Researching Children

download Researching Children

of 11

Transcript of Researching Children

  • 8/18/2019 Researching Children

    1/11

    CHILDREN SOCIETY VOLUME 10 (1996) pp. 145-154

    Researching Children:

    Methods and Ethics

    Ann Mahon and

    Caroline Glendinning

    National Primary Care

    Research and

    Development Centre

    University of Manchester

    Karen Qarke

    Department

     of

     Social

    Policy, University of

    Manchester

    Gary Craig

    School of Sodal Policy,

    University of

    Humberside, Hull

    Address for correspondence:

    Professor G ary Craig, School

     of

    Policy Studies, University

     of

    Humbers ide, Inglemire A venue,

    Hull HU6 7LU

      he appropriateness and desirability of researching children  have

    been issues of

     some

     debate. Children may be perceived as non-

    competent or vulnerable, and proxies have been used as children s

    representatives. Increasingly researchers  ar e speaking to  children

    directly. Why

     is

     this so and w hat are the methodological an d ethical

    implimtions of researching children s views? In this paper the

    authors draw on their own experiences of researching children in the

    fields of child carers and the im pact of the Child Support Act 1991.

    A number of

     social

    political and legal trends are identified w hich

    form  a background to  the growing interest in children as potential

    and actual participants

     in

     th e research process. The

      theoretical

    methodological ethical and p ractical issues involved are then

    identified and

     described

    using

     examples

     from two

     separate

     studies

    conducted

     by

     the authors.

    T

    his paper reflects

      on the

     experiences

     of

      two research

    studies involving semistructured interviews with

    children. The first stu dy focused  on the experiences

    of children and y oun g people caring for a family mem ber an d

    evaluated the impact

     of

     three projects specifically designe d

     to

    support young care givers  and  their families. Face-to-face,

    semistructured interviews were conducted with children and

    the person  who they w ere caring  for in 25  families. The

    yoimg carers' ages ranged from nine years to mid-twenties,

    although issues discussed here generally relate  to  yoimger

    carers  in  this Scimple. Wherever possible, interviews with

    children and parents were conducted  on separate occasions.

    The study was commissioned by the former Mersey Regional

    Health Authority, which  had  funded  the  young carers'

    support projects (Mahon and Higgins, 1995).

    The second study focused on the impact

     of

     the Child Support

    Act  99 on lone mothers and their children receiving certain

    means-tested benefits. Sem istructured interviews we re carried

    out with 12 children aged between 10 and 17, whose mothers

    had already been interviewed earlier  in the  study. The

    interviews explored  the  children's perceptions  of  family

    relationships, their views  on  parental obligations  and their

    C C C 0951-0605 /96 /020145-10

    © 1996

     by

     John WUey Sons, Ltd.

  • 8/18/2019 Researching Children

    2/11

    146 Ann Mahon and others

    knowledge of financial arrangements associated with the Child Support Act. Again,

    every attempt wa s made to interview the children separately from other family m emb ers.

    This was the last stage of a part-longitudinal stud y (Clarke, Craig and Glendinning, 1993,

    1994,1996) and was funded by the Joseph Rowntree Foundation.

    In designing and undertaking these studies we had to address a number of difficult

    ethical and methodological questions about involving children directly in research in this

    way. The first was about the competence of children to provide answers to questions of

    both fact and opinion: wo uld they know , for example, about the paym ent of ma intenance

    and other sources of household income; would they have views on the respective rights

    and obligations of parents and children in separated families? Could equally valid or

    more accurate responses not be obtained from the adults involved? A second issue was

    the particular vulnerability of children and their need for extra protection from potential

    exploitation by researchers. Should not children be shielded from intrusive and

    potentially distressing questioning, with proxies—parents, teachers, sodal workers—

    representing the views of children instead?

    In addressing these issues, we came to the view that in many research contexts it is

    neither theoretically nor m ethodologically app ropria te to rely on proxies to represent the

    views and experiences of children. On the contrary, children's views can an d o ught to be

    taken seriously. However, accessing and understanding these views raise a number of

    important issues for researchers across a wide range of disciplines.

    Social̂ political and legal pressures

    he trends outlined here, whilst not new, together create a set of imperatives for

    researching children's experiences within a wide range of theoretical and applied social

    research.

