Perspectives of Children With Physical Disabilities on ... · 302 Hutzler, Fliess, Chacham, and Van...

18
300 Yeshayahu Hutzler, Osnat Fliess, and Anat Chacham are with The Zinman College for Physical Education and Sport Sciences at Wingate Institute, 42902 Israel. Email: <[email protected]>. Yves Van den Auweele is with the Department of Psy- chology at the Catholic Univeristy of Leuven, Belgium. Perspectives of Children With Physical Disabilities on Inclusion and Empowerment: Supporting and Limiting Factors Yeshayahu Hutzler, Osnat Fliess, and Anat Chacham Zinman College, Israel Yves Van den Auweele Catholic University of Leuven, Belgium The purpose was to explore the personal experiences of children with physical disabilities in physical education (PE) and to identify supporting and limiting mechanisms to their inclusion and empowerment. A computerized analysis of individual profiles was performed based on in-depth interviews with 8 fe- males and 2 males, ages 9 to 15, who were included in regular PE classes. Two individual profiles served as examples for situations faced during inclusive settings and typical reaction patterns. A comparative qualitative analysis of interview themes generated five main categories of themes: assistive devices, physical activity, peers, important adults, and self. Experiences during physi- cal activity were identified as supporting or limiting empowerment within each category, based on selected criteria. An almost equal distribution of sup- porting and limiting factors was observed. Children with disabilities are increasingly being included in physical educa- tion (PE) lessons around the world (DePauw & Doll-Tepper, 2000; Downs, 2001; Hutzler, Yaakov, Almosny, & Bergman, 2001; Lienert, Sherrill, & Myers, 2001). Guidelines for inclusion practice have been delineated in many countries such as the USA (Block, 2000; Sherrill, 1998), Canada (Active Living Alliance of Canadi- ans with a Disability, 1995), Australia (Downs, 1995), Finland (Heikinaro- Johansson, Sherrill, French, & Huuhka, 1995), and Israel (Superintendent Ministry of Education, 1994). Published research covers mostly teachers’ and peers’ atti- tudes toward the included children (Block & Rizzo, 1995; Block & Zeman, 1996; Slininger, Sherrill, & Jankowski, 2000) and outcomes of the inclusion process (Houston-Wilson, Dunn, van der Mars, & McCubbin, 1997; Liebermann, Dunn, ADAPTED PHYSICAL ACTIVITY QUARTERLY, 2002, 19, 300-317 © 2002 Human Kinetics Publishers, Inc.

Transcript of Perspectives of Children With Physical Disabilities on ... · 302 Hutzler, Fliess, Chacham, and Van...

Hutzler, Fliess, Chacham, and Van den Auweele300

300

Yeshayahu Hutzler, Osnat Fliess, and Anat Chacham are with The Zinman Collegefor Physical Education and Sport Sciences at Wingate Institute, 42902 Israel. Email:<[email protected]>. Yves Van den Auweele is with the Department of Psy-chology at the Catholic Univeristy of Leuven, Belgium.

Perspectives of Children With PhysicalDisabilities on Inclusion and Empowerment:

Supporting and Limiting Factors

Yeshayahu Hutzler, Osnat Fliess, and Anat ChachamZinman College, Israel

Yves Van den AuweeleCatholic University of Leuven, Belgium

The purpose was to explore the personal experiences of children with physicaldisabilities in physical education (PE) and to identify supporting and limitingmechanisms to their inclusion and empowerment. A computerized analysis ofindividual profiles was performed based on in-depth interviews with 8 fe-males and 2 males, ages 9 to 15, who were included in regular PE classes. Twoindividual profiles served as examples for situations faced during inclusivesettings and typical reaction patterns. A comparative qualitative analysis ofinterview themes generated five main categories of themes: assistive devices,physical activity, peers, important adults, and self. Experiences during physi-cal activity were identified as supporting or limiting empowerment withineach category, based on selected criteria. An almost equal distribution of sup-porting and limiting factors was observed.

Children with disabilities are increasingly being included in physical educa-tion (PE) lessons around the world (DePauw & Doll-Tepper, 2000; Downs, 2001;Hutzler, Yaakov, Almosny, & Bergman, 2001; Lienert, Sherrill, & Myers, 2001).Guidelines for inclusion practice have been delineated in many countries such asthe USA (Block, 2000; Sherrill, 1998), Canada (Active Living Alliance of Canadi-ans with a Disability, 1995), Australia (Downs, 1995), Finland (Heikinaro-Johansson, Sherrill, French, & Huuhka, 1995), and Israel (Superintendent Ministryof Education, 1994). Published research covers mostly teachers’ and peers’ atti-tudes toward the included children (Block & Rizzo, 1995; Block & Zeman, 1996;Slininger, Sherrill, & Jankowski, 2000) and outcomes of the inclusion process(Houston-Wilson, Dunn, van der Mars, & McCubbin, 1997; Liebermann, Dunn,

ADAPTED PHYSICAL ACTIVITY QUARTERLY, 2002, 19, 300-10© 2002 Human Kinetics Publishers, Inc.

ADAPTED PHYSICAL ACTIVITY QUARTERLY, 2002, 19, 300-317© 2002 Human Kinetics Publishers, Inc.

Perspectives on Inclusion and Empowerment 301

van der Mars, & McCubbin, 2000; Vogler, van der Mars, Cusimano, & Darst,1990). Thus far, limited information has been reported regarding the includedchildren’s own perceptions of the inclusion process in PE lessons (Blinde &McCallister, 1998; Goodwin & Watkinson, 2000; Goodwin, 2001; Place &Hodge, 2001).

Inclusion, as used in this study, is defined as providing specially designedinstruction and support for students with special needs in regular PE settings (Moore& Gilbreath, 1998). Inclusion is guided by the fundamental principle of valuingdiversity (Rogers, 1993). Belonging, acceptance, and a sense of being supportedare essentials of an inclusive school environment, where all the educational needsof a child with a disability are expected to be met (Stainback & Stainback, 1990).However, because children with disabilities comprise the minority in schools, theirspecific needs are often not being met (Block, 1999). Empowerment is an alterna-tive to advocacy and professional management in minority and community orga-nization (Rappaport, 1981; Zimmerman & Rappaport, 1988). Our study linksinclusion and empowerment, based on the belief that personal empowerment ofchildren with disabilities may help their inclusion process.

