Processing Speed, Working Memory and Reasoning Ability From

6
 Processing speed, working memory and reasoning ability from childhood to old age Ted Nettelbeck * , Nicholas R. Burns School of Psychology, University of Adelaide, South Australia 5005, Australia a r t i c l e i n f o  Article history: Received 12 June 2009 Received in revised form 20 October 2009 Accepted 30 October 2009 Available online 27 November 2009 Keywords: Aging Processing speed Working memory Cognitive abilities a b s t r a c t The study investigated whether theoretical causative relations among declining cognitive abilities during adulthood and old age conform to a literal reversal of improving cognitive development during child- hood. Children aged 8–14 years ( n = 240) and adults aged 18–87 (n = 238) completed the same battery of psychometric tests, which dened latent traits for processing speed, working memory, and reasoning ability. Speeded performance improved during childhood and slowed across the adult range. Childhood performance was well described by a developmental cascade, whereby increasing chronological age is accompanied by faster processing speed, which inuences improved working memory, which in turn inuences improving reasoning ability. However, although adult performance resembled a cascade with diminishing reasoning ability mediated by processing speed and working memory, this was not a mirror image of the cascade for children. The main difference with adults was a direct causal path between age and working memory. Post hoc analysis located this among adults aged 55 years and over. This suggests that, whereas childhood cognitive development is substantially mediated by processing speed, declining reasoning ability in old age is inuenced by slower processing speed but also by age-related change(s) inuencing working memory that are independent from processing speed.  2009 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved. 1. Introduction This study tested a suggestion by  Jensen (2006)  that putative changes in brain structure responsible for declining cognitive abil- ities during adulthood and old age may be a mirror image of struc- tura l chan ges resp onsib le for impr oving cogni tive deve lopme nt during childhood. This proposition derives from theory that identi- es individual differences in proce ssing speed as central to an understanding of intelligence differences and also attributes aver- age changes in cognitive abilities during childhood and old age to spee d chan ges. There are well esta blish ed aver age changes in spee d with age on perc eptu al spee d and element ary cogniti ve tasks like reaction times (RT) and inspection time (IT), whereby processing becomes faster during childhood and adolescence but slows thro ugho ut adul thood. Thus,  Kail (1991)  establishe d that improvement to processing speed is more rapid during younger years but tends to asymptote close to early adult levels by about 14 years of age. Similarly,  Salthouse (1996)  summarized evidence that processing speed declines on average across adult years from 20 to 80, the mean trend being approximately linear. Salthouse (1996)  has deve lope d a the ory that substan tial ly attribut es age-related, reduced effectiveness of higher order cogni- tive functioning to general slowing in processing speed, which im- pos es a fun damenta l con str ain t on the efcie ncy of wor kin g memory. Thus, because only brief retention is possible without re- hearsal, slower processing results in slower execution of ongoing oper atio ns and poore r sync hron ization of task componen ts. In turn, a more limited working memory adversely impacts cognitive decision making. Salt house’s theo ry rese mble s what  Fry and Hale (1996 )  de- scribed as a ‘‘cognitive developmental cascade”, that is, a sequence of processi ng stage s within which t he effect iveness of processing at the rst stage has a ow-on effect for the next stage, which inu- ences the next, and so on. Fry and Hale described childhood cogni- tive maturation in terms of causal relations between increasing chronological age, processing speed, working memory, and uid inte llig ence , by cross-se ctio nal compa riso ns across years 7–19. Path analysis conrmed a developmental cascade whereby age- rela ted fast er proce ssing speed resu lted in impr oved working memo ry, which linke d to highe r uid performance (as demo n- strated in Fig. 1). After controlling statistically for age, Fry and Hale found that individual differences in speed inuenced memory, and when both age and speed were controlled, working memory inu- enced uid performance. Fry and Hale also discounted the possibil- ity that age-related improvement in uid ability was responsible for faster proce ssin g spee d, by demonstratin g marked, constant age differen ces acro ss diffe rent speede d task s in age samples matched on raw ability scores. The cascade model therefore pro- vided a good account for average mental age changes and for abil- ity differe nces within age band s, in proce ssing speed, working 0191-8869/$ - see front matter   2009 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2009.10.032 * Corresponding author. Tel.: + 61 8 8303 3764. E-mail add res ses :  [email protected],  ted.nettelbeck@ adelaide.edu.au (T. Nettelbeck). Personality and Individual Differences 48 (2010) 379–384 Contents lists available at  ScienceDirect Personality and Individual Differences journal homepage:  www.elsevier.com/locate/paid

