Ronneberg - The Protective Effects of Religiosity on Depression - A 2-Year Prospective Study

download Ronneberg - The Protective Effects of Religiosity on Depression - A 2-Year Prospective Study

of 12

Transcript of Ronneberg - The Protective Effects of Religiosity on Depression - A 2-Year Prospective Study

  • 8/18/2019 Ronneberg - The Protective Effects of Religiosity on Depression - A 2-Year Prospective Study

    1/12

    Page 1 of 12© The Author 2014. Published by Oxford University Press on behalf of The Gerontological Society of America. All rights reserved.

    For permissions, please e-mail: [email protected].

    The Protective Effects of Religiosity on

    Depression: A 2-Year Prospective Study

    Corina R. Ronneberg, MS,* Edward Alan Miller, PhD, MPA,

    Elizabeth Dugan, PhD, and Frank Porell, PhD

    Department of Gerontology, John E. McCormack Graduate School of Policy & Global Studies, University

    of Massachusetts Boston.

    *Address correspondence to Corina R. Ronneberg, MS, Department of Gerontology, John E. McCormack Graduate School

    of Policy & Global Studies, University of Massachusetts Boston, 100 Morrissey Blvd., Boston, MA 02125. E-mail: Corina.

    [email protected]

    Received October 29 2013; Accepted June 3 2014.

    Decision Editor: Rachel Pruchno, PhD

    Purpose of the Study: Approximately 20% of older adults are diagnosed with depression

    in the United States. Extant research suggests that engagement in religious activity, or

    religiosity, may serve as a protective factor against depression. This prospective study

    examines whether religiosity protects against depression and/or aids in recovery.

    Design and Methods: Study data are drawn from the 2006 and 2008 waves of the Health

    and Retirement Study. The sample consists of 1,992 depressed and 5,740 nondepressed

    older adults (mean age = 68.12 years), at baseline (2006), for an overall sample size

    of 7,732. Logistic regressions analyzed the relationship between organizational (service

    attendance), nonorganizational (private prayer), and intrinsic measures of religiosity anddepression onset (in the baseline nondepressed group) and depression recovery (in the

    baseline depressed group) at follow-up (2008), controlling for other baseline factors.

    Results: Religiosity was found to both protect against and help individuals recover from

    depression. Individuals not depressed at baseline remained nondepressed 2 years later

    if they frequently attended religious services, whereas those depressed at baseline were

    less likely to be depressed at follow-up if they more frequently engaged in private prayer.

    Implications: Findings suggest that both organizational and nonorganizational forms of

    religiosity affect depression outcomes in different circumstances (i.e., onset and recov-

    ery, respectively). Important strategies to prevent and relieve depression among older

    adults may include improving access and transportation to places of worship among

    those interested in attending services and facilitating discussions about religious activi-

    ties and beliefs with clinicians.

    Key words:  Organizational religiosity, Public religiosity, Nonorganizational religiosity, Private religiosity, Intrinsic

    religiosity, Religion, Social support, Mental health

    Depression is a major concern in the United States, as

    more than 5% of the general population over 12 years old

    reports being depressed at any given time (Pratt & Brody, 

    2008). The prevalence of depression becomes even more

    alarming at older ages as 20% of those 65 years and older

    report being depressed (Hurst, Williams, King, & Viken, 

    The Gerontologist , 2014, Vol. 00, No. 00, 1–12

    doi:10.1093/geront/gnu073

    Research Article

     The Gerontologist Advance Access published July 25, 2014

    mailto:[email protected]?subject=mailto:[email protected]?subject=mailto:[email protected]?subject=mailto:[email protected]?subject=

  • 8/18/2019 Ronneberg - The Protective Effects of Religiosity on Depression - A 2-Year Prospective Study

    2/12

    2008; Paukert et al., 2008). Because depression is one of

    the most common mental health issues facing older adults

    (Administration on Aging, 2012), the study of depression

    and its correlates has become a priority among scholars

    looking to improve the quality of life among this popula-

    tion (Administration on Aging, 2012).

    Although conflicting results have been reported (e.g., posi-

    tive, negative, or no relationship), recent investigations suggestthat involvement in religious activity may serve as a protective

    factor against depression (Blay, Batista, Andreoli, & Gastal, 

    2008; Blazer, 2010; Hayward, Owen, Koenig, Steffens, & 

    Payne, 2012; King et al., 2007; Koenig, 2007, 2009; Law 

    & Sbarra, 2009; Smith, McCullough, & Poll, 2003). These

    investigations also suggest that individuals who are more

    religious may not only be more likely to recover from cer-

    tain ailments such as acute myocardial infarction (Martin & 

    Levy, 2006) and severe mental illness (Webb, Charbonneau, 

    McCann, & Gayle, 2011), but do so more quickly, while

    experiencing shorter hospitalization stays (Contrada et al., 2004). Together, these findings suggest a potentially impor-

    tant avenue for preventing and/or promoting recovery from

    depression, especially given the large role that religion plays

    in the lives of most Americans, 90% of whom report believ-

    ing in God or a universal spirit (Gallup, 2013) and 90% of

    whom report engaging in prayer (Hill et al., 2000).

    The role that religion plays in people’s lives becomes

    more pronounced with age. One national survey, for

    example, found that nearly 70% of adults 50 years or

    older reported that religion is very important in their lives

    compared with 44% of adults under 30 years old (Cohen 

    & Koenig, 2003). Older adults also exhibit higher levels

    of religiosity or actual involvement in religious activities

    (Boswell, Kahana, & Dilworth-Anderson, 2006). The fact

    that religiosity appears to increase with age coupled with

    the high prevalence of depression among older adults sug-

    gests the need to further study the effects of religious beliefs

    and activities on depression among the elderly.

    The need to further study the effects of religiosity on

    depression is also suggested by current research. Most exist-

    ing research in this area has been cross-sectional (Blay et al., 

    2008; Branco, 2000; Lawler-Row & Elliott, 2009; Waddell 

    & Jacobs-Lawson, 2010; Yohannes, Koenig, Baldwin, & Connolly, 2008). That which is longitudinal has focused on

    limited population subsets (e.g., African Americans elders

    and adolescents with psychiatric conditions) (Dew et al., 

    2010; Ellison & Flannelly, 2009), local or regional popula-

    tions (Idler & Kasl, 1992; King et al., 2007; Koenig et al., 

    1997; Koenig, George, & Peterson, 1998; Sun et al., 2012),

    and non-U.S. samples (e.g., Australian, Lebanese, Israeli,

    European elders, or Brazilian) (Braam et al., 2001; Chaaya, 

    Sibai, Fayad, & El-Roueiheb, 2007; Iecovich, 2001; Law & 

    Sbarra, 2009; Payman, George, & Ryburn, 2008). Sample

    sizes also tend to be small and focus exclusively on, for

    example, the effects of religiosity on depression recovery,

    rather than both depression onset and recovery (Bosworth, 

    Park, McQuoid, Hays, & Steffens, 2003; Hayward et al., 

    2012; Koenig et al., 1998). There is a lack of consistency

    in measurement as well, with one or more religiosity meas-

    ures tending to be employed operationalizing such concepts

    as organizational religiosity, nonorganizational religiosity,intrinsic religiosity, religious salience, religious affiliation,

    religious orthodoxy, and religious coping, though indica-

    tors of the former three domains are most commonly used

    (Blay et al., 2008; Bosworth et al., 2003; Braam, Beekman, 

    Deeg, Smit, & Tilburh, 1997; Braam et al., 2004; Branco, 

    2000; Hayward et al., 2012; Idler & Kasl, 1992; King et al., 

    2007; Koenig et al., 1997, 1998; Levin, 2010; Schnittker, 

    2001; Sun et al., 2012).

