Pre-school Children’s Food Habits and Meal...

64
Comprehensive Summaries of Uppsala Dissertations from the Faculty of Social Sciences 114 _____________________________ _____________________________ Pre-school Children’s Food Habits and Meal Situation Factors Influencing the Dietary Intake at Pre-school in a Swedish Municipality BY HANNA SEPP ACTA UNIVERSITATIS UPSALIENSIS UPPSALA 2002

Transcript of Pre-school Children’s Food Habits and Meal...

Page 1: Pre-school Children’s Food Habits and Meal Situationuu.diva-portal.org/smash/get/diva2:161313/FULLTEXT01.pdf · school, the behaviour of the pre-school staff or the children’s

Comprehensive Summaries of Uppsala Dissertationsfrom the Faculty of Social Sciences 114

_____________________________ _____________________________

Pre-school Children’s Food Habitsand Meal Situation

Factors Influencing the Dietary Intake atPre-school in a Swedish Municipality

BY

HANNA SEPP

ACTA UNIVERSITATIS UPSALIENSISUPPSALA 2002

Page 2: Pre-school Children’s Food Habits and Meal Situationuu.diva-portal.org/smash/get/diva2:161313/FULLTEXT01.pdf · school, the behaviour of the pre-school staff or the children’s

Dissertation for the Degree of Doctor of Philosophy in Domestic Sciences presented atUppsala University in 2002

ABSTRACT

Sepp, H. 2002. Pre-school Children’s Food Habits and Meal Situation – Factors Influencingthe Dietary Intake at Pre-school in a Swedish Municipality. Acta Universitatis Upsaliensis.Comprehensive Summaries of Uppsala Dissertations from the Faculty of Social Sciences 114.64 pp. Uppsala. ISBN 91-554-5240-X

A pre-school-based dietary survey, using seven-day records, focus group interviews and semi-structured interviews, was carried out in a suburban area of Stockholm. The overall objectivewas to investigate the individual food and nutrient intake of pre-school children at all mealsduring the day, as well as factors that might influence children’s intake.

The average energy and nutrient intake per day for the whole week was satisfactory for the109 pre-school children, but the temporal distribution throughout the day was skewed. Theenergy and nutrient intakes of food at the pre-school were lower than recommended. Thiswas, however, compensated for by meals eaten at home. The children had a more varied foodintake during weekdays than weekend days. This study has not provided any evidence tosupport the selection of water versus milk as a preferable lunch beverage in terms of pre-school children's total milk consumption and general dietary quality. However, the dietaryanalyses showed that there could be a reason to limit pre-school children’s daily milk andfermented milk intake to half a litre, according to the existing guidelines.

The children associated food and eating with rules and norms. They did not categorise food asgood or bad, as adults often do, but as “food” and “non-food”; for example, sweets were notfood. The method used in this study, the focus group interview, was judged to be a useful toolfor exploring how children think about and jointly reflect upon food. The role of the teacherhad changed over the past years and they had not yet found a solid ground for integrating foodand meals into their everyday work.

Key words: children, food, meals, nutrient, pre-school, teachers

Hanna Sepp, Department of Domestic Sciences, Dag Hammarskjölds väg 21, SE-752 37 Uppsala, Sweden

© Hanna Sepp 2002

ISSN 0282-7492ISBN 91-554-5240-X

Printed in Sweden by Uppsala University, Tryck & Medier, Uppsala 2002

Page 3: Pre-school Children’s Food Habits and Meal Situationuu.diva-portal.org/smash/get/diva2:161313/FULLTEXT01.pdf · school, the behaviour of the pre-school staff or the children’s

”Att forska om barn behöver mjölk är somatt forska om båtar behöver vatten…”

Granne från Nossebro

Page 4: Pre-school Children’s Food Habits and Meal Situationuu.diva-portal.org/smash/get/diva2:161313/FULLTEXT01.pdf · school, the behaviour of the pre-school staff or the children’s

LIST OF ORIGINAL PAPERS

I Sepp H, Lennernäs M, Pettersson R, Abrahamsson L. Children’s nutrientintake at pre-school and at home. Acta Paediatrica, 90: 483-491, 2001.

II Sepp H, Abrahamsson L, Lennernäs Junberger M, Risvik E. Thecontribution of food groups to the nutrient intake and food pattern amongpre-school children. Food Quality and Preference, 13:107-116, 2002.

III Sepp H, Hofvander Y, Abrahamsson L. The role of milk in Swedish pre-school children’s diet. Scandinavian Journal of Nutrition, 45: 131-136,2001.

IV Wesslén A, Sepp H, Fjellström C. Swedish pre-school children'sexperience of food. International Journal of Consumer Studies. In press.

V Sepp H, Abrahamsson L, Fjellström C. Pre-school staffs' attitudes towardfoods in relation to the pedagogic meal. Manuscript

Page 5: Pre-school Children’s Food Habits and Meal Situationuu.diva-portal.org/smash/get/diva2:161313/FULLTEXT01.pdf · school, the behaviour of the pre-school staff or the children’s

CONTENTS

ABBREVIATIONS .................................................................................................................. 7

BACKGROUND....................................................................................................................... 9

Introduction .............................................................................................................................. 9The public meal for children................................................................................................... 9Studying children’s food habits ............................................................................................ 10

The socialisation and acquisition of food preferences in pre-school children .................. 10Children and new food.......................................................................................................... 12Influences on food preferences ............................................................................................. 12Cultural norms regarding body weight ................................................................................ 12

Methods in assessing food habits .......................................................................................... 13Quantitative methods ............................................................................................................ 13Qualitative methods .............................................................................................................. 15

Nutrition policies .................................................................................................................... 15

Energy and nutrition recommendations .............................................................................. 16Guidelines for pre-school ..................................................................................................... 17

GENERAL AIMS OF THE THESIS ................................................................................... 19

SUBJECTS, METHODS AND DISCUSSION .................................................................... 20

Selection of pre-schools.......................................................................................................... 20

Studies I, II and III................................................................................................................. 21Measuring and recording food intakes................................................................................. 21Validation principles............................................................................................................. 23Data processing, statistics and presentation ........................................................................ 23

Study IV .................................................................................................................................. 25

Study V.................................................................................................................................... 27

In addition............................................................................................................................... 28

RESULTS AND DISCUSSION............................................................................................. 29

Dietary intake ......................................................................................................................... 29Socio-economic background................................................................................................. 29Energy ................................................................................................................................... 29Minerals, vitamins, sucrose and dietary fibre ...................................................................... 31Food pattern.......................................................................................................................... 35

Page 6: Pre-school Children’s Food Habits and Meal Situationuu.diva-portal.org/smash/get/diva2:161313/FULLTEXT01.pdf · school, the behaviour of the pre-school staff or the children’s

Socialisation and acquisition of food habits in pre-school.................................................. 38Normative influences ............................................................................................................ 38The pedagogic meal .............................................................................................................. 41Ideal meal situation - the utopian meal ................................................................................ 44The public-pedagogic meal in the future .............................................................................. 44

SUMMARY AND CONCLUSIONS..................................................................................... 46

ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS................................................................................................... 48

REFERENCES ....................................................................................................................... 49

SAMMANFATTNING .......................................................................................................... 62

Page 7: Pre-school Children’s Food Habits and Meal Situationuu.diva-portal.org/smash/get/diva2:161313/FULLTEXT01.pdf · school, the behaviour of the pre-school staff or the children’s

ABBREVIATIONSBMI Body Mass IndexBMR Basal Metabolic RateEI Energy IntakeEU European UnionE% Energy percentageHS Hanna SeppM-children Children at pre-schools where milk was served as a meal beverage at pre-

school lunchMJ Energy, Mega Jouleg/MJ Nutrient density, gram/Mega JouleNFA Swedish National Food AdministrationNNR Nordic Nutrition RecommendationsPAL Physical Activity LevelPCA Principal Component AnalysisRDA Recommended Dietary AllowancesREE Resting Energy ExpenditureSD Standard DeviationSNR Swedish Nutrition RecommendationsW-children Children at pre-schools where water was served as a meal beverage at pre-

school lunchWHO World Health Organization

Page 8: Pre-school Children’s Food Habits and Meal Situationuu.diva-portal.org/smash/get/diva2:161313/FULLTEXT01.pdf · school, the behaviour of the pre-school staff or the children’s
Page 9: Pre-school Children’s Food Habits and Meal Situationuu.diva-portal.org/smash/get/diva2:161313/FULLTEXT01.pdf · school, the behaviour of the pre-school staff or the children’s

9

BACKGROUNDIntroductionThe roots of the Swedish childcare system can be traced back to the second half of the19th century. The first infant crèche was opened in 1854 in Stockholm for children ofsingle mothers who were forced to work for a living (Tallberg Broman, 1995). In aregulation from 1891 at a pre-school in Lund, food is mentioned as an important issue“Children from one to six years of age may be admitted at the crèche. Children at thecrèche receive nourishing food” (Tallberg, Broman, 1995, p.11). However, it took 90years before the pre-schools received governmental financial support, meaning theyalso became controlled by the state.

In the 1960s, female labour was in demand and calls for a major expansion ofchildcare facilities intensified. In 1968, the government appointed a specialcommission, the National Commission on Childcare (Government Report, 1972),which presented a proposition on how a childcare system that met social, educationaland supervisory requirements might be developed in Sweden. In 1985, the SwedishParliament decided that all children from the age of 18 months until school age were tohave access to the municipal childcare system (Government Report, 1985). In 1998,the pre-schools received their first curriculum (Government Report, 1998) and werethereby under the control of the National Agency of Education, meaning that what hadonce been a municipal affair was now a national one.

The public meal for childrenIn Sweden, three of every four children 1-5 years of age (National Agency forEducation, 2000) are enrolled in the municipal childcare system. Consequently, morethan 360,000 children are served one to three meals a day (breakfast, lunch, afternoonsnack) at pre-school (Sundström, 2000). Thus, for a majority of children, the childcaresystem is responsible for their dietary intake during most of the weekdays. Yet thepublic meal in pre-schools has not been regulated in any law, guidelines or curriculum,nor in any other documents from specifically departments of state. This is the casedespite the fact that, since 1968, the National Commission on Childcare hascharacterised the food and meal situation as a pedagogic activity, later defining it asthe pedagogic meal. It has, thus, been up to each municipality to set its own qualitystandards for the pre-school food system. The meal situation had been entirely amunicipal affair until 1996, when the National Food Administration in collaborationwith the Centre for Applied Nutrition within the Stockholm County Council and theSwedish Association of Local Authorities developed a guideline for pre-school mealsincluding energy and nutrient recommendations as well as recommendations for whatkind of foods should be served, how much and when (Wållberg et al., 1996). Howthese guidelines, aims and recommendations are received and followed is of greatinterest to parents, personnel responsible for childcare as well as public authorities, inthe latter case given the extensive use of childcare in Sweden. This is particularlyimportant when financial cutbacks are increasing in this as in all public services. The

Page 10: Pre-school Children’s Food Habits and Meal Situationuu.diva-portal.org/smash/get/diva2:161313/FULLTEXT01.pdf · school, the behaviour of the pre-school staff or the children’s

tightening of the childcare sector during the nineties was mainly caused by anincreasing birth rate in the absence of corresponding funding increases. The parentspay a fee, which in most areas is linked to the family’s income and/or the child’s totalhours of attendance. However, this fees only cover on average 17% of the actual costs(Nilsson & Pettersson, 2000).

Studying children’s food habitsFew studies investigating the diet of pre-school children in Sweden have beenperformed. These studies are mainly focused on children’s energy and nutrient intake,and in some cases food intake (Hagman et al., 1986; Jacobson & Nordlund, 1994;Koivisto et al., 1994b). In none of the studies do the meal environment at the pre-school, the behaviour of the pre-school staff or the children’s perception of food andeating seem to have been covered, although there are many reasons why research onpre-school children's food habits in a broader perspective is of interest. First of all,food habits determine nutritional intake, which promotes growth, development andhealth. Second, food choice is important in the establishment of eating habits (Casey &Rozin, 1989; Kelder et al., 1994; Birch, 1999; Koivisto Hursti, 1999; Rolls et al.,2000). Third, obesity and diabetes as well other eating disturbances are increasingamong young people in Western society (World Health Organization, 1997;International Life Sciences Institute, 2000). An understanding of the early developmentand interaction of different factors that influence the food choice with regard tonutritional intakes is therefore of importance. Studying pre-school children’s foodhabits, however, is complex and many parameters should be taken into consideration.In every culture there are, for example, rules and norms about which foods are edible,how they should be prepared and served and when and how they should be eaten; theserules also depend on age, sex, residential and social status. Therefore, in studies offood habits in general, it is important to look not only at the energy and nutrient intake,but also at the meal, table manners among children among pre-school staff, as well asother behaviours and interactions at the table.

The aim of the following presentation is to give an overall picture both of the researchon pre-school children’s food habits and of the complexity of studying thesephenomena.

The socialisation and acquisition of food preferences in pre-school childrenSince most children in Sweden between the ages of one and five are enrolled in themunicipal childcare system, parents share responsibility for their children’ssocialisation and acquisition of food habits with personnel responsible for the pre-school meals. Socialisation is the process whereby a culturally valued behaviouralnorm is passed on from generation to generation (Mennell et al., 1992); as shown inFigure 1, this process can be divided into primary, secondary and re-socialisation(Fieldhouse, 1996). The acquisition of food habits through primary socialisation during

Background

10

Page 11: Pre-school Children’s Food Habits and Meal Situationuu.diva-portal.org/smash/get/diva2:161313/FULLTEXT01.pdf · school, the behaviour of the pre-school staff or the children’s

Background

11

infancy and childhood is primarily accomplished though interaction with family andlater also with friends.

Figure 1. Socialisation and the acquisition of food habits (Reproduced with the permission of NelsonThornes Ltd from Food and Nutrition: Customs and Culture 2e, Fieldhouse, 1995)

The foundations of food choice are laid down in childhood and dietary habits learnedin childhood may persist into adult life (Birch, 1999; Koivisto Hursti, 1999; Rolls etal., 2000). Rozin and colleagues have suggested that in the development of foodselection, one of the things that children must learn between infancy and early schoolage is not only what to eat, but they must also acquire a more or less adult attituderegarding what is and is not food within their culture (Rozin et al., 1986).

The dietary awareness of children is subject to a range of complex interactive forces.The development of food preferences has been shown to be a function of social ratherthan genetic factors (Rozin & Millman, 1987; Rozin, 1990). It has been suggested thatthere is a critical period, ages 2-5 years, for the formation of food preferences (Birch,1980). During these formative years, parents largely influence the food choice, whichalso is shown in the Fieldhouse model in Figure 1. Parents’ beliefs are primarilyinfluenced by culture, dietary awareness, food preferences and socio-economic status.Numerous studies, including observational laboratory studies (Birch et al., 1984; Birchet al., 1987; Klesges et al., 1991) and field surveys (Constanzo & Woody, 1984;Constanzo & Woody, 1985; Patterson et al., 1986; Seagren & Terry, 1991; Koivisto etal., 1994b), indicate that parents shape not only their children’s food preferences, butalso where, with whom, when, and how food is eaten as well as the amount eaten.Restricting access to palatable food is not an effective means of inducing a dislike orreducing intake of such foods (Fisher & Birch, 1999b), contrary to parents' beliefs

Page 12: Pre-school Children’s Food Habits and Meal Situationuu.diva-portal.org/smash/get/diva2:161313/FULLTEXT01.pdf · school, the behaviour of the pre-school staff or the children’s

Background

12

(Casey & Rozin, 1989). Parental control might even negatively affect children’s eatingbehaviour (Fisher & Birch, 1999b), which may lead to a higher risk of adiposity(Johnson & Birch, 1994). Generally, approaches to controlling eating in children arethought to undermine their ability to develop self-control of eating behaviour (Johnson& Birch, 1994; Birch & Ficher, 1998).

Children and new foodTaste and familiarity are two major motives for food choice among children andadolescents. Children do not readily accept, and even tend to be afraid of, new foods.This neophobia is normal in children. Children consume foods they like and arefamiliar with, and many highly preferred foods are energy-dense foods, often high infat or sugar content (Ross, 1995; Koivisto & Sjödén, 1996; Koivisto Hursti, 1999;Koivisto Hursti & Sjödén, 1999). In addition, children have an inborn preference forsweets and aversion to bitter tastes (Rozin et al., 1986), and they seem to developdislikes or bad tastes more easily than likes or good tastes (Koivisto Hursti, 1999).Neophobia is reduced by repeated consumption of new foods that is not followed byany unpleasant experiences (Birch & Marlin, 1982; Pliner, 1982; Sullivan & Birch,1990; Pliner et al., 1993).

Influences on food preferencesAs childhood progresses, parental influence decreases in strength and additional forcesbegin to compete, for example peer pressure (Yperman & Vermeersch, 1979; Birch,1980; Greer et al., 1991; Fergus et al., 1998; Hendy & Raudenbush, 2000) and themedia (American Dietetic Association, 1997). The effectiveness of marketingcampaigns targeted to younger age groups is well known as regards the marketing ofbeer (Grube, 1994) and tobacco (Pierce, 1991). Increasing children’s knowledge ofbrand names, fostering more positive attitudes towards consumption of snacks,encouraging demands that cause parents to purchase advertised foods, and stimulatingdirect sales of advertised foods to children are also a well known phenomena inmarketing, according to a review by Nestle and colleagues (1998). In Sweden, specificlaws regulate advertisement aimed at children (Swedish Statute-book, 1996). In theEU, however, there are no regulations, which means that in the future Sweden mustadopt EU laws, provided that the EU does not come over to the Swedish point of viewon this issue.

