Running Head: MUSIC AND DRIVER AGGRESSIONfaculty.buffalostate.edu/hennesda/example_proposal...  ·...

21

Click here to load reader

Transcript of Running Head: MUSIC AND DRIVER AGGRESSIONfaculty.buffalostate.edu/hennesda/example_proposal...  ·...

Page 1: Running Head: MUSIC AND DRIVER AGGRESSIONfaculty.buffalostate.edu/hennesda/example_proposal...  · Web view2010-12-02 · Mild driver aggression has been defined as any intentionally

The Influence of Music on Mild Driver Aggression

Name

Student ID

Class

Page 2: Running Head: MUSIC AND DRIVER AGGRESSIONfaculty.buffalostate.edu/hennesda/example_proposal...  · Web view2010-12-02 · Mild driver aggression has been defined as any intentionally

The Influence of Music on Driver Stress

Mild driver aggression has been defined as any intentionally harmful action within the

driving context, including horn honking, swearing, and using hand gestures (Hauber, 1980;

Hennessy & Wiesenthal, 1999). Compared to more extreme acts of roadway violence (e.g.

fighting, chasing, and shooting) mild driver aggression is a fairly common response to

frustration, irritation, and hassles (Hennessy & Wiesenthal, 2001b). Recent research has found

that traffic congestion can be a powerful precursor to roadway aggression, largely due to

increased stress, arousal, and time urgency (Shinar, 1998; Gulian, Matthews, Glendon, Davies, &

Debney, 1989b; Hennessy & Wiesenthal, 1999). As traffic congestion increases, other drivers

are viewed as an obstacle to reaching a destination in a desired time frame, leading to stress

induced anger and frustration (Shinar, 1989; Stokols, Novaco, Stokols, & Campbell, 1978).

Music has been found to reduce stress, anger, agitation, and arousal due, in part, to

distraction (McCaffery, 1990; Wiesenthal, Hennessy, & Totten, 2000b; Wostratzky, Braun, &

Roth, 1988). According to Hoyos (1988), as the number of demands for attention increase, the

amount of resources available to any single source is decreased. Music acts as a distracter in that

it assumes a portion of cognitive or attentional resources that might otherwise be directed toward

a negative or demanding stimulus (Baron, 1986). As attention to music increases, cognitive

resources are diverted from negative stimuli, thus reducing negative affect and behavior. Within

the driving environment, frustration and provocation from other drivers represent common

negative events that have been found to increase the likelihood of mild aggression (McGarva &

Steiner, 2001). By redirecting attention from such conditions, music should consequently

minimize the motivation, justification, and objective behind mild driver aggression.

2

Page 3: Running Head: MUSIC AND DRIVER AGGRESSIONfaculty.buffalostate.edu/hennesda/example_proposal...  · Web view2010-12-02 · Mild driver aggression has been defined as any intentionally

Music may also influence negative behavior through relaxation (Kroener, Diergarten,

Diergarten, & Seeger, 1988; Stratton, 1992). Under demanding or undesirable conditions, heart

rate, blood pressure, general arousal and negative affect have been found to increase (Everly,

1989; Novaco Stokols, Campbell & Stokols, 1979). These in turn have been linked to increased

aggressive behaviors in a variety of settings, such as crowding (Worchel & Teddlie, 1976),

excessive noise (Donnerstein & Wilson, 1976), exercise (Zillmann, Katcher, & Milavsky, 1972),

and traffic congestion (Hennessy, 2000). However, music has been found to reduce both

physiological and psychological stress and arousal (Hammer, 1996; Parente, 1989; Takeshi &

Nakamura, 1991), particularly when it is preferred or self selected musical varieties (Allen &

Blascovich, 1994; Stratton & Zalanowski, 1984). The ability of music to reduce these processes

in the driving environment should also lead to decreased driver aggression.