    The primary goals of this study are to assess depres-

    sion levels both at baseline and 2 years later, in order to

    determine (a) whether religiosity protects against depres-sion and (b) whether religiosity aids in the recovery from

    depression. Shortcomings in extant research are addressed

    in several ways. First, we use a larger, more representative

    sample than the previous work, focusing expressly on older

    adults (residing in the United States), and employ a longi-

    tudinal perspective. Second, we employ a comprehensive

    set of religiosity indicators, including organizational, non-

    organizational, and intrinsic measures, as well as religious

    salience, affiliation, and presence of both friends and rela-

    tives at one’s place of worship.

    Religiosity, Depression, and Older Adults

    The biopsychosocial diathesis-stress model of depression

    (BPDS) posits that there are certain interconnected bio-

    logical, psychological, and social factors that can affect an

    individual’s predisposition to depression (Schotte, Bossche, 

    Doncker, Claes, & Cosyns, 2006; Stein, 2005). These

    include risk factors that can serve both as potential pre-

    cursors of depression and potential protective factors that

    can act as buffers against depression. Some individuals are

    more vulnerable to depression due to biological factors

    (e.g., age and gender), somatic factors (e.g., health status,chronic conditions, disability, or recent medical setbacks),

    psychological factors (e.g., mental illness and serious alco-

    holic use), and social influences (e.g., marital status, edu-

    cation, income, social support, volunteerism, and adverse

    life events). In this study, religiosity is conceptualized as a

    protective factor, which may help buffer against one’s total

    vulnerability for depression.

    Religiosity typically refers not only to a belief in a higher

    entity or something greater than oneself but also formal

    involvement in organized religious activities and specific,

    The Gerontologist , 2014, Vol. 00, No. 00 Page 2 of 12

  • 8/18/2019 Ronneberg - The Protective Effects of Religiosity on Depression - A 2-Year Prospective Study

    3/12

    measurable acts such as prayer, meditation, service attend-

    ance, religious readings, and affiliation with a particular reli-

    gion or place of worship (Hill et al., 2000; Iecovich, 2001;

    Yohannes et al., 2008). A key characteristic of religion is that

    it is organized in a hierarchical fashion with an identified

    authority figure such as a priest, pastor, or rabbi presiding.

    Although religion refers to someone’s belief system, religios-

    ity is the actual application of such beliefs in daily life.The literature distinguishes between three general types

    of religiosity: organizational, nonorganizational, and intrin-

    sic. Organizational religiosity typically involves public or

    group activities and is most commonly measured by one’s

    religious service attendance (Koenig et al., 1998; Sun et al., 

    2012). Nonorganizational religiosity, by contrast, is more

    private and typically occurs on a person’s own time, alone,

    encompassing activities such as reading religious texts,

    praying, and/or meditating (Koenig et al., 1998; Sun et al., 

    2012). Intrinsic religiosity is concerned with individuals’

    subjective meaning of religiosity and how religious beliefsaffect everyday life (Sun et al., 2012). Studies have shown

    that as individuals age, they are more likely to engage in

    nonorganizational activities as opposed to organizational

    modes of religious expression (Yohannes et al., 2008), pos-

    sibly shifting to more private religious activities, perhaps

    due to physical decline, rather than giving up on religious

    involvement altogether.

    Nearly 75% of older people who suffer from depres-

    sion or anxiety partake in some kind of religious activity at

    least monthly (Paukert et al., 2009). A meta-analysis of 147

    studies found higher religiosity to be associated with fewer

    depressive symptoms or indicators in more than three quar-

    ters of the studies analyzed (Smith et al., 2003). Furthermore,

    individuals who regularly attend religious services display

    lower rates of depression when compared with individu-

    als who either do not attend services or do attend services

    but on a more sporadic basis (Blazer, 2010; Braam et al., 

    2004; Koenig et al., 1997). In a prospective study focusing

    on African Americans over the age of 55, it was found that

    individuals who received guidance from their religion on a

    regular basis were less likely to suffer from major depression

    3–4 years later (Ellison & Flannelly, 2009).

    Extant research also demonstrates that religious involve-ment may benefit clinically depressed individuals (Koenig 

    et al., 1998; Murphy & Fitchett, 2009). Depressive symp-

    toms have been shown to decrease across time in persons

    engaged in organizational religiosity (Braam et al., 2004;

    Koenig, 2007; Law & Sbarra, 2009; Levin, 2010; Smith 

    et al., 2003). However, mixed results abound regarding

    nonorganizational modes of religious involvement. For

    instance, one cross-sectional study found nonorganiza-

    tional religiosity unrelated to depression (Koenig et al., 

    1997), whereas another found an inverse relationship

    between nonorganizational religiosity and depression

    cross-sectionally, but a U-shaped association longitudinally

    (King et al., 2007). Yet another study found nonorganiza-

    tional religiosity to be associated with lower depression

    severity after 3 months (Hayward et al., 2012). Findings

    around intrinsic religiosity are also inconsistent. Parker and 

    coworkers (2003) found no relationship between intrinsic

    religiosity and depression, King and coworkers (2007)  apositive relationship, and Koenig and coworkers (1998) 

    quicker remission from depression.

    Evidence suggests that the impact of religiosity on depres-

    sion is stronger among women who also tend to be more

    active participants in both organizational and nonorgani-

    zational religious activities than men, including, for exam-

    ple, religious affiliation and private prayer (Wink & Dillon, 

    2002; Yohannes et al., 2008). This tendency is reflected in

    a 2002 Health and Retirement Study (HRS) of older adults

    60 years or older, which found higher ratings of religious

    importance to be a protective factor against depression inwomen—but not men (Waddell & Jacobs-Lawson, 2010).

    Based on previous research and according to the BPDS

    model of depression, this study hypothesizes that (a)

    higher religiosity will be associated with a lower likelihood

    of depression 2 years later among older adults without

    depression at baseline (i.e., religiosity will protect against

    depression onset) and (b) higher religiosity will be associ-

    ated with a lower likelihood of depression 2 years later for

    respondents depressed at baseline (i.e., religiosity will aid in

    depression recovery).

    Methods

    Data Source

    The sample in this study was drawn from the 2006 and

    2008 waves of the HRS; the goal was to model depres-

    sion in 2008 based on respondent characteristics in 2006.

    The HRS is sponsored by the National Institute on Aging

    (grant number NIA U01AG009740) and is conducted by

    the University of Michigan (Health and Retirement Study, 

    2006 /2008). The HRS began collecting data in 1992 and

    continues to do so every 2 years. The HRS is a nationally

    representative study that contains rich information onmore than 22,000 community-dwelling older adults aged

    50 or older, with respect to respondent health, functional

    status, cognition, living arrangements, retirement, religious

    affiliation, and involvement in assorted activities.

    Sample

    The sample utilized in this study includes the subset of

    HRS respondents who completed the 2006 Leave-Behind

    Questionnaire (n = 7,732). The rationale for utilizing the

    The Gerontologist , 2014, Vol. 00, No. 00Page 3 of 12

  • 8/18/2019 Ronneberg - The Protective Effects of Religiosity on Depression - A 2-Year Prospective Study

    4/12

    Leave-Behind Questionnaire is that it contains much more

    comprehensive measures of religiosity than the basic HRS

    survey. In all, 944, or 12% of respondents, were missing

    information on at least one HRS item. Thus, for purposes

    of our analyses, we employed multiple imputation of miss-

    ing data to fill in the missing values. Twenty imputations

    were conducted and pooled results were used in the analy-

    ses reported.

    Measurement

    Dependent Variable (Depression)

    Depression status (in 2008) is the outcome variable in

    the present study. Depression is assessed with Center for

    Epidemiological Studies Depression scale (CESD-8). The

    presence of three or more symptoms, out of eight, indicates

    a higher likelihood of being clinically depressed (Steffick, 

    2000). Therefore, respondents reporting three or more

    depressive symptoms were coded 1 = depressed; those withzero, one, or two symptoms as 0 = not depressed.

    Independent Variables (Religiosity)

    Five religiosity questions are asked in the basic HRS.