Transcript of Processing Speed, Working Memory and Reasoning Ability From

Page 1: Processing Speed, Working Memory and Reasoning Ability From

7/21/2019 Processing Speed, Working Memory and Reasoning Ability From

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/processing-speed-working-memory-and-reasoning-ability-from 1/6

Processing speed, working memory and reasoning ability from childhood to old age

Ted Nettelbeck *, Nicholas R. Burns

School of Psychology, University of Adelaide, South Australia 5005, Australia

a r t i c l e i n f o

 Article history:

Received 12 June 2009

Received in revised form 20 October 2009Accepted 30 October 2009

Available online 27 November 2009

Keywords:

Aging

Processing speed

Working memory

Cognitive abilities

a b s t r a c t

The study investigated whether theoretical causative relations among declining cognitive abilities during

adulthood and old age conform to a literal reversal of improving cognitive development during child-

hood. Children aged 8–14 years (n = 240) and adults aged 18–87 (n = 238) completed the same batteryof psychometric tests, which defined latent traits for processing speed, working memory, and reasoning

ability. Speeded performance improved during childhood and slowed across the adult range. Childhood

performance was well described by a developmental cascade, whereby increasing chronological age is

accompanied by faster processing speed, which influences improved working memory, which in turn

influences improving reasoning ability. However, although adult performance resembled a cascade with

diminishing reasoning ability mediated by processing speed and working memory, this was not a mirror

image of the cascade for children. The main difference with adults was a direct causal path between age

and working memory. Post hoc analysis located this among adults aged 55 years and over. This suggests

that, whereas childhood cognitive development is substantially mediated by processing speed, declining

reasoning ability in old age is influenced by slower processing speed but also by age-related change(s)

influencing working memory that are independent from processing speed.

  2009 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.

1. Introduction

This study tested a suggestion by  Jensen (2006) that putative

changes in brain structure responsible for declining cognitive abil-

ities during adulthood and old age may be a mirror image of struc-

tural changes responsible for improving cognitive development

during childhood. This proposition derives from theory that identi-

fies individual differences in processing speed as central to an

understanding of intelligence differences and also attributes aver-

age changes in cognitive abilities during childhood and old age to

speed changes. There are well established average changes in

speed with age on perceptual speed and elementary cognitive

tasks like reaction times (RT) and inspection time (IT), whereby

processing becomes faster during childhood and adolescence but

slows throughout adulthood. Thus,  Kail (1991)   established thatimprovement to processing speed is more rapid during younger

years but tends to asymptote close to early adult levels by about

14 years of age. Similarly,  Salthouse (1996) summarized evidence

that processing speed declines on average across adult years from

20 to 80, the mean trend being approximately linear.

Salthouse (1996)   has developed a theory that substantially

attributes age-related, reduced effectiveness of higher order cogni-

tive functioning to general slowing in processing speed, which im-

poses a fundamental constraint on the efficiency of working

memory. Thus, because only brief retention is possible without re-

hearsal, slower processing results in slower execution of ongoing

operations and poorer synchronization of task components. In

turn, a more limited working memory adversely impacts cognitive

decision making.

Salthouse’s theory resembles what  Fry and Hale (1996)   de-

scribed as a ‘‘cognitive developmental cascade”, that is, a sequence

of processing stages within which the effectiveness of processing at

the first stage has a flow-on effect for the next stage, which influ-

ences the next, and so on. Fry and Hale described childhood cogni-

tive maturation in terms of causal relations between increasing

chronological age, processing speed, working memory, and fluid

intelligence, by cross-sectional comparisons across years 7–19.

Path analysis confirmed a developmental cascade whereby age-related faster processing speed resulted in improved working

memory, which linked to higher fluid performance (as demon-

strated in Fig. 1). After controlling statistically for age, Fry and Hale

found that individual differences in speed influenced memory, and

when both age and speed were controlled, working memory influ-

enced fluid performance. Fry and Hale also discounted the possibil-

ity that age-related improvement in fluid ability was responsible

for faster processing speed, by demonstrating marked, constant

age differences across different speeded tasks in age samples

matched on raw ability scores. The cascade model therefore pro-

vided a good account for average mental age changes and for abil-

ity differences within age bands, in processing speed, working

0191-8869/$ - see front matter    2009 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.doi:10.1016/j.paid.2009.10.032

*   Corresponding author. Tel.: + 61 8 8303 3764.

E-mail addresses:   [email protected],   ted.nettelbeck@

adelaide.edu.au (T. Nettelbeck).

