate distribution (MD) that is normal (n) by nature.

• Non-parametric techniques expect any shaped similar distributions (s). – This is a great news to anyone

who has collected real-life educational science empirical data and checked both univariate and multivariate variable distributions as usually almost all variables violate quite heavily against the normal distribution assumption with small sample sizes (e.g., below n = 100).

Issues in Study Design

Page 50: School of Education University of Tampere, Finland Issues in Study Design Petri Nokelainen petri.nokelainen@uta.fi.

• Some researchers try to force their indicators to follow multivariate normal distribution by applying various transformation techniques (e.g., logarithmic, square), but with varying success. – The motivation for transformations lies behind the fact that in

order to enable parametric analysis (i.e., based on, e.g., normal distribution) the bivariate or multivariate statistical dependencies (S) must be linear (l).

– It is important to note that this assumption does not hold for the Bayesian techniques.

Issues in Study Design

Page 51: School of Education University of Tampere, Finland Issues in Study Design Petri Nokelainen petri.nokelainen@uta.fi.

Non-parametric statisticsChi-square 2

Multiway Frequency Analysis 2

Spearman Rank Order Correlation rS

Mann-Whitney UWilcoxon Signed Rank

Kruskal-Wallis HFriedman

Bayesian dependency modeling (B-Course)

Logit analysis, Logistic regressionBayesian classification modeling (B-

Course)Categorical variable modeling (Mplus)

Parametric statistics

Pearson Product Moment Correlation rP

Independent-samples tPaired-samples tOne-way between-groups ANOVA FTwo-way repeated-measures ANOVA FANCOVA, MANOVARegression analysis RExploratory factor analysis Principal component analysis Cluster analysisDiscriminant analysis Classification analysisConfirmatory factor analysis

Issues in Study Design

Page 52: School of Education University of Tampere, Finland Issues in Study Design Petri Nokelainen petri.nokelainen@uta.fi.

• Sixth, extreme values, outliers (O), affect the results and, thus, the conclusions, of some parametric techniques severely (e.g., regression and discriminant analysis) and should be recognized and removed (see, e.g., Tabachnick & Fidell, 1996). – Non-parametric analysis

techniques are not affected by such values as their analysis is not based on multivariate normal assumption (i.e., linear dependencies between variables).

Issues in Study Design

Page 53: School of Education University of Tampere, Finland Issues in Study Design Petri Nokelainen petri.nokelainen@uta.fi.

• Seventh, when calculating correlations (C), Pearson product moment correlation (rP) should be applied with continuous indicators, and Spearman rank-order correlation (rS) with ordinal indicators. – Both techniques are valid

to detect linear dependencies.

Issues in Study Design

Page 54: School of Education University of Tampere, Finland Issues in Study Design Petri Nokelainen petri.nokelainen@uta.fi.

• The last point is to discuss about the two types of statistical dependencies (S) among the variables under analysis, namely linear (l) and non-linear (nl).

Issues in Study Design

Page 55: School of Education University of Tampere, Finland Issues in Study Design Petri Nokelainen petri.nokelainen@uta.fi.

• It is natural to assume, that both parametric and non-parametric techniques designed to detect linear dependencies work best with samples that contain linear dependencies. – However, there are non-

linear techniques, such as Bayesian analysis and neural networks that also allow the investigation of both dependency types.

Issues in Study Design

Page 56: School of Education University of Tampere, Finland Issues in Study Design Petri Nokelainen petri.nokelainen@uta.fi.

• The figure contains a reference to the qualitative analysis techniques, referring here to the empirical textual evidence based approach (e.g., individual or focus group interviews, narrative stories).

Issues in Study Design

Page 57: School of Education University of Tampere, Finland Issues in Study Design Petri Nokelainen petri.nokelainen@uta.fi.

• Firstly, it is obvious that qualitative research operates with small samples (usually n < 30).– There is nothing suspicious working with small samples:

Bartlett, Pavlov, Piaget and Skinner did that too!

Issues in Study Design

Page 58: School of Education University of Tampere, Finland Issues in Study Design Petri Nokelainen petri.nokelainen@uta.fi.