    Religious affiliation was self-reported as Protestant,

    Catholic, Jewish, or none/other religion. Respondents were

    asked about organizational religiosity, via the frequency

    of attendance at religious services: high (more than once

    a week or once a week), moderate (two to three times a

    month), and low/none (one or more times a year or not at

    all). Additionally, respondents were asked about the pres-

    ence of both friends and relatives in one’s congregation (yes

    or no). Lastly, respondents were asked to rate the impor-

    tance of religiosity: very, somewhat, or not important.

    Each of these items was coded as a series of dichotomous

    variables.

    The Leave-Behind Questionnaire includes two addi-

    tional measures of religiosity. The first is an index of

    religiosity, an intrinsic measure, composed of four items

    (α  = .92)—believe God watches over me, events unfold

    according to a divine/greater plan, carry religious beliefs

    into all dealings in life, find strength and comfort in reli-

     gion. Possible scores range from 1 (strongly disagree) to6 (strongly agree) (averaged across the four items) where

    higher scores indicate higher religiosity levels. The second

    Leave-Behind Questionnaire item measured the frequency

    of prayer in private contexts, a nonorganizational meas-

    ure. The scores (1–8) on this item were reverse coded

    so that higher scores denote higher frequency of private

    prayer.

    The potential for multicollinearity was examined

    in several ways. Neither variance inflation factors

    (all < 4.5) nor correlations (all < 0.62) among the seven

    religiosity measures revealed problematic multicollin-

    earity. Moreover, each of the seven religiosity variables

    was entered one by one into the model and as a block,

    both with and without covariates, both for the depressed

    and nondepressed samples. Results on the religiosity

    variables largely remained consistent across these vari-

    ous specifications. The final model therefore includes all

    seven religiosity variables described previously alongwith covariates.

    Covariates

    This study controls for biological, somatic, psychologi-

    cal, and social factors that have been found to be associ-

    ated with depression. Prior research suggests that older

    adults, females, and non-Hispanic Blacks exhibit higher

    rates of depression compared with their younger, male,

    and white counterparts (Law & Sbarra, 2009; Pratt & 

    Brody, 2008). Age  is measured as a continuous vari-

    able (number of years);  gender  as a dichotomous vari-able, with female = 1 and male = 0; and race/ethnicity 

    as a series of dichotomous variables for white, black,

    Hispanic, and other.

    In addition, somatic or health conditions may be

    related to depression status (Blazer, 2010; Centers for 

    Disease Control and Prevention, 2011a; Koenig, 1999; Lo 

    et al., 2010; Schotte et al., 2006). At baseline, respondents

    were asked whether they had ever been diagnosed with

    each of seven chronic ailments: high blood pressure, dia-

    betes, cancer, lung disease, heart conditions, stroke, and

    arthritis. The number of chronic conditions was summed

    (0–7) with a higher count indicating greater comorbidity.

    Respondents were also asked whether they had recently

    experienced each of three somatic life events in the last

    two years  (since baseline): stroke, heart attack, and/or

    cancer. It is important to account for the onset of illnesses

    such as these, as recently experiencing a negative event has

    been found to be associated with depression (Schnittker, 

    2001). A count (0–3) of somatic events was developed,

    with a higher number indicating greater comorbidity.

    Self-reported health  status was measured using a series

    of dichotomous indicator variables: excellent, very good,

    good, fair, or poor. Functional limitations were measuredusing counts of both instrumental activities of daily living

    (IADLs) (0–6) and basic activities of daily living (ADLs)

    (0–5).

    Alcohol abuse  appears to be associated with depres-

    sion (Blay et al., 2008; Braam et al., 2004; Centers 

    for Disease Control and Prevention, 2011a; Idler & 

    Kasl, 1992; Rodriguez, Schonfeld, King-Kallimanis, & 

    Amber, 2010). Consistent with previous research (Satre, 

    Gordon, & Weisner, 2007), respondents were identified

    as a serious drinker, or abuser of alcohol, if they were

    The Gerontologist , 2014, Vol. 00, No. 00 Page 4 of 12

  • 8/18/2019 Ronneberg - The Protective Effects of Religiosity on Depression - A 2-Year Prospective Study

    5/12

    (a) a woman that consumes more than two drinks per

    occasion or (b) a man that consumes more than three

    drinks per occasion. Also included is a dichotomous

    variable indicating whether or not an individual had

    ever been diagnosed with any emotional or psychiatric

    condition(s)  (Koenig et al., 1998). Both variables have

    been shown to increase the risk for, or coexist with,

    depression (Aina & Suman, 2006; Centers for Disease Control and Prevention, 2011b) and have been used as

    covariates in similar studies.

    Social factors have the potential to predispose individu-

    als toward depression (Schotte et al., 2006). The influences

    of social and economic considerations are reflected, in part,

    in sociodemographic variables, including marital status 

    (married, divorced/separated, widowed, or never married),

    education  (in years), and household income  (in quartile

    earnings). Social considerations are also reflected, in part,

    in living alone, volunteerism, and having family and friends

    nearby. Certain recent adverse life events such as “seriouslosses, threatening occurrences, or difficulties in life” are

    also predictive of depression (Schnittker, 2001; Schotte 

    et al., 2006, p. 314). These include experiencing divorce/ 

    separation, death of a spouse/partner, a nursing home stay,

    and residential move in the last 2 years. A count has been

    created (0–4), where a higher number indicates experi-

    encing more recent adverse social events. Lastly, whether

    respondents were living in a nursing home as opposed to a

    community setting at the time of the survey was recorded,

    in addition to whether survey responses were provided by

    a proxy or not.

    Analytical Plan

    Basic descriptive statistics are reported, followed by bivari-

    ate analyses comparing the baseline characteristics of the

    depressed and nondepressed samples (Table 1). Results

    from multivariate analyses are presented next, utiliz-

    ing logistic regression to model the relationship between

    depression and religiosity, controlling for other baseline

    factors. Two logistic regressions models were employed:

    one with baseline depressed respondents and the second

    with baseline nondepressed respondents (Table 2).

    Results

    Table 1  reports descriptive statistics for the entire sam-

    ple, as well as differentiated by depression status in 2006.

    Depressed and nondepressed samples had similar religious

    affiliations (χ2 = 3.94 (3), p > .05). At 45% and 37%, respec-

    tively, high frequency of religious service attendance wasmore likely to be reported by nondepressed than depressed

    respondents, whereas depressed respondents were more

    likely to report low or no service attendance than their non-

    depressed counterparts (51% vs. 43%) (χ2  = 43.19 (2),  p 

    < .001). A higher proportion of nondepressed respondents

    (59%) reported having friends at their congregation than

    depressed respondents (50%) (χ2  = 44.42 (1),  p  < .001);

    approximately one quarter of each group reported shar-

    ing their congregation with family (χ2 = 0.01 (1),  p > .05).