Personality and Individual Differences 48 (2010) 379–384

Contents lists available at   ScienceDirect

Personality and Individual Differences

j o u r n a l h o m e p a g e :   w w w . e l s e v i e r . c o m / l o c a t e / p a i d

Page 2: Processing Speed, Working Memory and Reasoning Ability From

7/21/2019 Processing Speed, Working Memory and Reasoning Ability From

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/processing-speed-working-memory-and-reasoning-ability-from 2/6

memory and reasoning. Kail (2007)   confirmed these results, alsoshowing that the cascade explanation held longitudinally for rea-

soning on a re-test after one year.

One interpretation of these results is that maturing brain struc-

tures improve processing speed, with concurrent improvement in

working memory and other cognitive functions. These structures

achieve optimal capacities by early adulthood but then gradually

deteriorate across the adult years. As Jensen (2006, p. 97) has sur-

mised, ‘‘the overall picture of mental decline . . . as shown by chro-

nometric tests is much like a mirror image of the developmental

curves from early childhood to maturity ”. However, although this

account appears consistent with changes in the psychological con-

structs outlined here, it is possible that cognitive deterioration dur-

ing old age involves more than simply the reverse of whatever

maturational changes improve capacities during childhood. If changes in old age reflect other than a mirror image of childhood

development, it should be possible to identify differences between

trends during childhood and old age.

Gregory, Nettelbeck, Howard, and Wilson (2009)   reported a

more complex cascade for people aged 70–91 years. They found

significant paths from age to fluid reasoning via perceptual speed

and working memory. However, cross-sectional and longitudinal

analyses also found direct paths between age and working memory

and between perceptual speed and reasoning. Gregory et al. there-

fore concluded that relations between age, processing speed, work-

ing memory and reasoning ability are more complex among elderly

adults than among children, perhaps because of age-related

changes other than slower speed that might directly impact work-

ing memory. They also proposed that a direct path from processingspeed to reasoning could reflect confounding by motor problems.

The present study extends previous research by testing the cas-

cade model cross-sectionally for an age range from 8 to 80 years.

The test battery was selected to define latent variables for process-

ing speed, working memory and fluid reasoning ability. Two

hypotheses, derived from past results from   Fry and Hale (1996)

and Salthouse (1996) were:

1. Age-related (i.e. improving ) childhood performance in process-

ing speed, working memory and reasoning ability is described

by a structural cascade model (see  Fig. 1). Specifically, signifi-

cant coefficients linking age to the latent variables are limited

to causal paths from age to processing speed, from processing

speed to working memory and from working memory to rea-soning ability.

2. The same cascade model describes  deteriorating   processing

speed, working memory and reasoning ability during adulthood

and old age. That is, older age causes slower processing speed,

which causes poorer working memory, which results in poorer

reasoning ability. As with the childhood model, significant coef-

ficients linking age to the latent variables are limited to the cau-

sal paths in Fig. 1 from age to processing speed, from processing

speed to working memory and from working memory to rea-

soning ability.

Support for both hypotheses would be consistent with Jensen’s

(2006)  suggestion that cognitive changes in old age are a mirror

image of the developmental trends found from early childhood to

adulthood.

2. Method

 2.1. Participants

Participants numbered 478 (288 males, 190 females;   M  =

28.1 years, SD = 21.7), recruited into four non-overlapping age

groups. These samples of convenience provided a wide age range

from childhood to old age. Youngest children were assumed capa-ble of understanding all test requirements in the common battery.

Children were in two groups, both readily accessible within metro-

politan schools; 8–10 years (mid-primary education   n = 120,

M  = 9.69 years, SD = 0.76) and 12–14 years (early secondary educa-

tion   n = 120,   M  = 13.3 years, SD = 0.74). Adults formed two non-

overlapping groups, split around middle age so as to maximise

age differences; 18–45 years (younger,   n = 146,   M  = 30.8 years,

SD = 7.7) and 55–87 years (older,  n  = 92, M  = 67.3 years, SD = 8.2).

They were recruited from the general community via advertise-

ment. Adults were paid Australian $30.

 2.2. Materials

Participants completed the following common battery.

 2.2.1. Processing speed (PS)

Four of the five tests were drawn from   O’Connor and Burns

(2003), who found that these loaded on a well-defined second or-

der general speed factor.

 2.2.2. Digit Symbol

Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scales-IV [WAIS-IV]; 2-min time

limit, number correct.

 2.2.3. Visual Matching 

Woodcock–Johnson Psycho-Educational Battery – Revised [WJ –

R]; 2-min time limit, number correct.

 2.2.4. Inspection time (IT, Nettelbeck (2001))

The target figure was two vertical lines joined at the top by a

horizontal line. The shorter vertical line appeared to left or right

with equiprobability. A warning cue (520 ms) preceded the target.