Cultural norms regarding body weightConcerns about weight problems, such as obesity and anorexia nervosa, have grown inrecent years, and this focus might influence food habits even among children (Edlund,1997). In Western society, looking good, especially for woman, has become associatedwith a pre-occupation with weight and shape (White, 1992). A slender physique is nowseen not only as a standard of physical attractiveness, but also as a public statementabout health and self-control in a culture of ambiguities and temptations (Lupton,1996). Unrealistic ideals for shape and weight are found everywhere, for example in

Page 13: Pre-school Children’s Food Habits and Meal Situationuu.diva-portal.org/smash/get/diva2:161313/FULLTEXT01.pdf · school, the behaviour of the pre-school staff or the children’s

Background

13

the dolls Barbie and Ken with their very unrealistic body size proportions (Brownell &Napolitino, 1995). Dyrenfoth et al. (1980) found that children rated obese children asless attractive and associated round fleshy bodies with laziness, meanness anddirtiness. In a Swedish study by Edlund et al. (1996), 28% of the 7-year-old girlsstudied stated that they wished to be thinner and 22% that they had made attempts tolose weight. Furthermore, it has been estimated that more than one-fourth of Americanchildren have overweight or eating problems that began before adolescence (Satter,1987; Feldman et al., 1988). Similar results are also found in Sweden (Larsson, 2001).

Methods in assessing food habits There is no clear definition of the term “food habits”. Mead (1943) defined food habitsas “the culturally standardised set of behaviours in regard to food manifested byindividuals who have been reared within a given cultural tradition. These behavioursare seen as systematically interrelated with other standardised behaviours in the sameculture”. Food habits are not defined in the “manual of methodology for foodconsumption” by Cameron and van Staveren (1988). However, food habits couldprobably be the comprehensive and overall term for the defining factors, such as food-related patterns, food patterns, meal patterns and eating patterns. Food is studiedwithin many disciplines. One main line of scholarly activity is centred on the scienceof nutrition. However, food items become meals and meals are essentially socialaffairs (Murcott, 1986). Therefore a broad perspective is needed when food habits arestudied. According to Prättälä (1989), previous research paid less attention to thecultural aspects of food habits, but the influence of psychological or individualapproaches has later come to emphasise Mead's definition. In 1990, Ekström reviewedresearch on food and culture parallel to that on food and health in Sweden (Ekström,1990). None of the research reports mentioned included children. Since 1990, Sjödénand colleagues seem to be the only research group in Sweden taking a socio-culturalapproach to pre-school children’s food habits. In order to study food habits in aninterdisciplinary way, many different methods are necessary, quantitative as well asqualitative. Choice of methods depends on the research question and the objective ofthe study, as well as on making reasonable demands on participants.

Quantitative methodsThere are several internationally established methods used in dietary studies, the mostcommon being described in the textbooks by Cameron & van Staveren and Gibson(1988;1990). The following short presentation of dietary methods is based on thesetextbooks. More precise dietary intake must be assessed by quantitative methods, i.e.to describe what has been eaten either in a retrospective or prospective way.Retrospective dietary assessment methods can be difficult for respondents since theymust memorise and estimate what they have been eating and drinking. Prospectivedietary recording methods, on the other hand, may have the effect of respondents

Page 14: Pre-school Children’s Food Habits and Meal Situationuu.diva-portal.org/smash/get/diva2:161313/FULLTEXT01.pdf · school, the behaviour of the pre-school staff or the children’s

Background

14

changing their food habits to facilitate registration of food intake or to conceal theiractual intake.

The most common retrospective methods for assessing individual intake are dietaryhistory, 24-hour recall and use of a food frequency questionnaire. The interviewmethod for obtaining dietary history has been developed and used with the purpose ofassessing an individual’s total food intake and meal pattern over a certain period oftime, usually the past month, six months or past year. This means that information onmeal composition, meal frequency and food intake is needed as well as thatconcerning usual portion sizes. Twenty-four-hour recall involves remembering, in aninterview situation, one's food intake over the previous 24-hour period. The foodfrequency questionnaire is a list of specific food items on which frequencies of intakesare registered over a given time. The record is obtained through interviews or self-administrated questionnaires. The questionnaires can be semi-quantitative whensubjects are asked to quantify usual portion sizes of food items.

Using the prospective method, study participants record the intake of all beverages andfoods consumed during a certain period of days. The quantities of food are usuallyestimated with the help of household measurements, food models or using pictures offood and food items; alternatively, all foods and beverages can be weighed. Dependingon the number of registration days, the estimation method can be used to assess theactual or usual intakes of individuals. These methods are time-consuming and theburden on the respondent is rather high. The validity of the results depends on theconscientiousness of the respondent and his/her ability to estimate quantities. Therespondent, the caretaker or the assistant must be literate. If all the conditions arefulfilled, using a weighed record is considered an accurate dietary assessment methodfor studying actual intake in groups of healthy people. This is particularly true foryoung children's intake, assessment of which is done by the caretaker and not thechildren themselves (Torun et al., 1996).

Some studies have shown that parental involvement has a marked effect on foodselection; when parents monitored children’s food intake, the number of non-nutritiousfoods was lower (Klesges et al., 1991) or certain foods were under- or over-estimated(Eck et al., 1989). When children have reported their own intake, the accuracy of theestimated dietary intake has depended on their preference for different foods (Lytle etal., 1998; Baxter et al., 1999). Some studies have compared parents' estimation ofchildren’s food intake with the children’s own estimation and a fairly good correlationhas been found, indicating that involving children is appropriate (Sobo & Rock, 2001).However, more research is needed to address possible psychological and social factorsthat might introduce bias into reporting on children's diets (Fisher et al., 2000).

In the case of all dietary assessment methods it is important to validate the results. Themost accurate method for validating energy intake is the double label water technique

Page 15: Pre-school Children’s Food Habits and Meal Situationuu.diva-portal.org/smash/get/diva2:161313/FULLTEXT01.pdf · school, the behaviour of the pre-school staff or the children’s

Background

15

(Torun et al., 1996). However, this method is time-consuming and expensive.Biological markers can also be used as an independent validity test for food or nutrientintake, but the complexities of nutrient metabolism and environmental factors mayaffect the usefulness of these methods (Johansson, 1990). This is also true of usingphysical activity measurements as an independent validity check. A commonly usedand cheap validity check is, however, comparison of the reported energy intake andestimated basal metabolic rate based on weight, height, age and sex.

Qualitative methodsFood habits cannot always be adequately analysed in terms of energy, nutrient andfood intake in different proportions and combinations as studied by quantitativemethods. Other factors of interest are behaviours, perceptions and attitudes as regardsfood. Qualitative methods such as focus group interviews, interviews or observationsmay in this case be appropriate. Focus group interviews involve a group assembled toanswer questions on a specific topic (Krueger, 1994; Morgan & Krueger, 1998).Interviews can be conducted in many ways, structured or unstructured, face-to-face orby telephone. The objective of the study, money and time determine which interviewtechnique will be chosen. Interviews are particularly valuable in meal research to helpresearchers understand and describe the respondent’s underlying attitudes and beliefsabout food (Lupton, 1996). Experiences from interviews with pre-school childrenabout food are reported by Counihan (1999). Observations based on video recordingsallow researchers to document visual perceptions of behaviours as they occur, ratherthan relying on self-reports of behaviour in tests, questionnaires and interviews (Polit& Hungler, 1995). Ethical considerations are of great importance when usingobservational methods, especially when such methods are concealed.

Nutrition policies According to Douglas, food is an encoded social event (Mennell et al., 1992); itexpresses hierarchy, inclusion and exclusion, boundaries and transactions acrossboundaries. In her study, drinks were shared with strangers, acquaintances andworkmen, but meals were shared only with family, close friends and honoured guests.However, today in Sweden a major part of the population eat at least one meal a dayoutside the home, and in the public care sector more than one meal, for example thechildren at pre-school or the elderly at institutions. The meal is no longer only a familymatter, but also a matter of public concern, which is highlighted by the NFA in thenutrition policy (The Swedish National Institute of Public Health & The SwedishNational Food Administration, 2000) and NFA guidelines for pre-school children(Wållberg et al., 1996) as well as for school children (The Swedish National FoodAdministration, 2001).

The focus of Swedish nutrition policy is specified within four areas containing elevennational aims. The four areas are education, support for local and regional nutritionprogrammes, consumer support and participation, mass catering and restaurants. Four

Page 16: Pre-school Children’s Food Habits and Meal Situationuu.diva-portal.org/smash/get/diva2:161313/FULLTEXT01.pdf · school, the behaviour of the pre-school staff or the children’s

Background

16

of the eleven aims are of special interest to food and meal service in public care ingeneral and childcare in particular; these four are:

Aim 1 The development of school settings for food-health-environmental issues should besupported.

Aim 9 Guidelines should be established for nutritional quality and meal organisation inprivate and public mass catering. The guidelines should be made available to therelevant parties in each target group.

Aim 10 The level of knowledge among production and meal staff involved in private and publicmass catering should be increased.

Aim 11 The authorities should have continuous dialogue with representatives from the masscatering sector.

In 1995, the Swedish Government appointed a National Committee in Public Health todevelop national goals for public health work (The Swedish National Committee inPublic Health, 1999). The goals should constitute guidance for the community in itswork to promote public health and prevent ill health. In the strategies and future goalsfor pre-school and pre-school children the following is stated: “Pre-school withmealtime activities. Children's attitudes towards food and various food-stuffs areestablished early in life. It is important that pre-school personnel take advantage of themealtime's educational possibilities.” (p 26). The report emphasises the importance ofepidemiological monitoring of different health risk factors. Few studies about foodhabits and food-related health risks in the Swedish population have been performed.Research on food habits should consequently be performed in a national perspectiveand on an individual level, with complete surveys targeting vulnerable groups. Presentknowledge about energy and nutrient intake, food intake and food habits in Swedentoday is primarily obtained from two national surveys, one from 1989 (The SwedishNational Food Administration, 1989) and a follow-up study from 1997 (The SwedishNational Food Administration, 1997c). In the follow-up study, children are notincluded despite the fact that several governmental departments have emphasised theimportance of children’s food habits in the promotion of public health.

Energy and nutrition recommendations The Swedish Nutrition Recommendations (SNR) (The Swedish National FoodAdministration, 1997d) are mainly based upon the Nordic Nutrition Recommendations(NNR) (The Nordic Nutrition Recommendations, 1996) and Recommended DietaryAllowances (RDA) (Food and Nutrition Board, 1989). According to SNR and NNR,energy and nutrient requirements can be met by eating a variety of foods from differentfood groups. Thus, a balanced diet is the foundation of SNR and NNR. SNR’s mainaim is to support the planning of diets for different groups, mainly healthy individualswith a low or medium level of physical activity. Given this purpose, the recommendedfigures for intake of nutrients are set to cover the requirements of almost allindividuals. For infants and children, the recommended amounts are set to maintain asatisfactory rate of growth and development. In cases of sickness or for groups with

Page 17: Pre-school Children’s Food Habits and Meal Situationuu.diva-portal.org/smash/get/diva2:161313/FULLTEXT01.pdf · school, the behaviour of the pre-school staff or the children’s

Background

17

special needs, composition of the diet must be developed for that group’s specificrequirements. Diet composition varies from meal to meal; thus SNR's daily values aremeant to be means of one week's consumption. The values represent the levels ofessential nutrients and adequate energy that should be consumed on average. Loss ofnutrients due to cooking should therefore be taken in consideration when planningdiets on the bases of recommendations (Bergström, 1998).

When results from dietary studies are to be evaluated, it is appropriate to use lowerlevels of nutrient intakes than used for planning diets. Such data are presented in SNR.There is also a level for lowest acceptable intake. As there is no value representing thelowest acceptable intake or average requirement for children, as there is in the case ofadults, it is difficult to evaluate findings from dietary studies.

The energy figures in SNR, NNR and RDA are reference values and represent theestimated mean requirements for the specific age groups. The resting energyexpenditure (REE) is the largest component of the total energy expenditure, unless thephysical activity level is very high. REE is commonly estimated from severalempirically derived equations. For children less than 10 years of age, the energyrequirement is estimated from intakes associated with normal growth. The secondlargest component of total energy expenditure is the energy expended in physicalactivity. The different types of activity undertaken by an individual can be identifiedand the time spent in each activity measured. Few studies have been performed onyoung children and therefore the given energy reference value does not take differentphysical activity levels into account.

Guidelines for pre-schoolThe NFA has developed a guideline with dietary recommendations for pre-schoolchildren, including recommendations regarding nutritional intake as well ascomposition of pre-school meals (Wållberg et al., 1996). The NFA guideline aims tocover children’s daily needs of energy, some key nutrients and dietary fibre at pre-school. The intention is that all children, independent of age, should be served thesame food and slowly, step by step, learn to eat a variety of foods. In Sweden twodifferent kinds of meals are typical at pre-school. One (breakfast and snack) is basedupon bread, breakfast cereals and milk products, while the other (lunch) is based uponpotatoes/rice/pasta, vegetables/root vegetables and meat/fish/eggs. Lunch is the mealthat should meet most of the iron, zinc and vitamin C requirement, while breakfast andsnack should meet most of the calcium requirement. In this connection, children’smilk intake has been of special interest, both the amount and distribution over differentmeals. Since 1973 there has been a recommendation to limit pre-school children’sintake of milk to half a litre per day (including fermented milk), since this is asufficient amount to cover 75% of the calcium and protein requirements (The SwedishNational Board of Health and Welfare, 1973; 1980; 1990; Expertgruppen, 1990). NFArecommends that milk and milk products should be served at breakfast and at snack

Page 18: Pre-school Children’s Food Habits and Meal Situationuu.diva-portal.org/smash/get/diva2:161313/FULLTEXT01.pdf · school, the behaviour of the pre-school staff or the children’s

Background

18

meals. Consequently, it is recommended that water be served at lunch and dinner,although milk has traditionally been served in most homes and at school lunch.

The NFA guideline on serving water as a meal drink instead of milk has been debated.The argument has focused on three themes: milk as energy source, milk and ironabsorption, and milk versus soft drinks. Milk may be considered a competitor to otherfood items, and if milk intake is high, there is a risk that especially iron and fibreintake will be low and protein intake too high (The Swedish National Board of Healthand Welfare, 1973; 1980; 1990; Wållberg et al., 1996). Scientific opinion as to theassociation between iron status and milk consumption is divided: one side argues thatmilk consumption in a single meal has an effect on iron absorption (Cook et al., 1991;Hallberg et al., 1991; Hallberg et al., 1992; Gleerup et al., 1995; Hallberg, 1998;Hallberg, 2000), the other side argues that, in the long run, it does not have such aneffect, since the body has mechanisms to regulate absorption and is able, within certainlimits, to fill the iron stores if necessary (Tidehag et al., 1995; Reddy & Cook, 1997;Minihane & Fairweather-Tait, 1998; Ames et al., 1999; Lönnerdal, 1999). In Westernsociety, consumption of milk is decreasing, and that of soft drinks and juicesincreasing, which has the effect that less nutrient rich beverages are replacing thenutrient-rich milk (Thomas et al., 1996; Dennison et al., 1997; Hammers, 1999;Harnack et al., 1999; Watt et al., 2000a; Watt et al., 2000b).

To conclude, in Sweden most children are enrolled in the municipal childcare systemowing to the high employment rate for men and woman and political intentions.Consequently, the pre-school and the parents share responsibility for children's dietaryintake as well as their socialisation and acquisition of food habits. There are officialpolicies, recommendations and guidelines about the public meal in pre-school. It isimportant to study, in an interdisciplinary way, how these official documents arefollowed.

Page 19: Pre-school Children’s Food Habits and Meal Situationuu.diva-portal.org/smash/get/diva2:161313/FULLTEXT01.pdf · school, the behaviour of the pre-school staff or the children’s

19

GENERAL AIMS OF THE THESISThe overall objective was to investigate the individual food and nutrient intake of pre-school children at all meals during the day at the pre-school and at home, as well as tostudy factors that may influence children’s intake. One specific aim was to comparefood and nutrient intake at pre-schools where milk was served at lunch with that at pre-schools serving water.

The thesis comprises five reports with the following aims:

I to describe and analyse the energy and nutrient intake of the children and toevaluate the intake in relation to the Swedish Nutrition Recommendations (SNR)and the Swedish National Food Administration guideline, as well as to comparethe nutritional intakes for groups of children with different socio-economicbackgrounds.

II to describe weekday and weekend food patterns, i.e., the frequency of consumptionof food items and the contribution of energy and nutrient intake from differentfood groups, and to explore how foods are related in pre-school children’s diet.

III to compare the respective effects of two serving systems at pre-school (oneoffering milk as lunch beverage, the other offering water) as regards children’sfood and nutrient intake. A further aim was to analyse the energy and nutrientintake of low and high consumers of milk.

IV to identify pre-school children’s perception and experiences of food with focus onwhat they consider to be good, bad and favourite foods as well as their overallexperiences of food in everyday life. One specific aim was to explore theappropriateness of focus group interviews with children aged 3-5 years.

V to identify how pre-school staff experience the role of food and meals as part ofthe pre-school daily activity and if the pedagogic meal was an establishedphenomenon, and in that case, what it meant to the staff.

Page 20: Pre-school Children’s Food Habits and Meal Situationuu.diva-portal.org/smash/get/diva2:161313/FULLTEXT01.pdf · school, the behaviour of the pre-school staff or the children’s

20

SUBJECTS, METHODS AND DISCUSSIONSelection of pre-schoolsThe pre-schools were selected in a suburb of Stockholm with a population of about60,000. The suburb was selected partly because of the availability of a dietician whowas both responsible for menus and supported the cooks as well as the principals ofthe pre-schools regarding food issues. Thus, all pre-schools had the same opportunitiesfor support and information to provide an optimal diet, but it was the principal whodecided whether they would follow the diet service menu, which used the NFAguideline (Wållberg et al., 1996) and SNR (The Swedish National FoodAdministration, 1997d) for the planning of the menus.