    There was a small but significant difference assigned to the

    importance of religion, with depressed respondents being

    Table 1. Descriptive and Bivariate Statistics for Baseline (2006) Depressed and Nondepressed Samples

    Covariates Entire sample (n = 7,732) Depressed (n = 1,992) Nondepressed (n = 5,740)   χ2(df )/ t 

    # (%)/mean (SD) # (%)/mean (SD) # (%)/mean (SD)

    Religiosity factors

      Religiosity (basic HRS questions)

      Religious affiliation

      Protestant 4,892 (63.3%) 1,276 (64.1%) 3,616 (70.0%) 3.94 (3)

      Catholica 2,048 (26.5%) 501 (25.2%) 1,547 (27.0%)

      Jewish 158 (2.0%) 48 (2.4%) 110 (1.9%)

      None/other 614 (7.9%) 162 (8.1%) 452 (7.9)

      Service attendance

      High 3,324 (43.0%) 740 (37.2%) 2,584 (45.0%) 43.19 (2)***

      Moderatea 917 (11.9%) 232 (11.7%) 685 (12.0%)

      Low/none 3,493 (45.2%) 1,021 (51.3%) 2,472 (43.1%)

      Friends in congregation 4,367 (56.5%) 998 (50.1%) 3,369 (58.7%) 44.42 (1)***

      Relatives in congregation 1,891 (24.5%) 489 (26.6%) 1,402 (24.4%) 0.01 (1)

      Importance of religion

      Very important 5,273 (68.2%) 1,390 (69.8%) 3,883 (67.7%) 10.60 (2)**

      Somewhat important 1,591 (20.6%) 422 (21.2%) 1,170 (20.4%)

      Not importanta 871 (11.3%) 183 (9.2%) 688 (12.0%)

    The Gerontologist , 2014, Vol. 00, No. 00Page 5 of 12

  • 8/18/2019 Ronneberg - The Protective Effects of Religiosity on Depression - A 2-Year Prospective Study

    6/12

    Covariates Entire sample (n = 7,732) Depressed (n = 1,992) Nondepressed (n = 5,740)   χ2(df )/ t 

    # (%)/mean (SD) # (%)/mean (SD) # (%)/mean (SD)

      Religiosity (LBQ)

      Index of religiosity 4.99 (1.39) 5.02 (1.33) 4.98 (1.40) −1.26

      Frequency private prayer 6.11 (2.32) 6.25 (2.26) 6.06 (2.34) −3.17**

    Biological variables

      Demographic variables

      Age 68.11 (1.39) 68.80 (11.44) 67.87 (10.40) −3.19***

      Female 4,543 (58.8%) 1,317 (66.1%) 3,226 (56.2%) 59.96 (1)***

      Race/ethnicity

      Whitea 6,009 (77.7%) 1,462 (73.4%) 4,547 (79.2%) 33.84 (3)***

      Black 1,004 (13.0%) 292 (14.8%) 712 (12.4%)

      Hispanic 601 (7.7%) 204 (10.2%) 397 (6.9%)

      Other 118 (1.5%) 34 (1.7%) 84 (1.5%)

    Somatic variables

      Health and functional limitation

      Chronic conditions 1.92 (1.33) 2.39 (1.37) 1.75 (1.27) −18.46***

      Self-reported health

      Excellent 904 (11.7%) 70 (3.5%) 834 (14.5%) 1174.17 (4)***

      Very good 2,350 (30.4%) 310 (15.6%) 2,040 (35.5%)

      Gooda 2,395 (31.0%) 547 (27.5%) 1,848 (32.2%)

      Fair 1,554 (20.1%) 702 (35.2%) 851 (14.8%)

      Poor 543 (7.0%) 369 (18.5%) 174 (3.0%)

      IADLs 0.34 (.82) 0.63 (1.07) 0.24 (0.70) −15.01***

      ADLs 0.34 (.94) 0.80 (1.38) 0.18 (0.66) −19.48***

      Somatic adverse life events 0.07 (0.27) 0.09 (0.30) 0.07 (0.26) −2.73**

    Psychological variables

      High alcohol use 351 (4.5%) 101 (5.2%) 250 (4.4%) 1.74 (1)

      Psychological issues 1,244 (16.1%) 655 (32.9%) 589 (10.3%) 558.35 (1)***

    Social variables

      Sociodemographic variables

      Marital status

      Marrieda 5,039 (65.2%) 1,029 (51.7%) 4,010 (70.0%) 216.04 (3)***

      Divorced/separated 966 (12.5%) 342 (17.2%) 624 (10.9%)

      Widowed 1,504 (19.5%) 542 (27.2%) 962 (16.8%)

      Never married 223 (2.9%) 79 (4.0%) 144 (2.5%)

      Education level 12.58 (3.11) 11.78 (3.30) 12.86 (2.99) −13.53***

      Household income

      Quartile 1a 1,747 (22.6%) 722 (36.2%) 1,025 (17.9%) 347.93 (3)***

      Quartile 2 1,922 (25.4%) 528 (26.5%) 1,394 (24.3%)

      Quartile 3 1,985 (25.7%) 391 (19.6%) 1,594 (27.8%)

      Quartile 4 2,078 (26.9%) 351 (17.6%) 1,727 (30.1%)

      Social adverse life events 0.12 (0.12) 0.12 (0.33) 0.12 (0.33) −0.45

      Social support variables

      Living alone 1,673 (21.6%) 614 (30.8%) 1,059 (18.4%) 133.54 (1)***

      Volunteer status 2,786 (36.0%) 475 (23.8%) 2,311 (40.2%) 172.90 (1)***

      Relatives live near 2,157 (27.9%) 577 (29.0%) 1,580 (27.5%) 0.94 (1)

      Friends live near 5,026 (65.0%) 1,241 (62.3%) 3,784 (70.0%) 11.80 (1)***

      Have proxy respondent 283 (3.7%) 63 (3.2%) 220 (3.8%) 1.88 (1)

      Live in nursing home 99 (1.3%) 48 (2.4%) 52 (0.9%) 18.96 (1)***

    Notes: ADLs = activities of daily living; HRS = Health and Retirement Study; IADLs = instrumental activities of daily living; LBQ = Leave-Behind Questionnaire.aDenotes reference groups.

    * p < .05. ** p < .01. *** p < .001.

    Table 1. Continued

    The Gerontologist , 2014, Vol. 00, No. 00 Page 6 of 12

  • 8/18/2019 Ronneberg - The Protective Effects of Religiosity on Depression - A 2-Year Prospective Study

    7/12

    Table 2. Logistic Regressions Modeling the Relationship Between Follow-Up (2008) Depression and Religiosity and Other

    Covariates Among Baseline (2006) Depressed and Nondepressed Samples

    Covariates Depressed at baseline

    (n = 1,992)

    Nondepressed at baseline

    (n = 5,740)

    Odds ratio  p value Odds ratio  p value

    Religiosity factors

      General HRS religiosity items

      Religious affiliation

      Protestant 0.91 0.465 1.03 0.768

      Catholica — — — —

      Jewish 2.05 0.040* 1.30 0.382

      None/other 0.907 0.644 1.19 0.325

      Frequency of attendance at religious services

      High 1.36 0.062 0.65 0.001***

      Moderatea — — — —

      Low/none 1.18 0.330 0.75 0.035*

      Friends in congregation 0.92 0.498 0.95 0.659

      Relatives in congregation 1.13 0.336 0.92 0.432

      Importance of religion

      Very important 0.81 0.332 1.23 0.275

      Somewhat important 1.00 0.995 1.01 0.977

      Not importanta — — — —

      Religiosity items (LBQ)

      Index of religiosity 1.10 0.052 1.00 0.949

      Frequency of private prayer 0.93 0.015* 0.98 0.476

    Biological variables

      Demographic variables

      Age 0.99 0.099 1.00 0.319

      Female 1.174 0.513 1.44 0.000***

      Race/ethnicity

      Whitea — — — —

      Black 0.81 0.158 0.873 0.304

      Hispanic 0.95 0.785 1.02 0.889

      Other 1.04 0.971 0.67 0.304

    Somatic variables

      Health and functional limitation variables

      Self-reported chronic conditions 1.06 0.127 1.09 0.020*

      Self-reported health

      Excellent 0.43 0.004** 0.54 0.000***

      Very good 0.80 0.140 0.72 0.002**

      Gooda — — — —

      Fair 1.32 0.029* 1.77 0.000***

      Poor 1.33 0.086 2.69 0.000***

      IADLs 1.03 0.640 0.896 0.113

      ADLs 1.04 0.412 1.13 0.058

      Somatic adverse life events 1.57 0.008** 1.16 0.106

    Psychological variables

      Serious alcohol use 0.803 0.329 1.03 0.877

      Psychological issues 1.607 0.000*** 1.93 0.000***

    Social variables

      Sociodemographic variables

      Marital status

      Marrieda — — — —

      Divorced/separated 0.99 0.972 0.88 0.407

      Widowed 0.72 0.048* 0.88 0.390

      Never married 0.70 0.197 0.62 0.103

    The Gerontologist , 2014, Vol. 00, No. 00Page 7 of 12

  • 8/18/2019 Ronneberg - The Protective Effects of Religiosity on Depression - A 2-Year Prospective Study

    8/12

    more likely to report “very important” (70% vs. 68%)

    and depressed respondents being less likely to report “not

    important” (9% vs. 12%) (χ2  = 10.60 (2),  p  < .01). No

    significant difference could be discerned between the two

    groups on the religiosity index (t  = −1.26, p > .05), but there

    was a significant difference in regard to the frequency of pri-

    vate prayer (t  = −3.17, p < .01) with depressed respondentsreporting higher frequency (6.25 vs. 6.06 [out of 8]).