The target Fig. appeared for varying SOA1, dependent on accuracy.

Exposure was controlled by a backward pattern mask (370 ms).

The participant indicated location of the shorter line by pressing

the left or right key on the computer mouse. Instructions empha-

sized accuracy. Following practice, IT estimation began with SOA

250 ms and followed an adaptive staircase algorithm, which re-

quired three correct responses at any SOA before reducing SOA by

17 ms. Following an error, SOA increased by 17 ms. IT (ms) was

the average SOA calculated over eight reversals of direction on the

staircase (associated probability for correct response .79).

 2.2.5. Simple reaction time (RT)

The apparatus was a scaled down version of the panel described

by Jensen and Munro (1979). One light was illuminated, immedi-

ately above the home button. Participants depressed the home but-

ton and, following light onset, released the button as quickly as

possible. Sixty trials followed 10 practice trials, with a pseudo-ran-

dom period of 1–8 s between each. Median RT (ms) was measured

as duration between light onset and button release.

 2.2.6. Odd man out – decision time (DT, Frearson & Eysenck (1986))

Apparatus was as for simple RT. For each trial, three of eight

possible lights were illuminated, with two adjacent and one fur-

 AGE   PS   WM   RA

Fig. 1.   Theoretical developmental cascade. PS = Perceptual speed, WM = working

memory, RA = reasoning ability.

1 Stimulus-onset-asynchrony: time between target onset and mask onset.

380   T. Nettelbeck, N.R. Burns / Personality and Individual Differences 48 (2010) 379–384

Page 3: Processing Speed, Working Memory and Reasoning Ability From

7/21/2019 Processing Speed, Working Memory and Reasoning Ability From

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/processing-speed-working-memory-and-reasoning-ability-from 3/6

ther away. Participants responded to the latter (‘‘odd-man-out”).

There were 20 practice trials, followed by 60 trials. Median

OMO-DT (duration between lights onset and home button release)

was recorded for correct responses.

 2.2.7. Working memory (WM)

WM was defined by three tests used by Burns, Nettelbeck, and

McPherson (2009), together with a widely used test of spanmemory.

 2.2.8. Picture Swaps (Stankov (2000))

Items consisted of three different cartoon pictures (animals,

familiar objects), presented simultaneously on the computer

screen in different locations (position 1, 2 or 3). Participants made

a series of mental ‘‘swaps” between locations as quickly and accu-

rately as possible (e.g. ‘‘swap 2 and 3”). A response (6-choice alter-

natives) was registered on the computer keyboard’s number pad.

The task had four levels: one swap (6 trials), two swaps (6 trials),

three swaps (12 trials), and four swaps (12 trials). So as to index

WM capacity, the measure was total correct responses in 20 min

or, if fewer than two correct responses were made within any six

consecutive trials, at termination time.

 2.2.9. Picture Recognition (modelled on the test from WJ – R)

Participants tried to remember one to seven pictures presented

on the computer screen for 5 s. These were followed by a new set

of two to six numbered pictures, one to four of which were in the

original presentation. On-screen instructions indicated how many

were from the original stimulus set. Participants indicated pictures

seen previously using the number pad. Following practice, there

were 30 trials that became progressively more difficult. A trial

was scored as correct only if all pictures were recognised. The task

ended either when participants scored zero on six consecutive tri-

als, or after 30 trials.

 2.2.10. Digit Span (modelled on the test from WAIS-IV)

Single digits, 1–9, were presented one at a time on the com-

puter screen for 1 s. Immediately following, participants attempted

to reproduce the sequence, in the same order, entering digits con-

secutively via the number pad. The task began at a span level of 

one digit, with four trials at each span level. Maximum span level

tested was 9. A minimum of two correct trials at any span level was

required to proceed to the next level. Maximum correct score was

36.

 2.2.11. Reasoning ability (RA)

Cattell Culture Fair Test   (computerized version of Scale 2, Form

A). This has four subtests: Series, Classification, Matrices, Condi-

tions (time limits 3 min, 4 min, 3 min, 2.5 min, respectively, as

per manual). Following practice there were 12, 14, 12, and 8 test

items for Series, Classification, Matrices and Conditions, respec-

tively. Participants could not return to an item having entered a re-

sponse for that item. Number correct was recorded for each

subtest.

 2.3. Procedure

The protocol had approval from the University of Adelaide Hu-

man Research Ethics Committee. Children had written parental

consent and adult participants affirmed informed consent in writ-

ing. Children were tested at schools in groups of up to six, over two

sessions of 50 min each. Adults attended the laboratory individu-

ally, completing the test battery in a single session lasting two

hours including rest breaks.