The director of the municipal childcare programme gave formal permission to engagepre-schools in her area, informing the principals of this. To be included, severalcriteria had to be fulfilled by the selected pre-schools. Firstly, they had to prepare themeals in their own kitchen. Secondly, three meals per day were to be served, i.e.breakfast, lunch and afternoon snack. Twenty-nine of the 36 pre-schools fulfilled thecriteria. Thirdly, all 29 pre-schools also fulfilled the criterion of having “full-timechildren” (normally present at the day-care centre at least 35 hours per week) aged 3-5years. Among these 29 schools, a random selection was made of six that offered water,and another six milk, as lunchtime drink. The personnel at all 12 schools accepted theselection of their school and were willing to take part in the investigation.

The author (HS) was responsible for all data collection in Studies I -V. The titles of thepapers, methods used, design of the studies and subjects included are presented inTable 1.

Table 1. Data collection methods, design of study and subjects participating in Studies I-V. All studies were conducted on the same population, included 12 pre-schools.Study Title Method Design SubjectsI Children's nutrient intake at pre-school

and at homeDietary record Quantitative survey:

Descriptive109 children

II The contribution of food groups to thenutrient intake and food pattern amongpre-school children

Dietary record Quantitative survey:Descriptive,comparative andexplorative

109 children

III The role of milk in Swedish pre-schoolchildren's diet

Dietary record Quantitative survey:Descriptive andcomparative

87 children

IV Swedish pre-school children's experienceof food

Focus groupinterviews

Qualitative:Descriptive andexplorative

103 children

V Pre-school staffs’ attitudes toward foodsin relation to the pedagogic meal

Semi-structuredinterviews

Qualitative:Descriptive andexplorative

18 (34) pre-school staff

Page 21: Pre-school Children’s Food Habits and Meal Situationuu.diva-portal.org/smash/get/diva2:161313/FULLTEXT01.pdf · school, the behaviour of the pre-school staff or the children’s

Subjects, Methods and Discussion

21

Studies I, II and IIIA quantitative approach, using an estimated and weighed dietary record, was used todescribe, compare and explore the dietary intake among pre-school children. The pre-school staff were asked to select a department with a maximum of 14 non lactoseintolerant/milk allergic children 3-5 years of age, i.e. 36-71 months, with parents whowere able to read and write Swedish. All parents were given oral and writteninformation in person at the school so that they could give informed consent for theirchild to participate in the study. In all 136 children and parents were invited to takepart in the study and 131 of these participated. There were complete food registrationsduring 7 days for 109 children. In Study III, 44 children were excluded according tocertain criteria.

The participating children spent, on average, 7.7 hours at pre-school per day. Theparents’ occupations were divided into three categories according to a scheme used byStatistics Sweden (1995): blue-collar, lower white-collar and middle/upper white-collar. Twenty-three percent of mothers and 23% of fathers were blue-collar, 26% ofmothers and 14% of fathers were lower white-collar, and 51% of mothers and 63% offathers were middle/upper white-collar. Fourteen families were one-parent households.No data were collected on the parents’ education, as this parameter is a basis for theoccupation categories mentioned above.

Measuring and recording food intakesEach pre-school participated in the study for three weeks. The first week the parentswere informed and asked to give their consent, and the research staff adjusted to thesituation, which meant getting to know the children, staff and routines at the pre-school. During the second week, the children got accustomed to eating according tothe new routines and serving procedures, which were to be used during the third week.Every child had its own tray with different pots for every component in the meal. Allfoods and drinks at pre-school were weighed before and after the meals on a standardelectronic scale and recorded individually during a 5-day period (Monday-Friday).

According to validation studies, there is a better agreement between energy intake (EI)and weighed dietary record in young children as compared with adults (Livingstone etal., 1992; Torun et al., 1996; Sichert-Hellert et al., 1998). According to Torun et al.,this could be because, for younger children, parents and other adults have overallcontrol of food intake and responsibility for dietary reporting. Younger children alsohave less unsupervised access to food. In the case of our study, we believe that theweighed dietary record is valid owing to the fact that the week before the actualrecording the children had the opportunity to adapt to new serving and mealprocedures. According to the pre-school staff, the children were adjusted to thesituation after 3 days and ate and drank the same amounts as they normally did. Thepre-school staff reported that, during the week of registration, the children behaved asif this were the normal meal situation.

Page 22: Pre-school Children’s Food Habits and Meal Situationuu.diva-portal.org/smash/get/diva2:161313/FULLTEXT01.pdf · school, the behaviour of the pre-school staff or the children’s

Subjects, Methods and Discussion

22

Parallel with recording at the pre-school, the parents recorded foods eaten at homeduring the 7-day period (Monday-Sunday). All parents were given oral and writteninformation in person at the school as how to record the food intake. They also haddaily opportunity during the three-week period to put questions to the research staff.At home the portion sizes were estimated by household measures or with the aid of abooklet, the Meal Model, with illustrations and photographs of typical Swedish dishesand food items (The Swedish National Food Administration, 1997a). When tested onadults at the group level, the Meal Model proved to be an acceptable tool for assessingportion sizes (Håglin et al., 1995). The Meal Model has not been validated specificallyfor use when estimating children’s intake. However, in this study, adults wereestimating the children’s food portions by comparing these to the pictures in thebooklet. Assessing food intake through estimated measures is associated withunderestimation. A mentioned above, Klesges et al. have shown that parentalinfluences have a marked effect on food selection when parents monitor children’sfood intake (Klesges et al., 1991). However, in our study the estimated records areconsidered valid owing to the fact that there was no significant difference in energyintake between weekdays and weekends, although the consumption pattern wasdifferent. The low-nutrient foods contributed more energy on weekend days than onweekdays, which indicates that parents were careful when monitoring their children’sfood intake. According to previous studies (Birch et al., 1991; Nicklas et al., 1997),although children’s food consumption is highly varied from meal to meal, the dailyenergy intake is relatively constant. Since all parents were promised a personal reportof the energy and nutrient intake of their child, we believe they were motivated to beextra careful when estimating food intake.

An important factor is the participants’ confidence: children, parents and pre-schoolstaff felt comfortable with the project staff and free to ask questions about the studybefore deciding on participation. To avoid jealousy between participating and non-participating children, all children were treated equally with regard to the mealsituation, i.e. all children at the same department had their own tray with their own potsirrespective of whether they were included in the study. The fact that there were fewdropouts may have resulted from a combination of the above factors.

The children also had their weight measured, undressed, to the nearest 0.1 kg. Heightwas measured to the nearest 5 mm. Basal Metabolic Rate (BMR) was calculated takinginto account age, sex, body weight and height according to Schofield’s equation(World Health Organization, 1985). The age of the children varied from 36 to 71months, with a mean of 56.6 months. Their weights (mean 18.9 kg), and heights (mean1.08 m), were all plotted and were within ± 3 standard deviations of the weight-for-agecurves of Swedish children (Niklasson & Karlberg, 1999), with the exception of onechild who was overweight. The mean of predicted BMR was 3.8 MJ/day. There were

Page 23: Pre-school Children’s Food Habits and Meal Situationuu.diva-portal.org/smash/get/diva2:161313/FULLTEXT01.pdf · school, the behaviour of the pre-school staff or the children’s

Subjects, Methods and Discussion

23

no significant differences between the included and excluded children as regardsanthropometrical measurements (Study I).

Validation principlesTwo dietary assessment methods were used, the direct measuring (weighing) by theresearch team of the children’s intake at the pre-school and, for practical reasons, theparents' estimated recording at home. The energy intake as reported by each of the twomethods for every child has been analysed. To check for underreporting, the ratio ofreported energy intake (EI) and estimated BMR was used, as described by Goldberg etal. (1991). When physical activity level (PAL) is not measured, which is the case inmost in dietary studies, EI/BMR is used as an estimation of PAL and a guide forvalidating the dietary assessment (Torun et al., 1996). A cut-off value of 1.2 ofEI/BMR was used for validating energy intake at the individual level, as suggested byGoldberg and Black (1998). None of the individual 7-day EI/BMR means were belowthe cut-off figure 1.2. However, this cut-off value is designed for adults. Childrengenerally have a higher PAL than adults and therefore the validation cut-off shouldprobably be raised. In our study, the mean value for EI/BMR was 1.7 and the EI/BMRvalue for seven days ranged from 1.27 to 2.25. Thus it appears the data are reliable.However, the variation of energy intake during weekends was great for two reasons.Food habits during Saturdays and Sundays are more flexible owing to more flexibleactivities on such days, and the mean represents only two days. For some children, themeans of the weekend energy intake were very low as compared with the estimatedenergy requirements of these children. However, the low figures for some childrenwere not primarily due to underestimation of their intake. The principal author (HS)was responsible for the data collection and became well acquainted with children andtheir parents. Against the background of the validation studies on children mentionedabove and the detailed knowledge of the families, no children were excluded becauseof a low EI/BMR value.

Data processing, statistics and presentationThere is no sex difference in SNR for children except for energy reference values, thusthe data were presented as one group, in absolute amounts (MJ and g) and relativeamounts as energy percentage (E%) and nutrient density (g/MJ), separate for home,pre-school, weekdays and weekend. The individual energy intake was compared withSNR (The Swedish National Food Administration, 1997d) and NNR (The NordicNutrition Recommendations, 1996). Since SNR does not have recommendations forevaluating dietary assessment in children, the levels used were those for planningmeals for children aged 4-6; furthermore, there is no protein recommendation forchildren, which is why the RDA (Food and Nutrition Board, 1989) was used.

The food intake data were processed by the computer program MATs the flexible(Nordin, 1997) including the NFA food database (The Swedish National FoodAdministration, 1997b). Recipes from the pre-schools and some new products were

Page 24: Pre-school Children’s Food Habits and Meal Situationuu.diva-portal.org/smash/get/diva2:161313/FULLTEXT01.pdf · school, the behaviour of the pre-school staff or the children’s

Subjects, Methods and Discussion

24

added to the database. The nutrients presented in the results are the same as the keynutrients in the NFA guideline. The statistical software Minitab (MINITAB, 1998) wasused for data analysis. T-tests were used to analyse differences between groups, and at-test of the mean was used to compare the recorded energy intake with recommendedintake. One-way ANOVA was used to compare socio-economic groups (three groups)and low and high milk consumers (four groups). In Study III, the data are presentedand divided according to the two different serving systems, six offering milk and water(M), and another six offering water (W) as beverage at lunch. In the results anddiscussion, the children are referred to as M- or W-children. A regression analysis wasused to compare the two different groups, age was used as a covariate. PrincipalComponent Analyses (PCA) were performed on 16 aggregated food groups (Figure 4)using the software The Unscrambler (CAMO, 1986-1999). All variables were scaled tounit variance before conducting the PCA (Study II). As older children might eat morefood and in different proportions than do younger children, this could influence thestatistical analysis of food pattern, thus statistical analyses were based upon nutrientdensity (g/MJ) in Study II. Results are presented as mean, standard deviation and p-values, and the differences were considered significant at p<0.05. As a large numberof statistical analyses were performed, some of the significant differences found mightbe caused by erroneously rejecting a true null hypothesis (type I error) and some of thenon significant results might be caused by not rejecting a false hypothesis (type IIerror).

The relationship between the great number of dietary parameters generable fromdietary studies – such as nutrient density, frequency and amounts of consumption ofindividual foods, meal patterns and background factors – can be difficult to describeand analyse in an economical way using traditional statistics. PCA was considered anappropriate multivariate method. PCA is a data reduction method whereby correlatedvariables can be grouped together, identifying underlying dimensions of the data. Anumber of studies on diet and eating patterns illustrate the value of PCA as a tool foridentifying eating habits (Barker et al., 1990; Whichelow & Prevost, 1996; North &Emmett, 2000). If these structures support emerging knowledge, there are reasons tobelieve that the described phenomena are of a reproducible nature. During cross-validation of the model, any deviant structure in these subsets would be detected and/orremoved. No such anomaly was detected in the present data. There are thereforereasons to believe that repeated studies would not add to the reproducibility of the data.It is interesting to note that all patterns shown were identical for both weekdays andweekend days; however, the patterns for the weekdays were slightly stronger. Shiftsaway from certain foods often enhance the consumption or combination of others.Visual representation is therefore necessary in interpreting the multidimensionalpicture. Traditional statistics more often treat variables one at a time, which does notfavour complex interpretations of changes in eating behaviours.

Page 25: Pre-school Children’s Food Habits and Meal Situationuu.diva-portal.org/smash/get/diva2:161313/FULLTEXT01.pdf · school, the behaviour of the pre-school staff or the children’s

Subjects, Methods and Discussion

25

Study IVA qualitative approach – focus group interviews – was used to explore pre-schoolchildren's attitudes, perception and experience with regard to food. Twenty-two focusgroup interviews were conducted during pre-school hours, with the permission of theparents, and held in rooms at the 12 pre-schools.

A total of 131 children were willing to participate in the project. Randomised samplingis not used in qualitative studies, but rather selective sampling, i.e. informants areselected who are willing to talk about specific topics or verbal enough to expressthemselves. After excluding the shy, non-informative children (on staffrecommendation), children at home with a cold, and children occupied with otheractivities at pre-school, we were left with 103 (56 boys and 47 girls) childrenparticipating in the focus groups. The moderator was trained in focus groupmethodology, as described by Morgan and Krueger (Krueger, 1994; Morgan &Krueger, 1998). An interview guide was developed containing questions about themeal situation, food attitudes and food perceptions and experiences. For the youngestchildren, a food collage was used to help the conversation. The food collage containedall kinds of different foods from all food groups and different types of dishes,vegetables, fruits, snacks, soft drinks, coffee, ice cream and so on. The interviewsranged from approximately 20 to 35 minutes in length, and were observed anddocumented by the assistant moderator (HS). The average size of the groups was fourparticipants. All interviews were videotaped. After each focus group, the moderatorand assistant moderator reviewed the notes, making general frequency notes, i.e. fieldnotes about the most frequent categories or phenomena that occurred during theinterviews. A systematic independent coding and categorising of the material by themoderator and assistant moderator – several times by viewing and carefully listening tothe video recordings – followed the preliminary analysis. The moderator and assistantmoderator discussed similarities and differences in their respective coding. Mainthemes and patterns that occurred in most focus groups, or that were deemed importantto a significant number of children, were identified and organised into three mainthemes. Using a form with a column for each preliminary theme that occurred, thecoded interview material was divided into these components, allowing the content tobe systematically verified. The three main themes were consistent throughout the work.

The method used in this study, focus group interviews, was a successful way to learnabout how children think about and jointly reflect upon food. This is also seen by otherscholars (Østberg & Thorsen, 1981). We seem to have had more success with thismethod than indicated by Counihans' (1999) experiences of interviewing pre-schoolchildren individually with non-specific abstract questions. This may be explained bythe fact that 1) all the children already knew one another, 2) the assistant moderatorwas well known to the children, 3) a food collage with pictures was used during thefocus group interview, and finally, 4) the children were inspired by one another in thegroup discussion. The reason why 22 focus groups were conducted was that the twelve

Page 26: Pre-school Children’s Food Habits and Meal Situationuu.diva-portal.org/smash/get/diva2:161313/FULLTEXT01.pdf · school, the behaviour of the pre-school staff or the children’s

Subjects, Methods and Discussion

26

pre-schools were participating in a larger study and all had been informed that focusgroup interviews were going to take place. Most focus group studies consist of three tosix group interviews and it was not necessary to conduct such a large number.However, this was a very special opportunity to conduct interviews with more than ahandful of groups. It was valuable for us from a practical point of view, as we got abroad experience how to conduct focus groups with small children. The children whotook part in this study found it fun and exciting to be interviewed, and we did notbelieve that they gave answers just to satisfy us. However, it seems to be veryimportant that the focus groups take place in a special room where the children areable to concentrate and not see their friends playing. Furthermore, when conductingfocus groups with children, it is vital to gain their confidence through knowing theirenvironment. The children were already used to the video camera, mainly because ithad been used in the dietary study, but also because the teachers sometimes used thecamera in teaching. Therefore the video recording did not affect the children’sspontaneity, which it often does in encounters with adults (Krueger, 1994).

Four children seemed to be a manageable group size, which is also found by Østberg(1981). The reason why all children spoke easily about the topic is probably becausefood consumption is a concrete experience to reflect upon. Even so, the childrensometimes talked about things that did not have any relevance to the study, asCounihan (1999) also expressed and group moderators must therefore be aware thatthere are great variations within the same age group.

The food collage was brought to all the interviews. In half of the interviews we did notuse it since the children were very talkative without it. However, when the childrenwere a little tense we began to show the collage and when they were comfortable in theinterview situation we took it away. Since the food collage covered all sorts of foods ofdifferent brands, such as soft drinks, confectionery, ice cream as well as potatoes, milk,fruits, vegetables and ready prepared dishes, for example fish fingers, boiled cod andhamburgers, it is assumed that the collage did not lead to bias.

The participating children had varying abilities of self-expression. The most talkativeand articulate were, of course, easier to conduct a focus group interview with, but weobserved that the shyer children, though less talkative, did express the same ideas.Therefore we have no reason to believe that staff selection of non-participatingchildren has led to bias, mostly because they were less than a dozen children out of131, but also because the results were so homogenous among the participatingchildren. Moreover, we have no reason to believe that the non-participating childrendiffer from the others in any respect other than shyness and fear of strangers, since theassistant moderator already knew the children.

Page 27: Pre-school Children’s Food Habits and Meal Situationuu.diva-portal.org/smash/get/diva2:161313/FULLTEXT01.pdf · school, the behaviour of the pre-school staff or the children’s

Subjects, Methods and Discussion

27

Study VThe design of this study was explorative, and the aim was to study how thephenomenon of the pedagogic meal was expressed and applied within the pre-school.In this specific part of the study, a qualitative approach, with semi-structuredinterviews, was chosen. Thirty-four interviews with pre-school staff were conductedduring pre-school hours and held in 12 different pre-schools.

The staff from three groups (11 principals, 15 pre-school teachers and 8 child minders)was recruited through staff participating in the dietary study. They were mostly womenwho had worked in the municipal care system for 12-36 years. Only one informant wasa man and he had worked for 2 years. His responses did not differ from those of therest of the groups, thus he was not treated separately in the analyses.