• Secondly, probability samples could also be used by qualitative researchers (as stated in the figure), but not as the only way to produce scientifically important findings.

• Gobo (2004) illustrates this by listing important qualitative research studies based solely on non-probability samples: – Alvin Gouldner (1920-1980)– Howard Becker (1928-)– Ernest De Martino (1908-1965)– David Sudnow (1938-2007)– Aaron Cicourel (1928-).

Issues in Study Design

Page 59: School of Education University of Tampere, Finland Issues in Study Design Petri Nokelainen petri.nokelainen@uta.fi.

• Gobo (2004) defines a new concept of generalizability for qualitative research by arguing that the concept of generalizability is based on the idea of social representativeness, which allows the generalizability to become a function of the invariance (regularities) of the phenomenon. – Thus, “The ethnographer does not generalize one case or event

… but its main structural aspects that can be noticed in other cases or events of the same kind or class.” (id., p. 453.)

Issues in Study Design

Page 60: School of Education University of Tampere, Finland Issues in Study Design Petri Nokelainen petri.nokelainen@uta.fi.

• Thirdly, both qualitative and Bayesian analysis techniques allow researcher to apply a priori input to the modeling process and update the model on the basis of increased level of knowledge.

Issues in Study Design

Page 61: School of Education University of Tampere, Finland Issues in Study Design Petri Nokelainen petri.nokelainen@uta.fi.

Issues in Study DesignWriting Scientific Reports

Ten Questions About YOUR Research

http://www.uta.fi/aktkk/lectures/sw

Page 62: School of Education University of Tampere, Finland Issues in Study Design Petri Nokelainen petri.nokelainen@uta.fi.

NO TURNING BACK!

Writing scientific reports

Literature review

Research questions / hypotheses

Design of the study

Methodology Link between RQ’s

and statistical analyses

Data collectionData analysisWriting scientific reportPeer Review

Conclusions

Theory

Discussion

RQ’sMethodology

Results

SampleMeasurements

Database of scientific

knowledge

Publication of the report

Original idea for the

research

Page 63: School of Education University of Tampere, Finland Issues in Study Design Petri Nokelainen petri.nokelainen@uta.fi.

Writing scientific reports

1. Title2. Author(s) name(s) and affiliation(s)3. Abstract and keywords4. Introduction / Goals or aims of the study (periodicals: research

questions)5. Theoretical framework / literature review (periodicals: research

questions)6. Research questions (dissertation)7. Method

7.1 Sample, participants7.2 Measures / instruments7.3 Procedure7.4 Statistical analyses

8. Results9. Conclusion(s) and/or Summary10. Discussion11. Acknowledgements / credits12. References13. Appendix(es)

Page 64: School of Education University of Tampere, Finland Issues in Study Design Petri Nokelainen petri.nokelainen@uta.fi.

• 1. Introduction• School leadership is currently one of the most widely studied and

published areas in social sciences. However, leadership as a social process, affecting both end products and personnel emotions, is seldom studied (Nokelainen & Ruohotie, 2006). In this sense one interesting direction to look at is Emotional Intelligence (EI) research that has recently become one of the most important constructs in modern psychological research. EI refers to “the competence to identify, express and understand emotions, assimilate emotions in thought, and regulate both positive and negative emotions in one and others” (Matthews, Zeidner, & Roberts, 2002, p. 123).

Writing scientific reports

Page 65: School of Education University of Tampere, Finland Issues in Study Design Petri Nokelainen petri.nokelainen@uta.fi.

• 2. Theoretical Framework • The theory as formulated by Salovey and Mayer (1990; Mayer &

Salovey, 1997) framed EI within a model of intelligence. Goleman’s model formulates EI in terms of a theory of performance (1998b). Goleman argues (2001) that an EI-based theory of performance has direct applicability to the domain of work and organizational effectiveness, particularly predicting excellence in jobs of all kinds, from sales to leadership. Goleman, Boyatzis and McKee further state (2002, p. 38) that EI characteristics are not innate talents, but learned abilities.

Writing scientific reports

Page 66: School of Education University of Tampere, Finland Issues in Study Design Petri Nokelainen petri.nokelainen@uta.fi.

