Preschoolers’ Memory for Distinctive...

1
Preschoolers’ Memory for Distinctive Information Stephanie E. Miller 1 , Stuart Marcovitch 1 , and Melissa D. McConnell Rogers 2 1 University of North Carolina at Greensboro, 2 Whitworth University Method Participants Forty-three 4-year-olds (M age = 4.58 years, SD = .27), 49 5-year-olds (M age = 5.45 years, SD = .30), and 47 6-year-olds (M age = 6.55 years, SD = .33) participated in the study. Design and Procedure Children were presented with two six-item picture lists from the categories of animals, foods, vehicles, or clothes. O In the Isolation List (Figure 1), the target item was categorically different compared to background items. O In the Control List (Figure 2), all items (including the target) were from the same category. The order of list presentation was counterbalanced. Categories were not repeated between lists for an individual child. Target items were always presented in the fifth position. One child’s isolate item (Figure 1) was presented as another child’s control item (Figure 2). For each list, the experimenter presented the picture cards one at a time and asked children to name the picture. If they labeled the picture incorrectly or failed to produce the label, children were told the name of the picture and asked to repeat it. After presentation of list 1, there was a 5-minute delay followed by a free recall period. There was a delay of at least 5 minutes before the presentation of list 2. Figure 1. Isolation List (target item highlighted) Results A mixed analysis of variance (ANOVA) was conducted on children’s recall of the critical target item with target type (control vs. isolate) as a within subjects variable and age group as a between subjects variable. O A marginal effect of target type, F (1, 136) = 3.03, p = .08, revealed that a higher percentage of children recalled the control item compared to the isolate item. O An age group by target type interaction, F (2, 136) = 2.47, p = .09, was also marginally significant. Within subjects planned comparisons revealed better memory of control items over the isolate item for the 4-year-olds, t(42) = 2.49, p =.02, but not for 5- or 6-year-olds, both ts < .47, ps > .64 (see Figure 3). Output position of the target item was also analyzed, as items salient in memory should come to mind more rapidly. A mixed analysis of variance (ANOVA) was conducted on output position of the target item with target type (control vs. isolate) as a within subjects variable and age group as a between subjects variable. Scores ranged from 1 to 7 with 6 being the last output location and 7 assigned when the target item was not recalled. O A marginal effect of target type, F (1, 136) = 3.25, p = .07, indicated that the isolate item was recalled later in free recall than the control item. O An age group by target type interaction, F (2, 136) = 3.43, p = .04, was also significant. Within subjects planned comparisons revealed that the isolate was recalled later than the control item for 4-year-olds, t(42) = 2.56, p = .02, but not for 5- and 6-year-olds, both ts < .67, ps > .39 (see Figure 4). Introduction The “von Restorff” or “isolation” effect occurs when better memory is exhibited for an item that appears salient or distinct relative to items that are categorically similar to each other (Hunt, 1995). An item appears salient when individuals process the differences (i.e., distinctive processing) between a unique item relative to similarly organized items (i.e., organizational processing, Hunt & Lamb, 2001). The isolation effect has not been observed in preschoolers for conceptually distinct information, but this may be due to changes in methodology: O Howe, Courage, Vernescu, and Hunt (2000) found no isolation effect when children were presented pictorially with three isolates (i.e., perceptual, conceptual, and numerical) amidst 8 items from the same category. However, multiple isolates may make it difficult to discern the background context and may increase distinctiveness processing of all items reducing salience of the isolates. O Marcovitch, Hunt, and Lange (2006) also found no memory benefit for distinctive information when children were asked to recall a conceptual isolate placed late in the pictorially presented list immediately after list presentation. However, the lack of delay between list presentation and recall may result in a recency effect, which would lead to increased recall of both the isolate and control items. The goal of the current study was to determine whether children younger than 6 years of age produce the isolation effect for a conceptual isolate in a more traditional paradigm. Specifically, there was only a single isolate within a list and there was a 5-minute delay before recall. Demonstration of an isolation effect would indicate that children spontaneously process information in an organizational and distinctive manner, whereas the lack of an isolation effect would suggest difficulty simultaneously processing differences relative to distinctive information while organizing background information. Discussion Young preschoolers are actually less likely to recall information meant to be distinct. O This finding is consistent with research demonstrating that the necessary organizational (e.g., Schwenck, Bjorklund, & Schneider, 2009) and distinctive processing strategies (e.g., Ornstein, Hale, & Morgan, 1977) are developing in early preschool. Young preschoolers also recall the isolate item later compared to the control item. O Failure or late recall of the isolate may indicate that young children initially concentrate on organizational processing by focusing on recalling and grouping similar items first. Organizational processing is typically spontaneous in adults but may be more effortful in children (Schwenck et al., 2009). The focus on effortful organizational processing may not allow children to engage in or benefit from distinctiveness processing of the isolate item relative to the organized context. References Howe, M. L., Courage, M. L., Vernescu, R., & Hunt, M. (2000). Distinctiveness effects in children’s long term-retention. Developmental Psychology, 36, 778-792. doi: 10.1037//0012-1649.36.6.778 Hunt, R. R., (1995). The subtlety of distinctiveness: What von Restorff really did. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 2, 105 - 112. Hunt, R. R., & Lamb, C. A. (2001). What causes the isolation effect? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning Memory and Cognition, 27, 1359 - 1366. doi: 10.1037// 0278-7393.27.6.1359 Marcovitch, S., Hunt, R. R., & Lange, G. W. (May 2006). Failing to find an isolation effect in preschoolers. Poster presented at the 18th Annual Convention of the Association for Psychological Science, New York, NY. Ornstein, P. A., Hale, G. A., & Morgan, J. S. (1977). Developmental differences in recall and output organization. Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society, 9, 29-32. Schwenck, C., Bjorklund, D. F., & Schneider, W. (2009) Developmental and individual differences in young children’s use and maintenance of a selective memory strategy. Developmental Psychology, 45, 1034-1050. Figure 2. Control List (target item highlighted) Figure 3. Proportion of Children Recalling Target Item by Age * * p < .05 Figure 4. Target Item Recall Position by Age * p < .05 *