 2.4. Statistical analyses

Latent variable analyses using all available data were conducted

using maximum likelihood estimation (MPlus v5.1;   Muthén &

Muthén, 2007). Model fit was assessed by chi-square-test of exactfit, comparative fit index (CFI), Tucker Lewis index (TLI), root mean

square error of approximation (RMSEA) and its 90% confidence

intervals (CI90), and standardized root mean square residual

(SRMR).

3. Results

Table 1 shows descriptive statistics for all variables, for the en-

tire sample and the four age subsamples. Fig. 2 shows the means of 

standard scores for Digit Symbol, Visual Matching, reflected IT, re-

flected simple RT, and reflected odd-man-out DT, together repre-

senting PS, regressed on age. There is an obvious reversal in

direction of the relationship between PS and age beyond 20 years.

We therefore initially tested the cascade model on two groups;

children 18–14 and adults 18 years and older.

Table 2 shows correlations between all variables for these two

groups, with children above the diagonal and adults below.  Fig. 3

illustrates the developmental cascade model instantiated by our

data. In addition to age, three latent variables represent PS (Digit

Symbol, Visual Matching, IT, simple RT, and odd-man-out DT);

WM (Picture Swaps, Picture Recognition, and Digit Span); and RA

(Series, Classifications, Matrices, and Conditions from Cattell Cul-

ture Fair).

Individual measurement models for latent variables for PS and

RA were well fit by the data for both children (CFI = .90,

 Table 1

Means and standard deviations for processing speed, working memory and reasoning ability.

Total (N  = 478) 8–10 years (n = 120) 12–14 years (n = 120) 18–45 ye ars (n = 146) 55+ years (n = 92)

M SD   Range   M SD   Range   M SD   Range   M SD   Range   M SD   Range

Digit Symbol 60.0 16.9 24–112 44.6 8.62 26–67 57.1 9.37 34–87 76.4 14.9 38–112 57.5 12.6 24–87

Visual Matching 41.6 11.5 16–65 28.3 4.77 16–45 40.2 8.61 23–60 52.6 6.97 34–65 43.1 6.72 26–54

Inspection time (ms) 72.7 29.5 25–274 80.3 20.0 31–179 62.9 24.4 25–274 59.3 14.5 29–139 98.7 43.3 60–274

Simple RT (ms) 262 40.1 180–436 298 40.3 233–436 252 28.5 197–372 242 32.1 184–358 260 33.5 180–349

Odd-man-out DT (ms) 760 414 309–5590 958 455 349–3329 639 204 309–1359 598 136 351–1245 910 634 446–5590

Picture Swaps 22.7 9.24 0–36 17.4 8.06 1–35 24.4 8.24 0–36 28.1 7.40 1–36 18.3 8.95 1–36

Picture Recognition 17.5 4.34 5–29 15.7 3.24 7–24 18.3 3.66 7–26 20.4 3.67 7–29 14.5 4.37 5–23

Digit Span 20.3 5.97 1–35 16.8 3.08 6–29 19.8 3.45 8–28 25.1 5.23 2–35 18.0 7.58 1–31

Series 8.89 1.93 0–12 8.53 1.65 4–12 9.44 1.25 6–12 9.80 1.56 3–12 7.14 2.29 0–12

Classifications 7.84 1.99 0–13 7.16 1.76 3–12 8.12 1.67 4–13 8.78 1.84 4–13 6.88 2.19 0–12

Matrices 8.73 2.16 0–12 7.78 1.86 3–11 9.38 1.78 1–12 9.86 1.55 4–12 7.34 2.50 0–11

Conditions 4.89 2.12 0–8 4.03 2.04 0–8 5.57 1.82 1–8 5.76 1.90 0–8 3.77 1.99 0–8

Note: N  and ns varied across tests. For any test, minimum listwise  N  = 443 and n = 117 (8–10 years), n = 109 (12–14 years), n = 139 (18–45 yrs), and  n = 78 (55+ years). For thewhole sample there were complete data for n  = 425.

T. Nettelbeck, N.R. Burns / Personality and Individual Differences 48 (2010) 379–384   381

Page 4: Processing Speed, Working Memory and Reasoning Ability From

7/21/2019 Processing Speed, Working Memory and Reasoning Ability From

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/processing-speed-working-memory-and-reasoning-ability-from 4/6

SRMR = .05; CFI = 1.0, SRMR = .01, respectively) and adults (CFI =

.99, SRMR = .05; CFI = 1.0, SRMR = .00) 2.