• 2. Theoretical Framework • Theoretical framework is summarized in Figure 1. Figure

represents self-regulation (Zimmerman, 1998, 2000) as a system concept (Boekaerts & Niemivirta, 2000) managing leadership behavior through interactive processes between motivation, volition, emotion, attention, metacognition and action control systems. As Markku Hannula (2006) points out, self-regulation should be seen to be much more than mere metacognition.

Writing scientific reports

Page 67: School of Education University of Tampere, Finland Issues in Study Design Petri Nokelainen petri.nokelainen@uta.fi.

Figure 1. Self-regulation as a system concept managing leadership competence through interactive processes between different control

systems. (Adapted from Zimmerman, 2000, p. 15-16.)

Writing scientific reports

Page 68: School of Education University of Tampere, Finland Issues in Study Design Petri Nokelainen petri.nokelainen@uta.fi.

• 2. Theoretical Framework • Daniel Goleman popularized the term emotional intelligence and

claimed that EI was “as powerful and at times more powerful than IQ” in predicting life success (1995, p. 34). He aimed to show in his studies that emotional and social factors are important (1995, 1998a), but his “views on EI often went far beyond the evidence available” (Brackett et al., 2004). A recent study showed that most popular EI and ability measures are only related at r < .22, i.e. about five per cent of common variance (Brackett & Mayer, 2003).

Brackett, M. A., Lopes, P., Ivcevic, Z., Pizarro, D., Mayer, J. D., & Salovey, P. (2004). Integrating emotion and cognition: The role of emotional intelligence. In D. Dai, & R. J. Sternberg (Eds.), Motivation, emotion, and cognition: Integrating perspectives on intellectual functioning (pp. 175-194). Mahawah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

Writing scientific reports

Page 69: School of Education University of Tampere, Finland Issues in Study Design Petri Nokelainen petri.nokelainen@uta.fi.

• 3. Method• 3.1 Sample• The non-probability sample consists of 124 Finnish teachers from

four comprehensive (n = 84) and two upper secondary (n = 40) schools. All the schools were located in Helsinki, capital of Finland (about 560 000 inhabitants, 9.3% of total population 5 223 442). Each respondent was personally invited to complete a paper and pencil version of the questionnaire. Participants were asked to evaluate their attitude towards the statements measuring emotional leadership.

Writing scientific reports

Page 70: School of Education University of Tampere, Finland Issues in Study Design Petri Nokelainen petri.nokelainen@uta.fi.

• 3. Method• 3.1 Sample• The respondents’ age was classified into four categories: (1) 21 to

30 years old (n = 18, 14.5%); (2) 31 to 40 years old (n = 25, 20.2%); (3) 41 to 50 years old (n = 34, 27.4%); (4) over 50 years old (n = 39, 31.5%). Seventy per cent of the respondents were females (n = 87, 70.2%), the rest were males (n = 29, 23.4%).

Writing scientific reports

Page 71: School of Education University of Tampere, Finland Issues in Study Design Petri Nokelainen petri.nokelainen@uta.fi.

• 3. Method• 3.2 Instrument• Emotional Leadership Questionnaire operationalises Goleman and

his colleagues (2002) four domains of emotional intelligence characteristics with 51 items: (1) self-awareness, (2) self-management, (3) social awareness and (4) relationship management. Table 1 depicts four EL domains and the eighteen associated characteristics (see Appendix for item level details).

Writing scientific reports

Page 72: School of Education University of Tampere, Finland Issues in Study Design Petri Nokelainen petri.nokelainen@uta.fi.
Page 73: School of Education University of Tampere, Finland Issues in Study Design Petri Nokelainen petri.nokelainen@uta.fi.

• 4. Results• Next, we examine with descriptive statistics how subordinates’

evaluated their superior’s emotional leadership. Table 1 shows that the school principals had quite strong self-awareness (M = 3.7 – 3.8, SD = 0.8 – 1.0). This finding is natural, as especially self-confidence is an important characteristic of a good leader. On the other hand, we suspect that this result is partly a self-fulfilling prophecy as teachers expect to see those atypical Finnish mentality characteristics strongly present in their leaders.