Arguably, the strongest evidence of a link between music and aggression has emerged

from the field of music therapy. Music has been consistently found to reduce agitation, anger,

negative thoughts, and aggression (Bright, 1986; Montello & Coons, 1998). Nonetheless, this

effect has been demonstrated most prominently in clinical and elderly samples with behavioral or

cognitive disorders (e.g. Gardiner, Furois, Tansley, & Morgan, 2000; Montello & Coons, 1998).

Currently it is not clear if this influence is directly generalizable to every day situations, such as

vehicular driving. Contrary evidence has begun to suggest that aggressive lyrics and symbols in

rock, heavy metal and rap music may actually increase anger and aggressive tendencies

(Barongan & Hall, 1995). This research has typically utilized overtly aggressive lyrics that are

not the norm across the majority of musical genres. Further, they have relied heavily on

laboratory based manipulations which lack contextual elements that can impact aggression in

3

Page 4: Running Head: MUSIC AND DRIVER AGGRESSIONfaculty.buffalostate.edu/hennesda/example_proposal...  · Web view2010-12-02 · Mild driver aggression has been defined as any intentionally

applied settings, such as personal interactions and aggressive cues (Berkowitz, 1993). In order to

understand the potential impact of music on driver aggression, situational realism is important.

According to Hennessy & Wiesenthal (1999), roadway aggression is dependent on the

interaction of the driver with their social and physical environment. While previous methods

have been used to collect driver responses immediately following a commute, such as tape

recorders (Underwood, Chapman, Wright, & Crundall, 1999) and diaries (Gulian, Glendon,

Matthews, Davies, & Debney, 1990), present study was designed as a more direct examination of

the influence of self selected music on the frequency of mild driver aggression in actual low and

high traffic congestion conditions. It is predicted that mild driver aggression will be influenced

by the interaction of congestion level X music. Specifically, drivers in low congestion

conditions will exhibit elevated driver aggression when not listening to music.

Method

Participants

The present study will include 20 female and 20 male volunteers that commute daily to

work in Buffalo New York. The ages will range from 20 – 65. They must commute at least 20

minutes daily, be the primary driver during their commute, and possess a minimum of 3 years

driving experience.

Materials

A Nokia cellular telephone with hands free and voice activated dialing capabilities will

be used to conduct driving interviews. Driver aggression will be measured using the State Driver

Aggression Questionnaire (SDAQ - Hennessy & Wiesenthal, 1997, 1999). This tool was

developed to measure the frequency of mild aggressive behaviors in actual driving situations and

4

Page 5: Running Head: MUSIC AND DRIVER AGGRESSIONfaculty.buffalostate.edu/hennesda/example_proposal...  · Web view2010-12-02 · Mild driver aggression has been defined as any intentionally

was designed to be administered verbally. Responses are placed on a Likert scale ranging from

0=“not at all” to 100=“very frequently”, indicating how frequently each behavior is used in the

specific driving situations during the previous five minutes. Items include horn honking out of

frustration, flashing high beams out of frustration, swearing/yelling at other drivers, purposely

tailgating, and using obscene gestures. An aggregate mild driver aggression score will then be

calculated as the mean response to the five individual items separately in low and high

congestion. Higher scores indicate a greater use of mild aggressive driving behaviors in that

condition. It has been found to have high internal reliability (alpha = .92; Hennessy, 2000).

Procedure

Instructions regarding the experimental procedure and cellular telephone operation will

be given during the initial appointment. Participants will then provide information regarding

their regular travel route along Highway 401, since all measures will be administered during

their usual daily commute. For each participant, two areas along their regular commuting route

will be chosen: one that is typically lowest and one that is typically highest in traffic congestion.

These will represent the two areas in which participants will later initiate cellular telephone calls

to the experimenter to complete the questionnaire. Both the low and high congestion telephone

interviews will be scheduled during a single journey.

Drivers will be randomly assigned to either a “music” or “non-music” group, with a

stipulation of equal gender representation in each group. The music group will be instructed to

select a cassette tape or compact disc (no radio will be allowed) of their favorite music to play

during their entire trip. The non-music group will be instructed specifically not to listen to music

5

Page 6: Running Head: MUSIC AND DRIVER AGGRESSIONfaculty.buffalostate.edu/hennesda/example_proposal...  · Web view2010-12-02 · Mild driver aggression has been defined as any intentionally

(or talk radio) during their entire journey. All participants will be alone in their automobiles for

the duration of their journey.