    Chan ging family pat terns

    Fundamental changes in the structure and stability of families in recent decades have

    major implications for how we approach family research. Even so, there is considerable

    discrepancy between  official  statistics on family status and the actual experiences of

    women, men and children. For example, whilst numbers of both lone and single

    (never married) parents appear to have increased rapidly, these mask growing trends to

    cohabitation prior to or instead of marriage. Evidence firom birth registrations suggests

    that increasing num bers of children are bom into stable cohabiting relationships (Kieman

    and Estaugh, 1993). There is no official documentation of the breakdown of such

    cohabitations—an increasingly common cause of lone parenthood (Bradshaw and Millar

    1991;

      Ford and others, 1994)—nor of the entry of wom en an d children into new cohabiting

    relationships.

    Official figures on remarriage are equally unhelpful, as they do not include growing

    num bers of children who live in unmarried step-parent families. Nor do they reveal w ays

    in which ad ults and children may experience a num ber of different family forms dur ing

    their lives. For example, a fifth of the lone moth ers in the C hild Sup port Act study had

    had children with more than one partner: some lone mothers had been m arried, divorced

    and subsequently cohabited with another partner with whom they had had further

    children; others had been married (and divorced) more than once; yet others had had

  • 8/18/2019 Researching Children

    3/11

    Researching Children: Me thods and Ethics 147

    children in a succession of short-term, non-cohabiting relationships (C larke, Glendinning

    and Craig, 1994).

    It is therefore not appropriate to rely solely on information about family structure and

    status to tell us how children experience their families and, through them, their wider

    social worlds. Assum ptions abo ut the impact of, say, marital break dow n an d entry into a

    step-family— even about the effects of such basic characteristics as age an d birth ord er—

    may be increasingly unreliable. For example, one lone mother in the C hild Support study

    had had her first son while married to a violent partner. After this marriage broke dow n,

    she had three m ore children with ano ther man who treated the older boy exactly the same

    as his own children; whe n interviewed, the boy confirmed that he regarded his mo ther's

    subsequent partner as more of a 'father' to him than his biological father. Even children

    who share the same biological parents may have very different relationships with each

    paren t; such differences in affection a nd allegiance become increasingly salient in shapin g

    the childre n's experiences if the parental relationship is in difficulty. We therefore cannot

    even assume similarities in the experiences of children wh o are currently m embers of the

    same family. The diversity and fluidity of family forms as children move through

    childhood th us impe l us increasingly to seek their  ov m  accounts and experiences, rather

    than assuming that we can automatically know them from demographic and structural

    information we have about their families.

    Pol i t ica l t ren ds

    The 1980s saw a political emphasis on 'consumerism' within public sector services,

    reflecting the introduction of a market ethos as a mean s of promoting user responsiveness.

    This has added to the pressures for the 'consumers' of public services—including,

    potentially, young 'consumers'—to be involved directly in the evaluation of service

    initiatives. Thus in the study of youn g care givers, the Regional He alth A uthority which

    commissioned the research assumed that the evaluation of the young carers' projects

    wo uld of course involve those service 'users ' whom the projects ha d been established to

    support, rather than simply relying on reports from parents or social services

      staff.

    Concern to involve young people directly in the research also reflected the child-centred

    approach adopted by the projects that were the subject of the evaluation study (Mahon

    an d H iggins, 1995). This, in ti un reflects,  i n t e r a l i a , the impact of legal trend s outlined later.

    L e g a l t r e n d s

    The rights of children and their status within the family have been addressed by several

    major recent pieces of legislation— the C hildren Act 1989, the Child Su pport Act  99 and

    the Criminal Justice Act 1991.  These Acts originated from very different policy concerns

    and have resulted in some marked inconsistencies in the current legal status of children

    among the separate social security, criminal justice and child welfare systems.

    Nevertheless, the flurry of legislative activity is itself a reflection of the rapid sodal and

    political changes referred to earlier.

    Of most significance here, the Children Act represents the culmination of a shift from

    children being the passive objects of parental rights to becoming legal subjects in their

    own right. In any decision und er the Act, the welfare of the child must be the param ount

    consideration and his or her views and wishes must be ascertained. Although the latter

    are to be 'considered in the light of his (sic) age and understanding' (S. 6.1), this never-

    theless represents an impo rtant formalisation of children's rights to be heard within the

    legal system.

