Relations Between Teacher and Classroom Activity Variables

download Relations Between Teacher and Classroom Activity Variables

of 30

Transcript of Relations Between Teacher and Classroom Activity Variables

  • 7/24/2019 Relations Between Teacher and Classroom Activity Variables

    1/30

    JOURNAL OF APPLIED DEVELOPMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY 16,253-282(1995)

    Relations Between Teacher and

    Classroom Activity Variables and the

    Classroom Behaviors of

    Prekindergarten Children in

    Chapter 1 Funded Programs

    MARTHA B BRONSON

    Boston College

    TERRENCE IVNAN

    Harvard University

    PATRICIAS SEPPANEN

    RMC Research Corporation

    This study of 586 children in 54 Chapter 1 funded classrooms in five geographical

    areas wos part of o national observational study of early childhood programs. The

    study examined relations between eight teacher and classroom activity variables and

    14 child variables. Correlational analyses indicated significant relations between

    program and teacher variables and childrens experiences in the classroom. More

    program ond teacher variables were associated with childrens mastery than social

    experiences. Results indicated some benefits ond trade offs when more teacher and

    classroom time wos spent on teaching and on cognitively focused activities.

    A growing body of research has demonstrated that children from low-income or

    high-risk environments benefit from participation in high-quality early childhood

    programs and that these benefits translate into reduced risk of school failure and

    economic benefits to society in terms of lower school expenditures for special

    education, savings in the criminal justice system, and greater economic indepen-

    dence through employment and earnings (Balasubramaniam & Tumbull, 1988;

    Bamett & Escobar, 1987; Berrueta-Clement, Schweinhart, Barnett, Epstein, &

    Weikart, 1984; Lazar, Darlington, Murray, Royce, & Snipper, 1982; McKey et

    al., 1985; Weikart, 1989). Research has also suggested that differences in pro-

    gram philosophy and curriculum have differential effects on childrens behavior

    Support for this study was provided by the U.S. Department of Education, Offke of Policy and

    Planning under Contract No. EALC890980.

    Correspondence and requests for reprints should be sent to Martha B. Bronson, School of

    Education, Boston College, 201B Champion Hall, Chestnut Hill, MA 02167-3813.

    253

  • 7/24/2019 Relations Between Teacher and Classroom Activity Variables

    2/30

    254 BRONSON, TIVNAN, AND SEPPANEN

    and development (Clark-Stewart, 1980; Prescott, 1973; Snow, 1983; Weikart,

    198 l), but relatively little is known about how specific aspects of programs affect

    children. Researchers have lamented their inability to connect specific program

    elements with outcomes (Barnett, Frede, Mobasher, & Mohr, 1987; Powell,

    1987; Sigel, 1990) and to disentangle the effects of curriculum content, specific

    activities, and teaching techniques (Miller, Bugbee, & Hybertson, 1985). This

    study examines relations between a set of observed teacher and classroom activ-

    ity variables, and child characteristics, including observed social and mastery

    behaviors in the classroom. Teacher and classroom activity variables with direct

    policy implications were selected for analysis.

    The study was part of the Observational Study of Early Childhood Programs,

    a national study of programs for disadvantaged children which had two related

    components: (a) a descriptive study of 119 early childhood classroom environ-

    ments, including Head Start programs, school-sponsored programs, and day-care

    centers (Layzer, Goodson, & Moss, 1993) and (b) a substudy of approximately

    750 children enrolled in 55 prekindergarten classrooms funded all or in part

    through Chapter 1 of the Hawkins-Stafford Elementary and Secondary School

    Improvement Amendments of 1988 (Seppanen, Godin, Metzger, Bronson, &

    Cichon, 1993). The overall intent of the national study was to provide

    observation-based empirical data on programs for young children.