The interviewer was trained in interview technique, as described by Kvale (1997). Aninterview guide was developed to cover questions about the meal situation at the pre-school, intentions and aims of the pre-school meal, food attitudes and food perceptionsand experiences in relation to their work at the pre-schools. Each staff member wasinterviewed on one occasion, the interviews lasting from 45 minutes up to one houreach. The interviews were tape-recorded and transcribed verbatim.

The preliminary analysis began during the fieldwork, during the interviews asdescribed by Kvale (1997). After the fieldwork period, all transcripts were analysed ina first step by the authors, discussing preliminary findings and themes. Since we foundthat there were similar beliefs and attitudes among the whole group of informants, itwas decided to further analyse, on a deeper level, a smaller sample. From the 34interviews, six interviews from each group of staff members were selected by drawingof lots, leaving a total of 18 interviews in the final analyses. This selection method isnot a practice associated with qualitative analyses, though in this case it was the easiestway to decide which of the interviews to use. The transcripts were then furthersystematically analysed by the interviewer, who used the interviews to confirmpreliminary findings or to break them down and re-analyse them to discover newcoherence (Kvale, 1997). Using one form for each theme, the interview material wasdivided into these components, allowing the contents to be systematically verified. Thecontent of each component was compared between the three groups to check forsimilarities and differences. The main findings were consistent throughout theanalyses, but also deepened.

All informants in this study were involved in the dietary study and were alreadyfamiliar with the interviewer in the pre-school environment. The interviews took placeat the pre-schools, which was a familiar place for both the informants and theinterviewer. This probably had a positive influence when starting up and conductingthe interviews. The informants gave the impression that they were comfortable andable to speak freely. There was no problem recording the interviews, which was

Page 28: Pre-school Children’s Food Habits and Meal Situationuu.diva-portal.org/smash/get/diva2:161313/FULLTEXT01.pdf · school, the behaviour of the pre-school staff or the children’s

Subjects, Methods and Discussion

28

probably due to the fact that they were used to being video recorded during the mealsituation with the children. One negative factor might be that informants knew that theinterviewer’s special interest was pre-school children’s food habits. The responsesmight have been different if another interviewer from another discipline hadinterviewed the informants or if the interviews had been conducted with another groupthat had not be exposed to dietary studies.

All informants, with the exception of the male one, had worked in the municipal caresystem for a long period of time (>12 years). The informant’s age was not asked, butthe range was approximately between 30-60 years. It would have been interesting tolook at possible age differences between the generations, since the majority of the staffspoke about how things used to be.

In additionAt every pre-school, some meals during the three-week period were video recorded.To accustom the children and staff to the video camera standing in front of their dinnertable, recordings were made on two separate days, three meals per day (breakfast,lunch, snack). The aim was to observe social exchange and the role of the pre-schoolteacher during the pre-school meals. The results from this study have been publishedas a bachelor's thesis at the Department of Domestic Sciences1. The results will bementioned briefly in the discussion.

All children participating in the dietary study received an individual written report oftheir energy and nutrient intake on weekdays and weekend days. They also got a lot(€3). All participating pre-schools received a financial contribution (€1000), withwhich they could buy toys or make an excursion, and all children at the participatingdepartment received a small present (a rubber ball).

The Ethical Committee of the Faculty of Medicine, Uppsala University, approved theproject protocol.

1 S Lind, T Kaarlejärvi, Bachelor thesis, Department of Domestic Sciences 2000

Page 29: Pre-school Children’s Food Habits and Meal Situationuu.diva-portal.org/smash/get/diva2:161313/FULLTEXT01.pdf · school, the behaviour of the pre-school staff or the children’s

29

RESULTS AND DISCUSSIONDietary intakeSocio-economic backgroundSocio-economic differences in the diet of children and adolescents have been shown insome Swedish and Finnish studies (Hagman et al., 1986; Laitinen et al., 1995;Höglund et al., 1998). In this study, there were no significant differences between thechildren’s energy and nutrient intake attributable to the parents’ occupations (Study I).Nor were there any differences in children’s food intake as a function of parentaloccupation (Study II). The children were spending, on average, almost 8 hours a day atpre-school, i.e., the pre-school was responsible for the major part of the intake duringweekdays, which might obscure possible effects of socio-economic background. Energy The reported mean energy intakes were 6.2 and 6.7 MJ/day for girls and boys,respectively, the same as or slightly lower than levels found in previous Swedish andFinnish studies of children aged 4-6 years (Samuelson, 1971; Räsänen et al., 1985;Hagman et al., 1986; Jacobson & Nordlund, 1994; Koivisto et al., 1994b; Lagström etal., 1999). It is problematic to compare the present results with SNR as a reference,because the SNR age interval does not correspond to the interval of 36-71 months inthis study. The mean reference values for energy intake in NNR and SNR for childrenaged 4-6 years are 6.8 and 7.1 MJ/day for girls and boys, respectively. The mean of theindividual energy requirement calculated according to the NNR as energy per kg bodyweight of the children in our study was 6.5 MJ/day for girls and 6.4 MJ/day for boys,this on the basis of the age intervals 37-48, 49-60, 61-72 months. According to RDA,the mean energy requirement is 6.1 MJ for children aged 4-6 years old, the same valuefor both girls and boys in this age group. According to these comparisons, the reportedenergy intake seems acceptable (Study I). However, the estimations of the energyrequirement in RDA and NNR are based upon data collected 20 or more years ago andpublished in a WHO report from 1985 (World Health Organization, 1985). NNR (TheNordic Nutrition Recommendations, 1996) discusses the apparent tendency thatphysical activity among young children is decreasing and therefore the referencevalues might be to high. The rise in obesity has also come to be seen as a problem oflow energy expenditure, and the result of a sedentary lifestyle (Gibson, 1997). Childrentoday are more likely to be driven to school than walk, and spend more time watchingtelevision than in previous generations (Prentice & Jebb, 1995). One of the factorsfound to lead to obesity in 3-year-old Japanese children was limited playtime outdoors(1 hour or less) (Takahashi et al., 1999).

However, the energy intake at the pre-schools in the present study fell short of theNFA guideline, i.e. 65% from three meals and 45% from two. Only one fourth of thechildren who had their meals at pre-school reached the 65% level, and one third ofthose who had two meals per day reached the 45% level. Thus the temporaldistribution of energy and nutrients during the day was skewed for most of the

Page 30: Pre-school Children’s Food Habits and Meal Situationuu.diva-portal.org/smash/get/diva2:161313/FULLTEXT01.pdf · school, the behaviour of the pre-school staff or the children’s

Results and Discussion

30

children. Previous Swedish studies (Öhlin et al., 1993; Jacobson & Nordlund, 1994;Hägg et al., 1998) show the same results: as compared with the NFA guideline,children had inadequate energy and nutrient intake during school meals, but this wascompensated for by the intake of food at home. It is important to consider, however,whether the recommendations – to cover 65% of the daily intake at pre-school – areadequate and realistic for children who are away from home more than 8 hours a day.One proposal might be that the major part of children’s intake should occur during theactive part of day. However, the scientific basis for such a suggestion must beestablished in future research. (Study I)

The main sources of energy were milk and cheese (17% of the total energy), meatproducts (17%) and bread and breakfast cereals (15%) (Study II). There were nosignificant differences between the children’s nutrient and energy intake that wereattributable to the serving system, i.e. water or milk as lunchtime beverage at the pre-school. The average intake of milk and flavoured milk at pre-school was about 1.75 dlfor both M- and W-children. The W-children consumed, on average, larger amounts ofmilk at breakfast and afternoon snack than did the M-children. When the children weredivided into four groups according to milk consumption, it was notable that the lowmilk consumers had a more even distribution of energy intake from different foodgroups than did the high milk consumers (Study III). For the purpose of the presentstudy, the foods were categorised as “high-nutrient foods” and “low-nutrient foods”.Vegetables, fruits, juice, potatoes and root vegetables, milk and cheese products,cereal products and fats were classified as “high-nutrient foods”, and confectionery,soft drinks, etc., as “low-nutrient foods”. The basis for distinguishing between “high-nutrient foods” and “low-nutrient foods” was a traditional Swedish food model, “thefood circle”. The food groups categorised here as “high-nutrient foods” were includedin the food circle, whereas the “low-nutrient foods” were not. In total, the “high-nutrient foods” contributed relatively more nutrients and less energy on weekdays(80%) than at weekends (68%) (Study II).

The respective mean intakes of protein, fat, carbohydrates and sucrose, expressed as apercentage of the total energy intake, were at pre-school 14, 38, 50 and 9, and at home14, 36, 52 and 12 weekdays, 14, 34, 55 and 16 weekend days, respectively (Table 2).There were no differences between the amounts of protein and fat consumed duringweekends as compared with weekdays, though there was a difference in energypercentage distribution. There was a high carbohydrate intake during the weekends, i.e.high intake of sucrose. The increased sugar intake may be due to either different mealhabits during weekends as compared with weekdays or to the consumption of“Saturday sweets”, an established habit among Swedish children. In most Westerncountries, the increasing risk for cardiovascular disease has led to a major public healthcampaign to reduce fat consumption (Keys et al., 1984). Compared to the SNR the fatcontent of pre-school children’s diet in this study was to high. However, according to areview by Michaelsen and Jorgensen (1995) there is no evidence to suggest any

Page 31: Pre-school Children’s Food Habits and Meal Situationuu.diva-portal.org/smash/get/diva2:161313/FULLTEXT01.pdf · school, the behaviour of the pre-school staff or the children’s

Results and Discussion

31

advantages associated with a diet including less than 30% of energy from fat forchildren and adolescents.

Table 2. Mean and standard deviation of energy (MJ) and macro nutrient (amount, g; energypercentage, E%) intakes of pre-school children (n=109); weekdays and weekend days, compared withSNR. Paired t-test p-values. Weekdays Weekend

Total Home p1 SNREnergy, MJ 6.4 ± 1.2 6.5 ± 1.5 ns 6.8a 7.1b

EI/BMR 1.7 ± 0.2 1.7 ± 0.4 nsProtein, g 53.1 ± 11.5 51.0 ± 13.6 nsProtein, E% 14 ± 1.7 14 ± 2.6 * 10-15Fat, g 60.8 ± 12.0 57.3 ± 16.7 *Fat, E% 37 ± 3.8 34 ± 5.7 *** <30Carbohydrate, g 193.3 ± 40.7 208.8 ± 54.7 **Carbohydrate, E% 51 ± 4.3 55 ± 6.7 *** 55-60Sucrose, g 41.1 ± 15.5 61.3 ± 29.1 ***Sucrose, E% 11 ± 3.4 16 ± 6.2 *** <10a girls b boys1 ***=p<0.001, **=p<0.01, *=p<0.05, ns= not significant

Minerals, vitamins, sucrose and dietary fibreTo eliminate the differences caused by age variation, the results were recalculated intonutrient density, despite the fact that SNR and NNR do not have any nutrient densityrecommendations for young children, but only for children >7 years of age. However,it is only vitamin D density that is higher for children 1-6 years when calculating thedensity from the energy reference values and the recommended intakes for nutrients.The mean total intake per day and nutrient density were in line with SNR, with theexception of selenium and dietary fibre (Table 3 and Figure 2). This has also beenfound in previous Swedish studies on adolescents (Bergström et al., 1993; Samuelsonet al., 1996; Samuelson, 2000). The recommended intake of selenium for children is 25mg in SNR and NNR, as compared to the 21 mg found in this study. The recommendedlevels in SNR and NNR are set to cover the requirement of almost all individuals. Asthere is no value representing the lowest acceptable intake or average requirement forchildren, it is difficult to evaluate the result. However, the average requirement foradults is about 25% less than the recommended intake, and the level representing thelowest acceptable intake for adults is about 50% below the recommended level inNNR. When the recommended values for children are reduced by 25% and 50%, theyamount to 18.7 mg and 12.5 mg, respectively. Twenty-eight percent of the childrenreached the recommended intake of 25 mg, and 62% reached 18.7 mg. Three of 109children had an intake below 12 mg. It should be noted that selenium values from fooddatabases are specifically uncertain due to the variation in selenium contents in the soilin different areas.

Page 32: Pre-school Children’s Food Habits and Meal Situationuu.diva-portal.org/smash/get/diva2:161313/FULLTEXT01.pdf · school, the behaviour of the pre-school staff or the children’s

Results and Discussion

32

SNR recommends 3 g/MJ of dietary fibre for adults and children. However, in NNRthe authors suggest that 2 g/MJ is more reasonable for pre-schoolers, since otherwisethe food will be too voluminous. Eighty-three children had fibre intakes below 2 g/MJand none had intakes exceeding 3 g/MJ (Study I). The low intake of dietary fibre wascaused by a low intake of cereal products, vegetables and fruits (Study II). Dwyer(1995) suggests that “age plus 5 g” is a reasonable recommendation for dietary fibreintakes for children older than 3 years of age, which is almost in line with the results inStudy I (11.0±2.5 g).

Table 3. Mean and standard deviation for mineral, vitamin and dietary fibre daily intakes of pre-school children (n=109); weekdays and weekend days, compared with SNR recommendations. Pairedt-test p-values. Weekdays Weekend

days

p1 SNRCalcium, mg 831 ± 229 768 ± 288 ** 600Iron, mg 8.1 ± 3.0 7.7 ± 3.2 ns 8Zinc, mg 7.2 ± 1.6 6.8 ± 2.2 ns 6Selenium, µg 22.1 ± 7.0 19.0 ± 7.2 *** 25Retinol equivalents, µg 841 ± 348 674 ± 484 ** 500Riboflavin, mg 1.3 ± 0.3 1.2 ± 0.4 ** 1Vitamin C, mg 76 ± 32 58 ± 35 *** 45Dietary fibre, g 11.5± 2.8 9.8 ± 3.7 *** a

a SNR no recommendation1 ***=p<0.001, **=p<0.01, *=p<0.05, ns= not significant

Page 33: Pre-school Children’s Food Habits and Meal Situationuu.diva-portal.org/smash/get/diva2:161313/FULLTEXT01.pdf · school, the behaviour of the pre-school staff or the children’s

Results and Discussion

33

Figure 2. Histogram with normal distribution curve of mineral, vitamin and dietary fibre daily intakesof pre-school children (n=109) during a seven-day period.

1,0 1,5 2,0

0

5

10

15

Riboflavin, mg

Freq

uenc

y

Histogram of Riboflavin, mg, with Normal Curve

0 100 200

0

10

20

Vitamin C, mg

Freq

uenc

y

Histogram of Vitamin C, mg, with Normal Curve

5 10 15 20

0

10

20

Dietary fibre, g

Freq

uenc

y

Histogram of Dietary fibre, g, with Normal Curve

10 20 30 40

0

10

20

Selenium, µg

Freq

uenc

y

Histogram of Selenium, µg, with Normal Curve

0 1000 2000

0

10

20

Retinol eq, µg

Freq

uenc

y

Histogram of Retinol eq, µg, with Normal Curve

3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12

0

10

20

Zinc, µg

Freq

uenc

y

Histogram of Zinc, µg, with Normal Curve

5 10 15

0

10

20

Iron, mg

Freq

uenc

y

Histogram of Iron, mg, with Normal Curve

400 500 600 700 800 900 1000 1100 1200 1300 1400

0

10

20

Calcium, mg

Freq

uenc

y

Histogram of Calcium, mg, with Normal Curve

Page 34: Pre-school Children’s Food Habits and Meal Situationuu.diva-portal.org/smash/get/diva2:161313/FULLTEXT01.pdf · school, the behaviour of the pre-school staff or the children’s

Results and Discussion

34

Our study, along with a few others, has looked at the “low-nutrient foods” (Leung etal., 1984; Campbell, 1993). These foods have often been viewed as non-nutritious and,therefore, unimportant. The “low-nutrient foods” in our study accounted for one thirdof the energy intake during the weekend days and approximately one fifth of the intakeof dietary fibre, calcium and vitamin C. Contrary to common assumptions, these foodscontributed more than sugar and fat. Our findings (Study II) are similar to those ofLeaman et al. (1997) as well as Hagman (1986).

In Western societies, there is great concern about the effects of carbonated beveragesin terms of obesity, tooth decay, osteoporosis and other health problems (Harnack etal., 1999; Watt et al., 2000a; Watt et al., 2000b; Wyshak, 2000). In the SNR, it isrecommended to limit the intake of refined sugars to achieve a sufficient intake ofnutrients and dietary fibre. In our study, the children drank on average more soft drinksand less milk during the weekend days than during the weekdays. There was atendency for low milk consumers to receive a greater part of their energy from softdrinks than was the case for high milk consumers. Several studies have shown thatbeverage choice can have a significant effect on the nutrient adequacy of the diets ofchildren and adolescents (Subar et al., 1998; Harnack et al., 1999; Ballew et al., 2000).In a British study, 72.5% of the pre-school children never drank plain water. Squashwas the most frequently consumed drink, and 15% consumed almost 50% of therecommended daily energy intakes from drinks (Petter et al., 1995). The consumptionof soft drinks in Sweden has increased rapidly (Swedbrewers, 1984-2000), while themilk consumption has decreased (Swedish Dairy Association, 1950-2000).

Dental experts have recommended that sugary foods and drinks be limited to mealtimes and that drinks such as milk and water be consumed between meals (Holm,1990). In today's Sweden, most parents are well informed and know that sweets arebad for teeth, but this does not prevent them from giving sweets to their children(Holm, 1990). Study II shows that the main sources of sucrose were soft drinks anddesserts on weekdays, and soft drinks and confectionery at weekends. It seems asthough consumption of “Saturday sweets” is still an established habit among Swedishchildren, whereas soft drinks have become an everyday beverage.