Transcript of Preschoolers’ Memory for Distinctive...

Page 1: Preschoolers’ Memory for Distinctive Informations_marcov/docs/SRCD_2011_Preschoolers_Memory.pdfPreschoolers’ Memory for Distinctive Information Stephanie E. Miller1, Stuart Marcovitch1,

Preschoolers’ Memory for Distinctive Information Stephanie E. Miller1, Stuart Marcovitch1, and Melissa D. McConnell Rogers2

1University of North Carolina at Greensboro, 2Whitworth University

MethodParticipants

• Forty-three 4-year-olds (M age = 4.58 years, SD = .27), 49 5-year-olds (M age = 5.45 years,SD = .30), and 47 6-year-olds (M age = 6.55 years, SD = .33) participated in the study.

Design and Procedure

• Children were presented with two six-item picture lists from the categories of animals, foods,vehicles, or clothes.O In the Isolation List (Figure 1), the target item was categorically different compared to

background items.O In the Control List (Figure 2), all items (including the target) were from the same category.

• The order of list presentation was counterbalanced. Categories were not repeated betweenlists for an individual child. Target items were always presented in the fifth position.

• One child’s isolate item (Figure 1) was presented as another child’s control item (Figure 2).

• For each list, the experimenter presented the picture cards one at a time and asked children toname the picture. If they labeled the picture incorrectly or failed to produce the label, childrenwere told the name of the picture and asked to repeat it.

• After presentation of list 1, there was a 5-minute delay followed by a free recall period.

• There was a delay of at least 5 minutes before the presentation of list 2.

Figure 1. Isolation List (target item highlighted)

Results

• A mixed analysis of variance (ANOVA) was conducted on children’s recall of the critical targetitem with target type (control vs. isolate) as a within subjects variable and age group as a betweensubjects variable.

O A marginal effect of target type, F (1, 136) = 3.03, p = .08, revealed that a higher percentageof children recalled the control item compared to the isolate item.

O An age group by target type interaction, F (2, 136) = 2.47, p = .09, was also marginallysignificant.• Within subjects planned comparisons revealed better memory of control items over the

isolate item for the 4-year-olds, t(42) = 2.49, p =.02, but not for 5- or 6-year-olds, both ts <.47, ps > .64 (see Figure 3).