    This study was part of the Chapter 1 substudy. Chapter 1 of Title 1 of the

    Elementary and Secondary Education Act of 19652 is one of the oldest, most

    well-established federal education laws intended to provide supplemental in-

    struction to low-achieving students in schools with low-income students. A

    small, but growing segment of the student population at the prekindergarten level

    is served through Chapter 1 funded programs. The increasing role of Chapter 1 in

    early childhood education has raised a number of issues for state and local

    education officials. Among these has been how best to create programs with

    characteristics that promote basic and advanced skills for young children at

    risk for school failure.

    To remedy the lack of connectedness between the long-term effectiveness of

    programs and specific program variables, there is growing interest in trying to

    determine how these effects occur by examining childrens experiences in these

    programs (Clarke-Stewart & Fein, 1983; Powell, 1987, Takanishi, 1979). A

    major goal of the Chapter 1 substudy was to provide empirical information about

    relations between Chapter 1 funded prekindergarten environments and childrens

    cognitive and social-emotional development, as revealed in a variety of

    observation-based measures. This study focuses on describing the relations be-

    U.S. Department of Education, Off& of Policy and Planning, Contract Number EALC890980.

    ZReauthorized by Congress as part of the Hawkins-Stafford Elementary and Secondary School

    Improvement Amendments of 1988.

  • 7/24/2019 Relations Between Teacher and Classroom Activity Variables

    3/30

    OBSERVATIONAL STUDY OF CHAPTER 1 PREKINDERGARTEN PROGRAMS

    255

    tween specific child-level and classroom-level observation data gathered in the

    Chapter 1 component of the Observational Study of Early Childhood Programs.

    METHOD

    Participants

    The sample consisted of 586 children in 54 classrooms3 distributed over 16

    school districts in five states (Florida, Texas, California, Michigan, and New

    Jersey). The sample of classrooms was considered to be representative of the

    range of quality in Chapter 1 funded prekindergarten programs in urban and

    suburban settings.

    Demographic characteristics of the sample of children4 are listed in Table 1.

    Slightly more than half of the children were girls. Eighty-five percent were from

    racial or ethnic minority groups, but teachers reported that English was the

    primary language spoken at home by over three fourths of the mothers (or

    primary care givers). Seventy-eight percent of the sample were eligible for free or

    reduced lunch.

    Thirty-five percent of the mothers of sample children and 27 of the fathers

    had at least some college experience. Approximately 22 of the mothers and

    fathers had completed less than 12th grade. About 30 of the mothers were

    under 19 years of age when they gave birth to their first child. Only about half of

    the parents were currently married.

    Teachers reported that approximately 14 of the families of sample children

    needed assistance due to substance abuse, economic problems, or behavioral,

    emotional, or mental problems. Teachers also indicated that 12 of the sample

    children needed assistance for a behavioral, emotional, or mental problem and

    that 9 had resided with someone other than their natural mother for more than 1

    month since school had started. Only small percentages of sample children were

    reported by teachers to have physical, emotional, or mental conditions that

    required frequent medical attention and treatment or that limited and prevented

    regular school attendance (6 ) or participation in all school activities (5 ).

    Classrooms

    Staff in the Chapter 1 funded prekindergarten classrooms were relatively highly

    educated. All teachers had at least a 4-year college degree and 42 had a

    masters degree or higher. Forty-five percent of the assistants and aides had some

    3The total number of children in the Chapter 1 substudy was 783 in 55 classrooms, but not all of

    these children had complete sets of test scores and classroom observations.

    4These characteristics are not necessarily representative of all Chapter I preschool children for

    two reasons: (a) the Chapter 1 substudy did not randomly sample Chapter 1 prekindergarten programs

    and (h) children were eligible for participation in the Chapter 1 substudy if they were simply enrolled

    in a class funded all or in part with Chapter 1 funds.