  • 8/18/2019 Researching Children

    4/11

    148 Ann Mahon and others

    Theoret ical problems

     N or m a l

    childhood

      and th e cons t ruct ion of

     social

      p r o b l e m s

    Academic acceptance of diversity in family form is wides pread . H owev er, the funding of

    applied research is still too often focused on situations and b ehaviours that are assu med ,

    implicitly or explicitly, to be deviant and problem atic. For example , studies of the effects

    of separation and divorce have tended to focus on the negative outcomes for children,

    compared with that in children in intact families; only recently has research begun to

    challenge this implicit accep tance of nega tive outco mes (Burghes, 1994) or to focus o n the

    factors that might help children adapt and cope (Cockett and Tripp, 1994). Yet in view of

    the diversity of family forms a nd re lationsh ips describe d earlier, the prefiguration of some

    types of family experience as 'normal' and some as 'deviant' seems increasingly

    inappropriate.

    Similarly, much research on child carers has assum ed th at informal caregiving as a w hole

    is necessarily problematic and may have pathological consequences for the people

    involved. This model has itself been vigorously criticised by disabled people, on the

    groun ds that it denies their autono my and agency by casting them as wholly passive and

    dependent (Keith, 1992; Morris, 1991). It is equally debatable whether this is an

    appropriate model to use when investigating the kinds of support that younger people  

    may give to adult family mem bers with physical or mental disabilities. Quite simply, we

    know very little about the range of responsibilities that children feel and carry towards

    their various family members, nor how these vary with age, gender and ethnic group. In

    the stud y of child carers a wide range of caring scenarios we re encountered, including

    variations in the illness or disabilities experienced by those being cared for, the t)rpes of

     

    caring tasks and responsibilities being undertaken by the young people, and their

    attitud es tow ards the tasks an d conse quence s of caring. Inde ed, one of the difficulties in

    interpreting these sorts of findings is that of distinguishing those tasks considered  

    'norm al' from those that are out of the ordinary, both within the particular families being

    studied and in the wid er social context.

    Val idi ty

    A second issue which emerged in our studies concerns the concept of validity. As

    indicated earlier, the increasing diversity of feimily situations means that we cannot

    assum e that adult 'pro xies' such as parents or teachers will be able to give valid accounts

    of children's own social worlds. In the Child Support Act study, lone mothers we re often

    awa re that their child had very different experiences of and feelings tow ards an 'absen t'

    parent from those of their own. Moreover, these mothers knew that children would

    sometimes conceal such feelings so as not to hurt them, even to the extent of hiding

    appare ntly straightforward 'facts' such as recent contact with their father. Valid accounts

    of children's attitudes and experiences could therefore only be obtained by engaging

    directly with the children and treating them as independent actors who were engaged in

    negotiating a complex set of relationships and loyalties.

    Similar questions of validity arise in relation to the use of siblings as 'proxies' for each

    other. Again, the Child Supp ort Act stud y revealed that siblings could have very different

    relationships with each of their parents. Consequently, the attitudes of children within

    the same family about parental obligations and the payment of maintenance could be as

    polarised as those of some lone mothers and their former partners. The study of child

    carers also found that children within the same family assumed different caring

  • 8/18/2019 Researching Children

    5/11

    Researching Children : Me thods an d Ethics 149

    responsibilities, held different views about their roles and experiences as carers and used

    the support offered to them through the projects in different ways.

    Methodological issues

    A key issue in researching children is the extent to which the researcher is seen as som e

    kind of expert—with the risk that 'public' accounts will be given that are thought to be

    acceptable to the 'exp ert' point of view of the researcher (Com well, 1984). This distinction

    between public and private accounts may have particular relevance for children and for

    the relationship between researcher and child. Younger children m ay be more suggestible,

    or more inclined to respond to an interviewer w ith answers that they think are expected

    and wa nted than are adu lts; but the problem is essentially the same as that faced in relation

    to adults. The options available in response to the problem are rather different howeve r.

    With adults , the interviewer can be matched to the person being interviewed on a range of

    social characteristics su ch as age, gende r or ethnic origin, in order to create a re lationship

    in which the person being interviewed feels rapport and identification with the

    interviewer. However, this is not always possible with (younger) children because of

    one of the most salient social characteristics in the situation— age (and, derivin g from th is,

    in most cultures, a uthority). Increasingly, how ever, children a re being involved in research

    projects, not just as subjects of research, but as researchers in their own right (Alderson,

    1995). The appropriatene ss of involving children a s researchers is not always appa rent and

    should be judged on the basis of the research topic, the research me thods, and the degree

    of skill and responsibility (and, hence, age or maturity) required of the researcher.