Empowerment refers to gaining control over one’s life and assuming re-sponsibility for changes that lead to a healthy, active lifestyle and positive mentalhealth (Hutzler & Sherrill, 1999). Empowerment has roots in social learning theory(Bandura, 1997) and particularly in social-constructivist theory (Paris & Byrnes,1989). Constructivism is defined as a set of beliefs about knowing and learningthat emphasizes the active role of learners in constructing their own knowledge(von Glasersfeld, 1989). In this view, the learner constructs knowledge in an at-tempt to integrate existing knowledge with new experiences. The social world of alearner includes the people who directly affect that person (e.g., teachers, friends,students, administrators, and participants in all forms of activity). Learning takesplace in contexts that are personally meaningful to students (Gergen, 1995). Im-portant factors for empowerment research from the latter view are (a) contextualrelativism (i.e., external stimuli have a mediating function, and their impact needsto be studied related to a context), and (b) personal construction of meaning (i.e.,learning is the process of making sense of what was experienced).

Past physical activity research has linked empowerment explicitly with ex-perienced athletes in wheelchair sport contexts (Hutzler, 1990; Pensgaard &Sorensen, 2002; Wuerch, 1996). In the present study, an attempt has been made tolink perspectives of school children with disability, mostly with cerebral palsy, ontheir inclusion and empowerment. The purpose was to explore the personal expe-riences of children with disabilities in PE and other physical activities within in-clusive environments and to identify supporting and limiting mechanisms tochildren’s empowerment. Criteria were used that were manifested within the frame-works of constructivism and theories related to coping with stressful life events.These relate to locus of control (Rotter, 1966), hardiness (Kobasa, 1979), and senseof coherence (Antonovsky, 1987). Based on these frames of reference, criteria forsupporting empowerment were (a) critical thinking and problem solving, (b) senseof self-efficacy and self-respect, (c) social competence and leadership, and (d)active learning, commitment, and adherence (Downs, 1995; Sadan, 1997). Crite-ria for limiting empowerment were (a) limited self-initiated learning, (b) fatalistreactions, (c) presenting helplessness behaviors, and (d) relying on external re-sources for learning, coping, and decision making.

Hutzler, Fliess, Chacham, and Van den Auweele302

Method

Participants

Participants were 10 children (8 females and 2 males) with physical disabilitieswhose ages ranged from 9 to 15 years. Access to medical files of school children inIsrael is not approved. Therefore, the participants were drawn out of a collective of110 children, who participated in leisure time activities organized by a nationalnonprofit organization for rehabilitation of children with disabilities during thepast 5 years. Fifty-six children of this collective were included in the regular schoolsystem in three of the five educational districts in Israel. A criterion-based sam-pling procedure (Goetz & Le Compte, 1984), also known as purposive sampling,was implemented. Criteria were established in order to assure diversity, expres-sive capability, and reliability in the following categories: (a) school levels (el-ementary, junior, and high school), (b) mode of locomotion (without aids, withbrace, with crutches, with wheelchair), (c) level of participation in PE (non, full,partial), (d) type of disability (spastic diplegia, spastic hemiplegia, spinal motoratrophy), and (e) gender (male and females). The 10 children comprising the finalsample reflected typical students with physical disabilities throughout Israel. Mosthad cerebral palsy. Children and parents completed an informed consent. Table 1describes the participants.

Procedure and Measures

We used the intraindividual correlational procedure developed by De Boeck andRosenberg (1988). This is a method for quantifying the frequency and hierarchy of

Table 1 Personal Details of the Participants

Partici-pation

ID# Gender Age Disability School Mobility in PE

1 Male 10 Spastic diplegia Elementary Crutches Full

2 Female 13 Spastic diplegia Junior HS Crutches Full

3 Female 13 Spastic diplegia Junior HS Crutches Partial a

4 Female 9 Spastic hemiplegia Elementary Independent Full

5 Female 14 Spinal motor atrophy Junior HS Wheelchair Partial

6 Female 14 Spastic hemiplegia Junior HS Independent Full

7 Female 12 Spastic hemiplegia Junior HS Independent Full

8 Female 13 Spastic hemiplegia Junior HS Independent Full

9 Male 15 Spastic diplegia High S Independent Full

10 Female 14 Limb-girdle High S Independent Partialmuscular dystrophy

aUsed to participate before entering junior high school.

Perspectives on Inclusion and Empowerment 303

relationships across qualitatively analyzed verbal comments acquired via asemistructured interview. This method was initially designed in Belgium for psy-chological counseling of elite athletes (De Boeck & Rosenberg 1988; Van Mele,Van den Auweele, & Rzewnicki, 1995). This was the first experience of using itwith children.

When working with children with disabilities who are integrated in the regu-lar school system, it is extremely difficult to find enough children with the samebackground and the same environmental contexts to meet requirements of experi-mental group research (i.e., homogenous group structure, random selection of groupmembers). Therefore, the intraindividual procedure is an alternative solution. Thismethod may be valid for children because, similar to elite athletes, the participantis facing distress that stems from the discrepancy between objective capabilitiesand/or beliefs in ability and desired outcomes.

The intraindividual correlational design uses the mathematical model of hi-erarchical class analysis. It contains two sets of categories: (a) specific situationsand (b) individual reactions: emotions, behaviors, and thoughts (De Boeck &Rosenberg, 1988). The computer analysis yields a hierarchical structure of rela-tionships between these sets of categories characterized as classes of situations(appearing at the bottom of each profile) and classes of reactions (emotional andbehavioral responses—appearing at the top of each profile). Figure 1 demonstratesa model profile, in which situations are represented in boxes and reactions in ovals.

The individual analysis performed in this study required four stages:

1. Collecting verbal descriptions of reactions (emotions, behaviors) and situa-tions reported by the participating children via semistructured in-depth in-terviews.

2. Categorizing the verbal descriptions into a grid of emotions/behaviors bysituation (see Table 2).

3. Rating the descriptors in the grid by the participants during a second session.4. Performing the hierarchical class analysis by means of a computer program

(De Boeck, 1992).