All participants will then drive their normal commuting route. Upon approaching their

first target area (which will be designated during the initial meeting with the experimenters), they

will telephone the researcher, utilizing the voice activated feature. Once a successful telephone

contact is made the SDAQ will be administered verbally. Upon completion of the first telephone

interview, the telephone call will be terminated and the participants will continue driving until

their second target area is reached. At that time a second call will be made, the SDAQ will be

completed verbally again, and the telephone call will be terminated. All measures will be

obtained on weekdays to maintain consistency in congestion levels and no measurements will be

collected during rain or snow in order to avoid confounding by poor weather.

6

Page 7: Running Head: MUSIC AND DRIVER AGGRESSIONfaculty.buffalostate.edu/hennesda/example_proposal...  · Web view2010-12-02 · Mild driver aggression has been defined as any intentionally

References

Allen, K., & Blascovich, J. (1994). Effects of music on cardiovascular reactivity among

surgeons. Journal of the American Medical Association, 272, 882-884.

Anderson, R. A., Baron, R. S., & Logan, H. (1991). Distraction, control, and dental stress.

Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 21, 156-171.

Baron, R. S. (1986). Distraction-conflict theory. Progress and problems. In L. Berkowitz (Ed.),

Advances in experimental social psychology, Vol. 19, (pp. 1-40). New York: Academic

Press.

Barongan, C., & Hall, G. C. N. (1995). The influence of misogynous rap music on sexual

aggression against women. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 19, 195-207.

Berkowitz, L. (1993). Aggression: Its causes and consequences. Boston: McGraw Hill.

Bright, R. (1986). The use of music therapy and activities with demented patients who

are deemed “difficult to manage.” Clinical Gerontologist, 6, Special Issue: The

elderly uncooperative patient, 131-144.

Brodsky, W. (2002). The effects of music tempo on simulated driving performance and

vehicular control. Transportation Research Part F, 4, 219-241.

Caspy, T., Peleg, E., Schlam, D., & Goldberg, J. (1988). Sedative and stimulative music effects:

Differential effects on performance impairment following frustration. Motivation and

Emotion, 12, 123-138.

Donnerstein, E., & Wilson, D. (1976). The effects of noise and perceived control upon ongoing

and subsequent aggressive behavior. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 34,

774-781.

Duckro, P. N., Korytnyk, N. X., & Vandenberg, B. R. (1989). The Stress-Arousal Checklist as a

Page 8: Running Head: MUSIC AND DRIVER AGGRESSIONfaculty.buffalostate.edu/hennesda/example_proposal...  · Web view2010-12-02 · Mild driver aggression has been defined as any intentionally

measure of situational stress versus simple arousal. Psychological Reports, 64, 239-242.

Everly, G. S. (1989). A clinical guide to the treatment of the human stress response. New York:

Plenum.

Furnham, A., & Bradley, A. (1997). Music while you work: The differential distraction of

background music on the cognitive test performance of introverts and extroverts.

Applied Cognitive Psychology, 11, 445-455.

Gardiner, J. C., Furois, M., Tansley, D. P., & Morgan, B. (2000). Music therapy and

reading as intervention strategies for disruptive behavior in dementia. Clinical

Gerontologist, 22, 31-46.

Gerdner, L. A. (2000). Effects of individualized versus classical "relaxation" music on the

frequency of agitation in elderly persons with Alzheimer's disease and related disorders.

International Psychogeriatrics, 12, 49-65.

Gowensmith, W. N., & Bloom, L. J. (1997). Effects of heavy metal music on arousal and anger.

Journal of Music Therapy, 34, 33-45.

Guar, S. D. (1988). Noise: Does it make you angry? Indian Psychologist, 5, 51-56.