Writing scientific reports

Page 74: School of Education University of Tampere, Finland Issues in Study Design Petri Nokelainen petri.nokelainen@uta.fi.

• 4. Results

Writing scientific reports

Page 75: School of Education University of Tampere, Finland Issues in Study Design Petri Nokelainen petri.nokelainen@uta.fi.

Figure 2. Comparison of disagreement (SD) between the three age groups on the IRSSQ dimensions.

Writing scientific reports

Page 76: School of Education University of Tampere, Finland Issues in Study Design Petri Nokelainen petri.nokelainen@uta.fi.

Figure 2. Comparison of disagreement (SD) between the three age groups on the IRSSQ dimensions.

Writing scientific reports

Page 77: School of Education University of Tampere, Finland Issues in Study Design Petri Nokelainen petri.nokelainen@uta.fi.

Figure 3. Bayesian network of Finnish school principals Emotional Leadership competencies.

Writing scientific reports

Page 78: School of Education University of Tampere, Finland Issues in Study Design Petri Nokelainen petri.nokelainen@uta.fi.

Figure 3. Bayesian network of Finnish school principals Emotional Leadership competencies.

Writing scientific reports

Page 79: School of Education University of Tampere, Finland Issues in Study Design Petri Nokelainen petri.nokelainen@uta.fi.

Writing scientific reports

Page 80: School of Education University of Tampere, Finland Issues in Study Design Petri Nokelainen petri.nokelainen@uta.fi.

Writing scientific reports

Page 81: School of Education University of Tampere, Finland Issues in Study Design Petri Nokelainen petri.nokelainen@uta.fi.

Box-plot

Writing scientific reports

Page 82: School of Education University of Tampere, Finland Issues in Study Design Petri Nokelainen petri.nokelainen@uta.fi.

• 5. Conclusions• In this paper, we presented a 51 item self-rating Likert-scale

Emotional Leadership Questionnaire (ELQ) that operationalises Goleman’s et al. (2002) four domains of emotional intelligence. Our goal in this paper was to study with an empirical sample the construct validity of the four-domain model (Goleman et al., 2002) of EL. The non-probability sample consisted of 124 Finnish school teachers from six different capital area schools.

Writing scientific reports

Page 83: School of Education University of Tampere, Finland Issues in Study Design Petri Nokelainen petri.nokelainen@uta.fi.

• 6. Discussion• We asked teacher’s to evaluate their superiors according to our

fixed, person-related questions. In the next version of the ELQ, we will add an additional scale measuring the importance of each question in a five-point Likert scale. This allows us to compare personal level EL factors to other measures, for example, the Multiple Intelligences Profiling Questionnaire (MIPQ), an operationalization of Howard Gardner’s’ MI theory, (Tirri, K., Komulainen, Nokelainen, & Tirri, H., 2002).

Writing scientific reports

Page 84: School of Education University of Tampere, Finland Issues in Study Design Petri Nokelainen petri.nokelainen@uta.fi.

• References (APA style, http://www.apa.org)• Bar-On, R., Tranel, D., Denburg, N. L., & Bechara, A. (2003). Exploring the

neurological substrate of emotional and social intelligence, Brain, 126(3), 1790-1800.

• Boekaerts, M., & Niemivirta, M. (2000). Self-regulation in learning: finding a balance between learning and ego-protective goals. In M. Boekaerts, P. R. Pintrich, & M. Zeidner (Eds.), Handbook of Self-regulation (pp. 417-450). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.

• Carmines, E. G., & Zeller, R. A. (1979). Reliability and Validity. Beverly Hills, CA: Sage Publications.

• EQ Symposium (2004). About Reuven BarOn’s Involvement in Emotional Intelligence. Retrieved April 13, 2007, from http://www.cgrowth.com/rb_biolrg.html

Writing scientific reports

Page 85: School of Education University of Tampere, Finland Issues in Study Design Petri Nokelainen petri.nokelainen@uta.fi.

Test your APA knowledge: What is WRONG with the following reference list?

Cohen, J. (1988) Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences. Second Edition. Hillsdale, NJ, Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

Hair, J. F., Anderson, R. E., Tatham R. L. & Black, W. C. (1995). Multivariate data analysis. Fourth edition. Englewood Cliffs: Prentice Hall.