  • 7/24/2019 Relations Between Teacher and Classroom Activity Variables

    4/30

    256

    BRONSON, TIVNAN, AND SEPPANEN

    TABLE 1

    Demographic Characteristics of Chapter 1 Samplea

    Background Characteristics

    Gender

    Female

    Ethnicity

    African-American

    Hispanic

    White

    Other

    Primary language Spoken at Home

    Mostly English

    Mostly Spanish

    Mixed English and Spanish

    Other

    Eligibility for Free or Reduced-Price

    Lunch

    Eligible

    Data not available

    Education of Father

    Grade 11 or less

    Grade 12 or GED

    At least some college

    Unknown

    Education of Mother

    Grade 11 or less

    Grade 12 or GED

    At least some college

    Unknown

    Mothers Age at Birth of First Child

    13-15

    16-18

    19-21

    22 or older

    Marital Status

    Married

    Evidence Family Needed Help Due

    to Substance Abuse, Economic

    Problems, Emotional, or Mental

    Problems

    of Sample

    52

    48

    31

    15

    7

    77

    8

    10

    5

    78

    14

    22

    44

    27

    7

    22

    41

    35

    1

    5

    24

    34

    35

    50

    14

    Evidence Child Needed Help Due to

    Behavioral, Emotional, or Mental

    Problem

    12

    (continued)

  • 7/24/2019 Relations Between Teacher and Classroom Activity Variables

    5/30

    OBSERVATIONAL STUDY OF CHAPTER

    1

    PREKINDERGARTEN PROGRAMS

    257

    TABLE 1 (Continued)

    Background Characteristics

    of Sample

    Child Resides Apart

    From Mother

    (> 1 Month)

    9

    Evidence of Conditions That

    Limit/Prevent

    Regular school attandance

    6

    Participation in all classroom activities 5

    Usual childhood activities

    4

    Evidence of Conditions That Require

    Frequent medical attention or treatment

    6

    Regular medicine or drugs 2

    Use of special equipment

    1

    aParent and teacher questionnaires and review of student

    records.

    college, 18 had a bachelors degree, and only 8 had less than a high-school

    diploma. Seventy-one percent of the teachers held a state early childhood certifi-

    cate or license and 87 held a state elementary education certificate or license.

    Eighteen percent of the assistants and aides held a state early childhood education

    certificate or license and 6 held an elementary license.

    Teachers had taught in early childhood for an average of 9.6 years, assistants

    and aides had 7.8 years of experience, and both had taught about two thirds of

    this time at their current programs. Both staff groups had an additional few years

    of grade-school teaching experience; teachers averaged 3.4 years and assistants

    and aides averaged 2.8 years.

    The average enrollment across all classrooms was 19.1 (SD = 4.3) but the

    average attendance was 16.0 SD = 3.2). Almost all classrooms had two staff

    members (one teacher and one assistant or aide) so the observed child:staff ratio

    children per class on any given day was 9.3 SD = 2.7), which is within the

    National Association for the Education of Young Children (NAEYC) guidelines

    for 4-year-olds (10: 1 for a group of 20).

    The majority (71 ) of the Chapter 1 funded classrooms were half-day pro-

    grams, with the rest providing an extended day (with lunch and a rest period) of 4

    to 6 hr.

    Measures

    A number of instruments were used to collect both child-level and classroom-

    level data. Because the overall study was observation based, the measures were

    selected after extensive review of available classroom-level (Goodson, 1990) and

    child-level (Seppanen & Love, 1990) observation instruments. In addition to

  • 7/24/2019 Relations Between Teacher and Classroom Activity Variables

    6/30

    258

    BRONSON, TIVNAN, AND SEPPANEN

    observational measures, a standardized cognitive test was selected and informa-

    tion was collected on child and family background characteristics.

    Child and Family Background Characteri stics.