Collecting Descriptions. The aim of the interview was to identify the sup-porting and limiting factors for empowerment of children with disabilities in ac-tivities requiring physical function and movement at school settings (e.g., physicaleducation, recess, excursions). The interview was semistructured, asking the chil-dren how they felt and what they did in reaction to situations related to physicalactivity in the inclusive environment. Two experienced postgraduate able-bodiedfemale professionals (ages 28-29) who were familiar with the children conductedinterviews. The interviews took place during summer at the children’s homes. In-terviews followed guidelines for conducting participant observations and inter-views with children (Alan Fine & Sandstrom, 1988). To assist the interviewers infacilitating children’s reactions, a guideline of activity categories (including allactivities requiring movement at school) was structured. These activity categorieswere identified in a pilot study, including 21 children of the same age and disabil-ity as in the present study (Hutzler, Chacham, & Fliess, 2000). Guideline criteriafor the interview follow:

1. Situations during PE lessons, recreational activities, and trips organized bythe school

2. Situations during alternative activities instead of PE lessons in school

Hutzler, Fliess, Chacham, and Van den Auweele304

3. Situations during physical activities outside school (such as sport center forchildren with disabilities)

4. Different situations that involved social support networks (parents, counse-lor, teachers, peer, other children from school, assistant, etc.)

5. Situations connected to getting feedback and marks in PE6. Situations connected to social activities in school7. Situations connected to the disability (with aspects of social popularity)8. Situations connected to different periods during personal development, such

as moving from one school to another, being hospitalized, etc.

Categorization. After the data had been collected, the interviewer con-structed an individual grid for each child. This grid contained the relevant catego-ries depicting situations and reactions (emotions/behaviors) mentioned by the par-ticipant. Table 2 shows an example of such a grid.

Rating. In the next stage, the child rated each of the situations and indi-cated to what extent each one of the emotions and behaviors was suitable to thesituation. An 11-point rating scale was used with verbal descriptions at 0 (not at allapplicable/never happened), 5 (just barely applicable/just barely happened), and10 (completely applicable/always happened) (Van den Auweele, De Cuyper, VanMele, & Rzewnicki, 1993; Van Mele et al., 1995).

Individual Profiling. Since the computer model analyzes only binary data(De Boeck & Rosenberg, 1988), the participants’ scores were electronically di-chotomized. In this way, all the numbers from 0 to 4 became 0, and all the numbersfrom 5 to 10 became 1. Table 2 shows a comparison of raw and dichotomizedcategories. The structural model (Figure 1) clarifies the connection between theoriginal grid (filled in by the participant) and the resulting scheme. The computeranalysis addressed to the following principles, which are paraphrased from De Boeck& Rosenberg (1988), Van den Auweele et al. (1993), and Van Mele et al. (1995).

1. A class of situation is a collection of equivalent situations (i.e., situationsin which the same reactions are present). “During break time” and “When I suc-ceed performing a difficult exercise,” for example (Class I in Figure 1) are bothconnected with the same reactions: “I feel good” and “I feel one of the group”(Class 2 in Table 2), thus belong to the same class. Situation classes are markedwith alphabetical characters in Figure 1.

2. A class of reaction is a collection of equivalent reactions connected withthe same situations. Reaction classes are marked with Arabic numbers in Figure 1:“I find a substitute, solution: I sit instead of stand; I walk instead of run” and “Iinsist on my own goals” (Class 5 in Figure 1) are connected with the same situa-tion: “When being confronted with a difficult exercise” (Class E in Figure 1).

3. A situation class can be in terms of its reaction patterning an extension orreduction of one or more other classes of situations. When a second class is anextension of a first class, the reaction pattern of the second class is enlarged withadditional reactions compared to the first class. In the example appearing in Figure1, the situations in Class B containing “being a child with disability in a regularschool” and “participating in PE lesson” is an extension of Classes D and E con-taining “getting the same attention (from teachers) like other kids” and “whenbeing confronted with a difficult exercise.” A reduction is the reverse of an extension.The situations in class I containing “during break time” and “when I succeed per-forming a difficult exercise” are a reduction of Class C: “when my classmates

Perspectives on Inclusion and Empowerment 305

cheered me, when I succeeded to jump a rope.” A class that is an extension ofanother one is put above the other and connected with it by means of an arrowfrom the extension class toward the reduction class. The result is a hierarchicalstructure of situation class. A class of reactions can likewise be considered in termsof the group of associated situations an extension or a reduction of one or morereaction classes. The number of classes characterizes the hierarchy among reac-tions where 1 is the highest.

4. The hierarchical relations between the reaction classes are represented inthe same way as for situations (i.e., with an arrow from the higher to the lowerclass). The equivalent classes of situations and reactions, together with their re-lated extensions and reductions, form a simultaneous hierarchical structure of situa-tion and reaction classes. The hierarchy among reactions is characterized by the numberof classes where A is the highest. The association between a situation class and itsdirect reaction pattern is represented in the profile with a zigzag line (Figure 1).

5. Goodness-of-fit is the degree of correspondence of attributes to a similarpattern. Goodness-of-fit scores appear as a decimal number to the right of eachreaction in the structural model of the hierarchical class analysis. The situation orreaction with the highest goodness-of-fit (1.00 or nearly 1.00) is the best prototypeof its class (Van den Auweele et al., 1993). An overall goodness-of-fit with a scoreabove .60 indicates that the model is acceptable (Van Meele et al., 1995). Thecomputerized analysis is performed from a low to a high rank (e.g., rank 1 to 5),fixed in advance by the researcher, so that several solutions are obtained. Thechoice of the definitive rank depends on (a) the complexity of the solution (manyclasses and relationships or not) compared to that of a higher or lower rank, (b) theincrease in goodness-of-fit when choosing a higher rank, and (c) the additionalinformation that would be obtained in the case of a higher rank. The interpretabil-ity of the overall model solution offers a content-related criterion.

Comparative Analysis. In order to cope with omission of details by usingthe computerized methodology, a traditional qualitative analysis of themes ap-pearing in the individual profiles (Strauss & Corbin, 1990) was also performed. Inthis second level analysis, the situations and reactions described in the individualprofiles were categorized into themes labeled “mediating factors” (Pensgaard &Sorensen, 2002). The contextual trend of these factors for each case was analyzedbased on the criteria of empowerment described in the introduction and identifiedas supporting or limiting empowerment, based on consensus among two experi-enced postgraduate professionals. If discrepancies arose between independent pro-fessionals’ suggestions, they discussed the topic until reaching an agreement.

Results and Discussion

The results are presented in two main sections: (a) example profiles representing apersonal (intraindividual) perspective and (b) a comparative analysis representinga group perspective.

Analysis of Individual Example Profiles

Figure 1 shows an example of the hierarchical profiles (participant #1) that werederived through the intraindividual analysis. Based on the individual data, a

Hutzler, Fliess, Chacham, and Van den Auweele306

Figure 1 — The individual model of participant #1 (goodness of fit = .90).

summary table for each participant (Table 3) was constructed, describing (a) themediating factors categorized into supporting and limiting factors, (b) the mostdominant situation, and (c) the most dominant reaction.