Howell, David (1997). Statistical Methods for Psychology. Belmont, CA: Wadsworth Publishing Company.

Nokelainen, P., & Tirri, H. (2007). The Essential Benefits of Using Bayesian Modeling in Professional Growth Research. In S. Saari & T. Varis (Eds.), Professional Growth, 413-423). Hämeenlinna, FI: RCVE.

Kerlinger, F. 1986. Foundations of Behavioral Research. Third Edition. New York: CBS College Publishing.

Tirri, K., and Nokelainen, P. (2008). Identification of multiple intelligences with the Multiple Intelligence Profiling Questionnaire III. Psychology Science Quarterly, 50(2), 206-221.

Writing scientific reports

Page 86: School of Education University of Tampere, Finland Issues in Study Design Petri Nokelainen petri.nokelainen@uta.fi.

Issues in Study DesignWriting Scientific Reports

Ten Questions About YOUR Research

Page 87: School of Education University of Tampere, Finland Issues in Study Design Petri Nokelainen petri.nokelainen@uta.fi.

Ten questions about YOUR research

Critical Appraisal Skills Programme (CASP) http://www.phru.nhs.uk/casp/casp.htm

• Rigor• Credibility• Relevance

(Abelson, 1995.)

Page 88: School of Education University of Tampere, Finland Issues in Study Design Petri Nokelainen petri.nokelainen@uta.fi.

Screening1. Was there a clear statement of the aims of the study?2. Is a qualitative/quantitative methodology appropriate?Research Design3. Was the research design appropriate to address the aims of the

research?4. Was the sampling technique/recruitment strategy appropriate

to the aims of the research?

Ten questions about YOUR research

Page 89: School of Education University of Tampere, Finland Issues in Study Design Petri Nokelainen petri.nokelainen@uta.fi.

Data collection5. Were the data collected in a way that addressed the research

issue?• Justification of the setting for data collection.• Clarification how data were collected (e.g., questionnaire, focus group,

semi-structured interview,..).• Justification of the chosen methods.• Explicit report on methods (e.g., how the instruments were delivered,

what were the instructions, how interviews were conducted, was there a topic guide, how data was stored, ..).

• Explicit report on modifications to the methods during the study.• Discussion of the sample size (effect size, power)/saturation of data.

Ten questions about YOUR research

Page 90: School of Education University of Tampere, Finland Issues in Study Design Petri Nokelainen petri.nokelainen@uta.fi.

Reflexivity6. Has the relationship between researcher and participants been

adequately considered?• Critical examination of researchers own role, potential bias and

influence during• formulation of research questions• data collection, including sample recruitment and choice of location.

• How researcher responded to events during the study.

Ten questions about YOUR research

Page 91: School of Education University of Tampere, Finland Issues in Study Design Petri Nokelainen petri.nokelainen@uta.fi.

Ethical issues7. Have ethical issues been taken into consideration?

• Detailed description how the research was explained to participants – so that reader is able to assess whether ethical standards were maintained.

• Discussion of the issues raised by the study.

Ten questions about YOUR research

Page 92: School of Education University of Tampere, Finland Issues in Study Design Petri Nokelainen petri.nokelainen@uta.fi.

Data analysis8. Was the data analysis sufficiently rigorous?

• In-depth description of the analysis process.• Selection of the statistical techniques/use of thematic analysis (e.g., how

the categories/themes were derived from the data?)• Qualitative: How the data presented was selected from the original

sample to demonstrate the analysis process?• Is a sufficient data presented to support the findings?• To what extent contradictory data are taken into account?• Whether the researcher critically examined their own role, potential bias

and influence during analysis (qualitative: selection of data for presentation.

Ten questions about YOUR research

Page 93: School of Education University of Tampere, Finland Issues in Study Design Petri Nokelainen petri.nokelainen@uta.fi.

Findings9. Is there a clear statement of findings?

• Explicit findings.• Adequate discussion of the evidence both for and against the

researchers arguments.• Discussion of the credibility of the findings (triangulation, respondent

validation, more than one analyst,..).• Discussion of the findings in relation to the original research questions.