    Background information was

    collected from three sources: a parent questionnaire, a teacher questionnaire, and

    examination of student records. A number of children in the sample had charac-

    teristics associated with risk for school failure. Other studies have suggested that

    although the effect of any one background variable may be negligible, the cu-

    mulative effect of a number of them may affect childrens performance on the

    child-level measures (Hauser-Cram, Pierson, Walker, & Tivnan, 1991; Sam-

    eroff, 1975; Sameroff, Seifer, & Zax, 1987). Schorr and Schorr (1988) found that

    it takes more than a single risk factor to elicit an adverse outcome (p. 25).

    To analyze the cumulative effects of child and family background risks on the

    child-level variables, a risk composite variable was used that was composed of

    nine child and family background variables. It was not possible to collect data on

    some of the more potent risk variables examined by Schorr and Schorr (1988;

    such as low birth weight, substance abuse, or child abuse), but nine available

    variables conceptually associated with risk were included. The variables in-

    cluded in the composite were: (a) English not spoken in the home, (b) limitation

    of school activities, (c) special education requirements, (d) teacher rated behav-

    ioral or emotional problems, (e) family problems, (f) living away from mother,

    (g) mothers age, and (h & i) parents education. The criterion for each risk

    variable component was based on analysis of the Fall 1990 family background

    data and on analyses of the distributions of background data collected during

    Spring 1991. Children were given one risk point if an aspect of their background

    met the established risk criterion for that variable. Each child received a compos-

    ite risk score that could vary from 0 to 9.

    Chi ld Level Measures. The design of the Chapter 1 study called for individu-

    al assessment of children in the fall and spring of the prekindergarten year. In a

    fall pretest and spring posttest, child characteristics were measured with a stan-

    dardized cognitive index, the Preschool Inventory (PSI) (Caldwell, 1976), and a

    teacher rating scale, the Child Behavior Rating Scale (CBRS; Bronson, Good-

    son, Layzer, & Love, 1990). In the spring, behavioral observations in the class-

    room environment were done using the Bronson Social and Task Skills Profile

    (BSTSP), 1991 revision (Bronson, 1991). This study examines data from the

    spring data collection. Table 2 lists the child-level variables included in the

    analyses.

    The PSI is a brief, 32-item assessment of cognitive achievement designed to

    be administered individually to children in the age range of 3 to 6 years old in

    either English or Spanish. It was developed originally to provide Head Start with

    a practical measure of preschool achievement. Correlations between PSI test

    scores and the Stanford Binet Intelligence test ranged (in the five age groups)

  • 7/24/2019 Relations Between Teacher and Classroom Activity Variables

    7/30

    OBSERVATIONAL STUDY OF CHAPTER

    1

    PREKINDERGARTEN PROGRAMS 259

    TABLE 2

    Child-Level Variables Included in the Analysis of Data From the Chapter 1 Study

    Variable Categories and Variables

    Standardized Cognitive Indicator

    (PSI)

    Score in the fall

    Score in the spring

    Teacher Rating (CBRS)

    Mean competence rating in the fall

    Mean competence rating in the spring

    Classroom Behavior Observations

    (BSTSP)

    Mastery variables

    Time in mastery activities (proportion of all observation time spent in a task with

    a goal)

    Use of organizing and planning strategies (rate of use of organizing and planning

    strategies during tasks with goals)

    Tasks completed successfully (proportion of tasks tried that are finished success-

    fully)

    Time distracted in mastery (proportion of mastery time spent distracted)

    Social variables

    Time in social activities (proportion of all observation time spent in social

    interaction)

    Time in organized social interaction (proportion of social time spent at the

    organized and cooperative level)

    Use of organizing strategies with peers (rate of use of strategies which organize

    interactions with peers during time in social activities)

    Use of accomodating strategies with peers (rate of use of social accomodating

    strategies, like sharing and taking turns, with peers during time in social

    activities)

    Success in influencing peers (proportion of attempts to influence peers that were

    successful)

    Use of verbal means to influence peers (proportion of attempts to influence peers

    that were verbal)

    Self-management variables

    Time uninvolved (proportion of all observation time not involved in any focused

    activity)

    Resists rules or teachers (rate of resisting rules or teachers during all observation

    time)

    from .39 to .65, with .44 being the correlation for the entire standardization

    sample. Split-half reliability, estimated with the Spearman-Brown formula, was

    reported as .95 on an earlier version of the PSI. The measure was selected

    because it had been used in numerous national early childhood studies in recent

    years. It yields an overall summary score that was included in the analyses for

    this study.