Two participants (#1 and #8) were selected to demonstrate the personal con-struction of meaning from what they experienced. Both had a high overall good-ness-of-fit (.90 and .84). The most dominant situation (best prototype) of #1 was“when my friends assist me” (e.g., jump rope, Class A). It is linked to reactionssuch as “I don’t care” (Class 1), “I feel like one of the group” (Class 2), “I feelnormal, like everybody else” (Class 2). For Participant # 8, the most dominant

Perspectives on Inclusion and Empowerment 307

Tab

le 2

Exa

mpl

e of

Par

t of

the

Gri

d—P

arti

cipa

nt #

1 (D

icho

tom

ized

)

0 (n

ever

hap

pene

d)5

10 (

alw

ays

happ

ened

)●

Situ

atio

ns1.

2.3.

4.5.

6.7.

Whe

n be

ing

Whe

n I

par-

Dur

ing

brea

kW

hen

my

Get

ting

the

Whe

n ki

dsB

eing

a c

hild

conf

ront

edtic

ipat

e in

time

frie

nds

assi

stsa

me

atte

n-as

k m

e w

hat

with

dis

abili

tyw

ith a

dif

fi-

PE le

sson

sm

e (e

.g.,

tion

(fro

mdo

I h

ave

in a

reg

ular

cult

exer

cise

jum

p ro

pe)

teac

hers

)(a

bout

my

scho

olR

eact

ions

like

othe

r ki

dsdi

sabi

lity)

1. I

fin

d a

subs

titut

e,1

10

00

01

solu

tion:

I s

it in

stea

dof

sta

nd; I

wal

kin

stea

d of

run

2. I

get

res

t1

01

00

00

3. I

fee

l nor

mal

, lik

e1

11

11

01

ever

y bo

dy e

lse

4. I

talk

with

my

frie

nds

00

10

00

0

5. I

fee

l tha

t the

y do

it, n

ot0

00

10

00

beca

use

of m

y di

sabi

lity,

but

beca

use

I am

thei

r fr

iend

Hutzler, Fliess, Chacham, and Van den Auweele308

Table 3 Situations, Reactions, and Mediating Factors

Most dominant Most dominant Supporting factors Limiting factors# situation reaction for inclusion for inclusion

1 “When my friends “It’s all right -My friendshelp me” (e.g., with me” -My PE teacherhelping me -Myself _______________jumping, etc.) -The physical

activity

2 “A friend in “I want to -My self -Other childrenelementary school participate” -My PE teacher -Alternativetold me that I can’t -My friends activity insteadparticipate in the of being withshow” my classmate

-failures in physicalactivity

3 “When I par- “I feel good -My friends -PE lessons inticipate in PE about it” -My classmates which I don’t takelesson” -PE and physical a part“When I do activity -When some boysport in Win- -My teachers mocked me andgate (in a program -My school teased mefor children with -The breakdisability)

4 “When I had to use “It is important -My parents -My disabilitya brace” for me to be like -Myself -An assistive

every body else” -My teacher device (brace)-Other teachers -When some boy-My best friends mocked me and-PE and physical teased meactivity

5 “A boy came to “It does not -Myselfme and told me bother me” -My sense ofthat he feels pity humorfor me” -My teachers

& stuff-The assistive de-vice (wheelchair)- Classmates-PE and physicalactivity

(continued)

Perspectives on Inclusion and Empowerment 309

situation (best prototype) was “I am asking my teacher to tell the classmates aboutmy disability” (Class A). It is linked to reactions such as “I am going to see whatmy teacher says” (Class 1), “try to cope myself” (Class 2), “try to be strong” (Class3). One interpretation of these reactions is that Participant # 1 has high self-confidence and an optimistic attitude to life. His friends accept and help him; hefeels comfortable and is one of the group. In contrast, Participant # 8 seeks supportfor coping in the adult world rather than with her peers.

In Class B, Participant #1 reports “participating in a class excursion in spiteof a difficult terrain,” linked to reactions such as “I feel like one of the group”

Table 3 (continued)

Most dominant Most dominant Supporting factors Limiting factors# situation reaction for inclusion for inclusion

6 “When I participate “I am telling -My mother -My disabilityin PE lesson” my mommy” -My girl friends -When some boy“During break time, from junior high mocked megossiping with my -My classmates -My girl friendsfriends” -PE and physical from elementary“When I activity schoolget the chance not -My teachers -Changing environ-to take a part in and stuff ment (framework)PE lesson”

7 “When I perform “I feel like every- -My classmates -Other children witha sport activity that body else” -Other weak peer disabilitiesno one forces me in class (anomaly -children who areto do (marathon classmate) mocking merun)” -PE and physical

activity

8 “I ask my teacher “I am going to -My teacher -My disabilityto tell my class- my teacher” -Other weak peer -PE lessonsmate about my in class (anomaly -The adult worlddisability” classmate) -Other nondisabled

children (classmate)

9 “When I get help “I feel -My classmate -An assistive device(assistance) embarrassed” -My friends (wheelchair) thatduringPE lessons” -Myself emphasizes my

-My teachers differenceand stuff

10 “Participating “I feel I am a -My classmates -Other childrenin sports in normal child” -My friend with disabilities.extracurricular -Myselfactivities”

Hutzler, Fliess, Chacham, and Van den Auweele310

(Class 2), “I feel normal, like everybody else” (Class 2). In the same Class B,Participant # 8 reported about difficulties during PE lessons, linked to reactionssuch as “sitting aside, partially participating” (Class 8), “leaving class” (Class 4),“trying to cope by myself” (Class 2), and “going to my teacher” (Class 1). Bothparticipants report a coping situation accompanied by completely different reac-tions. This example reflects the differences between an empowered child, high incommitment to his peer group and the other nonempowered child, who is likely towithdraw if anticipating stress and difficulties.

In Class C, Participant #1 describes a situation in which “my classmatescheered me when I succeeded to jump rope,” linked to reactions such as “I feelembarrassed, I feel that they do it, not because of my disability, but because I amtheir friend” (Class 3), and “I don’t care” (Class 1). In Class D, Participant #1reports “getting the same attention (from teachers) as other kids,” linked to “It’sobvious” (Class 4) and “I don’t care” (Class 1). On the other hand, Participant # 8reports in Class D, “I share with my teacher problems that I have” linked to “I feelgood about it” and “I feel that I get attention” (Class 5). Again, the same patternappears of # 1 being included in the peer group and receiving the same amount ofattention as peers, whereas # 8 persists on getting special attention from her teacher.