    In the research conducted with the children of lone mothers, the two interviewers

    employed both ha d extensive experience of working with and talking to children in social

    work and legal context. We have n o way of knowing how the identity of the interviewers

    that children constructed affected the kinds of issues that they were wiUing to talk about

    or the way s in which they expressed their views. However, it seems very likely that the

    nature of the conversations w hich took place were affected by the interviewers' identity

    in the child's mind. The 'prob lem' of adult au thority in relation to children m ay be more

    acute whe n the child and the researcher are together on a one-to-one basis. The ad option

    of more varied and imaginative research methods may make it possible to overcome

    these problems to some extent; for example, the use of group interviews and focus

    groups, interactive research methods such as video and drawings, and the involvement

    of young people themselves as researchers.

    When interview ing children it is important that children understan d the que stions asked

    of them and that researchers understand what children are saying; that the children are

    understood as well as (literally) understanding. So far, we have referred to 'children' as if

    they are a homoge neous group. Clearly, the problems that arise in relation to validity a nd

    other aspects of research methodology are very different for children of different

    chronological and developmental ages. The research with child carers, for example,

    involved children across a wide range of ages from as young as nine years. Less

    'successful' interviews, in terms of the depth of response and rapport established, were

    conducted with those in younger age groups, and with boys. In retrospect, alternative

    and more imaginative methods, perhaps including the use of a male researcher, might

    have yielded richer and more valid data. For the older age group, face-to-face interviews

    generally seemed an appropriate method. Obtaining iriformation from children in

    younger age groups may therefore require an approach different to that from older age

  • 8/18/2019 Researching Children

    6/11

    150 Ann Mah on and others

    grou ps and this involves different skills an d different research me thod s. Recent research

    exploring children's and young people's perceptions of cancer, for example, devised a

    'write and draw ' technique for the collection of data from children aged nine to ten years

    (Oakley an d others, 1995).

    Ethical issues

    In some of the discussions w ith colleagues and advisers arou nd our respective studies, we

    encountered anxieties that the children we planne d to interview wo uld not fully

    appreciate wh at was being asked of them; or, as discussed earlier, that they wo uld

    construct the researcher as an authority figure to whom they had to acquiesce, even

    though it might not have been in their own best interests to do so. In other word s, it wa s

    argued, children somehow needed extra 'protection' from the invasive questioning of

    researchers, who otherwise risked exploiting or abusing the children's lack of under-

     

    standing and/or lack of power. Thus, in the Child Support Act study, we were warned

    about the risk of interviewing children about relationships with their (separated) paren ts

    and their views on pare nts' continuing financial obligations, on the grou nds that this

    would prove too upsetting for the children.

    How ever, the researchers' view was that children are, in a variety of different way s,

    capable of deciding whether or not they wished to be interviewed. The children were

    given fuU information beforehand about the scope of the proposed interview and had a

    num ber of oppo rtunities to decline. Children were approac hed directly for their consent,

    with parents first being asked only for permission to make contact with the child. Parents

    were given vratten information about the aims and scope of the interviews with the

    children. A separate approach was then ma de to the child at which his or her consent wa s

    sought an d each child wa s asked again at the start of the interview wh ether he or she was

     

    willing to take part. At the end of the interview, in acknow ledgement of the time that they

    had given, a small payment was made to the chUd, although care was taken not to

    mention this paym ent in negotiating the child's consent. Children wh o did not wa nt to be

    interviewed either refused outright or did not keep appointments (here the interviewer  

    had to exerdse a considerable am ount of discretion in deciding wh ether to call back an d

    try and fix another interview).

    In the study of child carers it was decided at the outset that the children involved s hotdd

    be the main focus of the study rather than adu lts; indeed this was assum ed by the

    com missioners of the research an d reflected the child-centred focus ado pted by the

    projects. In this study pa rents a nd children we re contacted by letter at the same time a nd

    both were asked for their consent to approach the child to request consent to be

    interview ed. As the research also involv ed interview s v^dth pare nts or ano ther family

    member, children were asked for their consent to approach their par ent/re lative for

    interview. All of those contacted were m ade a ware of the right to refuse and it was ma de

    dear by the researcher that participation was not something which was  expected.  Careful

    reading of bod y language revealed some conflict at times with children's verbal consent

    and further probing led to a refusal to be interviewed in at least one case in this study. As

    experienced in the Child Support Act study, 'failure' to keep appointments may be an

    indirect wa y of refusing to participa te.