  • 7/24/2019 Relations Between Teacher and Classroom Activity Variables

    8/30

    260

    BRONSON, TIVNAN, AND SEPPANEN

    The CBRS is a 32-item behavioral rating scale designed to assess categories

    similar to the BSTSP. An earlier version of the CBRS was created by RMC

    Research Corporation for use in a Head Start evaluation (Meleen, Love, &

    Nauta, 1988). The CBRS is designed to evaluate a childs task behavior and

    social behavior with peers and adults. The 32 behaviors are rated on a 5-point

    scale ranging from 1 (never) to 5 (usually/always) to indicate how frequently

    they occur. It was used in the Giant-Step evaluation (Abt Associates, 1988) in

    which items on task orientation and strategies were more strongly correlated with

    the PSI than were the adult and peer interaction items. There was a fall-spring

    correlation of .67 for ratings of 364 Giant-Step children by their teachers. Inter-

    nal consistency (Cronbachs alpha) was .96 overall for the Giant-Step sample. A

    mean rating for all the items was computed to create the summary score used in

    these analyses.

    The BSTSP is an observation measure that was adapted from an earlier

    version of the instrument (Bronson, 1985) for use in the Chapter 1 substudy.

    The BSTSP uses structured categories to record a childs performance in plan-

    ning and organizing mastery activities (puzzles, matching and sorting tasks,

    number and letter tasks, etc.) and carrying them out successfully, in interacting

    cooperatively and successfully with others, and controlling and directing oneself

    appropriately in the school setting.

    Social and mastery task behaviors are recorded in structured categories by an

    observer who had been trained to a criterion of 90 interobserver agreement in

    every category. A modified time sampling method is used in which half of the

    observations are begun at the beginning of a social interaction and half are begun

    at the beginning of a memory task. Whenever possible, observations are carried

    out when children have some choice and control over the selection and direction

    of their activities. In this study, observations were carried out in the natural

    setting of the classroom with each child being observed for four to six lo-min

    periods over 1 to 2 weeks. Technical information from the use of the BSTSP in

    several studies is available (Bronson, 1985, 1994).

    Because the focus of this study was on documenting relations between

    classroom-level variables and the childs classroom experience, data from 12

    BSTSP child-behavior variables were included in the analyses. Variables in three

    areas (mastery, social, and self-management) were selected on the basis of their

    usefulness in other studies (Bronson, 1994; Bronson, Pierson, & Tivnan, 1984;

    Hauser-Cram, Bronson, & Upshur, 1993; Pierson et al., 1983). These are listed

    in Table 2 and described more fully in Appendix A.

    Classroom Level Measures. A number of instruments measuring the class-

    room environment, classroom activities, and teacher interactions with children

    were included in the descriptive component of the Observational Study of Early

    ?See Seppanen, Bronson, and Metzger (1992) for a description of the adaptation procedures.

  • 7/24/2019 Relations Between Teacher and Classroom Activity Variables

    9/30

    OBSERVATIONAL STUDY OF CHAPTER

    1

    PREKINDERGARTEN PROGRAMS

    261

    Childhood Programs, which studied 119 early childhood classrooms (Abt Asso-

    ciates, 1991). Seven of these instruments were used in the Chapter 1 substudy

    (Seppanen et al., 1993). This article includes data from variables selected from

    two of the instruments, chosen on the basis of their relevance to specific educa-

    tional practices. Table 3 lists the eight classroom-level variables selected for

    inclusion in the study.