As shown in  Fig. 3, there are six possible causal paths on the

structural model. Age predicts PS, WM, and RA (hereafter paths

1, 2, and 3, respectively); PS predicts WM and RA (paths 4 and

5); and WM predicts RA (path 6). This model was tested separately

for children and adults. First, the full model was tested. Second,

statistically non-significant paths were set to zero and the Wald

test used to compare the more constrained and full models. A final

model that retained only statistically significant paths was then

fitted.

For children the full cascade model fit was:   v2 (60) = 113.4,

 p < .001, CFI = .95, TLI = .93, SRMR = .05, RMSEA = .06, CI90 = [.043,

.078]. Paths from age to WM and RA, and from PS to RA were not

statistically significant. Setting these to zero and testing the effect

of these constraints via the Wald test found no significant deterio-

ration in fit  v2 (3) = 3.27, p  = .35. The fit of the final model was  v2

(63) = 123.7,  p  < .001, CFI = .94, TLI = .93, SRMR = .05, RMSEA = .06,

CI90 = [.047, .080]. Standardized parameter estimates for this mod-

el are shown in Fig. 4a. This result was therefore consistent with

previous findings, with children’s performance well described by

a cascade, from age to PS, to WM, to RA.

For adults the full cascade model fit was:   v2 (60) = 110.9,

 p < .001, CFI = .96, TLI = .95, SRMR = .05, RMSEA = .06, CI90 = [.042,.077]. Paths from age and PS to RA (3, 5) were not statistically sig-

Fig. 2.  Scatterplot of processing speed and age for the whole sample showing loess function (locally weighted polynomial regression). Boxplots along each axis summarize

the distributions of both variables.

 Table 2

Correlation matrix for all variables. Children above diagonal, adults below diagonal.

1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12

1. Digit Symbol .587   .399   .522   .416 .459 .424 .434 .296 .282 .424 .326

2. Visual Matching .801   .327   .406   .428 .369 .459 .358 .260 .263 .334 .276

3. Inspection time   .490   .483 .365 .339   .367   .322   .193   .222   .183   .263   .281

4. Simple RT   .418   .407 .273 .360   .387   .378   .357   .308   .245   .324   .284

5. Odd-man-out DT   .440   .394 .396 .344   .321   .291   .259   .401   .166   .289   .298

6. Picture Swaps .489 .507   .359   .243   .377 .432 .385 .385 .383 .390 .447

7. Pictur e Recognition .541 .520   .493   .299   .323 .492 .316 .234 .282 .363 .419

8. Digit Span .442 .412   .424   .292   .281 .554 .444 .251 .216 .344 .323

9. Series .505 .503   .476   .254   .393 .585 .587 .521 .319 .364 .314

10. Classifications .390 .385   .365   .322   .376 .399   .420 .378 .552 .461 .411

11. Matrices .518 .503   .443   .273   .378 .537 .561 .458 .643 .526 .404

12. Conditions .469 .449   .365   .308   .366 .578 .463 .428 .541 .435 .506

Note: All correlations calculated using pairwise deletion for missing values;  ns range from 216 to 239 for children and from 208 to 236 for adults.

2 With three indicators WM was a just identified model, so that the fit had to beperfect.

382   T. Nettelbeck, N.R. Burns / Personality and Individual Differences 48 (2010) 379–384

Page 5: Processing Speed, Working Memory and Reasoning Ability From

7/21/2019 Processing Speed, Working Memory and Reasoning Ability From

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/processing-speed-working-memory-and-reasoning-ability-from 5/6

nificant. Setting these to zero and testing the effect of these con-

straints via the Wald test showed no significant deterioration in

fit   v2 (2) = 1.02,   p  = .60. The fit of the final model was   v2 (62)

= 112.2,   p < .001, CFI = .97, TLI = .96, SRMR = .05, RMSEA = .06,

CI90 = [.041, .075]. Standardized parameter estimates are shownin Fig. 4b. This model was similar to that for the children but dif-

fered in one important respect; there was a direct path from age

to working memory, as found by Gregory et al. (2009).