Wilson and co-workers studied types of milk beverages served and found that thesehad no significant effect on consumption of other food items offered at the meal. Herconclusion is that children do not reduce the intake of other food items at a meal tocompensate for the increased energy intake from sucrose-sweetened milk, i.e. youngchildren cannot regulate their energy intake in a specific meal (Wilson, 1991; Wilson,2000). Wilson’s findings, however, are inconsistent with those of Birch and co-workers (Birch & Deyser, 1986; Birch et al., 1989). In Study III there were nosignificant differences in energy intake depending on whether milk or water was

Page 35: Pre-school Children’s Food Habits and Meal Situationuu.diva-portal.org/smash/get/diva2:161313/FULLTEXT01.pdf · school, the behaviour of the pre-school staff or the children’s

Results and Discussion

35

served, nor was energy intake affected at the afternoon snack. Similar findings wererevealed in an earlier study by Jacobson and Nordlund (1994). The results indicate thatthere is some reason to believe that children are able to regulate their energy intake in aspecific meal.

Food patternOne interesting finding of the present dietary study was that almost all children hadeaten the variety of foods offered at pre-school. This means that they had goodopportunities to experience unfamiliar foods, which is necessary for acquiringpreferences (Birch, 1999). This is important, since many children have their meals fivedays a week at pre-school, since the children spend, on average, 8 hours a day at pre-school, and NFA guidelines (Wållberg et al., 1996) recommend that 65% of the dailyenergy intake of children enrolled full-time be covered at pre-school. Understandinghow temporal distribution of food intake affects health will require furtherinvestigation.

The children consumed fruits in greater quantities than vegetables (Figure 3), which iscomparable with other studies on pre-school children and adolescents (Campbell,1993; Frost Andersen et al., 1995; Krebs-Smith et al., 1996; McKenzie et al., 1996;Samuelson et al., 1996; Gibson et al., 1998; Briley et al., 1999; Gibson, 2000). ThePrincipal Component Analyses (PCA) revealed four food clusters, i.e., warm foodwith meat, potatoes and cooked cereals (cluster 3), and three different kinds of snacks,bread and breakfast cereals (cluster 1), milk and cheese products (cluster 2), andconfectionery, soft drinks and buns (cluster 4), see Figure 4. The variation within theclusters describing bread and breakfast cereals not only showed a negative correlationto milk and cheese products, but also a positive correlation to meat, potatoes andcooked cereals, thus revealing that bread is utilised for several purposes.

Principal Component 1 (PC1) was, on the one hand, closely related to milk and cheeseconsumption and negatively associated with the consumption of bread, buns and softdrinks. PC3, on the other hand, explained that the consumption of milk and cheeseproducts was positively related to that of bread and breakfast cereals, but this was ofless importance when explaining variation in the material than were the phenomenadescribed by PC1. Milk and cheese products must, therefore, be assumed to have agreater role as between-meal snacks (PC1), and also a lesser role as an accompanimentto bread and breakfast cereals. However, when bread and milk/cheese products werecorrelated (PC3), there was a negative correlation between these foods and meat andpotatoes, which indicates that bread and milk/cheese products were an alternative mealto meat and potatoes (PC3 and PC4). Meat, potatoes and cooked cereals were alsonegatively correlated (PC2) to confectionery, buns and soft drinks, which indicate thatthese food groups were replacing each other. It is interesting to note that all patternsshown were the same for both weekdays and weekend days; however, the patterns forthe weekdays were slightly stronger.

Page 36: Pre-school Children’s Food Habits and Meal Situationuu.diva-portal.org/smash/get/diva2:161313/FULLTEXT01.pdf · school, the behaviour of the pre-school staff or the children’s

Results and Discussion

36

**

***

ns

***

***

ns

***

***

ns

*

*

***

ns

***

***

**

0 50 100 150 200 250 300 350 400 450 500

Vegetables: vegetables and legumes

Fruit: fruits and berries

Juice: fruit juices

Potatoes: potatoes and root vegetables

Milk and cheese: milk, fermented milk and cheese

Meat: meat, fish, poultry, eggs and seafood

Bread: bread, porridge, gruel, breakfast cereals

Cooked cereal: pasta, rice, other cereals, pizza, pancake

Fats: butter, margarine, oils

Confectionery: sweets, chocolate

Crisps: crisps, popcorn, nuts

Buns: buns, crackers, cakes

Soft drinks: soft drinks, fruit drinks

Ice cream: ice cream

Dessert: sugar, jam, rose hip soup, dessert

Condiments: ketchup, mustard, soya, vinegar

WeekendsWeekdays

Figure 3. Consumption of different food groups (g) during weekdays and weekend days by 109children. Paired t-test p-values. ***=p<0.001, **=p<0.01, *=p<0.05, ns= not significant

Page 37: Pre-school Children’s Food Habits and Meal Situationuu.diva-portal.org/smash/get/diva2:161313/FULLTEXT01.pdf · school, the behaviour of the pre-school staff or the children’s

Results and Discussion

37

Figure 4. Relationship between food groups on weekdays (WD) and weekend days (WE) andexplained variance (%).

Page 38: Pre-school Children’s Food Habits and Meal Situationuu.diva-portal.org/smash/get/diva2:161313/FULLTEXT01.pdf · school, the behaviour of the pre-school staff or the children’s

Results and Discussion

38

Socialisation and acquisition of food habits in pre-schoolNormative influencesThe design of Studies IV and V was explorative and qualitative, and wasintended to elucidate if and how the phenomenon of the pedagogic meal wasexpressed and applied within the pre-school. In my analysis of the interviewmaterial I have been inspired by Fieldhouse's model of the socialisation of food(Figure 1).

During the interviews (Study IV), it was clear that the pre-school childrenassociated food and eating with rules and norms, i.e. children described whatthey were and were not allowed to do in this context. Most children also knewwhat was acceptable and non-acceptable mealtime behaviour. For example, theysaid: “Everyone has to try at least a little bit of all the food that is served”; “It isnot allowed to spit food out of your mouth”. Some of them believed they wereexpected to eat everything on their plates, while others said, “You can eat asmuch as you please and leave the rest”. Rules associated with social aspects ofmeals were: “shouting is not allowed”, “you can speak to the children at yourtable, but not to the other children in the room”, “we can not leave the tableuntil all of us have finished the meal”. Even the youngest children knew whatwere considered acceptable table manners. However, when they talked aboutbehaviours at the table at pre-school, the majority of them began withaffirmations such as “do sit still on your chair”, “do try a little bit of everything”or “do not play at the dinner table”, “do not shout” and so on.

The present results, thus, indicate that children associate the meal situation at thepre-school with many rules and norms, where “good” table manners seem to beimplanted in the children's minds early in life, leaving them with less power –compared to the adults, i.e. staff and parents – to influence their own food choiceand behaviour in the meal situation. Perhaps the stories in which young childrenfrequently refer to food as a symbol of power (Counihan, 1999) illustrate theireveryday situation, one in which adults make the food decisions, thusdemonstrating their power over children. According to terminology inFieldhouse's model of the socialisation of food, these rules could be seen as earlycultural restrictions on children’s food experiences. Rozin also discusses thesocialisation of food and the importance of children developing an adult attitudetowards food and eating in their culture during the stage of primary socialisation(Rozin et al., 1986).

The majority of children were, however, positive when talking about actual food and itwas interesting that they described foods in terms of “likes” and did not think aboutthem in terms of “dislikes”. When asked to rate foods they “disliked”, they almostalways started to talk about their favourite foods. Thus, they had no problems talkingabout food they liked and preferred to eat. However, talking about food they did not

Page 39: Pre-school Children’s Food Habits and Meal Situationuu.diva-portal.org/smash/get/diva2:161313/FULLTEXT01.pdf · school, the behaviour of the pre-school staff or the children’s

Results and Discussion

39

like was, for some reason, more difficult (Study IV). It is, however, interesting to notethat Koivisto et al. found that the youngest children in their study had difficultiesexpressing what food they liked and disliked (Koivisto & Sjödén, 1996). Children,according to these authors, seem to develop dislikes easier than preferences for goodtaste, which could be interpreted as contradictory to our findings. The contradictionbetween our results and those of Kovisto et al. could be explained by the differentmethods chosen. In Koivisto’s study, the parents were present during the childinterviews, while only children took part in the focus groups in our study, as well as inCounihan’s interviews (Counihan, 1999). The type of questions posed might alsoaffect how children choose to respond.

In Study IV, the children also expressed cultural knowledge about what food isconsidered appropriate to eat and what is not, thus demonstrating another example ofFieldhouse's definition of cultural restrictions and an overall example of normativeinfluences. For instance, a number of children said that sweets, ice cream and cakesare not food. It is also interesting to note that the children said that sweets weresupposed to be consumed only on Fridays or Saturdays, i.e. they associated sweetswith weekends and holidays and not with pre-school. This finding indicates that youngchildren are learning early in life to associate particular foods with certain situations,and these children knew that different foods are consumed at pre-school and at home(Study IV). The children were, during this primary socialisation, influenced by local aswell as national norms. According to the Fieldhouse model, the Swedish tradition of“Saturday sweets” is an example of a national societal norm, while the above-mentioned rules and norms at table could be seen both as local (the norms set upwithin pre-school settings) and as national (norms developed in a societal context). Onthe other hand, allowing children to drink soft drinks on any weekday, as the parentsapparently did (Study II), could be interpreted as indicating that national nutritional-dental norms are changing. The traditional cultural norms in Sweden, that food high insucrose and especially sweets should be restricted to weekends, were still vital at thepre-school. The parents' acceptance of soft drink consumption all weekdays could beinterpreted in two ways: 1) soft drinks are no longer seen as food rich in sucrose or 2)the cultural norm of restricting such food to certain situations is fading. One might askwhy, in Swedish homes, sweets and soft drinks are not seen as belonging to onecategory of foods rich in sucrose?

Rozin also highlights that children's perceptions of foods tend to follow what adultscategorise as “good” or “bad” for children (Hammond et al., 1999). However, thechildren in our study did not use this categorisation, but instead “food” and “non-food”. The idea that “food is good for you” and makes you grow and therefore youshould eat it was a strategy with which a number of the children were familiar. Somechildren recalled that their parents and pre-school teachers used to tell them that theyhad to eat what was served in order to become big and strong (Study IV). In a recentFrench study, children aged 9-11 years old, view food primarily as a necessity of life,

Page 40: Pre-school Children’s Food Habits and Meal Situationuu.diva-portal.org/smash/get/diva2:161313/FULLTEXT01.pdf · school, the behaviour of the pre-school staff or the children’s

Results and Discussion

40

whereas mothers viewed food primarily as a pleasure for their child, with necessity andnutrition given a lower importance (Le Bigot Macaux, 2001). However, despite adults'beliefs in telling children that “food is good for you”, studies have shown thatencouraging children to eat a specific food is not a good method. This is particularlytrue when children are told “this is bad for you”. In such cases, the advice has verylittle effect (Casey & Rozin, 1989; Fisher & Birch, 1999b). Children may not fullyunderstand the reason underlying which foods are good for them, but nonetheless thereis reason to believe they have some ability to group foods from a nutritional point ofview, as was also shown by Anliker et al. (1990). In this sense, the children in ourstudy had not yet applied the traditional definitions of food, i.e. categorising it by thecultural restrictions or rather normative influences of good or bad, yet they did, at thisearly age, see food as either food or non-food.

Owen et al. (1997) state that descriptions of foods as “good” or “bad”, “healthy” or“unhealthy” should be avoided. This was expressed by Margaret Mead back in the1960s (Mead, 1980). In Western societies, however, we still tend to categorise food asbad or good, i.e. it is not preliminary children who do this, but adults who generatethese cultural norms in the society. Counihan discusses at what age children in Westernsocieties adopt this vocabulary. I mean that during primary socialisation, which takesplace already in pre-school in Sweden, children are learning what is prescribed by theculinary culture (Fieldhouse, 1996), i.e. they eat what is served and have littleinfluence on food choices. Yet they still have not adopted the more normativevocabulary of “good” or “bad” foods.

Since many studies show that food is the woman’s responsibility (Ekström, 1990; DeVault, 1991; Mennell et al., 1992; Wesslén, 2000), and since most of the staff in pre-school are woman, it is essential that they – as an important influential factor duringthis early socialisation – are aware of the subtle messages they give about body size,weight and dieting, as Lytle et al. have also pointed out (Lytle et al., 1997). Severalstudies have shown that dieting daughters have mothers who tend to be dieters (Hill etal., 1990; Pike & Rodin, 1991; Ruther & Richman, 1993). According to Hendy andRaudenbusch (2000), girls respond more to peer models of food behaviour than doboys, and even at three to five years of age, eating behaviour has more social relevancefor girls, especially when peers play an active role. Similarly, research has found that,even as children, females more often than males use eating behaviour to imitate,impress, or resist others (Mori et al., 1987; Cutting et al., 1999; Fisher & Birch,1999a). Because girls are at greater risk for developing eating disorders later in life,future research should examine early gender differences and effects of peers at pre-school, but also of other adults, i.e. pre-school staff. For instance, staff with problemsregulating their own eating and/or weight could influence the children, both girls andboys, in a negative way.

Page 41: Pre-school Children’s Food Habits and Meal Situationuu.diva-portal.org/smash/get/diva2:161313/FULLTEXT01.pdf · school, the behaviour of the pre-school staff or the children’s

Results and Discussion

41

The pedagogic mealIn Fieldhouse's model (Figure 1), secondary socialisation starts in school, meaning thatchildren in Sweden, already at the age of 1 to 5, are subject to secondary socialisationsince they are enrolled in pre-school. An important part of this process is the so-calledpedagogic meal. When encouraged to talk about the specific phenomenon of food andmeals at pre-school, the informants in Study V began by comparing the presentsituation with an earlier period, since most of them had worked for more than 12 yearswithin this organisation. In doing so, they revealed strong attitudes as well asambivalence towards how food-related activities should best be integrated into theirdaily pedagogic work. In the National Commission on Childcare in 1968 (GovernmentReport, 1972) pre-schools teachers were considered as role models for children attable. However, during the interviews, the informants declared that in the past they didnot eat with the children and therefore they had difficulties acting as role models. Atthe present time, however, the situation was different, i.e. they socialised with thechildren at table even though there were no official documents or recommendationsclearly describing that this should be done and how. At the time of the interviews, thepre-school staff were not actually officially considered as mealtime role models, butwere acting as such. Still, most of the staff in Study V had a clear perception of what it meant to practiceand carry out a pedagogic meal. This involved helping and encouraging the children tohelp themselves and serving as an adult model for the children at table, showing, forexample, how to handle cutlery, pass food to each other, sit on a chair, have aconversation without screaming as well as eat together. They also believed that theywere expected as pre-school teachers to show children that all foods served are edibleand tasteful by encouraging the children to taste and hopefully eat the foods served.The problem, as the staff expressed it, was that the daily meal, the routine meals thathad to be eaten every day, several times a day, so were not considered a pedagogicactivity. Instead the meals were experienced as something that disturbed other dailywork. In other words, the meal was not actually seen and accepted by the staff as apedagogic activity, whereas all other activities during the day were.

“People think it takes too much time away from other activities. But really it's anactivity too, though myself I can think it's just a detail.” Study V

The informants in Study V believed there had been a change in attitudes towards howto teach children good food habits. Nagging and forcing children to taste all foodsserved at lunch was considered common behaviour among the staff ten to fifteen yearsago. Especially the principals viewed this as unsatisfactory behaviour, whereas thechildminders and pre-school teachers believed tasting the foods served was stillimportant and should be pointed out to the children (Study V).

Page 42: Pre-school Children’s Food Habits and Meal Situationuu.diva-portal.org/smash/get/diva2:161313/FULLTEXT01.pdf · school, the behaviour of the pre-school staff or the children’s

Results and Discussion

42

There are reasons to believe that children at pre-school accept more new foods becausethe foods are presented to them repeatedly (Birch & Marlin, 1982) and because theyare eating with other children who can serve as models (Yperman & Vermeersch,1979; Birch, 1980; Greer et al., 1991; Hendy & Raudenbush, 2000). Perhaps earlierresearch could provide some explanations. Pliner et al. found that younger childrenoften rejected food primarily because it was unknown and secondly because they didnot like it (Pliner, 1994). However, Koivisto et al. found that the main reason forrejection was distaste (Koivisto & Sjoden, 1996). The majority of the children in StudyIV understood they were expected to taste all foods served, even if they had tried themat home “If you’ve tried it at home, well then you can try again ... and then if you thinkit’s good you can eat it” (Study IV). Our results parallel those from recent studies inthe US conducted by Gittelsohn et al. (2000) and Hertzler et al. (1999). Gittelsohn etal. found that the three most commonly reported food rules practised by 24 interviewedteachers were to encourage the children to finish their foods during the school meal, towash hands and to taste all foods. Hertzler et al. found that children’s food problems,according to the staff, were that they liked fast food and did not taste new foods. AsKoivisto Hursti has pointed out, it is essential that adults be made aware that foodneophobia is very common among children (Koivisto Hursti & Sjödén, 1999). InSwedish literature from the seventies and onwards it has been stated that tasting newfoods is not important since this may lead to decreased preferences, that is childrenshould not be forced to taste new foods (Virgin, 1970). Research would seem toindicate that it is important for children to be exposed to a variety of foods. However, itis up to the children to choose to taste or not taste new foods. Tasting all foods servedseems to be a deeply rooted norm in Western societies, and as shown in Study II, allchildren had actually eaten or tried a variety of foods served at the pre-school meals.