    In the Abt Associates Adult-Focused Observation (Abt Associates, 1991), an

    observer records the behavior of the classroom teacher in structured categories on

    a time-sampled basis. Observers were trained to a criterion of 85 interobserver

    agreement in each category. Recorded information includes the type of involve-

    ment with children (teaching, management, etc.), the number of children with

    whom he or she is involved, specific verbalizations, the content of teaching, and

    techniques for communicating. The instrument was developed for the Observa-

    tional Study of Early Childhood Programs and no psychometric analyses have

    yet been done. Two variables from this instrument were included in the analyses

    (see Table 3).

    The Preschool ClasSroom Snapshot (adapted by Abt Associates, 1991, from

    Stallings, 1975) provides a means for recording, in 24 categories, the types of

    activities and groupings that are in evidence in a classroom at a given moment.

    Observers were trained to a criterion of 85 percent interobserver agreement in

    each of these categories. Multiple snapshots were recorded in classrooms at

    TABLE 3

    Classroom-Level Variables included in the Analysis of Data From the Chapter 1

    Study

    Instruments and Variable Categories

    Variables From Abt Associates

    Adult-Focused Observationa

    Proportion of time teacher spends teaching

    Proportion of teaching focused on cognitive concepts

    Variables From Preschool Classroom

    Snapshotb

    Proportion of goal-directed activities (a composite of math and language, science and

    natural world, sewing, woodworking, cooking, block construction, table games and

    puzzles, looking at books)

    Proportion of math and language activities

    Proportion of activities involving block construction

    Proportion of sand and water play

    Proportion of dramatic and fantasy play

    Average child : staff ratio in observed snapshot instances

    avariables derived from the Abt Associates Adult-Focused Observation instrument

    are based on brief time samples of adult behaviors. bvariables derived from the

    Preschool Classroom Snapshot are proportions of snapshot instances in which the

    event occurred.

  • 7/24/2019 Relations Between Teacher and Classroom Activity Variables

    10/30

    262

    BRONSON, TIVNAN, AND SEPPANEN

    regular time intervals and aggregated to produce activity and grouping scores.

    Versions of the instrument were used in the National Day-Care Study (Stallings &

    Porter, 1980) and by the New York City Board of Education (BOE) to describe

    environments in BOE preschool and kindergarten programs (Jarvis, 1989). No

    additional technical information is available. Six variables from this instrument

    were included in the analyses for this study (see Table 3).

    The classroom-level instruments, and their use in the descriptive part of the

    Observational Study of Early Childhood Programs, are described in detail in

    Layzer, Goodson, Layzer, and Levin (1991) and in Layzer et al. (1993).

    Rationale for Classroom Level Variable Selection

    The two instruments previously described included many more classroom vari-

    ables (see Layzer, Goodson, & Moss, 1993) than the subset selected for inclu-

    sion in this study (Table 3). The two teacher variables from the Abt Associates

    Adult-Focused Instrument were selected because they showed ways teachers

    spend time in classrooms that seemed likely to be related to childrens experi-

    ences and cognitive or social outcomes. The five classroom activity variables

    from the Preschool Classroom Snapshot were selected to reflect the variety of

    activities typically found in classrooms for young children. The sixth variable

    from the snapshot instrument, child:staff ratio, was selected because variations in

    ratio have been associated with observed behavior differences in children in

    previous studies (Prescott, 1973; Ruopp, Travers, Glantz, & Coelen, 1979; Van-

    dell & Powers, 1983). All eight variables provide information on specific educa-

    tional practices that could be influenced by public policy.

    Data Collection Procedures

    This study focuses on data from the child-level and classroom-level measures

    obtained during Spring 1991.