Finally, in Class E, Participant #1 reports being confronted with a difficultexercise. It links to “I find a substitute, a solution: I sit instead of stand, I walkinstead of run,” and “I insist on my own goals” (Class 5). On the other hand,Participant # 8 reports “I get a mark that I don’t deserve—lower than the otherkids,” and “I don’t do things because of my disability.” These are linked to “I amangry” (Class 10), “It makes me feel bad, it’s bothering me, I cry, I talk to my PEteacher” (Class 6). Consistently, Participant # 1 is coping, adapting himself to thesituation, and insists on not giving up, while Participant # 8 gets into a conflictthrough comparing herself to peers and through her low perception of compe-tence. Therefore, furious emotions and extreme reactions are shown. Once again,her default choice is to turn to her teacher and seek help.

The major outcome of this personal analysis of example profiles was thedemonstration of individual differences in response structure while facing a simi-lar situation. One participant tended to lean on internal resources and his peergroup to expand his experience repertoire and to creatively suggest solutions topotentially stressful events, while the other depended exclusively on external, adultresources and tended to avoid confrontation with stressful life events. Researchindicates that internalizing locus of control through intervention is positively re-lated to academic and psychosocial achievements in non-PE settings (Ketchel &Bieger, 1989; Swan, 1990). It is recommended to explore this issue in PE as well.

Comparative Analysis of Mediating Factors

A total of five main categories (assistive devices, peers, physical activity, adults,and the self) were generated during the comparative content analysis. These cat-egories represent themes related to situations and reactions reported during theinclusive school activity.

Assistive Devices. In 2 participants (# 4 and #9), the assistive device (armbrace and wheelchair) was linked with feelings of distress (“It was embarrassing,”“I felt I am not worthy,” “I feel different”). It even caused one child to lie about thereason he wore a brace (“I’m saying that I broke my arm, and now I have to wear

Perspectives on Inclusion and Empowerment 311

it—I lie to them”). Contrarily, another child (#5) described situations in which herassistive device (wheelchair) became a supporting factor as a means for her class-mates to compete against each other to get permission to push her. This situationwas linked to positive feelings (e.g., “I like it,” “It makes me laugh,” “I feel a partof the group”). This example demonstrates the differences between a child whoperceives stressful life events (the use of assistive devices) as a challenge and anopportunity for social interaction, compared to a child who tries to avoid beingassociated with a device because of fear of its perceived negative social impact.

Peers. Classmates, best friends, and other children were mentioned fre-quently. The comments did not show a general supportive or limiting pattern. Foursubcategories of peers’ mediating factors follow.

Six out of the 10 participants gave information about being teased and ridi-culed by other children (e.g., imitating their walk, expressing pity, opening thebrace), with reactions mostly categorized as having a limiting function. These in-cluded emotional reactions (“I hate it,” “I am afraid,” “I feel different,” “I’m dis-appointed,” “I cry”) and help-seeking reactions (asking Mom or the class teacherto handle it). A few reactions could be categorized as supportive, reflecting copingbehaviors such as humor (“I make a joke out of it”). Three participants reportedexcluding behaviors (i.e., peers deliberately ignoring them during game situations).These caused reactions that were mostly categorized as limiting, with a similarstructure, as for teasing and mockery.

On the other hand, 4 participants mentioned peers insisting on their partici-pation and encouraging them to participate during game and exercise activities(e.g., helping to perform jump rope, lowering the cadence of a rhythm game, cheer-ing during participation, asking to come and join, changing and adapting gamerules). These behaviors caused reactions mostly categorized as supportive, includ-ing satisfaction (“I feel good,” “I like it”), but the most important reaction seemedto be the feeling of participation (“I feel one of the group, like everybody else,” “Ifeel something in common”).

A final observation related to peers was the relative limited reference toother children with a disability. Most participants had extracurricular interactionswith other children with a disability. Nevertheless, only two (# 7 and #10) referredto these children explicitly: “I wouldn’t like to be seen with them.” This may be anindication that the major role model accepted by our participants was that of theable-bodied children.

Physical Activity. The impact of physical activity as a potential mediatorfor inclusion and empowerment was explicitly investigated in this study. All par-ticipants were included in regular PE at least some time during their schoolinghistory.

All participants reported experiencing activities during PE lessons in whichthey were not successful. This fact reflects the most common method for includingchildren with disabilities in PE lessons in Israel and other countries: “Let them dowhat they can.” The child needs to experience failure and success in order to de-cide when to participate and when not. Very little structured adaptation was re-ported by our sample. Most children reacted to perceived failures with emotionssuggesting positive coping with the situation: “Trying to be strong, it’s somethingthat teaches me a lot, trying to cope, I accept it, feel like everybody else.” Somechildren expressed emotions related to ego-defending mechanisms: “It doesn’tbother me,” I feel good about it”. Four children expressed emotions of distress:

Hutzler, Fliess, Chacham, and Van den Auweele312

“embarrassed,” “disappointed,” “sad,” “it is hard for me,” “it makes me cry.” Twochildren suggested active coping: “I find an alternative solution, participating par-tially.” Four out of the 10 participants were relying on external help: “tell the PEteacher,” “tell the class teacher,” “tell Mom.”

Graded task difficulty is a basic element in education in general and in adaptedphysical education in particular (Block, 2000). However, our findings suggest thatfailing to master a task sometimes has an empowering function (i.e., “I find analternative solution.”) In fact, empowerment will not take place if only winningoccurs, because the individual has no need for it. Empowerment is initiated throughconflict and perceived deficits rather than through sufficient or abundant successand self-actualization (Sadan, 1997). Teachers and other adults following the fal-lacy that everyone is a winner or giving an A grade in PE in spite of limited partici-pation are not likely to produce empowerment. Without abandoning programaccommodations (such as allowing a bounce in volleyball or two bounces in wheel-chair tennis), the teachers, guardians, and even peers should challenge studentswith tasks they need to achieve, even though success will require much effort. Thisseems to be particularly important for high ego, low task-oriented children, whomight otherwise refrain from participation and apply an opportunistic attitude (i.e.,only participate if gratification is expected). Short-term combined with long-termtask oriented goal setting seems most applicable in such cases and has resulted inpositive outcomes in promoting sit-ups performance in children with behavioraldisorders (Bar-Eli, Hartman, & Levi-Kolker, 1994).