Gulian, E., Debney, L. M., Glendon, A. I., Davies, D. R., & Matthews, G. (1989a). Coping with

driver stress. In F. McGuigan, W. E. Sime, & J. M. Wallace (Eds.), Stress and tension

control (Vol. 3) (pp. 173-186). New York: Plenum.

Gulian, E., Matthews, G., Glendon, A. I., Davies, D. R., & Debney, L. M. (1989b).

Dimensions of driver stress. Ergonomics, 32, 585-602.

Gulian, E., Glendon, A. I., Matthews, G., Davies, D. R., & Debney, M. (1990). The stress of

driving: A diary study. Work and Stress, 4, 7-16.

Haigney, D. E., Taylor, R. G., & Westerman, S. J. (2000). Concurrent mobile (cellular) phone

Page 9: Running Head: MUSIC AND DRIVER AGGRESSIONfaculty.buffalostate.edu/hennesda/example_proposal...  · Web view2010-12-02 · Mild driver aggression has been defined as any intentionally

use and driving performance: Task demand characteristics and compensatory processes.

Transportation Research Part F, 113-121.

Hanser, S. B., & Thompson, L. W. (1994). Effects of a music therapy strategy on depressed

older adults. Journal of Gerontology, 49, 265-269.

Hauber, A. R. (1980). The social psychology of driving behavior and the traffic environment:

Research on aggressive behavior in traffic. International Review of Applied Psychology,

29, 461-474.

Hennessy, D. A. (2000). The interaction of person and situation within the driving environment:

Daily hassles, traffic congestion, driver stress, aggression, vengeance and past

performance. Dissertation Abstracts International: Section B: The Sciences and

Engineering. Vol. 60(8-B): 4301.

Hennessy, D. A., & Wiesenthal, D. L. (1997). The relationship between traffic congestion,

driver stress, and direct versus indirect coping behaviours. Ergonomics, 40, 348-361.

Hennessy, D. A., & Wiesenthal, D. L. (1999). Traffic congestion, driver stress, and driver

aggression. Aggressive Behavior, 25, 409-423.

Hennessy, D. A. & Wiesenthal, D. L. (2001a). Further validation of the Driving Vengeance

Questionnaire. Violence and Victims, 16, 565-573.

Hennessy, D. A., & Wiesenthal, D. L. (2001b). Gender, driver aggression, and driver violence:

An applied evaluation. Sex Roles, 44, 661 – 675.

Hennessy, D. A., Wiesenthal, D. L., & Kohn, P. M. (2000). The influence of traffic congestion,

daily hassles, and trait stress susceptibility on state driver stress: An interactive

perspective. Journal of Applied Biobehavioral Research, 5, 162-179.

Holland, P. (1995). The role of music in the effective use of stress. In T. Wigram, B. Saperston,

Page 10: Running Head: MUSIC AND DRIVER AGGRESSIONfaculty.buffalostate.edu/hennesda/example_proposal...  · Web view2010-12-02 · Mild driver aggression has been defined as any intentionally

& R. West (Eds.), The art and science of music therapy: A handbook (pp. 406-432).

Longshore, PA: Harwood Academic Press.

Hoyos, C. G. (1988). Mental load and risk in traffic behaviour. Ergonomics, 31, 571-584.

Kroener, H.B., Diergarten, D., Diergarten, D., & Seeger, S. R. (1988). Psychophysiological

reactivity of migraine sufferers in conditions of stress and relaxation. Journal of

Psychosomatic Research, 32, 483-492.

Litman, C. S. (1997). Effects of rap music on verbal and nonverbal aggressive and

disruptive behavior in boys' social interactions. Dissertation Abstracts International:

Section B: The Sciences & Engineering, 57(10-B): 6581.

Lou, M. F. (2001). The use of music to decrease agitated behaviour of the demented

elderly: The state of the science. Scandinavian Journal of Caring Sciences, 15,

165-173.

Mackay, C. J., Cox, T., Burrows, C. G., & Lazzerini, A. J. (1978). An inventory for the

measurement of self reported stress and arousal. British Journal of Social and Clinical

Psychology, 17, 283-284.