    Collection of ChiM Level Data. The data collection team at each site con-

    sisted of child assessors who were trained to complete either the cognitive mea-

    sures or the BSTSP, data collectors who were trained to complete the full battery

    of classroom-level measures, and a field coordinator. The BSTSP observations

    were completed by child assessors who spent approximately 2 weeks in each

    class on days in which PSI administrations or classroom-level observations were

    not scheduled. During each data collection period, teachers completed the CBRS

    rating forms.

    The quality of data collected was monitored by both the field coordinator and

    the data-analysis team. Observer reliability for the BSTSP was monitored twice

    in the field to be certain that the observers maintained the standard of 90

    interobserver agreement in each category achieved during training.

    Collection of Classroom Level Data. The classroom-level data collection was

    completed over a period of approximately 1 week per class. Each data collector

  • 7/24/2019 Relations Between Teacher and Classroom Activity Variables

    11/30

    OBSERVATIONAL STUDY OF CHAPTER

    1

    PREKINDERGARTEN PROGRAMS

    263

    completed the full battery of classroom-level observations for a particular class-

    room and also interviewed classroom staff.

    The quality of classroom-level observation and interview data was monitored

    primarily by the field coordinator at each of the sites. The initial training of the

    observers was reinforced subsequently in weekly review meetings among field

    coordinators and data collectors. Field coordinators collected field reliability data

    for the time-sample observation measure (Abt Associates Adult Focused Obser-

    vation) by observing and coding with every observer at least once during the data

    collection period.

    RESULTS

    Observed Variable Scores

    Chil l Level Vari ables. Table 4 lists the means and standard deviations of the

    child-level variables. The PSI mean score was 20.3 (SD = 4.9) and the CBRS

    mean score was 3.8 (SD = .68).

    The BSTSP classroom observation mastery variables showed children in-

    volved in mastery tasks during approximately 38 of their overall observation

    time and distracted during these tasks about 4 of the time.6 While working on

    tasks, children used organizing and planning strategies (gathering appropriate

    materials, grouping materials according to a criterion, sequencing activities ap-

    propriately) at a mean rate of 1.6 per min. They completed tasks successfully

    almost 83 of the time.

    Under the structured observation conditions that maximized time in mastery

    or social interactions, approximately 50 of the childrens time over all observa-

    tions was spent in social interactions, with 38 of this time (19 of total

    observation time) spent in social interactions with roles, rules, cooperative inter-

    action, or true conversation (organized interaction). Involvement in social and

    mastery activities could occur at the same time (a child could be working on a

    puzzle and talking to a peer), so the time in social activities does not indicate that

    the child was engaged exclusively in social interaction or a mastery task. During

    interactions with peers, children used organizing strategies (Lets play store,

    You be the mother) at the rate of approximately .6 instances per min (or about

    once every 2 min) and accommodating strategies (You can share mine, We

    could take turns using it) at the rate of about .15 times per min (about once

    every 7 min). Children were successful in their attempts to influence peers 82

    of the time and used verbal means to influence them 81 of the time.

    Variables reflecting lack of adequate self-management (time uninvolved and

    resisting rules or the teacher) were relatively low in frequency. Children in this

    6Time allocated to either mastery or social behaviors is partially an artifact of the data collection

    procedures that require that half the observations begin in a social interaction and half begin in a

    mastery task. Therefore, time allocations do not necessarily reflect how a childs behavior is distrib-

    uted in time during the ordinary course of a classroom day.