Gillis and Sabrez (1980) noted that pre-school teachers are in a position to directlyinfluence the quality and type of food available to the children as well as thedevelopment of food preferences. Nahikian-Nelms (1997) found a positive correlationbetween nutrition knowledge and behaviour at mealtime, nutrition knowledge andattitudes, and attitudes and caregiver behaviour. For example, if caregivers fail to sitwith children, consume the same foods, and create a positive pleasant environment,positive role modelling may not occur. Sjödén et al. (1985) found a positive correlationbetween children’s preferences for certain foods and positive behaviour/stimulationfrom adults in the meal situation. A negative correlation for preference was found if acertain food was followed by a reward, for example, “eat your porridge than you canwatch TV”. In a study by Fergus and co-workers (1998), it was found that combinedvideo-modelling and reward intervention resulted in major increases in consumption offruit and vegetables that children previously had consistently refused to eat. Almost allof the previously refused foods were eaten and the effect was largely maintained aftersix months. They conclude that eating practices of children are “extremely malleableand subject to strong influence by sociocultural factors” (p.76). According to Hendyand Raudenbusch (2000), teacher modelling is one of the most effective methods to

Page 43: Pre-school Children’s Food Habits and Meal Situationuu.diva-portal.org/smash/get/diva2:161313/FULLTEXT01.pdf · school, the behaviour of the pre-school staff or the children’s

Results and Discussion

43

encourage food acceptance by pre-school children. They showed that modelling bysilent teachers was ineffective in encouraging food acceptance, while enthusiasticteachers (“Mmm! I love mango!”) were effective. However, with edition of acompeting peer model, even enthusiastic teacher modelling was no longer effective inencouraging new food acceptance. Koivisto and co-workers showed that most parentalbehaviour was verbal, including negative statements to the child about non-eating(Koivisto et al., 1994a). These findings are similar to what the children expressed inStudy IV; they associated food and eating with rules and norms, i.e. children describedwhat they were and were not allowed to do in this context. Some of the meals duringthe first week at the pre-schools were even video recorded. The preliminary findingsfrom the video-recordings confirm that, between the children and the staff, the socialverbal exchange at the mealtime was insignificant. The children’s practical skillsseemed to be more important to the staff than creating meaningful conversations 2.

The respondents in Study V expressed the clear opinion that pre-school as anorganisation in Sweden looked after and took good care of the children involved inthis educational care sector of society. While the staff was satisfied with the pre-school's role of feeding the children, they expressed a concern, or even mistrustregarding the children’s parents. The general picture of the food served at home wasthat it was unbalanced and that parents mostly served food their children liked to eat,for example pizza, hamburgers and spaghetti. The majority of the staff did not believethat traditional Swedish dishes were commonly served at home. They also believedthat if the parents served breakfast at home, it was not a proper one, as opposed tothose served at pre-school (porridge or yoghurt, bread, milk, juice, cheese and someother spread). In most cases, the staff thought that children should eat breakfast eitherat home or at pre-school. The vision of the children’s situation at home in the earlymornings was that parents are very stressed, and therefore it must be better for childrento have breakfast at pre-school. One pre-school teacher even thought it might bedifficult for children to assimilate the energy and nutrients in the breakfast at homebecause it was too stressful. Overall, the staff thought that the children ate much betterat pre-school than at home and that food served at pre-school was more balanced andhealthier (Study V). This was, however, not shown to be the case among the childrenin this study. There were no significant differences in energy intake or food intakebetween the breakfasts consumed at home compared to those consumed at pre-school.3However, “high-nutrient foods” contributed relatively more energy and nutrients onweekdays than on weekend days (Study II).

“At home they don't get… I mean regular home-style meals have disappeared. Not justat pre-school but even for the families I think you hear it from the children, it'sMacDonald's and pizza and macaroni and meatballs …. Spaghetti too. So they have an

2 S Lind, T Kaarlejärvi, Bachelor thesis, Department of Domestic Sciences 2000 3 H Pernler, M Lennernäs Junberger, H Sepp, M Gillberg; manuscript

Page 44: Pre-school Children’s Food Habits and Meal Situationuu.diva-portal.org/smash/get/diva2:161313/FULLTEXT01.pdf · school, the behaviour of the pre-school staff or the children’s

Results and Discussion

44

unbalanced diet at home but they do have a balanced diet at day-care at least, at pre-school … I think.” Study V

Thus the staff saw meals at pre-school as more balanced and as a better guarantee forgood food habits than private meals at home. Yet, as discussed above, they also feltthat mealtime at pre-school was time consuming and a problematic situation during theday. The results can be interpreted in two ways: 1) that parents and pre-school staffhave shared responsibility for primary socialisation since children begin pre-school soearly in Sweden and spend long hours there every day, or 2) that secondarysocialisation begins earlier in Sweden than elsewhere. Ideal meal situation - the utopian mealHowever, the staff expressed a vision of an ideal meal situation. It was “nice andcosy”, and a situation in which they could carry on a conversation with the childrenaround the table, which was seen as one important activity (Study V). The food was tobe nicely presented, tasteful and a delightful scent of freshly prepared food shouldmeet the children at lunchtime. However, according to the pre-school teachers andchild minders, this was not always the reality. They experienced that meals were oftentime-consuming due to their pedagogic profile, and also that they were stressful. Allchildren wanted to be heard and seen during the meals, i.e. they were talking andinterrupting each other resulting in a far too high sound level. The principals, on theother hand, did not express any negative experiences of the actual everyday meal;instead they described it more or less in the same way as the ideal meal situation. Onecould discuss whether the principals’ responses expressed their true beliefs about themeal situation or the wish to give a politically correct answer.

The public-pedagogic meal in the futureThe focus of Swedish nutrition policy is specified within four areas: education, supportfor local and regional nutrition programmes, consumer support and participation, masscatering and restaurants. Four of the eleven aims are of special interest for the food andmeal service in public care in general and childcare in particular.

Aim 1 The development of school settings for food-health-environmental issues should besupported.

Aim 9 Guidelines should be established for nutritional quality and meal organisation inprivate and public mass catering. The guidelines should be made available to therelevant parties in each target group.

Aim 10 The level of knowledge among production and meal staff involved in private and publicmass catering should be increased.

Aim 11 The authorities should have continuous dialogue with representatives from the masscatering sector.

The importance of a supportive environment for health will benefit everyone, butespecially vulnerable children (Aim 1). In the schools, home economic teachers,

Page 45: Pre-school Children’s Food Habits and Meal Situationuu.diva-portal.org/smash/get/diva2:161313/FULLTEXT01.pdf · school, the behaviour of the pre-school staff or the children’s

Results and Discussion

45

physical training teachers and the school meal staff have a central role as regardsintegrating food-health-environmental issues. There is no clear specification as to whois responsible for food-health-environmental questions in pre-school. In the curriculumfor pre-school, the environmental issues only reflect an ecological aspect: “The pre-schools shall place great emphasis on environmental and conservation issues” (p.10).Many factors probably underlie the fact that food-health-environmental issues are noton the agenda. However, when they are not highlighted, nor included in the curriculumand teaching, the signals given are that they do not have a high priority.

One way to establish the goals of nutritional quality and a positive meal situation is todevelop guidelines (Aim 9); this has been done for pre-schools (Wållberg et al., 1996).However, an evaluation of the acceptance of and adherence to these guidelines showedthat few people involved in the pre-schools activities had failed to take note of theguidelines (The Swedish National Food Administration, 1998). As a goal, the NutritionPolicy suggested establishment of a position for a dietary expert in each municipalitywho would be responsible for menus supporting the cooks as well as the principals ofthe pre-schools regarding food-health-environmental issues. If there is no oneresponsible in the municipality, it will be difficult to achieve aim 11, i.e. the authoritiesmust have a continuous dialogue with representatives from the mass catering sector.

The pre-schools are often small units, some of them have their own kitchen with acook, while others receive food from a mass catering kitchen or a pre-school close by.Aim 10 highlights the notion that education is important for the production as well asthe meal staff, but if there is no cook at the pre-school, it is even more important toeducate the meal staff, i.e. the pre-school teachers.

To conclude, the messages provided by the authorities are not consistent. The SwedishNational Food Administration and The Swedish National Institute of Public Healthhighlight the importance of development of school settings for food-health-environmental issues, while such questions are not mentioned in the curriculum forpre-school or in the education of pre-school teachers. The fact that children consume alarge portion of their daily diet on school grounds highlights the importance ofsupporting food-health-environment and education.

Page 46: Pre-school Children’s Food Habits and Meal Situationuu.diva-portal.org/smash/get/diva2:161313/FULLTEXT01.pdf · school, the behaviour of the pre-school staff or the children’s

46

SUMMARY AND CONCLUSIONSIn order to study food habits in an interdisciplinary manner, many different methodsare necessary, both quantitative and qualitative. Such a twofold approach has beentaken in the present thesis. A pre-school-based dietary survey, using seven-day records,focus group interviews and semi-structured interviews, was carried out in a suburbanarea of Stockholm. The overall objective was to investigate the individual food andnutrient intake of pre-school children at all meals during the day at the pre-school andat home, as well as to elucidate factors that might influence children’s intake. Onespecific aim was to compare food and nutrient intake at pre-schools serving milk atlunch with that at pre-schools serving water.

The average energy and nutrient intake per day for the whole week was satisfactory forthe 109 pre-school children, but the temporal distribution throughout the day wasskewed. The energy and nutrient intakes of food at the pre-school were lower thanrecommended. That, however, was compensated for the meals served at home. Therelative amount of carbohydrates was higher for weekends than for weekdays, owing toa higher sucrose intake. There were no significant differences between the children’senergy and nutrient intake attributable to the parents’ occupations. Understanding howthe temporal distribution of food intake affects health calls for further investigation.(Study I)

Almost all children had eaten the variety of foods offered at pre-school, thereby givingthem a chance to taste and become accustomed to new foods. The intake of fruits andvegetables seems to have increased during the past decades in Sweden. Stillconsumption of fruits and vegetables needs to be increased to meet the recommendedfive servings per day. The children's food intake was more varied during weekdaysthan weekend days. It seems to be a trend that “low-nutrient foods” have a greater roleto play in terms of energy as well as nutrient intake, especially in weekend days. Theutilisation of PCA showed that milk and cheese consumption has changed from whatwas previously assumed; dairy products now have a greater role as between-mealsnacks. (Study II)

Serving milk or water for pre-school lunch did not affect the total milk intake at pre-school as the children compensated at other meals for the absence of milk at lunch. Thelow milk consumers had a more even distribution of energy intake from different foodgroups than did the high milk consumers. This comparative study has not provided anyevidence to support the selection of water versus milk as a preferable lunch beveragein terms of pre-school children's total milk consumption and general dietary quality.The dietary analyses showed that there could be a reason to limit pre-school children’sdaily milk and fermented milk intake to half a litre. (Study III)

The children associated food and eating with rules and norms. Most children describedthese rules and norms as well as what they were and were not allowed to do. They

Page 47: Pre-school Children’s Food Habits and Meal Situationuu.diva-portal.org/smash/get/diva2:161313/FULLTEXT01.pdf · school, the behaviour of the pre-school staff or the children’s

Summary and Conclusions

47

knew very well the difference between acceptable and non-acceptable mealtimebehaviour, and were especially aware of what they were not allowed to do. Whenchildren were asked to rate foods they “disliked”, they spoke instead about theirfavourite foods. They did not categorise food as good or bad, as adults often do, but as“food” and “non-food”; for example, sweets were not food. The method used in thisstudy, the focus group interview, was judged to be a useful tool for exploring howchildren think about and jointly reflect upon food. (Study IV)

The role of the teachers had changed over the past years and they had not yet found asolid ground for integrating food and meals into their everyday work as pre-schoolteachers and childminders. Their role as teachers at present could be regarded more asguides, or mentors, serving as mealtime role models. (Study V)

To conclude, ¾ of children in Sweden are enrolled in the Swedish tax-financed pre-school system. Thus, a majority has a great proportion of their meals and food intakeat the school facility. According to the study, although the pre-school diet wasadequate for most children, more could be done to fulfil the aim of the Swedishnutrition policy, i.e. to develop school settings for food-health-environmental issues.One important step is to include various items concerning food and meals in the pre-school curriculum, thereby sanctioning responsibly for these issues and consequentlydemanding formal competence among pre-school staff.

Page 48: Pre-school Children’s Food Habits and Meal Situationuu.diva-portal.org/smash/get/diva2:161313/FULLTEXT01.pdf · school, the behaviour of the pre-school staff or the children’s

48

ACKNOWLEDGEMENTSI would like to express my sincere gratitude to all who, in different ways, have supported meand made the completion of this thesis possible. My special thanks, however, go to:

All children, pre-school staff and parents participating in the study and the girls, PatriciaEresjö, Anna-Lena Hermansson, Sofia Ridaeus, Kristine Rundström Katarina Virhammar,Sara Virhammar and Teresia Wällstedt who helped me with the data collection at the pre-schools. And a very special thanks to Elisabet Collén who was my helping hand during thewhole project.

My supervisor, Lillemor Abrahamsson, for her enthusiasm, for sharing her extensiveknowledge about so many topics, for her brilliant thinking, for handling my moods and me.

My co-supervisors, Christina Fjellström and Yngve Hofvander, for their enthusiasm, valuableadvise and all the support they have given to me.

Maria Lennernäs Junberger, Einar Risvik, Lisbeth Johansson and Annika Wesslén forstimulating, creative & fruitful discussions and for teaching me a lot.

Ulla Hagman for taking the initiative to the study, Roland Pettersson for his valuable opinionsin statistics and Kerstin Trygg for a useful discussion in dietary assessment methods.

Anette Jonsäll and Ylva Mattson Sydner, colleagues, roommates and also dear friends. Therewould be no thesis without you!

All my colleagues at the Department of Domestic Sciences, for always being so nice andhelpful, and especially to the FoU-group for stimulating discussions and helpful advice.

Malcom Forbes, Kurt Johansson and Karen Williams for improving my English.

Sina Alfredsson, 5 years, for the lovely drawing on the front cover.

Erik Rolfhamre, without your support and humour during a tuff period, this work would havebeen considerably more difficult.

Ewa Petersson, my dearest and closest friend, for always being there for me. Last but not least I want to thank my family and friends for all your support. I could not havedone this without you!

Thank you all!

This thesis was financially supported by the Faculty of Social Sciences, Swedish DairyAssociation, Stiftelsen Uppsala hushållsskolas fond, Stiftelsen Uppsala Hemsysterskolas fond,Stiftelsen Engelbrekts Barnavårds- och Husmodersskola, Svenska LivsmedelstekniskaFöreningen and Lundellska fondförvaltningen.

Page 49: Pre-school Children’s Food Habits and Meal Situationuu.diva-portal.org/smash/get/diva2:161313/FULLTEXT01.pdf · school, the behaviour of the pre-school staff or the children’s

49

REFERENCESAmerican Dietetic Association (1997) Nutrition trends surveys. Chicago IL: AmericanDietetic Association.

Ames SK, Gorham BM & Abrams SA (1999) Effects of high compared with low calciumintake on calcium absorption and incorporation of iron by red blood cells in small children.Am J Clin Nutr 70, 44-48.

Anliker JA, Laus MJ, Samonds KW & Beal VA (1990) Parental messages and the nutritionawareness of pre-school children. Society for Nutrition Education 22, 24-29.

Ballew C, Kuester S & Gillespie C (2000) Beverage choices affect adequacy of Children'snutrient intakes. Arch Pediatr Adolesc Med 154, 1148-1152.

Barker ME, McClean SI, Thompson KA & Reid NG (1990) Dietary behaviours andsociocultural demographics in Northern Ireland. Br J Nutr 64, 319-329.

Baxter SD, Thompson WO, Davis HC & Litaker MS (1999) Children's dietary recalls: thesalience of entree and liking for foods on accuracy and order of reporting. Nutrition 15, 848-853.

Bergström E, Hernell O & Persson L (1993) Dietary changes in Swedish adolescents. ActaPaediatr 82, 472-480.

Bergström L (1998) Nutrient losses and gains in the preparation of food. Uppsala: TheSwedish National Food Administration.

Birch L (1980) Effects of peer models' food choice and eating behaviours on pre-schoolers'food preferences. Child Dev 51, 489-496.

Birch L, Johnson S, Andresen G, Peters J & Schultze M (1991) The variability of youngchildren's energy intake. N Engl J Med 324, 232-235.

Birch L, McPhee L, Shoba BC, Steinberg L & Krehbiel R (1987) "Clean up your plate":effects of child feeding practicies on the conditioning of meal size. Learn Motiv 18.301-317

Birch LL (1999) Development of food preferences. Annu Rev Nutr 19, 41-62.

Birch LL & Deyser M (1986) Caloric compensation and sensory specific satiety, evidence forself regulation of food intake by young children. Appetite 7, 323-331.

Birch LL & Ficher JO (1998) Development of eating behaviours among children andadolescents. Pediatrics 101, S539-549.

Page 50: Pre-school Children’s Food Habits and Meal Situationuu.diva-portal.org/smash/get/diva2:161313/FULLTEXT01.pdf · school, the behaviour of the pre-school staff or the children’s

References

50

Birch LL & Marlin DW (1982) I don't like it; I never tried it: effects of exposure on two-year-old children's food preferences. Appetite 3, 353-360.

Birch LL, Marlin DW & Rotter J (1984) Eating as the "means" activity in a contingency:effect on young children's food preferences. Child Dev 55, 431-439.

Birch LL, McPhee L & Sullivan S (1989) Children's food intake following drinks sweetenedwith sucrose or aspartame: time course effect. Physiol Behav 45, 387-395.

Briley ME, Jastrow S, Vickers J & Roberts-Gray C (1999) Dietary intake at child-care centersand away: are parents and care providers working as partners or at cross-purposes? J Am DietAssoc 99, 950-954.

Brownell KD & Napolitino MA (1995) Distorting reality for children: Body size proportionsof Barbie and Ken dolls. Int J Eat Disord 18, 295-298.

Cameron M & Staveren van W (1988) Manual of methodology for food consumption studies:Oxford University Press.

CAMO (1986-1999) The Unscrambler. Trondheim: CAMO ASA.

Campbell M (1993) The nutrient intake, growth, and eating pattern of preschool children withsingle, employed mothers. Journal of Canadian Dietetic Association 54, 151-156.

Casey R & Rozin P (1989) Changing children's food preferences: parent opinions. Appetite12, 171-182.

Costanzo O & Woody E (1984) Parental perspective on obesity in children: the importance ofsex differnces. Journal of Social Clinical Psychology 2, 305-313.