McCaffery, M. (1990). Nursing approaches to nonpharmacological pain control. International

Journal of Nursing Studies, 27, 1-5.

McGarva, A. R., & Steiner, M. (2000). Provoked driver aggression and status: A field study.

Transportation Research Part F, 3, 167-179.

Montello, L., & Coons, E. E. (1998). Effects of active versus passive group music therapy on

preadolescents with emotional, learning, and behavioral disorders. Journal of Music

Therapy, 35, 49-67.

Novaco, R. W. (1991). Aggression on roadways. In R. Baenninger (Ed.), Targets of violence

Page 11: Running Head: MUSIC AND DRIVER AGGRESSIONfaculty.buffalostate.edu/hennesda/example_proposal...  · Web view2010-12-02 · Mild driver aggression has been defined as any intentionally

and aggression (pp. 253-326). North-Holland: Elsevier Science Publisher.

Novaco, R. W., Stokols, D., Campbell, J., & Stokols, J. (1979). Transportation, stress, and

community psychology. American Journal of Community Psychology, 7, 361-380.

Parente, A. B. (1989). Music as a therapeutic tool in treating anorexia nervosa. In L. M.

Hornyak and E. K. Baker (Eds.), Experiential therapies for eating disorders (pp. 305-

328). New York: Guilford Press.

Poulton, E. C. (1979). Composite model for human performance in continuous noise.

Psychological Review, 86, 361-375.

Rubin, A. M., West, D. V., & Mitchell, W. S. (2001). Differences in aggression, attitudes

toward women, and distrust as reflected in popular music preferences. Media

Psychology, 3, 25-42.

Selye, H. (1974). Stress without distress. Philadelphia: Lippincott.

Shinar, D. (1998). Aggressive driving: The contribution of the drivers and the situation.

Transportation Research Part F, 1, 137-160.

Sidorenko, V. N. (2000). Effects of Medical Resonance Therapy Music in the complex

treatment of epileptic patients. Integrative Physiological & Behavioral Science, 35,

212-217.

Stern, E. (1999). Reactions to congestion under time pressure. Transportation Research Part C,

7, 75-90.

Stokols, D., Novaco, R. W., Stokols, J., & Campbell, J. (1978). Traffic congestion, Type A

behaviour, and stress. Journal of Applied Psychology, 63, 467-480.

Stratton, V. N. (1992). Influence of music and socializing on perceived stress while waiting.

Perceptual and Motor Skills, 75, 334.

Page 12: Running Head: MUSIC AND DRIVER AGGRESSIONfaculty.buffalostate.edu/hennesda/example_proposal...  · Web view2010-12-02 · Mild driver aggression has been defined as any intentionally

Stratton, V. N., & Zalanowski, A. H. (1984). The relationship between music, degree of liking,

and self-reported relaxation. Journal of Music Therapy, 21, 184-192.

Takeshi, H. & Nakamura, M. (1991). Can antistress music tapes reduce mental stress? Stress

Medicine, 7, 181-184.

Underwood, G., Chapman, P., Wright, S., & Crundall, D. (1999). Anger while driving.

Transportation Research Part F, 2, 55-68.

Wiesenthal, D. L., Hennessy, D. A., & Gibson, P. (2000a). The Driving Vengeance

Questionnaire (DVQ): The development of a scale to measure deviant drivers' attitudes.

Violence and Victims, 15, 115-136.

Wiesenthal, D. L., Hennessy, D. A., & Totten, B. (2000b). The influence of music on driver

stress. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 30, 1709-1719.

Worchel, S., & Teddlie, C. (1976). The experience of crowding: A two-factor theory. Journal

of Personality and Social Psychology, 34, 30-40.

Wostratzky, S., Braun, E., & Roth, N. (1988). The influence of distraction on coping with stress

in dentistry. Activitas Nervosa Superior, 30, 120-121.

Zillmann, D., Katcher, A. H., & Milavsky, B. (1972). Excitation transfer from physical exercise

to subsequent aggressive behavior. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 8, 247-

259.