    Significant differences could also be discerned with

    respect to all covariates but high alcohol use (χ2 = 1.74 (1),

     p > .05), social adverse life events (r = .45, p > .05), the pres-

    ence of relatives living nearby (χ2 = 0.94 (1), p > .05), and

    having a proxy respondent (χ2 = 1.88 (1),  p > .05). Thus,

    nondepressed respondents were more likely than depressed

    respondents to be younger (r  = −3.19,  p  < .001), male

    (χ2 = 59.96, p < .001), non-Hispanic white (χ2 = 33.84, p <

    .001), in excellent very good/good health (χ2 = 1174.17, (4),

     p < .001), married (χ2

     = 216.04, p < .001), have higher edu-cation (t  = 13.53, p < .001), have higher income (χ2 = 347.93

    (3), p < .001), volunteer (χ2 = 172.90, p < .001), and have

    friends living nearby (χ2  = 11.80,  p < .001). By contrast,

    depressed respondents were more likely than nondepressed

    respondents to be chronically ill (t   = −18.46,  p  < .001),

    IADL (t  = −15.01, p < .001) and ADL (t  = −19.48, p < .001)

    impaired, suffer from adverse somatic life events (t  = −2.73,

     p < .01), have psychological issues (χ2 = 558.35, p < .001),

    live alone (χ2 = 133.54 (1),  p < .001), or live in a nursing

    home (χ2 = 18.96 (1), p < .001).

    Two main logistic regression models were estimated. The

    first model is composed of individuals who were depressed

    at baseline (n = 1,992) (Table 2). Two religiosity variables

    were significant. The odds of being depressed at follow-up

    were two times higher among depressed respondents with

    a Jewish affiliation (odds ratio [OR] = 2.05,  p < .05) but

    lower for those with more frequent engagement in privateprayer (OR = 0.93, p < .05).

    The first model also indicates that depressed individuals

    who were in excellent health (OR = 0.43,  p < .01), wid-

    owed (OR = 0.72, p < .05), had higher household income

    (OR = 0.73,  p < .05; OR = 0.66,  p < .05), or who lived

    in a nursing home (OR = 0.41,  p < .05) had a decreased

    likelihood of being depressed at follow-up. In contrast,

    depressed persons who reported more somatic life events

    (OR = 1.57, p < .01), had psychological issues (OR = 1.61,

     p  < .001), reported more social adverse life events

    (OR = 1.46,  p  < .05), and lived closer to their relatives(OR = 1.31, p < .05) were more likely to remain depressed

    2 years later.

    The second model is composed of individuals who were

    not depressed at baseline (n  = 5,740) (Table 2). Service

    attendance was the only religiosity variable to prove sig-

    nificant. In particular, nondepressed respondents with high

    service attendance were 35% less likely to be depressed

    at follow-up (OR = 0.65,  p < .01), and respondents with

    low/no service attendance were 25% less likely to become

    depressed (OR = 0.75, p < .05), in comparison to those with

    Covariates Depressed at baseline

    (n = 1,992)

    Nondepressed at baseline

    (n = 5,740)

    Odds ratio  p value Odds ratio  p value

      Education level 0.98 0.155 0.99 0.333

      Household income

      Quartile 1a — — — —

      Quartile 2 0.73 0.017* 0.93 0.528

      Quartile 3 0.81 0.189 0.82 0.143

      Quartile 4 0.66 0.023* 0.69 0.017*

      Social adverse life events 1.461 0.011* 1.42 0.002**

      Social support variables

      Living alone 1.12 0.421 1.35 0.035*

      Volunteer status 1.16 0.227 0.90 0.275

      Relatives live near 1.31 0.018* 1.23 0.025*

      Friends live near 0.954 0.659 0.91 0.291

      Have proxy respondent 0.000 1.000 0.236 0.000***

      Live in a nursing home 0.411 0.049* 0.741 0.538

    −2 log likelihood: 1941.1*** −2 log likelihood: 3648.2***

    Notes: ADLs = activities of daily living; HRS = Health and Retirement Study; IADLs = instrumental activities of daily living; LBQ = Leave-Behind Questionnaire.aDenotes reference groups.

    * p < .05. ** p < .01. *** p < .001.

    Table 2. Continued

    The Gerontologist , 2014, Vol. 00, No. 00 Page 8 of 12

  • 8/18/2019 Ronneberg - The Protective Effects of Religiosity on Depression - A 2-Year Prospective Study

    9/12

    moderate service attendance. Other factors associated with

    increased likelihood of depression onset included female

    gender (OR = 1.44, p < .001), chronic illness (OR = 1.09,

     p < .05), fair or poor health (OR = 1.77, OR = 2.69, both

     p  < .001), psychological issues (OR = 1.93,  p  < .001),

    adverse social events (OR = 1.42,  p  < .01), living alone

    (OR = 1.35,  p  < .05), and having relatives live nearby

    (OR = 1.23, p < .05). By contrast, factors that appeared toguard against depression onset included reporting excellent

    or very good health (OR = 0.54,  p < .001; OR = 0.72,  p 

    < .01), higher income (OR = 0.69,  p < .05), and having a

    proxy respondent (OR = 0.24, p < .001).

    Discussion

    This study sought to understand whether religiosity (a)

    protects against future depression and (b) plays a role in

    depression recovery, among older adults. Consistent with

    prior research (Blazer, 2010; Koenig, 2007; Smith et al., 2003), results provide evidence supporting both of these

    expectations, though the specific aspect of religiosity found

    to protect against depression (frequent service attendance)

    was different from the component found to aid in depres-

    sion recovery (private prayer frequency). Relative to those

    with moderate service attendance, individuals who were

    not depressed at baseline (in 2006) were less likely to be

    depressed 2 years later if they frequently attended reli-

    gious services. It was expected that high service attend-

    ance would protect against depression, perhaps due to the

    availability, promotion, or benefits of social support found

    in one’s place of worship. In particular, the protective path-

    way stemming from service attendance may derive from

    the comparatively higher levels of social capital resulting

    from engagement in public modes of behavior, specifically

    the interpersonal relationships formed and sustained by

    active participation in a religious congregation. The pres-

    ence of more social connections, in turn, may reduce the

    likelihood of isolation and loneliness, two factors associ-

    ated with depression.

    Counterintuitively, individuals with low service attend-

    ance who were not depressed at baseline were also less

    likely to be depressed 2 years later relative to those withmoderate service attendance. It is possible that persons

    with low or no service attendance may be less likely to

    be depressed at follow-up because they are more likely to

    engage in other, less public forms of religiosity that, in turn,

    provide protective benefits from depression and other ail-

    ments. That this may be the case is suggested by the moder-

    ate, significant inverse correlation between private prayer

    frequency and level of service attendance (r  = −.496,  p <

    .05). Thus, whereas the high service attendance group may

    be disproportionately devoted to organizational forms of

    religiosity, the low service attendance group may be dis-

    proportionately devoted to nonorganizational forms. In

    contrast, the moderate service attendance group may not

    be disproportionately devoted to either form of religious

    behavior and, as such, may be less likely to experience the

    benefits that derive from each.