  • 7/24/2019 Relations Between Teacher and Classroom Activity Variables

    12/30

    264

    BRONSON, TIVNAN, AND SEPPANEN

    TABLE 4

    Means and Standard Deviations of the Child-Level Variables=

    Child-Level Variable

    M

    SD

    PSI

    Spring score

    CBRS (Teacher Rating)

    Spring summary score

    BSTSP (Classroom Observations)

    Mastery variables

    Proportion time in mastery activities

    Rate organizing and planning strategies

    Proportion tasks completed successfully

    Proportion time distracted in mastery

    BSTSP (Classroom Observations)

    Social variables

    Proportion time in social activities

    Proportion social time at organized level

    Rate uses organizing strategies

    Rate uses accomodating strategies

    Proportion success in influencing peers

    Proportion use of verbal means to influence

    20.3 4.9

    3.8 .68

    37.6 13.5

    1.6 1.0

    82.7 22.2

    4.0 5.9

    49.8

    16.4

    38.3

    29.1

    .58 .54

    .15

    .20

    81.5

    13.6

    81 .O

    13.7

    BSTSP (Classroom Observations)

    Self-management variables

    Proportion time uninvolved

    Rate resists rules or teacher

    1.7 3.0

    .05

    .I3

    aN = 586 children.

    sample were uninvolved less than 2 of the time and resisted rules or the teacher

    at the rate of .05 instances per min.

    Classroom Level Var iables.

    Table 5 shows the mean classroom scores on

    each of the eight classroom-level variables selected for this study.

    Information from the Abt Associated Adult-Focused Instrument indicated that

    39 of all teacher-child interactions were focused specifically on teaching (indi-

    viduals or groups) and 44 of the teaching interactions involved teaching cogni-

    tive concepts (language, reading, math, science). Mean percentages of activities

    from the Preschool Classroom Snapshot records indicated that children were

    involved in goal-directed activities 34 of the time. On average, 7 of all

    observed activities involved math and language, 4 involved block construction

    Goal-directed activities

    included math and language activities, science and natural world activ-

    ities, block construction, sewing, cooking, woodworking, table games and puzzles, and looking at

    books or pictures.

  • 7/24/2019 Relations Between Teacher and Classroom Activity Variables

    13/30

    OBSERVATIONAL STUDY OF CHAPTER

    1

    PREKINDERGARTEN PROGRAMS

    265

    TABLE 5

    Means and Standard Deviations of Classroom-Level Variables8

    Classroom-Level Variables

    M SD

    Variables From Abt Associates

    Adult-Focused Observationb

    Time teaching

    Teaching focused on cognitive concepts

    Variables From Preschool Classroom

    Snapshotc

    Goal-directed activities

    Math and language activities

    Block construction activities

    Sand and water play activities

    Dramatic and fantasy play activities

    Average child : staff ratio

    39 16

    44 17

    34

    7

    4

    4

    10

    9

    13

    a

    5

    5

    5

    3

    aN = 54 classrooms. bProportions based on time samples. CPro-

    portions based on presence or absence of event in total number of class-

    room snapshots.

    4

    involved sand and water play, and 10 involved dramatic or fantasy play.

    The average enrollment across all the sample classrooms was approximately 19

    (SD = 4.3), but the average attendance was only 16 so the observed child:staff

    ratio was about nine children per adult in the classroom.

    Table 6 gives the intercorrelations among the eight classroom-level variables.

    Over all classrooms, teacher time spent in teaching was positively related to

    teaching focused on cognitive concepts (r = .40, p < .Ol) and to the proportion

    of snapshot instances in which goal-directed activities were observed (r = .36,

    p

  • 7/24/2019 Relations Between Teacher and Classroom Activity Variables

    14/30

    266

    BRONSON, TIVNAN, AND SEPPANEN

    TABLE 6

    Correlations Among the Classroom-Level Variablesa

    Variables 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 6

    Time teaching

    Teaching cognitive .40**

    concepts

    Goal-directed

    .36** .33*

    activities

    Math and language

    .27

    .40** .63***

    activities

    Block construction

    .I3

    -.02

    .I7 -.13

    Sand and water -.12 -.Ol -.38**

    -.30* -.I1

    activities

    Dramatic and -.07 -.I3

    -.I4 -.45***

    .26 .23

    fantasy activities

    Child

    :

    staff ratio .Ol -.20

    .07

    .24

    -.08 -.33* -.15

    aN = 54.

    p