Several children reported specific inclusion techniques. One child (#5) re-ported using an individualized worksheet and practicing alone, while other class-mates did another activity. Her main reaction was ego-defending (“It doesn’t botherme”). Another child (#9) reported working with a teacher’s assistant in the after-noon. His strongest reaction was “embarrassed,” “I prefer to do it all alone.”

Although all children participated in complementary extracurricularnoninclusive physical activity, only 2 children (#5 and #10) explicitly reportedparticipation in activities of a sport center for children with disabilities. Both indi-cated, “It’s a good idea.” An interesting question is why so few children mentionedthis aspect. Perhaps the separated activity was perceived as therapeutic and func-tional rather than social. Another reason may be that children identified with thephysical activity models of able-bodied society rather than that of persons withdisabilities and did not perceive their activities and achievements in a separateenvironment as equally impressive. Children with disabilities consistently andunanimously tried to identify themselves with their able-bodied peers. One of themeven said that she is ashamed to be seen with other children with disabilities. Preju-dice and stereotypes may spread among peers generalizing a negative attitude to-ward “the disabled” which is often shared by individuals with disabilities themselves(Sherrill, 1998). Excessive avoidance of peer models with disability and insistingon normative models may increase the gap between ideal and real images of selfand be linked to maladaptive behaviors such as avoidance and anxiety.

We suggest that children with disabilities and their parents be informed aboutnetworks, sport clubs, and associations of children with disabilities where theirunique cases receive an audience and generate peer support. The power perceivedamong comparable peers could then be transferred to the relations with nondisabledchildren. In one case, a child reported doing this by showing her able-bodied peersthe medals she had won in a swimming competition for children with disabilities.

Perspectives on Inclusion and Empowerment 313

The able-bodied peer group was the most significant support system observed inthis study. Thus, peer education using fact sheets for explaining disabilities inchildren’s terms is strongly recommended. One girl in this study reported on writ-ing a book in which she describes her own disability. Both expert and child-writtenmaterials may be useful for this purpose. We encourage general PE teachers tohelp nondisabled peers be more empathetic and accepting of their disabled peers.While some studies (Block & Rizzo, 1995; Block & Zeman, 1996) suggest thatthis is a maintainable goal, recent findings show this to be a huge challenge(Goodwin & Watkinson, 2000; Goodwin, 2001; Place & Hodge, 2001).

One child (#7) reported participation in an extracurricular activity in an in-clusive environment (a road race). Her emotional reaction was supportive in termsof inclusion: “I feel like everybody else.” Four children described confusion re-garding their PE grade. Two participants reported receiving a high grade in spite ofalmost no participation in PE classes. The children felt this was somewhat odd,and one of them said, “I make a joke out of it.” In contrast, 2 children reportedreceiving a low grade, which did not take their limitations into account. In thesesituations children reported feeling “embarrassed,” “being different,” “bothered.”Adapted grading criteria and norm tables such as those included in the BrockportFitness Test (Winnick & Short, 1999) could help normalizing children’s experi-ences in this regard.

Four children referred to class excursions and one to a dance performance asa reason for exclusion. Two of them (#1 and #5) reported participation in spite ofthe physical difficulties, and the other two (#3 and #4) were excluded from theseactivities. The participating children reacted with emotions of “being one of thegroup,” “feeling integrated,” but also “don’t let them feel pity for me.” In contrast,the excluded children reported feeling “different,” “resigned,” “disappointed.” Theyexpressed their desire to participate, but also displayed an ego-defending mecha-nism: “I feel good about it.” Both children consulted with their mom and the classteacher, and one of them summarized, “too bad, but there is no other choice.”Goodwin and Watkinson (2000) also reported exclusion from special events andperformances similarly resulted in frustration.

Adults. Participants mentioned several important adults as mediators. Re-actions to this particular type of mediators follow. The impact of the PE teacher onthe children’s emotions and behavior in our sample seems minor. One child re-ferred to a teacher offering participation and another to a teacher excluding from aspecific activity. The reactions were fairly equal: “It is important for me to partici-pate,” “I want to be like everybody else.” This finding suggests increasing thepreservice and in-service training related to adapted physical activity for PE teach-ers in Israel. Based on recent publications (Block, 1999; Lienert et al., 2001), itseems that adapted physical activity training and expertise is still lacking, even incountries with a longer tradition of inclusion. One reason for PE teachers tendingto avoid inclusion of children with physical disabilities may be that they are forcedinto a situation that requires extra preparation and safety measures without any prepa-ration and sometimes without even knowing for sure the child’s limitations. Propertraining and access to practical information could decrease avoidance in this regard.

The function of the class teacher and other staff members was mentionedoften (#3, 5, 8 and 9). These individuals were approached whenever a child haddifficulties in coping with the situation. Probably the children felt more confidentwith these individuals than with the PE teacher.

Hutzler, Fliess, Chacham, and Van den Auweele314

Parents, particularly the mother (mentioned by 3 female participants), had arelieving function when children seemed to be frustrated by a situation and haddifficulties in coping with it by themselves. We hypothesize that because the mothertypically symbolizes a caring and protecting agent, she is mentioned whenever thechild avoids coping and seeks being cared for.

The Self. Although the self was not explicitly referred to as a factor inconstructing the situation, an impact of individual power was obvious in manyparticipants. Five of the 10 children used statements demonstrating their own powersand coping: “I insist on what I want,” “I find a solution,” “I don’t let them havepity for me,” “I suggest to the teacher which exercises I should do,” “I let themread the book I wrote,” “I want to prove that I can,” “I take part in the decision.”These statements describe an internal type of personal resource in contrast to ex-ternal resources, such as Mom and the class teacher.