    Consistent with expectations, persons who started

    out depressed at baseline were less likely to be depressed2 years later if they more frequently engaged in private

    prayer. This finding suggests that persons who become

    depressed may turn to their faith for support and as a

    means of coping from adverse life events—financial,

    health, social, or otherwise. Subsequent engagement in

    private prayer may serve, in part, to cultivate hope for

    the future, potentially activating cognitive resources that

    eventually counter depression.

    Interestingly, Jewish respondents were much more likely

    to remain depressed at follow-up than other respondents.

    One possible explanation for this finding could be thelong-term, negative implications that belonging to a reli-

    gious minority has on mental health (Berger, 1977). This

    may be particularly important for the population surveyed

    because anti-Semitism was much more visible and prev-

    alent during our respondents’ formative years than it is

    today. Another possible explanation could be that Jewish

    elders may not benefit in the same way from religious

    involvement as members of other religious affiliations.

    Take Christian doctrine, for example, which emphasizes

    the afterlife or Heaven at which point the body may be

    restored and a reunion takes place with long deceased

    loved ones (Gillman, 2007). This belief can be great source

    of solace, hope, and comfort for those going through hard

    times (e.g., depression), which may, in turn, support cop-

    ing and recovery. This is in contrast to Jewish doctrine,

    which in downplaying the hereafter in favor of the “here

    and now,” may not provide the same level of solace, hope,

    and comfort (Gillman, 2007).

    One interesting yet surprising finding emerging from

    this study is that having relatives living nearby was associ-

    ated with depression at follow-up among both the baseline

    depressed and nondepressed samples analyzed. It is plausi-

    ble that there are certain unwanted expectations inherentwhen family members live closer—whether, for example,

    caring for a frail and disabled parent or other relative in

    need of long-term care or watching a young grandchild

    in need of after school care, that results in burdens and

    stresses that might not otherwise exist if family lived fur-

    ther away. Simply measuring proximity, moreover, does

    not account for the frequency or quality of the interactions

    that take place. For example, some relationships, even with

    relatives, may not be pursued no matter how convenient, if

    those relationships are not fulfilling.

    The Gerontologist , 2014, Vol. 00, No. 00Page 9 of 12

  • 8/18/2019 Ronneberg - The Protective Effects of Religiosity on Depression - A 2-Year Prospective Study

    10/12

    There are several limitations worth noting. First, depres-

    sion is measured using the CESD-8, a self-report tool,

    rather than using the clinical diagnosis of depression by a

    health or mental health professional. Although the latter

    may be the gold standard, the CESD-8 is a commonly used

    and accepted measure of depression in studies such as this

    one where clinical diagnosis was not possible (e.g., Steffick, 

    2000). Second, data from the CESD-8 were not utilized todevelop a measure of depression based on a continuous

    count of depressive symptoms but instead used to place

    individuals into depressed or nondepressed categories based

    on the presence of three or more of the eight symptoms

    assessed. One implication is that potentially useful varia-

    tion may have been missing. This “cutoff approach,” how-

    ever, is typically employed in studies utilizing the CESD-8

    (Steffick, 2000). A third limitation is related to the length of

    the study. Given the episodic nature of depression, a 2-year

    longitudinal study may miss signs of depression occur-

    ring after the time period analyzed. Future research shouldextend the follow-up period studied over a longer period

    of time as additional waves of the HRS become available.

    Last, the Health and Retirement Study only includes meas-

    ures of religiosity but not spirituality. Thus, this study is

    focused exclusively on the former but not the latter. This

    limitation is important to point out because extant research

    suggests that spirituality may be associated with lower

    rates of depression and mental illness as well (Skarupski, 

    Fitchett, Evans, & Mendes de Leon, 2010). Moreover, a

    growing body of research suggests that spirituality to be a

    unique construct, though related to religiosity (Underwood 

    & Teresi, 2002). Beyond this understanding experts hold

    differing views regarding the distinction between religiosity

    and spirituality, some maintaining that religiosity may be a

    part of spirituality, whereas others viewing spirituality as a

    part of religiosity (Hill et al., 2000; MacKinlay, 2006).

    Conclusion

    Several implications for policy and practice follow from

    the results of this study. One is related to transportation

    availability and the provision of better access to places of

    worship so that older adults who are interested in religiousservices are able to attend and subsequently benefit from

    organizational, or public, forms of religiosity. Moreover,

    given the high prevalence of depression among older

    adults, clinicians should be cognizant of the benefits asso-

    ciated with both religious service attendance and involve-

    ment in private prayer, assess individuals’ religious needs

    and involvement, and determine whether their clients face

    any barriers to attending services or pursuing their faith if

    they so desire. Through these assessments, clinicians could

    help connect interested clients to such services within their

    communities or help them overcome any barriers they may

    be experiencing, hindering involvement private prayer. Care

    plans or therapy goals can be developed, which address

    these issues as well.

    References

    Administration on Aging. (2012). Older Americans behavio-

    ral health. Retrieved from http://www.aoa.gov/AoARoot/ 

    AoA_Programs/HPW/Behavioral/docs2/Issue%20Brief%20

    Series%20Overview.pdf 

    Aina, Y., & Suman, J. L. (2006). Understanding comorbidity with

    depression and anxiety disorders.  Journal of the American

    Osteopathic Association, 106, S9–S14. Retrieved from http:// 

    www.jaoa.org/content/106/5_suppl_2/S9.full.pdf 

    Berger, D. M. (1977). The survivor syndrome: A problem of nosol-

    ogy and treatment. American Journal of Psychotherapy, 31,

    238–251. Retrieved from http://www.ajp.org

    Blay, S. L., Batista, A. D., Andreoli, S. B., & Gastal, F. L. (2008). The

    relationship between religiosity and tobacco, alcohol use, anddepression in the elderly community population. The American

     Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry, 16, 934–943. doi:10.1097/ 

     JGP.0b013e3181871392

    Blazer, D. G. (2010). The origins of late-life depression. Psychiatric

    Annals, 40, 13–18. doi:10.3928/00485718-20091229-01

    Boswell, G. H., Kahana, E., & Dilworth-Anderson, P. (2006).

    Spirituality and healthy lifestyle behaviors: Stress counter-balanc-

    ing effects on the well-being of older adults. Journal of Religion

    and Health, 45, 587–602. doi:10.1007/s10943-006-9060-7

    Bosworth, H. B., Park, K. S., McQuoid, D. R., Hays, J. C., & Steffens,

    D. C. (2003). The impact of religious practice and religious cop-

    ing on geriatric depression. International Journal of Geriatric

    Psychiatry, 18, 905–914. doi:10.1002/gps.945Braam, A. W., Beekman, A. T. F., Deeg, J. J. H., Smit, J. H., & Tilburh,

    W. V. (1997). Religiosity as a protective or prognostic factor of

    depression in later life; Results from a community survey in

    The Netherlands. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavia, 96, 199–205.

    doi:10.1111/j.1600-0447.1997.tb10151.x

    Braam, A. W., Hein, E., Deeg, D. J. H., Twisk, J. W. R., Beekman, A.

    T. F., & Tilburg, W. V. (2004). Religious involvement and 6-year

    course of depressive symptoms in older Dutch citizens: Results

    from the Longitudinal Aging Study Amsterdam. Journal of Aging

    and Health, 16, 467–489. doi:10.1177/0898264304265765

    Braam, A. W., Van Den Eeden, P., Prince, M. J., Beekman, A. T. F.,

    Kivela, S. L., Lawlor, B. A., … Copeland, J. R. M. (2001).

    Religion as a cross-cultural determinant of depression inelderly Europeans: Results from the EURODEP collabora-

    tion. Psychological Medicine, 31, 803–814. doi:http://dx.doi.

    org/10.1017/S0033291701003956

    Branco, K. J. (2000). Religiosity and depression among nursing home

    residents: Results of a survey of ten states. Journal of Religious

    Gerontology, 12, 43–61. doi:10.1300/J078v12n01_05

    Centers for Disease Control and Prevention. (2011a). Mental health

    basics. Retrieved from http://www.cdc.gov/mentalhealth/basics.htm

    Centers for Disease Control and Prevention. (2011b). Depression.