Costanzo P & Woody E (1985) Domain specific parenting styles and their impact on thechild's development of particular deviance; the example of obesity proneness. J Soc ClinPsychol 3, 425-445.

Cook JD, Dassenko SA & Whittaker P (1991) Calcium supplementation: effect on ironabsorption. Am J Clin Nutr 53, 106-111.

Counihan C (1999) The anthropology of food and body: gender, meaning and power. p 129-155. New York: Routledge.

Cutting TM, Fisher JO, Grimm-Thomas K & Birch LL (1999) Like mother, like daughter:familial patterns of overweight are mediated by mothers' dietary disinhibition. Am J Clin Nutr69, 608-613.

De Vault ML (1991) Feeding the family: the social organization of caring as gendered work.Chicago: The University Chicago press.

Page 51: Pre-school Children’s Food Habits and Meal Situationuu.diva-portal.org/smash/get/diva2:161313/FULLTEXT01.pdf · school, the behaviour of the pre-school staff or the children’s

References

51

Dennison BA, Rockwell HL & Baker SL (1997) Excess fruit juice consumption by preschool-aged children is associated with short stature and obesity [published erratum appears inPediatrics 1997 Oct;100(4):733]. Pediatrics 99, 15-22.

Dwyer JT (1995) Dietary fiber for children: How much? Pediatrics 96, S1019-1022.

Dyrenforth SR, Wooley OW & Wooley SC (1980) A woman’s body in a man's world: Areview of findings on body image and weight control. New Jersey: Prentice Hall Inc.

Eck LH, Klesges RC & Hanson CL (1989) Recall of a child's intake from one meal: areparents accurate? J Am Diet Assoc 89, 784-789.

Edlund B (1997) Dieting in Swedish children and adolescents, Dissertation from the Facultyof Social Sciences, Uppsala university.

Edlund B, Halvarsson K & Sjödén P-O (1996) Eating behaviours, and attitudes to eating,dieting and body image in 7-year-old Swedish girls. European Eating Disorders Review 4, 40-53.

Ekström M (1990) Kost, klass och kön (Food class and gender), Umeå University. [InSwedish, abstract in English]

Expertgruppen (1990) Mjölkens och mejeriprodukternas betydelse för energi- ochnäringstillförseln. Vår Föda 42, 186-191. [In Swedish]

Feldman W, Feldman E & Goodman J (1988) Culture versus biology: Children's attitudestoward thinness and fatness. Pediatrics 81.190-194.

Fergus L, Dowey A & Horne P (1998) Changing what children eat. p 57-80. In The Nation’sDiet [ A Murcott editor] New York: Addison Wesley Longman.

Fieldhouse P (1996) Food and nutrition. Customs and culture, 2 ed. p 3-6.Cheltenham:Stanley Thornes Ltd.

Fisher JO & Birch LL (1999a) Restricting access to foods and children's eating. Appetite32.405-419.

Fisher JO & Birch LL (1999b) Restricting access to palatable foods affects children'sbehavioral response, food selection, and intake. Am J Clin Nutr 69, 1264-1272.

Fisher JO, Johnson RK, Lindquist C, Birch LL & Goran MI (2000) Influence of bodycomposition on the accuracy of reported energy intake in children. Obes Res 8, 597-603.

Food and Nutrition Board (1989) Recommended dietary allowances. Washington DC:National Academy of Sciences.

Page 52: Pre-school Children’s Food Habits and Meal Situationuu.diva-portal.org/smash/get/diva2:161313/FULLTEXT01.pdf · school, the behaviour of the pre-school staff or the children’s

References

52

Frost Andersen L, Nes M, Sandstad B, Bjørneboe G-E & Drevon C (1995) Dietary intakeamong Norwegian adolescents. Eu J Clin Nutr 49, 555-564.

Gibson EL, Wardle J & Watts CJ (1998) Fruit and vegetable consumption, nutritionalknowledge and beliefs in mothers and children. Appetite 31, 205-228.

Gibson R (1990) Principles of nutritional assessment: Oxford University Press.

Gibson S (1997) Obesity: is it related to sugar in children's diets? Nutrition & Food Science 5,184-187.

Gibson SA (2000) Associations between energy density and macronutrient composition in thediets of pre-school children: sugars vs. starch. Int J Obes Relat Metab Disord 24, 633-638.

Gillis DE & Sabry JH (1980) Daycare teachers: Nutrition knowledge, opinions and use offoods. Journal of Nutrition Education 12, 200-204.

Gittelsohn J, Toporoff EG, Story M, Evans M, Anliker J, Davis S, Sharma A & White J(2000) Food perceptions and dietary behavior of American-Indian children, their caregivers,and educators: Formative assessment findings from pathways. Journal of Nutrition Education1, 2-13.

Gleerup A, Rossander-Hulthén L, Gramatkovski E & Hallberg L (1995) Iron absorption fromthe whole diet: comparison of the effect of two different distributions of daily calcium intake.Am J Clin Nutr 61, 97-104.

Goldberg G & Black A (1998) Assessment of the validity of reported energy intakes - reviewand recent developments. Scand J of Nutr 42, 6-9.

Goldberg G, Black A, Jebb S, Cole T, Murgatroyd P, Coward W & Prentice A (1991) Criticalevaluation of energy intake data using fundamental principles of energy physiology: 1.Derivation of cut-off limits to identify under-recording. Eur J of Clin Nutr 45, 569-581.

Government Report (1972) Förskolan Del 1 och 2. Betänkande avgivet av 1968 årsbarnstugeutredning. SOU 1972:26,27. Stockholm: Socialdepartementet. [In Swedish]

Government Report (1985) Om förskola för alla barn. Stockholm. [In Swedish]

Government Report (1998) Läroplan för förskolan Lpfö 98. Västerås: Regeringskansliet,Skolverket och CE Fritzes AB.[In Swedish]

Greer RD, Dorow L, Williams G, McCorkle N & Asnes R (1991) Peer-mediated proceduresto induce swallowing and food acceptance in young children. J Appl Behav Anal 24, 783-790.

Page 53: Pre-school Children’s Food Habits and Meal Situationuu.diva-portal.org/smash/get/diva2:161313/FULLTEXT01.pdf · school, the behaviour of the pre-school staff or the children’s

References

53

Grube J & Wallack L (1994) Television beer advertising and drinking knowledge, beliefs, andintentions among school children. Am J Publ Health 84, 254-259.

Hagman U, Bruce Å, Persson L-Å, Samuelson G & Sjölin S (1986) Food habits and nutrientintake in childhood in relation to health and socio-economic conditions. Acta Paediatr 328Suppl, 1-56.

Hallberg L (1998) Does calcium interfere with iron absorption? Am J Clin Nutr 68, 3-4.

Hallberg L (2000) New tools in studies on iron nutrition principles, applications andconsequences. Scand J Nutr 44, 150-154.

Hallberg L, Brune M, Erlandsson M, Sandberg AS & Rossander-Hultén L (1991) Calcium:effect of different amounts on nonheme- and heme-iron absorption in humans. Am J Clin Nutr53, 112-119.

Hallberg L, Rossander-Hultén L, Brune M & Gleerup A (1992) Calcium and iron absorption:mechanism of action and nutritional importance. Eur J Clin Nutr 46, 317-327.

Hammers L (1999) Basmätning mjölk 1999. Stockholm: LUI Marknadsinformation AB. [InSwedish]

Hammond KM, Wyllie A & Casswell S (1999) The extent and nature of televised foodadvertising to New Zealand children and adolescents. Aust N Z J Public Health 23, 49-55.

Harnack L, Stang J & Story M (1999) Soft drink consumption among US children andadolescents: nutritional consequences. J Am Diet Assoc 99, 436-441.

Hendy HM & Raudenbush B (2000) Effectiveness of teacher modelling to encourage foodacceptance in preschool children. Appetite 34, 61-76.

Hertzler A & Frary R (1999) Preschool children's food problems and food-related caregivingtechniques. Journal of Consumer Studies & Home Economics 23, 147-154.

Hill AJ, Weaver C & Blundell JE (1990) Dieting concerns of 10-years-old girls and theirmothers. Br J Clin Psychol 29, 346-348.

Holm A-K (1990) Education and diet in the prevention of caries in the preschool child. J Dent18, 308-314.

Håglin L, Hagman U & Nilsson M (1995) Evaluation of the Meal Model "Matmallen". Ameans of estimating consumed amounts of food. Scand J Nutr 39, 79-83.

Hägg A, Jacobsson T, Nordlund G & Rössner S (1998) Effects of milk or water on lunchintake in pre-school children. Appetite 31, 83-92.

Page 54: Pre-school Children’s Food Habits and Meal Situationuu.diva-portal.org/smash/get/diva2:161313/FULLTEXT01.pdf · school, the behaviour of the pre-school staff or the children’s

References

54

Höglund D, Samuelson G & Mark A (1998) Food habits in Swedish adolescents in relation tosocioeconomic conditions. Eu J Clin Nutr 52, 784-789.

International Life Sciences Institute E (2000) Overweight and obesity in European childrenand adolescents. Causes and consequences - prevention and treatment. Brussels: InternationalLife Sciences Institute.

Jacobson T & Nordlund G (1994) Förskolebarnens matvanor - Om måltidsmönster, energi-och näringsintag i förskolan och i hemmet. Umeå: Pedagogiska institutionen.[In Swedish]

Johansson G (1990) Effects of a shift from a mixed diet to a lactovegetarian diet on somecolon cancer associated characteristics, Karolinska Institutet.Stockholm.Johnson SL & Birch LL (1994) Parents' and children's adiposity and eating style. Pediatrics94, 653-661.

Kelder SH, Perry CL, Klepp KI & Lytle LL (1994) Logitudinal tracking of adolescentsmoking, physical activity and food choice behaviors. Am J Public Health 84, 1121-1126.

Keys A, Menotti A, Aravanis C, Blackburn H, Djordevic BS, Buzina R, Dintas AS, FidanzaF, Karvonen MJ, Kimura N, et al (1984) The seven countries study: 2,289 death in 15 years.Prev Med 13, 141-154.

Klesges RC, Stein RJ, Eck LH, Isbell TR & Klesges LM (1991) Parental influence on foodselection in young children and in relation to childhood obesity. Am J Clin Nutr 53.859-864.

Koivisto Hursti U-K (1999) Factors influencing children's food choice. Ann Med 31, S26-32.

Koivisto Hursti U-K & Sjödén P-O (1999) Relations of taste and earlier experience with thelikelihood of future consumption of specific foods in Swedish families with children aged 7-17. Ecology of Food and Nutrition 37, 429-453.

Koivisto UK, Edlund B & Sjödén P-O (1994a) Exposure to milk or water at preschool lunchfor 3 months influences children's choice of elementary school lunch drink 4 months later.Appetite 23, 265-273.

Koivisto U-K, Fellenius J & Sjödén P-O (1994b) Relations between parental mealtimepractices and children's food intake. Appetite 22, 245-258.

Koivisto UK & Sjödén P-O (1996) Reasons for rejection of food items in Swedish familieswith children aged 2-17. Appetite 26, 89-103.

Krebs-Smith SM, Cook A, Subar AF, Cleveland L, Friday J & Kahle LL (1996) Fruit andvegetable intakes of children and adolescents in the United States. Arch Pediatr Adolesc Med150, 81-86.

Page 55: Pre-school Children’s Food Habits and Meal Situationuu.diva-portal.org/smash/get/diva2:161313/FULLTEXT01.pdf · school, the behaviour of the pre-school staff or the children’s

References

55

Krueger R (1994) Focus groups: a practical guide for applied research. Thousand Oaks,Carlifonia: Sage Publications.

Kvale S (1997) Den kvalitativa forskningsintervjun. Lund: Studentlitteratur.[In Swedish]

Lagström H, Seppänen R, Jokinen E, Niinikoski H, Rönnemaa T, Viikari J & Simell O (1999)Influence of dietary fat on nutrient intake and growth when child's age increases from 1 to 5year; the special Turku coronary risk factor intervention project. Am J Clin Nutr 69, 516-523.

Laitinen S, Rasanen L, Viikari J & Akerblom HK (1995) Diet of Finnish children in relationto the family's socio-economic status. Scand J Soc Med 23, 88-94.

Larsson M (2001) Allt fler överviktiga barn och ungdomar. In Vår Föda.4-6. [In Swedish]

Le Bigot Macaux A (2001) Eat to live or live to eat? Do parents and children agree? PublicHealth Nutrition 4, 141-146.

Leaman M & Evers S (1997) Dietary intake by food groups of preschool children in low-income communities in Otario. Journal of Canadian Dietetic Association 58, 184-190.

Leung M, Yeung DL, Pennell MD & Hall J (1984) Dietary intakes of preschoolers. J Am DietAssoc 84, 551-554.

Livingstone B, Prentice A, Coward A, Strain J, Black A, Davies P, Stewart C, McKenna G &Whitehead R (1992) Validation of estimates of energy intake by weighed dietary record anddiet history in children and adolescents. Am J Clin Nutr 56, 29-35.

Lupton D (1996) Food, the body and the self. London: SAGE Publication Ltd.

Lytle LA, Eldrige AL, Kotz K, Piper J, Williams S & Kalina B (1997) Children'sinterpretation of nutrition messages. Journal of Nutrition Education 29, 128-136.

Lytle LA, Murray DM, Perry CL & Eldridge AL (1998) Validating fourth-grade students' self-report of dietary intake: results from the 5 A Day Power Plus program. J Am Diet Assoc 98,570-572.

Lönnerdal B (1999) Does a high dietary intake of calcium adversely affect iron status inhumans? Scan J Nutr 43, 82-84.

McKenzie J, Dixon LB, Smiciklas-Wright H, Mitchell D, Shannon B & Tershakovec A(1996) Change in nutrient intakes, number of servings, and contributions of total fat from foodgroups in 4- to 10-year-old children enrolled in a nutrition education study. J Am Diet Assoc96, 865-873.

Mead M (1943) The problem of changing food habits: with suggestions for psychoanalyticcontributions. Bulletin of the Menninger Clinic. 7, 57-61.

Page 56: Pre-school Children’s Food Habits and Meal Situationuu.diva-portal.org/smash/get/diva2:161313/FULLTEXT01.pdf · school, the behaviour of the pre-school staff or the children’s

References

56

Mead M (1980) A perspective on food patterns. Issues in Nutrition for 1980's. Monterey:Wadsworth Inc.

Mennell S, Murcott A & Otterloo van AH (1992) The sociology of food. London: SAGEPublications Ltd.

Michaelsen KF & Jørgensen MH (1995) Dietary fat content and energy density during infancyand childhood; the effect on energy intake and growth. Eur J Clin Nutr 49, 467-483.

Minihane AM & Fairweather-Tait SJ (1998) Effect of calcium supplementation on dailynonheme-iron absorption and long-term iron status. Am J Clin Nutr 68, 96-102.

MINITAB (1998) Statistical software. USA: Mintab Inc.

Morgan K & Krueger R (1998) The focus group kit. Thousand Oaks, California: SagePublications.

Mori D, Chaiken S & Pliner P (1987) "Eating lightly" and the self-presentation of femininity.J Pers Soc Psychol 53, 693-702.

Murcott A (1986) Opening the "black box": food and eating and household relationships.Sosiaalilääketieteellinen Aikakauslehti 23, 85-92.

Nahikian-Nelms M (1997) Influential factors of caregiver behavior at mealtime: A study of 24child-care programs. J Am Diet Assoc 97, 505-509.

National Agency for Education (2000) Descriptive data on child care and schools in Swedenin 2000, report no 192. Stockholm: National Agency for Education.

Nestle M, Wing R, Birch L, DiSogra L, Drewnowski A, Middleton S, Sigman-Grant M, SobalJ, Winston M & Economos C (1998) Behavioral and social influences on food choice.Nutrition Reviews 56, S50-S74.

Nicklas T, Farris R, Bao W, Webber L & Berenson G (1997) Differences in reported dietaryintake of 10-year-old children on weekdays compared to Sunday: The Bogalusa heart study.Nutr Res 17, 31-40.

Niklasson A & Karlberg P (1999) Tillväxtdiagram. Stockholm: The National Board of Healthand Welfare.[In Swedish]

Nilsson A & Pettersson A (2000) Vad kostar verksamheten i din kommun: bokslut 1999.Stockholm: Svenska kommunförbundet, Statistiska centralbyrån.[In Swedish]

Nordin M (1997) MATs. A nutrient calculation system designed for research and education.Västerås: Rudans Lättdata.[In Swedish]

Page 57: Pre-school Children’s Food Habits and Meal Situationuu.diva-portal.org/smash/get/diva2:161313/FULLTEXT01.pdf · school, the behaviour of the pre-school staff or the children’s

References

57

North K & Emmett P (2000) Multivariate analysis of diet among three-year-old children andassociations with socio-demographic characteristics. The Avon Longitudinal Study ofPregnancy and Childhood (ALSPAC) Study Team. Eur J Clin Nutr 54, 73-80.

Owen S, Schickler P & Davies J (1997) Food choice: how to assess attitudes of pre-adolescentchildren. Nutrition & Food Science 97, 5-11.

Patterson RE, Typpo JT, Typpo MH & Krause GF (1986) Factors related to obesity inpreschool children. J Am Diet Assoc 86, 1376-1381.

Petter LP, Hourihane JO & Rolles CJ (1995) Is water out of vogue? A survey of the drinkinghabits of 2-7 year olds. Arch Dis Child 72, 137-140.

Pierce J (1991) Does tobacco advertising target young people to stop smoking? Evidence fromCalifornia. JAMA 266, 3154-3158.

Pike KM & Rodin J (1991) Mothers, daughters, and disorded eating. J Abnorm Psychol 100,198-204.

Pliner P (1982) The effects of mere exposure on liking for edible substances. Appetite 3, 283-290.

Pliner P (1994) Development of measures of food neophobia in children. Appetite 23, 147-163.

Pliner P, Pelchat M & Grabski M (1993) Reduction of neophobia in humans by exposure tonovel foods. Appetite 20, 111-123.