Ten questions about YOUR research

Page 94: School of Education University of Tampere, Finland Issues in Study Design Petri Nokelainen petri.nokelainen@uta.fi.

Value of the research10. How valuable is the research?

• Contribution to existing knowledge and understanding.• Identification of new areas where research is necessary.• Transferability (generalizability/representativeness) of findings to other

populations.• Consideration of other ways how the research may be used.

Ten questions about YOUR research

Page 95: School of Education University of Tampere, Finland Issues in Study Design Petri Nokelainen petri.nokelainen@uta.fi.

References

• Abelson, R. P. (1995). Statistics as Principled Argument. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

• Anderson, J. (1995). Cognitive Psychology and Its Implications. Freeman: New York.

• Bannan-Ritland, B. (2003). The Role of Design in Research: The Integrative Learning Design Framework. Educational Researcher, 32(1), 21-24.

• Brannen, J. (2004). Working qualitatively and quantitatively. In C. Seale, G. Gobo, J. Gubrium, & D. Silverman (Eds.), Qualitative Research Practice (pp. 312-326). London: Sage.

• Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences. Second edition. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

• Fisher, R. (1935). The design of experiments. Edinburgh: Oliver & Boyd.• Flyvbjerg, B. (2004). Five misunderstandings about case-study research. In C.

Seale, J. F. Gubrium, G. Gobo, & D. Silverman (Eds.), Qualitative Research Practice (pp. 420-434). London: Sage.

Page 96: School of Education University of Tampere, Finland Issues in Study Design Petri Nokelainen petri.nokelainen@uta.fi.

• Gigerenzer, G. (2000). Adaptive thinking. New York: Oxford University Press.• Gigerenzer, G., Krauss, S., & Vitouch, O. (2004). The null ritual: What you

always wanted to know about significance testing but were afraid to ask. In D. Kaplan (Ed.), The SAGE handbook of quantitative methodology for the social sciences (pp. 391-408). Thousand Oaks: Sage.

• Gobo, G. (2004). Sampling, representativeness and generalizability. In C. Seale, J. F. Gubrium, G. Gobo, & D. Silverman (Eds.), Qualitative Research Practice (pp. 435-456). London: Sage.

• Hair, J. F., Anderson, R. E., Tatham, R. L., & Black, W. C. (1998). Multivariate Data Analysis. Fifth edition. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall.

References

Page 97: School of Education University of Tampere, Finland Issues in Study Design Petri Nokelainen petri.nokelainen@uta.fi.

• Jackson, S. (2006). Research Methods and Statistics. A Critical Thinking Approach. Second edition. Belmont, CS: Thomson.

• Lavine, M. L. (1999). What is Bayesian Statistics and Why Everything Else is Wrong. The Journal of Undergraduate Mathematics and Its Applications, 20, 165-174.

• Luoma, M., Nokelainen, P., & Ruohotie, P. (2003, April). Learning Strategies for Police Organization - Modeling Organizational Learning Prerequisites. Paper presented at the Annual Meeting of American Educational Research Association (AERA 2002). New Orleans, USA.

• Nokelainen, P. (2006). An Empirical Assessment of Pedagogical Usability Criteria for Digital Learning Material with Elementary School Students. Journal of Educational Technology & Society, 9(2), 178-197.

References

Page 98: School of Education University of Tampere, Finland Issues in Study Design Petri Nokelainen petri.nokelainen@uta.fi.

• Nokelainen, P. (2008). Modeling of Professional Growth and Learning: Bayesian Approach. Tampere: Tampere University Press.

• Nokelainen, P., & Ruohotie, P. (2005). Investigating the Construct Validity of the Leadership Competence and Characteristics Scale. In the Proceedings of International Research on Work and Learning 2005 Conference, Sydney, Australia.

• Nokelainen, P., & Ruohotie, P. (2009). Non-linear Modeling of Growth Prerequisites in a Finnish Polytechnic Institution of Higher Education. Journal of Workplace Learning, 21(1), 36-57.

• Thompson, B. (1994). Guidelines for authors. Educational and Psychological Measurement, 54(4), 837-847.

• de Vaus, D. A. (2004). Research Design in Social Research. Third edition. London: Sage.

References