    Perhaps the greatest anxieties or concerns that emerged from our respective studies were

    those relating to confidentiality and disclosure, both in relation to paren ts an d in relation 

  • 8/18/2019 Researching Children

    7/11

  • 8/18/2019 Researching Children

    8/11

    152 Ann Mah on and others

    Finally, it is important to bear in mind that children, more than adults, may have

    expectations that issues and prob lems w hich they identify in the course of the interview

    might somehow be put right as a result of taking part in the research. It is clearly very  

    impo rtant wh en explaining the purp ose of the research to spell out its limitafions and the

    restricted role of the researcher in relation to the child's day-to-day situation.

    Practical issues

    Many of these m ethodological issues have imp ortant practical implications for research

    with children. For example, the age and development of children should be considered as

    part of the selecfion criteria for a stud y pop ulation becau se of the implication s that these

    have for the choice of research m ethods and the skills and un ders tand ing required of both

    researcher and child. Hav ing de dd ed on the study popu lation, the researcher mu st be

    able to gain access to the children. Again the process of gain ing access will vary acco rding

    to the characteristics of the children being studied. For examp le, child carers are often

    referred to as a 'hidden' or 'secret army' and studies attempting to gain access to child

    carers have often experienced difficulfies. In the study of child carers children were  

    recruited via supp orting projects; other studies of children have recruited them throu gh

    schools (A lderson, 1995; Oakley and others , 1995).

    The process of gaining informed consent and maintaining motivafion are of particular

    importance when researching children and young people. In the study of child carers it

    was m ade de ar on the first approach and at the beginning of the interview that the child

    or you ng person was not und er any obligafion to parfidp ate. This wa s impo rtant because

    the you ng people may have regarded the interviewer as an authority figure or

    succumbed to parental pressure to partidpate in the research. Requesting consent from

    children should , whenev er possible, be don e privately, away from other family mem bers.

     

    In term s of mofivafion, the choice of research me thod ma y ha ve different implications for

    the length of time that children with different interests, personalities, gender and so on

    can con centrate and ma intain an interest in the interview. In the stud y of child carers, the

    age of the child was directly related to the length of the interview, which in turn was  

    related to the dep th of response. Declining mofivafion du ring an interview could also be

    und erstood as an indirect way of withholding or withd raw ing consent to parfidpate, if a

    you ng per son found it difficult to do so directly.

    Finally there are pracfical considerafions relating to privacy, location and the need to

    conduct interviews away from paren ts or other family membe rs. This is impo rtant, bu t

    not always feasible. W here should interviews with children be carried out? At hom e, at

    school, in a bu rger bar? It is frequently desirable to interview peo ple in their ow n hom es

    and this often provides important observafional data for the researcher. In the child

    carers study , several interviews were conducted in burge r bars or other 'neu tral' settings,

    as du ldr en did not wa nt to be interviewed at hom e. On other occasions interviews were

    conducted under very difficult circumstances with constant interrupfions and back-

    ground noise. The locafion and environmen t in which interviews are conducted has

    implicafions not just for privacy and the quality of data collected bu t also for how the

    data are recorded. In both the child carers study and the Child Support Act study,

    interviews were taped and it wa s anfidpated that there migh t have been some problem s

    in doing this. In practice no problems were encountered in gaining consent to tape-

    record. Giving children control over the tape recorder—b y giving it to them to hold and

    to turn o n and off as they wishe d— was found to aid not orJy the willingness to be taped

    but also rapport between researcher and respondent.

     

  • 8/18/2019 Researching Children

    9/11

    Researching Children: Methods and Ethics 153

    Conclusions

    We have started from the viewpoint that, for important theoretical, social and legal

    reasons, children's perspe ctives on the issues and experiences which affect them can and

    should b e investigated. It is no longer appropriate (if indeed it ever was) to rely on the

    accounts of others or to try and make deductions from knowledge about children's

    location within a given family structure. At this stage, we would like to draw two main

    conclusions, although our work in this research area is continuing.