From an empowerment perspective, the ability to decide, to suggest solu-tions, and to insist on one’s own goals are desirable. In these participants, the selfis being experienced as a most supportive mechanism. In those children who haveto rely on external support, the self fails to become such a mechanism. It may beassumed that some children experience an event or situation as supporting em-powerment while others experience it as supporting helplessness because they havedifferent ways of constructing meaning from experiences. A number of psycho-logical constructs in social and cognitive learning theories can be used to explainthis phenomenon. Among the most common are self-efficacy theory (Bandura,1997) and goal perspective theory (Duda, 1989). Self-efficacy stands for the strengthand magnitude of perceived ability to accomplish a certain task. Goal perspectivetheory includes two types of orientations valuing the individual level of efficacy:(a) a self-referenced task orientation, focusing on mastery and (b) a norm-refer-enced ego orientation, focusing on comparison with others. Facing failure ornoninclusion, children who are low on both ego orientation and self-efficacy maywant to rely on others to solve their problems, as Participant #8 in our sample. Incontrast, children who are high on task/ego orientations and self-efficacy, as Par-ticipant #1 in our sample, would probably insist on being included, modifying thetask and suggesting ways the teacher can help. Children high on ego orientationand low on self-efficacy (such as Participant #10 in our sample) may use ego-defending mechanisms (i.e., give up) and say “It doesn’t matter, I don’t like doingit.” If, however, task accomplishment is highly valued by significant others, theymight use maladaptive behaviors such as cheating or risking their own health tosecure mastery (White & Duda, 1993). Therefore, a differential and versatileapproach to empowering children is warranted, based on the analysis of (a)their perceived sense of efficacy, (b) their goal perspectives, and (c) theirsupport systems.

Summary

The methods of analysis revealed no general trend suggesting inclusion as sup-porting or limiting empowerment. Goodwin and Watkinson (2000) posed an eco-logical perspective referring to “good days” and “bad days” as central ideas fororganizing children’s reactions. In the present study, we have used a socialconstructivist approach, valuing individual powers (self-efficacy, goal perspective)adjacent to external powers (peers, important adults, physical activity) as mediat-ing factors for empowerment.

Perspectives on Inclusion and Empowerment 315

We found that over half of the comments related to experiencing failureduring physical activity supported empowerment. This depicts the importance ofexposure to real-life within an inclusive setting, rather than instruction by gradedtask difficulty in a segregated setting. We recommend that future researchers focuson a variety of inclusive settings while measuring outcome effects related to em-powerment. Hedrick’s unique study of practicing and competing in wheelchairtennis (1985) is an example for a design, representing an experimental approach toexamine effects on performance, self-efficacy, and anxiety. This design could berepeated with further outcome effects, populations, and task circumstances.

Specific questionnaires designed to measure the relationship between inclu-sion and mediating factors outlined in this study (i.e., peers, important adults, physi-cal activity variables, and personal attributes) should be used in large scale andcross cultural studies. On the other hand, case studies are warranted to depict theindividual process of coping in an inclusive setting. A triangulation of quantitativeand qualitative methods, as performed by Vogler, Koranda, and Romance (2000),is particularly suitable for this purpose.

References

Active Living Alliance of Canadians with a Disability. (1995). The inclusion action pack:Increasing active living opportunities for persons with a disability. Glouchester,Ontario: Author.

Alan Fine, G., & Sandstrom, K.L. (1988). Knowing children: Participant observation withminors. Newbury Park, CA: Sage.

Antonovsky, A. (1987). Unraveling the mystery of health. San Francisco: Jossey Bass.Bandura, A. (1997). Self-efficacy: The exercise of control. New York: Freeman.Bar-Eli, M., Hartman, I., & Levi-Kolker, N. (1994). Using goal setting to improve physical

performance of adolescents with behavior disorders: The effect of goal proximity.Adapted Physical Activity Quarterly, 11, 86-97.

Blinde, M.E., & McCallister, G.S. (1998). Listening to the voices of students with physicaldisabilities. Journal of Physical Education, Recreation and Dance, 69(6), 64-68.

Block, M.E. (2000). A teacher’s guide to including students with disabilities in regularphysical education (2nd ed). Baltimore: Paul H. Brooks.

Block, M.E. (1999). Did we jump on the wrong bandwagon? Problems with inclusion inphysical education (Part I). Palaestra, 15(3), 30-38; 55-56.

Block, M.E., & Rizzo, T.L. (1995). Attitudes and attributes of physical education teacherstoward including students with severe and profound disabilities into regular physicaleducation. Journal of the Association for Persons with Severe Handicaps, 20, 80-87.

Block, M.E., & Zeman, R. (1996). Including students with disabilities on regular physical edu-cation: Effects on nondisabled children. Adapted Physical Activity Quarterly, 13, 38-49.

De Boeck, P. (1992). Hierarchical classes analysis release 2. Faculty of Psychology andEducation, University of Leuven, Tiensestraat 102, 3000 Leuven, Belgium.

De Boeck P., & Rosenberg, S. (1988). Hierarchical classes: Model and data analysis.Psychometrika, 53, 361-381.

DePauw, K. & Doll-Tepper, G. (2000). Toward progressive inclusion and acceptance: Mythor reality? The inclusion debate and bandwagon discourse. Adapted Physical Activ-ity Quarterly, 17, 135-143.

Downs, P. (2001, July). A society for all through adapted physical activity. Paper presentedat the 13th International Symposium Adapted Physical Activity. Vienna, Austria.

Downs, P. (1995). Willing and able: An introduction to inclusive practices. Canberra, Aus-tralia: The Australian Sport Commission.

Hutzler, Fliess, Chacham, and Van den Auweele316

Duda, J.L. (1989). Goal perspectives and behavior in sport and exercise settings. In C.Ames & M. Maehr (Eds.), Advances in motivation and achievement (Vol. 4, pp. 81-115). Greenwich, CT: JAI Press.

Gergen, K.J. (1995). Social construction and the educational process. In L.P. Steffe & J.Gale(Eds), Constructivism in education (pp. 17-39). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.

Goetz, J.P., & LeCompte, M.D. (1984). Ethnography and qualitative design in educationalresearch. Orlando, FL: Academic Press.

Goodwin, D.L. (2001). The meaning of help in PE: Perceptions of students with physicaldisabilities. Adapted Physical Activity Quarterly, 18, 289-303.

Goodwin, D.L., & Watkinson, E.J. (2000). Inclusive physical education from the perspec-tive of students with physical disabilities. Adapted Physical Activity Quarterly, 17,144-160.

Hedrick, B.N. (1985). The effect of wheelchair tennis participation and mainstreamingupon the perceptions of competence of physically disabled adolescents. TherapeuticRecreation Journal, 19, 34-46.

Heikinaro-Johansson, P., Sherrill, C., French, R., & Huuhka, H. (1995). Adapted physicaleducation consultant model to facilitate integration. Adapted Physical Activity Quar-terly, 12(1), 12-33.