• Output position of the target item was also analyzed, as items salient in memory should come tomind more rapidly. A mixed analysis of variance (ANOVA) was conducted on output position ofthe target item with target type (control vs. isolate) as a within subjects variable and age group asa between subjects variable. Scores ranged from 1 to 7 with 6 being the last output location and 7assigned when the target item was not recalled.

O A marginal effect of target type, F (1, 136) = 3.25, p = .07, indicated that the isolate item wasrecalled later in free recall than the control item.

O An age group by target type interaction, F (2, 136) = 3.43, p = .04, was also significant.• Within subjects planned comparisons revealed that the isolate was recalled later than the

control item for 4-year-olds, t(42) = 2.56, p = .02, but not for 5- and 6-year-olds, both ts <.67, ps > .39 (see Figure 4).

Introduction

• The “von Restorff” or “isolation” effect occurs when better memory is exhibited for an item thatappears salient or distinct relative to items that are categorically similar to each other (Hunt, 1995).

• An item appears salient when individuals process the differences (i.e., distinctive processing)between a unique item relative to similarly organized items (i.e., organizational processing, Hunt &Lamb, 2001).

• The isolation effect has not been observed in preschoolers for conceptually distinct information,but this may be due to changes in methodology:

O Howe, Courage, Vernescu, and Hunt (2000) found no isolation effect when children werepresented pictorially with three isolates (i.e., perceptual, conceptual, and numerical) amidst 8items from the same category. However, multiple isolates may make it difficult to discern thebackground context and may increase distinctiveness processing of all items reducingsalience of the isolates.

O Marcovitch, Hunt, and Lange (2006) also found no memory benefit for distinctive informationwhen children were asked to recall a conceptual isolate placed late in the pictorially presentedlist immediately after list presentation. However, the lack of delay between list presentationand recall may result in a recency effect, which would lead to increased recall of both theisolate and control items.

• The goal of the current study was to determine whether children younger than 6 years of ageproduce the isolation effect for a conceptual isolate in a more traditional paradigm. Specifically,there was only a single isolate within a list and there was a 5-minute delay before recall.Demonstration of an isolation effect would indicate that children spontaneously processinformation in an organizational and distinctive manner, whereas the lack of an isolation effectwould suggest difficulty simultaneously processing differences relative to distinctive informationwhile organizing background information. Discussion

• Young preschoolers are actually less likely to recall information meant to be distinct.

O This finding is consistent with research demonstrating that the necessary organizational(e.g., Schwenck, Bjorklund, & Schneider, 2009) and distinctive processing strategies (e.g.,Ornstein, Hale, & Morgan, 1977) are developing in early preschool.

• Young preschoolers also recall the isolate item later compared to the control item.

O Failure or late recall of the isolate may indicate that young children initially concentrate onorganizational processing by focusing on recalling and grouping similar items first.

• Organizational processing is typically spontaneous in adults but may be more effortful inchildren (Schwenck et al., 2009). The focus on effortful organizational processing may not allowchildren to engage in or benefit from distinctiveness processing of the isolate item relative to theorganized context.

References

Howe, M. L., Courage, M. L., Vernescu, R., & Hunt, M. (2000). Distinctiveness effects in children’s long term-retention.Developmental Psychology, 36, 778-792. doi: 10.1037//0012-1649.36.6.778

Hunt, R. R., (1995). The subtlety of distinctiveness: What von Restorff really did. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 2, 105 - 112.Hunt, R. R., & Lamb, C. A. (2001). What causes the isolation effect? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning Memory and

Cognition, 27, 1359 - 1366. doi: 10.1037// 0278-7393.27.6.1359Marcovitch, S., Hunt, R. R., & Lange, G. W. (May 2006). Failing to find an isolation effect in preschoolers. Poster presented at the

18th Annual Convention of the Association for Psychological Science, New York, NY.Ornstein, P. A., Hale, G. A., & Morgan, J. S. (1977). Developmental differences in recall and output organization. Bulletin of the

Psychonomic Society, 9, 29-32.Schwenck, C., Bjorklund, D. F., & Schneider, W. (2009) Developmental and individual differences in young children’s use and

maintenance of a selective memory strategy. Developmental Psychology, 45, 1034-1050.

Figure 2. Control List (target item highlighted)

Figure 3. Proportion of Children Recalling Target Item by Age

*

* p < .05

Figure 4. Target Item Recall Position by Age

* p < .05

*