    Retrieved from http://www.cdc.gov/mentalhealth/basics/mental-

    illness/depression.htm

    The Gerontologist , 2014, Vol. 00, No. 00 Page 10 of 12

    http://www.aoa.gov/AoARoot/AoA_Programs/HPW/Behavioral/docs2/Issue%20Brief%20Series%20Overview.pdfhttp://www.aoa.gov/AoARoot/AoA_Programs/HPW/Behavioral/docs2/Issue%20Brief%20Series%20Overview.pdfhttp://www.aoa.gov/AoARoot/AoA_Programs/HPW/Behavioral/docs2/Issue%20Brief%20Series%20Overview.pdfhttp://www.jaoa.org/content/106/5_suppl_2/S9.full.pdfhttp://www.jaoa.org/content/106/5_suppl_2/S9.full.pdfhttp://www.ajp.org/http://www.cdc.gov/mentalhealth/basics.htmhttp://www.cdc.gov/mentalhealth/basics/mental-illness/depression.htmhttp://www.cdc.gov/mentalhealth/basics/mental-illness/depression.htmhttp://www.cdc.gov/mentalhealth/basics/mental-illness/depression.htmhttp://www.cdc.gov/mentalhealth/basics/mental-illness/depression.htmhttp://www.cdc.gov/mentalhealth/basics.htmhttp://www.ajp.org/http://www.jaoa.org/content/106/5_suppl_2/S9.full.pdfhttp://www.jaoa.org/content/106/5_suppl_2/S9.full.pdfhttp://www.aoa.gov/AoARoot/AoA_Programs/HPW/Behavioral/docs2/Issue%20Brief%20Series%20Overview.pdfhttp://www.aoa.gov/AoARoot/AoA_Programs/HPW/Behavioral/docs2/Issue%20Brief%20Series%20Overview.pdfhttp://www.aoa.gov/AoARoot/AoA_Programs/HPW/Behavioral/docs2/Issue%20Brief%20Series%20Overview.pdf

  • 8/18/2019 Ronneberg - The Protective Effects of Religiosity on Depression - A 2-Year Prospective Study

    11/12

    Chaaya, M., Sibai A. M., Fayad, R., & El-Roueiheb, Z. (2007).

    Religiosity and depression in older people: Evidence from under-

    privileged refugee and non-refugee communities in Lebanon.Aging

    & Mental Health, 11, 37–44. doi:10.1080/13607860600735812

    Cohen, A. B., & Koenig, H. G. (2003). Religion, religiosity, and spirit-

    uality in the biopsychosocial model of health and ageing. Ageing

    International , 28, 215–241. doi:10.1007/s12126-002-1005-1

    Contrada, R. J., Goyal, T. M., Cather, C., Rafalson, L., Idler,

    E. L., & Krause, T. J. (2004). Psychosocial factors in out-comes of heart surgery: The impact of religious involvement

    and depressive symptoms. Health Psychology, 23, 227–238.

    doi:10.1037/0278-6133.23.3.227

    Dew, R. E., Daniel, S. S., Goldston, D. B., McCall, W. V., Kuchibhatla,

    M., Schleifer, C., … Koenig, H. G. (2010). A prospective study

    of religion/spirituality and depressive symptoms among adoles-

    cent psychiatric patients.  Journal of Affective Disorders, 120,

    149–157. doi:10.1016/j.jad.2009.04.029

    Ellison, C. G., & Flannelly, K. J. (2009). Religious involvement and

    risk of major depression in a prospective nationwide study

    of African American adults.  Journal of Nervous and Mental

    Disease, 197, 568–573. doi:10.1097/NMD.0b013e3181b08f45

    Gallup. (2013). Religion. Retrieved from http://www.gallup.com/ 

    poll/1690/Religion.aspx#1

    Gillman, N. (2007). How will it all end? Eschatology in science and

    religion. Cross Currents, 57, 38–50. Retrieved from http://www.

    crosscurrents.org/GillmanSpring07.pdf 

    Hayward, R. D., Owen, A. D., Koenig, H. G., Steffens, D. C., &

    Payne, M. E. (2012). Longitudinal relationships of religion with

    posttreatment depression severity in older psychiatric patients:

    Evidence of direct and indirect effects. Depression Research and

    Treatment , 2012, 1–8. doi:10.1155/2012/745970

    Health and Retirement Study (HRS 2006 Core; HRS 2008 Core)

    public use dataset. (2006/2008). Produced and distributed by

    the University of Michigan with funding from the National

    Institute on Aging (grant number NIA U01AG009740). Ann

    Arbor, MI. Retrieved from http://hrsonline.isr.umich.edu/index.

    php?p=regcou.

    Hill, P. C., Pargament, K. I., Hood, R. W., Jr., McCollough, M.

    E., Swyers, J. P., Larson, D. B., & Zinnbauer, B. J. (2000).

    Conceptualizing religion and spirituality: Point of commonality,

    points of departure. Journal for the Theory of Social Behaviour,

    30, 51–77. doi:10.1111/1468–5914.00119

    Hurst, G. A., Williams, M. G., King, J. E., & Viken, R. (2008).

    Faith-based intervention in depression, anxiety, and other men-

    tal disturbances. Southern Medical Journal , 101, 388–392.

    doi:10.1097/SMJ.0b013e318167a97a

    Idler, E. L., & Kasl, S. V. (1992). Religion, disability, depression, and

    the timing of death. American Journal of Sociology, 97, 1052–

    1079. doi:10.1086/229861

    Iecovich, E. (2001). Religiousness and subjective well-being among

     Jewish female residents of old age homes in Israel. Journal of

    Religious Gerontology, 13, 31–46. doi:10.1300/J078v13n01_04

    King, D. A., Lyness, J. M., Duberstein, P. R., He, H., Tu, X.

    M., & Seaburn, D. B. (2007). Religious involvement and

    depressive symptoms in primary care elders. Psychological

    Medicine, 37, 1807–1815. doi:http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/ 

    S0033291707000591

    Koenig, H. G. (1999). The healing power of faith. New York: Simon

    & Schuster.

    Koenig, H. G. (2007). Spirituality and depression: A look at the evi-

    dence. Southern Medical Journal , 100, 737–739. doi:10.1097/ 

    SMJ.0b013e318073c68c

    Koenig, H. G. (2009). Research on religion, spirituality, and mental

    health: A review. The Canadian Journal of Psychiatry, 54, 283–

    291. Retrieved from http://publications.cpa-apc.org/browse/ 

    documents/464

    Koenig, H. G., George, L. K., & Peterson, B. L. (1998). Religiosity

    and remission of depression in medically ill older patients.American Journal of Psychiatry, 155, 536–542. Retrieved from

    http://ajp.psychiatryonline.org/article.aspx?articleid=172792#A

    bstract

    Koenig, H. G., Hays, J. C., George, L. K., Blazer, D. G., Larson, D. B., &

    Landerman, L. R. (1997). Modeling the cross-sectional relation-

    ships between religion, physical health, social support, and depres-

    sive symptoms. The American Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry, 5,

    131–144. doi:10.1097/00019442-199700520-00006

    Law, R. W., & Sbarra, D. A. (2009). The effects of church attendance

    and marital status on the longitudinal trajectories of depressed

    mood among older adults.  Journal of Aging and Health, 21,

    803–823. doi:10.1177/0898264309338300

    Lawler-Row, K. A., & Elliott, J. (2009). The role of religious

    activity and spirituality in the health and well-being of

    older adults.  Journal of Health Psychology, 14, 43–52.

    doi:10.1177/1359105308097944

    Levin, J. (2010). Religion and mental health: Theory and research.