To gauge further the extent to which these models mirrored one

another, we examined the relative loadings of the manifest vari-

ables on the three latent variables in the two models. Establishing

full measurement invariance here is not relevant because the

means of the latent variables were not expected to be the same;

the sign of the regression for children of PS on age was reversed

from that for adults; and the regression of WM on age was fixed

at zero in children but freely estimated in adults. However, the ex-

tent to which the point estimates of the manifest variables within

each group fall within the confidence intervals for these in the

other group provides an indication of similarity. As seen from Table

3, although there was some overlap between the 95% confidence

intervals across children and adults, in only 10 from a possible

24 comparisons did a point estimate for one group fall within the

range for the other group.Finally, we explored further whether the direct causal path be-

tween age and WM held throughout the full span of the adult age

range. We fitted and refined the cascade model (as described

above) to data for the younger adults (18–45 year olds,   n = 146)

and the older participants (55–87 years,   n = 92). For 18–45 year-

olds the fit statistics and model parameters were  v2 (63) = 68.90,

 p = .29, CFI = .99, TLI = .98, SRMR = .17, RMSEA = .03, CI90  = [.00,

.06]. This model did not include a causal path between age and

WM. For the older adults, after including the direct path between

age and WM, fit statistics and model parameters were  v2 (62) =

84.00,   p = .03, CFI = .93, TLI = .91, SRMR = .08, RMSEA = .06, CI90 =

[.02, .09].

Fig. 3.   Developmental cascade model showing all possible causal paths. PS = per-

ceptual speed, WM = working memory, RA = reasoning ability, DS = Digit Span;

VM = Visual Matching, IT = inspection time, RT = simple reaction time, DT = odd-

man-out decision time, Sw = Picture Swaps, PR = Picture Recognition, Sp = Digit

Span, Se = Series, Cl = Classification, Ma = Matrices, Co = Conditions.

Fig. 4.  (a) Final structural model for children showing standardized parameter estimates. PS = perceptual speed, WM = working memory, RA = reasoning ability, DS = Digit

Span; VM = Visual Matching, IT = inspection time, RT = odd-man-out decision time, Sw = Picture Swaps, PR = Picture Recognition, Sp = Digit Span, Se = Series, Cl = Classi-

fication, Ma = Matrices, Co = Conditions. (b) Final structural model for adults showing standardized parameter estimates. PS = perceptual speed, WM = working memory,

RA = reasoning ability, DS = Digit Span; VM = Visual Matching, IT = inspection time, RT = simple reaction time, DT = odd-man-out decision time, Sw = Picture Swaps,PR = Picture Recognition, Sp = Digit Span, Se = Series, Cl = Classification, Ma = Matrices, Co = Conditions.

 Table 3

Parameter estimates and 95% CI for loadings of manifest variables on latent variables

processing speed (PS), working memory (WM) and reasoning ability (RA).

Children Adults

Digit Symbol? PS .75 [.68, .82] .89 [.85, .93]

Visual Matching? PS .79 [.73, .85] .87 [.83, .92]

Inspection Time? PS .49 [.38, .60] .62 [.53, .72]

Simple RT? PS .66 [.57, .74] .48 [.37, .58]

Odd-man-out DT? PS .38 [.26, .50] .28 [.16, .40]Picture Swaps?WM .70 [.61, .79] .77 [.71, .84]

Picture Recognition?WM .63 [.54, .73] .72 [.65, .79]

Digit Span?WM .56 [.46, .67] .67 [.58, .75]

Series? RA .53 [.43, .64] .82 [.77, .88]

Classifications? RA .60 [.50, .70] .64 [.55, .72]

Matrices? RA .69 [.60, .78] .77 [.71, .83]

Conditions? RA .65 [.55, .74] .68 [.60, .76]

T. Nettelbeck, N.R. Burns / Personality and Individual Differences 48 (2010) 379–384   383

Page 6: Processing Speed, Working Memory and Reasoning Ability From

7/21/2019 Processing Speed, Working Memory and Reasoning Ability From

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/processing-speed-working-memory-and-reasoning-ability-from 6/6

4. Discussion

Hypothesis 1 was supported, confirming a developmental cas-

cade for children and adolescents, as found by   Fry and Hale

(1996). PS and WM mediated improved reasoning ability with

age, with 70% of variance in PS accounted for by age, with PS

accounting for 66% of variance in WM and the latter accounting

for 78% of variance in RA.With adults, however, the simple cascade consistent with Salt-

house (1996)  was not found. Instead, results confirmed a direct

path from age to WM as reported by  Gregory et al. (2009) and un-

der circumstances where the latent variable has been more clearly

defined. Moreover, the relative loadings of manifest to latent vari-

ables within the models for children and adults were markedly dif-

ferent, with fewer than half of the possible comparisons between a

manifest loading on the latent variable falling within confidence

intervals for the other group and with the three latent variables

appreciably less well differentiated among the children than

among the adults.

Although Fig. 2 suggests slow linear decline in PS, with compa-

rable scatter across the adult years, as expected from past research,

performance among 18–45 year-olds was substantially superior to

that of 55–87 year-olds on all tests except SRT (refer to  Table 1).