Polit DF & Hungler BP (1995) Nursing Research. Principles and methods. 5th edition.Philadelphia, Pennsylvania: J B Lippinocott Company.

Prentice AM & Jebb SA (1995) Obesity in Britain: gluttony or sloth? British Medical Journal311, 437-439.

Prättälä R (1989) Young people and food. Socio-cultural studies of food consumptionpatterns, University of Helsinki.

Reddy MB & Cook JD (1997) Effect of calcium intake on nonheme-iron absorption from acomplete diet. Am J Clin Nutr 65, 1820-1825.

Rolls BJ, Engell D & Birch LL (2000) Serving portion size influences 5-year-old but not 3-year-old children's food intakes. J Am Diet Assoc 100, 232-234.

Ross S (1995) "Do I really have to eat that?": a qualitative study of schoolchildren's foodchoice and preferences. Health Educ J 54, 312-321.

Page 58: Pre-school Children’s Food Habits and Meal Situationuu.diva-portal.org/smash/get/diva2:161313/FULLTEXT01.pdf · school, the behaviour of the pre-school staff or the children’s

References

58

Rozin P (1990) Acquisition of stable food preferences. Nutrition Reviews 48, 106-113.

Rozin P, Hammer L, Oster H, Horowitz T & Marmora V (1986) The child's conception offood: differentiation of categories of rejected substances in the 16 months to 5 year age range.Appetite 7, 141-151.

Rozin P & Millman L (1987) Family environment, not heredity, accounts for familyresemblances in food preferences and attitudes: a twin study. Appetite 8, 125-134.

Ruther NM & Richman CL (1993) The relationship between mothers' eating restraint andtheir children's attitudes and behaviours. Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society 31, 217-220.

Räsänen L, Ahola M, Kara R & Uhari M (1985) Atherosclerosis precursors in Finnishchildren and adolescents. VIII. Food consumption and nutrient intakes. Acta Peadiatr Scand318 Suppl, 135-153.

Samuelson G (1971) An epidemiological study of child health and nutrition in a NorthernSwedish county. I. Food consumption survey. Acta Paediatr Scand Suppl 214, 5-44.

Samuelson G (2000) Dietary habits and nutritional status in adolescents over Europe. Anoverview of current studies in the Nordic countries. Eur J Clin Nutr 54 Suppl, 21-28.

Samuelson G, Bratteby L-E, Enghardt H & Hedgren M (1996) Food habits and energy andnutrient intake in Swedish adolescents approaching the year 2000. Acta Paediatr 415 Suppl,1-20.

Satter E (1987) How to get your kid to eat... But not too much: Palo Alto, California: BullPublishing Company.

Seagren JS & Terry RD (1991) WIC mothers' attitudes and behaviors towards their children'seating behavior. Journal of Nutrition Education 23, 223-230.

Sichert-Hellert W, Kersting M & Schöch G (1998) Underreporting of energy intake in 1 to 18year old German children and adolescents. Z Ernährungswiss 37, 242-251.

Sjödén P-O, Lappalainen R & Fellenius J (1985) Psykologiska aspekter på kostvanor hosbarn. Näringsforskning (Scand J Nutr) 29. [In Swedish]

Sobo EJ & Rock CL (2001) "You ate all that!?": caretaker-child interaction during children'sassisted dietary recall interviews. Med Anthropol Q 15, 222-244.

Statistics Sweden (1995) Socioekonomisk indelning (SEI). 128. Örebro. [In Swedish]

Subar AF, Krebs-Smith SM, Cook A & Kahle LL (1998) Dietary sources of nutrients amongUS children, 1989-1991. Pediatrics 102, 913-923.

Page 59: Pre-school Children’s Food Habits and Meal Situationuu.diva-portal.org/smash/get/diva2:161313/FULLTEXT01.pdf · school, the behaviour of the pre-school staff or the children’s

References

59

Sullivan S & Birch L (1990) Pass the sugar, pass the salt: experience dictates preference. DevPsychol 26, 546-551.

Sundström J (2000) Storhushållsguide, pp. 37-40. Oskarshamn: Delfi Marknadspartner AB &Jan Sundstöm, JS Analys. [In Swedish]

Swedbrewers (1984-2000) Statistics regarding consumption, sales, export and import:www.swedbrewers.se

Swedish Dairy Association (1950-2000) Dairy Statistics, summary in English: Swedish DairyAssociation. www.svenskmjolk.se

Swedish Statute-book (1996) Radio- och TV-lag. 7 kap. Reklam och annan annonsering.Stockholm. [In Swedish]

Takahashi E, Yoshida K, Sugimori H, Miyakawa M, Izuno T, Yamagami T & Kagamimori S(1999) Influence factors on the development of obesity in 3-year-old children based on thetoyama study. Prev Med 28, 293-296.

Tallberg Broman I (1995) Perspektiv på förskolans historia. Lund: Studentlitteratur.The Nordic Nutrition Recommendations (1996). Kopenhagen: Nordiska ministerrådet.[InSwedish]

The Swedish National Board of Health and Welfare (1973) Kost och fysisk aktivitet ibarnaåldern. Vällingby: Socialstyrelsen. [In Swedish]

The Swedish National Board of Health and Welfare (1980) Kost och aktivitet i barnaåldern.Stockholm: Liber. [In Swedish]

The Swedish National Board of Health and Welfare (1990) Kost, aktivitet och hälsa hos barnoch ungdom. Stockholm: Allmänna Förlaget. [In Swedish]

The Swedish National Committee in Public Health (1999) Levnadsvanor 2. Stockholm: TheSwedish Government. [In Swedish]

The Swedish National Food Administration (1989) Hulk.Uppsala. [In Swedish]

The Swedish National Food Administration (1997a) The Meal Model: The Swedish NationalFood Administration NFA. Uppsala. [In Swedish]

The Swedish National Food Administration (1997b) PC-kost 1-99. Uppsala: The SwedishNational Food Administration. Uppsala. [In Swedish]

The Swedish National Food Administration (1997c) Riksmaten. Uppsala. [In Swedish]

Page 60: Pre-school Children’s Food Habits and Meal Situationuu.diva-portal.org/smash/get/diva2:161313/FULLTEXT01.pdf · school, the behaviour of the pre-school staff or the children’s

References

60

The Swedish National Food Administration (1997d) SNR Tredje upplagan av de svenskanäringsrekommendationerna. Vår Föda, 7-14. [In Swedish]

The Swedish National Food Administration (1998) Utvärdering av boken Barn och Matlust.Påkostad och väl lanserad men har den blivit läst? Uppsala: The Swedish National FoodAdministarion. [In Swedish]

The Swedish National Food Administration (2001) Riktlinjer för grundskolan. Uppsala. [InSwedish]

The Swedish National Institute of Public Health & The Swedish National FoodAdministration (2000) Swedish national aims and strategies for nutrition 1999-2004, pp. 14.Sundsvall: The Swedish National Institute of Public Health, The Swedish National FoodAdministration, Uppsala. [In Swedish]

Thomas LF, Keim KS, Long EM & Zaske JM (1996) Factors related to low milk intake of 3-to 5-year-old children in child care settings. J Am Diet Assoc 96, 911-912.

Tidehag P, Sandberg AS, Hallmans G, Wing K, Türk M, Holm S & Grahn E (1995) Effect ofmilk and fermented milk on iron absorption in ileostomy subjects. Am J Clin Nutr 62, 1234-1238.

Torun B, Davies P, Livingstone M, Paolisso M, Sackett R & Spurr G (1996) Energyrequirements and dietary energy recommendations for children and adolescents 1 to 18 yearsold. Eur J Clin Nutr 50 Suppl, S37-S81.

Watt RG, Dykes J & Sheiham A (2000a) Preschool children's consumption of drinks:implications for dental health. Community Dent Health 17, 8-13.

Watt RG, Dykes J & Sheijham A (2000b) Drink consumption in British preschool children:relation to vitamin C, iron and calcium intakes. J Hum Nutr Dietet 13, 13-19.

Wesslén A (2000) Teenagers and food. The impact of culture and gender on attitudes towardsfood. Uppsala University.

Whichelow MJ & Prevost AT (1996) Dietary patterns and their associations withdemographic, lifestyle and health variables in a random sample of British adults. Br J Nutr 76, 17-30.

White JH (1992) Woman and eating disorders, part I: Significance and sociocultural riskfactors. Health Care for Women International 13, 351-362.

Wilson J (1991) Preschool children maintain intake of other foods at a meal includingsugared choclate milk. Appetite 16, 61-67.

Page 61: Pre-school Children’s Food Habits and Meal Situationuu.diva-portal.org/smash/get/diva2:161313/FULLTEXT01.pdf · school, the behaviour of the pre-school staff or the children’s

References

61

Wilson JF (2000) Lunch eating behavior of pre-school children: Effects of age, gender, andtype of beverage served. Physiol Beha 70, 27-33.

Virgin E (1970) Skolmåltider - förr och nu. Stockholm: Utbildningsförlaget. [In Swedish]

World Health Organization (1985) Energy and protein requirements. Geneva:FAO/WHO/UNU.

World Health Organization (1997) Obesity. Preventing and managing the global epidemic.Geneva.

Wyshak G (2000) Teenaged girls, carbonated beverage consumption, and bone fractures [seecomments]. Arch Pediatr Adolesc Med 154, 610-613.

Wållberg L, Becker W, Enghardt H, Hagman U & Tille A (1996) Riktlinjer förbarnomsorgens måltider. Uppsala: The Swedish National Food Administration NFA.Stockholms läns landsting, Kommunförbundet.[In Swedish]

Yperman AM & Vermeersch JA (1979) Factors associated with children's food habits.Journal of Nutrition Education 11, 72-76.

Öhlin A, Callmer E, Hoel L & Lindeskog P (1993) Vad äter barn på dagis? Huddinge:Hälsomålet. [In Swedish]

Østberg B & Thorsen LE (1981) Intervjuteknikk. In Muntlige Kilder. Om bruk av intervjuer ietnologi, folkeminnevetenskap og historie [B Hodne, K Kjelstadli and G Rosander, editors].Drammen: Universitetsforlaget.

Page 62: Pre-school Children’s Food Habits and Meal Situationuu.diva-portal.org/smash/get/diva2:161313/FULLTEXT01.pdf · school, the behaviour of the pre-school staff or the children’s

62

Dissertation for the Degree of Doctor of Philosophy in Domestic Sciences presented atUppsala University in 2002

SAMMANFATTNINGSepp, H. 2002. Pre-school Children’s Food Habits and Meal Situation – FactorsInfluencing the Dietary Intake at Pre-school in a Swedish Municipality. ActaUniversitatis Upsaliensis. Comprehensive Summaries of Uppsala Dissertation from theFaculty of Social Sciences 114. 64 pp. Uppsala. ISBN 91-554-5240-X.

Det finns flera skäl att studera förskolebarns matvanor. Ett skäl är att barn generelltoch globalt är en s.k. sårbar grupp såväl nutritionellt som socio-ekonomiskt. En viktigaspekt är på vilket sätt strukturella faktorer, dvs. förutsättningar i barnens näraomgivning påverkar barns matvanor. I Sverige är ca ¾ av barnen inskrivna i den välutbyggda barnomsorgen och äter där en stor del av sina måltider. Det innebär att deflesta föräldrar överlämnar en del av ansvaret för barnens mat och ätbeteende under enstor del av dagen till ansvariga inom barnomsorgen. Samhället påtar sig detta ansvarbl.a. genom att Statens Livsmedelsverk utfärdar näringsrekommendationer och ävenriktlinjer för hur måltiderna inom barnomsorgen bör vara sammansatta. Därmed visaräven myndigheten att det är viktigt vilken mat barnen serveras inom barnomsorgen.Anvisningarna innehåller bl.a. förslag till livsmedelssammansättning på olikamåltidstyper. Hur dessa anvisningar uppfattas och följs inom förskolan är av intresseför såväl föräldrar, förskoleansvariga som myndigheter. Det är särskilt viktigt när deekonomiska ramarna minskar, som under senare år krävts inom all offentligverksamhet.

Det övergripande syftet med studien var att empiriskt kartlägga förskolebarns energi-,närings- och livsmedelsintag. Ett delsyfte var att jämföra barnens dagliga energi- ochnäringsintag beroende på om mjölk respektive vatten serverades som måltidsdryck tilllunch på förskolan. Ett annat delsyfte var att studera värderingar och beteendenbeträffande barnens mat och måltider som förskolepersonal visar under sitt arbete ochsom kan influera på barnens matvanor samt att beskriva barnens egna erfarenheter avmat och måltider på förskolan.

Totalt har tolv förskolor i en kranskommun till Stockholm studerats. Urvalet var attalla förskolor hade ett eget tillagningskök och att de serverade tre måltider om dagen,att hälften av dem serverade vatten och hälften av dem serverade mjölk sommåltidsdryck till lunch. Etthundranio barn i åldern 3-5 år kostregistrerades under 7dagar, all mat på förskolan vägdes individuellt, varje barn fick sin egen bricka medsina egna små uppläggningskärl så att den serverade maten och även resterna kundevägas. Av praktiska skäl skattades intaget hemma med hjälp av hushållsmått och/ellerLivsmedelsverkets bilderbok Matmallen.

Page 63: Pre-school Children’s Food Habits and Meal Situationuu.diva-portal.org/smash/get/diva2:161313/FULLTEXT01.pdf · school, the behaviour of the pre-school staff or the children’s

Sammanfattning

63

Barnens och personalens attityder och värderingar samt kommentarer och beteendenkring kostfrågor beskrevs genom fokusgruppsintervjuer med barnen och halv-strukturerade intervjuer med personalen.

Resultaten visar att barnen hade ett tillfredsställande energi- och näringsintag totalt settunder dagen jämfört med nordiska näringsrekommendationer. Däremot var detgenomsnittliga energiintaget på förskolan lågt jämfört med riktlinjerna förbarnomsorgen. Detta kompenserades dock genom måltiderna hemma, vilketresulterade i att barnens genomsnittliga intag per dag låg i nivå med referensvärdet föråldersgruppen.

Med tanke på att många barn vistas många timmar på förskolan, i den här studien varsnittet knappt 8 timmar, så kan man anta att rekommendationerna för barnomsorgen ärsatta på en rimlig nivå, dvs. 65% av sitt energiintag bör de få på förskolan om de vistasdär full tid. Barnen i den här studien har inte riktigt nått upp till dessa nivåer. Medhänsyn till resultaten bör inte antalet måltider på förskolan minskas, utan att det börserveras åtminstone frukost, lunch och eftermiddagsmål.

Maten som serverades på förskolorna var variationsrik och av god kvalité med hänsyntill näringstäthet. I stort sett åt alla barnen all mat som serverades på förskolan, vilketinnebar att de fick en god chans att smaka och bekanta sig med eventuella nyamaträtter och livsmedel. Livsmedelsintaget var mer varierat på vardagen än helgen.Mat med låg näringstäthet och/eller högt energiinnehåll, t ex läsk, saft, bullar ochgodis, bidrog med 20% av energin på vardagen och 33% under helgen. PrincipalComponent Analys visade ett motsatsförhållande mellan å ena sidan mejeriprodukter åandra sidan bröd och frukostcerealier. Resultaten kan tolkas som att dessamejeriprodukter intogs i huvudsak utan bröd och frukostcerealier och främst sommellanmål.

Barnens dagliga energi- och näringsintag var inte beroende på om mjölk respektivevatten serverades som måltidsdryck till lunch på förskolan. Frånvaro av mjölk vidlunch kompenserades vid de andra måltiderna. Studien ger inga bevis om mjölk ellervatten skall serveras som måltidsdryck med hänsyn till den totala mjölkkonsumtionenoch den allmänna kostkvalitén. Barnen med en låg mjölkkonsumtion hade en merjämn fördelning av energi från olika livsmedelsgrupper än barn med en högmjölkkonsumtion. Kostanalyserna stödjer rekommendationen att en begränsning till enhalv liter mjölk och fil/yoghurt per dag verkar vara en rimlig mängd.

I fokusgruppsintervjuerna framkom det att barnen förknippade mat och ätande medregler och normer. De flesta barnen beskrev dessa regler och normer i form av maninte får göra. De visste mycket väl vad som var accepterat och icke accepterat beteendekring matbordet, i synnerhet vad man inte fick göra. Barnen blev tillfrågade att berättaom vilken mat som de inte tyckte om, men de pratade de istället om vilken mat de

Page 64: Pre-school Children’s Food Habits and Meal Situationuu.diva-portal.org/smash/get/diva2:161313/FULLTEXT01.pdf · school, the behaviour of the pre-school staff or the children’s

Sammanfattning

64

tyckte om. Vuxna kategoriserar ofta mat som bra och dålig, medan barnen i vår studiepratade om mat och inte mat. Till exempel är godis inte mat. Fokusgruppsintervjuerbedömdes som en bra metod att undersöka barns tankar och reflektioner om mat.

Personalens brist på utbildning och kunskap om mat och måltidens betydelseresulterade i en ambivalens och osäkerhet om deras roll som vägledare imåltidssituationen. Trots detta hade de flesta en klar bild över vad som menas med enpedagogisk måltid. Det var att hjälpa och stödja barnen att klara sig själva samt attvara en förebild vid måltiden. Till skillnad mot förr, var det inte vanligt förekommandeatt mat användes i den pedagogiska verksamheten. Samtidigt som personalen var nöjdmed förskolans roll att tillfredställa barnens kostbehov uttryckte de en oro eller t.o.m.misstro mot föräldrarna. Trots, eller kanske p.g.a. brist på kunskap om mat ochnutrition och bristen på specifika mål med den pedagogiska måltiden, trodde de att de iförskolan var bättre rustade än föräldrarna för att utbilda barn om mat och en bättregarant för barnens kostintag och matvanor. Sammanfattningsvis, förskolepersonalensroll har förändrats de senaste åren och de saknar en solid grund att integrera mat ochmåltider i det dagliga arbetet som förskolelärare och barnskötare.