Houston-Wilson, C., Dunn, J.M., van der Mars, H., & McCubbin, J. (1997). The effects ofpeer tutors on the motor performance in integrated physical education classes. AdaptedPhysical Activity Quarterly, 14, 298-313.

Hutzler, Y. (1990). The concept of empowerment in rehabilitative sports. In G. Doll-Tepper,C. Dahms, B. Doll, & H. von-Selzam, (Eds.), Adapted Physical Activity: An Interdis-ciplinary Approach (pp. 43-51). Heidelberg, Germany: Springer Verlag.

Hutzler, Y., Yaakov, T., Almosny, Y., & Bergman, U. (2001). Including children with physicaldisability in school and community: A textbook. Tel Aviv, Israel: The Mofet Institute forResearch, Curriculum, and Program Development for Teacher Educators (in Hebrew).

Hutzler, Y., Chacham, A., & Fliess, O. (2000). Empowerment of children with dis-abilities within inclusive physical activity settings. Final research report, ZinmanCollege for Physical Education and Sport Science and the Israel Sport Author-ity. (in Hebrew).

Hutzler, Y., & Sherrill, C. (1999). Disability, physical activity, psychological well-beingand empowerment: A life-span perspective. In R. Lidor and M. Bar-Eli (Eds.), Sportpsychology: Linking theory and practice (pp. 281-300). Morgantown: Fitness Infor-mation Technology, 1999.

Ketchel, J.A., & Bieger, G.R. (1989, April). The efficacy of a psychosocially based drugprevention program for young adolescents. Paper presented at the annual meeting ofthe New England Educational Research Organization. Portsmouth, NH.

Kobasa, S. (1979). Stressful life events, personality, and health: An inquiry into hardiness.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 37(1), 1-11.

Liebermann, L.J., Dunn, J.M., van der Mars, H., & McCubbin, J. (2000). Peer tutors’ ef-fects on activity levels of deaf students in inclusive elementary physical education.Adapted Physical Activity Quarterly, 17, 20-39.

Lienert, C., Sherrill, C., & Myers, B. (2001). Physical educators’ concerns about integratingchildren with disabilities: A cross-cultural comparison. Adapted Physical ActivityQuarterly, 18, 1-17.

Moore, C., & Gilbreath, D. (1998). Educating students with disabilities in general educa-tion classrooms: A summary of the research. Eugene, OR: Western regional resourcecenter web-site. http://interact.uoregon.edu/wrrc/AKInclusion.html.

Paris, S.G., & Byrnes, J.P. (1989). The constructivist approach to self-regulation and learn-ing in the classroom. In B. Zimmerman & D. Schunk (Eds.), Self-regulated learningand academic achievement: Theory, research and practice (pp. 169-200). New York:Springer Verlag.

Perspectives on Inclusion and Empowerment 317

Pensgaard, A.M., & Sorensen, M. (2002). Empowerment through the sport context: A modelto guide research for individuals with disability. Adapted Physical Activity Quar-terly, 19, 48-67.

Place, K., & Hodge, S.R. (2001). Social inclusion of students with physical disabilities ingeneral education: A behavioral analysis. Adapted Physical Activity Quarterly, 18,389-404.

Rappaport, J. (1981). In praise of paradox: A social policy of empowerment over preven-tion. American Journal of Community Psychology, 9(1), 1-25.

Rogers, J. (1993). The inclusion revolution. Phi Delta Kappa Research Bulletin, 11(4), 1-6.Rotter, J.B. (1966). Generalised expectancies for internal versus external control of rein-

forcement. Psychological Monographs, 80, (1, Whole No. 609).Sherrill, C. (1998). Adapted physical activity, recreation and sport: Crossdisciplinary and

lifespan. (5th ed.). Dubuque IA: WCB/McGraw.Sadan, E. (1997). Empowerment and community planning. Tel Aviv, Israel: Hakkibutz

Hameuchad (Hebrew).Slininger, D., Sherrill, C., & Jankowski, C.N. (2000). Children’s attitudes toward peers

with severe disabilities: Revisiting contact theory. Adapted Physical Activity Quar-terly, 17, 176-196.

Strauss, A., & Corbin, J. (1990). Basics of qualitative research. Newbury Park, CA.:Sage.

Stainback, S., & Stainback, W. (1990). Inclusive schooling. In W. Stainback & S. Stainback(Eds.), Support networks for inclusive schooling (pp. 3-24). Baltimore: Paul H.Brookes.

Superintendent Ministry of Education (1994). Changes in the policy of treating childrenwith difficulties in normal education and placing students in special education. Spe-cial newsletter No 12. Jerusalem, Israel: Ministry of Education (in Hebrew).

Swan, K. (1990, April). Perceived locus of control and computer-based instruction. Paperpresented at the Annual Meeting of the American Educational Research Association,Boston, MA.

Van Mele, V., Van den Auweele, Y., & Rzewnicki, R. (1995). An integrative procedure forthe diagnosis of an elite athlete: A case study. The Sport Psychologist, 9, 130-147.

Van den Auweele Y., De Cuyper, B., Van Mele, V., & Rzewnicki, R. (1993). Elite perfor-mance and personality: From description and prediction to diagnosis and interven-tion. In R.N. Singer, M. Murphey, & L.K. Tennant (Eds.), Handbook of research onsport psychology (pp. 257-289). New York: MacMillan

Vogler, E.W., Koranda, P., & Romance, T. (2000). Including a child with severe cerebralpalsy in physical education: A case study. Adapted Physical Activity Quarterly, 17,161-175.

Vogler, E.W., van der Mars, H., Cusimano, B., & Darst, P. (1990). Relationship of presage,context, and process variables to ALT-PE of elementary-level mainstreamed students.Adapted Physical Activity Quarterly, 7, 298-313.

von Glasersfeld, E. (1989). Cognition, construction of knowledge, and teaching. Synthese,80, 121-140.

White, S.A., & Duda, J.L. (1993). Dimensions of goals and beliefs among adolescent ath-letes with physical disabilities. Adapted Physical Activity Quarterly, 10, 125-136.

Winnick, J., & Short, F. (1999). The Brockpoert physical fitness test manual. Champaign,IL: Human Kinetics.

Wuerch, G.A. (1996). Sport as empowerment: Perspectives of women wheelchair road rac-ers. Unpublished Master’s thesis in Physical Education, Texas Women’s University,Denton, TX.

Zimmerman, M.A., & Rappaport, J. (1988). Citizen participation, perceived control andpsychological empowerment. American Journal of Community Psychology, 16, 725-750.