This provided  a priori  grounds to explore further whether the di-

rect causal path in the adult cascade model between age and

WM held throughout the full age range. Despite the smaller num-

bers involved, both the model for 18–45 year-olds and that for 55–

87 years fitted the data well; but it was necessary to include the di-

rect path between age and WM for the older group.

These results suggest that the simple developmental cascade

model describing childhood cognitive development is reversed at

around 20 years of age and that the reversed trend may prevail un-

til middle age. Beyond about 55 years, however, the onset of old

age appears to be accompanied by age-related changes indepen-

dent from processing speed. Taken together, the differences be-

tween the models for children and particularly older adults were

not consistent with Jensen’ (2006)   suggestion that the childhooddevelopmental cascade model and the model that describes cogni-

tive change during old age are mirror images of one another. In

short, therefore, whereas the simple cascade model provides a

good description for the trajectory of improving performance dur-

ing childhood, and declining performance during early adulthood,

the model that applies to deteriorating performance during old

age is more complex.

However, even among older adults we did not find a direct path

from PS to RA, as reported by Gregory et al. (2009) and it is possible

that their result reflected a speed constraint on Raven’ scores,

although similar speed constraints also applied here to the four

subtests in the Cattell Culture Fair Test. Arguably, these time con-

straints were sufficiently severe to cause confounding between

power and speed aspects of this test and future research should ex-plore this issue. Similarly, beyond fitting five speeded tasks to a la-

tent processing speed model, this study has not addressed the

question of whether speed of processing is well described as a sin-

gle, task-independent construct. Thus, it is clear from   Fig. 4   that

there were considerable differences between the five speeded tests

used here in the extent to which the different tasks tapped pro-

cessing speed as defined here. IT and DT showed much higher

specificity than the other three tests and future research should

seek to determine what additional sources of variance other than

speed these tasks are measuring.To summarize, the implication of these results is that the cas-

cade model for maturing childhood cognition and the model that

describes cognitive decline during old age are not simply mirror

images of one another. Slower processing is causatively linked to

cognitive decline in old age, moderating the relationship between

age and WM, and accounting for about 78% of WM variance. This

is substantial but other unidentified age-related variables, inde-

pendent from processing speed, also appear to exert a direct influ-

ence on WM. Our data offer no leads as to what these influences

might be. However, one possibility, speculative but worth explor-

ing, is that individual differences may exist in age-related deterio-

ration in the short-term span component of WM3. If so, this may be

independent from processing speed, which may principally affect

attentional aspects of WM (Burns et al., 2009).

 Acknowledgement

This work was supported by Australian Research Council Dis-

covery Grant DP0211113 to both authors.

References

Burns, N. R., Nettelbeck, T., & McPherson, J. (2009). Attention and intelligence: Afactor analytic study.  Journal of Individual Differences, 30, 44–57.

Frearson, W., & Eysenck, H. J. (1986). Intelligence, reaction time (RT) and a new‘odd-man-out’ RT paradigm.   Personality and Individual Differences, 7 , 807–817.

Fry, A. F., & Hale, S. (1996). Processing speed, working memory, and fluidintelligence: Evidence for a developmental cascade.  Psychological Science, 7 ,237–241.

Gregory, T., Nettelbeck, T., Howard, S., & Wilson, C. (2009). A test of the cascademodel in the elderly.  Personality and Individual Differences, 46 , 71–73.

 Jensen, A. R. (2006).  Clocking the mind: Mental chronometry and individual differences.Amsterdam: Elsevier.

 Jensen, A. R., & Munro, E. (1979). Reaction time, movement time and intelligence.Intelligence, 3, 121–126.

Kail, R. (1991). Developmental change in speed of processing during childhood andadolescences. Psychological Bulletin, 109, 490–501.

Kail, R. V. (2007). Longitudinal evidence that increases in processing speed andworking memory enhance children’s reasoning.   Psychological Science, 18,312–313.

Muthén, L. K., & Muthén, B. O. (2007).  Mplus User’s Guide (5th ed.). Los Angeles, CA:Muthén & Muthén.

Nettelbeck, T. (2001). Correlation between inspection time and psychometricabilities: A personal interpretation.  Intelligence, 29, 459–474.

O’Connor, T. A., & Burns, N. R. (2003). Inspection time and general speed of processing. Personality and Individual Differences, 35, 713–724.

Salthouse, T. A. (1996). The processing-speed theory of adult age differences incognition. Psychological Review, 103, 403–428.

Stankov, L. (2000). Complexity, metacognition, and fluid intelligence.  Intelligence, 28, 121–143.

3 We are grateful to an anonymous reviewer for this suggestion.

384   T. Nettelbeck, N.R. Burns / Personality and Individual Differences 48 (2010) 379–384