    International Journal of Applied Psychoanalytic Studies, 7, 102–

    115. doi:10.1002/aps.240

    Lo, C., Lin, J., Gagliese, L., Zimmermann, C., Mikulincer, M., &

    Rodin, G. (2010). Age and depression in patients with meta-

    static cancer: The protective effects of attachment security and

    spiritual wellbeing. Aging & Society, 30, 325–336. doi:10.1017/ 

    S0144686X09990201

    MacKinlay, E. (2006). Spiritual care: Recognizing spiritual needs

    of older adults.  Journal of Religion, Spirituality & Aging , 18,

    59–71. doi:10.1300/J496v18n02_05

    Martin, K. R., & Levy, B. R. (2006). Opposing trends of religious

    attendance and religiosity in predicting elders’ functional recov-

    ery after an acute myocardial infarction. Journal of Religion &

    Health, 45, 440–451. doi:10.1007/s10943-006-9037-6

    Murphy, P. E., & Fitchett, G. (2009). Belief in a concerned God predicts

    response to treatment for adults with clinical depression. Journal

    of Clinical Psychology, 65, 1000–1008. doi:10.1002/jclp.20598

    Parker, M., Roff, L. L., Klemmack, D. L., Koenig, H. G., Baker, P., &

    Allman, R. M. (2003). Religiosity and mental health in southern,

    community-dwelling older adults. Aging & Mental Health, 7,

    390–397. doi:10.1080/1360786031000150667

    Paukert, A. L., Phillips, L., Cully, J. A., Loboprabhu, S. M., Lomax,

     J. W., & Stanley, M. A. (2009). Integration of religion into cog-

    nitive-behavioral therapy for geriatric anxiety and depression.

     Journal of Psychiatric Practice, 15, 103–112. doi:10.1097/01.

    pra.0000348363.88676.4d

    Payman, V., George, K., & Ryburn, B. (2008). Religiosity of

    elderly depressed inpatients. International Journal of Geriatric

    Psychiatry, 23, 16–21. doi:10.1002/gps.1827

    Pratt, L. A., & Brody, D. J. (2008). Depression in the United States

    Household Population, 2005–2006. Centers for Disease Control

    and Prevention: U.S. Department of Health and Human Services.

    Retrieved from http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/19389321

    The Gerontologist , 2014, Vol. 00, No. 00Page 11 of 12

    http://www.gallup.com/poll/1690/Religion.aspx#1http://www.gallup.com/poll/1690/Religion.aspx#1http://www.crosscurrents.org/GillmanSpring07.pdfhttp://www.crosscurrents.org/GillmanSpring07.pdfhttp://hrsonline.isr.umich.edu/index.php?p=regcouhttp://hrsonline.isr.umich.edu/index.php?p=regcouhttp://publications.cpa-apc.org/browse/documents/464http://publications.cpa-apc.org/browse/documents/464http://ajp.psychiatryonline.org/article.aspx?articleid=172792#Abstracthttp://ajp.psychiatryonline.org/article.aspx?articleid=172792#Abstracthttp://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/19389321http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/19389321http://ajp.psychiatryonline.org/article.aspx?articleid=172792#Abstracthttp://ajp.psychiatryonline.org/article.aspx?articleid=172792#Abstracthttp://publications.cpa-apc.org/browse/documents/464http://publications.cpa-apc.org/browse/documents/464http://hrsonline.isr.umich.edu/index.php?p=regcouhttp://hrsonline.isr.umich.edu/index.php?p=regcouhttp://www.crosscurrents.org/GillmanSpring07.pdfhttp://www.crosscurrents.org/GillmanSpring07.pdfhttp://www.gallup.com/poll/1690/Religion.aspx#1http://www.gallup.com/poll/1690/Religion.aspx#1

  • 8/18/2019 Ronneberg - The Protective Effects of Religiosity on Depression - A 2-Year Prospective Study

    12/12

    Rodriguez, C. A., Schonfeld, L., King-Kallimanis, B., & Amber,

    M. G. (2010). Depressive symptoms and alcohol abuse/mis-

    use in older adults: Results from the Florida BRITE project.

    Best Practice in Mental Health: An International Journal , 6,

    90–102. Retrieved from http://essential.metapress.com/content/ 

    r1k65231237068k1/ 

    Satre, D., Gordon, N. P., & Weisner, C. (2007). Alcohol consump-

    tion, medical conditions and health behavior in older adults.

    American Journal of Health Behavior, 31, 238–248. doi:http:// dx.doi.org/10.5993/AJHB.31.3.2

    Schnittker, J. (2001). When is faith enough? The effects of religious

    involvement on depression.  Journal for the Scientific Study of

    Religion, 40, 393–411. doi:10.1111/0021-8294.00065

    Schotte, C. K. W., Bossche, B. V. D., Doncker, D. D., Claes, S., &

    Cosyns, P. (2006). A biopsychosocial model as a guide for psy-

    choeducation and treatment of depression. Depression and

    Anxiety, 23, 321–324. doi:10.1002/da.20177

    Skarupski, K. A., Fitchett, G., Evans, D. A., & Mendes de Leon, C.

    F. (2010). Daily spiritual experiences in a biracial, community-

    based population of older adults. Aging & Mental Health, 14,

    779–789. doi:10.1080/13607861003713265

    Smith, T. B., McCullough, M. E., & Poll, J. (2003). Religiousness

    and depression: Evidence for a main effect and the moderating

    influence of stressful life events. Psychological Bulletin, 129,

    614–636. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.129.4.614

    Steffick, D. E. (2000). HRS/AHEAD documentation report.

    Retrieved March 7, 2011, from http://hrsonline.isr.umich.edu/ 

    sitedocs/userg/dr-005.pdf 

    Stein, H. F. (2005). It ain’t necessarily so: The many faces of the

    biopsychosocial model. Families, Systems, & Health, 23, 440–

    443. doi:10.1037/1091-7527.23.4.440

    Sun, F., Park, N. S., Roff, L. L., Klemmack, D. L., Parker, M., Koenig,

    H. G., & Allman, R. M. (2012). Predicting the trajectories of

    depressive symptoms among southern community-dwelling

    older adults: The role of religiosity. Aging & Mental Health, 16,

    189–198. doi:10.1080/13607863.2011.602959

    Underwood, L. G., & Teresi, J. A. (2002). The daily spiritual expe-rience scale: Development, theoretical description, reliability,

    exploratory factor analysis, and preliminary construct validity

    using health related data. Annals of Behavioral Medicine, 24,

    22–33. doi:10.1207/S15324796ABM2401_04

    Waddell, E. L., & Jacobs-Lawson, J. M. (2010). Predicting positive well-

    being in older men and women. International Journal of Aging and

    Human Development , 70, 181–197. doi:10.2190/AG.70.3.a

    Webb, M., Charbonneau, A. M., McCann, R. A., & Gayle, K. R.

    (2011). Struggling and enduring with God, religious support,

    and recovery from severe mental illness.  Journal of Clinical

    Psychology, 67, 1161–1176. doi:10.1002/jclp.20838

    Wink, P., & Dillon, M. (2002). Spiritual development across

    the adult life course: Findings from a longitudinal study.

     Journal of Adult Development , 9, 79–94. doi:10.102

    3/A:1013833419122

    Yohannes, A. M., Koenig, H. G., Baldwin, R. C., & Connolly, M. J.

    (2008). Health behaviours, depression and religiosity in older

    patients admitted to intermediate care. International Journal

    of Geriatric Psychiatry, 23, 735–740. doi:10.1002/gps.1968

    The Gerontologist , 2014, Vol. 00, No. 00 Page 12 of 12

    http://essential.metapress.com/content/r1k65231237068k1/http://essential.metapress.com/content/r1k65231237068k1/http://hrsonline.isr.umich.edu/sitedocs/userg/dr-005.pdfhttp://hrsonline.isr.umich.edu/sitedocs/userg/dr-005.pdfhttp://hrsonline.isr.umich.edu/sitedocs/userg/dr-005.pdfhttp://hrsonline.isr.umich.edu/sitedocs/userg/dr-005.pdfhttp://essential.metapress.com/content/r1k65231237068k1/http://essential.metapress.com/content/r1k65231237068k1/