    First, we d o not claim that an y of the problems w e have encou ntered a nd the solutions we

    have described in this paper are particularly original to sodal research. The ways in

    which we have been forced to think these issues through has brought home to us a

    number of practical and methodological issues that are important and relevant to

    research in other contexts as well. In other words, the experience has had an additional

    heuristic value as well, in helping us to think our way more dearly through some of the

    issues involved in a wider variety of research contexts.

    Secondly, it has made us aware of the importance of drawing on and building on the

    experiences of others. We we re lucky in that we ha d available to us work carried ou t, for

    example, by the National Children's Bureau and the major national children's welfare

    organisations, which have already developed guidelines on involving children in

    research. We w ere able to recruit sldlled an d sensitive interviewers. We w ere also able to

    talk to people w ith expe rience of carry ing out research o n very difficult subjects,

    indu ding the immediate aftermath of marital breakdown. We we re also aware that there

    is a wealth of experience of researching children to draw on in other disdp lines, such as

    developmental psychology. These contacts should help us to develop good practice in

    what is likely to become a growing research area. They may also prove valuable allies

    against those who maintain that children's voices cannot or should not be heard.

    References

    Alderson, P (1995)   L i s t e n i n g t o C h i l d r e n : C h i l dr e n E t h i cs a n d Social  R e s e a r c h . Ba ma r d o s

    Brad shaw , J and Millar , J (1991)

      L o n e P a r e n t F a m i l ie s in t h e U K .

     DSS Research R eport 6. HM SO

    Burghes, L (1994)

      L o n e P a r e n t h o o d a n d F a m i l y D i s r u p t io n . T h e O u tc om e s f or C h i l dr e n .

     Fam ily Po licy

    Studies Centre

    Qarke, K, Craig, G and Glendinning, C (1993)

      C h i ld r e n C o m e F i r s t?

      B a m a d o s

    Clarke , K, Craig, G and Glendinning, C (1996)

      S m a l l C h a n g e : T u r n Y e a r s o f t h e C h i l d S up p o r t A c t.

    Family Policy Studies Centre

    Clarke , K, Glendinning, C and Craig, G (1994)   L o s i n g Su p p o r t : C h i l d r e n a n d t h e C h i l d S u p p o r t A c t.

    Children 's Socie ty

    Cockett , M and Tripp, J (1994)   T h e E x e t e r F a m i l y S t ud y .  Universi ty of Exeter Press

    Comwell , J (1984)  Ha rd Earn ed L ives: Account s o f Heal th an d I l lne ss f rom East London.  Tavistock

    Publications

    Ford, R and others (1994)  Lone Par en thood in 1989,1991 an d 1993: Thre e  Cross section  Survey Report .

    Policy Studies Insti tute

    Keith, L (1992) 'W ho cares wins? W om en, car ing emd disabil i ty '.

      Di sabi l i t y , Handicap and Soc ie t y ,

     7(2),

    167-76

    Kieman, K and Estaugh, V (1993)   C o h a b i t a t i o n .  Family Policy Studies Centre

    M aho n, A and H iggin s, J (1995) 'A l ife of our o wn . . . '

      Y o un g C a r e r s : An E va l u a t i on o f T h r e e R H A

    Funded Projec t s i n Merseys ide.  Health Services Management Unit , Universi ty of Manchester

    Morris, J (1991)  Pr ide ag ai ns t Prejudice: Tran s form ing a t t i tudes to Disabi l i t y . Wo men 's P ress

    Oakley, A and o thers (1995) 'He alth and cemcer prevention : know ledg e and beliefs of children and

    young people ' ,

      Br i t i sh Medical

     Journal 3 1 0 ,1 0 2 9 - 3 3

  • 8/18/2019 Researching Children

    10/11

    154 Ann Mahon and others

    Contributors' details

    Ann M ahon

     is a research associate at the Nation al Primary C are Research an d Development Centre,

    University of Manchester.

    Gary Craig  is Professor at the School of Social Policy, University of Humberside, Hull.

    Caroline Glendinning

      is a seriior research fellow at the National Primary Care Research and

    Development Centre, University of Manchester. 

    Karen Clarke

      works in the Department of Social Policy, University of Manchester.

    Professor

      Gary Craig, Caroline Glendinning,

      an d

      Karen Qarke

      collaborated in the study of the

    Child Support Act, 1993-96, and   Ann Mahon conducted the study of young carers, 1994-95.

  • 8/18/2019 Researching Children

    11/11